ars.els-cdn.com  · web viewfor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness...

64
Supplementary Materials Altered Topological Organization of High-Level Visual Networks in Alzheimer’s Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment Yanjia Deng 1 , Lin Shi 2,4 , Defeng Wang 1,3,# , for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative* # Corresponding author: Defeng Wang; Tel: +852- 26975027; Fax: +852- 26493761; Email: [email protected] IALE analysis for locating “what” and “where” visual cortices 1) Literature selection criteria We searched the studies that investigated HLV functions

Upload: others

Post on 16-Jul-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

Supplementary Materials

Altered Topological Organization of High-Level Visual Networks in Alzheimer’s

Disease and Mild Cognitive Impairment

Yanjia Deng1, Lin Shi2,4, Defeng Wang1,3,#, for the Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging

Initiative*

# Corresponding author: Defeng Wang; Tel: +852- 26975027; Fax: +852- 26493761;

Email: [email protected]

I.ALE analysis for locating “what” and “where” visual cortices

1) Literature selection criteria

We searched the studies that investigated HLV functions in the Web of Pubmed

database (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed). For more specific and effective

search, the MeSH (medical subject headings) entry terms [1] were used, including

"Visual Perception", "Color Perception", "Motion Perception", "Space Perception",

"Depth Perception", "Form Perception", "Size Perception" and "Magnetic Resonance

Imaging".

Until February 2015, this search revealed 3095 published, peer reviewed papers.

Finally, 74 studies matched the following criteria and were included for ALE

analysis: (1) Papers were published in English and in which subjects were healthy

adults; (2) fMRI was adopted as image modality; (3) The whole brain was scanned

and the coordinates of the activation clusters were reported in Talairach and Tournoux

Page 2: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

(1988) or Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space; (4) Only one visual functions

was tested in one task; (5) Results were obtained results by using low level contrasts

(target vision vs. baseline, noise or scrambled meaningless images).

2) Data analysis

Among the included 74 studies, studies on recognition perception (including face,

object, alphabetic word/ letter, scene, and body vision) were grouped into “what”

vision, and those focused on motion or spatial vision were categorized into “where”

vision (Supplementary Table 1). The meta-analyses were performed using the revised

algorithm of the ALE implemented in the Ginger ALE software, version 2.0

(available at www.brainmap.org/ale). Coordinates reported in the Talairach space

were transformed into the MNI space using the Lancaster transform [2]. Foci from

each individual study were smoothed by a full width half maximum (FWHM) value

scaled by the study’s sample size to model the uncertainty in spatial location of the

activations [3]. The statistical level to determine the null distribution of the ALE value

was at p < 0.01 (multiple comparisons corrected by false discovery rate (FDR pN)

method) [3].

II. Subjects and neuropsychological tests

A total of 162 subjects, including 44 normal controls (CN), 52 early MCI (EMCI), 35

late MCI (LMCI), 31 AD, were included in this study. Briefly, the cognitive level (i.e.

CN, EMCI, LMCI or AD) was judged by combining the memory complaints of

patients or their families, the Wechsler Memory Scale - Logical Memory II

(Wechsler, 1987), the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE) scores, and the Clinical

Page 3: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

Dementia Rating (CDR) of subjects. Specifically, the major difference of the

diagnostic criteria for EMCI and LMCI is in the score of Logical Memory II subscale.

The score of Logical Memory II subscale for defining EMCI is: a. 9-11 for 16 or more

years of education; b. 5-9 for 8-15 years of education; c. 3-6 for 0-7 years of

education, which is between the scale for CN and LMCI. Therefore, the EMCI and

LMCI refer to the severity of cognitive impairments. The detailed inclusion and

exclusion criteria were found in the ADNI2 and ADNI GO protocol

(http://adni.loni.usc.edu/methods/documents/). For the current study, subjects with

glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects

was excluded due to excessive head motion during acquisition of rs-fMRI (see data

preprocess section). Finally, there were 44 CN, 52 EMCI, 35 LMCI and 30 AD

subjects.

All participants were administered the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) [4].

The MMSE is a widely used general cognitive screening test. In the ADNI database,

the clock drawing test was performed through the ADNI program, which has two

components: a command condition in which the participant draws a clock to verbal

instructions, and a copy condition in which the participant copies a model clock

drawn at the top of response form. This test, especially the copy condition, is

considered correlated with the visuospatial perception [5]. Thus, we included the

results of copy conditions of clock drawing test for further analysis.

III. Network construction and calculations

Previously established network quantification method based on graph theory was used

Page 4: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

in this study to calculate the network parameters [6]. Firstly, the association matrices

based on the pair-wise interregional RSFC at 0.021-0.045Hz were created

respectively for “what” (29 × 29) and “where” (25 × 25) vision networks of each

subject. Then the matrices were thresholded into binary matrices. Here, a range of

network densities (0.06 – 0.40, with an interval of 0.01) were used to binarize the

matrics to ensure that the resultant networks of different subject could have the same

number of edges thus to make the networks comparable. Therefore, the binarized

matrix was recognized as a graph, each ROI as a node, and the RSFC between two

nodes as an edge.

Two fundamental quantities, the characteristic path length (L) and clustering

coefficient (C), were used in this study to respectively quantify the capability of

integrated and segregated information processing of the brain network. The L is

defined as the average of the shortest path lengths between nodes [7].

Here N represents the total number of the nodes, i and j stand for two generic nodes,

and Lij is the shortest path length between i and j. Thus a shorter path length

represents greater network integration. The C is the average of the local clustering

coefficients of all nodes which are calculated by dividing the number of existing

connections among the node and its neighbors by all the possible connections.

,

Page 5: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

Here G and Gi respectively represent the whole network and the sub-network which is

composed of a node i and its nearest neighbors (e.g., node j and k). Thus the C

quantifies the extent to which neighboring brain regions are connected with each

other, i.e. the network segregation [8].

IV. ADNI dataset

The ADNI was launched in 2003 as a public-private partnership, led by Principal

Investigator Michael W. Weiner, MD. The primary goal of ADNI has been to test

whether serial MRI, positron emission tomography (PET), other biological markers,

and clinical and neuropsychological assessment can be combined to measure the

progression of MCI and AD.

ADNI is funded by the National Institute on Aging, the National Institute of

Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering, and through generous contributions from

the following: AbbVie, Alzheimer’s Association; Alzheimer’s Drug Discovery

Foundation; Araclon Biotech; BioClinica, Inc.; Biogen; Bristol-Myers Squibb

Company; CereSpir, Inc.; Eisai Inc.; Elan Pharmaceuticals, Inc.; Eli Lilly and

Company; EuroImmun; F. Hoffmann-La Roche Ltd and its affiliated company

Genentech, Inc.; Fujirebio; GE Healthcare; IXICO Ltd.; Janssen Alzheimer

Immunotherapy Research & Development, LLC.; Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceutical

Research & Development LLC.; Lumosity; Lundbeck; Merck & Co., Inc.; Meso

Scale Diagnostics, LLC.; NeuroRx Research; Neurotrack Technologies; Novartis

Pharmaceuticals Corporation; Pfizer Inc.; Piramal Imaging; Servier; Takeda

Pharmaceutical Company; and Transition Therapeutics. The Canadian Institutes of

Page 6: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

Health Research is providing funds to support ADNI clinical sites in Canada. Private

sector contributions are facilitated by the Foundation for the National Institutes of

Health (www.fnih.org). The grantee organization is the Northern California Institute

for Research and Education, and the study is coordinated by the Alzheimer's Disease

Cooperative Study at the University of California, San Diego. ADNI data are

disseminated by the Laboratory for Neuro Imaging at the University of Southern

California.

Supplementary Tables

Supplementary Table 1 Summary of studies selected for the ALE analysis of

"what" and "where" vision

Category/study(author) Year No. of

Subjects

Contrast No. of

Foci

“What” vision

Leube et al. [9] 2003 13 Face > baseline 5

Badzakova-Trajkov et al. [10] 2010 155 Face > baseline 18

Love et al. [11] 2011 20 Face > baseline 10

Vuilleumier et al. [12] 2003 13 Face > baseline 11

Henson et al. [13] 2003 21 Face > scrambled image 4

Furl et al. [14] 2007 12 Face > baseline 7

Fruhholz et al. [15] 2011 24 Face > scrambled image 21

Platek et al. [16] 2009 12 Face > baseline 1

Schwartz et al. [17] 2013 8 Face > baseline 3

Page 7: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

Joseph et al. [18] 2011 43 Face > baseline 11

Jehna et al. [19] 2011 30 Face > scrambled image 5

Benuzzi et al. [20] 2007 24 Face > noise 36

Rossion et al. [21] (2003) 2003 11 Face > baseline 4

Rossion et al. [22] 2012 40 Face > scrambled 12

Gobbini et al. [23] 2011 12 Face > baseline 30

Hoffman et al. [24] 2000 9 Face > baseline 8

Pagliaccio et al. [25] 2013 52 Face > baseline 1

Ramon et al. [26] 2010 13 Face > scrambled image 13

Blonder et al. [27] 2004 14 Face > baseline 6

Liu et al. [28] 2009 17 Face > baseline 11

Orlov et al. [29] 2010 12 Face > baseline 10

Olulade et al. [30] 2013 15 Word> baseline 8

Joseph et al. [31] 2006 11 Word > baseline 7

Minati et al. [32] 2008 10 Word > baseline 15

Szwed et al. [33] 2011 16 Word > scrambled image 9

Longcamp et al. [34] 2011 10 Word > baseline 1

Pegado et al. [35] 2011 14 Letter > baseline 4

Dehaene et al. [36] 2001 15 Word > noise 6

Dehaene et al. [37] 2004 26 Word > noise 6

Casarotto et al. [38] 2006 8 Word > baseline 4

Peng et al. [39] (2003) 2003 8 Word > baseline 34

Gold et al. [40] 2007 16 Word > baseline 2

Gros et al. [41] 2001 6 Letter > baseline 2

Page 8: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

James et al. [42] 2009 19 Letter > baseline 12

Thoma et al. [43] 2011 17 Object > scrambled image 8

Pegado et al. [35] 2011 14 Object > baseline 6

Yamamoto et al. [44] 2008 6 Object > baseline 3

Podzebenko et al. [45] 2005 16 Object > baseline 13

Minati et al. [32] 2008 10 Object > baseline 23

Szwed et al. [33] 2011 16 Object > scrambled image 7

Creem-Regehr et al. [46] 2007 23 Object > scrambled image 10

Durand et al. [47] 2009 17 Object > scrambled image 6

Cardin et al. [48] 2011 14 Object > baseline 9

Valyear et al. [49] 2006 7 Object > baseline 1

Niemeier et al. [50] 2005 10 Object > baseline 2

Casarotto et al. [38] 2006 8 Object > baseline 1

Peelen et al. [51] 2012 26 Object > baseline 2

Bucher et al. [52] 2006 16 Object > baseline 7

Blonder et al. [27] 2004 14 Object > baseline 6

Amemiya et al. [53] 2012 27 Scene > baseline 10

Jehna et al. [19] 2011 30 Scene > baseline 4

Blonder et al. [27] 2004 14 Scene > baseline

Claeys et al. [54] 2004 16 Color > baseline 10

Leh et al. [55] 2009 1 Color > baseline 4

Kaufmann et al. [56] 2008 12 Color > baseline 1

Orlov et al. [29] 2010 12 Body > baseline 18

Page 9: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

“Where” vision

Homola et al. [57] 2012 24 Motion > baseline 2

Schraa-Tam et al. [58] 2008 22 Motion > baseline 12

Morito, Tanabe et al. [59] 2009 28 Motion > baseline 17

Caplan et al. [60] 2006 12 Motion > baseline 16

Katsuyama et al. [61] 2011 31 Motion > baseline 3

Podzebenko et al. [45] 2005 16 Motion > baseline 9

Braddick et al. [62] 2001 3 Motion > noise 14

Braddick et al. [63] 2000 4 Motion > baseline 4

Oreja-Guevara et al. [64] 2004 9 Motion > baseline 61

Bucher et al. [52] 2006 16 Motion > baseline 12

Santi et al. [65] 2003 10 Motion > baseline 14

Aso et al. [66] 2007 12 Motion > baseline 5

Iwami et al. [67] 2002 10 Spatial > baseline 11

Grady et al. [68] 2014 14 Spatial > baseline 11

Joseph et al. [69] 2003 10 Spatial > baseline 4

Joseph et al. [69] 2003 11 Spatial > baseline 5

Joseph et al. [69] 2003 10 Spatial > baseline 3

Joseph et al. [69] 2003 11 Spatial > baseline 3

Zeidman et al. [70] 2012 19 Spatial > baseline 2

Yamamoto et al. [44] 2008 13 Spatial > baseline 30

Katsuyama et al. [61] 2011 31 Spatial > baseline 7

Fraedrich et al. [71] 2010 18 Spatial > baseline 6

Georgieva et al. [72] 2008 18 Spatial > scrambled image 12

Creem-Regehr et al. [46] 2007 23 Spatial > scrambled image 6

Page 10: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

Kana et al. [73] 2013 14 Spatial > baseline 11

Wu et al. [74] 2012 19 Spatial > baseline 9

Valyear et al. [49] 2006 3 Spatial > baseline 1

Braddick et al. [63] 2000 4 Spatial > baseline 8

Kaufmann et al. [56] 2008 12 Spatial > baseline 3

Sterzer et al. [75] 2005 12 Spatial > baseline 4

Brouwer et al. [76] 2005 7 Spatial > baseline 7

Cant et al. [77] 2007 9 Spatial > baseline 10

Schoth et al. [78] 2007 22 Spatial > baseline 12

Dumoulin et al. [79] 2004 4 Spatial > noise 2

Baecke et al. [80] 2009 26 Spatial > baseline 6

Gros et al. [41] 2001 6 Spatial > baseline 2

Ritzl et al. [81] 2003 11 Spatial > baseline 10

Negawa et al. [82] 2003 13 Spatial > scrambled image 13

Supplementary Table 2 Activation likelihood estimation results of “what” vision

Cluster

No.

x y z Anatomic region (AAL) Broadmann area

1 -40 -52 -20 Fusiform_L* 37

-44 -76 -6 Occipital_Mid_L* 19

-46 -68 -14 Temporal_Inf_L 19

-28 -84 -16 Occipital_Inf_L* 18

-30 -96 6 Occipital_Mid_L* 17

Page 11: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

2 40 -48 -22 Fusiform_R* 37

48 -74 -6 Temporal_Mid_R* 19

42 -62 -14 Temporal_Inf_R 37

26 -66 -10 Fusiform_R 19

52 -64 6 Temporal_Mid_R 37

3 44 10 28 Frontal_Inf_Tri_R* 44

4 52 -40 8 Temporal_Sup_R* 21

5 30 -92 2 Occipital_Mid_R* 18

6 20 -6 -16 Hippocampus_R* -

7 -46 4 30 Frontal_Inf_Oper_L* 44

8 -32 24 0 Insula_L* 47

9 -20 -8 -14 Amygdala_L* -

10 12 -92 6 Occipital_Sup_L* 17

11 -56 -46 10 Temporal_Sup_L* 22

12 -10 -96 2 Occipital_Sup_L* 17

13 -26 -70 -10 Fusiform_L* 18

14 -22 -100 -2 Occipital_Mid_L* 17

15 22 -30 0 Thalamus_R* -

16 -38 -46 48 Parietal_Inf_L* 40

17 32 24 -2 Frontal_Inf_Orb_R* 47

18 48 2 52 Precentral_R* 6

19 -42 18 22 Frontal_Inf_Tri_L* 48

20 -26 -62 62 Parietal_Sup_L* 7

21 32 -56 52 Parietal_Inf_R* 7

Page 12: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

22 -28 -70 46 Parietal_Sup_L* 7

23 56 -52 14 Temporal_Sup_R* 21

24 0 22 44 Frontal_Sup_Medial_L

*

32

25 -18 -68 -16 Lingual_L* 18

Statistical level: p< 0.01; coordinates are in MNI spaces

* coordinate chosen for defining the nodes of “where” vision.

L: left; R: right; Inf: inferior; Mid: middle; Sup: superior.

Supplementary Table 3 Activation likelihood estimation results of “where” vision

Cluster

No.

x y z Anatomical region (AAL) Broadmann area

1 -44 -72 -12 Occipital_Inf_L* 19

-46 -72 4 Temporal_Mid_L* 37

-42 -84 4 Occipital_Mid_L 19

-38 -60 -12 Occipital_Inf_L 37

2 52 -68 4 Temporal_Mid_R* 37

44 -70 -12 Temporal_Inf_R* 19

42 -74 -4 Occipital_Mid_R 19

42 -84 2 Occipital_Mid_R 19

36 -84 -14 Occipital_Inf_R 19

3 30 -86 8 Occipital_Mid_R* 18

26 -76 34 Occipital_Sup_R* 19

Page 13: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

34 -78 28 Occipital_Mid_R 19

36 -88 14 Occipital_Mid_R 19

32 -78 12 Occipital_Mid_R 19

26 -92 24 Occipital_Sup_R 18

4 22 -62 58 Parietal_Sup_R* 7

10 -62 60 Precuneus_R 7

28 -54 54 Parietal_Sup_R 7

5 -26 -56 60 Parietal_Sup_L* 7

6 30 4 60 Frontal_Mid_R* 8

7 40 -34 46 Postcentral_R* 2

8 -20 -96 12 Occipital_Sup_L* 17

9 -44 -48 56 Postcentral_L* 40

10 -44 -14 56 Precentral_L* 6

-36 -16 62 Precenral_L* 6

11 -26 -88 28 Occipital_Mid_L* 18

12 -42 -32 58 Postcentral_L* 3

13 -10 -22 46 Cingulum_Mid_L*

14 -38 -48 -22 Fusiform_L* 37

15 30 -58 -20 Fusiform_R* 37

16 -28 -70 42 Parietal_Sup_L* 7

17 40 -46 -26 Fusiform_R* 37

18 -24 -2 52 Frontal_Mid_L* 6

19 38 -44 60 Hippocampus_R* 2

Page 14: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

20 20 -28 0 Thalamus_R*

21 -22 -8 58 Frontal_Sup_L* 6

22 -34 -44 52 Postcentral_L* 40

Statistical level: p< 0.01; coordinates are in MNI spaces

* coordinate chosen for defining the nodes of “where” vision.

L: left; R: right; Inf: inferior; Mid: middle; Sup: superior.

Supplementary figures

Page 15: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

Supplementary figure 1. The ALE map of “what” vision (L, left; R, right).

Supplementary figure 2. The ALE map of “where” vision (L, left; R, right).

References:

[1] M.H. Coletti, H.L. Bleich, Medical subject headings used to search the

biomedical literature, J Am Med Inform Assoc 8 (2001) 317-323.

Page 16: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

[2] J.L. Lancaster, D. Tordesillas-Gutierrez, M. Martinez, F. Salinas, A. Evans, K.

Zilles, J.C. Mazziotta, P.T. Fox, Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates

analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template, Hum Brain Mapp 28 (2007) 1194-

1205.

[3] A.R. Laird, P.M. Fox, C.J. Price, D.C. Glahn, A.M. Uecker, J.L. Lancaster, P.E.

Turkeltaub, P. Kochunov, P.T. Fox, ALE meta-analysis: controlling the false

discovery rate and performing statistical contrasts, Hum Brain Mapp 25 (2005) 155-

164.

[4] M.F. Folstein, S.E. Folstein, P.R. McHugh, “Mini-mental state”: A practical

method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician, J Psychiat Res 12

(1975) 189-198.

[5] R.W. Parks, S.N. Thiyagesh, T.F. Farrow, L. Ingram, K. Wilkinson, M.D. Hunter,

I.D. Wilkinson, C. Young, P.W. Woodruff, Performance on the clock drawing task

correlates with FMRI response to a visuospatial task in Alzheimer's disease, Int J

Neurosci 120 (2010) 335-343.

[6] D.J. Watts, S.H. Strogatz, Collective dynamics of 'small-world' networks, Nature

393 (1998) 440-442.

[7] E.T. Bullmore, D.S. Bassett, Brain graphs: graphical models of the human brain

connectome, Annu Rev Clin Psychol 7 (2011) 113-140.

[8] M.R. Brier, J.B. Thomas, A.M. Fagan, J. Hassenstab, D.M. Holtzman, T.L.

Benzinger, J.C. Morris, B.M. Ances, Functional connectivity and graph theory in

preclinical Alzheimer's disease, Neurobiol Aging 35 (2014) 757-768.

Page 17: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

[9] D.T. Leube, H.W. Yoon, A. Rapp, M. Erb, W. Grodd, M. Bartels, T.T. Kircher,

Brain regions sensitive to the face inversion effect: a functional magnetic resonance

imaging study in humans, Neurosci Lett 342 (2003) 143-146.

[10]G. Badzakova-Trajkov, I.S. Haberling, R.P. Roberts, M.C. Corballis, Cerebral

asymmetries: complementary and independent processes, PLoS One 5 (2010) e9682.

[11]S.A. Love, F.E. Pollick, M. Latinus, Cerebral correlates and statistical criteria of

cross-modal face and voice integration, Seeing Perceiving 24 (2011) 351-367.

[12]P. Vuilleumier, J.L. Armony, J. Driver, R.J. Dolan, Distinct spatial frequency

sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions, Nat Neurosci 6 (2003)

624-631.

[13]R.N. Henson, Y. Goshen-Gottstein, T. Ganel, L.J. Otten, A. Quayle, M.D. Rugg,

Electrophysiological and haemodynamic correlates of face perception, recognition

and priming, Cereb Cortex 13 (2003) 793-805.

[14]N. Furl, N.J. van Rijsbergen, A. Treves, R.J. Dolan, Face adaptation aftereffects

reveal anterior medial temporal cortex role in high level category representation,

Neuroimage 37 (2007) 300-310.

[15]S. Fruhholz, B. Godde, P. Lewicki, C. Herzmann, M. Herrmann, Face recognition

under ambiguous visual stimulation: fMRI correlates of "encoding styles", Hum Brain

Mapp 32 (2011) 1750-1761.

[16]S.M. Platek, S.M. Kemp, Is family special to the brain? An event-related fMRI

study of familiar, familial, and self-face recognition, Neuropsychologia 47 (2009)

849-858.

Page 18: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

[17]B.L. Schwartz, C.J. Vaidya, D. Shook, S.I. Deutsch, Neural basis of implicit

memory for socio-emotional information in schizophrenia, Psychiatry Res 206 (2013)

173-180.

[18]J.E. Joseph, A.D. Gathers, R.S. Bhatt, Progressive and regressive developmental

changes in neural substrates for face processing: testing specific predictions of the

Interactive Specialization account, Dev Sci 14 (2011) 227-241.

[19]M. Jehna, C. Neuper, A. Ischebeck, M. Loitfelder, S. Ropele, C. Langkammer, F.

Ebner, S. Fuchs, R. Schmidt, F. Fazekas, C. Enzinger, The functional correlates of

face perception and recognition of emotional facial expressions as evidenced by

fMRI, Brain Res 1393 (2011) 73-83.

[20]F. Benuzzi, M. Pugnaghi, S. Meletti, F. Lui, M. Serafini, P. Baraldi, P. Nichelli,

Processing the socially relevant parts of faces, Brain Res Bull 74 (2007) 344-356.

[21]B. Rossion, R. Caldara, M. Seghier, A.M. Schuller, F. Lazeyras, E. Mayer, A

network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform

gyrus is necessary for normal face processing, Brain 126 (2003) 2381-2395.

[22]B. Rossion, B. Hanseeuw, L. Dricot, Defining face perception areas in the human

brain: a large-scale factorial fMRI face localizer analysis, Brain Cogn 79 (2012) 138-

157.

[23]M.I. Gobbini, C. Gentili, E. Ricciardi, C. Bellucci, P. Salvini, C. Laschi, M.

Guazzelli, P. Pietrini, Distinct neural systems involved in agency and animacy

detection, J Cogn Neurosci 23 (2011) 1911-1920.

[24]E.A. Hoffman, J.V. Haxby, Distinct representations of eye gaze and identity in

Page 19: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

the distributed human neural system for face perception, Nat Neurosci 3 (2000) 80-84.

[25]D. Pagliaccio, J.L. Luby, M.S. Gaffrey, A.C. Belden, K.N. Botteron, M.P. Harms,

D.M. Barch, Functional brain activation to emotional and nonemotional faces in

healthy children: evidence for developmentally undifferentiated amygdala function

during the school-age period, Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 13 (2013) 771-789.

[26]M. Ramon, L. Dricot, B. Rossion, Personally familiar faces are perceived

categorically in face-selective regions other than the fusiform face area, Eur J

Neurosci 32 (2010) 1587-1598.

[27]L.X. Blonder, C.D. Smith, C.E. Davis, M.L. Kesler-West, T.F. Garrity, M.J.

Avison, A.H. Andersen, Regional brain response to faces of humans and dogs, Brain

Res Cogn Brain Res 20 (2004) 384-394.

[28]J. Liu, J. Tian, J. Li, Q. Gong, K. Lee, Similarities in neural activations of face

and Chinese character discrimination, Neuroreport 20 (2009) 273-277.

[29]T. Orlov, T.R. Makin, E. Zohary, Topographic representation of the human body

in the occipitotemporal cortex, Neuron 68 (2010) 586-600.

[30]O.A. Olulade, D.L. Flowers, E.M. Napoliello, G.F. Eden, Developmental

differences for word processing in the ventral stream, Brain Lang 125 (2013) 134-

145.

[31]J.E. Joseph, M.A. Cerullo, A.B. Farley, N.A. Steinmetz, C.R. Mier, fMRI

correlates of cortical specialization and generalization for letter processing,

Neuroimage 32 (2006) 806-820.

[32]L. Minati, C. Rosazza, I. Zucca, L. D'Incerti, V. Scaioli, M.G. Bruzzone, Spatial

Page 20: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

correspondence between functional MRI (fMRI) activations and cortical current

density maps of event-related potentials (ERP): a study with four tasks, Brain Topogr

21 (2008) 112-127.

[33]M. Szwed, S. Dehaene, A. Kleinschmidt, E. Eger, R. Valabregue, A. Amadon, L.

Cohen, Specialization for written words over objects in the visual cortex, Neuroimage

56 (2011) 330-344.

[34]M. Longcamp, Y. Hlushchuk, R. Hari, What differs in visual recognition of

handwritten vs. printed letters? An fMRI study, Hum Brain Mapp 32 (2011) 1250-

1259.

[35]F. Pegado, K. Nakamura, L. Cohen, S. Dehaene, Breaking the symmetry: mirror

discrimination for single letters but not for pictures in the Visual Word Form Area,

Neuroimage 55 (2011) 742-749.

[36]S. Dehaene, L. Naccache, L. Cohen, D.L. Bihan, J.F. Mangin, J.B. Poline, D.

Riviere, Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming,

Nat Neurosci 4 (2001) 752-758.

[37]S. Dehaene, A. Jobert, L. Naccache, P. Ciuciu, J.B. Poline, D. Le Bihan, L.

Cohen, Letter binding and invariant recognition of masked words: behavioral and

neuroimaging evidence, Psychol Sci 15 (2004) 307-313.

[38]S. Casarotto, A.M. Bianchi, S. Cerutti, N. Vanello, E. Ricciardi, C. Gentili, L.

Sani, D. Bonino, M. Guazzelli, P. Pietrini, L. Landini, G.A. Chiarenza, Combination

of event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging during single-

letter reading, Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 1 (2006) 984-987.

Page 21: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

[39]D.L. Peng, D. Xu, Z. Jin, Q. Luo, G.S. Ding, C. Perry, L. Zhang, Y. Liu, Neural

basis of the non-attentional processing of briefly presented words, Hum Brain Mapp

18 (2003) 215-221.

[40]B.T. Gold, K. Rastle, Neural correlates of morphological decomposition during

visual word recognition, J Cogn Neurosci 19 (2007) 1983-1993.

[41]H. Gros, K. Boulanouar, G. Viallard, E. Cassol, P. Celsis, Event-related

functional magnetic resonance imaging study of the extrastriate cortex response to a

categorically ambiguous stimulus primed by letters and familiar geometric figures, J

Cereb Blood Flow Metab 21 (2001) 1330-1341.

[42]K.H. James, I. Gauthier, Letter processing automatically recruits a sensory-motor

brain network, Neuropsychologia 44 (2006) 2937-2949.

[43]V. Thoma, R.N. Henson, Object representations in ventral and dorsal visual

streams: fMRI repetition effects depend on attention and part-whole configuration,

Neuroimage 57 (2011) 513-525.

[44]T. Yamamoto, S. Takahashi, T. Hanakawa, S. Urayama, T. Aso, H. Fukuyama,

Y. Ejima, Neural correlates of the stereokinetic effect revealed by functional magnetic

resonance imaging, J Vis 8 (2008) 11-14.

[45]K. Podzebenko, G.F. Egan, J.D. Watson, Real and imaginary rotary motion

processing: functional parcellation of the human parietal lobe revealed by fMRI, J

Cogn Neurosci 17 (2005) 24-36.

[46]S.H. Creem-Regehr, V. Dilda, A.E. Vicchrilli, F. Federer, J.N. Lee, The influence

of complex action knowledge on representations of novel graspable objects: evidence

Page 22: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

from functional magnetic resonance imaging, J Int Neuropsychol Soc 13 (2007) 1009-

1020.

[47]J.B. Durand, R. Peeters, J.F. Norman, J.T. Todd, G.A. Orban, Parietal regions

processing visual 3D shape extracted from disparity, Neuroimage 46 (2009) 1114-

1126.

[48]V. Cardin, K.J. Friston, S. Zeki, Top-down modulations in the visual form

pathway revealed with dynamic causal modeling, Cereb Cortex 21 (2011) 550-562.

[49]K.F. Valyear, J.C. Culham, N. Sharif, D. Westwood, M.A. Goodale, A double

dissociation between sensitivity to changes in object identity and object orientation in

the ventral and dorsal visual streams: a human fMRI study, Neuropsychologia 44

(2006) 218-228.

[50]M. Niemeier, H.C. Goltz, A. Kuchinad, D.B. Tweed, T. Vilis, A contralateral

preference in the lateral occipital area: sensory and attentional mechanisms, Cereb

Cortex 15 (2005) 325-331.

[51]M.V. Peelen, A. Caramazza, Conceptual object representations in human anterior

temporal cortex, J Neurosci 32 (2012) 15728-15736.

[52]K. Bucher, T. Dietrich, V.L. Marcar, S. Brem, P. Halder, S. Boujraf, P. Summers,

D. Brandeis, E. Martin, T. Loenneker, Maturation of luminance- and motion-defined

form perception beyond adolescence: a combined ERP and fMRI study, Neuroimage

31 (2006) 1625-1636.

[53]S. Amemiya, K. Ohtomo, Effect of the observed pupil size on the amygdala of

the beholders, Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 7 (2012) 332-341.

Page 23: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

[54]K.G. Claeys, P. Dupont, L. Cornette, S. Sunaert, P. Van Hecke, E. De Schutter,

G.A. Orban, Color discrimination involves ventral and dorsal stream visual areas,

Cereb Cortex 14 (2004) 803-822.

[55]S.E. Leh, A. Ptito, M. Schonwiesner, M.M. Chakravarty, K.T. Mullen, Blindsight

mediated by an S-cone-independent collicular pathway: an fMRI study in

hemispherectomized subjects, J Cogn Neurosci 22 (2010) 670-682.

[56]L. Kaufmann, S.E. Vogel, G. Wood, C. Kremser, M. Schocke, L.B.

Zimmerhackl, J.W. Koten, A developmental fMRI study of nonsymbolic numerical

and spatial processing, Cortex 44 (2008) 376-385.

[57]G.A. Homola, S. Jbabdi, C.F. Beckmann, A.J. Bartsch, A brain network

processing the age of faces, PLoS One 7 (2012) e49451.

[58]C.K. Schraa-Tam, A. van der Lugt, M.A. Frens, M. Smits, P.C. van Broekhoven,

J.N. van der Geest, An fMRI study on smooth pursuit and fixation suppression of the

optokinetic reflex using similar visual stimulation, Exp Brain Res 185 (2008) 535-

544.

[59]Y. Morito, H.C. Tanabe, T. Kochiyama, N. Sadato, Neural representation of

animacy in the early visual areas: a functional MRI study, Brain Res Bull 79 (2009)

271-280.

[60]J.B. Caplan, T.L. Luks, G.V. Simpson, M. Glaholt, A.R. McIntosh, Parallel

networks operating across attentional deployment and motion processing: a multi-

seed partial least squares fMRI study, Neuroimage 29 (2006) 1192-1202.

[61]N. Katsuyama, N. Usui, I. Nose, M. Taira, Perception of object motion in three-

Page 24: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

dimensional space induced by cast shadows, Neuroimage 54 (2011) 485-494.

[62]O.J. Braddick, J.M. O'Brien, J. Wattam-Bell, J. Atkinson, T. Hartley, R. Turner,

Brain areas sensitive to coherent visual motion, Perception 30 (2001) 61-72.

[63]O.J. Braddick, J.M. O'Brien, J. Wattam-Bell, J. Atkinson, R. Turner, Form and

motion coherence activate independent, but not dorsal/ventral segregated, networks in

the human brain, Curr Biol 10 (2000) 731-734.

[64]C. Oreja-Guevara, R. Kleiser, W. Paulus, W. Kruse, R.J. Seitz, K.P. Hoffmann,

The role of V5 (hMT+) in visually guided hand movements: an fMRI study, Eur J

Neurosci 19 (2004) 3113-3120.

[65]A. Santi, P. Servos, E. Vatikiotis-Bateson, T. Kuratate, K. Munhall, Perceiving

biological motion: dissociating visible speech from walking, J Cogn Neurosci 15

(2003) 800-809.

[66]T. Aso, T. Hanakawa, K. Matsuo, K. Toma, H. Shibasaki, H. Fukuyama, T.

Nakai, Subregions of human parietal cortex selectively encoding object orientation,

Neurosci Lett 415 (2007) 225-230.

[67]T. Iwami, Y. Nishida, O. Hayashi, M. Kimura, M. Sakai, K. Kani, R. Ito, A.

Shiino, M. Suzuki, Common neural processing regions for dynamic and static

stereopsis in human parieto-occipital cortices, Neurosci Lett 327 (2002) 29-32.

[68]C.L. Grady, C.J. Mondloch, T.L. Lewis, D. Maurer, Early visual deprivation from

congenital cataracts disrupts activity and functional connectivity in the face network,

Neuropsychologia 57 (2014) 122-139.

[69]J.E. Joseph, A.D. Gathers, Effects of structural similarity on neural substrates for

Page 25: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

object recognition, Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 3 (2003) 1-16.

[70]P. Zeidman, S.L. Mullally, D.S. Schwarzkopf, E.A. Maguire, Exploring the

parahippocampal cortex response to high and low spatial frequency spaces,

Neuroreport 23 (2012) 503-507.

[71]E.M. Fraedrich, S. Glasauer, V.L. Flanagin, Spatiotemporal phase-scrambling

increases visual cortex activity, Neuroreport 21 (2010) 596-600.

[72]S.S. Georgieva, J.T. Todd, R. Peeters, G.A. Orban, The extraction of 3D shape

from texture and shading in the human brain, Cereb Cortex 18 (2008) 2416-2438.

[73]R.K. Kana, Y. Liu, D.L. Williams, T.A. Keller, S.E. Schipul, N.J. Minshew, M.A.

Just, The local, global, and neural aspects of visuospatial processing in autism

spectrum disorders, Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) 2995-3003.

[74]X. Wu, W. Li, M. Zhang, J. Qiu, The neural basis of impossible figures: evidence

from an fMRI study of the two-pronged trident, Neurosci Lett 508 (2012) 17-21.

[75]P. Sterzer, A. Kleinschmidt, A neural signature of colour and luminance

correspondence in bistable apparent motion, Eur J Neurosci 21 (2005) 3097-3106.

[76]G.J. Brouwer, R. van Ee, J. Schwarzbach, Activation in visual cortex correlates

with the awareness of stereoscopic depth, J Neurosci 25 (2005) 10403-10413.

[77]J.S. Cant, M.A. Goodale, Attention to form or surface properties modulates

different regions of human occipitotemporal cortex, Cereb Cortex 17 (2007) 713-731.

[78]F. Schoth, T.D. Waberski, T. Krings, R. Gobbele, H. Buchner, Cerebral

processing of spontaneous reversals of the rotating Necker cube, Neuroreport 18

(2007) 1335-1338.

[79]S.O. Dumoulin, R.F. Hess, Cortical specialization for concentric shape

Page 26: ars.els-cdn.com  · Web viewFor the current study, subjects with glaucoma or congenital blindness were further excluded. Besides, one AD subjects was excluded due to excessive head

processing, Vision Res 47 (2007) 1608-1613.

[80]S. Baecke, R. Lutzkendorf, C. Tempelmann, C. Muller, D. Adolf, M. Scholz, J.

Bernarding, Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (efMRI) of depth-

by-disparity perception: additional evidence for right-hemispheric lateralization, Exp

Brain Res 196 (2009) 453-458.

[81]A. Ritzl, J.C. Marshall, P.H. Weiss, O. Zafiris, N.J. Shah, K. Zilles, G.R. Fink,

Functional anatomy and differential time courses of neural processing for explicit,

inferred, and illusory contours. An event-related fMRI study, Neuroimage 19 (2003)

1567-1577.

[82]T. Negawa, S. Mizuno, T. Hahashi, H. Kuwata, M. Tomida, H. Hoshi, S. Era, K.

Kuwata, M pathway and areas 44 and 45 are involved in stereoscopic recognition

based on binocular disparity, Jpn J Physiol 52 (2002) 191-198.