number 1 serial no. 65 · number 1 serial no. 65 page main editor karina weichold issbd bulletin...

43
Contents ISSBD SPECIAL SECTION EMOTION REGULATION ACROSS THE LIFE SPAN Supplement to International Journal of Behavioral Development Volume 38 Issue 3 May, 2014 Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science University of Jena, Am Steiger 3/Haus 1 D-07743 Jena, Germany Email: [email protected] Co-Editor Deepali Sharma ISSBD Bulletin Department of Psychology Christ University, Hosur Road Bangalore 560029 India Email: [email protected] ISSBDBulletin B S I S D ISSBD Bulletin (Print) ISSN 2040-5235 ISSBD Bulletin (Online) ISSN 2040-5243 1 Introduction to Emotion Regulation across the Life Span Karina Weichold and Deepali Sharma 2 The Importance of Authentic Emotion Regulation Junilla K. Larsen and Tammy English 6 Parental Assessment of Emotional Symptoms and Psychological Abnormalities in Adolescents Marc Schipper, Dennis Nitkowski, Ute Koglin, and Franz Petermann 10 Parenting and Emotion Regulation in the Adaptive and Academic Competencies of Chinese American Youth Jeffrey Liew 13 Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: An Integrative Examination of Parenting and Sadness Regulation in Italy and the United States Laura Di Giunta and Anne-Marie R. Iselin 17 Emotion Regulation in Intimate Relationships Claudia M. Haase 22 Are we Really Getting Better? Lifespan Differences in Emotion Regulatory Ability from the Perspective of Developmental Functionalism Natalie Tuck, Iris Mauss, and Nathan S. Consedine COMMENTARIES 27 A Commentary on Emotion Regulation across the Life Span Susan T. Charles 30 The Elusiveness of a Life-Span Model of Emotion Regulation Tamara L. Sims and Laura L. Carstensen COUNTRY FOCUS 33 Towards a Child Inclusive and Child Focused Child Custody Decision Making Model in Zimbabwe Gwatirera Javangwe SOCIETY 38 Notes from the President Wolfgang Schneider NEWS 41 News from the IJBD Editor: Announcing the New Editorial Team for the International Journal of Behavioral Development Brett Laursen 42 Major Conferences of Interest

Upload: others

Post on 30-Jul-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

ContentsISSBD SPECIAL SECTIONEMOTION REGULATION ACROSS THE LIFE SPAN

Supplement to International Journal of Behavioral Development Volume 38 Issue 3 May, 2014

Number 1 Serial No. 65

Page

Main EditorKarina WeicholdISSBD BulletinDepartment of Developmental PsychologyCADS – Center for Applied Developmental ScienceUniversity of Jena, Am Steiger 3/Haus 1D-07743 Jena, GermanyEmail: [email protected]

Co-EditorDeepali SharmaISSBD BulletinDepartment of Psychology Christ University, Hosur RoadBangalore 560029IndiaEmail: [email protected]

ISSBDBulletinBSI

S

D

ISSBD Bulletin (Print) ISSN 2040-5235ISSBD Bulletin (Online) ISSN 2040-5243

1 Introduction to Emotion Regulation across the LifeSpan Karina Weichold and Deepali Sharma

2 The Importance of Authentic Emotion Regulation Junilla K. Larsen and Tammy English

6 Parental Assessment of Emotional Symptoms and Psychological Abnormalities in AdolescentsMarc Schipper, Dennis Nitkowski, Ute Koglin, and Franz Petermann

10 Parenting and Emotion Regulation in the Adaptive andAcademic Competencies of Chinese American YouthJeffrey Liew

13 Adolescent Depressive Symptoms: An IntegrativeExamination of Parenting and Sadness Regulation in Italy and the United States Laura Di Giunta and Anne-Marie R. Iselin

17 Emotion Regulation in Intimate RelationshipsClaudia M. Haase

22 Are we Really Getting Better? Lifespan Differences in Emotion Regulatory Ability from the Perspective of Developmental Functionalism Natalie Tuck, Iris Mauss, and Nathan S. Consedine

COMMENTARIES

27 A Commentary on Emotion Regulation across theLife Span Susan T. Charles

30 The Elusiveness of a Life-Span Model of EmotionRegulation Tamara L. Sims and Laura L. Carstensen

COUNTRY FOCUS

33 Towards a Child Inclusive and Child Focused ChildCustody Decision Making Model in ZimbabweGwatirera Javangwe

SOCIETY

38 Notes from the President Wolfgang Schneider

NEWS

41 News from the IJBD Editor : Announcing the NewEditorial Team for the International Journal ofBehavioral Development Brett Laursen

42 Major Conferences of Interest

JBD_Bull_Cover:420 x 280mm 3/21/14 2:18 PM Page 1

Page 2: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Special Section:Introduction to EmotionRegulation across the Life Span

Karina WeicholdUniversity of Jena, GermanyE-mail: [email protected]

and

Deepali SharmaUniversity of Bangalore, IndiaE-mail: [email protected]

Every day, individuals are confronted with many stimuli ofpotentially arousing impact. To respond to these stimuli inan inappropriate and extreme fashion (with regards tothoughts, actions, and social interactions) hinders positivepsychological adaptation within a given society and relatesto the emergence of problem behaviors. Thus, individualshave to learn to regulate their emotions to optimally copewith daily demands. Emotion regulation across the lifespan is a fascinating topic within developmental science:In children, knowledge of how to regulate strong emotionsgrows, and they begin to regulate their emotional expres-sions according to standards for social appropriateness(mostly with the help of external scaffolding). During ado-lescence, inner capacities for emotion regulation increasetremendously, also as a result of advances in brain develop-ment. Finally, emotion regulation in adults still continues toevolve and shape social interactions tremendously.

This special section brings together a series of featurearticles focusing on emotion regulation issues by applyinga developmental perspective. First, Larsen and English dis-cuss the importance of authentic emotion regulation. Then,

three papers deal with emotion regulation processes in ado-lescence with a specific focus on the emergence of problembehaviors (Schipper et al.; Di Giunta & Iselin), and compe-tence development (Liew), with some focus on a cross-cul-tural perspective. One other paper addresses emotionregulation in intimate relationships in adulthood (Haase),and another investigates the ability to regulate emotionsacross the life span (Tuck et al.). These articles are discussedby two extraordinary experts in the field, Susan Charles andLaura Carstensen (together with their colleague TamaraSims), who developed the well-known life-span theory onemotion regulation within social relationships. We aregrateful to all authors within the special section who pres-ent and discuss on their work, and we hope that the readersenjoy reading this interesting selection of new work in thearea of emotion regulation as much as we did.

This Bulletin also includes the notes by the presidentof ISSBD, Wolfgang Schneider. His position as presidentwill end this summer – we would like to thank him forhis enduring support of the ISSBD Bulletin, which wereally appreciated and enjoyed during the past years. Also,Brett Laursen as the new Editor of the IJBD introduces itsnew editorial team to the readers – we would like to takethe opportunity to wish him lots of success in this newposition.

Finally, as usual, we would like to invite the readers ofthe Bulletin to share their ideas with us to improve this out-let of the International Society for the Study of BehavioralDevelopment, to give us feedback, and to discuss with ustopics for upcoming special issues.

•1

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

Page 3: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

The Importance of AuthenticEmotion Regulation

Junilla K. LarsenRadboud University Nijmegen, The NetherlandsE-mail: [email protected]

and

Tammy EnglishWashington University in St. Louis, USAE-mail: [email protected]

Authenticity is an important psychological construct,reflecting the unobstructed operation of one’s true, or core,self in one’s daily enterprise (Kernis, 2003). In this featurearticle, we discuss how authenticity operates in the contextof emotion regulation (ER). In particular, we propose thatindividuals differ in the extent to which they engage inauthentic emotion regulation and this individual differencepredicts how effectively emotions will be regulated. First,we provide a broad overview of existing emotion regula-tion frameworks. Next, we define authentic emotion regu-lation. Finally, we discuss the implications that authenticemotion regulation has for emotional well-being and sug-gest some potential future directions.

Definitions and existing emotion regulationframeworks

Recently, there has been a growing interest in the constructof emotion regulation. Different definitions of emotion regu-lation exist in the developmental and the social psychologyliteratures. However, most researchers agree that emotionregulation stands in service of accomplishing goals and facil-itating adaptive responses to the environment. The predomi-nant framework is Gross’ process model of emotionregulation (Gross, 1998). The process model distinguishesbetween different types of emotion regulation strategiesbased on where they occur in the emotion generative pro-cess. In total, it identifies five categories of emotion regula-tion: situation selection, situation modification, attentionaldeployment, cognitive change, and response modulation.The first four categories are termed ‘‘antecedent-focused’’,because they include efforts to regulate emotions before theactivation of a full emotional response. The fifth category,response modulation, is termed ‘‘response-focused’’, becauseit occurs after a full emotional response has been activated.

The process model of emotion regulation has been a use-ful heuristic for guiding systematic research into emotionregulation deficits, and recently this model has receivedincreased attention in the developmental literature. Two keystrategies that have received significant empirical attentionin this regard are the antecedent-focused strategy of cognitivereappraisal (reinterpreting emotional stimuli in terms thatmodify the emotional impact) and the response-focused strat-egy of expressive suppression (inhibiting the behavioral

display of emotion). Response-focused (late) strategies(e.g., suppression) are thought to be less effective than ante-cedent-focused (early) strategies (e.g., reappraisal) becausetiming is regarded as a proxy for emotional intensity.Although there has been some support for this position,recent work suggests that the story may be more compli-cated. For instance, emotion timing and intensity seem tobe more important for certain types of regulation (reapprai-sal but not distraction) (e.g., Sheppes & Gross, 2011).

Most empirical investigations, including our own work,have examined the overall effectiveness of specific strate-gies without taking into account the context in which theyare deployed. Considering that the effectiveness of regula-tion strategies may differentially vary across situations,optimal regulation requires that people be sensitive to thesituational context, have a diverse repertoire of regulatorystrategies to draw on, and are able to monitor feedbackabout the effectiveness of a strategy (Bonanno & Burton,2013). We propose that components of authentic emotionregulation are important in enabling people to be more sen-sitive to the situational context and to monitor feedbackabout the relative efficacy of a chosen regulatory strategy.

Authentic emotion regulation:A conceptual framework

Several theoretical models view authenticity as integral towell-being, although there has been some definitional con-fusion regarding this construct (Kernis, 2003; Wood, Linley,Maltby, Baliousis, & Joseph, 2008). We define authenticemotion regulation based on Kernis’ conceptualization ofauthenticity as an individual difference construct that dis-tinguishes between four different components: awareness,acceptance, action, and relational. The awareness compo-nent of authenticity involves knowledge of one’s needs,values or feelings. Non-awareness has also been definedas self-alienation and refers to the mismatch between con-scious awareness and actual experience (Wood et al.,2008). Focusing on authentic emotion regulation, the aware-ness component is synonymous with emotional clarity(people’s meta-knowledge of their affective experience).The acceptance component of authenticity involves theunbiased processing of self-relevant information. In thecontext of authentic emotion regulation, this componentwould be reflected in objectivity and acceptance of one’semotions, instead of denial of the existence of certain emo-tions. The action component of authenticity focuses onwhether people live in accord with one’s true self. In termsof authentic emotion regulation, this refers to the use ofstrategies that are in line with one’s values, preferences, andneeds. Finally, the relational component of authenticityrefers to being genuine and not ‘‘fake’’ in one’s relationshipwith close others. With regards to authentic emotion regu-lation, this relational aspect would involve sharing one’s

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•2

Page 4: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

genuine emotions and subsequent regulation efforts withclose others.

Similar to global authenticity, we regard authentic emo-tion regulation as the unobstructed operation of one’s true,or core, self in one’s daily emotional experiences. The four-stage model likely reflects an ongoing developmental pro-cess. You must first come to understand and accept whatyou are feeling in order to facilitate behavior that deals withyour inner values about authentic emotion regulation andto share this process with close others. There can also befeedback loops between these different components. Forexample, objective emotional acceptance may facilitate fur-ther emotional clarity. Obstructions to authentic regulationcan occur at many steps along the way (e.g., fear of rejectionmay interfere with authentic display of emotion). Next,we review existing empirical evidence relevant to the fourdifferent components of authentic emotion regulation, anddiscuss whether and how each separate component mayinfluence the effectiveness of emotion regulation and impactbroader well-being.

Emotional clarity

The first component of authentic emotion regulation refersto people’s meta-knowledge of their affective experience,which typically increases during development. Individualdifferences in emotional clarity are a core component inseveral higher order constructs, including emotional intel-ligence and alexithymia. If you don’t know how you arereally feeling, you’ll have a hard time trying to managethose feelings, which likely explains why people who haverelatively less insight into their own feelings appear to be athigher risk for developing diverse forms of psychopathol-ogy, including depression and anxiety disorders (O’Toole,Hougaard, & Mennin, 2013).

There is some evidence to suggest that emotional clarityis associated with better executive functioning (Koven &Thomas, 2010) and self-regulatory strength (Muraven &Baumeister, 2000), and that the benefits of clarity may alsocome from availability of cognitive resources (Wilkowski &Robinson, 2008). As such, emotional clarity might be mostimportant for ‘cognitive engagement’ strategies (Parkinson& Totterdell, 1999), including rumination and reappraisal,which rely on deliberate, controlled, executive processes(e.g., Koster, De Lissnyder, Derakshan, & De Raedt, 2011).In line with this idea, survey studies have shown interactiveeffects of low emotional clarity with the use of frequent cog-nitive reappraisal in explaining psychopathology andfuture problematic substance-use outcomes in adults(Boden, Bonn-Miller, Kashdan, Alvarez, & Gross, 2012;Boden, Gross, Babson, & Bonn-Miller, 2013). Moreover,people with better updating ability of emotional informa-tion in working memory (an executive process) report lessnegative emotions when reappraising and ruminating atboth the trait level and in daily life (Pe, Raes, & Kuppens,2013). The moderating role of working memory functioningfor efficacy of emotion regulation is also supported byexperimental research (Schmeichel, Volokhov, & Dernaree,2008). Individuals who have low emotional clarity may letthemselves be more overwhelmed by emotional situationsdue to executive dysfunction, and then try to repair thisproblem afterwards by using different emotion regulation

strategies, some of which may be less successful whenimplemented later (e.g., cognitive reappraisal).

From a developmental perspective, it would be interest-ing to examine emotional clarity, executive function, and theeffectiveness of ‘cognitive engagement’ emotion regulationstrategies across different age groups. If emotional clarityincreases with age, some emotion regulation strategies thatare considered generally adaptive (such as reappraisal)might not necessarily work as effectively for adolescents asfor adults. Taking a life-span approach, older adults maybe less successful using strategies that require cognitive con-trol, but may compensate for lowering cognitive control bychanges in emotional preferences, insights, and optimaliza-tion of emotion regulation through lifelong learning andpractice (Scheibe & Carstensen, 2010; Shiota & Levenson,2009).

Objective emotional acceptance

The second component of authentic emotion regulationinvolves the unbiased processing of self-relevant informa-tion, including both objectivity and acceptance of one’semotions. Acceptance is not conceptualized as a passive pro-cess; instead, it is regarded as a process of non-judgmentallyengaging with negative emotions. The judgment that anemotion is unacceptable may lead to negative emotions andmaladaptive efforts to regulate these subsequent emotions,whereas acceptance may present opportunities to enhanceemotional clarity and flexibility and may reduce maladap-tive regulation strategies. A meta-analysis of experimentalstudies has shown that induced emotional acceptance,regarded as a specific emotion regulation strategy, is super-ior to other emotion regulation strategies with respect to paintolerance, but not for pain intensity and negative affect(Kohl, Rief, & Glombiewski, 2012). However, experimentalparadigms only investigate short-term effects of acceptanceof specific, manipulated emotions. Randomized controlledtrials that assign participants to acceptance interventions(e.g., mindfulness interventions) have proven effective andseveral survey studies have shown a negative associationbetween acceptance and negative affect, with acceptancemediating the relationship between age and negative emo-tions (Shallcross, Ford, Floerke, & Mauss, 2013).

As already mentioned, emotional acceptance as an indi-vidual difference variable may importantly determine well-being through influencing the effectiveness of emotionregulation strategies. People having difficulties acknowled-ging certain emotions in themselves may develop defensiveprocesses to deny their emotions, which can impede effec-tive regulation. Along these lines, research has for instancefound that rumination is no longer associated with depres-sive symptoms after controlling for negative evaluation ofone’s emotions, suggesting that a lack of emotional accep-tance is an important mechanism explaining the maladap-tive effects of rumination (Rude, Maestas, & Neff, 2007).Moreover, we propose that emotional acceptance may beparticularly important for facilitating the effectiveness ofemotion regulation strategies in the context of uncontrollablelife events. Recent work has shown that reappraisal isdifferentially associated with depression depending on thecontrollability of the experienced stress, with reappraisal onlybeing effective among people experiencing uncontrollable

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•3

Page 5: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

stress (Troy, Wilhelm, Shallcross, & Mauss, 2010). Futureresearch is needed to better understand the interactionbetween emotion acceptance and regulation strategies indifferent contexts (i.e., controllable versus uncontrollable lifeevents), mechanisms underlying the effectiveness of emo-tional acceptance, and possible age-related changes in accep-tance during development.

Emotion regulation in accord with one’sinner authentic experiences

The third component of authentic emotion regulation refersmost directly to the process of authentic emotion regulationitself. Specifically, it involves regulating emotions in accordwith one’s inner authentic experiences—that is, choosingstrategies that fit with one’s own attributes. We proposethat emotion regulation in accord with one’s inner authen-tic experiences is important in all situations. However,expressive suppression is an example of an emotion regula-tion strategy that might be more likely to lead to feelings ofincongruence and inauthenticity, as the direct outer displayof one’s emotions is not in accord with one’s inner feelings.Recent studies among (young) adults support the idea thatsubjective authenticity is important, finding that it mediatesthe association between use of expressive suppression indaily life and socio-emotional well-being (English & John,2013; Impett et al., 2012). However, this strategy does notnecessarily have to lead to feelings of incongruence. Forinstance, individuals from Eastern, interdependent cultures(e.g., Chinese) may value expressive suppression because itcan help to preserve interpersonal harmony, and thereforethey may feel more authentic when suppressing their visi-ble display of emotions. We propose that the adverse effectsof suppression will be reduced if individuals do not feelinauthentic when suppressing their emotions. In supportof this idea, a 14-day daily-experience study of people inromantic relationships showed that Canadian undergradu-ates with higher levels of interdependence experiencedboosts in personal well-being and relationship quality ifthey suppressed their negative emotions during sacrifice(Le & Impett, 2013). In addition, many studies have failedto find a relationship between suppression and well-beingin more collectivistic cultures, such as China or other Asiancountries (e.g., Soto, Perez, Kim, Lee, & Minnick, 2011).Another group that might not feel inauthentic suppressingtheir emotions is adolescents: Although suppression hasbeen shown to have clear socio-emotional costs among(young) adults (e.g., English, John, Srivastava, & Gross,2012; Srivastava, Tamir, McGonigal, John, & Gross, 2009),these effects do not seem to emerge among adolescents(Larsen et al., 2012; Larsen et al., 2013). Future research isneeded to determine whether and why this might be thecase. Moreover, future studies should specifically ask aboutwhether the emotion regulation strategy leads one to feelauthentic, and test how this translates to well-being.

Balanced sharing of emotions withclose others

The final component of authentic emotion regulationincludes sharing of emotions with close others. We proposethat a balance is needed because both too much and too

little sharing emotions with close others can be maladap-tive. The well-known ‘writing paradigm’ of Pennebakerand colleagues has shown the cathartic view of expression(Pennebaker, 2004). Social sharing of emotion is also impor-tant because of the reactions it can elicit from others. Forinstance, individuals can benefit from sharing their emo-tions with close others by social integration, social support,and help in putting things in perspective. On the otherhand, the prolongation of sharing processes can be mala-daptive (Curci & Rime, 2012), as in the case of coruminativeinteractions (Rose, Carlson, & Waller, 2007). We foundthat depressive symptoms preceded the development ofincreased use of expressive suppression over time, and thiseffect was mediated by less experienced parental supportfor adolescent girls (Larsen et al., 2012). Depressive peopleoften seek excessive reassurance of affective bonds andoverly express their emotions, which may elicit stress intheir close relationships (Hankin, Stone, & Wright, 2010).Our findings suggest that inhibition of the outer displayof feelings may sometimes be a reaction to the experiencedlowering of support that can occur after overly expressingone’s negative emotions. Future research is needed to fur-ther examine this proposition and the prolongation of theemotional sharing processes.

Short concluding remarks

We hope that the four components of authentic emotionregulation outlined in this feature article will provide someinsight into when regulatory efforts will be most effectiveand stimulate research on the development of authenticemotion regulation. We suggest that individuals will expe-rience the greatest well-being if all four components havebeen fulfilled, that is, when they fully understand what theyare feeling, can accept it, can mostly act (i.e., regulate) inaccordance with their inner authentic experiences, and areable to have a balanced sharing of their emotions with closeothers. These components of authentic regulation can helppeople be sensitive to the context and monitor the effective-ness of ER strategies (e.g., if you are more aware of youremotions and accept them then you’ll have a better senseof the impact of your regulation efforts). We suggest thatthe success of context-sensitive emotion regulation effortsis not determined by the type of regulation strategies perse, but rather by whether strategies are combined withaspects of authentic emotion regulation.

References

Boden, M. T., Bonn-Miller, M. O., Kashdan, T. B.,Alvarez, J., & Gross, J. J. (2012). The interactive effectsof emotional clarity and cognitive reappraisal in Post-traumatic Stress Disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders,26, 233–238.

Boden, M. T., Gross, J. J., Babson, K. A., & Bonn-Miller, M.O. (2013). The interactive effects of emotional clarity andcognitive reappraisal on problematic cannabis useamong medical cannabis users. Addictive Behaviors, 38,1663–1668.

Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory flexibil-ity: An individual differences perspective on coping andemotion regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Science,8, 591–612.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•4

Page 6: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Curci, A., & Rime, B. (2012). The temporal evolution of socialsharing of emotions and its consequences on emotionalrecovery: A longitudinal study. Emotion, 12, 1404–1414.

English, T., & John, O. P. (2013). Understanding the socialeffects of emotion regulation: The mediating role ofauthenticity for individual differences in suppression.Emotion, 13, 314–329.

English, T., John, O. P., Srivastava, S., & Gross, J. J. (2012).Emotion regulation and peer-rated social functioning:A 4-year longitudinal study. Journal of Research in Person-ality, 46, 780–784.

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation:An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2,271–299.

Hankin, B. L., Stone, L., & Wright, P. A. (2010). Corumina-tion, interpersonal stress generation, and internalizingsymptoms: Accumulating effects and transactionalinfluences in a multiwave study of adolescents. Develop-ment and Psychopathology, 22, 217–235.

Impett, E. A., Kogan, A., English, T., John, O., Oveis, C.,Gordon, A. M., & Keltner, D. (2012). Suppression sourssacrifice: Emotional and relational costs of suppressingemotions in romantic relationships. Personality and SocialPsychology Bulletin, 38, 707–720.

Kernis, M. H. (2003). Toward a conceptualization of optimalself-esteem. Psychological Inquiry, 14, 1–26.

Kohl, A., Rief, W., & Glombiewski, J. A. (2012). How effec-tive are acceptance strategies? A meta-analytic review ofexperimental results. Journal of Behavior Therapy andExperimental Psychiatry, 43, 988–1001.

Koster, E. H. W., De Lissnyder, E., Derakshan, N., & DeRaedt, R. (2011). Understanding depressive ruminationfrom a cognitive science perspective: The impaired dis-engagement hypothesis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31,138–145.

Koven, N. S., & Thomas, W. (2010). Mapping facets of alex-ithymia to executive dysfunction in daily life. Personalityand Individual Differences, 49, 24–28.

Larsen, J. K., Vermulst, A. A., Eisinga, R., English, T.,Gross, J. J., & Hofman, E., . . . Engels, R. C. (2012). Socialcoping by masking? Parental support and peer victimi-zation as mediators of the relationship between depres-sive symptoms and expressive suppression inadolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 41, 1628–1642.

Larsen, J. K., Vermulst, A. A., Geenen, R., van Midden-dorp, H., English, T., & Gross, J. J., . . . Engels, R. C. M. E.(2013). Emotion regulation in adolescence: A prospectivestudy of expressive suppression and depressive symp-toms. Journal of Early Adolescence, 33, 184–200.

Le, B. M., & Impett, E. A. (2013). When holding back helpssuppressing negative emotions during sacrifice feelsauthentic and is beneficial for highly interdependentpeople. Psychological Science, 24, 1809–1815.

Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulationand depletion of limited resources: Does self-controlresemble a muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259.

O’Toole, M. S., Hougaard, E., & Mennin, D. S. (2013). Socialanxiety and emotion knowledge: a meta-analysis. Journalof Anxiety Disorders, 27, 98–108.

Parkinson, B., & Totterdell, P. (1999). Classifying affect-reg-ulation strategies. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 277–303.

Pe, M. L., Raes, F., & Kuppens, P. (2013). The cognitivebuilding blocks of emotion regulation: Ability to updateworking memory moderates the efficacy of ruminationand reappraisal on emotion. Plos One, 8, e69071.

Pennebaker, J. W. (2004). Theories, therapies, and tax-payers: On the complexities of the expressive writingparadigm. Clinical Psychology-Science and Practice, 11,138–142.

Rose, A. J., Carlson, W., & Waller, E. M. (2007). Prospectiveassociations of co-rumination with friendship and emo-tional adjustment: Considering the socioemotionaltrade-offs of co-rumination. Developmental Psychology,43, 1019–1031.

Rude, S. S., Maestas, K. L., & Neff, K. (2007). Paying atten-tion to distress: What’s wrong with rumination? Cogni-tion & Emotion, 21, 843–864.

Scheibe, S., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Emotional Aging:Recent Findings and Future Trends. Journals of Gerontol-ogy Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 65,135–144.

Schmeichel, B. J., Volokhov, R. N., & Dernaree, H. A. (2008).Working memory capacity and the self-regulation ofemotional expression and experience. Journal of Personal-ity and Social Psychology, 95, 1526–1540.

Shallcross, A. J., Ford, B. Q., Floerke, V. A., & Mauss, I. B.(2013). Getting better with age: The relationship betweenage, acceptance, and negative affect (vol 104, pg 734,2013). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105,718–719.

Sheppes, G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Is timing everything? Tem-poral considerations in emotion regulation. Personalityand Social Psychology Review, 15, 319–331.

Shiota, M. N., & Levenson, R. W. (2009). Effects of aging onexperimentally instructed detached reappraisal, positivereappraisal, and emotional behavior suppression. Psy-chology and Aging, 24, 890–900.

Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y. H., Lee, E. A., & Minnick, M.R. (2011). Is expressive suppression always associatedwith poorer psychological functioning? A cross-culturalcomparison between European Americans and HongKong Chinese. Emotion, 11, 1450–1455.

Srivastava, S., Tamir, M., McGonigal, K. M., John, O. P., &Gross, J. J. (2009). The social costs of emotional sup-pression: A prospective study of the transition to col-lege. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96,883–897.

Troy, A. S., Wilhelm, F. H., Shallcross, A. J., & Mauss, I. B.(2010). Seeing the silver lining: cognitive reappraisalability moderates the relationship between stress anddepressive symptoms. Emotion, 10, 783–795.

Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2008). Clear heads arecool heads: Emotional clarity and the down-regulationof antisocial affect. Cognition & Emotion, 22, 308–326.

Wood, A. M., Linley, P. A., Maltby, J., Baliousis, M., &Joseph, S. (2008). The authentic personality: A theoreticaland empirical conceptualization and the development ofthe authenticity scale. Journal of Counseling Psychology,55, 385–399.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•5

Page 7: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Parental Assessment ofEmotional Symptoms andPsychological Abnormalitiesin Adolescents

Marc SchipperUniversity of Bremen, GermanyE-mail: [email protected]

Dennis NitkowskiUniversity of Bremen, GermanyE-Mail: [email protected]

Ute KoglinUniversity of Bremen, GermanyUniversity of Oldenburg, GermanyE-mail: [email protected]

and

Franz PetermannUniversity of Bremen, GermanyE-mail: [email protected]

In the present study emotional symptoms, social compe-tence and behavioral deficits were assessed in 333 studentsbetween 12 and 17 years of age. Additionally these factorswere assessed by the students’ parents in order to gain infor-mation about their viewpoint on the students’ competences.For assessment the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire(SDQ) was administered, using both the parent and childversions. The aim of this study is to observe to what degreeparents are able to infer certain psychological problems oftheir children, such as emotional symptoms or conductproblems, by comparing students’ survey responses withthose of their parents. Parents’ estimation of their children’spsychological abnormalities differs significantly from theadolescents’ own assessment in three scales of the SDQ:Adolescent emotional and conduct problems are estimatedhigher by the parents compared to the adolescents’ ownindications whereas hyperactivity scores are lower in paren-tal assessment. These discrepancies imply that parents doindeed recognize their children’s behavioral or emotionalproblems but may not be able to reliably infer the degreeof these problems, or whether psychological interventionis advisable.

That more attention needs to be paid to adolescent men-tal disorders was shown by the Bremer Adolescent study(Essau, Karpinski, Petermann, & Conradt, 1998) more thana decade ago. Almost half of the study’s participants ful-filled the diagnostic criteria for at least one of the mentaldisorders assessed. Current data indicates that anxiety andaffective disorders are the most common mental disordersin adolescence. The average age of depression onset liesin early and mid-youth (Groen & Petermann, 2011; Peter-mann, 2011). Affective disorders in adolescence accompany

high relapse rates and an increased risk for mental disor-ders in adulthood (Fombonne, Wostear, Cooper, Harring-ton & Rutter, 2001). Furthermore such disorders oftenengender distinct feelings of loneliness and isolation (Las-gaard, Goossens, & Elklit, 2011) and heighten the risk ofrepeated suicide attempts (Monnin et al., 2012). Such seri-ous consequences illustrate the importance of a profoundlevel of awareness concerning the etiology and prevalenceof adolescent mental disorders. In recent years severalattempts have been made to increase this awareness (Peter-mann et al., 2012).

Another important factor in dealing with adolescentmental disorders is, to what degree and at what stage cansuch disorders or precursors of mental disorders be recog-nized? During adolescence, distinguishing between normaland abnormal behavior, in particular within the emotionaldomain, is a difficult task – difficult not only for the adoles-cents themselves, but also for an external observer (from thethird-person-perspective).

Parents face a difficult challenge in deciding whethertheir child’s behavior is normal or might deviate intoabnormality; this particularly holds for weighing the rele-vance of emotional problems. During adolescence emotionalproblems increase. Because for a considerable number ofadolescents these problems can become chronic (Laufer &Laufer, 1984; Dodge & Pettit, 2003), it is vital to recognizethem.

Parental support remains a very important factor withregard to emotional problems, although during adoles-cence the network of ‘‘significant others’’ is restructured.During childhood and in early adolescence this networkis dominated by parents but with ongoing adolescence peerrelations develop increasing importance (Helsen, Volle-bergh, & Meeus, 2000; Meeus, Iedema, Helsen, & Vollen-bergh, 1999; Steinberg, 2001). Yet several investigationshave found that support from parents during adolescenceprovides a better indicator of positive development thandoes peer support (Barrera, Chassin, & Rogosch, 1993; Deko-vic & Meeus, 1995; Laible, Carlo, & Raffaelli, 2000). Goodrelationships with parents are found to be significant forpositive self-esteem (Blyth & Traeger, 1988), social compe-tence (Cauce, 1986; Engels, Finkenauer, Meeus, & Dekovic,2001) and general well-being (Raja, McGee, & Stanton, 1992).

Considering the positive effects of parental support onadolescent mental problems the question arises as to howlikely parents are to recognize such problems in theiradolescent children. A frequently used tool for assessingbehavioral abnormalities and emotional problems as wellas social competence in children and adolescents is the

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•6

Page 8: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman,1997). The SDQ was not developed solely for child andadolescent assessment but can be completed by parents,too.

In this contribution we report our findings on differ-ences between adolescent assessment and parental estima-tion in order to gain more insight into how well parents areable to recognize their adolescent children’s mental prob-lems. We assumed that adolescent and parental responsesmight for instance differ in the domain of emotional symp-toms, and used the SDQ as assessment tool: The presence ofa parental version makes the SDQ a suitable tool for inves-tigating differences between the parental estimation of theiradolescent children’s behavioral attributes (such as emo-tional problems) and the adolescents’ own assessment.

Method

A total of 104 Adolescents (51% female; 49% male) agedbetween 12 and 17 years (M ¼ 14,12;SD ¼ 1,33) and theirparents agreed to take part in the assessment. Both adoles-cents and their parents completed the SDQ. Parentsreceived the parental version, a slightly modified infor-mant-rated version of the SDQ (Schmidt et al., 2012), inorder to assess their children’s strengths and difficulties.The SDQ is a 25-item measure of emotional and behavioralproblems in young people. It was translated by a Germanchildren’s psychiatrist and a professional translator, andimproved and standardized by Woerner and Colleagues(2002). The SDQ consists of five scales that assess emotionalsymptoms, conduct problems, hyperactivity, peer prob-lems and prosocial behavior. Each item is answered on athree-point scale ranging from ‘‘not true’’ to ‘‘somewhattrue’’ to ‘‘certainly true.’’ Test-retest reliability of 0.70-0.85and internal consistency of 0.51-0.76 have been demon-strated for the scales of the measure (Goodman, 1997). Eventhough SDQ scores can be used as continuous variablesthey are often classified as normal, borderline or abnormal.In order to generate dichotomous traits we decided to poolborderline and abnormal within one category: deviant. Hence,the resulting two categories are normal and deviant. For ourpurposes this step proves adequate regarding our aim toinvestigate at what SDQ scale parents can recognizewhether their child’s behavior deviates from normal.

Results

The collected data was analyzed using the statistical soft-ware IBM SPSS Statistics 20. For analysis the rating scaleswere dichotomized in ‘‘normal’’ versus ‘‘deviant’’. Adoles-cent and parent group differences were computed by Pear-son’s chi-squared test. To test whether adolescents andparents differ in the individual SDQ Subscales as well asin the overall SDQ score we performed a Chi2-Test. Addi-tionally an interrater reliability analysis using the Kappastatistic was performed to determine consistency amongraters (Table 1).

Differences between adolescent and parental scoringwere found in the SDQ-subscales emotional symptoms, con-duct problems, and hyperactivity. Here parents seem to mis-judge their children’s behavior. In the remaining subscales

as well as in the overall SDQ score adolescent and parentalscoring do not significantly differ from each other.

Parents’ estimates of the children’s emotional symptomsdiffer significantly from the children’s own assessment (w2¼4.67; p<.031). Also the parents’ estimates of their children’sconduct problems differ significantly from the children’sown assessment (w2 ¼ 16.71; p ¼ .000). A descriptiveview of the data shows that parents estimate the children’semotional symptoms and conduct problems higher thanadolescents do themselves. The contrary is the case forhyperactivity. Parents’ estimates of their children’s hyperac-tivity differs significantly from the children’s own assess-ment (w2 ¼ 5.95; p ¼ .015), but here parents estimate thechildren’s hyperactivity lower than adolescents do them-selves (Figure 1).

For observing the interrater reliability Cohen’s Kappawas calculated. For claiming a good level of agreement aKappa of at least .6 is necessary. Significant interrateragreements were found for all subscales in whichsignificant differences have already been found betweenparental and adolescent estimations as well. These are:emotional symptoms (Kappa ¼ .178, p ¼ .031), conductproblems (Kappa ¼ .396, p ¼ .000) and hyperactivity(Kappa ¼ .204, p < .015). While being statistically significantthese Kappa values only indicate a poor or slight agreement.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate whether differencescan be found between parental estimation of their adoles-cent children’s behavioral attributes (like emotional prob-lems) and the adolescents’ own assessment. We assumedthat especially within the SDQ subscale emotional symptomsparents might misjudge their children’s behavior – anassumption arising due to the domain of emotional process-ing being a very dynamic factor during adolescence.

In fact, next to hyperactivity and conduct problems,emotional symptoms represent the subscale in which ado-lescent and parental scores differ significantly. Interestingly,parents estimate the children’s emotional symptoms as wellas conduct problems higher than adolescents do themselvesand, vice versa, parents estimate their children’s hyperactiv-ity lower than adolescents do themselves.

A reason for this imbalance might be that during ado-lescence children are emotionally unstable and may seemdeviant to adults, although in actuality attributes whichcould be classified as deviating from normal in other phasesof life might still be in a normal range during adolescence.Therefore parents might overestimate their children’s emo-tional instability as being not normal. The same might hold

Table 1. Parent-Adolescent comparisons for the individual SDQ-subscales. Presented are Chi2 and Kappa values.

SDQ-Scales Chi2 p Kappa

SDQ: total difficulties 2.83 .093 .166SDQ: emotional symptoms 4.67 .031 .178SDQ: conduct problems 16.71 .000 .396SDQ: hyperactivity 5.95 .015 .204SDQ: peer Problems 1.25 .263 .108SDQ: prosocial behavior .72 .397 .083

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•7

Page 9: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

for the estimation of conduct problems: parents might over-estimate their children’s inconvenient behaviors as deviat-ing from normal although these behaviors are still in anormal range within the framework of adolescence.

On the other hand our results show that hyperactivityscores deviating from normal are more frequently identifiedby the adolescent group, indicating that parents might notalways recognize a too high amount of hyperactive behaviorin their children, again possibly due to their affiliation ofhyperactive behavioral attributes to adolescent behavior.

Remarkably, parental estimation and adolescent assess-ment do not differ in the SDQ total difficulties score, whichbuilds the sum over all subscales. Significant differences areonly found in the above-presented subscales. This indicatesan agreement of the parental and the adolescent estimationof deviance concerning strengths and difficulties in general:Both sides seem to recognize that problems are present. Theonly problem is the lack of agreement between parents andadolescents on where the problems are exactly based.

One important factor to be taken into account when dis-cussing discrepancies between self-assessments and thirdperson estimations are judgement biases, which implicitlyrepresent a limitation of this study. Judgement biases canbe represented by Halo effects (the valuation of a perceivedtrait is influenced by another perceived trait), the differen-tial extremity bias (the tendency – varying from observer toobserver – to choose extreme values) or biases associatedwith social desirability (Asendorpf, 2004).

In conclusion, the results of this study show that par-ents indeed recognize their children’s behavioral prob-lems but often seem unable to reliably infer the degreeof these problems, here shown for the SDQ subscales emo-tional symptoms, conduct problems and hyperactivity. Hence,parents might often have problems in deciding whethertheir children’s behavior does or does not deviate fromwhat is normal, which may affect the decision to engagepsychological intervention or not. In consideration of the

fact that mental problems in childhood and adolescencecan represent precursors and constitute an increased riskfor mental disorders in adulthood this decision has a highrelevance.

The question emerges as to how this discrepancy be-tween children’s assessments on certain behavioral prob-lems and their parents’ estimations of these factors can beovercome. One way could be offering a better parental psy-chological education: providing deeper knowledge for par-ents on what is normal and what might be classified asdeviating from normal in adolescence. These results pointto the need for further research, to investigate in more detailparents’ ability to recognize potential behavioral problemsof their adolescent children. Parental understanding andrecognition of hyperactivity, emotional symptoms and con-duct problems in adolescents should be probed in depth.Special attention should be paid to the role of judgementbiases.

References

Asendorpf, J. (2004). Psychologie der Personlichkeit. 3., uber-arb. u. aktualisierte Aufl. [Psychology of personality, 3rdrevised and updated ed.] .Berlin: Springer.

Barrera, M., Chassin, L., & Rogosch, F. (1993). Effects ofsocial support and conflict on adolescent children ofalcoholic and nonalcoholic fathers. Journal of Personal andSocial Psychology, 64, 602–612.

Blyth, D. A., & Traeger, C. (1988). Adolescent self-esteemand perceived relationships with parents and peers. InS. Salzinger, J. Antrobus & M. Hammer (Eds.), SocialNetworks of Children, Adolescents, and College Students(pp. 171–194). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cauce, A. M. (1986). Social networks and social compe-tence: Exploring the effects of early adolescent friend-ships. American Journal of Communication Psychology, 14,607–628.

Figure 1. Parental misjudgements.Note: Percentage of participants classified as deviant by self-assessment (in case of adolescents) or respectively by third-person-estimation (in case of parents) are presented. Parents estimate the adolescent children’s emotional and conduct problems higherthan the children themselves whereas parents score hyperactivity lower.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•8

Page 10: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Dekovic, M., & Meeus, W. (1995). Emotional problems inadolescence. In M. Bois-Reymond, R. Diekstra, K. Hurrel-mann & E. Peters (Eds.), Coping: Strategies and Status Tran-sitions in Adolescence (pp. 225–241). Berlin: De Gruyter.

Dodge, K. A., & Pettit, G. S. (2003). A biopsychosocialmodel of the development of chronic conduct problemsin adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 39, 349–371.

Engels, R., Finkenauer, C., Meeus, W., & Dekovic, M. (2001).Parental attachment and adolescents’ emotional adjust-ment: The associations with social skills and relationalcompetence. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48, 428–439.

Essau, C. A., Karpinski, N. A., Petermann, F., & Conradt, J.(1998). Haufigkeit und Komorbiditat psychischer Storun-gen bei Jugendlichen: Ergebnisse der Bremer Jugendstu-die [Frequency and comorbidity of mental disorders inadolescents: results of the Bremen Youth Study]. Zeits-chrift fur Klinische Psychologie, Psychiatrie und Psychothera-pie, 46, 105 –124.

Fombonne, E., Wostear, G., Cooper, V., Harrington, R., &Rutter, M. (2001). The Maudsley long-term follow-upof child and adolescent depression. British Journal of Psy-chiatry, 179, 210 –217.

Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Ques-tionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology andPsychiatry, 38, 581 –586.

Groen, G., & Petermann, F. (2011). Depressive Kinder undJugendliche [Depressed children and adolescents]. (2.,uberarb. Aufl.) Gottingen: Hogrefe.

Helsen, M., Vollebergh, W., & Meeus, W. (2000). SocialSupport from Parents and Friends and Emotional Prob-lems in Adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence,29, 319–335.

Laible, D., Carlo, G., & Raffaelli, M. (2000). The differentialrelations of parent and peer attachment to adolescentadjustment. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 29, 45–59.

Lasgaard, M., Goossens, L., & Elklit, A. (2011). Loneliness,depressive symptomatology, and suicide ideation inadolescence: Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 137–150.

Laufer, M., & Laufer, M. E. (1984). Adolescence and develop-mental breakdown. London: Yale University Press.

Meeus, W., Iedema, J., Helsen, M., & Vollenbergh, W. (1999).Patterns of adolescent identity development: Reviewof literature and longitudinal analysis. DevelopmentalReview, 19, 419–461.

Monnin, J., Thiemard, E., Vandel, P., Nicolier, M., Tio, G., &Courtet, P. (2012). Sociodemographic and psychopatho-logical risk factors in repeated suicide attempts: Genderdifferences in a prospective study. Journal of AffectiveDisorders, 136, 35–43.

Petermann, F. (2011). Depressive Kinder und Jugendliche[Depressive children and adolescents]. MonatsschriftKinderheilkunde, 159, 985–992.

Petermann, F., Jascenoka, J., Koglin, U., Karpinski, N.,Petermann, U., & Kullik, A. (2012). Zur Lebenszeitprava-lenz Affektiver Storungen im Jugendalter [Lifetime pre-valence of affective disorders in adolescence]. Kindheitund Entwicklung, 21, 209–218.

Raja, S., McGee, R., & Stanton, W. (1992). Perceived attach-ments to parents and peers and psychological well-beingin adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 21,471–485.

Schmidt, S., Ender, S., Schultheiß, J., Muller, G. G.-v.,Gerber, W.-D., & Steinmann, E. (2012). Das ADHS-Camp. Langzeiteffekte einer intensiv-therapeutischenMaßnahme bei Kindern mit ADHS [Standardizationand evaluation of the German parents’ version of theStrengths and Difficulties: the ADHD camp. Long-termeffects of intensive therapeutic measures in childrenwith ADHD. Kindheit und Entwicklung, 21, 90–102.

Steinberg, L. (2001). We Know Some Things: Parent–Ado-lescent Relationships in Retrospect and Prospect. Journalof Research on Adolescence, 11, 1–19.

Woerner, W., Becker, A., Friedrich, C., Rothenberger, A.,Klasen, H., & Goodman, R. (2002). Normierung undEvaluation der deutschen Elternversion des Strengthsand Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Ergebnisse einerreprasentativen Felderhebung [Standardization andevaluation of the German parents’ version of theStrengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Resultsof a representative field study]. Zeitschrift fur Kinder- undJugendpsychiatrie und Psychotherapie, 30, 105–112.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•9

Page 11: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Parenting and EmotionRegulation in the Adaptiveand Academic Competenciesof Chinese American Youth

Jeffrey LiewTexas A&M University, USAE-mail: [email protected]

This research was supported by a grant from the HoggFoundation for Mental Health to Jeffrey Liew. In 2010, Liewlaunched Project CASL (Chinese American Successful Liv-ing) to study adolescents’ social-emotional and academicadjustment, and project findings are shared in this report.Project CASL 2.0 is in progress, with data being collectedon the original adolescent sample who are now emergingadults in college. The Liew Human Development Labexpresses gratitude for the families that participated in thisresearch, as well as the Asian American community organi-zations and social services agencies and all the researchassistants who helped and contributed to the efforts of Pro-ject CASL.

Chinese parenting has often been characterized asoverly strict and controlling. While strict parental controlhas been considered a defining feature of traditional Chi-nese parenting that dates back to Confucian, Buddhist, andTaoist teachings (Shek, 2007), modern Chinese parentingpractices appear to have changed with the times with devel-opmental science and a growing body of studies confirmingchanges in Chinese parenting practices (Kim, Wang,Orozco-Lapray, Shen, & Murtuza, 2013; Way et al., 2013).In the 21st century, Chinese parenting is a combination oftraditional and modern ideologies and practices, consistingof guan ( ) and jiao xun ( ) with autonomy support; thistype of strictness-supervision counterbalanced by auton-omy support is the yin and yang in parenting (Liew, Kwok,Y. Chang, B. Chang, & Yeh, in press). This report highlightsstudy findings that challenge the popular notion that overlycontrolling or restrictive parenting is the best way to raiseacademically successful children from Chinese and immi-grant backgrounds.

Chinese parenting and semantics

Guan and jiao xun are Chinese concepts that exemplify Chi-nese parental duties (Chao, 1994). The literal translation ofguan ( ) is ‘‘to rule’’ or ‘‘to control’’, and the literal transla-tion of jiao xun ( ) is ‘‘to reprimand’’ or ‘‘to punish’’.However, literal translations often lead to mistranslationsor misinterpretations. Semantic translation may better cap-ture or signify the semantics and nuances of the originalconcepts. From a semantic translation approach, guan ( )could be interpreted as ‘‘to look after or govern’’ and ‘‘to

safeguard’’ while jiao xun ( ) could be interpreted as‘‘to teach or train’’ and ‘‘to discipline.’’ While the literaltranslation conveys cold harshness, the semantic transla-tion conveys Chinese parental duties of safeguarding anddemanding excellence from children (Liew et al., in press).

To those unfamiliar with Chinese culture, guan and jiaoxun may easily be interpreted as intrusive or oppressive.However, Chinese parents practice guan and jiao xun outof love and caring for their children. In fact, these parentingconcepts overlap with constructs in the developmental sci-ence literature on parental control (Shek, 2007). In the studyof parental control, it is important to distinguish betweenparental structure and parental psychological control (Grol-nick & Pomerantz, 2009). Parental structure includes mon-itoring and setting limits, whereas parental psychologicalcontrol includes pressuring and being intrusive. In ourresearch, we view guan and jiao xun as more aligned withparental structure than psychological control. We proposethat when guan (safeguarding) and jiao xun (demanding-ness of excellence) are counterbalanced by parental auton-omy support, this form of parental control is akin toauthoritative parenting which consists of high responsive-ness and high demandingness.

Autonomy support and emotion regulation

Parental autonomy support is often reflected in parentalwarmth, support, and responsiveness that predict adoles-cents’ emotional well-being, and this pattern has been foundin studies conducted in North America and Asia (Wang,Pomerantz, & Chen, 2007). Emotion regulation researchindicates that empathic and encouraging parenting beha-viors promote children’s self-regulation. Parents’ expres-sions of warmth and positive emotions predict children’seffortful control two years later (Eisenberg et al., 2005), andparents’ expressions of warmth and positive emotions pre-dict children’s physiological and behavioral self-regulation(Liew, Johnson, Smith, & Thoemmes, 2011). By contrast, par-ental psychological control undermines children’s emotionregulation (Wang et al., 2007).

Emotion-related self-regulation (henceforth termed emotionregulation for brevity) refers to ‘‘processes used to manageand change if, when, and how one experiences emotions andemotion-related motivational and physiological states, aswell as how emotions are expressed behaviorally’’ (Eisen-berg, Hofer, & Vaughan, 2007, p. 288). The voluntary and‘‘top-down’’ aspects of emotion regulation include capacitiessuch as effortful control and executive functions (Blair &Raver, 2012; Liew, 2012). In contrast, negative emotionality

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•10

Page 12: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

is a dimension of temperament that is more reactive. Inour research, we conceptualize the construct of emotion reg-ulation as representing high levels of voluntary aspectsof self-regulation and low or modest levels of negativeemotionality.

Findings from Project CASL

Project CASL was launched to study factors associated withsocial-emotional and academic competencies in ChineseAmerican adolescents from immigrant backgrounds. Wetargeted this sample because these youths often experiencebicultural and intergenerational pressures or conflicts.Families were recruited from the Houston area in Texas,USA. Texas is the second most populous and the second-largest of the 50 U.S. states, with Houston being the largestcity in Texas. In terms of Asian population in the U.S., Texasranks third largest in the U.S., with the Houston area havingthe largest Asian population in the state (U.S CensusBureau, 2010). Our sample consisted of 92 Chinese Ameri-can adolescents aged 14 to 18 years and their parents. Whilethe majority of adolescents (80%) were born in the UnitedStates, all parents were first generation Chinese immigrants(majority born in China or Taiwan).

Data for this study were collected with online surveys on92 parent-adolescent dyads, with the parent and adolescentcompleting surveys separately. Measures included parentalautonomy support, adolescent emotion regulation, adoles-cent adaptive skills, and adolescent academic achievement.Parental autonomy support (low parental psychologicalcontrol) was assessed using parent and adolescent ratingsof reverse-scored items from the Parental Control Scale(PCS; Barber, 1996). Adolescent emotion regulation andadaptive skills were assessed using parent ratings on the

adolescent version of the Behavior Assessment System forChildren - 2 (BASC-2; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004). Adoles-cents reported on their school grades in the subjects of Eng-lish, math, science, and social studies.

We tested a measurement model for the latent con-struct of emotion regulation, consisting of emotional self-control, negative emotionality, executive functioning, andanger control. Our measurement model fit the data well.In addition, we examined the mechanisms by which par-enting may influence academic achievement. Our findingsshowed that emotion regulation mediated the effect of par-ental autonomy support on adaptive skills, while adaptiveskills mediated the effect of emotion regulation on aca-demic achievement. Our findings are consistent with theview that overly controlling and restrictive parenting is notthe only way to achieve academic success (Kim et al.,2013). For Chinese American adolescents with first gener-ation immigrant parents, parental autonomy support andemotion regulation are promotive factors in adaptive andacademic competencies.

Future directions

Our research group has begun work on Project CASL 2.0, afollow-up study on the original sample of adolescents toexamine their adjustments as emerging adults and theirtransition to college and work life. While we used thereversed score of parental psychological control as a proxyfor autonomy support in Project CASL, we have includeddirect measures of parental autonomy support in the fol-low-up study. For children from collectivistic cultures suchas Chinese Americans, limited data exists on parenting andchild factors that relate to adjustment outcomes during thetransition period from adolescence to early adulthood.

Project CASL members: Yu-pei Chang (lower left) and Bonny Chang (upper right) at the Asian American Psychological Association AnnualConvention.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•11

Page 13: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Collectivistic cultures tend to emphasize family obligationsand respect for the parents, and a sense of family obligationmay have implications for educational, employment, andadjustment outcomes (Fuligni & Pedersen, 2002). The LiewHuman Development Lab is committed to continuingresearch on child and parent factors that contribute to chil-dren growing into socially, emotionally, and academicallycompetent young adults.

References

Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: Revi-siting a neglected construct. Child Development, 67,3296–3319. doi:10.2307/1131780

Blair,C.,&Raver,C.C.(2012).Individualdevelopmentandevo-lution: Experiential canalization of self-regulation. Develop-mental Psychology, 48, 647–657. doi:10.1037/a0026472

Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritar-ian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parentingthrough the cultural notion of training. Child Develop-ment, 65, 1111–1120. doi:10.2307/1131308

Eisenberg, N., Hofer, C., & Vaughan, J. (2007). Effortful con-trol and its socioemotional consequences. In J. J. Gross(Eds.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 287–306). NewYork, NY: Guilford.

Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R.A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting,children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: Athree-wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76,1055–1071. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00897.x

Fuligni, A. J., & Pedersen, S. (2002). Family obligation andthe transition to young adulthood. Developmental Psy-chology, 38, 856–868. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.856

Grolnick, W. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2009). Issues and chal-lenges in studying parental control: Toward a new con-ceptualization. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 165–170.

Kim, S. Y., Wang, Y., Orozco-Lapray, D., Shen, Y., &Murtuza, M. (2013). Does ‘‘tiger parenting’’ exist? Par-enting profiles of Chinese Americans and adolescentdevelopmental outcomes. Asian American Journal of Psy-chology, 4, 7–18. doi: 10.1037/a0030612

Liew, J. (2012). Effortful control, executive functions, andeducation: Bringing self-regulatory and social-emotionalcompetencies to the table, Child Development Perspectives,6, 105–111. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00196.x

Liew, J., Johnson, A. Y., Smith, T. R., & Thoemmes, F.(2011). Parental expressivity, child physiological andbehavioral regulation, and child adjustment: Testinga three-path mediation model, Early Education andDevelopment, 22, 549–573. doi:10.1080/10409289.2010.481551

Liew, J., Kwok, O., Chang, Y., Chang, B. W., & Yeh, Y. (inpress). Parental autonomy support predicts academicachievement through emotion-related self-regulationand adaptive skills in Chinese American adolescents.Asian American Journal of Psychology.

Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). Behavior Assess-ment System for Children (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN:American Guidance Service.

Shek, D. T. L. (2007). Perceived parental control based onindigenous Chinese parental control concepts in adoles-cents in Hong Kong. American Journal of Family Therapy,35, 123–137.

U.S. Census Bureau, State and County QuickFacts. Census2010 population, demographic, and housing informa-tion. Retrieved from http://quickfacts.census.gov/qfd/states/48000lk.html

Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E. M., & Chen, H. (2007). The role ofparents’ control in early adolescents’ psychologicalfunctioning: A longitudinal investigation in the UnitedStates and China. Child Development, 78, 1592–1610.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01085.x

Jeffrey Liew with Project CASL 2.0 team members Xin Dong, Michelle SooHoo and Amanda Lomax (from left to right).

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•12

Page 14: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Adolescent DepressiveSymptoms: An IntegrativeExamination of Parenting andSadness Regulation in Italy andthe United States

Laura Di GiuntaSapienza University of Rome, ItalyE-mail: [email protected]

and

Anne-Marie R. IselinUniversity of North Carolina Wilmington, USAE-mail: [email protected]

The transition to adolescence gives rise to a wide range ofexternalizing and internalizing problems (e.g., Loeber &Farrington, 2000) that have long-term effects on both phys-ical and mental health in adulthood (e.g., Odgers et al.,2007). Self-regulation related processes have figured promi-nently in scientific inquiries about factors that contribute tothe onset and duration of mental health symptoms. Emo-tion regulation is of growing empirical interest in thedomain of self-regulation research. Emotion regulationinvolves initiating, avoiding, inhibiting, maintaining, ormodulating feelings and related physiological processes,cognitions, and behaviors in the service of accomplishingsocial competency (Eisenberg & Spinrad, 2004).

Throughout childhood and adolescence, emotion dys-regulation is related to both externalizing behaviors andinternalizing symptoms (e.g., Mullin & Hinshaw, 2007;Yap, Allen, & Sheeber, 2007). These associations may, how-ever, depend on both the specific emotion being managedand the type of the strategy being used to deal with thatemotion. For example, hostile rumination has been foundto actively increase anger and aggression (e.g., Caprara,Barbaranelli, & Zimbardo, 1996), whereas depressive rumi-nation have been found to actively increase sadness anddepression (e.g., Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky,2008). Overall, the consensus is that it is crucial to test emo-tion-specific regulation strategies.

Parenting behaviors are likely early antecedents of therelation between emotion regulation and psychologicaladjustment. Several studies show that parenting behaviorsinfluence how youth maintain, alter, and modulate theiremotional experiences and expression (e.g., Morris, Silk,Steinberg, Aucoin, & Keyes, 2011). There is also strongempirical evidence that negative parenting behaviors pre-dict both internalizing and externalizing problems in chil-dren and adolescents (e.g., Barber, 2002; Gershoff, 2002).Feng et al., (2009) found that it is the interplay betweenparenting behaviors and children’s emotion regulation that

predicts maladjustment. Negative parenting (i.e., lowacceptance) prompted a disturbance in girls’ sadness regu-lation followed by depressive symptoms one year later.Researchers are just beginning to understand the uniqueand complex effect of parenting behaviors on children’semotion regulation and its associated outcomes. Our studyexpands this data by examining a mediational pathwayfrom parenting behaviors through emotion regulation topsychological adjustment.

Our study also seeks to expand the literature on chil-dren’s emotion regulation by examining the influence ofculture on this mediational pathway. One way to examineculture is through its effect on parenting behaviors (e.g.,Lansford et al., 2005). For example, Bornstein et al. (2008)found that Italian mothers were more responsive to andinvolved with their children than mothers from the UnitedStates. Within a specific country there can be regional andethnic differences in parenting styles. For example, mothersfrom Italian northern regions (i.e., presumably more edu-cated and open to modern values) were more responsiveto and less controlling of their children than mothers fromItalian southern regions (Genta, Brighi, Costabile, & Wijn-roks, 1995). Within the United States, African American andLatino parents use more physical discipline than EuropeanAmerican parents (e.g., Deater-Deckard, Dodge, Bates, &Pettital, 1996). More recent cross-cultural investigationshighlight the importance of examining the perceived nor-mativeness of parental behaviors within a specific culturalcontext (Lansford et al., 2005). The detrimental effect of neg-ative parenting on child adjustment appears to be weakerwithin cultural contexts characterized by high normative-ness of a certain parental behaviors. In this report, we exam-ine how the cultural normativeness of parenting behaviorinfluences the mediation pathway from parenting behaviorthrough emotion regulation to psychological adjustment.

MethodParticipants and procedure

This study is part of the Parenting Across Cultures project(PAC; e.g., Lansford et al., 2010). PAC has followed 1,417children and their parents from 13 cultural groups (China[Jinan and Shanghai]; Colombia; Italy [Rome and Naples];Jordan; Kenya; the Philippines; Sweden; Thailand; andthe United States [European American, African American,and Latino families]) beginning when children were

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•13

Page 15: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

approximately 8 years old. Participants in the current studyincluded 456 parent-child dyads from five different culturalgroups: Naples, Italy (n ¼ 93); Rome, Italy (n ¼ 95); U.S.European Americans (n ¼ 101); U.S. African Americans(n ¼ 91); and U.S. Latinos (n ¼ 76). Participants were 10.59years old (SD ¼ 0.69) at Time 1 and 12.62 years old (SD ¼0.69) at Time 2. Half of the sample was female. Specificsocio-demographic dimensions are available for Italy(Bombi et al., 2011) and the U.S. (Lansford et al., 2011). PACprocedures can be found elsewhere (Bornstein, Putnick, &Lansford, 2011).

Measures

Time 1 measures. Youth-reported psychological control wasassessed using the Psychological Control and AutonomyGranting scale (Silk, Morris, Kanaya, & Steinberg, 2003).Parent-reported normativeness of psychological controlwas assessed across several questions that asked about hownormative psychological control was among other parentsin their community.

Time 2 measures. Participants reported their emotionregulation strategies in response to ambiguous vignettesinvolving peer social situations. Using questions from priorresearch measures (Abela, Brozina, & Haigh, 2002; Zeman,Shipman, & Penza-Clyve, 2001), youth reported how theywould respond to such situations in terms of dysregulatedsadness expression (e.g., make a sad face or cry) anddepressive rumination (e.g., keep thinking and thinkingabout how sad you feel). Parents and children completedthe Child Behavior Checklist (Achenbach, 1991) as a mea-sure of depressive symptoms.

Results

Table 1 presents partial correlations among all variableswithin our mediation model for the aggregate sample (N¼ 456) controlling for the child’s gender.

Across the five cultural groups, partial correlationswere generally consistent, especially between emotion dys-regulation and depressive symptoms. Given these results,we tested the mediation pathway from psychological con-trol through sadness rumination to depressive symptomsagain for the aggregate sample. The indirect effect (0.04)was significant (95% bootstrapped asymmetric confidenceinterval ¼ 0.01-0.07). The indirect effect for dysregulatedsadness expression was not significant, but we hypothe-sized this was because of the influence of culture on psy-chological control. Mean scores on the normativeness ofpsychological control differed significantly across the 5 cul-tural groups [F(4, 451) ¼ 17.47, p < .01; see Table 2].

We therefore tested whether the normativeness of psy-chological control moderated the mediational pathways byincluding an interaction term between psychological con-trol and normativeness of psychological control using pro-cedures outlined by Preacher and colleagues (2007). Asseen in Figure 1, the strength of the mediated effect variesbased on levels of the normativeness of psychological con-trol (where -1 ¼ 1 SD below the mean, 0 ¼ the mean, 1 ¼ 1SD above the mean). The indirect effect of psychologicalcontrol on depressive symptoms through dysregulated sad-ness expression is significant only when the use of

psychological control is not normative (values < 0). Weobtained highly similar results from a moderated media-tion model testing sadness rumination as the mediator.

Discussion

We longitudinally examined whether two facets of emotiondysregulation—dysregulated sadness expression and sad-ness rumination—mediated the relation between parentalpsychological control and youth depressive symptomsamong 5 different cultural groups. Youth whose parentsused more psychological control engaged in more sadnessrumination which was related to more parent- and child-reported depressive symptoms. This was not the case fordysregulated sadness expression; though we theorized thatcultural differences in the normativeness of psychologicalcontrol were influencing our mediation pathways. Wefound evidence in support of this supposition. The strengthof the pathways from psychological control to depressivesymptoms through both sadness rumination and dysregu-lated sadness expression varied based on levels of norma-tiveness of psychological control (i.e., parents’ perceptionabout how frequently other parents use psychological con-trol within a specific cultural context). Both indirect effectswere attenuated as psychological control became more nor-mative and were stronger as psychological control becameless normative. In other words, the detrimental effects ofpsychological control on emotion regulation and in turnon mental health symptoms are more pronounced whenpsychological control is less culturally normative. Consid-ering that psychological control is generally less normativein the United States than in Italy, the effects of psychologi-cal control on emotion regulation and ultimately on chil-dren’s well-being might correspond to an even worsescenario in the United States than in Italy.

This study highlights the importance of consideringcultural characteristics that may interact with predictorsof emotion regulation. Given that many interventions foryouth target parenting, emotion regulation, or some combi-nation of both mechanisms, findings from our study haveclear implications for enhancing existing empirically-basedinterventions that target children’s well-being. Using anintervention developed in one culture with families fromdifferent cultural backgrounds and without considering thecultural values surrounding the family may produce lessthan optimal results. It will be important for treatmentdevelopers and clinicians to consider cultural variables thatmight influence the effectiveness of a given intervention.For instance, depending on a family’s cultural background,interventions may target different parenting behaviors oremotion-regulation strategies.

Table 1. Partial correlations among predictor, mediators, and outcome.

PsychologicalControl

DepressiveSymptoms

Psychological Control — .03Sadness Rumination .14** .27**Dysregulated Sadness Expression .09 .22**

Note: ** ¼ p < .01.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•14

Page 16: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Notes: Jennifer Lansford is the Principal Investigator ofPAC and Concetta Pastorelli and Anna Silvia Bombi serveas the Principal Investigators of Italian data for PAC. ThePAC project has been funded by NICHD and the JacobsFoundation. Laura Di Giunta and Anne-Marie Iselinreceived funding for their portion of the project from ayoung scholars grant by the Jacobs Foundation, the DukeUniversity Transdisciplinary Prevention Research Center,and the Josiah Trent Memorial Foundation.

References

Abela, J. Z., Brozina, K., & Haigh, E. P. (2002). An exami-nation of the response styles theory of depression inthird- and seventh-grade children: A short-term longi-tudinal study. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30,515–527.

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4-18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington, VT: University ofVermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Barber, B. K. (2002). Intrusive parenting: How psychologicalcontrol affects children and adolescents. Washington, DC,US: American Psychological Association.

Bombi, A. S., Pastorelli, C., Bacchini, D., Di Giunta, L.,Miranda, M. C., & Zelli, A. (2011). Attributions and atti-tudes of mothers and fathers in Italy. Parenting: Scienceand Practice, 11(2-3), 129–141.

Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., Heslington, M., Gini, M.,Suwalsky, J. D., Venuti, P., & Zingman de Galperın, C.(2008). Mother-child emotional availability in ecologicalperspective: Three countries, two regions, two genders.Developmental Psychology, 44, 666–680.

Bornstein, M. H., Putnick, D. L., & Lansford, J. E. (2011)Parenting attributions and attitudes in cross-culturalperspective. Parenting: Science and Practice, 11, 214–237.

Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Zimbardo, P. (1996).Under-standing the complexity of human aggression: Affective,

cognitive and social dimensions of individual differencesin propensity toward aggression. European Journal of Per-sonality, 10, 133–155.

Deater-Deckard, K., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, G. S.(1996). Physical discipline among African Americanand European American mothers: Links to children’sexternalizing behaviors. Developmental Psychology, 32,1065–1072.

Eisenberg, N., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Emotion-related reg-ulation: Sharpening the definition. Child Development, 75,334–339.

Feng, X., Keenan, K., Hipwell, A. E., Henneberger, A. K.,Rischall, M. S., & Butch, J., . . . Babinski, D. E. (2009).Longitudinal associations between emotion regulationand depression in preadolescent girls: Moderation bythe caregiving environment. Developmental Psychology,45(3), 798–808.

Genta, M. L., Brighi, A., Costabile, A., & Wijnroks, L. (1995).Maternal interactive styles: A comparison between twogroups of mothers in Central-Northern and Southern Italy.Paper presented at the VIIth European Conference onDevelopmental Psychology, Krakow, Poland.

Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents andassociated child behaviors and experiences: A meta-ana-lytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 128,539–579.

Lansford, J.E., Alampay, L., Al-Hassan, S., Bacchini, D.,Bombi, A.S., & Bornstein, M.H., . . . Zelli, A. (2010). Cor-poral punishment of children in nine countries as a func-tion of child gender and parent gender. InternationalJournal of Pediatrics, Article ID672780.

Lansford, J. E., Bornstein, M. H., Dodge, K. A., Skinner, A. T.,Putnick, D. L., & Deater- Deckard, K. (2011). Attributionsand attitudes of mothers and fathers in the United States.Parenting: Science and Practice, 11, 199–213.

Lansford, J., Chang, L., Dodge, K., Malone, P., Oburu, P., &Palmerus, K., . . . Quinn, N. (2005). Physical disciplineand children’s adjustment: Cultural normativeness as amoderator. Child Development, 76, 1234–1246.

Loeber, R., & Farrington, D. P. (2000). Young children whocommit crime: Epidemiology, developmental origins,risk factors, early interventions, and policy implications.Development and Psychopathology, 12, 737–762.

Morris, A., Silk, J. S., Morris, M. S., Steinberg, L., Aucoin, K. J.,& Keyes, A. W. (2011). The influence of mother–child emo-tion regulation strategies on children’s expression of angerand sadness. Developmental Psychology, 47, 213–225.

Mullin, B., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2007). Emotion regulationand externalizing disorders in children and adoles-cents. In J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation(pp. 523–541). New York: Guilford.

Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008).Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on PsychologicalScience, 3, 400–424.

Table 2. Mean scores (SD) on normativeness of psychological control.

Naplesa Romeab US-European Americanbc US-Latinocd US-African Americand

3.49 (0.76) 3.23 (0.69) 3.10 (0.65) 2.82 (0.91) 2.63 (0.85)

Note: Shared superscripts indicate that means are not significantly different from each other.

Figure 1. Indirect effect from psychological control to depressivesymptoms via dysregulated sadness expression as moderated bynormativenss of psychological control.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•15

Page 17: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Odgers, C. L., Caspi, A., Broadbent, J. M., Dickson, N.,Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., & Moffitt, T. E. (2007). Pre-diction of differential adult health burden by conductproblem subtypes in males. Archives of General Psychia-try, 64, 476–484.

Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F. (2007). Asses-sing moderated mediation hypotheses: Theory, method,and prescriptions. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42,185–227.

Silk, J.S., Morris, A. S., Kanaya, T., & Steinberg, L. (2003).Psychological control and autonomy granting: Opposite

ends of a continuum or distinct constructs. Journal ofResearch on Adolescence, 13, 113–128.

Yap, M. B. H., Allen, N. B., & Sheeber, L. (2007). Using anemotion regulation framework to understand the roleof temperament and family processes in risk for adoles-cent depressive disorders. Clinical Child & Family Psy-chology Review, 10, 180–196.

Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Penza-Clyve, S. (2001). Develop-ment and initial validation of the Children’s SadnessManagement Scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25,187–205.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•16

Page 18: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Emotion Regulation in IntimateRelationships

Claudia M. HaaseNorthwestern UniversityE-mail: [email protected]

Intimate relationships are one of the most important socialrelationships in the lives of many people. According to theUS census, 96% of US adults over the age of 65 have beenmarried at least once in their life.

Social relationships in general (Butler, 2011; Campos,Walle, Dahl, & Main, 2011) and intimate relationshipsin particular (Levenson, Haase, Bloch, Holley, & Seider,2013) are hotbeds of emotions. Navigating these complexsocioemotional landscapes requires considerable regula-tory efforts. In fact, the vast majority of emotion regulationepisodes takes place in social contexts (reports suggest upto 98%; Gross, Richards, & John, 2006).

Although emotion regulation is a blooming researchtopic (e.g., Gross, 2013), most studies have focused onemotion regulation in individuals. In a review of studiesconducted since 2001, Campos and colleagues (2011) esti-mated that less than 12% of the studies assessed emotionregulation in the presence of another person (and this isan optimistic estimate, including studies involving ima-gined as well as real others).

The present article takes a (selective) look at emotionregulation in intimate relationships, zooming in on definingqualities, reviewing key developmental periods, highlight-ing consequences, presenting some of our recent empiricalfindings, and outlining suggestions for future research andapplications.

Defining qualities

Most existing definitions emphasize individual emotionregulation focusing, for example, on ‘‘the processes bywhich individuals [emphasis added] influence which emo-tions they have, when they have them, and how they expe-rience and express these emotions (Gross, 1998, p. 275).Clearly, there are aspects of emotion regulation that arerelevant for both individuals and couples. In individualsand couples, emotion regulation can be explicit (effortful)or implicit (automatic) (Gyurak, Gross, & Etkin, 2011),successful or unsuccessful (Gross & Levenson, 1993), andin both contexts it can be difficult to distinguish betweenemotion reactivity and emotion regulation (Gross, Sheppes,& Urry, 2011).

However, emotion regulation in couples also has anumber of defining, special qualities (Levenson et al.,2013). Emotion regulation in couples is (a) dynamic anditerative as couples engage in an ongoing emotional dancewith partners acting and reacting to each other’s emotion.Emotion regulation in couples is (b) bidirectional as cou-ples engage in both down- and upregulation of emotions

(although downregulating emotion is important, spousescan become quite frustrated when their partner engagesin constant downregulation). Emotion regulation in cou-ples is (c) bivalent as couples’ regulation may target bothnegative and positive emotion (upregulating positive anddownregulating negative emotion are important, butdownregulating positive and upregulating negative emo-tion may be critical, for example when one partner needssupport and consolation; Clark, Ouellette, Powell, & Mil-berg, 1987). Finally, emotion regulation in couples is co-reg-ulatory (a partner may regulate not only their own but alsotheir partner’s emotions and both partners may have quitedifferent regulatory goals, strategies, and blind spots).Clearly, some of these qualities (e.g., bidirectionality, biva-lence) are also important for individuals, but they maybecome crucial in intimate relationships.

To illustrate these defining qualities, consider a couplewho is driving to visit the husband’s family over the holi-days. As the wife is driving, the husband is starting to getexcited to see his family. His wife, however, wants to con-centrate on the drive, which in turn starts to irritate thehusband (dynamic and iterative regulation). He says to her:‘‘Can’t you just relax and enjoy yourself a little bit?’’(attempted co-regulation). The wife remains silent, takes acouple of deep breaths to calm herself down, and contin-ues driving (downregulating negative emotion). The husbandtries hard to keep a cheerful look on his face (upregulatingpositive emotion) when the wife suddenly turns to him andsays: ‘‘I would love to be excited, but it’s just hard for me.When we visit your family, I start thinking of my own familyand . . . ‘‘; she starts to tear up (upregulating negative emotion).Suddenly realizing that his wife is really sad, the husbandsays: ‘‘I understand, honey. I am sorry. How about we stopsomewhere and get something to eat?’’ She smiles, nods,and turns up a song that he likes on the radio (mutualco-regulation).

Development across the life span

We are part of many intimate relationships throughout thelife span. We focus here on three dyadic relationships thatare key for the development of emotion regulation, par-ent-infant relationships, early romantic relationships, andintimate relationships in late life.

Parent-infant relationship. The parent-infant relation-ship is the cradle of emotion regulation. As emotion regula-tion skills are just beginning to develop, infants relyprimarily on their caregivers to regulate their emotions(Thompson, 1991). The focus is often on downregulatingnegative emotion (e.g., managing distress), but upregulat-ing positive emotion (e.g., engaging in amusing and calm-ing activities) is fundamental as well (e.g., Tronick, 1989).If all goes well, successful regulation of emotion in the

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•17

Page 19: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

parent-infant dyad sets the stage for infants’ developmentof their own emotion regulation skills.

Attachment theory and research (Bowlby, 1988)reminds us that parent-infant dyads greatly differ in theirattachment styles and, accordingly, in their capacity foremotion regulation. The ‘‘Strange Situation’’ paradigm(Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978) uses observationof mother-infant separation episodes (analyzing infants’separation anxiety, exploration, stranger anxiety, andreunion behavior) to classify attachment styles and shedslight on how deeply attachment and emotion regulation areintertwined. Securely attached infants are distressed whenthe mother leaves, but easily soothed and happy when shereturns; insecurely attached infants are not. Exciting longi-tudinal research has documented the implications of par-ent-infant attachment for long-term developmentaloutcomes including emotion regulation in intimate rela-tionships later in life (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007; Sroufe,Coffino, & Carlson, 2010).

Early romantic relationships. Choosing a romanticpartner and starting an intimate relationship have tradi-tionally been regarded as developmental tasks of youngadulthood (Havighurst, 1976). However, individuals mayenter romantic relationships throughout the life span (i.e.,14% of US singles between age 57 and 85 are dating; Brown& Shinohara, 2013).

The early stages of a romantic relationship often focuson upregulating positive emotions such as passionate love,affection, and excitement (Gable, Gonzaga, & Strachman,2006). Romantic love has been coined a ‘‘mammalian sys-tem for mate choice’’ (Fisher, Aron, & Brown, 2006) and isassociated with feelings of euphoria, obsessive thinkingabout and craving for connection with the partner;expanded sense of self; and increased energy (Aron, Fisher,Mashek, Strong, Li, & Brown, 2005; Aron & Aron, 1996),which may assume an almost addictive quality (Aron etal., 2005; Fisher et al., 2006). Downregulating negative emo-tion is important for couples at all stages of developmentwith managing jealousy thought to be particularly criticalfor early romantic relationships.

Relationships in late life. As individuals grow older,they face many new challenges; earlier sources of meaning(e.g., family building, career choice) may no longer be asrelevant (Erikson, 1950); social networks become smaller(Wrzus, Hanel, Wagner, & Neyer, 2012); physical abilitiesand health may become impaired (albeit with considerableindividual differences, Rowe & Kahn, 1997); and cognitivefunctioning may decline (Salthouse, 2004). However,numerous aspects of emotional functioning are preservedor even enhanced in late life, with (aspects of) emotionregulation being a prime example (Gross, Carstensen, Pasu-pathi, Tsai, Skorpen, & Hsu, 1997; Shiota & Levenson, 2009).

Socioemotional selectivity theory (e.g., Carstensen,2006; Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999) posits that,as time horizons shrink, older adults prioritize emotion reg-ulation (i.e., upregulating positive and downregulatingnegative emotions) over other goals. Positive emotions havesoothing effects (Fredrickson & Levenson, 1998), whichmay be one reason for why they become so important inlate life. Intimate relationships in turn are key for emotionregulation in late life, enhancing positive emotions suchas affection and reducing negative emotions such as fear(Coan & Maresh, 2013).

Consequences

Emotion regulation matters for many developmental out-comes including well-being, mental health, physical health,and social functioning (e.g., Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, &Schweizer, 2010; Cote, Gyurak, & Levenson, 2010; Gross& John, 2003; John & Gross, 2004; Lopes, Salovey, Cote,Beers, & Petty, 2005; Nelis et al., 2011).

A number of studies have examined consequences ofemotion regulation in couples (most interesting perhapswhen looking at actual couples’ interactions) for individual(e.g., cognition; Richards, Butler, & Gross, 2003) as well asrelationship (e.g., spousal abuse; McNulty & Hellmuth,2008; marital satisfaction; Smith, Cribbet, Nealey-Moore,Uchino, Williams, MacKenzie, & Thayer, 2011) outcomes.One laboratory-based paradigm has looked at the simpleact of spouses holding hands and documented its threat-reducing effects, especially in happily married couples(Coan, Schaefer, & Davidson, 2006).

Another laboratory-based paradigm (Levenson & Gott-man, 1983) has also provided insights into the conse-quences of couples’ emotion regulation. In this paradigm,couples engage in several unrehearsed 15-minute conversa-tions on relationship topics (e.g., events of the day, area ofdisagreement, pleasant topic). During these conversations,a number of physiological measures (e.g., heart rate, skinconductance) are measured continuously from both part-ners. Couples are videorecorded so that trained raters cancode their emotional behavior later. After the conversa-tions, partners view these videotapes and use a rating dialto indicate how positive or negative they felt (Gottman &Levenson, 1985). These streams of continuous multi-method data (physiology, behavior, subjective experience)can be used to create measures of emotion reactivity andregulation for the individual partners as well as the couple.

Studies utilizing this paradigm show how the regula-tion of (a) physiology (e.g., low levels of physiological arou-sal and physiological linkage; Gottman & Levenson, 1992;Levenson & Gottman, 1985); (b) emotional behavior (e.g.,high ratio of positive to negative emotional behaviors; Gott-man & Levenson, 1992), and (c) subjective experience (e.g.,negative emotional experience by one partner followed bynegative emotional experience by the other partner; Leven-son & Gottman, 1983; Levenson & Gottman, 1985) predictsgreater marital happiness and/or lower risk for divorce.

New findings from a 20-year longitudinalstudy of marriage

I want to highlight some findings from a 20-year longitudi-nal study of long-term married middle-aged and older cou-ples directed by Robert W. Levenson, Laura L. Carstensen,and John M. Gottman. In this study, couples visited thelaboratory to engage in a series of unrehearsed 15-minuteconversations in 1989/1990 and were followed up longitud-inally over 20 years. This study has produced a rich body offindings (e.g., Levenson, Carstensen, & Gottman, 1993;Levenson, Carstensen, & Gottman, 1994). Important earlierstudies demonstrated the power of couples’ emotion regu-lation for predicting concurrent relationship outcomes. Lessescalation of negative emotional behavior was found to beassociated with higher marital satisfaction (Carstensen,

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•18

Page 20: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Gottman, & Levenson, 1995). Greater levels of physiologicalsoothing were associated with higher positive emotionalbehavior (Yuan, McCarthy, Holley, & Levenson, 2010).

More recently, we have been able to start analyzinglongitudinal associations utilizing these data. We (Bloch,Haase, & Levenson, in press) developed a new perfor-mance-based measure of emotion regulation by determin-ing how quickly spouses reduce signs of negative emotionin experience, behavior, and physiological arousal afternegative emotion events during the marital conflict discus-sion. Our findings show that greater downregulation ofwives’ negative experience and negative behavior pre-dicted greater marital satisfaction for wives and husbandsconcurrently. Moreover, greater downregulation of wives’negative behavior predicted positive changes in wives’marital satisfaction over time. Wives’ use of constructivecommunication mediated the longitudinal associations.These results demonstrate the benefits of wives’ downregu-lation of negative emotion for marital satisfaction and high-light wives’ constructive communication as a mediatingpathway. Interestingly and defying common stereotypes,wives and husbands did not differ in their actual abilityto downregulate emotion; the gender difference emergedin whether emotion regulation mattered for marital satis-faction or not, suggesting that women may be perceivedas the emotional centers of the marriage (at least in thesecohorts). In another study, we (Holley, Haase, & Levenson,2013) analyzed changes in demand-withdraw behaviors inthis sample over a 13-year period and found stability inmost behaviors and increases in avoidance behaviors (e.g.,diverting attention or changing topics). We interpret thisfinding as reflecting couples’ capacity for greater disengage-ment from conflict as they are growing older (cf. Haase,Heckhausen, & Wrosch, 2013a; Heckhausen, Wrosch, &Schulz, 2010).

We are currently exploring longitudinal links betweenemotion regulation and outcomes beyond relationshipsatisfaction. Initial findings suggest that wives’ and hus-bands’ ability to downregulate negative emotion predictsanxiety and depression longitudinally above and beyondmarital satisfaction (Haase, Bloch, & Levenson, 2014, April).Other analyses are currently underway, examining age-related changes in positive and negative emotional beha-vior and links between emotion and empathy. Moreover,we have studied how genetic polymorphisms moderate theassociation between emotion and long-term changes inmarital satisfaction (Haase et al., 2013b).

Future directions

We need more research on emotion regulation in socialrelationships in general and couples in particular. Thisresearch will provide exciting insights into the nature ofemotion regulation, its development, consequences, andsources; and it has tremendous potential for informingapplications.

First, studies are needed that go beyond self-reportmeasures to assess actual emotion regulation in couplesby examining multiple response systems (e.g., subjectiveexperience, emotional behavior, language, autonomic andcentral physiology) and collecting data in the laboratoryas well as in the field. These studies may be particularly

interesting if they capture the dynamic, iterative, and co-regulatory qualities of couples’ emotion regulation, probenot only downregulation but also upregulation, and payattention to the regulation of both positive and negativeemotion.

Second, more research is needed on the development,looking at both change and continuity, of emotion regulationin couples across the life span. It will be greatly interestingto learn more about how couples’ emotion regulatory stylesand skills develop over time, when they are malleable, andwhen they become stable.

Third, more research is needed on the consequences ofemotion regulation, with special attention given to linkingdifferent kinds of emotion regulation with different kindsof outcomes (examining not only relationship stability andquality but also individuals’ well-being, mental health, andphysical heath).

Fourth, more research is needed on the sources of emo-tion regulation in couples, looking both at biological (e.g.,genetic, temperamental) and psychological (e.g., personal-ity, attachment history) factors that predispose individualsand couples to develop particular regulatory styles.

Finally, we believe that this research can inform as wellas benefit from couples’ counseling and therapy. Coupleswho struggle often struggle with regulating emotions, beit with downregulating negative emotion (e.g., fights overmoney, child rearing, in-laws) or with upregulating posi-tive emotions (e.g., loss of intimacy, sexual interest, joy).Couples therapies often zoom in on the non-emotionalaspects of these problems, addressing early experiences,individual psychopathology, or communication problems.While these factors are clearly important, addressing emo-tion and emotion regulation in the couple may be a partic-ularly fruitful point of entry for intervention (see, forexample, our finding that better emotion regulation pre-dicts improvements in communication for wives; Bloch etal., in press). There are a number of therapeutic approachesthat afford particular attention to emotion (e.g., Gottman &Gottman, 2008; Lebow, Chambers, Christensen, & Johnson,2012). More cross-talk between researchers and practi-tioners may be greatly informative.

In sum, the potential is enormous and we are lookingforward to future research on emotion regulation in inti-mate relationships.

Author noteThis article draws from a recent chapter written by RobertW. Levenson, myself, Lian Bloch, Sarah Holley, and Benja-min J. Seider (Levenson et al., 2013).

References

Ainsworth, M. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S.(1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of thestrange situation. Oxford: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010).Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology:A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30,217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

Aron, A., Fisher, H., Mashek, D. J., Strong, G., Li, H., &Brown, L. L. (2005). Reward, motivation, and emotionsystems associated with early-stage intense romantic

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•19

Page 21: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 94, 327–337. doi:10.1152/jn.00838.2004

Aron, E. N., & Aron, A. (1996). Love and the expansion ofthe self: The state of the model. Personal Relationships,3(1), 45–58.

Bloch, L., Haase, C. M., & Levenson, R. W. (in press). Emo-tion regulation predicts marital satisfaction: More than awives’ tale. Emotion.

Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachmentand healthy human development. New York, NY: BasicBooks.

Brown, S. L., & Shinohara, S. K. (2013). Dating relationshipsin older adulthood: A national portrait. Journal of Mar-riage and Family, 75, 1194–1202. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12065

Butler, E. A. (2011). Temporal interpersonal emotion sys-tems: the ‘‘TIES’’ that form relationships. Personality andSocial Psychology Review, 15, 367–393. doi: 10.1177/1088868311411164

Campos, J. J., Walle, E. A., Dahl, A., & Main, A. (2011).Reconceptualizing emotion regulation. Emotion Review,3, 26–35. doi: 10.1177/1754073910380975

Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time onhuman development. Science, 312, 1913–1915. doi:10.1126/science.1127488

Carstensen, L. L., Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1995).Emotional behavior in long-term marriage. Psychologyand Aging, 10(1), 140–149.

Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999).Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selec-tivity. American Psychologist, 54, 165–181. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.3.165

Clark, M. S., Ouellette, R., Powell, M. C., & Milberg, S.(1987). Recipient’s mood, relationship type, and helping.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 94–103.doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.94

Coan, J. A., & Maresh, E. L. (2013). Social baseline theoryand the social regulation of emotion. In J. J. G (Ed.)(Ed.), The handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed.). NewYork: The Guilford Press.

Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Lend-ing a hand: Social regulation of the neural response tothreat. Psychological Science, 17, 1032–1039. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.x

Cote, S., Gyurak, A., & Levenson, R. W. (2010). The ability toregulate emotion is associated with greater well-being,income, and socioeconomic status. Emotion, 10, 923–933.doi: 10.1037/a0021156

Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. [Book]. NewYork, NY, US: W W Norton & Co, 397.

Fisher, H. E., Aron, A., & Brown, L. L. (2006). Romanticlove: A mammalian brain system for mate choice. Philo-sophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: BiologicalSciences, 361, 2173–2186. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.1938

Fredrickson, B. L., & Levenson, R. W. (1998). Positive emo-tions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae ofnegative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 191–220.doi: 10.1080/026999398379718

Gable, S. L., Gonzaga, G. C., & Strachman, A. (2006). Willyou be there for me when things go right? Supportiveresponses to positive event disclosures. Journal of Person-ality and Social Psychology, 91, 904–917.

Gottman, J. M., & Gottman, J. S. (2008). Gottman methodcouple therapy. In A. S. Gurman (Ed.), Clinical handbook

of couple therapy (4th ed., pp. 138–164). New York, NY:Guilford Press.

Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1985). A valid procedurefor obtaining self-report of affect in marital interaction.Journal of Consulting & Clinical Psychology, 53(2), 151–160.

Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1992). Marital processespredictive of later dissolution: Behavior, physiology,and health. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,63, 221–233. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.221

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation:An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2(3),271–299.

Gross, J. J. (2013). Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed.Vol. The Guilford Press): New York, NY.

Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J.,Skorpen, C. G., & Hsu, A. Y. C. (1997). Emotion andaging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychologyand Aging, 12(4), 590–599.

Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences intwo emotion regulation processes: Implications foraffect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personal-ity and Social Psychology, 85(2), 348–362.

Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppres-sion: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 64(6), 970–986.

Gross, J. J., Richards, J. M., & John, O. P. (2006Emotion Regu-lation in Everyday LifeSnyder, Douglas K.; Simpson, Jeffry;Hughes, Jan N. (2006). Emotion regulation in couples and fam-ilies: Pathways to dysfunction and health. (pp. 13-35). Washing-ton, DC, US: American Psychological Association. xiv, 332 pp.Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Gross, J. J., Sheppes, G., & Urry, H. L. (2011). Taking one’slumps while doing the splits: A big tent perspective onemotion generation and emotion regulation. Cognition andEmotion, 25, 789–793. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.586590

Gyurak, A., Gross, J. J., & Etkin, A. (2011). Explicit andimplicit emotion regulation: A dual-process framework.Cognition and Emotion, 25, 400–412. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2010.544160

Haase, C. M., Bloch, L., & Levenson, R. W. (2014, April).Emotion regulation during marital conflict: Concurrentand longitudinal links with anxiety and depression. Pos-ter presented at the meeting of the Society for AffectiveScience, Bethesda, MD.

Haase, C. M., Heckhausen, J., & Wrosch, C. (2013a). Devel-opmental regulation across the life span: Toward a newsynthesis. Developmental Psychology, 49, 964–972. doi:10.1037/a0029231

Haase, C. M., Saslow, L. R., Bloch, L., Saturn, S. R., Casey, J.J., Seider, B. H., Lane, J., Coppola, G., & Levenson, R. W.(2013b). The 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in the serotonintransporter gene moderates the association betweenemotional behavior and changes in marital satisfactionover time. Emotion, 13, 1068–1079. doi: 10.1037/a0033761

Havighurst, R. J. (1976). Developmental tasks and education (3rd ed.). New York: David McKay.

Heckhausen, J., Wrosch, C., & Schulz, R. (2010). A motiva-tional theory of lifespan development. PsychologicalReview, 117, 32–60. doi: 10.1037/a0017668

Holley, S. R., Haase, C. M., & Levenson, R. W. (2013). Age-related changes in demand-withdraw communicationbehaviors. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 75, 822–836. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12051

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•20

Page 22: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthyemotion regulation: Personality processes, individualdifferences, and life span development. Journal of Person-ality, 72, 1301–1333.

Lebow, J. L., Chambers, A. L., Christensen, A., & Johnson, S.M. (2012). Research on the treatment of couple distress.Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 38(1), 145–168.

Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., & Gottman, J. M.(1993). Long-term marriage: Age, gender, and satisfac-tion. Psychology and Aging, 8, 301–313. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.8.2.301

Levenson, R. W., Carstensen, L. L., & Gottman, J. M.(1994). The influence of age and gender on affect, phy-siology, and their interrelations: A study of long-termmarriages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,67, 56–68.

Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1983). Marital interac-tion: Physiological linkage and affective exchange. Jour-nal of Personality & Social Psychology, 45(3), 587–597.

Levenson, R. W., & Gottman, J. M. (1985). Physiologicaland affective predictors of change in relationship satis-faction. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49(1),85–94.

Levenson, R. W., Haase, C. M., Bloch, L., Holley, S., &Seider, B. J. (2013). Emotion regulation in couples. In J.J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (2nd ed.).New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Lopes, P. N., Salovey, P., Cote, S., Beers, M., & Petty, R. E.(2005). Emotion regulation abilities and the quality ofsocial interaction. Emotion, 5(1), 113–118.

McNulty, J. K., & Hellmuth, J. C. (2008). Emotion regulationand intimate partner violence in newlyweds. Journal ofFamily Psychology, 22, 794–797. doi: 10.1037/a0013516

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adult-hood: Structure, dynamics, and change. New York, NY:Guilford Press.

Nelis, D., Kotsou, I., Quoidbach, J., Hansenne, M.,Weytens, F., Dupuis, P., & Mikolajczak, M. (2011).Increasing emotional competence improves psychologi-cal and physical well-being, social relationships, andemployability. [Clinical Trial]. Emotion, 11(2), 354–366.doi: 10.1037/a0021554

Richards, J. M., Butler, E. A., & Gross, J. J. (2003). Emotionregulation in romantic relationships: The cognitive con-sequences of concealing feelings. Journal of Social and Per-sonal Relationships, 20, 599–620. doi: 10.1177/02654075030205002

Rowe, J. W., & Kahn, R. L. (1997). Successful aging. The Ger-ontologist, 37(4), 433–440.

Salthouse, T. A. (2004). What and when of cognitive aging.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 140–144.doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00293.x

Shaver, P. R., & Mikulincer, M. (2007). Adult attachmentstrategies and the regulation of emotion. In J. J. Gross(Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation. (pp. 446–465). NewYork, NY: Guilford Press.

Shiota, M. N., & Levenson, R. W. (2009). Effects of aging onexperimentally instructed detached reappraisal, positivereappraisal, and emotional behavior suppression. Psy-chology and Aging, 24(4), 890–900.

Smith, T. W., Cribbet, M. R., Nealey-Moore, J. B., Uchino, B.N., Williams, P. G., MacKenzie, J., & Thayer, J. F. (2011).Matters of the variable heart: Respiratory sinus arrhyth-mia response to marital interaction and associationswith marital quality. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 100(1), 103–119.

Sroufe, L. A., Coffino, B., & Carlson, E. A. (2010). Concep-tualizing the role of early experience: Lessons from theMinnesota longitudinal study. Developmental Review,30, 36–51. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.12.002

Thompson, R. A. (1991). Emotional regulation and emo-tional development. Educational Psychology Review, 3,269–307. doi: 10.1007/bf01319934

Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional communica-tion in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.44.2.112

Wrzus, C., Hanel, M., Wagner, J., & Neyer, F. J. (2012).Social network changes and life events across the lifespan: A meta-analysis. [Peer Reviewed]. PsychologicalBulletin, 2012. doi: 10.1037/a0028601

Yuan, J. W., McCarthy, M., Holley, S. R., & Levenson, R. W.(2010). Physiological down-regulation and positive emo-tion in marital interaction. Emotion, 10, 467–474. doi:10.1037/a0018699

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•21

Page 23: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Are we Really Getting Better?Lifespan Differences in EmotionRegulatory Ability from thePerspective of DevelopmentalFunctionalism

Natalie TuckUniversity of Auckland, New ZealandE-mail: [email protected]

Iris MaussUniversity of California, Berkeley, USAE-mail: [email protected]

and

Nathan S. ConsedineUniversity of Auckland, New ZealandE-mail: [email protected]

Despite assertions that emotion regulation improves withage, evidence objectively testing this claim is uncommon.In this article, we briefly review data relevant to this impor-tant lifespan thesis, arguing that we are nearing the limitsof the insights we can gain with cross-sectional, self-reportdata; designs in which regulatory skills are objectivelyassessed are needed. Next, we summarize developmentalfunctionalism, a framework that makes specific predic-tions regarding the types of regulation that might beexpected to improve (and decline) across the adult life-span. This view suggests that while skills based in devel-opmentally-acquired knowledge such as situationselection may generally improve with age, skills that relyon capacities that decline (e.g., executive processing) mayshow age-linked decrements. Finally, we present earlydata from a study testing aspects of this model. In thestudy, 64 adults from across the lifespan were requiredto enhance and suppress anger and sadness expressionsafter being randomized to being either warned (experi-mental) or not warned (control) about the forthcomingtasks. Preliminary analyses examining whether suppres-sive and enhancement ability improves with age and isconsistent for anger and sadness across warned versusnon-warned conditions are presented. Implications forthe objective study of age differences in emotion regula-tory abilities and later life adaptation are discussed anddirections for future research are given.

Method

Although it is widely accepted that emotion regulatoryfunctioning improves across the adult lifespan (Blan-chard-Fields, 2007; Carstensen, Fung, & Charles, 2003;

Carstensen & Mikels, 2005; Urry & Gross, 2010), surpris-ingly little empirical evidence is directly demonstrative ofthis claim. Broadly speaking, there are three classes of datarelevant to this assertion: self-reported affect balance data,self-reported improvements/differences in emotional con-trol, and experimental data in which regulation is objec-tively assessed. Inferring better regulation based on self-reported emotion is problematic, self-reports on traits orabilities may not correspond with objective assessments(Bonanno, Pat-Horenczyk, & Noll, 2011; Schwartz, Neale,Marco, Shiffman, & Stone, 1999), and studies that objec-tively assess regulatory performance are scanty. Commen-tators tend to infer superior regulation based on greaterpositive affect or accept self-reported regulation as evi-dence of improved skill. Insights based on inferential meth-odologies are limited, and the systematic study of agedifferences in objectively assessed regulatory performanceis a necessary next step in this area.

A recent review of lifespan studies in which emotionregulatory skills were objectively assessed (Consedine &Mauss, 2014) concludes by suggesting that although agingdoes not appear to bring a unilateral decline in ability, dif-ferent types of regulatory task show distinct patterns ofimprovement and decline, and distinct tactics may beemployed to accomplish the same regulatory ends (Conse-dine, 2011a; Emery & Hess, 2011; Magai, Consedine, Kri-voshekova, McPherson, & Kudadjie-Gyamfi, 2006).Overall, there may be improvements in forms of emotionregulation linked to positive states (Isaacowitz, Toner, &Neupert, 2009; Phillips, Henry, Hosie, & Milne, 2008; Shiota& Levenson, 2009), social contexts or use of social supports(Akiyama, Antonucci, Takahashi, & Langfahl, 2003; Birditt& Fingerman, 2005; Opitz, Gross, & Urry, 2012), situationselection or modification (Blanchard-Fields, 2007; Blan-chard-Fields, Mienaltowski, & Seay, 2007; Charles & Car-stensen, 2008; Charles, Piazza, Luong, & Almeida, 2009),and, perhaps, in acceptance (Shallcross, Ford, Floerke, &Mauss, 2013). However, skills relying on executive pro-cesses may decline. Expressive suppression, for example,shows few age differences (Emery & Hess, 2011; Kunz-mann, Kupperbusch, & Levenson, 2005; Magai, et al.,2006; Phillips, et al., 2008; Shiota & Levenson, 2009) andstudies of reappraisal to decrease negative emotion suggestreduced ability (Opitz, Rauch, Terry, & Urry, 2012; Shiota &Levenson, 2009), despite greater use with age (John &Gross, 2004).

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•22

Page 24: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Lifespan differences in emotionregulatory skill – the view fromdevelopmental functionalism

Developmental functionalism is a discrete emotions-basedapproach to the study of emotions (Consedine & Magai,2003; Consedine, Magai, & Bonanno, 2002; Consedine &Moskowitz, 2007) and emotion regulation (Consedine,2011a, 2011b; Consedine & Mauss, 2014; Magai, et al.,2006), that pays explicit attention to lifespan development.In this view, changes in emotions and emotion regulationacross the lifespan involve the conjoint influences of devel-opmental variation in tasks, capacities and tactics. Theapproach suggests that understanding emotion regulationrequires an examination of developmental variation in reg-ulatory targets (the states, experiences or expressions thatwe are regulating towards or away from – the ‘‘task’’), thecapacities available to accomplish different forms of regula-tion (e.g., emotional understanding, executive resources),and the strategies that can be used to accomplish tasks giventhe available resources.

Because different emotion regulatory tasks are based indistinct resource or capacity sets, that have normative trajec-tories of improvement and decline, it is possible to make pre-dictions regarding the specific regulatory skills that mightimprove or decline. Developmental functionalism organizesthe capacities relevant to emotion regulation – self-aware-ness, cultural referencing, executive functioning, linguisticability, knowledge of others, and the like – into two broadcategories: basic biological capacities and acquired charac-teristics (Consedine, 2011a). Predictively then, capacities inwhich learned improvement seems likely (reflectiveness,awareness of emotion, emotional and situational knowl-edge) might enhance forms of emotion regulation occurringearlier in the regulatory process (Gross, 1998), while norma-tive declines in somatic resources, energy, and executivecapacities may interfere with ‘‘online’’ forms of regulation.

Furthermore, the aging process itself can be seen as cre-ating a pressure to accomplish regulation within the con-straints imposed by fluctuating capacities. While someregulatory tasks may become automatized and requirefewer resources (Mauss, Bunge, & Gross, 2007; Mauss,Evers, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2006), changes in capacity necessi-tate changes in both the targets of regulation and the tacticsused to attain them. We should expect changes in tactics,

with a general increase in the ‘‘efficiency’’ of regulation anda tendency to (a) regulate earlier in the emotion-generationprocess or (b) use available resources to offset reductionsin the capacities needed for online regulation. Below, wepresent preliminary data from a study conducted within thisconceptual framework.

Results

Preliminary data from an ongoing study

In this initial report, 64 (of a target 120) adults grouped into<40 year and 40þ year groups completed regulatory tasks(enhance and suppress expression during anger and sad-ness-inducing films) after either being warned or notwarned regarding the forthcoming tasks. We expected thatwhen participants were warned, performance would becomparable across age groups because the warning wouldallow the older group to offset declines in online processingby drawing on other resource sets. However, in the absenceof a warning, we expected the younger group to demon-strate greater flexibility in expressive regulation.

Two independent raters, blind to condition, codedexpressivity relative to a neutral condition in four 50-sec-ond videos (angry and sad, enhanced and suppressed) foreach participant; scores were coded to indicate greater sup-pression or enhancement ability. A 2 (age group) x 2 (con-dition) repeated-measures ANOVA with emotion (anger/sad) and task (suppress/enhance) as within subject vari-ables, and age group and condition as between subject vari-ables was conducted.

Early analyses (see Figure 2) suggest that warned parti-cipants were marginally more successful, F (1, 56) ¼ 2.57,p < .10, with greater success in modulating anger versus sad-ness expressions, F (1, 56) ¼ 18.45, p < .001, and had betterenhancement (versus suppressive) ability, F (1, 56) ¼ 9.11,p < .05. While there was no main effect for age or evidence forthe expected interaction between age and warning, a trend-ing 2-way interaction between task and warning suggestedthat warnings promoted better enhancement, but did notalter suppression of expression, F (1, 56) ¼ 3.74, p < .10.

This interaction was qualified by 3-way interactionbetween age, emotion, and warning condition, F (1, 56) ¼3.34 p < .10; when warned, both younger and older adultswere better at regulating anger than sadness. Whenunwarned, however, older adults were no better at regulat-ing anger than sadness. Finally, there was a 4-way interac-tion between emotion, task, age and warning, F (1, 56) ¼5.03, p <.05. Follow-up t-testing indicated that while theolder group tended to benefit from a warning when enhan-cing anger (p ¼ .068) the younger group did not. Conver-sely, the younger adults benefitted from warnings whenattempting to enhance sadness (p < .01) while the oldergroup did not. There were also additional differences withinthe older group, who were better at enhancing (versus sup-pressing) sadness (p < .05), but better at suppressing angerthan sadness (p < 0.05).

Discussion, interpretations,and future directions

Although the number of experimental reports examininglifespan differences in emotion regulation has increased

Use more efficientregulatory tactics

TACTICS

Inhibit visibleexpression of emotion

Declining onlineregulatory capacities

TASK CAPACITY

Figure 1. The task, capacity, and tactic framework as applied tochanges in emotion regulation across the adult lifespan (adaptedfrom Consedine & Mauss, 2014).

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•23

Page 25: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

across the past decade, the field remains in its infancy (Con-sedine, 2011a). Few experimental studies have investigateddevelopmental variation in regulatory targets or attemptedto experimentally manipulate which resources participantsare able to use in regulation (the warning manipulation).Consistent with prior work, these preliminary analysesfound no overall age differences in broad regulatory ability.When participants were warned regarding the upcomingtasks, both younger and older groups performed compar-ably, being more successful in enhancing (versus suppres-sing) expressions and being more able to regulate anger(versus sad) expressions. However, warnings appeared tohelp the older group but not the younger group enhanceanger, while the younger group differentially benefitedfrom a warning when seeking to enhance sadness. Consis-tent with notions that motivational priorities may lead tomore socially-facilitative emotion regulation, the oldergroup were better at suppressing anger than sadness, butbetter at enhancing versus suppressing sadness.

Although these data are clearly preliminary and our anal-yses underpowered, they provide indications for at least sev-eral important possibilities. First, consistent with indicationsfrom other lifespan research (see Consedine & Mauss, 2014for a review), there were no age-related differences in theability to suppress emotional expressions; most effects werein the enhance component of the tasks where the older groupperformed more poorly when unwarned. While this mayreflect issues in coding suppression (i.e., degrees of ‘‘less’’expression are harder to reliably score than degrees of‘‘more’’), it may also be that enhancement is more demandingbecause it requires the communication of a specific targetwhile suppression simply requires the elimination of allexpression. Equally, it may be that differences in suppressionare masked because current cohorts of older adults are dispo-sitionally more prone to suppress and thus derive benefitsfrom automatization (i.e., reduced resource demand).

Second, it is also notable that it was in the older groupperformance during the ‘naturalistic’ (unwarned) condition

Figure 2. Enhancement and suppression scores for anger and sadness inductions under warned and unwarned conditions in two age groups.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•24

Page 26: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

that most differences emerged; despite being marginallylower in performance overall, this group was differentiallybetter at (a) suppressing anger and (b) enhancing sadness.Such a pattern may reflect age-related practice and/or prior-itizations in reducing the expression of interpersonally-damaging (versus facilitating) expression. It is possible, forexample, that the older sample perform more poorly on spe-cific tasks requiring the up-regulation of anger because theymust override a tendency to automatically downplay suchexpressions before they are able to enhance them.

Effective emotion regulation is a critical adaptive capac-ity in both younger (Bonanno, Papa, Lalande, Westphal, &Coifman, 2004; Westphal, Seivert, & Bonanno, 2010) andolder (Carstensen, et al., 2003; Charles & Carstensen, 2010;Consedine, 2011a) adults. Although these preliminary anal-yses have taken small steps towards identifying specificpatterns of change, they raise as many questions as theyanswer. Does anticipating a regulatory task impact successdifferently in adults of different ages? If so, for which emo-tions? Future studies are needed to identify how the targetsof emotion regulation vary across the lifespan and howcapacities and tactics interact to determine the efficacy withwhich regulatory targets are attained. Additional questionsregarding the links between regulatory skill and adaptivepsychological, social and physical health outcomes are alsosalient and worth further investigation.

References

Akiyama, H., Antonucci, T., Takahashi, K., & Langfahl, E. S.(2003). Negative interactions in close relationshipsacross the life span. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B:Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58B(2), 70–79.

Birditt, K. S., & Fingerman, K. L. (2005). Do we get better atpicking our battles? Age group differences in descrip-tions of behavioral reactions to interpersonal tensions.The Journals of Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciencesand Social Sciences, 60B(3), 121–128.

Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). Everyday problem solving andemotion: an adult developmental perspective. CurrentDirections in Psychological Science, 16(1), 26–31.

Blanchard-Fields, F., Mienaltowski, A., & Seay, R. B.(2007). Age differences in everyday problem-solvingeffectiveness: Older adults select more effective strate-gies for interpersonal problems. The Journals of Gerontol-ogy: Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences,62B(1), 61–64.

Bonanno, G. A., Papa, A., Lalande, K., Westphal, M., &Coifman, K. (2004). The importance of being flexible: Theability to both enhance and suppress emotional expres-sion predicts long term adjustment. Psychological Science,15(7), 482–487.

Bonanno, G. A., Pat-Horenczyk, R., & Noll, J. (2011). Copingflexibility and trauma: The perceived ability to Copewith Trauma (PACT) scale. Psychological Trauma: Theory,Research, Practice, and Policy, 3(2), 117–129.

Carstensen, L. L., Fung, H. H., & Charles, S. T. (2003). Socio-emotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emo-tion in the second half of life. Motivation and Emotion,27(2), 103–123.

Carstensen, L. L., & Mikels, J. A. (2005). At the intersec-tion of emotion and cognition. Aging and the positivity

effect. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14,117–121.

Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2008). Unpleasantsituations elicit different emotional responses in you-nger and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 23(3),495–504.

Charles, S. T., & Carstensen, L. L. (2010). Social and emo-tional aging. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 383–409.

Charles, S. T., Piazza, J. R., Luong, G., & Almeida, D. M.(2009). Now you see it, now you don’t: age differencesin affective reactivity to social tensions. Psychology andAging, 24(3), 645–653.

Consedine, N. S. (2011a). Capacities, targets, and tactics:Lifespan emotion regulation viewed from developmen-tal functionalism. In I. Nyclicek, A. Vingerhoets & M.Zeelenberg (Eds.), Emotion regulation and wellbeing (pp.13–29). New York: Springer.

Consedine, N. S. (2011b). Emotion, regulation, and learningacross the adult lifespan: Implications from develop-mental functionalism. In C. Hoare (Ed.), Oxford handbookof reciprocal adult development and learning (2nd ed., pp.132–154). New York: Oxford University Press.

Consedine, N. S., & Magai, C. (2003). Attachment andemotion experience in later life: The view from emo-tions theory. Attachment & Human Development, 5,165–187.

Consedine, N. S., Magai, C., & Bonanno, G. A. (2002). Mod-erators of the emotion inhibition-health relationship:A review and research agenda. Review of General Psychol-ogy, 6(2), 204–228. doi: 10.1037//1089-2680.6.2.204

Consedine, N. S., & Mauss, I. B. (2014). Tasks, capacities,and tactics - A skill-based conceptualization of emotionregulation across the lifespan. In P. Verhaeghen & C.Hertzog (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, social cognition, andeveryday problem solving in adulthood: Oxford UniversityPress.

Consedine, N. S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2007). The role of dis-crete emotions in health outcomes: a critical review. Jour-nal of Applied and Preventive Psychology, 12(2), 59–75. doi:10.1016/j.appsy.2007.09.001

Emery, L., & Hess, T. M. (2011). Cognitive consequences ofexpressive regulation in older adults. Psychology andAging, 26(2), 388–396. doi: 10.1037/a0020041

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation:An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2,271–299.

Isaacowitz, D. M., Toner, K., & Neupert, S. D. (2009). Use ofgaze for real-time mood regulation: Effects of age andattentional functioning. Psychology and Aging, 24(4),989–994.

John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthyemotion regulation: Personality processes, individualdifferences, and life span development. [Electronic Elec-tronic; Print]. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1301–1333.

Kunzmann, U., Kupperbusch, C. S., & Levenson, R. W.(2005). Behavioral inhibition and amplification duringemotional arousal: a comparison of two age groups. Psy-chology and Aging, 20(1), 144–158.

Magai, C., Consedine, N. S., Krivoshekova, Y. S.,McPherson, R., & Kudadjie-Gyamfi, E. (2006). Emotionexperience and expression across the adult lifespan:Insights from a multi-modal assessment study. Psychol-ogy & Aging, 21(1), 303–317.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•25

Page 27: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Mauss, I. B., Bunge, S. A., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Automaticemotion regulation. Social and Personality Psychology Com-pass, 1, 146–167. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00005.x

Mauss, I. B., Evers, C., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2006).How to bite your tongue without blowing your top:Implicit evaluation of emotion regulation predicts affec-tive responding to anger provocation. Personality andSocial Psychology Bulletin, 32(5), 589–602.

Opitz, P., Gross, J. J., & Urry, H. (2012). Selection, optimiza-tion, and compensation in the domain of emotion regu-lation: applications to adolescence, older age, and majordepressive disorder. Social and Personality PsychologyCompass, 6(2), 142–155.

Opitz, P., Rauch, L. C., Terry, D. P., & Urry, H. (2012).Prefrontal mediation of age differences in cognitivereappraisal. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(4), 645–655.

Phillips, L. H., Henry, J. D., Hosie, J. A., & Milne, A. B. (2008).Effective regulation of the experience and expression ofnegative affect in old age. The Journals of Gerontology: Series

B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 63B(3), P138–P145.

Schwartz, J. E., Neale, J., Marco, C., Shiffman, S. S., &Stone, A. A. (1999). Does trait coping exist? A momen-tary assessment approach to the evaluation of traits.Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 77, 360–369.

Shallcross, A. J., Ford, B. Q., Floerke, V. A., & Mauss, I. B.(2013). Getting better with age: The relationship betweenage, acceptance, and negative affect. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 104(4), 734–749.

Shiota, M. N., & Levenson, R. W. (2009). Effects of aging onexperimentally instructed detached reappraisal, positivereappraisal, and emotional behavior suppression. Psy-chology and Aging, 24(4), 890–900.

Urry, H. L., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation in olderage. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(6), 352–357. doi: doi: 10.1177/0963721410388395

Westphal, M., Seivert, N. H., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010).Expressive flexibility. Emotion, 10(1), 92–100.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•26

Page 28: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

ACommentary on EmotionRegulation across the Life Span

Susan T. CharlesUniversity of Southern California, USAE-mail: [email protected]

The current group of studies considers aspects of emotionalexperience and emotion regulation across populationsranging from adolescents to older adults, covering a diverseset of emotion regulation strategies, situations, and out-comes. These papers provide insight into different facetsof emotion regulation, and in so doing highlight the chal-lenges that researchers face when capturing this complexprocess using a single definition. In their work, they alsopoint to gaps in the literature and future directions forlife-span research. Below, I discuss each of these issues andcomment on the papers by Tuck et al., Larsen, Liew, Haase,Giunta and Iselin, and Schipper et al., beginning with thechallenges we have in the definition of emotion regulation.

Defining emotion regulation. Although different defi-nitions of emotion regulation exist, almost all of theminclude a complex range of behaviors and experiences thatencompass which emotions are felt, and how and whenthey occur and are expressed. Often, a definition is framedwithin a time series that describes emotion regulation stra-tegies used immediately before an emotional event isencountered, while the event is occurring, and after theevent has passed. Such a depiction organizes emotion reg-ulation around a specific emotion-eliciting event. One con-cern with such a focus, however, is whether pre-existingfactors, such as personality traits or other situational vari-ables, belong in this definition. Emotional experiences donot start and stop, and pre-existing emotional states as wellas more enduring characteristics influence how reactivepeople are when encountering emotion-eliciting stimuli.Two papers from this series discuss such factors.

Tuck and colleagues investigate the importance ofknowledge about an emotional event before it occurs. Theyfind that warning participants of the type of emotion-elicit-ing stimuli that will be shown leads to anger enhancementamong older adults and sadness enhancement amongyounger adults. In addition to illustrating the importanceof foreknowledge about the stimuli, they further discussthe role of dispositional traits. They consider how suppres-sion may be a dispositional tendency that is generallygreater among older adults, and thus will influence theirperformance on this emotion regulation strategy. By inves-tigating the key role that foreknowledge plays when pro-cessing emotional stimuli as well as the potential cohortdifferences in trait characteristics of emotion regulation,Tuck and colleagues highlight the importance of predis-posing factors that influence the emotion regulation pro-cess. Moreover, understanding how these factors mayhave differential effects across age groups and types ofemotions brings a nuanced, complex approach to emotionregulation research.

The emphasis on individual differences such as person-ality, emotional intelligence, and other psychosocial mea-sures for predicting emotional states is not new, yet it isunclear how researchers should include these characteris-tics in definitions of emotion regulation. Should they beportrayed as confounds that need to be controlled, orshould they be included as factors that predict successfulor unsuccessful emotion regulation? In her review, Larsendiscusses the importance of emotional authenticity, focus-ing on how people understand and interpret their ownemotional goals, needs and experiences. She stresses thesignificance of authenticity in both how people organizetheir lives to navigate and structure where emotions areexperienced (an emotion regulation strategy often referredto as situation selection), and how they acknowledge,accept and act in response to these experiences. Thisdescription of emotional authenticity, including the deter-mination of how and when emotions are experienced, par-allels many definitions of emotion regulation. Rarely,however, are these dispositional traits included in modelsof emotion regulation in life-span research. Larsen’s workpresents a notable exception to most models.

More than up or down-regulation. Perhaps studies ofemotion regulation often ignore dispositional traits becausethey focus on the dynamic modulation of emotional states.Studies of age differences in emotion regulation oftenexamine movement of emotional states, either by up-regu-lating or down-regulating positive and negative affect. Thedominance of this model makes sense earlier in the lifespan, when younger children lack the cognitive capacitiesfor other emotion regulation strategies, such as planningactivities that allow them to navigate their environment tocontrol the types of emotions they have, and when theyexperience them. As a result, researchers focus on emo-tional reactivity and recovery, such as how likely childrenare to become distressed and their response to an upsettingevent. With further cognitive development, older childrenare more capable of anticipating emotional states and enga-ging in antecedent strategies. Studies of older children’semotion regulation, however, often continue to focus pri-marily on stress reactivity, in both naturalistic studies usingdaily sampling and laboratory studies capturing reactionsto experimental stimuli. Liew avoids this common trap inhis study. He incorporates not only the importance of pre-disposing factors as mentioned before (in his case parentalcontrol), but examines emotion regulation strategies neces-sary to successfully avoid unpleasant outcomes in his stud-ies. The capabilities necessary to anticipate and regulate theenvironment – executive functioning emotional control –allow adolescents to attain their personal goals. Liew mea-sures these goals of adaptive functioning, such as academicsuccess, as indicators of successful emotion regulation.

When studying people across adulthood, researchersfocus on both antecedent-focused and response-focused

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•27

Page 29: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

strategies, and many investigators make different age-related predictions dependent on the type of strategiesexamined. Strength and Vulnerability Integration, for exam-ple, posits that older adults more often engage in strategiesthat allow them to avoid or limit their exposure to negativeexperiences altogether, and only when people are placedin situations of sustained arousal do age-related benefitsattenuate or disappear completely (Charles, 2010; Charles& Piazza, 2009).

The distinction, then, between antecedent versusresponse-focused strategies, is important in predicting agedifferences in emotion regulation abilities. Yet, definitionsof emotion regulation often narrow to either activelydown-regulating or up-regulating subjective emotionalstates. As a result, important information about age differ-ences in emotional experience is lost. For example, onestudy examining goals to regulate emotions found thatolder age was related to greater desire to maintain positiveaffective states (Riediger, Schmiedek, Wagner, & Linden-berg, 2009). Should this be seen as successful emotion reg-ulation, as indicated by the higher levels of well-beingreported by the older adults in this study? Or, should weinterpret these findings as not relevant to emotion regula-tion, because the respondents did not encounter a problemwhere modulation of emotion was necessary? Moreover,how can we further study the extent to which the olderadults engaged in actions that allowed them to experiencedesired emotion-states, or whether they were simply fortu-nate to be in such salubrious surroundings?

More emphasis on socio-cultural context of emotionregulation. Researchers often discuss how adults shapetheir social networks consistent with their emotional goals(e.g., Carstensen, 1992, 2006), but rarely do researchers dis-cuss how people at younger ages shape their social net-work in ways that influence their emotional well-being.Haase emphasizes the importance of interactional pro-cesses in her review. She describes how some of our stron-gest emotions are experienced within social situations, andemotion regulation strategies frequently include dynamicinteractions with others. She provides specific examplesof interaction patterns that serve as emotion regulationindicators, such as how quickly husbands and wivesdown-regulate negative emotions at the time they areexperienced, and assesses their success with specific socialoutcomes (i.e., relationship satisfaction). This is an excitingnew envisioning of emotion regulation that can be exam-ined across different age groups.

In further probing the powerful influence of others inemotion regulation processes, Giunta and Iselin’s paperfocuses on the influence of parental behavior on the emo-tion regulation abilities of adolescents. Importantly, how-ever, they describe how the cultural context – specificallysocial norms – influences the strength of these effects. Theyfind that negative parental behavior (parental control) isrelated to poorer emotion regulation behaviors of their ado-lescent (dysregulated sadness expression; depressive rumi-nation), which in turn are related to their adolescent’sdepressive symptoms. They also found, however, that thestrength of these associations is related to the cultural normof parental control. When parental control is more norma-tive in a culture, it has less impact on depressive symptoms,as indicated by the attenuation of the indirect associationbetween parental control and depressive symptoms.

Measuring emotion regulation and its success. Muchof this discussion relates to methodology: how do we incor-porate (or not) dispositional traits in models of emotion reg-ulation; how do we interpret the same concept (e.g.,parental control) in different environments; how do weexpand definitions of emotion regulation to include interac-tive processes. These questions also highlight the difficultyencountered when making comparisons across people whorely on different types of emotion regulation strategies. Forexample, imagine two people: one who carefully navigateshis or her environment to avoid potential negative situa-tions, and as a result experiences high levels of overallwell-being with few fluctuations in negative affect. The otherperson takes no preemptive emotion regulation measures,and encounters daily stressors often. However, this personreacts less strongly to these stressors when they occur thandoes the first individual. What, if anything, can we say aboutoverall differences in emotion regulation? And how often dopeople flexibly move across different types of emotion regu-lation strategies (e.g., Bonanno & Burton, 2013), and howdoes this flexibility vary across age groups?

Comparison across groups of people who engage in suchdifferent emotion regulation strategies is further complicatedby variations in methods of measuring successful emotionregulation. For those examining antecedent strategies, per-haps the number of daily stressors (or reported events) maybe a guide, although these outcomes are complicated by theopportunities and challenges afforded by the environment.Studies of reactivity and recovery, in contrast, focus on thetime needed for physiological arousal or subjective state toreturn to a baseline measure. These different measures ofemotion regulation raise questions concerning what are themost important indicators of emotion regulation, and howbest to capture them. On a related note, what are the impor-tant outcomes? These papers explore the range of emotionregulation literature, in studies that examine diverse out-comes such as depressive symptoms, facial expressions, aca-demic performance, relationship satisfaction, emotionalnegativity, and other well-being indicators. Perhaps it is timefor us to examine how strategies generalize, or not, to thesediverse outcomes.

Studying a range of outcomes inspires questions regard-ing what is the best indicator of emotion regulation. Schipperand colleagues present findings that raise the intriguingquestion of who is the best judge of these outcomes. In theirstudy, they had adolescents and their parents rate the adoles-cent’s functioning using the Strengths and Difficulties Ques-tionnaire (SDQ), a standardized measure that provides bothan overall score of emotional and behavioral functioning andseparate subscales for five areas of psychosocial functioning.They found that although the overall score on the SDQ didnot differ between the adolescents and their parent-infor-mants, the pairs were discrepant on three subscale ratings.Specifically, the parents rated their adolescent higher onemotional and conduct problems that did the adolescents,and the adolescents rated their hyperactivity as higher thandid their parents. Given such differences, these results raisequestions regarding whose reports we weight more heavily,and whose are more predictive of later problems across awide range of individual and social areas.

Conclusion. The current set of studies produces inter-esting results and raises intriguing questions regardingemotion regulation across the life span. Definitions of

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•28

Page 30: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

emotion regulation are necessarily expansive, but theseencompassing definitions present challenges with regardto how to incorporate the many facets of emotion regulationinto a single model, and how to compare findings acrossdifferent strategies. The papers also provide direction forour future research. They point to factors that influenceemotional functioning and emotional experience thatshould be incorporated in studies of emotion regulation.They highlight the need to become more aware of the socio-cultural influences that surround these processes, and theypoint to the challenges of integrating different indicatorsand outcomes for the complex set of processes that fallunder the term emotion regulation.

References

Bonanno, G. A., & Burton, C. L. (2013). Regulatory Flexibil-ity: An Individual Differences Perspective on Coping

and Emotion Regulation. Perspectives on Psychological Sci-ence, 8, 591–612.

Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns inadulthood: support for socioemotional selectivity the-ory. Psychology and Aging, 7, 331–338.

Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of timeon human development. Science, 312, 1913–1915.

Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and vulnerability integra-tion: a model of emotional well-being across adult-hood. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1068–1092.

Charles, S. T., & Piazza, J. R. (2009). Age differences in affec-tive well-being: Context matters. Social and PersonalityPsychology Compass, 3, 711–724.

Riediger, M., Schmiedek, F., Wagner, G. G., &Lindenberger, U. (2009). Seeking pleasure and seekingpain: Differences in prohedonic and contra-hedonicmotivation from adolescence to old age. PsychologicalScience, 20, 1529–1535.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•29

Page 31: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

The Elusiveness of a Life-SpanModel of Emotion Regulation

Tamara L. SimsStanford University, USAE-mail: [email protected]

and

Laura L. CarstensenStanford University, USAE-mail: [email protected]

The articles published in this special issue offer an excellentcharacterization of the research questions and approachesthat psychologists apply to the study of emotion regulationacross the life span. Each of the articles addresses importantinputs into the regulation of emotion, from the influence ofparents and peers on the regulatory capacities of childrenand adolescents, to the roles of social norms and marriagein adulthood, to the effects of age on the regulation of dis-crete emotions.

Di Giunta and Iselin illustrate, for example, how emo-tional functioning in childhood relies heavily on parents andcaretakers. Children are explicitly taught basic strategiesthat lay the groundwork for healthy emotional develop-ment. In western cultures, these primary strategies involveteaching children to up-regulate positive and down-regulatenegative emotional states. The failure to do so can have det-rimental consequences for well-being, particularly in societ-ies that value these strategies. Liew examines how parentalautonomy support shapes children’s capacity for emotionregulation among Chinese immigrants, challenging thebelief that Chinese parenting is restrictive and controlling.Rather, he maintains, parental guidance of emotion regula-tion reflects cultural norms and sets the foundation for chil-dren’s healthy academic development.

Schipper, Nitkowski, Koglin, and Petermann addressthe transitional period of adolescence, in which emotionregulation becomes increasingly self-initiated while simul-taneously influenced by peers. During adolescence, regula-tion takes on a new level of complexity as individuals learnto regulate emotions in the service of establishing andmaintaining social connections outside of the family, learn-ing how to up-regulate negative (expressing sadness to soli-cit comfort from friends) and down-regulate positive (e.g.,downplaying receiving an award to fit in with friends)emotions. The departure from parental guidance and aprioritization of peer acceptance may explain why the emo-tional lives of adolescents can be particularly unstable.Schipper et al.’s findings about contrahedonic motivationsand the difficulty parents have in inferring emotional statesof their adolescent children complement those from a recentreport by Michaela Reidiger and colleagues based on expe-rience sampling spanning adolescence to very old age (Rei-diger, Schmiedek, Wagner & Lindenberger, 2009). Whenpaged, participants were asked what they were feeling andwhether they wanted to increase those feelings or decrease

them. Among the adults, the predicted patterns wereobserved. When positive emotions were reported, partici-pants wanted to maintain or enhance them; when negativeemotions were experienced, participants expressed a motiveto contain or diminish them. Adolescents, however, pre-sented an important exception: The typical response to neg-ative emotions was the desire to heighten the negativeexperience. These findings are consistent with Larsen’s dis-cussion of authenticity. Larsen’s emphasis on authenticitychallenges the assumption that effectively down-regulatingnegative experience is the inevitable ‘‘appropriate’’response, an observation that may be particularly true ofadolescents.

Haase’s review suggests that emotion regulation in inti-mate relationships raises additional challenges. In the con-text of marriage, the ability to infer sometimes subtle cuesand up-regulate or down-regulate one’s own emotionsaccordingly is critical. Indeed, marital well-being, at leastin modern western cultures, appears to rest fundamentallyon partners’ abilities to manage their own emotions whilesimultaneously attending to those of their significant others.

Tuck et al. question the widely held view that emotionregulation improves with age. By isolating an emotional eli-citor (the need to up- or down-regulate either anger or sad-ness) and by the explicitness of the regulatory demand, theyobserve more age group similarities than differences. Asthey acknowledge, limitations in statistical power rendertheir findings highly tentative. However, their theoreticalframework and methodological approach offer readers aview of the types of hypothess researchers test and howthey test them when investigating the capacity for emotionregulation.

Individually, each of the papers raises important ques-tions and issues. Together, the set also illustrates the dis-jointed conceptualization of emotion regulation evident inthe broader field of life-span development. Not only are theresearch traditions and methods adopted by those studyingchild, adolescent and adult development different from oneanother, the implicit assumptions about emotion andrelated questions vary by life stage. Research on regulationin young children tends to focus on parental styles and rela-tionships, thereby placing the emphasis squarely in socialcontext. When studying increasingly older age groups,however, researchers focus on individuals, often on emo-tion regulation in the laboratory devoid of social context.Indeed, Tuck et al. maintain that this is the only way toreally know whether or not emotion regulation improveswith age.

The tacit assumption is that emotion regulation at thebeginning of life is externally supported and graduallymoves inward to the point where regulation can be studiedoutside of social and cultural contexts. In all likelihood, theassumption that regulation is externally resourced in earlylife and grows increasingly self-initiated is, at least

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•30

Page 32: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

partially, true. Unable to coordinate movement and lackingneural pathways that allow them to quell emotional bursts,infants rely on touch and the vocal expressions of care-givers to regulate their emotions. Shortly after childrenbegin to speak, researchers begin to study explicit strategiesthat individuals employ once an emotion has been elicited.Steadily, researchers move away from consideration ofsocial context, asking whether individual differences inrumination styles place people at risk for depression, andwhether the contexts in which people find themselves sti-mulate ruminative thinking or fail to offer opportunities fordistraction.

In adulthood, a strong research tradition has developed,illustrated by Tuck et al., to study emotional regulation in thelaboratory by eliciting specific emotions and observing theresponse as indexed by facial expressions, subjective reportsand physiological reactivity. This research tradition tacitlydownplays environmental contexts. The approach hasmethodological appeal. Yet, as Campos and colleagues(2011) have eloquently argued, it is unlikely that emotionsare ever unregulated. Rather, emotion regulation is adynamic, ongoing process. Indeed, emotional experience isarguably the best measure of the effectiveness of emotionregulation in daily life, and there is considerable evidence– both cross-sectional and longitudinal using a range ofmethods – that emotional experience improves with age(Carstensen, Pasupathi, Mayr, & Nesselroade, 2000; Car-stensen et al., 2011; Charles, Reynolds, & Gatz, 2001; Gruhn,Kotter-Gruhn, & Rocke, 2010; Stone, Schwartz, Broderick, &Deaton, 2010; Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998).

Left unexamined, differences in theoretical and metho-dological approaches can also inadvertently obscure theways that people actually regulate emotions, and leadinvestigators to overlook potential continuities and discon-tinuities across the life span. Through the lens of socioemo-tional selectivity theory (SST; Carstensen, 2006), selection isseen as a key emotion regulation strategy that presents itselfvery early in life – six-month-old infants bury their heads inthe shoulders of caregivers when strangers approach, forexample – and represents an increasingly effective andcommon regulatory strategy throughout life.

To elaborate, the SST life-span theory of motivationmaintains that socioemotional goals change systematicallyas a function of time horizons. When time horizons are longand nebulous, as they typically are in youth, people engagein exploration and seek to acquire knowledge in prepara-tion for an uncertain future. As time horizons grow increas-ingly constrained, people focus more on savoring existingrelationships and enhancing emotional meaning. Theseshifting time horizons shape our social environments andtheir associated emotion regulatory demands. Indeed, thesocial world changes systematically with age (Wrzus,Hanel, Wagner, & Neyer, 2013) in ways suggestive of activepruning of networks such that they increasingly comprisewell-known and emotionally significant partners (Lang &Carstensen, 2002).

Early adulthood ushers in a burgeoning number of rela-tionships that promise opportunities for learning andexpansion of the social world. Large and diverse networksalso demand considerable flexibility and a range of regula-tory strategies. Social networks include the widest range ofpartners at this stage in life; relationships with family andclose friends open to include novel acquaintances,

professional associates, and eventually intimate relation-ships. As people move through adulthood, social networksbecome increasingly exclusive and meaningful; thesesmaller social networks hold benefits for emotional well-being (English & Carstensen, 2014).

According to SST, the expansion early in life adaptivelyprepares young people for the long and nebulous futuresthat they likely face. Chronically pursuing exploration, how-ever, demands emotional risk-taking and can entail anxiety,frustration and anger. Ample research documents the highlyemotional quality of adolescence and young adulthood (Lar-son & Sheeber, 2008). As time horizons shrink, goals change.Emotionally meaningful experiences are prioritized. Theo-retically, networks change to reflect changes in goals. Selec-tion is the key regulatory strategy. By focusing on importantrelationships, selection privileges emotionally meaningfulexperience and allows for a deepening of close bonds. Socialnetworks that are comprised of emotionally close relation-ships allow goals to be achieved more effectively. Evidencesuggests that older people, compared to their younger coun-terparts, are more likely to both pursue and realize affectivegoals (Scheibe, English, Tsai & Carstensen, 2013).

Even though selection is categorized as an antecedentemotion regulatory strategy, it gets relatively little attentionin the field of emotion regulation. Yet selectivity serves as akey regulatory strategy across the life span and is arguablythe most effective of strategies. When selectivity is effec-tively deployed, there is no need to suppress facial expres-sions or down-regulate negative emotions. Young childrenstay physically close to caretakers in the presence of stran-gers. Parents explicitly teach selection (Gross & Thompson,2007) to their children, urging them to interact with peopleand in situations that make them feel good and to avoidones that elicit negative emotions. With age, people useselection increasingly frequently. As they enter adulthood,they reliably choose products, activities, and people thathelp them feel how they want to feel (Tsai, 2007; Sims, Tsai,Koopmann-Holm, Thomas, & Goldstein, 2014). Goals directcognitive resources. Scores of studies now show that age isassociated with selective attention to positive over negativeemotional stimuli (Reed, Chan, & Mikels, in press).

As people age and become more adept at selection (in partbecause they are better at predicting how a certain context willmake them feel; e.g., Scheibe, Mata, & Carstensen, 2011), effec-tiveness of selection in regulating emotional states improvesas well. True, selection is not always a viable option and in dis-tressing situations, age may not offer an emotion regulatoryadvantage. Susan Charles developed an elegant model, Stressand Vulnerability Integration (SAVI) that aims to predict howwell older people regulate emotions when they are unable touse selection (Charles, 2010).

As Consedine and Magai (2006) have argued, a fullunderstanding of emotional changes with age requires con-sideration of distinct emotions and affective states accord-ing to their social function. We agree as do other authorsin this issue. For example, Di Giunta and Iselin found thatthe impact of regulating negative emotion on mental well-being is largely shaped by culturally normative parentingpractices; in the same vein, Larsen finds that suppressionis not harmful to adolescents in the same way it may befor adults. As such, the interpretation and importance offindings examining emotional regulation across the lifespan vary as a function of social context and motivation.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•31

Page 33: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Relatively poor execution of strategies that are rarely, ifever, used is not as important as understanding the effec-tiveness of strategies deployed on a regular basis. It maybe, for example, that although younger adults often findthemselves in situations (e.g., confrontation) that signal theutility of anger (Tamir & Ford, 2012), through selectionolder adults circumvent similar situations (Blanchard-Fields, Mienaltowsi, & Seay, 2007).

Despite the conceptual and methodological limitationsever present in the study of emotional development, thework described in this bulletin offers a variety of insightfulperspectives on the course of emotion regulation rangingacross naturalistic and experimental settings. Notably, onecommonality tying together these diverse approaches is theinsight that how people regulate their emotions is largelydetermined by their socioemotional goals. Integratingsocioemotional goals into models of emotion regulationacross the life span raises questions about how we concep-tualize and operationalize emotion regulation and emo-tional experience.

References

Blanchard-Fields, F., Mienaltowski, A., & Seay, R. B. (2007).Age differences in everyday problem-solving effective-ness: Older adults select more effective strategies forinterpersonal problems. Journals of Gerontology: Psycholo-gical Science, 62, 61–64.

Campos, J. J., Walle, E. A., Dahl, A., & Main, A. (2011).Reconceptualizing emotion regulation. Emotion Review,3(1), 26–35.

Carstensen, L. L. (2006). The influence of a sense of time onhuman development. Science, 312, 1913–1915.doi:10.1126/science.1127488

Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Mayr, U., & Nesselroade, J.(2000). Emotional experience in everyday life across theadult life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,79, 644–655. doi: I O.1037//O022-3 514.79.4.64

Carstensen, L. L., Turan, B., Scheibe, S., Ram, N., Ersner-Hershfield, H., Samanez-Larkin, G., Brooks, K., &Nesselroade, J. R. (2011). Emotional experience improveswith age: Evidence based on over 10 years of experiencesampling. Psychology and Aging, 26, 21–33. doi: 10.1037/a0021285

Charles, S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz, M. (2001). Age-related differences and change in positive and negativeaffect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 80, 136–151.

Charles, S. T. (2010). Strength and Vulnerability Integration(SAVI): A Model of Emotional Well-Being Across Adult-hood. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 1068–1091. doi: 10.1037/a0021232

Consedine, N. S., & Magai, C. (2006). Emotion developmentin adulthood: A developmental functionalist review andcritique. In C. Hoare (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of adultdevelopment and learning (pp. 209–244). New York:Oxford University Press.

English, T, & Carstensen, L. L. (2014). Selective narrowing ofsocial networks across adulthood is associated with

improved emotional experience in daily life. InternationalJournal of Behavioral Development, 38, 195–202. doi:10.1177/0165025413515404

Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation:Conceptual foundations. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook ofEmotion Regulation (pp. 3–24): Guilford Press.

Gruhn, D., Kotter-Gruhn, D., & Rocke, C. (2010). Discreteaffects across the lifespan: Evidence for multidimension-ality and multidirectionality of affective experiences inyoung, middle-aged and older adults. Journal of Researchin Personality, 44, 492–500.

Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Futuretime perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychol-ogy and Aging, 17, 125–139.

Larson, R. W., & Sheeber, L. B. (2008). The daily emotionalexperience of adolescents: Are adolescents more emo-tional, why, and how is that related to depression, inAdolescent emotional development and the emergence ofdepressive disorders, 11–32.

Mroczek, D. K., & Kolarz, C. M. (1998). The effect of age onpositive and negative affect: A developmental perspec-tive on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 75, 1333–1349.

Reed, A. E., Chan, L., & Mikels, J. A. (in press). Meta-anal-ysis of the age-related positivity effect: Age differencesin preferences for positive over negative information.Psychology and Aging.

Reidiger, M., Schmiedek, F., Wagner, G., & Linden-berger, U. (2009). Seeking pleasure and seeking pain:Differences in prohedonic and contra-hedonic motiva-tion from adolescence to old age. Psychological Science.20, 1529-1535. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02473.x

Scheibe, S., English, T., Tsai, J. L., & Carstensen, L. L. (2013).Striving to feel good: Ideal affect, actual affect, and theircorrespondence across adulthood. Psychology and Aging.28, 160–71. doi: 10.1037/a0030561.

Scheibe, S., Mata, R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2011). Age differ-ences in affective forecasting and experienced emotionsurrounding the 2008 US presidential election. Cognition& emotion, 25(6), 1029–1044. DOI:10.1080/02699931.2010.545543

Sims, T., Tsai, J. L., Koopmann-Holm, B., Thomas, E. A.C., & Goldstein, M. K. (2014). Choosing a physiciandepends on how you want to feel: The role of idealaffect in health-related decision making. Emotion,14(1), 187–192. doi: 10.1037/a0034372

Stone, A.A., Schwartz, J.E., Broderick, J.E., & Deaton, A.(2010). A snapshot of the age distribution of psycholo-gical well-being in the United States. 107(22):9985–90.

Tamir, M., & Ford, B. Q. (2012). When feeling bad isexpected to be good: Emotion regulation and outcomeexpectancies in social conflicts. Emotion, 12(4), 807–816.doi: 10.1037/a0024443

Tsai, J. L. (2007). Ideal affect: Cultural causes and behavioralconsequences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3),242–259.

Wrzus, C., Hanel, M., Wagner, J., & Neyer, F. J. (2013). Socialnetwork changes and life events across the life span:A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 53–80.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•32

Page 34: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Country Focus

Towards a Child Inclusive and ChildFocused Child Custody DecisionMaking Model in Zimbabwe

Gwatirera JavangweUniversity of Zimbabwe, Zimbabwe.E-mail: [email protected]

Introduction

Child participation has been studied globally in anumber of contexts. Although this global trend has to dateproduced impressive results and provides assurance aboutchild sensitive care, the same cannot be said about theprevailing situation in Zimbabwe. One area where thereis a dearth of literature and child inclusive practice in Zim-babwe is that of child custody decision making. Despite theprinciple that the best interests of the child play a pivotalrole in all matters concerning children, there appears tobe limited scope in the application of this concept inZimbabwe family law contexts, particularly when parentsseparate or divorce. This is clearly shown in the study byNcube ( 1998a, p. 5) in which he indicated that ‘‘most of themodern international human rights law on children isbased on the assumption that there are universal values andnorms.’’ This assumptive universalism is not upheld in anumber of contexts. Ncube (1988b) further notes that priorto the invention of childhood, children enjoyed the samerights as adults.

Gender specific bias in awarding childcustody: how favoring the father leadsto bruising of children’s ego

Ruombwa (2004) noted that there is gender specific bias inthe way child custody and guardianship is determined inZimbabwe. The bias tips the scale in favor of the fathers.Giving stronger rights to fathers often fails to uphold thebest interest of children. This is clearly shown in case law.For example, in Zvorwadza v. Zvorwadza, where the partieswere married, the trial judge awarded custody to the fatherdespite the father not even having indicated willingness tohave custody of the children. Interestingly, this case furthernegated the children’s rights to be heard as enshrined inthe Convention of the Rights of the Child and the AfricanCharter on the Rights and Welfare of the Child. Thus thechildren were never given an opportunity to be heard orconsulted. This appears to be a noxious arrangement thatstill exists in the Zimbabwean courts of law. This furtheris demonstrated by Chirawu (2013, p. 9) who noted that‘‘Zimbabwean courts have not been consistent in the appli-cation of the best interests of the child concept.’’ In concur-rence, Mutambara, (2005, p. 2) questioned the adequacy of

the Zimbabwean laws with regards to ‘‘compliance withinternational instruments on children’s rights.’’ It followstherefore that the ’’golden rule’’ of awarding custody to thefather also further silences the children. The children reel inagony as it is considered uncouth for children to questionthe actions of adults.

Contradictions in Zimbabwean family lawpractice and impact on children’semotion regulation

Surprisingly and contrary the provisions of the law, Zim-babwean courts have also violated their own rules or pro-duced some contradictions. This is well captured byChirawu (2013) in the Zvorwadza v. Zvorwadza case in whichcustody was given to a third party, the maternal grand-mother, despite the remaining parent, the father, beingalive; or custody was given to the great grand-mother asin the Domboka case. It can be argued that the emotionaldevelopmental needs of the children tend to be neglectedin these cases. As such, children may develop complextrauma which can manifest in disorganized and dysfunc-tional behaviors. These behaviors may encompass a varietyof troubling self-regulatory and social-interactional adapta-tions, including aggression toward self and others, impulsecontrol problems, anxiety, hypervigilance, coercive interac-tion cycles, and social withdrawal. Hussey et al. furthernote that complex trauma manifests in children with his-tories of both child maltreatment and attachment disrup-tions. This issue is further highlighted in a case lawobservation in Z v Z where the mother subjected the chil-dren to physical abuse. Children’s emotional turmoil fromabuse by a parent is rarely addressed.

It appears that in Zimbabwe the courts will only solicitthe views of the children with respect to child custodywhen both parents qualify for custody (Chirawu, 2013).This was clearly illustrated by the Zvorwadza case in whichthe court faced a dilemma about which parent to grant cus-tody to. The court, which did not consult the children,showed itself to be insensitive to the children’s care, welfareand mental health needs. Part of the problem may lie in thelegal practitioners’ lack of knowledge and expertise in childparticipatory methodologies, even when the Judge or pre-siding Magistrate hears the children’s views in Chambersor Children’s Court respectively. Another area that isreplete with controversy and related to child custody isguardianship following the death of both parents. Thisis adequately captured in the study by Nyamapfeni (2012)in which she noted that the guardianship decisions regard-ing double orphans often does not benefit the children.Thus where children have lost both parents due to HIV andAIDS, the extended families in Zimbabwe ‘‘strategize onwho will be responsible for the care and welfare of the chil-dren’’ (p, 28). Subsequent to that the brother or sister of the

•33

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

Page 35: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

deceased is usually appointed the Sarapavana (guardian).This culminates in the Sarapavana presiding over who getsguardianship of which child. In all these scenarios the chil-dren are not consulted. In most cases the children eventu-ally become internally displaced persons. Their situations

may be worsened by having to live apart from each otherand in different locations with limited communication.These children are highly likely to bottle up a lot of emo-tions as a result of all these arrangement but cannot ventthem other than engaging in acting out behaviour. The

Ministry of Justice, Legal & Parliamentary Affairs Approval

Recruit participating magistrates’ courts (n=3) and legalofficers(n=6)

Recruit divorcing parents (n=60) and children (n=60)

Baseline data, interviews with magistrates, lawyersand collection of biographical data& parental acrimony, child participation in custody decision making data

Participants receive usual services

Treatment (n= 30) child inclusive + (n=30) child focused custody

Parenting programme, CBT and Anger management 12 weekly sessions & fortnightly supervision

Control (n-60)

Recruitcounsellors

Post treatment assessments, reports on parental acrimony & child participation in custody decision making, interview with magistrates, lawyers & counsellors

Post treatment assessments

Monitor treatment fidelity

6 months post treatmentassessments, follow up interviews with practitioners and counselors, collection of acrimony and child custody data

6 months post treatment assessments, follow up interviews with magistrates, counselors, collection of parental acrimony and child participation in custody dataRecruit

counsellors

Parenting programme, CBT and Anger management 12 weekly sessions & fortnightly supervision

Randomize parents and children to either

Data analysis and report write up

Post treatment and reports on parental acrimony and child participation data

Figure 1. Conceptualizing child inclusive and child focused custody decision making in Zimbabwe.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•34

Page 36: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

observation from the study by Nyamapfeni contradictssome observations in stating that the extended family inZimbabwe cares for orphans and children affected byadversities (Chiedza Child Care, 2010; Bourdillon, 1993).

Even in the few cases of guardianship and adoption of dou-ble orphans that are presided over by the courts of law, thechildren’s voice is not heard.

Towards a child sensitive model of childcustody decision making

Consulting and informing children about what is happen-ing is not only necessary when children are the mostaffected, but provides a way of gauging how responsiblewe are as adults. Family crises provide us with an opportu-nity to demonstrate how much we care for children duringdifficult and trying times. In this context, another way ofgauging the best interests of the child is how well we createplatforms for their protection, by upholding the ethicalprinciple of confidentiality and anonymity when legal bat-tles occur. It appears that strategies adopted and adaptedby practitioners entrusted with the responsibility for solicit-ing views about separation or divorce and post livingarrangements in Zimbabwe (and in many other parts ofthe world) can be described as chaotic at best. This disarray

Figure 2a. Therapeutic letter by a 14 year old girl.

Figure 2b. sample therapeutic letter by a mother to her daughter.

Figure 2c. sample therapeutic letter by a father to his daughter.

Figure 3. River map by 13 year old boy showing his experience ofcustody determination in the High Court of Zimabwe.

Figure 4. River map by 8 year old girl.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•35

Page 37: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

is aggravated by the dearth of a model to accompany the setof rules for use by professionals working in public and pri-vate agencies dealing with separation and divorce. Theabsence of uniform procedures for interviewing children,along with the lack of accountability by interviewers, leadsto recurrent difficulties in child custody decision makingand custody determination in Zimbabwe (Chirawu, 2013).If a child sensitive procedure were in place, there wouldmost likely be a dramatic decline in the number of casescontested. This is because the procedure would not just bechild sensitive but also be child inclusive and child focused.Adopting a nationally recognized protocol for use whengauging children’s perspectives when parents separate canprovide a way of defining and refining both chidren’s andparents’ participation in custody decision making. Suchan approach would also benefit the professionals involvedin that it would restore integrity considering the adversarialtrap they inadvertently find themselves in.

Providing the ingredients and unveilingsteps towards the proposed child inclusiveand child focused custody decision makingmodel in Zimbabwe

The figures above detail the methodological overview thatI used in an attempt to gain an understanding of how chil-dren whose parents have divorced or separated have psy-chologically experienced participation in custody decisionmaking. This framework provides the sequencing of thevarious components of the study. The framework was alsoinformed by the need to design and implement an interven-tion for gauging (a) the thinking around child participationin custody decision making in Zimbabwe, (b) providingchildren and their parents with mechanisms for dialogue,and (c) for facilitating children’s participation in custodydecisions within the Zimbabwean context.

The above feat was achieved through a series of nineinterwoven studies in which each study was conducted

at three time periods over an 18 month period. The studiessought to explore how adults are prepared to give up powerto children when confronted by separation or divorce. Theadults’ views formed the perspectives on child participationin custody decision making while the reports of the childrenhighlighted children’s experiences with participation in cus-tody decision making in Zimbabwe. Study 1 used therapeu-tic letters, expressive writing and ‘‘message in a bottle’’ inwhich children wrote letters to their parents about the thingsthey were not happy about, recounting their fears andwishes regarding custody, parental communication, andpost-divorce or post-separation living arrangements.

The same techniques allowed the parents to read thechildren’s letters and to respond. Most parents cried whenthey read their children’s letters, apologized and startedfocusing their thoughts and actions towards their children’sneeds. Some parents also stopped the acrimonious way ofrelating to one another.

Study 2 used the river map in which the children drewtheir family as a river. The children were also asked toexplain what their rivers meant.

The results indicated different types of rivers. Some riv-ers were wavy and meandering, some had a lot of obstacles,such as crocodiles and snakes. In some of the rivers the chil-dren showed family members separated by the river.

In Study 3 the kinetic family drawing allowed the chil-dren to draw members of their family doing something andto provide a narration.

The children showed family members standing furtheraway; some fathers and mothers were missing. These wereemotional indicators of the way the separation or divorcehad affected the children. As the children drew and nar-rated what their drawings represented, they gave meaningto their experiences, predicament and hopes. Study 4 usedthe self portraits in which the children made a drawingthat represented how they viewed themselves. Studies 5and 6 were done with parents only to gauge their acrimonyand adversarial trap. Study 7 focused on the way profes-sionals (judges, magistrates, lawyers, psychologists, social

Figure 5. Kinetic family drawing by a 6-year-old boy showing parental fights.

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•36

Page 38: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

workers and counselors) viewed the role of children in cus-tody decision making. This study revealed that legal practi-tioners did not very much believe that children should havea say. In addition, the legal practitioners were not sureabout how to involve children. Study 8 involved providingthe legal practitioners with the letters that the children andtheir parents had written. Study 9 was an attempt to quan-tify the children’s emotional experiences of the currentmodel using randomized control trial study design. Study9 provides a comparison of the views, behavior and atti-tudes of parents and children regarding child participationin custody decision making in Zimbabwe.

Conclusion: Development of holistic adaptivecapacities

The several related studies (Study 1 to Study 9) provided aclearer, broader and in-depth understanding of children’sexperiences with parental separation or divorce. The find-ings of the studies also show that through expressive andparticipatory techniques such as kinetic family drawings,self-portraits, river maps, message in A bottle, children’scapacity to cope with parental separation was enhanced.It was through the process of being actively engaged inmatters that concern them that the children’s adaptivecapacities were developed and sustained. On another note,the parents were also aided in adapting to the separationand/or divorce without being entrapped in the acrimonyand adversarial pattern that usually punctuates the dis-course of separation and divorce. At an individual leveleach parent developed their adaptive capacity too.For legal and mental health practitioners, their ability toprovide child sensitive mediation was developed andenhanced. In the same vein they also had to adjust theirframes of operation in synch with the need to uphold chil-dren’s rights and the best interests of the child. The practi-tioners were also given an opportunity to adapt to the newchanges that the current way of thinking and acting ush-ered in at the conceptual, practical and professional levels.

This present submission provides an overview of the impetusfor child-sensitive, child inclusive and child focused custodydecision making in Zimbabwe.

References

Bourdillon, M. F. C. (1993). Where are the ancestors? Univer-sity of Zimbabwe Publications.

Chiedza Child Care (2010). Centre Beneficiaries, Studied inMbare, Sunningdale, and Ardbernie, Harare.

Chirawu, S. (2013). Longing for the wisdom of King Solo-mon: Custody and the best interests of the child concept.University of Zimbabwe. Law Student Journal, 1, 1-22.

Mutambara, L. (2005). Do custody laws protect the bestinterests of children in Zimbabwe in the light of interna-tional instruments? Unpublished Thesis submitted inpartial fulfillment of the Master of Women’s Law Degree,University of Zimbabwe.

Ncube, W. (1998a). Re-evaluating Law, Tradition, Customand Practice: Custody and Access to Non-marital Chil-dren in Zimbabwe. In Ncube, W. (Ed.), Law, Culture, Tra-dition and Children’s Rights in Eastern and Southern Africa,pp. 150-181.

Ncube, W. (1998b). Prospects and Challenges in Easternand Southern Africa: The Interplay between Interna-tional Human Rights Norms and Domestic Law, Tradi-tion and Culture. In Ncube, W. (Ed.), Law, culture,tradition and Children’s Rights in eastern and SouthernAfrica, pp. 1-10.

Nyamapfeni, P. (2012). Rethinking the general law of guar-dianship and adoption in line with the socio-economicrealities of double orphans in Zimbabwe. UnpublishedThesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the Master ofWomen’s Law Degree, University of Zimbabwe.

Ruombwa, V. (2004). Exploring the legal and social para-meters and joint parental responsibility of minor childrenin Zimbabwe, Unpublished Thesis submitted in partialfulfillment of the Master of Women’s Law Degree, Uni-versity of Zimbabwe.

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•37

Page 39: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

Notes from The PresidentNotes from The PresidentThis is my last ISSBD Bulletin note. My term as president ofISSBD will end at the Business and Awards Ceremony in Shang-hai in July 2014. I look back to almost four busy years in office,and think that we have accomplished quite a bit during this timeperiod. This was only possible because I have received strongand continuous support by the members of the ISSBD SteeringCommittee and the Executive Committee. I learned a lot aboutthe dynamics of the ISSBD management system and the chal-lenges you are confronted with when the task is to take the leadin a truly international organization. This experience was worth-while and certainly helped in widening my personal horizon.When I started the new job, Past President Anne Petersen wasparticularly helpful. She patiently and carefully introduced meinto all relevant topics and provided me with the basic know-ledge concerning past developments and the information aboutnecessary next steps in a broad variety of ISSBD ‘‘problemzones.’’ Later on, I benefited from the valuable support andadvice of President-elect Xinyin Chen and the Steering Commit-tee members Katariina Salmela-Aro and Ingrid Schoon. I am alsovery grateful to Kerry Barner from SAGE who supported ISSBDefforts in many respects, in particular, regarding membershipissues and the ISSBD flagship journal IJBD. Moreover, I appre-ciated the strong commitment of our EC members. It has beenreal fun to work with this stimulating international group of dis-tinguished scientists! Last not least, special thanks go to JacobsFoundation as a wonderfully supportive funder: I very muchappreciated the generous support and valuable assistance pro-vided by Gelgia Fetz and Simon Sommer through the years.

Although I said before that we have accomplished a greatdeal together during the past years, this is not true for all of thegoals that I had in mind when I started my presidency. One goalhigh on my priority list was to significantly increase the numberof ISSBD members Although Ann Sanson, chair of the Member-ship Committee, Xinyin Chen, Membership Secretary, KerryBarner and the members of the committee worked hard on thisproblem, success has only been modest. As I already told you inrecent notes, the number of ISSBD members increased consider-ably from about 300 to about 1000 between 1980 and 1990 but hasnot changed much during the past 25 years or so. Since 2010, ourISSBD membership committee has come up with several attrac-tive revisions and incentives regarding the membership feestructure, and we also recruited several new regional coordina-tors to improve the membership situation in countries where wewere not well represented in the past. We hope that these mea-sures will turn out to be successful in the long run, even thoughthe immediate effects are not yet visible.

It seems important to note in this regard that at least oneaspect of our recent efforts, namely, the recruitment of and sup-port for early career scholars, has been fairly successful. Thevarious initiatives to increase the attractiveness of the Societyfor young scholars first undertaken by Past Presidents RainerSilbereisen and Anne Petersen have paid off in the end. TheYoung Scholar Initiatives introduced and successfully estab-lished by Karina Weichold, Deepali Sharma, and Zena Melloat the Biennial Meetings in Melbourne and Wurzburg were fol-lowed by similar activities at the ISSBD meetings in Lusaka andEdmonton. My thanks go to Karina, Deepali, and the YoungScholar Representatives Zena Mello, Jaap Denissen, and JulieBowker. As a consequence of these efforts, the number of earlycareer scholars attending our Biennial Meetings has steadilyincreased.

It certainly helped that the EC established a new committeein 2010 which worked out a master plan regarding systematicsupport of young scientists in our Society. Past President Anne

Petersen and I came up with a proposal for a new ‘‘Jacobs-ISSBD Mentored Fellowship Program for Early Career Scholars’’in early 2011 that was eventually approved by Jacobs Founda-tion. This program contains a long-term support contract withJacobs Foundation and secures funding for several of ISSBD’searly career scholar activities, including travel grants for ISSBDpreconference workshops and the attendance of InternationalRegional Workshops. One special element of the new Jacobs-ISSBD contract is that it also includes two different Early CareerScholarship Programs, one open to applicants from all countriesin the world, the second focusing on early career scholars from‘‘currency restricted’’ countries. In a first cycle of this programwhich started in January, 2012, we recruited a first cohort of10 doctoral students who receive financial support and benefitfrom extended academic mentorship for a time period of 3 years.A second cohort of another 10 early career scholars will beselected at the end of this year. Thus this contract ensures thata total of 20 young scientists from all over the world are sup-ported by the Jacobs-ISSBD program over a 6-year period.

I am particularly grateful for the enormous support wereceived in this matter by the members of the EC and the Com-mittee on Research and Training of Young Scientists which waschaired first by Ulman Lindenberger and now by Toni Anto-nucci. Toni is currently making preparations for the recruitmentof the second cohort. The progress report she recently deliveredon the activities of the first cohort sounded very encouraging.I am fully convinced that this program will be a success story.Simon Sommer and Gelgia Fetz from Jacobs Foundation as wellas Ulman and Toni together with the committee membersdeserve our deep gratitude.

This is not the only early career activity that ISSBD supports.We started an ISSBD Developing Country Fellowship (DCF) ini-tiative in 2010 which supports research projects of early careerscholars from majority world countries for 2 years, includingtravel and accommodation support for ISSBD Biennial Meetings.A committee chaired by Peter Smith, who also proposed andmanaged the DCF program, so far has selected 10 early careerscholars out of about 60 candidates in three tranches of applica-tions. The fellowship not only provides financial support butalso ensures continuous advice by academic mentors. The expec-tation is that the 7 fellows of the last two tranches will presenttheir projects at the next biennial ISSBD meeting in Shanghai.Our special thanks go to Peter Smith and his dedicated commit-tee for their active engagement in this important task.

Another issue of major interest to most past presidents andalso to myself has been to build capacity for the study of humandevelopment in the developing world. In order to fulfill this partof its mission, ISSBD has always been active in organizingregional workshops and conferences. I fully agree with my pre-decessors that such workshops and conferences underpin theSociety’s most important functions. That is, they provide oppor-tunities for our members from currency-restricted countries aswell as parts of the world that are not well represented in ISSBDto become acquainted with recent trends concerning researchon behavioral development, and also to benefit from methodo-logical advances in the various disciplines represented in ourSociety. So far, these efforts have been very successful. Duringmy presidency, regional workshops were held in Chandigarh,India, in Lagos, Nigeria, Moscow, Russia, Budapest, Hungary,and Pretoria, South Africa. I attended most of these events andwas always impressed by the careful organization, the high qual-ity of scientific lectures, and last but not least, the motivation andcommitment of the participants. There is no doubt for me thatthese workshops have proved to be effective from a scientific

•38

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

Page 40: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

point of view, and that they also helped in increasing ISSBDmembership in the participating countries. At the ISSBD meetingin Edmonton, about 40% of the delegates belonged to this group.I hope that this trend will continue at our next ISSBD meeting inShanghai.

A third issue requiring a lot of effort and time for ISSBD pre-sidents concerns our Biennial Meetings, that is, our showcase ofinternational research on human development. My predecessorsworked hard to make our Biennial Meetings more appealing toall generations of scientists, offering interesting information forresearchers interested in all periods of life. This has always beenan important goal within the time period of my presidency. Inmy view, we managed to continuously increase the scientificstandard of our meetings, and the latest one in Edmonton,Canada, was no exception to this rule. My impression was thatall those who attended this meeting were impressed by thehigh-standard program with many highlights. Congratulationsand sincere thanks go to Nancy Galambos, Lisa Strohschein, JeffBisanz, and their team for all their hard work in organizing sucha great event.

I am confident that we will have another exciting BiennialMeeting in Shanghai, China, in July 2014. The conference chairBiao Sang and his team at East China Normal University, Shang-hai, together with the International Program Committee workedhard to make final selections for keynote speakers, invitedspeakers, and invited symposium organizers. Most of the devel-opmental scientists invited to present their work at the BiennialMeeting meanwhile accepted the invitation. Thus there is nodoubt for me that our ISSBD 2014 meeting in Shanghai willbecome a very attractive event. I hope to see many of you at theconference and also at ISSBD’s Business and Awards Ceremonymeeting, where you will learn more about recent progress andthe award winners.

Again, we are grateful to Jacobs Foundation for providingfinancial support (in terms of travel grants) for early career scho-lars who will attend the preconference workshops at the ISSBDmeeting in Shanghai. My special thanks go to the Early CareerScholar Travel Grant Committee chaired by Suman Vermawhich worked very effectively when dealing with numerousapplications for the 2014 ISSBD meeting. All early career scho-lars and developmental scientists from currency-restricted coun-tries whose abstracts were accepted were encouraged to applyfor travel grants at ISSBD and send proposals. Although weknow from previous rounds that not all applications will be suc-cessful, we hope to be able to support many of you.

A final issue that I pursued concerned the archiving of his-torically important developments within our Society. A largenumber of ISSBD documents are stored in the North HollandArchives in Haarlem, The Netherlands, unfortunately still inboxes. Marcel van Aken and I tried very hard during the pastthree years to change this suboptimal situation. Although ourplans to reactivate and reorganize these ISSBD documents werenot very successful for a long time, Marcel eventually managedto hire a librarian (paid by ISSBD) at the Archives to go throughthe files, categorize the materials, and also identify core docu-ments. The librarian delivered her first encouraging progressreport at the end of January, 2014, and there is hope that we canpresent a success story at the next ISSBD meeting in Shanghai.Given the fact that there are new electronic archiving options,it should be possible to have core documents illustrating the his-tory of ISSBD and its structural development digitalized and elec-tronically available to ISSBD members. For the EC members,particularly, it could be helpful to know about all actions andmotions described in the EC Minutes. We have already come upwith an overview of EC actions and decisions based on EC minutescovering the time period between 1992 and 2013. We hope to beable to extend and complement this document fairly soon.

ISSBD continues to be in good shape financially and other-wise, and certainly has promising perspectives. Through its veryactive program of conferences and workshops, the Society hasbecome an important player in the field of Developmental Sci-ence. ISSBD’s major journal, the International Journal of BehavioralDevelopment (IJBD), has developed flagship properties and canbe considered a very noteworthy publication outlet in the fieldof life-span and cross-cultural developmental science. Duringthe last decade, its editors Rainer Silbereisen, Bill Bukowski, andMarcel van Aken have managed to continuously increase thejournal’s impact factor, which deserves our deepest respect. Theterm of our IJBD editor Marcel von Aken ended at the end of2013. At our Executive Committee meeting in Seattle last year,we thanked Marcel and his team of associate editors as wellas the managing editor of IJBD for their great work and accom-plishments. From early 2014 on, Brett Laursen serves as thenew IJBD editor. Given his rich journal editing experience andhis special abilities in this regard, I am very confident that Brettand his new team of associate and managing editors will beequally successful and able to improve the journal’s qualityeven further.

The transfer of IJBD to the publisher SAGE has certainlycontributed to the positive development. We are particularlygrateful to Kerry Barner, Ed Mottram, and the other SAGE col-leagues for not only professionalizing our membership manage-ment but also for all the assistance they provided with regardto journal issues as well as the ISSBD website. Regarding the lat-ter, Ed Mottram just came up with a revised version that in myview looks much better than the previous one. I invite all mem-bers to visit the new ISSBD website. Thank you, Ed, for thisimpressive job!

As I already stated in previous notes, a similarly positivedevelopment can be reported for the ISSBD Bulletin, and weare very grateful to the editor-in-chief, Karina Weichold, the for-mer co-editor Bonnie Barber, and the current co-editor DeepaliSharma. I am convinced that the Bulletin will continue to bea valuable publication instrument stimulating the exchangeamong ISSBD members.

In addition, I want to thank our Social Media Editor JosafaCunha for his great efforts. He has managed to develop a num-ber of social media activities, including the ISSBD E-Newsletter,pages on Facebook and Twitter and also a video channel onYouTube. This channel continues to provide ISSBD memberswith updates on activities and events, especially for time sensi-tive information. Josafa is supported by several members of theExecutive Committee, and also by SAGE through Kerry Barnerand Ed Mottram.

As I noted at the beginning, my term as a president willend in July 2014, and the same is true for the terms of the Treas-urer, the Secretary General, the Membership Secretary, andthree EC members. We had ISSBD elections concerning thesepositions in the Fall of 2013. The new Secretary General willbe Karina Weichold. Nancy Galambos will serve as new Treas-urer, and Tina Malti was elected as new Membership Secretary.Our new Executive Committee members are Charissa Cheah,Marcel van Aken, and Sabine Walper. Congratulations to allof them!

I want to thank the members of the ISSBD Steering Commit-tee, Katariina Salmela-Aro and Ingrid Schoon, as well as the ECmembers Toni Antonucci, Elena Grigorenko, Ulman Lindenber-ger, and Ann Sanson, whose term will end in July, 2014. We defi-nitively owe them a lot!

Overall, I think that ISSBD is in very good shape and haspromising perspectives. Through its very active program of con-ferences and workshops, the Society has become an importantplayer in the field of Developmental Science. ISSBD provides arich resource for developmental scientists. ISSBD’s success is

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

•39

Page 41: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

mainly due to its active members and its hard-working execu-tive. I have had many pleasant and reinforcing experienceswhen communicating and working with the members of theSteering Committee, the EC, and all the other committees thatkeep ISSBD alive. I am very impressed with the good spiritshown by all members, and to them I would like to expressmy sincere gratitude. We have a terrific organization. Thanks a

lot to all of you for your enormous efforts. Working with you hasmade my presidency a great experience!

Wolfgang SchneiderUniversity of Wurzburg, Germany

E-Mail: [email protected]

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•40

Page 42: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

News

News from the IJBD EditorAnnouncing the New Editorial Teamfor the International Journalof Behavioral Development

Brett LaursenFlorida Atlantic University, USAE-mail: [email protected]

Editor-in-Chief

Brett Laursen (Florida Atlantic University)

Associate Editors

Patricio Cumsille (Pontificia Universidad Catolica de Chile)

Denis Gerstorf (Humboldt University Berlin)

Peter J. Marshall (Temple University)

Letitia R. Naigles (University of Connecticut – starts 1 July2014)

Olivier Pascalis (Universite Pierre Mendes France – until30 June 2014)

Melanie J. Zimmer-Gembeck (Griffith University)

Methods and Measures Editor

Todd D. Little (Texas Tech University)

Managing Editor

Donna Marion (Florida Atlantic University)

In 2012, IJBD had a two-year impact factor of 1.59 anda five-year impact factor of 2.10. Credit for these strongnumbers goes to the current Editor-in-Chief, Marcel vanAken, his editorial team, and our friends at SAGE, whohave worked tirelessly to raise the journal’s profile.

•41

2014 BULLETIN Number 1 Serial No. 65

Page 43: Number 1 Serial No. 65 · Number 1 Serial No. 65 Page Main Editor Karina Weichold ISSBD Bulletin Department of Developmental Psychology CADS – Center for Applied Developmental Science

MAJOR CONFERENCES OF INTERESTJuly 2–4, 20147th European Congress on Positive Psychology(ECPP 2014)Amsterdam, The NetherlandsWeb: www.enpp.eu/index.php/events/ecpp2014-in-amsterdam

July 8–12, 201423rd Biennial Meeting of the InternationalSociety for the Study of BehavioralDevelopment (ISSBD)Shanghai, ChinaWeb: www.issbd2014.com

July 8–13, 201428th International Congressof Applied PsychologyParis, FranceWeb: www.icap2014.com

July 14–16, 20149th International Conference on Child andAdolescent PsychopathologyLondon, United KingdomWeb: http://www.roehampton.ac.uk/Research-Centres/Centre-for-Applied-Research-and-Assessment-in-Child-and-Adolescent-Wellbeing/Child-and-Adolescent-Psychopathology-Conference/

July 15–19, 201422nd International Congress of theInternational Association for CrossCultural PsychologyReims, FranceWeb: www.iaccp2014.com

September 3–6, 201414th Biennial Conference of theEuropean Association for Research onAdolescence (EARA)Izmir, TurkeyWeb: www.eara2014.org/welcome

International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development

•42