the dissertation - full

99
DEPARTMENT OF SOCIAL SCIENCES Communication and Media Studies A dissertation in part fulfilment of the regulations governing the Degree The Language and Body Language of American Politicians: The Microanalysis of Political Communication Toby Pughe 1

Upload: toby-pughe

Post on 13-Apr-2017

70 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Dissertation - Full

DEPARTMENT OF SOCIAL SCIENCES

Communication and Media Studies

A dissertation in part fulfilment of the regulations governing the Degree of Bachelor of Science in Communication and Media Studies,

of Loughborough University

Faculty of Social Sciences March 2015and Humanities

The Language and Body Language of American Politicians: The Microanalysis of Political Communication

Toby Pughe

1

Page 2: The Dissertation - Full

Contents

CONTENTS 2

ABSTRACT 3

ILLUSTRATIONS 4

INTRODUCTION 6

THE DISSERTATION OUTLINE 7

LITERATURE REVIEW 8

ARISTOTLE 8

POLITICAL RHETORIC 9

CONTEMPORARY POLITICAL ORATION 11

METAPHORS IN POLITICAL DISCOURSE 13

PRESIDENTIAL RHETORIC 17

METHODOLOGY 22

ANALYSIS 27

POLITICAL MYTHS 27

CLAPTRAP 31

INVITED AND UNINVITED APPLAUSE 38

BODY LANGUAGE 43

CONCLUSION 60

APPENDICES 62

REFERENCES 66

2

Page 3: The Dissertation - Full

Abstract

How is it that politicians encourage audience applause and invite their

appreciation? How do they use gestures and non-verbal communication to aid

their ability to invite applause? Why do they do this, and what are the results of

their actions?

Questions such as this are posed and answered in my study on political speeches.

They were based on presidential nomination acceptance speeches at The

Democratic National Convention (1960 and 2008) and The Republican National

Convention (1980) in the United States of America. Both non-verbal and verbal

aspects are examined; these include the use of claptrap, audience applause,

audience interruption and hand gestures.

By using the technique of microanalysis, we have a better understanding of how

political orators use techniques to invite applause, as well as a greater awareness

of how and why uninvited applause occurs. We can evaluate more effectively

how and why political orators use non-verbal communication to add emphasis to

what they are saying and how they go about presenting themselves in the best

possible light. Political speeches are often dismissed as mere ‘claptrap’, but this

detailed microanalysis brings fresh ideas and visions into both the use of

claptrap and non-verbal communication in political speeches.

This analysis gives an interdisciplinary insight of politics, communication and

microanalysis. The findings will be suitable reading for students and academics

in the field of linguistics, communication studies, social psychology and political

science.

3

Page 4: The Dissertation - Full

Illustrations

Table 1 Details regarding Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) 22 and Obama’s (2008) speeches

Table 2 Atkinson’s (1984) claptrap techniques used to 26analyse Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama’s (2008) speeches

Table 3 Example of hand gestures noted 26

Table 4 Duration of speeches, claptraps used and audience 32applause details

Table 5 Atkinson’s (1984) claptrap techniques used in 32analysis of Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama’s (2008) speeches

Table 6 Shows when audience applause took place during 38 Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama’s (2008) Speeches

Table 7 The number of and type of hand gestures used in 56Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama’s (2008)speeches

Table 8 28 hand gestures used in Kennedy (1960), Reagan 57(1980) and Obama’s (2008) speeches

Table 9 What each hand gesture looks like and means 63

Figure A Crowd member, Kennedy’s (1960) speech 24

Figure B Aristotle’s (1806) human sphere 43

Figure C Aristotle (1806) hand gestures 43

Figure D Hand gestures in Obama’s (2008) speech 44

Figure E Hand gestures in Obama’s (2008) speech 46

Figure F Kennedy (1960) speech hand gestures 52

Figure G Kennedy (1960) assertive hand gesture 53

Figure H Kennedy (1960) sincere smile/pride 53

Figure I Reagan (1980) neutral hands 55

4

Page 5: The Dissertation - Full

Figure J Reagan (1980) small shoulder extension 55

Figure K Obama (2008) finger jab (political point) gesture 59

Figure L Obama (2008) left handed fast ball gesture 59

Figure M Obama (2008) crowd quieting gesture 59

Figure N Obama (2008) the claw hand gesture 59

Figure O Obama (2008) finger gun hand gesture 59

Figure P Kennedy (1960) self touch pacifier 59

Chart 1 Atkinson’s (1984) claptrap techniques used in 35analysis of Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama’s (2008) speeches

Chart 2 Number of external and internal attacks in 38Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama’s (2008)speech

Chart 3a When audience applause took place (Bull, 2003) 40in Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama’s speech

Chart 3b When audience applause took place (Bull, 2003) 42in Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama’s speechwith annotations

Extract 1 Reagan discussing the four previous years 33

Extract 2 Reagan creating the ‘monster’ 36

Extract 3 Obama ‘last, best, hope’ 45

Extract 4 Kennedy, we cannot afford such a luxury 50

Extract 5 Lincoln, Wilson and Roosevelt 53

Appendix A Obama vs. McCain and Obama vs. Romney results 62

Appendix B Table 9 63

Introduction

5

Page 6: The Dissertation - Full

The digital boom of television, smart phones and tablets has brought politicians

directly into our lives. Whether we choose this or not is unavoidable, with them

now becoming part of the background noise of everyday life. Progression in

technology has allowed politicians to share their views with the world more than

ever before. But with these improvements has also come aids to us, the audience,

allowing us to scrutinize politicians themselves, how they conduct themselves

and go about winning the hearts and votes of their given nation. Therefore, the

aim of this dissertation is to show what can be learned from political

communicative techniques by watching and listening to them in ways that the

everyday viewer is not familiar with. Where the everyday onlooker would

merely see a politician giving a speech, I am going to delve deeper and observe

each and every movement and word that American politicians use to encourage

their audience to perform their desired actions, which is to applaud.

With the aid of video recordings and transcriptions, the workings of claptrap

devices and non-verbal communication can be bought into view and provide

evidence for questions such as: how do some political speakers generate more

audience applause than others? How do political speakers use their bodily

movements and gestures along with their words to generate applause? Does

simply using ‘trick’ devices and non-verbal communicative techniques always

result in applause? How do they do it, how do they capture the minds of people

they don’t know and persuade them that they are the person who should be

leading the country? Observing political speeches has left wondering all of these

questions. It is obvious that the ability to capture the minds of strangers plays a

vital role in winning or losing an election, but what isn’t so obvious is how

political speakers can correctly differentiate between successful and

unsuccessful oratory. Students of politics are therefore deprived of any

noticeable clues as to what it is that political speakers have that strikes chords

with their audience and through my dissertation, I aim to educate the reader on

being able to spot these techniques and how political orators go about using

them successfully.

Since political speeches are widely available with relative ease, it gave me a

variety of speeches to choose from. The reason for choosing three American

6

Page 7: The Dissertation - Full

presidential candidates is because America is the most powerful country in the

world and those who take office immediately cement themselves as one of, if not

the, most powerful human beings on earth. Therefore, who better to analyse than

a group of the most powerful and influential people on earth. The simple desire

of knowing how these elite orators persuade audiences has led to the writing of

this dissertation.

The dissertation outline

My literature review is made up of five sections: Aristotle, political rhetoric,

contemporary political oratory, metaphors in political discourse and presidential

rhetoric. The first section, discusses the different elements involved in achieving

good persuasive oratory, such as the ethos, pathos and logos, according to

Aristotle. Secondly is political rhetoric, this section draws on research to form a

discussion of the best ways for political speakers to use rhetoric to capture the

attention of the audience, drawing on examples such as Petty and Cacioppo’s

(1981) elaboration likelihood theory. Then comes contemporary political

oration, this section discusses the reliability of the information discussed in

political rhetoric and contemporary political oratory and whether these same

methods can be applied to modern day political oration. This section also

touches on how modern day audiences are viewing politicians as ‘stars of

political show business’ (pg. 9) and whether we, as the audience, are now

holding the form of the orator in a higher regard than their content. The fourth

section is metaphors in political discourse and covers what metaphors are and

how politicians use them to tell the audience the ‘right’ story, for example the

American Dream. The final section of my literature review is, presidential

rhetoric. In this section I draw on authors who have written essays on previous

presidents such as Abraham Lincoln and Theodore Roosevelt. In these essays

they discuss the character of each president and look at how they were

successful in their use of rhetoric and what personality traits are suitable to be a

successful president of the United States.

Literature Review

7

Page 8: The Dissertation - Full

Firstly, this chapter will discuss Aristotle’s ideas surrounding oratory, the

different types and what is required to become the perfect orator. I will then go

onto discuss briefly, the history of rhetoric and its recent developments. Then I

shall discuss political rhetoric by drawing on research such as the Yale Studies of

Persuasion and Petty and Cacioppo’s elaboration likelihood model (1981).

Thirdly, I will discuss the reliability of these studies and whether they are still

applicable in modern day political oratory. I will also discuss audience’s

perception of politician’s character. Then comes metaphors in political discourse,

how and why politicians use metaphors during their speeches. And finally is

presidential rhetoric, which covers written essays about past presidents and

looks at why they were so successful.

Aristotle

Aristotle states there is three forms of oratory, deliberative/political oratory,

forensic oratory (used in the court of law) and epideictic oratory, which he

suggested was, comprised ‘of praise delivered at the funerals of notable figures’

(Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 222). Aristotle states that for differing types of

oratory, as seen above, different persuasive strategies should be adopted. Firstly,

the quality of a speech depends on the moral character of the speaker (ethos);

the second concerns the observers and putting them into a suitable frame of

mind (pathos), and finally the quality of the speech and whether it gives the

intended message (logos). If these are achieved to a high standard then,

according to Aristotle, the orator ‘persuades by moral character when his speech

is delivered in such a manner as to render him worthy of confidence’ (Freese,

1926, pg. 17). Although confidence is important, he suggests that the confidence

must come from the speech itself and not from a ‘preconceived view of the

speaker’s character’ (Freese, 1926, pg. 17), Aristotle is in disagreement about

this and states ‘moral character, so to say, constitutes the most effective means of

proof’ (Freese, 1926, pg. 17).

Until about 150 years ago, rhetoric was a specialty of Western education. The

discipline of rhetoric began to be replaced by psychology, sociology and

8

Page 9: The Dissertation - Full

linguistics. It wasn’t until the mid twentieth century that rhetoric returned to the

public domain with scholars such as Kenneth Burke, who was one of the first to

divert away from traditional rhetoric and view literature as symbolic action in

which human behaviour and language combines (Burke, 1962). The so-called

‘return’ of rhetoric has continued in several new directions, such as the creation

of discursive psychology in the 1990s, which is a form of discourse analysis that

focuses on psychological themes (Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003)

Political Rhetoric

Social psychological experiments have offered answers in the hope that

questions surrounding rhetoric are finally answered. Scientific evidence can

potentially determine when pathos is most effective and suggest how important

the ethos of a speaker is. These were the main goals of The Yale Studies of

Persuasion, in which Brewster Baxter has noted as ‘constituting the new

rhetoric’ (Smith, 1981, pg. xii). The ideas surrounding this research came from

World War ll, where Carl Hovland was hired by the Information and Education

division of the United States War Department to perform analysis and to come to

a conclusion on the quality of propaganda film. The department aimed to

‘persuade the troops that the war still might continue for a long time’ (Sears,

Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 223). The problem the department had, was they were

unsure whether both sides of the argument should be present in the pieces of

propaganda or whether the intended point of view should be communicated on

its own. After creating two films, Hovland found some interesting results

regarding one-sided and two sided messages, that were to be tested on viewers.

He concluded that the one sided message was less effective on people with a

better education. After the war Hovland et al (Hovland, Janis and Kelley, 1953;

Hovland, Lumsdaine and Sheffield, 1949; Hovland and Weiss, 1951) continued

intensive research during the 1950s at Yale University, although in these future

studies a number of the stimuli were not political, some of the studies continued

to surround themselves in political rhetoric (Sherif & Hovland, 1961)

The purpose of the Yale Studies of Persuasion was to find broad laws of

persuasion, and even though Hovland’s work on persuasive communication was

9

Page 10: The Dissertation - Full

influential ‘the program of results initiated by the Hovland group… produced

very few generalizable conclusions’ (Fishebein & Ajzen, 2010, pg. 339). To

support this claim, one study conducted by Hovland and Weiss (1951) wanted to

show that having a good ‘ethos’ (high credibility) increased the quality of

persuasion. However, research has found that when focusing on behavioural

change and not just attitude, a less credible source is more effective than a

source that has high credibility (Dholakia and Sternthall, 1977; Tybout 1978).

The main issue was their experimental research wasn’t constructed around

psychological theory of thinking, if it was, then these theories could be used to

understand the contrasting findings.

Important findings have been gathered by experimental attitude research, which

indicates that ‘the relationship between political message and audience can be

seen as a form of rhetorical dialogue in which the nature of argumentation is

crucial’ (Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 224). These findings include William

McGuire’s ‘inoculation theory’ (1964). This is similar to the Yale Studies in that

research ideas arose from practical issues. The issue here was the American

governments concern surrounding the capture of American troops in the Far

East, and potential brainwashing through intensive propaganda which would

lead to them supporting communism. The American government wanted the

development of psychological techniques to counteract the effect of propaganda.

McGuire hypothesized that the soldiers who were most susceptible to political

propaganda were those who’s ‘basic ideological beliefs had never been

challenged’ (Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 224). He and colleagues suggested

these soldiers were especially vulnerable because they ‘…exist in a kind of “germ

free” environment in which their validity is never challenged’ (Baumeister &

Bushman, 2010, pg. 294). Therefore, they concluded that soldiers should be

exposed to small doses of propaganda, which allows them to create counter

arguments intrinsically. McGuire’s research underlined the importance of

Aristotle’s notion of logos or rhetorical argument. Argumentation wasn’t just a

message given by someone but also ‘of recipients thought process’ (Sears,

Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 224). Therefore McGuire showed that the connection

10

Page 11: The Dissertation - Full

between a given message and a recipient is extremely rhetorical, built around

logos and anti-logos (Billig, 1987)

Further research that contributed to the movement of the Yale Studies, towards

a better rhetorical and psychological understanding, was Petty and Cacioppo’s

elaboration likelihood model (Petty & Cacioppo, 1981). Elaboration refers to

how motivated individuals are in thinking about the arguments presented in

persuasive communication. This model is very popular in understanding the

difference between persuasion by content and persuasion by form and focuses

on attitude change. The model states there are two ways to change attitude. First

of all is the central route, this route involves thoughtful elaboration processes.

Elaboration can be acknowledged by ‘evaluating the quality of arguments

presented in the persuasive communication’ (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2005, pg.

26). Petty and Cacioppo hypothesized that when individuals take an interest in a

topic and give it their full attention, then the strength of the argument (logos)

will influence their final behaviour. With the central route individuals are likely

to create counterarguments when the argument being presented to them is

weak, the central route requires a great amount of cognition (Petty & Cacioppo,

1984). When an individual is not interested in a topic then they are more likely

to be persuaded by the peripheral route. According to Petty and Cacioppo’s

model, this route is much shallower than the central route. The level of

persuasion involves presentation, the use of humour and the credibility of the

persuader. This being what Aristotle called ethos and pathos, where individuals

relate to the presenter as a person rather than the details of their message

(Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 225).

Contemporary Political Oration

In contemporary politics, it’s debatable whether the traditional discrepancies

between an orator’s ethos, pathos and logos can still be upheld in today’s

political environments, which in turn affects the applicability of Petty and

Cacioppo’s (1981) model. Kathleen Jamieson (1988) states the new digital age

11

Page 12: The Dissertation - Full

has severely changed the style of political oratory. It would seem that the

traditional style of political oratory, which included loud voices and powerful

gestures to a large audience, is no longer required with a more conversational

style being adopted. Political interview has, to some degree, always been a form

of conversation with turn taking (Bull et al., 1996), however with strict time slots

and the requirement to keep audiences engaged, the editing of political

interviews on television may give the impression that politicians answers are not

replies to questions (Ekström, 2001). Parliamentary debates can be used as an

example, there is turn taking, which is controlled by formal and informal codes of

practice (Carbó, 1997; Shaw, 2000). Furthermore the differences between

private and public life have become unclear (Meyrowitz, 1985). According to

Jamieson (1988), politicians are encouraged, and almost expected, to showcase

the ‘true’ them to the public. Castells (1999) named this expectation the

‘personalization of politics’.

It would be understandable to see politics in this light. Becoming trivialized, with

audiences viewing political figures as stars of political show business, who are

becoming increasingly influenced by peripheral characteristics rather than the

argument they are proposing (Postman, 1985). The arguments presented here

are suggesting that in an era of personalized politics, audiences are putting form

over content, regarding the peripheral influence of ethos and pathos over the

central factors of logos (Postman, 1985). Nevertheless, Petty and Cacioppo’s

elaboration likelihood model does have its flaws and other interpretations have

raised doubt about its applicability to politics. Nowadays the difference in

mainstream political parties ideologies and beliefs are often very small, which is

a reason as to why personalization of political orators is occurring. Voters want

to vote for a leader who they believe they can trust and one whom will have the

ability to rise above hysteria in the face of crisis. In the 1992 presidential

campaign, Simons (1996) analysed the Gore-Perot debates and stated it was very

important for both speakers ‘to appear reasoned and knowledgeable on an issue

like North American Free Trade Agreement’ (Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 226)

because it would be unlikely that the audience had read the 12,000 page

document before congress. After the speech Perot was seen to come out of it

12

Page 13: The Dissertation - Full

much worse than Gore because of his piercing tone and his personal attacks on

Gore. In a speech where extremely complex political issues were being discussed,

it shows that ethos, much to the contrary of Aristotle and Petty and Cacioppo’s

ideas, is a central issue to viewers.

In addition, an alternate interpretation could be that visual imagery is ‘the

semantic and technical unit of the modern mass media at the heart of the post-

war popular culture’ (Evans & Hall, 1999, pg. 2). Jamieson (1988) stated that

voters have now gained knowledge of how to interpret a politician’s character

from close up shots of televised political speeches. This idea shows us the

significance of what Roland Barthes (1977) called the ‘rhetoric of the image’ that

hasn’t been covered in the verbal form of traditional rhetoric (see Barthes and

Lavers, 1972). In a situation where the visual imagery of politicians is as

important, if not more so, than their words, then their credibility is not a

peripheral issue that might only ‘…influence those uninterested in politics’

(Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 226). As an alternative it may be something that

voters are specifically looking at and politicians are now aware of this. If this is

the case, then the customary difference between content and form which makes

up the foundation of Petty and Cacioppo’s model may not be suitable for rhetoric

in present day politics. It is not to say that this research ignores the impacts of

visual rhetoric, but that the suggested differences between peripheral and

central routes of persuasion may not be enough to cater for the complicated

verbal and visual rhetoric in modern day politics.

Metaphors in Political Discourse

In political discourse, a technique commonly used by orators is the use of

metaphors. They can be used for ideological purposes, as ‘it advocates

unconscious emotional associations and thereby contributes to myth creation:

politicians use metaphor to tell the right story’ (Charteris-Black, 2005, pg. 28).

Rhetorical metaphors play a part in how political issues are represented in the

minds of the audience, which makes the issues being presented more difficult to

understand. Looking at how metaphors impact political speeches enables us to

13

Page 14: The Dissertation - Full

understand how they contribute towards our knowledge of political rhetoric and

how politicians use persuasive language to communicate worldviews. According

to Aristotle, a metaphor is ‘giving the thing a name that belongs to something

else’ (Charteris-Black, 2005, pg. 31). The idea of metaphor is that meanings are

transferred from and to (Charteris-Black, 2005). I will interpret the term

metaphor as a word or phrase that is used with the sense that is different from

another common sense of that word or phrase. When we are using a metaphor

but are referring to the common sense, literal meaning understanding of the

word then the term ‘source domain’ is used. When we are using the metaphorical

sense of the word, this is referred to as the ‘target domain’ of the metaphor

(Charteris-Black, 2005, pg. 31). Therefore from this we can define a metaphor, as

a change in the use of a word to give it new meaning. It is the adaptation of

meaning that allows metaphors to conjure up emotional responses and since the

terms ‘emotion’ and ‘motion’ come from the same source, we can appreciate

metaphors as the carriers of meaning.

Audiences usually know what the literal meaning or the common sense meaning

of a word is. Metaphors come from audiences’ expectations about their existing

knowledge of how words are usually used. So a metaphor is a phrase or word

that changes from the literal, common sense understanding to another sense that

is different from the typical use (Charteris-Black, 2005, pg. 31). For that reason,

metaphor only becomes apparent with discourse knowledge. The use of the

common senses of words can be understood differently between individuals

according to their experience, education and understanding of English. What is

unusual for one may be well known for another, and these understandings can

be down to social influences. An individual may not immediately understand that

a word has been used metaphorically, since they may not recognize the use of

the common sense word in a different way to its basic form. However, if they

understand more about the original sense of the word then they may accept the

word as a metaphor. If individuals don’t have this understanding then political

metaphors will often be missed. It’s harder to understand and immediately

recognize the use of a metaphor. For example, when a politician uses the term

‘milestone’, an audience who know the literal use of the word ‘milestone’ may

14

Page 15: The Dissertation - Full

consider it a metaphor, but those who only ever hear the term used in political

speeches may think it isn’t a metaphor (Charteris-Black, 2005). This is why

certain audiences pick up on metaphors and others do not.

With regards to political rhetoric, metaphors are used to ‘frame how we view or

understand political issues by eliminating alternative points of view’ (Charteris-

Black, 2005, pg. 32). Metaphors are used by politicians in negative situations that

are seen as troublesome and positive situations that look towards future

scenarios that are created as solutions to negative problems. Metaphors are also

used for the negative representation of the out-group (e.g. opponents) and

positive representations of the in-group (e.g. supporters). Chilton (2004) gives

an outline of the politician’s motivation in discourse:

‘… political discourse involves, among other things, the promotion of

representations, and a pervasive feature of representation is the evident

need for political speakers to imbue their utterances with evidence,

authority and truth, a process that we shall refer to in broad terms, in the

context of political discourse, as ‘legitimization’. Political speakers have

the guard against the operation of their audience’s cheater detectors and

provide guarantees for the truth of their sayings.’ (Chilton, 2004, pg. 23)

In much agreement with Aristotle’s ideology of ethos and pathos, the use of

metaphor in political speeches is to prove the speaker as a rightful source of

authority by ‘sounding right’. ‘Sounding right’ can also involve criticizing

opponents (out-groups) behaviour and their policies verbally, Chilton (2004)

again explains this further:

‘Delegitimization can manifest itself in acts of negative other presentation,

acts of blaming, scapegoating, marginalizing, excluding attacking the

moral character of some individual or group, attacking the

communicative cooperation of the other, attacking the rationality and

sanity of the other. The extreme is to deny the humanness of the other’

(Chilton, 2004, pg. 47)

15

Page 16: The Dissertation - Full

For most people, a lot of political issues are complex and difficult to understand.

Therefore, it’s important that political audiences are presented with image-based

metaphors to aid their understanding on abstract topics. This ensures the

message becomes better defined. Explained further by Mio (1997), ‘because of

information-processing demands, people cannot pay attention to all aspects of

political evidence. Therefore, something is needed to simplify decision making,

and metaphor and other shortcut devices (e.g. cognitive heuristics) address this

need’ (Mio, 1997, pg. 130). A metaphor like ‘changes in the wind’ is more

understandable than describing the whole concept of ‘decolonization’. The aims

are therefore to simplify potentially confusing, abstract issues by encouraging

audience’s pre-existing knowledge to help them understand.

The metaphors that politicians tend to use the most are between contemporary

and familiar uses of language. Politicians are unlikely to have the language

capabilities of a poet so tend to apply conventional metaphors such as ‘the road

to victory’ (Charteris-Black, 2005, pg. 34). In a study by Charteris-Black (2009),

he compared male and female politicians use of metaphors, and concluded that

males used more conventional metaphors than females. He attributed this to

male politicians having a longer experience of using political discourse

(Charteris-Black, 2009). He did however find that occasionally they spoke of ‘an

iron curtain descending across Europe’ (Churchill, 1946), ‘the winds of change

(Macmillan, 1960) or ‘a river of blood’ (Powell, 1968). At first these expressions

were seen as creative, but they eventually became traditionalized to become a

fast way of talking about the Cold War, decolonization and immigration anxieties.

Nevertheless, according to Charteris-Black (2009) this will happen on numerous

levels, for example comparing ‘the Iron Curtain’ and the ‘Iron Lady’. The first

became popular among the mainstream significantly quicker in contrast to the

‘Iron Lady’ that kept its status as an original metaphor for a longer period of

time. This may be down to the fact that it’s less likely a woman is made of metal,

than curtains that are made of metal (Musolff & Zinken, 2009)

16

Page 17: The Dissertation - Full

Depending on what certain words mean in their literal term is a deciding factor

in whether they will be used metaphorically in different cultures. For example

the use of ‘fight’ and ‘battle’ hold negative connotations in many cultures and

therefore they avoid metaphoric use. In Asia the term ‘Iron Curtain’ was replaced

with the “Bamboo Curtain’ when referring to the boundaries between non-

communist and communist China, because in Asia bamboo is more of an

everyday occurrence than iron (Charteris-Black, 2005, pg. 34). If audiences are

to be persuaded, it’s important they know the difference between the common

sense of words/phrases and the unfamiliar use of a word/phrase as a metaphor,

otherwise metaphors will only be understood by linguists who know the dual

purpose of the word. When other ways of talking are replaced with a metaphor,

language has impacted the world by settling contrasting ways of thinking, and by

doing this ‘aids our understanding of the political world’ (Charteris-Black, 2005,

pg. 35).

Presidential Rhetoric

Rhetoric is something that is a key component to leadership. Political leaders do

not simply command commitment from individuals and encourage them to

follow a direct action simply because of their status in a political group. Neither

do they have special abilities in persuading people ‘… that is beyond human ken’

(Dorsey, 2002, pg. 252). Leaders must participate in the method of investigation

that will allow them to find the most suitable option for their audience, and use

this knowledge to construct a message that would allow the audience to

associate themselves with that alternative. At the heart of presidential rhetorical

leadership is prudence. In one sense, presidents should be rational and level

headed when applying their knowledge to the future goals and policies of their

country. On the other hand, presidents must have a noble character to ensure

that the decisions they make are not done so without careful consideration to

ethical sense, as well as the technical ones. The most important, however, is the

president must possess the skill set to ‘express these inner virtues of thought and

character as well as the awareness of how to balance them in a public

performance’ (Dorsey, 2002, pg. 253). The performative element of a president’s

17

Page 18: The Dissertation - Full

discretion shouldn’t be seen as them ‘acting’ in the negative sense, but rather it

means that they must capture the audience to underline their understanding of

the connection between people, opportunities, future goals and other

occurrences. This needs to be done before the audience would be prepared to

take on board the presidents argument and follow them in pursuing a common

goal.

The best examples of rhetorical leadership by presidents are shown in the

moments where they are seen to be able to balance their decision-making skills

and their intentions of good will (Dorsey, 2002). In Zarefsky’s (2002) essay he

concludes that the most successful presidents in history showed their astuteness

and personal character by taking serious political moments and transforming

them into some of the most memorable speeches of all time, that brought nations

together and united them as one. In his essay about the first US president George

Washington, Lucas (2002) describes his ability to communicate his humble

character, his bravery and use his knowledge of historical events to lead the

nation forward. According to H. W. Brands in ‘The Last Romantic’, Theodore

Roosevelt was able to control the public’s perception of him, as his rugged

character added to the credibility of his underlined intentions, which as a result

caused his often-uncompromising opponents to reconsider their positions (H. W.

Brands, 2002). James R. Andrews and J. Michael Hogan write that the legacy of

Woodrow Wilson’s rhetorical leadership and strong character came from his

ability to express his vision of American citizens to become part of a morally

responsible nation (Andrews & Hogan, 2002). In his critical essay on Franklin

Roosevelt’s last speech, Thomas Benson suggests that the president positively

encouraged his beliefs about the New Deal by showcasing his level-headedness

and his kindness (Benson, 2002). G. Thomas Goodnight states, that using the

American Dream but with some reconsideration, Ronald Reagan was able to

show his morality and to encourage the audience by showcasing his

trustworthiness on the world stage (Weilder, Barnett & Pearce, 1992). In his

essay, John Murphy demonstrates how Bill Clinton used ‘cunning’ as a way of

survival. This was along with his ability to take on the character and roles of

18

Page 19: The Dissertation - Full

admired past presidents and to evoke feelings of shared emotion or belief among

audiences that made them more open to his leadership (Dorsey, 2002).

It would be foolish to believe that presidents always succeed in positive

rhetorical leadership. If a president cannot balance their practical knowledge

and their personal character, then it is unlikely they can persuade others. In

order to understand rhetorical leadership further, it is important to give these

instances some attention.

According to Farrell, John Adams was concerned about the public’s perception of

him as president, especially when it came to the relations between the

Americans and the French (Dorsey, 2002). Adams was so concerned about his

public perception that he wrote essays and letters whilst in office, to ensure the

public perception of him would be positive and he was viewed as a ‘… Ciceronian

ideal of a prudent leader’ (Dorsey, 2002, pg. 254). This raises the question that if

Adams felt it was important to defend his time in office to the degree he did, was

his standard of rhetorical leadership sub-par? Adams own recollection of his

presidency made him ‘certain he had done the right thing, whether or not we, as

his posterity, would be prepared to recognize his prudence’ (Dorsey, 2002, pg.

254) and Farrell therefore implied that his performances were indeed weak.

From Farrell’s statements they show a detachment between the audience’s

judgment of his performance and Adam’s performance itself. In other words, the

performance of an orator can only be so good when they feel compelled to tell

the audience of its merit. Therefore it’s understandable that Adam’s oratory

skills were seen as self-serving and not of aid to the situation. Farrell notes, ‘for

Adams, fame remained a powerful motive, and drove him always to conduct

himself with an eye toward posterity and the historical record’ (Dorsey, 2002, pg.

254). Therefore the lesson here for successful rhetorical leadership is perhaps

that leaders should be cautious in situations that require them to be so.

Regarding his essay on Thomas Jefferson, Lawrence Kaplan clearly shows how

Jefferson failed in his attempt for successful rhetorical leadership. Undeniably,

Kaplan does state that external forces that affected his rhetorical ability were not

19

Page 20: The Dissertation - Full

always under his control. Lloyd Bitzer called these situations needs that cannot

be modified (Bitzer, 1968). These forces did however help Jefferson as well as

obstruct him. But Kaplan states that Jefferson’s performances weren’t good

enough in situations that were open to his influence (Kaplan, 2002), this was

down to a combination of his pride getting in the way of his rational decision

making ability and his undervalued attention of Napoleon Bonaparte’s deceitful

plans. For example, Cicero (1951) says a leader ‘anticipates the future, calculates

the chances for good or for evil, decides how to meet every contingency, and is

never reduced to the necessity of saying: ‘this is not what I expected’ (Cicero,

1951, pg. 32) and in the case of Thomas Jefferson these challenges were not met.

In contrast to Jefferson, and detailed by Fred Greenstein and Meena Bose (2006)

in their essay about Dwight Eisenhower’s presidency, Eisenhower was an

example of successful rhetorical leadership. Unlike Jefferson, Eisenhower was

able to adapt to the situation and prospered in the limelight of political arenas.

But Greenstein and Bose did spot that Eisenhower didn’t perform in his usual

manner when confronted with the missile-gap controversy and they concluded

that his behaviour appeared to neglect the American citizens anxiety

surrounding the Soviet Union launch of Sputnik (Greenstein & Berman, 2006).

During the time Eisenhower should have been showing empathy to citizens of

America he appeared to be unaware of their concerns. Greenstein & Berman

(2006) suggested that in the testing times, Eisenhower should have revealed his

plans for the future that would have settled the nerves of the public. As a result

of Eisenhower not taking into consideration the concerns of others, and simply

focusing on his own sense of rationality, he missed the chance to showcase

effective ‘… prudential leadership’ (Fishman, 2001, pg. 48).

From the points covered, it demonstrates that during the course of leadership

presidents need to consider a number of variables if they want to be successful.

Successful oratory calls for leaders who are able to act rationally in a given

situation but also ethically, while also being able to show their personal

character and their balance of reason. In successful examples of persuasion, and

those not so, the important lesson behind good judgment and successful

20

Page 21: The Dissertation - Full

rhetorical leadership is a speaker’s ability to adapt their style to the moment. It’s

the opportunities that political speakers seize and those that they do not. If a

president wants to be a successful rhetorical leader then it is vital they are able

to distinguish between the two.

21

Page 22: The Dissertation - Full

Methodology

The findings in this dissertation are concerned with how American politicians

use rhetoric, specifically political myth and claptrap, and non-verbal

communication to encourage audience applause. The sample used is three

presidential nomination acceptance speeches at either the Democratic National

Convention or the Republican National Convention. Analysis on applaudable

message techniques such as three part lists and contrastive pairs have taken

inspiration from Atkinson’s (1984) work. Also, analysis on when the audience

chose to applaud is an extension of Bull’s (2003) work on invited and uninvited

applause. The three speeches chosen for analysis can be seen below:

Table 1: shows detail regarding the speeches

The reason I have chosen to analyse presidential nomination acceptance

speeches is because I wanted to study a speech that was important to the future

of the speaker. In other words, a speech where they had to perform a high

standard or risk being questioned whether they should have been nominated for

the presidency. The reason I have chosen these three presidents specifically is

because they are three of the most recognizable names to take office in recent

time. Also, because they are a similar distance away from each other in terms of

years (Kennedy - Reagan 20 years, Reagan - Obama 28 years), which enables me

to measure any changes or developments in the techniques, I have chosen to

analyse over time.

A rhetorical analysis was used for the three chosen speeches. Rhetorical analysis

examines ‘content to understand the nature of persuasion and argumentation’

(Treadwell, 2014, pg. 252) and is used here with the aim of ‘examining

22

When did it take place?

Where did it take place?

Speech duration

John F. Kennedy

1960 Democratic National Convention

22 minutes 26 seconds

Ronald Reagan

1980 Republican National Convention

42 minutes 23 seconds

Barack Obama

2008 Democratic National Convention

44 minutes 58 seconds

Page 23: The Dissertation - Full

communication content to identify and assess its persuasive strategies’

(Treadwell 2014, pg. 234) in the three speeches outlined. The goal of a

rhetorical analysis is to understand both what a speaker says, and how they say

it. To do this, speeches will be analysed for the strategies used to achieve the goal

of being a persuasive speaker. Rhetorical analysis will allow me to study not just

rhetoric as a whole, but specifically the techniques underlined by Atkinson

(1984) that play a critical role in how orators invite applause. Also, rhetorical

analysis will aid me in analysing body language and how American politicians

use this to enhance their persuasiveness. Below are details regarding rhetoric as

a whole, claptrap and the analysis of body language.

Rhetoric is the parallel to dialectic. Everybody uses both to discuss and support

their ideas, to stand for their point and to condemn others. Every person does

this whether it be unintentional or by rehearsal, some with more success than

others. Rhetoric has multiple meanings. It can allude to not only the ways in

which a speaker tries to persuade audiences but also the scholastic ‘study of

oratorical persuasion’ (Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 222). Rhetoric originated

in ancient Greece, Sophists believed the teaching of rhetoric would ‘equip pupils

with the skills of persuasion’ (Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 222). Aristotle

disagreed in that he believed rhetoric should be approached in an analytical

spirit and not as a practical discipline, he claimed rhetorica was ‘not to persuade,

but to discover the available means of persuasion in each case’ (Benson and

Prosser, 1988, pg. 56). The work done by Aristotle on rhetoric was the

foundation of European education from the Roman times to the late 19th century

(Matsen, Rollinson & Sousa, 1990).

Claptrap works in a way similar to ‘hip, hip, hooray!’. This phrase is used to

encourage a group of people to perform the same action at once. It’s clear when

it comes to political speeches it would be far too obvious for orators to say hip,

hip, hooray after they have said something that warrants approval. Therefore,

they use more discrete techniques that are often just as reliable, yet less

identifiable. The technical term for this technique is claptrap. Claptrap is defined

as a ‘trick, device or language designed to catch applause’ (Atkinson, 1984, pg.

23

Page 24: The Dissertation - Full

48). It is important to note that claptrap is not ‘one thing’ as such; it is made up of

many things such as hand movements, posture, voice projection and many more.

Orators have to treat their audience like a choir of which they are the conductor.

Table 2 (see below) is copied from my claptrap chapter and shows an example of

the claptrap techniques I have chosen to look for in each of the political speeches.

Audience applause is measured by the starting point of applause until the same

segment of applause has come to an end.

The use of body language cues alongside words

can heavily influence the quality of delivery.

Politicians are well known for their use of hand

gestures, therefore to give myself a wide range of

resources I deemed political speeches appropriate

for my analysis. Previous research such as Bull and

Connelly (1985) focused their attention on

conversations that used students in laboratory

settings. Therefore, the aim of my analysis was to conduct similar research to

theirs, but in a naturalistic setting which gave me the chance to see whether

hand gestures are used more often than in a strict controlled environment.

Gestures are mainly seen during the speech itself but they are also ‘synchronized

with linguistic units’ (Bull, 2003, pg. 27). Gesture and speech work alongside one

another in creating meaning, for example, gestures can clarify the meaning of

speech and speech can clarify the meaning of gesture (Kelly et al., 1999). In other

words, gesture shouldn’t be viewed as an alternative to speech, but as an

additional aid, which is just one part of a ‘multichannel system of

communication’ (Bull, 2003, pg. 27). The cohesion of gesture and speech

provides skilled speakers with further routes to convey meaning to their

audience. Figure A is a screenshot from Kennedy’s speech, and shows how

important not only orator’s words are, but also their body language, dress and

posture.

The table below is just a few of the hand gestures I have noted in full detail

within the body language chapter. The gestures noted in this table, and

Figure A(Kennedy, 1960)

24

Page 25: The Dissertation - Full

throughout the analysis, are regarded as a single act, for example a single finger

point is counted as one gesture. If in the same pointing gesture there is a

stabbing motion where five smaller points are made, this is counted as five more

gestures. Every time a gesture changes in appearance it is counted as a new

gesture. Bilateral movements are counted as one gesture unless each arm is

performing a different motion, if a bilateral movement is conducted with, for

example, a right finger point and a left hand open palm chop, this would be

classed as two separate gestures. Gestures can also fit more than one category at

once, for example a left-handed brush away with an open palm, a gesture to

quieten the audience with an open palm and a left handed multiple finger point

with a left handed chopping motion.

25

Page 26: The Dissertation - Full

Hand gesture John F. Kennedy Ronald Reagan Barack ObamaLeft hand jab 0 0 25 (2%)Left finger point 1 (0.3%) 0 250 (20.%)Right hand jab 137 (40.9%) 0 70 (5.7%)Right finger point 34 (10.1%) 0 101 (8.3%)Two handed point 0 0 3 (0.2%)Two handed spread 4 (1.2%) 5 (50%) 175 (14.5%)

President List of three

Projecting a name

Contrastive pairs

Reference to “us” “we”

“ourselves”

Negative towards

opposition

Rhetorical questions

John F. Kennedy

15 18 4 48 18 3

Ronald Reagan

27 11 2 166 56 14

Barack Obama

23 23 2 77 34 4

26

Page 27: The Dissertation - Full

Table 3: an example of some of the hand gestures that have been identified and tallied for each of the orators during their speech

27

Page 28: The Dissertation - Full

Analysis

Political Myths

A main goal of using a metaphor is to play a part in evolving political myths. A

value of the metaphor is that since it isn’t specific or exact in what is being talked

about, it leaves an open interpretation and alongside other persuasive

techniques, gives the listener the opportunity to create their own meanings. This

idea can be suitable applied to the myth of the ‘American Dream’:

‘I came to this hallowed chamber two years ago on a mission:

to restore the American dream for all our people and to make

sure that we move into the 21st century still the strongest force

for freedom and democracy in the entire world’

(Bill Clinton, 24th January 1995)

‘This is our time-to put our people back to work and open doors

of opportunity for our kids; to restore prosperity and promote

the cause of peace; to reclaim the American dream and reaffirm

that fundamental truth-that out of many, we are one, that while

we breathe, we hope, and where we are met with cynicism

and doubt,And those who tell us that we can’t, we will respond

with that timelessCreed that sums up the spirit of a people:

yes, we can.’

(Barack Obama, 15th June 2008)

Like all myths, the American Dream is a story that has been told and lived by

people throughout history, and therefore there is a great level of inevitability.

Positivity is associated with the myth, but the hopes of a nation are different

depending on individuals themselves, and groups they belong to. ‘Dream’ is used

metaphorically and is a flexible concept that can be applied to anyone regardless

of his or her aspirations. The narrative of the American dream is based on the

apparent ‘…golden age of the past’ (Charteris-Black, 2005, pg. 39). This is

28

Page 29: The Dissertation - Full

supported by elements of the passage above, the use of ‘restore’ and ‘acclaim’

give the impression that ‘we’ the people can get something back and be the next

person to reclaim glory. The myth of the American Dream is in the minds of

many American's to this day, who have moved to North America to enhance their

lives, whether it be financially or to prevent further suffering, however, it cannot

be applied to the wider population within America, as there are those who

haven’t achieved the ‘American Dream’ and are living their lives far from what

they would want (Charteris-Black, 2005). Nevertheless, since how flexible the

‘dream’ metaphor is it can be applied to individual hope or social hope and this

flexibility is what allows it to be exploited easily in political debate. This

versatility is what underlines what has been described as a logico-rhetorical

module (Sperber, 2001). The explanation of a ‘dream’ in a personal context is

very much a right wing republican myth, whereas the concept of a dream being

social is a democratic, left wing myth, as a result of a dream being either is why

the logico-rhetorical module is activated. Political arguments have arisen as a

result of differing views, as to who has access to the American Dream. After the

Vietnam War, it’s claimed by the Baby Boom generation that everybody has

access to the American Dream, which differs to followers of an American global

hegemony, which stems from capitalism who hold the view that the American

Dream ‘…had to be earned’ (Charteris-Black, 2005, pg. 40) and therefore

suggested that it could only be achieved by those who work hard. Something that

both these narratives share is that while the dream is seen in the future, they

indicate that the experience is in the present. Rhetorically the influence of

metaphors originates in the meanings created by the original sense of the word,

the literal meaning. ‘Dream’ often means ‘hope’ and when it does, positive

connotations are felt that are not completely clear in the ordinary sense of

‘dream’, as can be seen in Obama’s use of the word on page 36 and 37.

Nonetheless, the optimism placed on the American Dream and the positive links

to hope and success, surround the word ‘dream’ which shows us the potential

the metaphor can have in mythic narrative.

Although the American Dream is different for each person, as was showed by

Stud Terkel who spent years travelling around the United States finding out what

29

Page 30: The Dissertation - Full

people’s interpretation of the American Dream was (Terkel, 1980). From his

findings it appeared that explanations share certain foundations. The American

Dream looks towards the future since the metaphor refers to situations that

don’t exist yet and couples personal ambition, with social aspiration. The

American Dream is the belief that one’s future life will be better than it currently

is and has been. The myth of the American Dream is, and has been used

frequently by current commander in chief Barack Obama, and it shouldn’t be a

surprise that his first published book in 2004 had ‘dream’ in the title, ‘Barack

Obama: Dreams from my Father’ (Obama, 2004). Before this book was released

however, Obama touched upon the American Dream in his civil rights speech on

October 25th 2005 in which he said:

‘It’s a simple dream, but it speaks to us so powerfully because

it is our dream – one that exists at the very centre of the American

experience. One that says if you’re willing to work hard and take

responsibility, then you’ll have the chance to reach for something

else; for something better’

(Obama, 25th October 2005)

The central ideologies of the American Dream are if individual effort is shown,

then rewards will follow. During this speech the word ‘dream’ is used 35 times,

and was repeatedly used as a synonym of ‘hope’. The use of ‘dream’ was also

used along with other metaphors that were often based on the values of the

nation being a family:

‘It is that promise that has always set this country apart, that

through hard work and sacrifice, each of us can pursue our

individual dreams but still come together as one American Family,

to ensure that the next generation can pursue their dreams as well’

(Obama, 28th August 2008)

Here Obama shows no struggle between the individual aspiration and the

achievement of society. If we look at several presidents/political leaders

30

Page 31: The Dissertation - Full

throughout history, lots of them used myths to empower their speeches, for

example Ronald Reagan and his myth ‘From the swamp to the stars’. But as

Fairclough (1992) has noted, metaphors can also be used during ‘politics as a

war’, and found that in the British general election discourse ‘naturalized’ how

the audience think about and understand politics. Looking at Austrian political

discourse, De Cillia, Reisigl and Wodak (1999) found that the use of metonyms

such as ‘the foreigner’ could reduce individualism and therefore approving ‘…

nationalist generalizations’ (Sears, Huddy, Jervis, 2003, pg. 228). Although it

should be noted that not all political metaphors in discourse have such a

traditional effect. Chilton and Ilyin (1993) looked at the roots of metaphor when

used to describe Europe as a ‘common house’, showing how it worked between

the western and Russian political discourse, they suggested that new metaphors

‘can break up the rigid conceptual frames of an existing political order’ (Chilton

& Ilyin, 1993, pg. 10).

31

Page 32: The Dissertation - Full

Claptrap

This chapter will look at the use of claptrap devices in Kennedy (1960), Reagan

(1980) and Obama’s (2008) presidential nomination acceptance speeches

through techniques underline by Atkinson (1984), and how they use these

techniques to encourage audience applause. In this chapter I have purposely

analysed Reagan’s speech more than Kennedy and Obama’s. This is because

Kennedy and Obama have very prominent displays of non-verbal communication

and it will therefore be beneficial to my analysis if they make up the majority of

the body language chapter.

When the audience observe and understand when an insult to another party or a

boast about their own has occurred, they can prepare themselves to applaud at

the completion of the orators point. For the audience to understand when to

applaud the speaker must make it obvious when they have entered the final

stages of their message early on in the speech so the audience can anticipate

when the point is coming to an end. This can be done by using techniques such as

projecting names, using contrastive pairs such as ‘us’ and ‘them’ and by using

lists of three. Highlighting the importance of these techniques for example, is

Barack Obama who uses a list of three in his first sentence of his speech, ‘To

Chairman Dean (1) and my great friend Dick Durbin (2), and to all my fellow

citizens of this great nation… (3)’. This is just one example of claptrap

techniques; I will refer to more examples later on.

According to Atkinson (1984) the typical applause length is seven, eight or nine

seconds (+/-1). It should be noted the results gathered by Atkinson (1984) were

from actual audience members and not under experimental conditions.

Kennedy’s speech had a duration of 22 minutes and 26 seconds and had a total of

26 claptraps, the audience spent a total of 189 seconds (3 minutes 9 seconds)

clapping with the longest period of applause being 33 seconds and the longest

time in-between claptraps was 134 seconds (2 minutes 14 seconds). Ronald

Reagan’s speech had a duration of 42 minutes 23 seconds and had a total of 50

32

Page 33: The Dissertation - Full

claptraps, the audience spent a total of 512 seconds (8 minutes 53 seconds)

clapping with the longest period of applause being 30 seconds and the longest

time in-between claptraps was 124 seconds (2 minutes 4 seconds). During

Barack Obama’s speech there were 75 claptraps in 44 minutes 58 seconds, the

audience spent a total of 994 seconds (16 minutes 56 seconds) clapping with the

longest period of applause being 47 seconds. The longest time between the uses

of claptrap was 58 seconds (table 4).

Table 4: shows various detail regarding the three chosen speeches President List of

threeProjecting a name

Contrastive pairs

Reference to “us” “we” “ourselves

Negative towards opposition

Rhetorical question/question

John F. Kennedy (1960)

15 18 4* 48 18 3

Ronald Reagan (1980)

27* 11 2 166* 56* 14*

Barack Obama (2008)

23 23* 2 77 34 4

President Duration of speech (Min/Sec)

Total number of claptraps

Total duration of audience applause (Min/Sec)

Longest period of applause (Sec)

Largest time in-between the use of a claptrap (Min/Sec)

John F. Kennedy (1960)

22 minutes 26 seconds

26 3 minutes 9 seconds

33 seconds 2 minutes 14 seconds

Ronald Reagan (1980)

42 minutes 23 seconds

50 8 minutes 53 seconds

30 seconds 2 minutes 4 seconds

Barack Obama (2008)

44 minutes 58 seconds

75 16 minutes 56 seconds

47 seconds 0 minutes 58 seconds

Table 5: shows the number of occurrences of Atkinson’s (1984) applaudable message techniques with the addition of ‘rhetorical questions’ (* marks the highest out of the three speakers in each category)

33

Page 34: The Dissertation - Full

Looking at the three politicians in a ranking order of hand gestures used, Reagan

comes last by a long shot. My point here is, that Reagan isn’t as dominant when it

comes to non-verbal communication, but when it comes to content, he is head

and shoulders above the rest. Looking at table 5 we can see that Reagan uses

Atkinson’s (1984) applaudable message techniques far more than Kennedy and

Obama. Out of the six techniques I have analysed, Reagan used the most in four

(list of three, reference to “us”, “we” or “ourselves”, negative towards the

opposition and rhetorical questions). This doesn’t always equate to the audience

applauding as much as they did in Obama’s speech (50 claptraps vs. 75), but

considering Reagan’s lack of hand gestures, it shows that the content is just as

important.

Reagan uses more emotive language than Kennedy and Obama, although they

too use emotive language, Reagan uses it in a way that includes his audience into

his speech. Supported by the fact he referred to “us”, “we” or “ourselves’ 216%

(89) more than Obama and 346% (118) more than Kennedy. He also involved

the audience in his speech more than either Obama or Kennedy, and did so a

total of 14 times through asking questions or rhetorical questions in contrast to

Obama’s 4 and Kennedy’s 3. According to Atkinson (1984) the use of both of

these techniques is likely to encourage applause and this proved correct for

Reagan, and at times worked too well since he was interrupted by collective

applause more than either Obama and Kennedy at 17 times (table 6, pg. 39).

Below is an example of how Reagan involved the audience in his speech by

asking them questions:

Extract 1

Reagan: ‘Can anyone look at the record of this Administration and say “well ‘well done’?

Audience: NO!

Reagan: ‘Can anyone compare the state of our economy when the Carter Administration took office with where we are today and say ‘keep up the good work’?

Audience: NO!

34

Page 35: The Dissertation - Full

Reagan: ‘can anyone look at our reduced standing in the world today and say, ‘lets have four more years of this’?

Audience: NO!

Reagan: ‘I believe the American people are going to answer these questions the first week of November and their answer will be, ‘no – we’ve had enough’ XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX

(Republican National Convention, 1980)

This extract is unlike anything in Kennedy and Obama’s speeches. Through the

use of a three-part list not only has Reagan involved the audience so they are

collectively answering his questions, but he also gets the audience to contribute

their own three-part list. The combination of this applaudable technique and the

fact Reagan is actively communicating with his audience and not just too them,

leads 14 seconds of applause, double what Atkinson (1984) described as the

typical length. It’s extracts like this that have allowed me to suggest that Reagan

uses more emotive language than Obama and Kennedy. Within this passage

Reagan also says ‘… we’ve had enough’, consider if this message was worded

differently and instead Reagan said: ‘… America you’ve had enough’. This has

nowhere near the same emotional appeal. The term ‘you’ distances the speaker

from those who they are referring to, whereas the use of ‘we’ involves his fellow

people, it shows them that he’s on their team, he understands what they want

and that he can be the man to lead them forward.

The fact that Reagan involves his audience so much through the use of questions

and reference to ‘us’ leads me to my next point, which is how often they

interrupted his speech and forced him to stop speaking (17). This was more than

either of the other orators analysed. Usually when the audience interrupts a

speaker it can mean two things, the claptrap has been poorly delivered so the

audience don’t know when to applaud, or because they agree with what the

speaker says so much they cannot contain their applause. For Reagan the later is

definitely the case. This is supported by my findings, as isolated applause is likely

to occur at the end of a poorly delivered claptrap, yet Reagan’s speech produced

35

Page 36: The Dissertation - Full

the least incidents of isolated applause (2) in contrast to Obama (7) and Kennedy

(9). Therefore, these results lead me to believe that although the audience

interrupted Reagan often, this was to show their agreement as opposed to being

unsure when to applaud.

Chart 1

Another claptrap technique that Reagan used to great effect was attacking the

opposition (referred to in table 5 as ‘negative towards opposition’). Reagan

spoke about the opponent negatively 167% (22) more than Obama and 312%

(38) more than Kennedy. By highlighting the mistakes that the opposition have

made it allows him to form a strong ‘us’ vs. ‘them’ mentality among his party.

Below is an extract that highlights this:

List of t

hree

Projec

ting a

name

Contrasti

ve pair

s

Refere

nce to

“us”

“we”

“ours

elves

Negati

ve towar

ds opposit

ion

Rhetoric

al ques

tions

0

40

80

120

160

Chart to show the number of occurrences of Atkinson's (1984) applaudable message techniques in Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama's (2008)

presidential nomination acceptance speeches

John F. KennedyRonald ReaganBarack Obama

Applaudable message technique

Nu

mb

er o

f occ

urr

ence

s

36

Page 37: The Dissertation - Full

Extract 2

Reagan: ‘they tell us they have done the most that humanly could be done. They say that the United States has had its day in the sun; that our nation has passed its zenith. They expect you to tell your children that the American people no longer have the will to cope with their problems; that the future will be one of sacrifice and few opportunities. My fellow citizens I utterly reject that value’

XXXXXXXX

(Republican National Convention, 1980)

Aaaaaa Represents the use of ‘they’Aaaaaa Represents ‘us’, ‘we’, ‘our’Aaaaaa Represents children, future, use of negative word and three part list Represents emphasis

As can be seen in table 5, Reagan uses Atkinson’s (1984) applaudable message

techniques more often than either Kennedy or Obama (the most in 4/7

categories). This passage from Reagan highlights his constant use. He refers to

the opposition party as ‘they’ 5 times, made reference to ‘us’/’our’ 3 times, the

emotional capture and fear for ‘your’ children's future and speaks about the

future using an aggressive word that carries no positive connotations, ‘sacrifice’.

Reagan creates a metaphorical monster out of the opposition party, one that has

ruined lives in the past, one that impacts your children's future, and one that is

attacking ‘our America’. Countries justify going to war by using a similar

technique, Reagan is going to war, but in a different context.

This extract comes early in Reagan’s speech, so the audience is on his side from

the start and they are clear on whom the ‘enemy’ is. By doing this Reagan is

showing the audience his first point that should be applauded (i.e. when he

attacks the opposition). He does this (also done in the previous example) by

speaking with a steady vocal pitch when talking about the opposition, but when

he says ‘I utterly reject that value’ his vocal pitch deepens and becomes more

assertive. This is done not only to add emphasis to what is being said but also to

tell the audience that he has come to the end of his applaudable message.

Constantly attacking the opposition may bring people to question Reagan’s

character, to combat this he talks about ‘us’, ‘we’ and ‘ourselves’ frequently, far

37

Page 38: The Dissertation - Full

more than Kennedy and Obama (table 5 and chart 1). Put simply, if Reagan spent

the whole speech openly attacking the opposition his character may come into

question, therefore by using ‘we’ he can pass of his negative views towards the

opposition as a shared, collective opinion.

So how do Kennedy, Reagan and Obama attack the opposition without coming

across as too aggressive? They did it in a way that is far less aggressive than has

been seen with Reagan’s example above, by attacking the opposition implicitly.

Chart 4 below shows how all three politicians balance their strong external

attacks and their implicit attacks. If they were to attack the opposition openly

throughout the whole speech it would come across as too strong and give the

impression they have a personal vendetta against their opponents (e.g. Gore vs.

Perot). Through implicit attacks they can attack the opposition but in a much

more subtle way as can be seen in Obama’s example below.

Obama: ‘We’re a better country than one where a man in Indiana has to pack up his equipment that he's worked on for 20 year and watch as it’s shipped off to China … We are more compassionate than a government that lets veterans sleep on our streets and families slide into poverty’

(Democratic National Convention, 2008)

If this statement were to fit the external attack category it would read something like this:

‘What sort of government takes away a man’s job after working hard for 20 years? The Republicans don’t care about you, but we do. We won’t let our veterans sleep on the street or our family’s slip into poverty. We will look after you’

Although on its own this isn’t too strong, if Obama attacked the opposition with

the same bluntness in all of his 34 examples then its more than likely going to

reflect his personality in a negative light rather than the opposition. All three of

the speakers I have analysed follow this trend with them all having an almost

equal balance between external attacks and implicit attacks to combat any

potential questions about their personal character.

38

Page 39: The Dissertation - Full

Chart 2

Invited and uninvited applause

Table 6: shows when audience applause took place during the three speeches

John F. Kennedy Ronald Reagan Barack ObamaApplause at completion point

28 45 36

Applause not at completion point

0 6 47

Interruptive applause (speaker pauses)

3 17 12

Speaker talking over applause

0 11 61

Isolated applause 9 2 7Clapping an applaudable message

0 4 12

Clayman (1993) suggested that audiences can co-ordinate their behaviour in two

ways. One is ‘independent decision-making’, where people in the crowd act

independently but still manage to co-ordinate their behaviour with others.

John F. Kennedy

Ronald Reagan

Barack Obama

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

8

24

16

10

32

18

The number of external attacks and implicit attacks on the opposition by Kennedy (1960),

Reagan (1980) and Obama (2008)

External attacksImplicit attacks

Speaker

Nu

mb

er o

f occ

urr

ence

s

39

Page 40: The Dissertation - Full

Secondly, there is ‘mutual monitoring’ where individual responses are guided by

the behaviour of others in the audience. In Obama’s speech the audience fall into

the independent decision making category. Strong collective applause often

occurred before he had finished speaking (120), in contrast to Kennedy’s

audience who at times, had an awkward pause at the end of a poorly delivered

applaudable message. This led to a ‘staggered’ onset or isolated applause (9)

(table 6) which then prompted others to respond. The staggered beginning to

applause in Kennedy’s speech gives the impression that mutual monitoring is

also involved in applause. At times the audience had a 1 second pause before

isolated applause occurred, this may be down to the fact that some individuals

simply agreed with what was being said rather than copying the behaviour of

others to prevent feeling uncomfortable. Isolated applause was seen in Obama’s

speech more than was expected, however, the audience was eager and loud

throughout the speech (arrow 1). With such a large crowd it’s expected that

small sections would applaud on their own, so the incidents of isolated applause

(arrow 2) is more to do with the audience than the quality of Obama’s claptrap

delivery. For example, Obama had to speak over applause 61 times (arrow 1),

some statements were so popular with the audience they applauded regardless

of whether a rhetorical device was used (arrow 4). For example, Obama’s

statement on health care, ‘Now – now is the time to finally keep the promise of

affordable, accessible health care for every single American’. This was followed

not only by audience applause but also cheering. This statement doesn’t contain

a rhetorical device yet still produces rapturous applause, this goes to show that

the speech content, i.e. the policy, is so popular it would receive applause

regardless of whether rhetorical devices were used or not. Such results raise

serious question marks about Heritage and Greatbatch’s argument that

audiences are much more likely to applaud a message if it’s delivered with a

rhetorical device. It also suggests that Atkinson et al (Atkinson, 1984; Heritage

and Greatbach, 2006) have seriously underestimated the importance of speech

content when using techniques to encourage applause.

Chart 3a

40

Page 41: The Dissertation - Full

Another implication regards the occurrence of spontaneous audience applause,

which Atkinson had some doubt about. He states: ‘Professional politicians would

no doubt prefer us to think of displays of approval as wholly spontaneous

responses to the depth and wisdom of their words. Unfortunately, however, the

available evidence provides few grounds for so doing’ (Atkinson, 1984, pg. 45-

46). Looking at the results from Obama’s speech, it suggests there is now

available evidence because Obama’s audience showed more examples of

spontaneous applause (arrows 1, 3, 4, 5) than applause that came when,

according to Atkinson, it should (arrow 6). Suggesting the content of the speech

had more of an impact than rhetorical devices used.

Nevertheless, it would be naïve to assume that uninvited applause is completely

spontaneous, the use of claques (a group hired to applaud a performance) in

French theatres in times gone by is well known, and it would be hard to believe

political activists do not collude in the same way. Nonetheless, it’s noteworthy

that the most occurrences of uninvited applause in this study came in Barack

Obama’s speech (61), who compared to his battle against Mitt Romney in 2012

Applause

at co

mplet

ion poin

t

Applause

not at c

omplet

ion point

Interru

ptive a

pplause

(spea

ker pau

ses)

Spea

ker ta

lkin

g over

applau

se

Isolat

ed ap

plause

Clappin

g with

out it b

eing a

n applau

dable

mes

sage

0

20

40

60

Chart to show when audience applause took place in John F. Kennedy (1960), Ronald Reagan (1980) and Barack Obama's presidential nom-

ination acceptance speeches

John F. KennedyRonald ReaganBarack Obama

Applause timing

Nu

mb

er o

f occ

ure

nce

s

41

Page 42: The Dissertation - Full

won by a landslide in 2008 (see appendix A) and therefore suggests that the

uninvited applause may just be a sign of his popularity at the time. This opens up

a whole new debate of whether levels of applause have a direct correlation to the

popularity of the speaker’s character.

42

Page 43: The Dissertation - Full

Chart 3b

Applause

at co

mplet

ion poin

t

Applause

not at c

omplet

ion point

Interru

ptive a

pplause

(spea

ker pau

ses)

Spea

ker ta

lkin

g over

applau

se

Isolat

ed ap

plause

Clappin

g with

out it b

eing a

n applau

dable

mes

sage

010203040506070

Chart to show at what points the audience began clapping dur-ing Kennedy (1960), Reagan (1980) and Obama (2008) presi-

dential nomination acceptance speeches

John F. KennedyRonald ReaganBarack Obama

Nu

mb

er o

f occ

urr

ence

s

Arrow 1: Obama's eager audi-ence

Arrow 2: Obama's isolated applause

Arrow 3: uninvited ap-plause

Arrow 4: un-invited ap-plause

Arrow 5: uninvited ap-plause

Arrow 6: invited ap-plause

43

Page 44: The Dissertation - Full

Body Language

Gilbert Austin, a pioneer of Chironomia,

describes gesture as the ‘action and

position of all the parts of the body’ (Austin,

1806, pg. 133) and highlights the

importance of gesture to convey meaning. It

should be noted that gestures are different

to non-verbal physical communications,

which don’t convey specific messages, such

as proxemics (Wilkins, 2006). By following

guidelines set by Austin, orator’s can

improve their performance by combining

their gestures and words. Austin highlights

the importance of gesture in speaking

spontaneously but also warns that gesture

should only be used when appropriate and

with restraint (Austin, 1806).

Austin created a system of notations; the

first stage is the idea that the body is placed

in an imaginary sphere (figure A). He states

that the speaker then moves their hands, feet or body in the direction of one of

the points in the sphere. Each of the movements, whether that is towards a

certain section of the sphere or movements of the arm, body or head, is given a

notation that states the direction and the way in which the speaker should move.

These movements should, according to Austin, be noted within the speech itself

so the speaker knows the exact

moment when to perform the

desired action. If the required

movement is to do with the hands

then the notation is written above

Figure CClasped hands, crossed hand, and folded hand positions (Austin, 1806)

Figure B“The human figure being supposed to be so place within this sphere, that the centre of the breast shall coincide with its centre, and that the diameter of the horizontal circle perpendicular to a radius is drawn to the projecting point, shall pass through the shoulders, the positions and motions of the arms are referred to and determined by these circles and their intersections” (Austin, 1806, pg. 510)

44

Page 45: The Dissertation - Full

the text and if it’s to do with the legs then below. Austin uses this example, ‘Bcl. E

f sh. above a word indicates that the speaker should clasp both hands and extend

them forward at shoulder height in a shaking motion. The notation L 1 x under a

word indicates that the speaker should advance the left foot and bend the right

knee’ (Austin, Robb and Thonssen, 1966, pg. 4) (see figure B). After much

influence across Europe and America, Chronomia fell out of favour by the end of

the 19th century because it was believed to be too mechanical for modern

political oratory. However these techniques, although maybe to a lesser degree,

are still used in political speeches and it could be said they always will be.

Condon and Ogston (1966) suggested that the body of orators and people in

general move closely in time with their words, they termed this ‘self-synchrony’.

Self-synchrony is not solely to do with hand gestures. The movement of other

areas of the body are also closely linked with speech. However, this doesn’t mean

that every bodily movement is associated with discourse.

Watching the speeches and

viewing the transcriptions to

look at the relationship between

vocal pitch and hand gestures,

the speeches by Kennedy (1960),

Reagan (1980) and Obama

(2008) have been analysed and

the findings can be found in table

7 (pg. 58). Two out of the three

speakers, especially with Kennedy, showed a considerable number of hand

gestures in direct relation with vocal stress, in other words the majority of their

gestures were timed to take place at a similar time to vocal stress. The remaining

hand gestures cannot be labelled as unrelated to vocal stress, as some are

considered to be preparatory gestures. For example Kennedy would often use a

right hand jab (40.8%) for the first two elements of a three part list and then use

a right handed finger point (10.1%) to emphasize the stressed and most

important word. Similarly so for Obama, who used a right hand jab to build

suspense (5.8%) and then switched to his dominant left hand and performed a

Figure D (Democratic National Convention, 2008)

45

Page 46: The Dissertation - Full

left finger point (20.6%) to emphasize the word. This contrasts to his right-

handed jab (5.8%) and the right finger point (8.4%) (table 8). Obama uses these

gestures in a similar way in which a boxer throws a jab before throwing a power

punch on his strongest hand. By looking at an example of Obama using a 3-part

list this can be explained in further detail.

Extract 3

Obama: And I will restore our moral standings so that America is once again that last, best hope for all who are called to the cause of

XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXfreedom, who long for lives of peace, and who yearn for a better XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXfuture

Audience: XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXXxxx

Hand begins to chopHand rises higher and chops again, open palmFinger points and jabs down 4 times XXX marks audience applause Marks emphasis

Figure E

‘… so that American is once again that…’

‘And I will restore our moral standings…’

46

Page 47: The Dissertation - Full

At the precise point where Obama says the word ‘restore’ this is the start of the

applaudable message. Obama performs the first in a series of sharp chopping

motions with his left hand (7.5%). His voice is raised firstly at ‘restore’ and again

at ‘American’ and ‘last, best hope’, with him continuing to shout the rest of the

message at the same volume before applause commences. Pointing gestures, as

can be seen in figure E, occur in time with stressed vowels. Obama is beating the

rhythm of the word out with the aim of making sure that his point is driven into

the audience and therefore taken on board. Obama’s left arm reaches maximum

point of extension when it comes to the last of the 3 part list (hope) before the

camera pans out to the audience, it’s a safe prediction that Obama carries on this

sharp finger pointing in his second 3 part list. This same hand gesture was seen

in 20% of all of Obama’s hand gestures.

From the early stages of this sequence, the audience is littered with many signals,

all of which are done to encourage the audience to follow in the same direction.

‘Hope’ is underlined by Obama as the place where the audience should respond

since it comes at the end of the applaudable message. This first arises with an

increase in tone and volume, rhythmic emphasis and hand gestures. ‘Hope’ is

also the third word in the list which is made especially clear by both the deeper

vocal pitch on the last beat, and by the most vigorous pointing gesture so far.

Therefore, it is hardly surprising the audience began clapping at the end of

‘hope’. It didn’t matter that Obama had not finished his main applaudable

message and it was likely done on purpose so it sounded as if the audience could

no longer contain their agreement with the smaller claptrap snowballing into

one huge ovation at the end of ‘future’ which resulted in 13 seconds of applause,

almost double what Atkinson (1984) stated was normal for audience applause.

If we look at table 7 and the total hand gestures used by Kennedy and Obama,

why is it that Obama uses more gestures than Kennedy? Despite the fact that

Kennedy’s speech was half the length of Obama’s, Obama had used considerably

more gestures by the same time that Kennedy’s had concluded. One reason why

47

Page 48: The Dissertation - Full

Obama was able to use such a variety of hand gestures (25/28) compared to

Kennedy (16/28) was because he wasn’t referring to a written script. When it

comes to using non-verbal signals orators who rely entirely on scripts are ‘much

more restricted…’ (Atkinson, 1984, pg. 66). This is because gestures can have a

shortage of natural, fluid movements and not be as coordinated as words that are

off the cuff (Atkinson, 1984). From Kennedy’s point of view, referring to notes

makes it harder to produce flamboyant hand gestures while also looking down

and reading a script. If Kennedy was to look up and perform the appropriate

gesture after he had finished speaking, it’s extremely likely to be badly timed and

potentially raise suspicion from the audience that gestures have been written

into the speech itself (i.e. following Austin’s technique). Another problem is if

Kennedy was to stray too far from his structured script then there is the chance

he may lose his place, this could be a reason as to why Kennedy had more speech

errors (4) and more incidents of isolated applause (9) than either of the other

two speakers. When it comes to Obama, he uses modern day technology to aid

with script reading. This allows him to use gestures more freely than those who

use written scripts. Transparent teleprompter screens are two Perspex screens

to the left and right of the speaker where the words are transmitted from the

back of the room allowing only the speaker to see what is written, to the

audience it simply looks like two sheets of glass. An advantage of this technology

is it gives the appearance that the speaker is constantly addressing the audience,

as their heads move from side to side (i.e. one screen to another).

Although improved technology can be used to explain why Obama used more

gestures than Kennedy, why is it that Reagan who also used Teleprompters only

used 8 hand gestures (none of which occurred during the actual speech, just

before in an attempt to quieten the audience) in a speech that was just over 2

minutes shorter than Obama’s? It’s understandable that one might presume the

more gestures a speaker uses the more natural and forthcoming they seem, but

this opinion comes into question if we were to look at a British survey in which

47 academics who specialize in American history and politics were asked to rate

all US presidents (not including Obama) on five categories: domestic leadership,

moral authority, positive historical significance of their legacy, vision/agenda

48

Page 49: The Dissertation - Full

setting and foreign policy leadership. From their findings Reagan, who used no

hand gestures other than thanking the audience and attempting to quieten them

before the speech began, was ranked as high as 8 and was dubbed ‘The Great

Communicator’ (Young, 2003). These findings are supported by an article in

Time magazine (Atkinson, 1984, pg. 93), which highlighted Reagan’s success in

speaking spontaneously and suggested that despite the amount of work that

went into preparing the speech, it wasn’t noticeable during his delivery. This is

the first piece of evidence that suggests there is more to the quality of an orator’s

performance than gestures and rhetorical devices.

“The entire speech sounded as though it was delivered off the top of

Reagan’s head, that the thoughts had just occurred to him and, darn it, he

was going to share them with his friends all over America. Said

Wisconsin’s Republican Governor Lee Dreyfus, a Ph.D. in

communications: ‘I’d give him an A if he were in my class.’ That was the

mark generally awarded to Reagan … Iowa’s G.O.P. Governor Robert Ray

… called the address ‘dynamite. He touched the soul of America. He's off to

a flying start.”

(Atkinson, 1984, pg. 93)

In the first example below, which covers Kennedy’s use of hand gestures,

Kennedy uses a smaller applaudable message before a bigger one in the same

way Obama has above. The applaudable message begins when Kennedy says the

word ‘but’. After this point Kennedy’s hand gestures are far more prominent than

the text leading up. He uses a series of hand gestures that begins with the closed

palm right hand jab (40.8%) and as he progresses through the three-part list he

begins to use more sharp chopping motions with his right hand (29.5%). To

highlight an early difference to Obama’s hand gestures, the majority of Kennedy’s

gestures are with his right hand where the majority of Obama’s are with his left;

this is because they have opposing dominant hands (Obama is left handed,

Kennedy right).

49

Page 50: The Dissertation - Full

Extract 4

‘ … But this nation cannot afford such a luxury. Perhaps we could afford a Coolidge following Harding. And perhaps we could afford a Pierce following Fillmore …’

Hand rises up to chin heightHand begins to chop down with a closed palm and political pointHand rises to chest height with an open palmOpen palm chops down towards the groundHand returns to chest height and chops with an open palm Marks emphasis

Kennedy raises his voice when he says the word ‘but’ which tells the audience

that he has something important to say and requires their full attention. The

deepening of vocal pitch is used along with the raising of his right hand, which

acts as an attention cue. A physically visible cue for the audience to look at in the

same way clicking the fingers would capture a person’s attention. There is a

distinctive pattern in this passage between vocal stress and hand gestures.

Kennedy utters two words with extra emphasis here, both being ‘afford’. This

word is the reason Kennedy didn’t seek applause during this first three-part list,

but rather the second. Here Kennedy’s referring to previous presidents who

were, in his opinion, not fit for purpose. He’s reminding the audience of those

who have made mistakes, and by repeatedly emphasizing the word ‘afford’

Kennedy is reminding the audience of his rival parties inadequacy to run the

country. Put simply, he’s planted the seed and is building the applaudable

message. The two times Kennedy says the word ‘afford’ is when his strongest

hand gestures and vocal stress are seen and heard. As can be seen in figure F,

Kennedy’s hand reaches the highest point when the word ‘afford’ is uttered and

this coincides with the fastest and longest chopping motion. The right hand chop

is used 29.5% (99) of the time only second to the right hand jab (137/40.8%)

which was often used in preparation to the right hand chop.

Similarly to Obama, Kennedy beats out the rhythm of the words early on, before

building to larger hand gestures. This can be seen in figure F, where Kennedy is

50

Page 51: The Dissertation - Full

using the right hand jab (political point). His right hand doesn’t come as high as it

does in the second picture, and the length of extension is also far smaller. If he

were to use expansive gestures for all words then no real emphasis would be

added to the final, most important word. Kennedy’s hand gestures, which from

my research is atypical for delivering three-part lists, reduce towards the third

segment. This technique, as was shown in my analysis of Obama, is commonly

used by political speakers to draw the audience into an applaudable message and

then take it away at the last minute so they are left wanting to applaud.

Therefore, when his second three-part list is delivered the audience is ready to

perform rapturous collective applause. Obama used this same technique, but

where he was unable to contain the excitement of his audience who began

applauding before he had finished his three-part list, Kennedy’s audience waited

for the message to be complete before applauding at the completion point (table

6 pg. 39). It’s difficult to know whether this is down to the speaker or the

audience, as a different audience on a different day may have reacted differently.

Figure F

‘ … perhaps we could afford …’

‘ … but this nation cannot afford such a luxury …’

51

Page 52: The Dissertation - Full

Extract 5

‘ … But after Buchanan this nation needed Lincoln; after Taft we needed Wilson; and after Hoover we needed Franklin Roosevelt’ XXXXXXXXXXXXXXX

Hand rises up to chin height with open palmOpen palm chops downRight hand jab Political point rises to shoulder heightPolitical point chops downChin rises Marks emphasisXXX marks loud audience applause

This passage immediately followed and is the

true applaudable message. Here Kennedy

shows a similar extension from his shoulders

and elbow when performing gestures during

his first three-part list. He combines assertive

gestures (figure G) alongside contrasting

pairs and a three-part list in a passage that is

littered with techniques that are likely to

encourage applause. Kennedy uses

contrastive pairs to compare less popular

presidents (Buchanan, Taft and Hoover) with presidents that were ranked in the

top 10 US presidents of all time (Morgan, 2011). When he mentions the popular

presidents (Lincoln, Wilson and Roosevelt) his hand gestures come to life. His

elbows are fully extended, the chopping

motion is far more rigorous

than seen in his preparatory

gestures and the tempo of his speech is faster

to mirror the speed of his hand movements.

Figure H: sincere smile and pride(Democratic National

Figure G: assertive hand gesture (Democratic National Convention, 1960)

(Democratic National Convention, 1960)

52

Page 53: The Dissertation - Full

He concluded his three-part list by mentioning one of the most notable

Democratic leaders of all time, Franklin Roosevelt. The most notable non-verbal

behaviour I found from Kennedy’s speech is what I believe made him so popular

among fellow Democrats and the American people. When Kennedy says ‘Franklin

Roosevelt’ (figure H) he shows a sincere smile, which is seen by ‘crow feet

wrinkles’ appearing at the corner of the eyes, a relaxed forehead and only his top

teeth visible (Nevid, 2006). If this wasn’t enough to show his fondness for

Roosevelt, Kennedy also shows a big display of pride. Pride is seen when the

head is tilted back and the chin is raised whilst displayed with positive facial

emotions such as smiling (Ekman, 1975). This is likely to go down well, as the

majority of people at Kennedy’s speech will be democrats and appreciate the

historic actions of Roosevelt. Some people may show signs of jealousy when

talking about a person that achieved so much success in terms of public

appreciation, therefore these two body language signals show Kennedy’s

sincerity and warmth towards fellow party affiliates. This is likely to be

appreciated by the audience, as it shows he can put his ego to one side and show

appreciation for others.

It is difficult to cover Reagan in this section of analysis because, as can be seen in

table 7, Reagan used next to no hand gestures throughout his speech. When

Reagan did display non-verbal gestures, they occurred before the speech had

begun in an attempt to quieten the audience who applauded him for well over a

minute. It’s hard to believe how Reagan’s hands are positioned in figure I is how

they remained for the rest of the speech, but this is the case. Even when he does

display a gesture to quieten the crowd it is still a small one in comparison to

Obama (figure M, pg. 65). Where Obama has large extension from the elbow and

shoulder, Reagan has a smaller extension that doesn’t get to shoulder height, as

can be seen in figure J. With Reagan’s acting career before his journey into

politics it would be understandable to think he would be the master of gesturing,

but as has been talked about in my claptrap chapter he puts far more emphasis

on what he is saying and how he says it, not that Kennedy and Obama don’t, just

that Reagan’s style is very different to theirs, but is just as effective.

Figure H: sincere smile and pride(Democratic National

53

Page 54: The Dissertation - Full

Figure IReagan’s neutral hand positioning

(Republican National Convention, 1980)

Figure JHand gesture to quieten the audience

54

Page 55: The Dissertation - Full

John F. Kennedy Ronald Reagan Barack Obama

Vocal stress with gesture 253 (76.3%) 0 424 (48.1%)

Gesture indirectly used with stress1) Preparatory gesture2) Terminating gesture3) Pacifier/self touch

17 (5.1%)24 (7.2%)7 (2.1%)

01 (12.5%)0

197 (22.4%)75 (8.5%)5 (0.5%)

Gesture unrelated to stress, including the touching of notes 31 (9.3%) 7 (87.5%) 180 (20.5%)

Speech errors 4 2 2

John F. Kennedy Ronald Reagan Barack Obama

1 handed gestures 326 (98.2%) 3 (37.5%) 595 (67.5%)

2 handed gestures 6 (1.8%) 5 (62.5%) 286 (32.5%)

Total gestures 332 8 881

Table 7: shows the number of hand gestures used in Kennedy, Reagan and Obama’s presidential nomination acceptance speeches, looking at how many gestures were related/unrelated to vocal stress, whether the gestures were used in a preparatory, terminating or self touch way and finally how many gestures occurred on each hand. Note: all of Reagan’s hand gestures were before his actual speech had started, i.e. saying thank you to the audience for the introduction and attempting to calm them down

55

Page 56: The Dissertation - Full

56

Hand gesture John F. Kennedy Ronald Reagan Barack ObamaLeft hand jab 0 0 25 (2%)Left finger point 1 (0.3%) 0 250 (20.6%)Right hand jab 137 (40.8%) 0 70 (5.8%)Right finger point 34 (10.1%) 0 101 (8.4%)Two handed point 0 0 3 (0.2%)Two handed spread 4 (1.2%) 5 (45.4%) 175 (14.5%)Resting crossed hands 1 (0.3%) 0 27 (2.4%)Left hand palm showing 4 (1.2%) 0 77 (6.4%)Right hand palm showing 31 (9.2%) 2 (18.2%) 37 (3%)Left hand multiple finger point 0 0 52 (4.3%)Right hand multiple finger point 5 (1.5%) 0 12 (0.9%)Left handed brush aside 6 (1.8%) 0 20 (1.6%)Right handed brush aside 0 0 62 (5.2%)Two handed brush aside 0 0 4 (0.3%)Left hand fast ball 0 0 16 (1.3%)Right hand fast ball 0 0 2 (0.1%)Left handed claw 0 0 3 (0.2%)Right handed claw 0 0 10 (0.8%)Left hand chop 7 (2.1%) 0 90 (7.5%)Right hand chop 99 (29.5%) 0 29 (2.3%)Two handed chop 1 (0.3%) 0 129 (10.7%)Gesture to quieten audience 0 2 (18.2%) 11 (0.9%)One handed body touch 1 (0.3%) 0 6 (0.4%)Two handed body touch 0 0 2 (0.1%)Two hands closing together 1 (0.3%) 1 (9.1%) 0Microphone rearrangement 1 (0.3%) 0 0Self touch pacifier 3 (0.8%) 1 (9.1%) 0Red flag gestures 0 0 1 (0.1%)Total 336 11 1214

Page 57: The Dissertation - Full

Above: table 8 shows the number of hand gestures used in the speeches and what hand gestures specifically were used. In total 28 different gestures have been recorded

Note: there are more types of specific hand gestures than the number of gestures recorded, as one gesture could be performed in more than one motion, for example a right hand jab could have been performed in a chopping motion, therefore fitting the ‘right/left hand chop’ and the ‘right/left hand jab’ category

57

Page 58: The Dissertation - Full

Images below are examples of gestures that may be harder to visualize (Democratic Nation Convention, 2008; 1960)

Figure L – an example of the left-handed fastball, note the difference to the claw

Figure M – gesturing to quieten the audience

Figure K – the finger jab aka the political point

Figure P – example of the self-touch pacifier, Kennedy putting his hand in his pocket

Figure O – the finger gun, a hand gesture that carries negative connotations

Figure N –the claw, subtle difference in appearance to the fastball, big difference in meaning

58

Page 59: The Dissertation - Full

Conclusion

One distinguishing feature from the results I have gathered, is that if we are to consider

communication as a skill then we can safely presume that it can be improved through

forms of training. Although this dissertation wasn’t concerned directly with political

communicative training, the findings may be considered to have some practical

significance. Results can help potential politicians and orators become, in general, more

perceptive. By analysing these techniques it made it possible for us to recognize

whether a politician is using techniques to invite applause in their speech.

From the viewpoint of the politician, it is easy to see how the microanalysis of political

communication in this dissertation could be used to their advantage. In recent decades

politicians have given more thought to their presentation and how the quality of this

can impact their public perception. The presentation of party National Conventions, for

example, has developed drastically over the years. Speeches by presidential candidates

are broadcast on multiple media platforms around the world and are therefore reaching

a wider audience than ever before. The microanalysis of these speeches has provided

politicians with tips on how they can improve their oratory performance. The study of

rhetorical devices in Atkinson’s (1984) Our Masters’ Voices is basically a manual on how

a script should be written to encourage applause at certain points of the speech. The

findings I have gathered can also provide useful tips. For example, in both the claptrap

(page 32) and body language chapter (Page 44) it shows how the use of gestures and

vocal pitch can specify when a rhetorical device is used with the intention of inviting

applause, but also when they are used with the intention not to invite immediate

applause. In general the delivery of rhetorical devices was vital if audience applause

was to be synchronous with speech. Therefore, if rhetorical devices are to be delivered

with full effect, the mastery of effective delivery is vital for political orators.

Another valuable source of information for political orators is the quality of audience

applause. Cases of uninvited applause can carry great significance. If while the orator is

still speaking the audience interrupt with uninvited passionate applause, this can be a

useful gauge of agreement and popular feeling, and as was seen with the vast amount of

interruptive applause in Obama’s speech, it can signify strong support on certain issues.

59

Page 60: The Dissertation - Full

Again, the timing of applause deserves attention. Members of the audience may

accommodate the speaker by applauding at the end of a message, i.e. the completion

point. Whereas Obama’s audience often applauded before the completion point,

Kennedy’s audience fit this category, who only ever applauded at the completion point.

But as was seen in Reagan’s speech, the audience started to applaud before, during or

after a completion point. Interruptive applause is usually a sign of audience enthusiasm,

whereas delayed applause, which was seen in Kennedy’s speech, can be quite the

opposite.

In this dissertation, an original study of American politicians use of political myth,

claptrap devices and non-verbal communication has been presented. The aim of this

research has been to show how politicians use techniques that may not be known to the

everyday onlooker to encourage audience applause. In conclusion, the use of rhetorical

devices does lead to an increase in audience applause. However, my results suggest that

audience applause still occurs without the use of these devices, which implies

Atkinson’s (1984) theory that audience applause is very rarely spontaneous is now out-

dated. Results also suggest that non-verbal communication, especially hand gestures,

does encourage audience applause when used as part of an applaudable message

technique.

With the regular occurrence of political speeches and the increasing number of

recordings of them, the possibility for analysis is endless. If I were to take this

dissertation further I would continue to study the orators I have. As I have only studied

their presidential nomination acceptance speeches, I would continue my analysis from

the time they took office to the end of their spell. I would analyse their speech content

and body language to see if their performance as orators changes when they become

president and try to find reasons as to why. By doing this, a verbal and non-verbal

communication timeline could be formed, highlighting when and why certain

techniques and behaviours are more likely to occur. For example, potential findings

could suggest that the content of speech is more important when trying to win an

election, but once president, dominant and assertive body language becomes

increasingly important.

13,697

60

Page 61: The Dissertation - Full

Appendices

Appendix A

United States presidential results 2008 election

(Realclearpolitics, 2012)

United States presidential results 2012 election

(Realclearpolitics, 2008)

61

Page 62: The Dissertation - Full

Appendix B

Table 9: table to show what gestures look like and what they connote

Type of Hand gesture What the gesture looks like and meansLeft/right hand jab This is known as the political point and is the thumb pressed on the outer

side of the index finger, with the index finger curling round the thumb, the same motion as a point but not with the sharp ended finger. This is said to be a softer, less aggressive motion and can be seen in figure K

Left/right finger point This is when all fingers with the exception of the index finger are curled into the palm of the hand and the index finger is fully extended pointing ahead. Used in the wrong context this gesture can be offensive, but in the context of hammering down a point it can be used in a positive way. Therefore due to the potentially damming connotations, this gesture should not be overused, if overused rapport between speaker and audience can be destroyed

Two handed point This is the same as described above however both hands are pointing either the thumbs or the index fingers forward bilaterally. Like the single-handed finger point this gesture can be offensive/aggressive if used to point at the target with negative intent. Like the single finger point if used alongside words that are not aggressive this gesture can have positive, enthusiastic connotations

Two handed spread This is where the hands are taken from a neutral position and separated laterally, this is usually to at least the width of a basketball to as wide as the arms can extend. Often you will see politicians with both their palms facing one another as if they are holding an imaginary basketball. Also known as the basketball steeple, it is the alpha-beta hybrid to the more conventional steeple, where by the fingertips on each hand touch at their ends, this gesture connotes assertiveness, strength and confidence

Resting crossed hands This is the crossing of hands in a neutral position with fingers interlocking while placed on the lectern. This doesn’t carry detailed connotations apart

62

Page 63: The Dissertation - Full

from if the hands are crossed and the fingers are moving/stroking the hand. If this is the case it would be a sign of anxiety and therefore fit the self-pacifier group

Left/right palm showing This is where the palm is visible when a hand gesture has occurred. Connotations of open palms suggest honesty and openness. If you hug someone you would separate your arms with palms showing to invite them into your personal space. In ancient times, open palms signalled to people that you were not in possession of a weapon

Left/right hand multiple finger point This is the same idea as an individual finger point but with all four fingers pointing towards the target at the same time. Using multiple fingers to point has the same connotations as an individual finger but applies greater emphasis to what is being said.

Left/right hand brush aside This motion is the movement of the hands from their starting point moving laterally in a sweeping motion, as if to swat away a fly. This gesture is mostly used to brush aside others behaviour/views which is found mildly annoying

Two handed brush aside The same motion as above but bilaterallyLeft/right handed ‘fast ball’ This is where the palms are facing forward and the index finger, the

middle finger and the thumb are curled as if to be holding a baseball, the outer two fingers are curled into the palm, and this can be seen in figure L below. This is a softened version of ‘the claw’. This gesture is not as aggressive but is the perfect balance of an alpha-beta hybrid, not too aggressive not too reserved

Gesture to quieten the audience This can be done through a variety of gestures, but is usually in the style of figure M, one or two hands raised with palms showing moving in a downward motions as if to turn down the volume with ones hand

Left/right hand claw This gesture is palms facing forward with all four fingers spread as if to be imitating a bear claw and is a very dominant alpha display. Much like the finger point this gesture should be used with caution as if it’s displayed too regularly it will come across as too strong and destroy rapport between speaker and audience (figure N)

Left/right hand chop This involves the hands moving in a downward motion towards the floor

63

Page 64: The Dissertation - Full

in a fast movement in a similar fashion to chopping something with a knife, it can be done with fingers spread of with a close fist. It is often used alongside the hand jab (political point). Chopping motion is an authoritative gesture and is used by a person who has made their mind up and is unlikely to change their mind. Using this gesture too much will be seen as too aggressive and potentially destroy rapport

Two handed chop The same motion as above but bilaterallyOne/two handed body touch This is where the hand/s touches the body of the orator, and is usually

done by the palm touching the chest and is used to emphasize a point about themselves, for example ‘I believe this policy’, ‘I believe in this nation’. A person using this gesture is likely to have a desire to be believed by their audience

Two hands close together This is the opposite of the two handed spread. The hands are naturally set wider and move in a lateral motion towards the centre of the body, as if to squash the imaginary basketball. This is mostly seen where the hands are returning to their neutral position just outside the centre of the body and touching notes or the lectern

Microphone rearrangement Rearrangement of the microphone by the oratory and is sometimes considered a pacifier

Finger gun This gesture is where a person uses their fingers and thumb to mimic a gun. If this gesture is directed towards persons then it can have connotations of violence/threat, elite orators should avoid this gesture (figure O)

Self touch pacifier This is the touching of the face, nose, hand stroking, hand/s in the pocket, tie rearrangement etc. and is a sign of anxiety and is usually carries negative connotations. A person using a self-touch pacifier is attempting self-reassurance that ‘everything is going to be alright’. Kennedy used a self-touch pacifier when he put his hand in his pocket on two occasions; this gives off negative connotations and is associated with mistrust and reluctance (figure P)

64

Page 65: The Dissertation - Full

References

Andrews, J. and Hogan, M. (2002). Presidential Leadership and National Identity: Woodrow Wilson and the Meaning of America. In: L. Dorsey, ed., The Presidency and Rhetorical Leadership, 1st ed. Texas: Texas A&M University Press, pp.129-144.

Atkinson, J. (1984). Our masters' voices. London: Methuen, pp.45, 46, 48, 66, 93Austin, G. (1806). Chironomia; or, A treatise on rhetorical delivery. London: Printed for T.

Cadell and W. Davies in the Strand; by W. Bulmer, and Co, pp.133, 510.Austin, G., Robb, M. and Thonssen, L. (1966). Chironomia. Carbondale: Southern Illinois

University Press, p.4.Barthes, R. and Lavers, A. (1972). Mythologies. New York: Hill and Wang.Barthes, R. and Heath, S. (1977). Image, music, text. New York: Hill and Wang.Baumeister, R. and Bushman, B. (2010). Social psychology and human nature. London:

Cengage learning, p.294.Benson, T. and Prosser, M. (1988). Readings in classical rhetoric. California: Hermagoras

Press, p.56.Benson, T. (2002). FDR at Gettysburg: The New Deal and the Rhetoric of Presidential

Leadership. In: L. Dorsey, ed., The Presidency and Rhetorical Leadership, 1st ed. Texas: Texas A&M University Press, pp.145-183.

Bitzer, Lloyd F. "The Rhetorical Situation." Philosophy and Rhetoric 1, 1 (1968): 1-14.Billig, M. (1987). Arguing and thinking. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Brands, H. (2002). Politics as Performance Art: The Body English of Theodore Roosevelt. In:

L. Dorsey, ed., The Presidency and Rhetorical Leadership, 1st ed. Texas: Texas A&M University Press.

Brown, J. (2015). Body Language Success. [online] Bodylanguagesuccess.com. Available at: http://www.bodylanguagesuccess.com [Accessed 18 Jan. 2015].

Bull, P. and Connelly, G. (1985). Body movement and emphasis in speech. J Nonverbal Behaviour, 9(3), pp.169-187.

Bull, P., Elliott, J., Palmer, D. and Walker, L. (1996). Why politicians are three-faced: The face model of political interviews. British Journal of Social Psychology, 35(2), pp.267-284.

Bull, P. (2003). The microanalysis of political communication. London: Routledge, p.27.Burke, K. (1962). A grammar of motives, and A rhetoric of motives. Cleveland: World Pub.

Co.Carbó, T. (1997). Who are they? The Rhetoric of Institutional Policies Toward the

Indigenous Populations of Post revolutionary Mexico. In S. H. Riggins (Ed.)., The Language and Politics of Exclusion (pg. 88-108). Thousand Oaks, California: Sage

Castells, M. (1999). An Introduction to the Media Age. In H. Mackay & T. O’Sullivan (Eds.), The Media Reader (pg. 398-410). London: Sage.

Charteris-Black, J. (2005). Politicians and rhetoric. Houndmills, Basingstoke, Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan, pp.28, 31, 32, 34, 35, 39, 40.

Charteris-Black, J, (2009) ‘Metaphor and Political Communication’, in Musolff, A. and Zinken, J. (2009). Metaphor and discourse. Basingstoke [England]: Palgrave Macillan, pp.97-115.

Chilton, P. (2004). Analysing political discourse. London: Routledge, pp.23, 47.Chilton, P. and Ilyin, M. (1993). Metaphor in Political Discourse: The Case of the `Common

European House'. Discourse & Society, 4(1), pp.7-31.

65

Page 66: The Dissertation - Full

Churchill, W. (Director) (1946, March 5). Winston Churchill's Iron Curtain Speech. General Meeting of the West Midlands Area Conservative Political Centre . Lecture conducted from , Missouri.

Cicero, M. (1951). The Basic works of Cicero. New York: Modern Library, p.32.Clayman, S.E. (1993) Booing: the anatomy of a disaffiliative response. American

Sociological Review, 58, 110-130Clinton, B. (Director) (1995, January 24). Bill Clinton at the 1995 State of the Union address.

State of Union Address. Lecture conducted from , Washington.Condon, W. and Ogston, W. (1966). Sound Film Analysis Of Normal and Pathological

Behaviour Patterns. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 143(4), pp.338-347.De Cillia, R., Reisigl, M. and Wodak, R. (1999). The Discursive Construction of National

Identities. Discourse & Society, 10(2), pp.149-173.Democratic National Convention, (1960). Democratic National Convention Nomination

Acceptance Address "The New Frontier". [video] Available at: http://www.americanrhetoric.com/speeches/jfk1960dnc.htm [Accessed 24 Jan. 2015].

Democratic National Convention, (2008). Barack Obama Democratic National Convention 2008. [video] Available at: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=nmEI9Doctqs [Accessed 24 Jan. 2015].

Dholakia, R. and Sternthal, B. (1977). Highly Credible Sources: Persuasive Facilitators or Persuasive Liabilities?. Journal of Consumer Research, 3(4), p.223

Dorsey, L. (2002). The presidency and rhetorical leadership. College Station: Texas A & M University Press, pp.252, 253, 254.

Ekman, P. (1973). Darwin and facial expression. New York: Academic Press.Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. (1975). Unmasking the face. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-

Hall.Ekman, P. (2004). Emotions revealed. London: PhoenixEkman, P. (2007). Emotions revealed. New York: Henry Holt.Ekstrom, M. (2001). Politicians Interviewed on Television News. Discourse & Society, 12(5),

pp.563-584.Evans, J., & Hall, S. (1999). What is Visual Culture? In J. Evans & S. Hall (Eds.), Visual

Culture (pg. 1-7). London: SageFairclough, N. (1992). Discourse and Social Change. Cambridge, UK: Polity.Fishebein, M. and Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior. New York:

Psychology Press, p.339.Fishman, E. (2001). The Prudential Presidency: An Aristotelian Approach to Presidential

Leadership. Connecticut: Praeger Publishers, p.48.Freese, J. (1926). Aristotle, with an English translation. London: W. Heinemann, p.17Greenstein, F. and Berman, L. (2006). The art of political leadership. Lanham: Rowman &

Littlefield Publishers.Hagger, M. and Chatzisarantis, N. (2005). Social psychology of exercise and sport.

Maidenhead, England: Open University Press, p.26.Heritage, J. and Greatbatch, D. (2006). On the institutional character of institutional talk.Hovland, C., Lumsdaine, A. and Sheffield, F. (1949). Experiments on mass communication.

Princeton: Princeton University Press.Hovland, C. and Weiss, W. (1951). The Influence of Source Credibility on Communication

Effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15(4), p.635.Hovland, C., Janis, I. and Kelley, H. (1953). Communication and persuasion. New Haven:

Yale University Press.Jamieson, K. (1988). Eloquence in an electronic age. New York: Oxford University Press.

66

Page 67: The Dissertation - Full

Kaplan, L. (2002). Jefferson vs. Napoleon: The Limits of Rhetoric. In: L. Dorsey, ed., The Presidency and Rhetorical Leadership, 1st ed. Texas: Texas A&M University Press.

Kelly, S.D., Barr, D.J., Church, R.B. and Lynch, K. (1999) Offering a hand to pragmatic understanding: the role of speech and gesture in comprehension and memory. Journal of Memory and Language, pp. 440-505

Lucas, S. (2002). George Washington and the Rhetoric of Presidential Leadership. In: L. Dorsey, ed., The Presidency and Rhetorical Leadership, 1st ed. Texas: Texas A & M University Press, pp.42-72.

Macmillan, H. (Director) (1960, February 3). Harold Macmillan: The Wind of Change. Tour of Africa. Lecture conducted from , Cape Town.

Manusov, V. and Patterson, M. (2006). The SAGE handbook of nonverbal communication. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Sage Publications.

Matsen, P., Rollinson, P. and Sousa, M. (1990). Readings from classical rhetoric. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.

McGuire, W. J. (1964). Inducing Resistance to Persuasion: Some Contemporary Approaches. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 1, (pg. 191-229). New York: Academic Press

Meyrowitz, J. (1985). No Sense of Place: The Impact of the Electronic Media on Social Behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.

Mio, J. (1997). Metaphor and Politics. Metaphor and Symbol, 12(2), pp.113-133.Morgan, I. (2011). The top US presidents: First poll of UK experts. [online] BBC News.

Available at: http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/world-us-canada-12195111 [Accessed 24 Jan. 2015].

Müller, C. (2013). Body, language, communication. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.Musolff, A. and Zinken, J. (2009). Metaphor and discourse. Basingstoke [England]: Palgrave

Macillan, pp.97-115.Nevid, J. (2006). Essentials of psychology. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Co., p.288.Obama, B. (2004). Dreams from my father. New York: Three Rivers Press.Obama, B. (Director) (2005, October 25). Civil Rights . Death of Rosa Parks Senate Floor

Statement. Lecture conducted from , Detroit.Obama, B. (Director) (2008, June 15). Barack Obama: Father's Day Speech. Father's Day

Speech. Lecture conducted from , Chicago.Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and persuasion. Iowa City, IA: Brown.Petty, R. and Cacioppo, J. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument

quantity and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(1), pp.69-81.

Postman, N. (1985). Amusing ourselves to death. New York: Viking.Powell, E. (Director) (1968, April 20). Enoch Powell - Rivers of blood. General Meeting of

the West Midlands Area Conservative Political Centre . Lecture conducted from , Birmingham.

Realclearpolitics.com, (2008). 2008 - General Election: McCain vs. Obama | RealClearPolitics. [online] Available at: http://www.realclearpolitics.com/epolls/2008/president/us/general_election_mccain_vs_obama-225.html [Accessed 24 Jan. 2015].

Realclearpolitics.com, (2012). 2012 - General Election: Romney vs. Obama | RealClearPolitics. [online] Available at: http://www.realclearpolitics.com/epolls/2012/president/us/general_election_romney_vs_obama-1171.html [Accessed 24 Jan. 2015].

Sears, D., Huddy, L. and Jervis, R. (2003). Oxford handbook of political psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp.222, 223, 224, 225. 226, 228.

67

Page 68: The Dissertation - Full

Shaw, S. (2000). Language, Gender and Floor Apportionment in Political Debates. Discourse & Society, 11(3), pp.401-418.

Sherif, M. and Hovland, C. (1961). Social Judgement. Connecticut: Yale University Press.Simons, H. (1996). Judging a policy proposal by the company it keeps: The Gore‐Perot

NAFTA debate. Quarterly Journal of Speech, 82(3), pp.274-287.Smith, M. B. (1981). Foreword to R. E. Petty, T. M. Ostrom, & T. C. Brock (Eds.), Cognitive

responses in persuasion (pp. xi-xii). Hillsdale: Erlbaum, pp.xiiSperber, D. (2001). An Evolutionary Perspective on Testimony and Argumentation.

Philosophical Topics, 29(1), pp.401-413.Terkel, S. (1980). American dreams, lost and found. New York: Pantheon Books.Treadwell, D. (2014). Introducing communication research. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: SAGE

Publications, pp.234, 252.Tybout, A. (1978). Relative Effectiveness of Three Behavioral Influence Strategies as

Supplements to Persuasion in a Marketing Context. Journal of Marketing Research, 15(2), pp.229.

Weiler, M. and Pearce, W. (1992). Reagan and public discourse in America. Tuscaloosa: University of Alabama Press.

Wilkins, D. (2006). Adam Kendon (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Gesture, 6(1), pp.119-144.

Young, J. (2003). Great communicator. Greensboro, N.C.: Morgan Reynolds Pub.Zarefsky, D. (2002). The Presidency Has Always Been a Place for Rhetorical Leadership. In:

L. Dorsey, ed., The Presidency and Rhetorical Leadership, 1st ed. Texas: Texas A&M University Press, pp.20-41.

68