investor's decision and management forecasting

Upload: tanvir-khan-marwat

Post on 02-Jun-2018

235 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    1/22

    The effects of analyst forecasts and earnings trends onperceptions of management forecast credibility

    Lisa M. Gaynora, Andrea S. Keltonb

    aSchool of Accountancy, University of South Florida, Tampa, FL, USAbSchools of Business, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC, USA

    Abstract

    We examine whether analyst forecasts influence investors perceptions of the credi-

    bility of a good news management earnings forecast. We hypothesize that the effectof analyst forecasts will depend on whether the analyst forecast confirms manage-

    ments forecast and the extent to which managements forecast is consistent with

    the prior earnings trend. Findings indicate that the positive effect of a confirming

    analyst forecast is greater when the management forecast is trend inconsistent than

    when it is trend consistent. The negative effect of a disconfirming analyst forecast

    does not differ based on management forecast trend consistency.

    Key words: Analyst forecast; Management earnings forecast; Disclosure credibi-

    lity; Investor expectations

    JEL classification: M41

    doi: 10.1111/j.1467-629X.2012.00505.x

    We thank Anna Cianci, Shana Clor-Proell, Sukari Farrington, Frank Hodge, Lisa Koo-nce, Molly Mercer, Norma Montague, Sundaresh Ramnath, Jane Thayer, Shankar Venk-ataraman, Julie Wayne, Ya-Wen Yang, Tina Zamora and particularly, Steven Cahan (theEditor) and two anonymous referees for helpful comments and suggestions. We alsoacknowledge comments provided by the reviewers and participants of the 2010 AAAAnnual Meeting, the 2009 ABO Research Conference, and the New England BehavioralAccounting Research Series. We would like to express our gratitude to Robin Dillon-Merill, Dawn Porter, and David Post for assistance in recruiting participants. Finally, we

    thank Lee Kersting, Norma Montague and Nicole Siroonian for research assistance. Pro-fessor Kelton gratefully acknowledges financial support from the Wake Forest School ofBusiness.

    Received 26 July 2011; accepted 14 June 2012 by Steven Cahan (Editor in Chief).

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

    Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    2/22

    1. Introduction

    Investors often use management earnings forecasts to make investment-related

    judgments and decisions. However, investor reliance on management forecastsdepends on the credibility of the disclosure (Jennings, 1987; Mercer, 2004; Hirst

    et al., 2007). Investors know that managers have incentives to release biased fore-

    casts, and investors must often use other information to corroborate the infor-

    mation contained in a management forecast (Huttonet al., 2003). One source of

    information that investors may use to determine the credibility of a management

    forecast is analyst forecasts (Mercer, 2004). While much prior accounting

    research has investigated the independent effects of management forecasts (e.g.

    Coller and Yohn, 1997) and analyst forecasts (e.g. Lys and Sohn, 1990), little

    research has investigated investors combined use of both of these informationsources. Because it is common for investors to utilize multiple sources of infor-

    mation in their judgments and decisions, we investigate these joint effects.

    Because management forecasts are voluntary disclosures, managers incentives

    for issuing forecasts and the credibility of the forecasts are important factors

    affecting investors use of these forecasts (Healy and Palepu, 2001). Forecast

    credibility is of particular concern when the forecast conveys good news1 because

    managers have market incentives to provide good news (Ajinkya and Gift,

    1984). Consequently, investors often question disclosures that are seemingly dri-

    ven by managements incentives (Hodgeet al., 2006) and may require additional

    information to judge the credibility of such disclosures. For example, results

    from Huttonet al. (2003) suggest that investors only consider good news fore-

    casts to be credible when management provides verifiable forward-looking dis-

    closures that corroborate the forecast.2 Because the impact of management

    forecasts on investors judgments depends on the perceived credibility of such

    forecasts, understanding the factors that influence investor perceptions of the

    credibility of management forecasts, especially good news forecasts, is important

    (Hirst et al., 2007). Thus, the purpose of this study is to investigate how inves-

    tors perceptions of the credibility3 of a management earnings forecast are jointly

    1 Hutton et al. (2003) use good news to indicate a management forecast that is higherthan an analyst forecast. Hirst et al. (2007) use good news to indicate a managementforecast that exceeds prior years actual reported earnings. In our setting, a good newsforecast is consistent with that used by Hirst et al.(2007).

    2 Huttonet al.(2003) also report that good news forecasts are more likely than bad newsforecasts to be supplemented with the additional disclosures, which suggests that manag-ers are aware of the credibility concerns associated with good news forecasts and makeefforts to increase the credibility of the good news disclosures.

    3 Mercer (2004, p. 186) defines disclosure credibility as investors perceptions of the believ-ability of a particular disclosure and differentiates between disclosure credibility and man-agement credibility. Although management credibility may influence disclosure credibility(Mercer, 2004), our study focuses on disclosure credibility.

    190 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    3/22

    influenced by whether the analyst earnings forecast corroborates managements

    forecast and the consistency of a management earnings forecast with the prior

    earnings trend. We specifically examine these effects under the good news fore-

    cast scenario as these forecasts are deemed to be less credible.Analyst forecasts are useful to investors in investment-related judgments and

    decisions (e.g. Hirstet al., 1995; Ackert et al., 1997). Analyst forecasts are often

    released subsequent to managements forecasts and provide information to inves-

    tors regarding management forecast credibility (Jennings, 1987). However, recent

    financial scandals have led to questions related to the independence between

    financial analysts and firm management and have highlighted the related

    incentives of analysts to provide overly optimistic disclosures. For example, Feng

    and McVay (2010) show that incentives to please management often result in

    analysts issuing biased short-term earnings forecasts. Research suggests thatinvestors are aware of these incentives and, as a result, discount both negative

    and positive information from analysts (Kothariet al., 2009). Thus, the nature

    of the relationship between management and analysts and the effect of this rela-

    tionship on investors use of analyst forecasts make this an interesting and

    important area of research.

    The extent to which investors will rely on analyst forecasts will likely depend

    on characteristics of managements forecast, and specifically whether manage-

    ments forecast is consistent with their expectations. Absent additional informa-

    tion, investors develop future earnings expectations based on a firms historical

    reported earnings and use earnings trend as a benchmark to evaluate future per-

    formance (Grahamet al., 2005; Koonce and Lipe, 2010). Investors are thus also

    likely to evaluate an earnings forecast, specifically the credibility of the forecast,

    based on the extent to which the forecast is consistent with the prior earnings

    trend (Mercer, 2004).4 Research in psychology (Hastie, 1984) and accounting

    (Earley, 2002; Ballou et al., 2004) suggests that investors may evaluate subse-

    quent information (e.g. analyst forecasts) differently based on whether manage-

    ments forecast is consistent with the prior earnings trend (hereafter, trend

    consistency). Specifically, research shows that individuals expend more effort

    processing information that is inconsistent with their expectations such that theirreliance on the information, and any subsequent information received, is more

    extreme (Earley, 2002; Ballou et al., 2004; Clor-Proell, 2009). As such, we

    posit that, when presented with analyst forecasts, changes to investors credi-

    bility judgments and decisions will be greater when managements forecast is

    4 Investors may use other information to form expectations (e.g. other management-pro-vided information); however, we chose to focus on the prior years earnings trend as itshould be perceived as relatively credible as such earnings would have been audited. Weutilize Mercer (2004, 192) who states, an earnings growth forecast of 10 percent is proba-bly less credible coming from a firm that reported three consecutive years of negativeearnings growth than one that reported three consecutive years of positive earningsgrowth.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 191

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    4/22

    inconsistent with the prior earnings trend than when it is consistent with the

    prior earnings trend.

    We conduct an experiment that utilizes a 22 between-participants research

    design. Across the four groups of participants, we vary the consistency of a goodnews management forecast with the prior earnings trends (trend consistent versus

    trend inconsistent) and whether the analyst forecast confirms (or contradicts) man-

    agements forecast. Study participants are Master of Business Administration

    (MBA) students who are asked to assume the role of investors. Participants review

    information regarding a potential investment opportunity, including earnings fore-

    casts from the companys management and from analysts. To isolate the incremen-

    tal effect of the analyst forecast on credibility judgments, we measure participants

    perceptions of management forecast credibility, as well as their own estimates of

    earnings per share (EPS), both prior to and after receiving the analyst forecast. Weutilize credibility and EPS change scores to examine the effect of the analyst fore-

    cast on investor judgments. In all scenarios, managements forecast is a good news

    forecast (i.e. the forecasted EPS is higher than the prior years reported EPS).

    Ceteris paribus, we expect that analyst forecast confirmation and management

    forecast consistency will jointly affect investors perceptions of management fore-

    cast credibility. We predict that the positive effect of a confirming analyst fore-

    cast will be moderated by management forecast consistency. Research shows

    that investors often discount favourable analyst reports (Hirstet al., 1995); thus,

    we expect investors, when given a confirming (i.e. favourable) analyst forecast,

    to also consider management forecast consistency. Specifically, because of the

    expectations violation caused by a trend inconsistent management forecast, we

    predict that the positive effect of a confirming analyst forecast will be greater for

    trend inconsistent forecasts than for trend consistent forecasts. Alternatively,

    research suggests that investors consider negative information to be more rele-

    vant to their investment decisions than positive information (Cianci and Falsetta,

    2008; Coram, 2010) and bad news analyst forecasts are more useful to investors

    than good news analyst forecasts (Frankelet al., 2006). Thus, we do not expect

    investors negative reactions to a disconfirming analyst forecast to differ based

    on management forecast consistency.Collectively, our results inform both research and practice. Our results confirm

    findings from archival accounting research (e.g. Jennings, 1987), which suggest

    that analyst forecasts provide information about management forecast credibil-

    ity. However, our results show that the informativeness of analyst forecasts

    about management forecast credibility depends on the agreement between the

    forecasts and on the consistency of managements forecast with investors earn-

    ings expectations. Extant research in accounting has focused mostly on under-

    standing factors thatnegatively influence management credibility, and very few

    studies examine factors that may improve the credibility of management earningsforecasts (Mercer, 2004). We show that a confirming analyst forecast can

    improve investor perceptions of management forecast credibility, even when

    managements forecast is inconsistent with investors expectations.

    192 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    5/22

    On a practical level, our results provide information to managers on specific

    factors that affect the credibility of their earnings forecasts, specifically those

    forecasts that are perceived as inherently less credible (i.e. good news forecasts).

    According to Hirstet al. (2008, p. 329), managers have to expend greater effortto make good news credible. We find that the damage to credibility from an

    unexpected (i.e. trend inconsistent) management forecast can be offset by a

    confirming analyst forecast. Thus, our results suggest that to increase forecast

    credibility among investors, managers may have to expend additional effort

    in convincing intermediaries, like financial analysts, that their information is

    credible. Such efforts may be more rewarding in instances where the manage-

    ment forecast contains a surprise element (i.e. deviates from expectations). This

    finding is consistent with recent evidence that suggests managers use earnings

    guidance to lead analysts towards beatable earnings targets (Cotteret al., 2006).However, our results also show that companies may be penalized, at least tempo-

    rarily, if analyst forecasts are below their own forecasts.

    The remainder of this paper is organized as follows. In the next section, we

    provide the theoretical background and present the motivation for our hypothe-

    ses. In the third section, we discuss the research method used in the study, and in

    the fourth section, we present and discuss the results. We provide concluding

    remarks in the final section.

    2. Theory and hypothesis

    Jennings (1987) provides evidence that analyst forecasts made subsequent to

    the release of a management earnings forecast provide information about the

    credibility of the management forecast. Subsequent studies in accounting use

    analyst forecast revisions as a proxy for management earnings forecast credibility

    (e.g. Koch, 2005). These studies presume that analyst forecasts represent market

    expectations and, therefore, analyst forecast revisions proxy for investors assess-

    ment of the credibility of managements forecast (i.e. the bigger the deviation of

    the analyst forecast from managements forecast, the lower the perceived credi-

    bility of managements forecast). While this presumption may be true, otherresearch finds that investors expectations may differ from analyst expectations

    and that investors (mentally) adjust analyst forecasts to incorporate other infor-

    mation. For example, Hirst et al. (1995) find that investors incorporate both

    characteristics of the analyst (e.g. their relationship with management) and of

    the analyst report (e.g. the favourability of the news conveyed) in their judg-

    ments. In general, they find that investors place less weight on analyst reports

    that are from analysts with ties to management (i.e. less credible).5 Results from

    5 Hirst et al. (1995) do not specifically measure their participants credibility judgmentsbut instead measure the mean stock performance ratings of stocks based on their manipu-lated variables.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 193

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    6/22

    Ackertet al.(1997) and Pinello (2008) suggest that investors use analyst forecasts

    for investment-related decisions, but that investors recognize and adjust analyst

    forecasts to reduce the effects of perceived forecast bias. Thus, while research

    shows that investors impound analyst information into their judgments and deci-sions, their use of analyst forecasts depends on other factors. In this study, we

    examine the interactive effects of two such factors: analyst forecast confirmation

    and management forecast trend consistency.

    Collectively, both archival and behavioural accounting research indicates that

    analyst forecasts are informative to investors (e.g. Hirst et al., 1995; Ackert

    et al., 1997), and we expect that investors will utilize analyst forecasts to assess

    the credibility of management earnings forecasts. Specifically, we expect that an

    analyst forecast that confirms (does not confirm) a management forecast will be

    perceived as positive (negative) information and will thus positively (negatively)affect investors perceptions of management forecast credibility.

    Investors reliance on analyst forecasts will also depend to some extent on

    whether the management forecast is consistent with the prior earnings trend

    (i.e. is consistent with investor expectations).6 Expectations violations theory

    predicts that information that is inconsistent with or deviates from expecta-

    tions will cause individuals to expend more effort in processing the informa-

    tion (Hastie, 1984). In general, individuals engage in deeper cognitive

    processing in an attempt to discover the cause of the unexpected information

    (i.e. causal reasoning; Hastie, 1984). Information that is inconsistent with

    expectations is also more salient (Erdfelder and Bredenkamp, 1998) and

    therefore has a greater influence on judgments (Fiske and Taylor, 1991).

    Information that is inconsistent with expectations is also often considered to

    be of lower quality (Koehler, 1993). While such findings are robust in psy-

    chology research (Stangor and McMillan, 1992), they are also found in

    accounting settings. For example, Earley (2002) examines the effect of consis-

    tent and inconsistent information on auditors judgments related to the rea-

    sonableness of a clients real estate valuation. She predicts and finds that

    when initial information is consistent with auditors expectations, auditors

    process subsequent information less deeply, which in turn leads to decreasedjudgment quality. In addition, Ballou et al. (2004) find that auditors are less

    likely to consider relevant subsequent information when a clients strategic

    position is consistent with industry norms (i.e. their expectations). Clor-Proell

    (2009) examines whether financial statement users judgments of a firms

    accounting choice (i.e. recognition versus disclosure) are influenced by the

    6 Prior research shows that managements reporting reputation (i.e. prior forecast accu-racy) is an important factor influencing investor judgments of management forecasts (Wil-liams, 1996; Hirst et al., 1999). In this study, we control for managements forecastingreputation by informing participants that the company has been consistently recognizedfor its high financial reporting quality by the Chartered Financial Analyst Institute. Thisdesign choice should bias against finding results.

    194 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    7/22

    extent to which the firms choice matches their expectations. Her findings

    show that users judgments of management credibility and investment deci-

    sions are more extreme when the actual accounting choice does not match

    expectations. In addition, Clor-Proell (2009) finds that users are more apt toseek out additional information when their expectations are violated.7 In

    sum, the above research provides evidence that expectations violations invoke

    additional processing, influence task outcomes (Earley, 2002; Ballou et al.,

    2004) and credibility judgments (Clor-Proell, 2009) and affect individuals

    search for additional information (Clor-Proell, 2009).

    The effect of an expectations violation (i.e. a trend inconsistent manage-

    ment forecast) should be most prevalent when individuals engage in sequen-

    tial information processing such as when investors view various pieces of

    information in evaluating a companys forecast. During sequential informa-tion processing, an individual develops an initial belief and then revises it to

    account for subsequent information. The magnitude of revision often depends

    on the extent to which new information deviates from prior beliefs (Hogarth

    and Einhorn, 1992). In addition, the causal reasoning processes that are

    evoked by inconsistent (sequential) information are most evident in settings

    in which it is natural or adaptive to consider the motives, intentions or cir-

    cumstances of the provider of the information (Hastie, 1984), as is the case

    in forming credibility perceptions.

    Based on the above, when managements forecast is inconsistent with

    expectations, we expect that investors will consider it less credible and

    expend more effort processing additional (subsequent) information (Hastie,

    1984). Thus, investors will rely more on analyst forecasts when the forecast

    is trend inconsistent than when it is trend consistent. However, we expect

    that this effect will be most pronounced in the confirming analyst forecast

    condition.

    Prior research suggests that positive information from analysts often

    lacks credibility. For example, Hirst et al. (1995) find that investors

    discount favourable analyst reports and view unfavourable reports as

    unexpected. Additionally, archival accounting research shows that themarket reacts more strongly to negative (versus positive) analyst forecasts

    (Frankel et al., 2006), suggesting that bad news analyst forecasts are more

    useful to investors. Consistent with these findings, we expect investors to

    discount good news (confirming) analyst forecasts and to consider other

    factors, such as management forecast consistency, in their judgments.

    Owing to the expectations violation caused by the trend inconsistent

    management forecast, we expect the positive effect of a confirming analyst

    7 Clor-Proell (2009) measures participants propensity to search for additional informa-tion but does not assess actual search behaviour. This study extends findings from Clor-Proell (2009) by examining the effect of expectations violations on investors actual use ofadditional information.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 195

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    8/22

    forecast to differ between management forecast consistency conditions.

    Alternatively, given investors tendency to react more to negative news,

    we expect investors to place more weight on the disconfirming analyst

    forecast and thus do not expect investors reliance on the disconfirminganalyst forecast to differ between management forecast consistency condi-

    tions. Formally stated:

    H1: The positive effect of a confirming analyst forecast on investor perceptions of the credi-

    bility of a good news management earnings forecast will be greater for trend inconsistent

    forecasts than for trend consistent forecasts.

    H2: The negative effect of a disconfirming analyst forecast on investor perceptions of the

    credibility of a good news management earnings forecast will not differ for trend inconsistent

    and trend consistent forecasts.

    We expect results of this study to confirm findings from prior research that ana-

    lyst forecasts are informative to investors and are used to assess management

    forecast credibility (e.g. Hirst et al., 1995; Ackert et al., 1997). Because these

    findings are well documented in the research, we make no formal hypotheses

    although they are discussed in the results.

    3. Method

    3.1. Design

    To test our hypotheses, we conduct an experiment using a 2 2 between-par-

    ticipants design. The first factor relates to the relationship between the analysts

    earnings forecast and the management earnings forecast. Specifically, the analyst

    forecast either confirms or disconfirms managements forecast. The second

    factor, management forecast consistency, relates to whether the management

    earnings forecast is consistent with the firms prior earnings trend. In the trend

    inconsistent (consistent) condition, the company has experienced a steady

    decrease (increase) in EPS over the past 3 years. In all conditions, managements

    forecast is the same ($0.81) and is greater than the most recent years reportedEPS ($0.71 in all conditions) (i.e. good news) and is thus either trend consistent

    or trend inconsistent.

    3.2. Participants

    One hundred and forty MBA students from the United States participated

    in the study as proxies for nonprofessional investors.8 Prior experimental

    financial accounting research often uses MBA students to proxy for nonpro-

    8 There were 142 participants in our initial pool; however, two participants did notcomplete the instrument and were omitted from our analyses.

    196 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    9/22

    fessional investors (e.g. Maines and McDaniel, 2000; Hodge et al., 2004).

    According to Elliott et al. (2007), MBA students are reasonable proxies for

    nonprofessional investors for experimental tasks that are relatively low in inte-

    grative complexity.9

    Our experimental task is similar to those classified by El-liott et al. (2007) as exhibiting relatively low integrative complexity; thus,

    MBA students are appropriate participants for our task. Approximately

    88 per cent of participants reported having personal investing experience aver-

    aging 4.8 years. Approximately 94 per cent of participants reported experience

    conducting financial statement analysis. On average, students reported having

    taken 2.4 (2.1) graduate-level accounting (finance) courses.10

    3.3. Procedures

    Participants were instructed to assume the role of a potential investor

    in ProMed Corp, a hypothetical company that develops, manufactures and

    markets surgical products. Participants were provided with instructions for

    completing the case, general company background and management

    information,11 and the companys actual reported EPS for the past 3 years.

    Actual EPS for the past 3 years revealed either a downward (trend inconsis-

    tent) or an upward (trend consistent) trend with the most current years EPS

    held constant across all conditions. All participants then received ProMeds

    9 Elliott et al. (2007, p. 140141) define integrative complexity as the complexity ofconnections involved in making a judgment or decision.

    10 When included as a covariate, we noted no significant effects of any of our experiencemeasures in our results. Results excluding those participants without financial statementanalysis experience and with no previous graduate accounting and finance courses arequalitatively similar to those reported in the paper.

    11 Management credibility (Mercer, 2004) and managements incentives (Hodge et al.,2006) affect disclosure credibility. Our instrument indicated that ProMeds manage-

    ment has enjoyed a good reputation in the industry. Additionally, participants wereinformed that company officers do not receive performance-based compensation andthat ProMed has been consistently recognized by the CFA Institute for high-qualityfinancial reporting. We measured participants perceptions of ProMed managementscompetency at providing financial disclosures (competency) and ProMed manage-ments trustworthiness (trustworthy) (Mercer, 2005). Management forecast consistencydid not affect competency (p = 0.98) or trustworthy perceptions (p = 0.33). How-ever, participants in the confirming analyst forecast condition perceived ProMedsmanagement as significantly more competent and significantly more trustworthy(both p < 0.01) than those in the disconfirming analyst forecast condition. Resultsincluding competent and trustworthy as covariates are qualitatively similar and, assuch, are not included in the analyses as reported in this paper. Participants alsoassessed the likelihood that ProMed will file for bankruptcy in the next year and thelikelihood that ProMeds management has incentives to overstate estimates of futureearnings and meet analyst forecasts. As expected, these measures did not differacross treatment conditions and are not reported.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 197

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    10/22

    earnings forecast and were asked to make several judgments regarding the

    forecast and to provide their own EPS forecast. Next, participants were pro-

    vided with the mean analyst earnings forecast from Thomson First Call,

    depending on the experimental condition. Participants in the confirming (dis-

    confirming) analyst forecast condition were given an analyst forecast equal to

    (less than) managements forecast.12 Participants then made several judgments

    regarding the information, provided a revised EPS forecast and completed thepost-experimental questionnaire. Figure 1 provides a description of the experi-

    mental procedures.

    Three most recentyears audited EPS

    (Oldest to Most Current)

    $0.61 $0.66 $0.71

    Managementearningsforecastreleased

    $0.81

    Analystforecastreleased

    (Confirming )

    $0.81

    Analystforecastreleased

    (Disconfirming)

    $0.71

    Participantprovides revised

    credibilityperception and

    expected actualEPS

    Participantprovides revised

    credibilityperception andexpected actual

    EPS

    Participantprovides revised

    credibilityperception andexpected actual

    EPS

    Participantprovides revised

    credibilityperception andexpected actual

    EPS

    Participantprovides initial

    credibilityperception andexpected actual

    EPS

    Three most recentyears audited EPS

    (Oldest to Most Current)

    $0.79 $0.75 $0.71

    Managementearningsforecastreleased

    $0.81

    Analystforecastreleased

    (Confirming)

    $0.81

    Trend inconsistent management forecast

    Trend consistent management forecast

    Analystforecastreleased

    (Disconfirming)

    $0.71

    Participantprovides initial

    credibilityperception andexpected actual

    EPS

    Figure 1 Experimental procedures.

    12 Investors reliance upon information from analysts depends on characteristics of theanalyst and the information, such as whether the report is favourable or unfavourable(Hirst et al., 1995). Participants were informed that Thomson First Call has a reputationfor high-quality forecasts and no mention was made about analyst revisions or any initialforecasts (i.e. analyst forecasts that may have been made prior to managements forecast).We measured participants perceptions of analyst forecast credibility using an 11-pointscale anchored by (0) not at all credible and (10) extremely credible. Analyst forecastcredibility is significantly different between analyst forecast confirmation conditions(p < 0.01). Interestingly, participants perceived the analyst as more (less) credible whenthe analyst confirmed (did not confirm) managements forecast (means = 6.36 vs. 5.12).

    198 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    11/22

    The experiment was conducted in two settings. Sixty-six per cent of partici-

    pants completed the experiment during scheduled class time using hard-copy

    materials. Thirty-four per cent of participants completed the experiment at their

    convenience on the Internet. The materials used in both settings were identicalexcept for the method in which they were obtained (hard-copy versus web

    based). Analysis of the dependent measures indicates no significant differences

    owing to experimental setting.13

    3.4. Dependent variable

    Our primary dependent variable is participants perceptions of management

    earnings forecast credibility which we measure at two points in time: (i) after par-

    ticipants receive managements forecast (initial measure) and (ii) after partici-pants receive the analyst earnings forecast (revised measure). We ask

    participants to assess both the credibility and the believability of managements

    forecast using 11-point scales anchored at (0) not at all credible (believable) and

    (10) extremely credible (believable). We average these two measures into one

    credibility score for further analysis (Hirstet al., 2007).14 To assess the incremen-

    tal effect of the analyst forecast on management forecast credibility, we construct

    a credibility change score calculated as the participants revised measure minus

    his/her initial measure. As we are interested in both the direction and the magni-

    tude of participants reactions to the analyst forecast, we use both the change

    score and the absolute value of the change score in our analyses.

    4. Results

    4.1. Manipulation checks

    To assess the effectiveness of the analyst forecast confirmation manipulation,

    we asked participants whether ProMeds forecast was greater than, less than or

    equal to the analyst forecast. One hundred and twenty-seven (90.7 per cent) par-

    ticipants correctly identified the relationship. For the management forecast con-sistency manipulation, we asked participants to indicate ProMeds prior years

    earnings trend. One hundred and thirty-three (95 per cent) participants correctly

    identified the earnings trend.15

    13 Prior research suggests that research conducted online and in a laboratory setting pro-vides similar results (Alexander et al., 2006).

    14 The initial (revised) measures of believability and credibility are significantly correlated

    with a Pearson correlation = 0.818, p < 0.01 (0.926, p < 0.01). Additionally, Cron-bachs alpha for the initial (revised) items is 0.899 (0.962).

    15 Results excluding participants that did not correctly respond to either question arequalitatively similar to those reported in the paper.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 199

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    12/22

    Additionally, we asked participants to indicate on an 11-point scale, after

    receiving prior years reported earnings but prior to receiving managements

    forecast and the analyst forecast, whether they believed the next years EPS

    would be much less (scale value = )

    5), no different (scale value = 0) ormuch greater (scale value = +5) than last years EPS. Participants in the

    trend consistent condition indicated significantly different expectations for Pro-

    Meds future EPS (t = 14.20, p < 0.01, one-tailed) than participants in the

    trend inconsistent condition, and expectations were directionally consistent with

    our manipulation (means = 1.94 vs. )1.53). Participants also reported the extent

    to which they agreed that ProMeds earnings forecast was consistent with their

    expectations using an 11-point scale anchored by ()5) strongly disagree and

    (5) strongly agree. Responses were significantly different between the management

    forecast consistency conditions (means =)

    0.56 vs.)

    1.45, t = 2.45, p < 0.01,one-tailed). These items provide additional support for our manipulations.

    4.2. Hypothesis tests

    Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for participants initial and revised credi-

    bility judgments and credibility change scores. Participants initial credibility

    assessments are significantly greater in the trend consistent condition than in

    the trend inconsistent condition (means = 5.72 vs. 5.00, t = 2.37, p < 0.01,

    one-tailed), suggesting that management forecast consistency affects the credibil-

    ity of management forecasts. Credibility change scores in all conditions are sig-

    nificantly different from zero (allp < 0.01), indicating that participants revised

    their initial credibility perceptions to incorporate the analyst forecast. A confirm-

    ing analyst forecast has a significantly positive effect on management forecast

    credibility (mean = +1.16,p < 0.01), and a disconfirming analyst forecast has

    a significantly negative effect on management forecast credibility (mean =

    )1.45, p < 0.01). These results confirm that investors believe that analyst fore-

    casts are useful for assessing management forecast credibility.

    Panel A of Table 2 presents results from an ANOVA on the credibility

    change scores,16 and Figure 2 provides a graphical summary of the results.Taken together, H1 and H2 predict an ordinal interaction between analyst

    forecast confirmation and management forecast consistency. The first contrast

    presented in Panel B of Table 2 is a comprehensive test of our predictions.

    Consistent with prior research on the negativity bias (Ito et al., 1998), we

    expect participants credibility change scores to be greater in the disconfirming

    analyst forecast conditions than in the confirming analyst forecast conditions.

    As predicted in H1, we also expect a confirming analyst forecast to affect

    credibility change scores more when the management forecast is trend inconsis-

    16 Nonparametric analysis provides qualitatively similar results (KruskalWallischi-square test = 71.613, p < 0.01).

    200 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    13/22

    tent compared to trend consistent.17 As predicted in H2, we do not expect the

    effect of a disconfirming analyst forecast to differ between management fore-

    cast consistency conditions. The overall contrast is significant (t = 1.346,

    p = 0.090) providing support for the predicted pattern described above.

    Simple effect tests show that the positive effect of a confirming analyst forecast

    Table 1

    Descriptive statistics

    Management forecast credibility judgments*,

    Mean (median) [standard deviation]

    Management forecast consistency

    Analyst forecast Trend Trend

    Confirmation Consistent Inconsistent Overall

    Confirming analyst

    forecast

    Initial measure 5.99 (6.00) [1.71] 4.95 (5.00) [1.78] 5.47 (5.50) [1.81]

    Revised measure 6.89 (7.00) [1.59] 6.39 (6.50) [1.77] 6.63 (7.00) [1.69]

    Change score +0.88 (1.00) [1.29] +1.44 (1.00) [2.00] +1.16 (1.00) [1.69]n = 38 n = 37 n = 75

    Disconfirming analyst

    forecast

    Initial measure 5.43 (5.00) [2.19] 5.07 (5.00) [1.42] 5.25 (5.00) [1.86]

    Revised measure 3.97 (4.00) [2.00] 3.61 (3.00) [1.34] 3.80 (4.00) [1.71]

    Change score )1.46 ()1.00) [1.55] )1.45 ()1.50) [1.33] )1.45 ()1.00) [1.44]

    n = 34 n = 31 n = 65

    Overall

    Initial measure 5.72 (5.50) [1.96] 5.00 (5.00) [1.62]

    Revised measure 5.50 (5.75) [2.30] 5.13 (5.00) [2.11]

    Change score )

    0.22 (0.00) [1.83] +0.13 (0.00) [2.25]n = 72 n = 68

    *Trend consistent (inconsistent) management forecasts are good news management forecasts

    made in situations where the prior earnings trend is increasing (decreasing) and thus are consis-

    tent (inconsistent) with the prior trend. Confirming (disconfirming) analyst forecasts are analyst

    forecasts that are equal to (less than) the management forecast. Participants assessed the credi-

    bility and believability of managements forecast using 11-point scales anchored by (0) not at all

    credible (believable) and (10) extremely credible (believable). We average these two measures

    into one score, which we refer to as credibility. Participants assessed credibility at two points in

    time after receiving managements forecast (initial measure) and after receiving the analyst

    earnings forecast (revised measure). The change score is computed as the revised measure minusthe initial measure.

    17 Contrast weights were assigned as follows: )3 for the confirming analyst forecast/trendconsistent condition, )1 for the confirming analyst forecast/trend inconsistent conditionand +2 for both disconfirming analyst forecast conditions.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 201

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    14/22

    Table 2

    Hypothesis tests

    Panel A: Results of analysis of variance for credibility change scores

    Source df SS MSE F p-value

    Corrected model 3 243.88 81.29 32.71

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    15/22

    is greater for trend inconsistent forecasts than for trend consistent forecasts

    (means +1.44 vs. +0.88, p = 0.076). The negative effect of a disconfirming

    analyst forecast is not significantly different between management forecast

    consistency conditions (means )

    1.45 vs. )

    1.46, p = 0.991). These results areconsistent with our hypotheses.

    4.3. Additional analysis of credibility perceptions

    We also analyse participants revised credibility measures to gain additional

    insight into the effects of analyst forecasts on management forecast credibility.

    As previously mentioned, ceteris paribus, investors perceive trend consistent

    forecasts as more credible than trend inconsistent forecasts. However, descriptive

    statistics presented in Table 1 suggest that an analyst forecast may mitigate thisfinding. Specifically, we find that a trend inconsistent forecast that has been con-

    firmed by an analyst is significantly more credible than a trend consistent fore-

    cast that has been disconfirmed by an analyst (means 6.39 vs. 3.97,p < 0.01).

    Interestingly, participants perceive a trend inconsistent forecast to be just as cred-

    ible as a trend consistent forecast when the forecast is confirmed by an analyst

    (means 6.39 vs. 6.89, p = 0.11). These findings suggest that investors rely more

    on information from analysts than characteristics of the management forecast

    (i.e. management forecast consistency) when assessing management forecast

    credibility.

    4.4. Additional analysis of EPS estimates

    The capital market consequences of forecast credibility are well documented in

    the accounting literature (e.g. Jennings, 1987; Coller and Yohn, 1997). Hirst

    et al. (2007) show that investor perceptions of forecast credibility are positively

    related to estimated price-earnings multiples, that is, investors assess a value pre-

    mium for firms with more credible earnings forecasts. Similarly, in our setting,

    we expect participants to provide higher EPS estimates when the management

    forecast is trend consistent and when the credibility of the management forecastis strengthened by a confirming analyst forecast. Therefore, while not our main

    dependent variable, we also examine the effects of analyst forecast confirmation

    and management forecast consistency on investors EPS judgments.

    Participants provided EPS estimates after receiving ProMeds forecast (initial

    EPS) and after receiving the analyst forecast (revised EPS). Panel A of Table 3

    presents descriptive statistics for participants initial and revised EPS estimates

    and the change scores (revised minus the initial EPS estimate). Interestingly, the

    initial EPS estimates of participants in both management forecast consistency

    conditions are significantly less than managements forecasted EPS (0.81 in allconditions) (p < 0.01) but significantly greater than the most recent years

    reported EPS (0.71 in all conditions) (p < 0.01). Thus, participants in this study

    appear to discount managements estimate when determining their own EPS esti-

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 203

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    16/22

    Table 3

    Additional analysis

    Panel A: Descriptive statistics for earnings per share estimates

    Mean (median) [standard deviation]

    Management forecast consistency

    Analyst forecast

    confirmation

    Trend

    Consistent

    Trend

    Inconsistent

    Overall

    Confirming analyst forecast

    Initial measure 0.776 (0.780) [0.026] 0.756 (0.760) [0.039] 0.766 (0.770) [0.035]

    Revised measure 0.787 (0.790) [0.023] 0.779 (0.790) [0.034] 0.783 (0.790) [0.029]

    Change score +0.011 (+0.010) [0.019] +0.023 (+0.020) [0.029] +0.017 (+0.010) [0.025]

    n = 38 n = 37 n = 75Disconfirming analyst forecast

    Initial measure 0.768 (0.780) [0.038] 0.760 (0.760) [0.032] 0.764 (0.760) [0.036]

    Revised measure 0.754 (0.750) [0.035] 0.745 (0.750) [0.029] 0.749 (0.750) [0.032]

    Change score )0.015 ()0.020) [0.038] )0.016 ()0.020) [0.029] )0.015 ()0.100) [0.034]

    n = 34 n = 31 n = 65

    Overall

    Initial measure 0.772 (0.780) [0.032] 0.758 (0.760) [0.036]

    Revised measure 0.771 (0.775) [0.034] 0.764 (0.760) [0.036]

    Change score )0.001 (0.000) [0.032] +0.005 (0.000) [0.035]

    n = 72 n = 68

    Panel B: Results of analysis of variance of earnings per share change scores

    Source df SS MS F p-value

    Corrected model 3 0.038 0.013 14.860

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    17/22

    mate but still provide a good news/optimistic EPS estimate, even when manage-

    ments forecast lacks credibility (i.e. is trend inconsistent).

    Earnings per share change scores in all conditions are all significantly different

    from zero (all p < 0.01), indicating that participants revised their initial EPSestimates to incorporate the analyst forecast. In Panel B of Table 3, we present

    results from an ANOVA of participants EPS change scores, and Figure 3

    provides a graphical summary of the results. Consistent with our primary results

    for credibility, the overall contrast presented in Panel C of Table 3 is significant

    (t = 2.570, p = 0.006). Simple effect tests show that the positive effect of a con-

    firming analyst forecast on EPS estimates is greater for a trend inconsistent forecast

    than for a trend consistent forecast (means +0.023 vs. +0.011,p = 0.019). The

    negative effect of a disconfirming analyst forecast does not differ between manage-

    ment forecast consistency conditions (means)

    0.016 vs.)

    0.015,p = 0.926).Similar to Hirst et al. (2007) and Clor-Proell (2009), we examine whether

    investor perceptions of management forecast credibility mediate the effect of ana-

    lyst forecast confirmation on investors revised EPS estimates. The following

    must exist to demonstrate mediation: (i) a significant analyst forecast confirma-

    tion effect on EPS estimates; (ii) a significant analyst forecast confirmation effect

    on credibility judgments; and (iii) a significant effect of credibility judgments on

    EPS estimates when analyst forecast confirmation is included in the analysis

    (Barron and Kenny, 1986). Untabulated analyses indicate that analyst forecast

    confirmation significantly affects both revised EPS estimates (F= 42.326,

    p < 0.01) and revised credibility judgments (F= 96.666, p < 0.01), meeting

    the first two criteria above. To complete the mediation test, we conduct an

    ANCOVA with revised EPS estimates as the dependent variable and analyst

    +0.011

    0.015

    +0.023

    0.016

    Confirming analyst

    forecast

    Disconfirming analyst

    forecast

    Trend consistent

    Trend inconsistent

    Figure 3 Earnings per share change scores.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 205

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    18/22

    forecast confirmation as the independent variable and include participants

    revised credibility judgments as a covariate. Participants revised credibility

    judgments are significant (F= 63.295,p < 0.01); however, analyst forecast con-

    firmation is no longer significant (F= 1.085, p = 0.299). Thus, participantscredibility judgments appear to fully mediate the effect of analyst forecast confir-

    mation on EPS estimates.

    Further examination of participants revised EPS estimates shows that consis-

    tent with Pinello (2008), participants in this study discounted information from

    analysts when determining their own EPS estimate. As previously mentioned,

    participants in the confirming analyst forecast/trend consistent condition were

    informed that both management and analysts forecasted the same EPS (0.81).

    However, these participants provided a mean final EPS estimate that was

    significantly less than that which was provided by management and analysts(mean = 0.787,t =)6.264,p < 0.01).

    5. Conclusions

    Management forecast credibility is an important topic as management fore-

    casts are not useful to investors unless they are credible. As managers have incen-

    tives to voluntarily disclose good news, forecast credibility is of particular

    concern for good news earnings forecasts and investors often require additional

    information to confirm good news forecasts in order to perceive the forecast as

    credible (Hutton et al., 2003). Analyst forecasts provide useful information to

    market participants making investment-related decisions. Prior research (e.g.

    Jennings, 1987) suggests that analyst forecasts represent market expectations

    and, accordingly, provide information regarding the credibility of management

    earnings forecast. However, research has not examined investors joint use of

    analyst forecasts and the prior earnings trend in determining the credibility of

    managements forecasts (Mercer, 2004).

    In this study, we provide experimental findings on the effects of analyst fore-

    cast confirmation and management forecast trend consistency on investor per-

    ceptions of management forecast credibility. Our results show that informationfrom analysts can act as either a substitute for or a complement to information

    from management, depending on the situation. In the presence of a confirming

    analyst forecast, investors credibility judgments depend on whether manage-

    ments forecast is consistent with expectations (i.e. complements). However, in

    the presence of a disconfirming analyst forecast, investors rely primarily on infor-

    mation from analysts and do not consider characteristics of managements fore-

    cast in their credibility judgments (i.e. substitutes).

    Our results are important to both research and practice. Consistent with prior

    research that shows investors use analyst forecasts to evaluate actual firm perfor-mance (e.g. Bartov et al., 2002), our results suggest that investors also use ana-

    lyst forecasts to evaluate firms earnings forecasts and, specifically, the credibility

    of those forecasts. Recent research suggests that analyst forecasts suffer from

    206 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    19/22

    credibility concerns because of a perceived lack of independence between ana-

    lysts and management and analysts incentives to please management (Kothari

    et al., 2009). Results from our study suggest that despite these concerns, inves-

    tors still perceive analyst forecasts as useful information.This study also provides information on the role that analyst forecasts play in

    investors judgment and decision-making. Archival research in accounting often

    uses analyst forecast revisions as a proxy for the credibility of management earn-

    ings forecasts and shows that the market reacts to analyst forecast revisions (e.g.

    Koch, 2005); yet little research to date has shown the situations and circum-

    stances under which this relationship holds. Hirstet al. (2008) discuss the need

    within the management earnings forecast literature for interaction tests that spe-

    cifically identify and examine potential moderating variables. Specifically, they

    state that such tests should help researchers understand the conditions in whichspecific effects may or may not hold. This study answers such calls by examining

    the interactive effects between characteristics of management and analyst fore-

    casts. Our results show that the effect of a confirming analyst forecast on man-

    agement forecast credibility depends on whether managements forecast is

    consistent with the prior earnings trend and investor expectations. When inves-

    tors receive a disconfirming analyst forecast, the analyst forecast appears to be a

    substitute for managements forecast in their judgments of management forecast

    credibility.

    Our results provide information to managers on how to improve the credibility

    of their earnings forecasts, specifically those forecasts that are inherently less

    credible (i.e. good news forecasts). Our results show that the credibility losses

    experienced by an unexpected (i.e. trend inconsistent) management forecast can

    be eliminated by a confirming analyst forecast. Recent evidence suggests that

    managers use earnings guidance to steer analysts down towards beatable earn-

    ings targets (Cotteret al., 2006). However, because we find that companies may

    be penalized if an analyst forecast is below their own forecast, management

    should be cautious in providing such guidance.

    From a regulatory perspective, our results also provide information regarding

    investors use of analyst earnings forecasts. Recent research suggests that therelationship between management and analysts provides incentives for analysts

    to issue biased forecasts (Libby et al., 2008; McEwen et al., 2008). Although

    investors often adjust for the perceived bias in analyst forecasts when making

    EPS predictions (Ackert et al., 1997; Pinello, 2008), our results suggest that

    investors rely upon analyst forecasts (and often more so than managements pre-

    dictions) to determine management forecast credibility. Given the economic con-

    sequences of forecast credibility (e.g. Jennings, 1987; Hirst et al., 2007) and

    evidence of the negative consequences of management and analyst relationships

    (Libby et al., 2008), regulators should be interested in the role of analyst fore-casts on investor perceptions of the credibility of good news management earn-

    ings forecasts.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 207

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    20/22

    As with all studies, this study is not without limitations. First, we use MBA

    students as surrogates for investors. Although our participants report having

    investing experience, we do not know whether our results would generalize to

    different populations of investors. Second, we limit the amount of informationparticipants received making this experimental setting less complex but poten-

    tially reducing the generalizability of this study. Future research could examine

    the effect of other information (e.g. audited financial statements, MD&A) on

    investors use of analyst forecasts.

    Our findings suggest several areas for future research. Although findings from

    Hodge et al. (2006) suggest that good reporting reputation may mitigate

    concerns about management forecast credibility, we do not know to what extent

    managements reporting reputation (controlled for in our experimental design)

    influenced this result. Future research could examine potential interactive effectsbetween management forecast consistency and reporting reputation and other

    characteristics of management earnings forecasts (e.g. timeliness, accuracy). In

    addition, our results suggest that in some circumstances, an analyst forecast is a

    substitute for managements forecast. Future research could examine other

    information available to investors and how investors use this information rela-

    tive to analyst forecasts and management forecasts when making investment-

    related judgments.

    References

    Ackert, L. F., B. K. Church, and M. Shehata, 1997, An experimental examination of theeffects of forecast bias on individuals use of forecasted information, Journal ofAccounting Research35, 2542.

    Ajinkya, B. B., and M. J. Gift, 1984, Corporate managers earnings forecasts and sym-metrical adjustments of market expectations, Journal of Accounting Research 22,425444.

    Alexander, R. M., A. D. Blay, and R. K. Hurtt, 2006, An examination of convergentvalidity between in-lab and out-of-lab Internet-based experimental accounting research,Behavioral Research in Accounting18, 207217.

    Ballou, B., C. E. Earley, and J. S. Rich, 2004, The impact of strategic-positioning infor-mation on auditor judgments about business-process performance, Auditing: A Journalof Practice & Theory 23, 7188.

    Barron, R., and D. Kenny, 1986, The moderator-mediator variable distinction in socialpsychological research: conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations, Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology51, 11731182.

    Bartov, E., D. Givoly, and C. Hayn, 2002, The rewards to meeting or beating earningsexpectations,Journal of Accounting and Economics 33, 173204.

    Cianci, A. M., and D. Falsetta, 2008, Impact of investors status on their evaluation ofpositive and negative, and past and future information, Accounting and Finance 48,719739.

    Clor-Proell, S. M., 2009, The effects of expected and actual accounting choices on judg-ments and decisions,The Accounting Review84, 14651493.

    Coller, M., and T. L. Yohn, 1997, Management forecasts and information asymmetry: anexamination of bid-ask spreads,Journal of Accounting Research41, 653679.

    208 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    21/22

    Coram, P. J., 2010, The effect of investor sophistication on the influence of nonfinancialperformance indicators on investors judgments,Accounting and Finance50, 263280.

    Cotter, J., I. Tuna, and P. D. Wysocki, 2006, Expectations management and beatable tar-gets: how do analysts react to explicit earnings guidance?, Contemporary Accounting

    Research23, 593624.Earley, C. E., 2002, The differential use of information by experienced and novice audi-

    tors in the performance of ill-structured audit tasks,Contemporary Accounting Research19, 595614.

    Elliott, W. B., F. D. Hodge, J. J. Kennedy, and M. Pronk, 2007, Are M.B.A. students agood proxy for nonprofessional investors?, The Accounting Review82, 139168.

    Erdfelder, E., and J. Bredenkamp, 1998, Recognition of script-typical versus script-atypi-cal information: effects of cognitive elaboration, Memory and Cognition26, 922938.

    Feng, M., and S. McVay, 2010, Analysts incentives to overweight management guidancewhen revising their short-term earnings forecasts, The Accounting Review 85,16171646.

    Fiske, S. T., and S. E. Taylor, 1991,Social Cognition(Mc-Graw-Hill, New York, NY).Frankel, R., S. P. Kothari, and J. Weber, 2006, Determinants of the informativeness of

    analyst research,Journal of Accounting and Economics 41, 2954.Graham, J. R., C. R. Harvey, and S. Rajgopal, 2005, The economic implications of cor-

    porate financial reporting,Journal of Accounting and Economics 40, 373.Hastie, R., 1984, Causes and effects of causal attribution, Journal of Personality and

    Social Psychology46, 4456.Healy, P. M., and K. G. Palepu, 2001, Information asymmetry, corporate disclosure, and

    the capital markets: a review of the empirical disclosure literature, Journal of Account-ing and Economics31, 405440.

    Hirst, D. E., L. Koonce, and P. J. Simko, 1995, Investor reactions to financial analysts

    research reports,Journal of Accounting Research33, 335351.Hirst, D. E., L. Koonce, and J. Miller, 1999, The joint effect of managements prior fore-

    cast accuracy and the form of its financial forecasts on investor judgment, Journal ofAccounting Research37, 101124.

    Hirst, D. E., L. Koonce, and S. Venkataraman, 2007, How disaggregation enhances thecredibility of management earnings forecasts, Journal of Accounting Research 45,811837.

    Hirst, D. E., L. Koonce, and S. Venkataraman, 2008, Management earnings forecasts: areview and framework,Accounting Horizons22, 315338.

    Hodge, F., J. J. Kennedy, and L. A. Maines, 2004, Does search-facilitating technologyimprove the transparency of financial reporting?, The Accounting Review79, 687703.

    Hodge, F., P. E. Hopkins, and J. Pratt, 2006, Management reporting incentives and clas-sification credibility: the effects of reporting discretion and reputation, Accounting,Organizations and Society31, 623634.

    Hogarth, R. M., and H. J. Einhorn, 1992, Order effects in belief updating: the belief-adjustment model,Cognitive Psychology24, 155.

    Hutton, A. P., G. S. Miller, and D. J. Skinner, 2003, The role of supplementary state-ments with management earnings forecasts, Journal of Accounting Research 41,867890.

    Ito, T. A., J. T. Larsen, N. K. Smith, and J. T. Cacioppo, 1998, Negative informationweighs more heavily on the brain: the negativity bias in evaluative categorizations,Journal of Personality and Social Psychology75, 887900.

    Jennings, R., 1987, Unsystematic security price movements, management earnings fore-casts, and revisions in consensus analyst earnings forecasts, Journal of AccountingResearch25, 90110.

    L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210 209

    2012 The AuthorsAccounting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ

  • 8/10/2019 Investor's Decision and Management Forecasting....

    22/22

    Koch, A., 2005, Financial distress and the credibility of management earnings forecasts,Working paper (Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA, USA).

    Koehler, H. L., 1993, The influence of prior beliefs on scientific judgments of evidencequality,Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 56, 2853.

    Koonce, L., and M. G. Lipe, 2010, Earnings trends and performance relative to bench-marks: how consistency influences their joint use, Journal of Accounting Research 48,859884.

    Kothari, S. P., X. Li, and J. E. Short, 2009, The effect of disclosures by management, ana-lysts, and business process on cost of capital, return volatility, and analyst forecasts: astudy using content analysis, The Accounting Review84, 16391670.

    Libby, R., J. E. Hunton, H. Tan, and N. Seybert, 2008, Relationship incentives and theoptimistic/pessimistic pattern in analysts forecasts, Journal of Accounting Research 46,173198.

    Lys, T., and S. Sohn, 1990, The association between revisions of financial analysts earn-ings forecasts and security-price changes, Journal of Accounting and Economics 13,

    341363.Maines, L., and L. S. McDaniel, 2000, Effects of comprehensive-income characteristics

    on nonprofessional investors judgments: the role of financial statement presentationformat,The Accounting Review75, 179207.

    McEwen, R. A., C. R. Mazza, and J. E. Hunton, 2008, Effects of managerial discretion infair value accounting regulation and incentives to go along with management onanalysts expectations and judgments,Journal of Behavioral Finance9, 240251.

    Mercer, M., 2004, How do investors assess the credibility of management disclosures?,Accounting Horizons18, 185196.

    Mercer, M., 2005, The fleeting effects of disclosure forthcomingness on managementsreporting credibility,The Accounting Review80, 723744.

    Pinello, A. S., 2008, Investors differential reaction to positive versus negative earningssurprises,Contemporary Accounting Research25, 891920.

    Stangor, C., and D. McMillan, 1992, Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: a review of the social and social developmental literatures,Psychological Bulletin111, 4261.

    Williams, P. A., 1996, The relation between a prior earnings forecast by management andanalyst response to a current management forecast, The Accounting Review 71,103115.

    210 L. M. Gaynor, A. S. Kelton/Accounting and Finance 54 (2014) 189210

    2012 The AuthorsAcco nting and Finance 2012 AFAANZ