virginia tech - a comparison of the effects two ......ing about new insights concerning approaches...

170
A COMPARISON OFTHE EFFECTS OF TWO TEACHING STYLES ON TUMBLING SKILL ACQUiSITION OFCOLLEGE STUDENTS by Paulette Walker Johnson Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OFEDUCATION in Curriculum and Instruction APPROVED: MaYgaretL. Driscoll, Chairman Karl Sctiwaab Lawrence Perlmuter May, 1982 Blacksburg, Virginia Robert Foutz Judith Beale

Upload: others

Post on 27-Jan-2021

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • A COMPARISON OF THE EFFECTS OF TWO TEACHING STYLES

    ON TUMBLING SKILL ACQUiSITION OF COLLEGE STUDENTS

    by

    Paulette Walker Johnson

    Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the

    Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University

    in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

    DOCTOR OF EDUCATION

    in

    Curriculum and Instruction

    APPROVED:

    MaYgaret L. Driscoll, Chairman

    Karl Sctiwaab

    Lawrence Perlmuter

    May, 1982

    Blacksburg, Virginia

    Robert Foutz

    Judith Beale

  • DEDICATION

    The dedication of this study is shared by the following 11very

    special 11 people:

    To my dear parents, Elsie and Robert Walker, for their love,

    encouragement, and years of sacrificing fer their children. I love

    you both and deeply appreciate everything you have done for me.

    To my brother, Wayne \~alker, for believing in me and giving me

    . a reason to always do my best. Thank you 111 ittl e brother. 11

    And especially to my wonderful husband and 11fri end, 11 T .A. for

    enduring months of being sorely neglected. It was T.A.1 s untiring

    love, patience, understanding and support that helped to make this

    dream come true.

    ii

  • ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    The author wishes to acknowledge with gratitude the following

    individuals who provided encouragement, motivation, advice, support

    and assistance in the completion of the doctoral program in general

    and this study in particular:

    Dr. Margaret Driscoll, for her role as chairman, teacher and

    friend. Dr. Driscoll patiently and diligently directed me through my

    graduate studies as well as this dissertation. Thank you Dr. 11011 for

    contributing so much to my personal, professional and educational

    growth.

    Dr. Karl Schwaab, Dr. Robert Foutz. Dr. Judith Beale and Dr.

    Lawrence Perlmuter each willingly gave both time and support to this

    study. Their patient understanding and time-consuming appraisals of

    my work proved to be most helpful.

    I am especially grateful to my colleagues and friends, Kat and

    Pete Bennett. It was their encouragement that kept me going through

    difficult times.

    Appreciation is extended to Elizabeth Wood and Harold Hutcherson

    for the assistance they so freely gave during the testing phase of

    this study. I would a.lso like to thank Beth Burch for the hours she

    spent typing; Dr. James Hunter for his cooperation; and Dr. Laverne

    Young for being a true friend.

    iii

  • Table of Contents

    DEDICATION ...

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    LIST OF TABLES

    Chapter

    I.

    II.

    III.

    INTRODUCTION AND PURPOSE.

    Statement of the Problem.

    Justification ..•..

    Statement of the Hypothesis

    Limitations

    Assumptions

    Definition of Terms

    REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE.

    Research on the Spectrum of Teaching Styles . . . . . . . . • . . . . .

    Research on Motor Skill Learning •••

    Research on the Effects of Feedback

    Research Comparing Teacher Centered and Student Centered Styles of Teaching •...

    METHODS AND PROCEDURES

    Subjects ..

    Experimental Period

    Experimental Treatments

    iv

    Page

    ii

    iii

    vii

    1

    3

    3

    4

    4

    5

    5

    8

    8

    12

    15

    19

    22

    22

    23

    23

  • IV.

    V.

    REFERENCES

    APPENDICES

    A.

    B. C.

    D.

    E.

    F.

    G.

    H.

    I.

    Instructional Procedures

    Evaluation Instrument

    Testing Procedures .

    Design of the Study

    Statistical Procedures

    ANALYSIS AND DISCUSSION OF RESULTS

    Statistical Analysis

    Discussion .....

    SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, RECOMMENDATIONS

    Summary

    Conclusions

    Recorrmendations

    ..........................

    SPECTRUM OF TEACHING STYLES

    RECIPROCAL STYLE TASK CARDS

    BACKGROUND DATA FORM ...

    TREATMENT GROUP SCHEDULES

    COMMAND TREATMENT LESSON PLANS

    RECIPROCAL TREATMENT LESSON PLANS

    TUMBLING SKILLS TEST ... . . TUMBLING TEST SCORE SHEETS .

    PANELISTS ......

    V

    Page

    24

    25

    27

    29

    31

    32

    32

    65

    69

    69

    71

    72

    74

    80

    81

    84

    94

    98

    101

    118

    135

    138

    151

  • J.

    K.

    L.

    VITA .

    ABSTRACT

    INTERRATER RELIABILITY COEFFICIENTS

    DIAGRAM OF TESTING PROCEDURES

    DATA ON RATERS ....... .

    vi

    Page

    153

    155

    157

    159

  • LIST OF TABL!:S

    Table Page

    l. Group Means for Pretreatment Scores on Twelve Skills for Groups I and II . 33

    2. ANOVA Summary Table of Headstand Pre-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . . 35

    3. ANOVA Summary Table of Forward Roll Pretreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . 36

    4. ANOVA Summary Table of Forward Roll Without Hands Pretreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . . . . . . . 37

    5. ANOVA Summary Table of Handstand Pre-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . . . 38

    6. ANOVA Summary Table of Backward Roll Pretreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . 39

    7. ANOVA Summary Table of Dive Roll Pretreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . 40

    8. ANOVA Summary Table of Chest Roll Pretreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . 41

    9. ANOVA Summary Table of Cartwheel Pre-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . . . . 42

    l O. ANOVA Summary Table of Back Extension Pretreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . 43

    11. ANOVA Summary Table of Limbre Pre-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . 44

    12. ANOVA Summary Table of Back Halkover Pretreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . 45

    13. ANOVA Summary Table of Roundoff Pre-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . . 46

    14. Group Means of Posttreatment Scores on Twelve Tumbling Stunts for Groups I and II . . . 47

    vii

  • Table Page

    15. ANOVA Summary Table of Headstand Post-treatment Scores for Groups I and II 49

    16. ANOVA Summary Table of Fon•1ard Roll Posttreatment Scores for Groups I and I I . . . . . 50

    17. ANOVA Summary Table of Fonvard Roll Without Hands Posttreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . 51

    18. ANOVA Summary Table of Handstand Post-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . . 52

    19. ANOVA Summary Table of Backward Roll Posttreatment Scores for Groups I and II . . 53

    20. ANOVA Summary Table of Dive Roll Post-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . 54

    21. ANOVA Summary Table of Chest Roll Post~ treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . . 55

    22. ANOVA Summary Table of Cartwheel Post-treatment Scores for Groups I and TI . . . . . . 56

    23. ANOVA Summary Table of Back Extension Posttreatment Scores for Groups I and I I . . . 57

    24. ANOVA Summary Table of Limbre Post-treatment Scores for Groups I and TI . . . 58

    25. ANOVA Summary Table of Back Walkover Post-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . . 59

    26. ANOVA Summary Table of Roundoff Post-treatment Scores for Groups I and II . 60

    27. Comparison of Group Mean Scores on Pretreatment and Posttreatment Tumbling Tests for Groups I and II . . . . . 61

    28. ANOVA Summary Table of Post~reatment Mean Scores for Groups I and II . . . . 63

    29. Mean and Standard Deviation Scores on Tumbling Skill Tests for Command and Reciprocal Groups . . . 64

    viii

  • Chapter I

    introduction and Purpose

    Decisions that confront educators are often varied, complex and

    far reaching in importance, but none outweighs in significance those

    decisions governing selection of an effective teaching method. Vari-

    ous approaches to instruction have received criticism from several

    sources. As a result of complaints and criticisms lodged against

    instructional methodology, educators have sought to improve methods

    by which they can effectively facilitate learninq (Hoffman, 1971;

    Robinson, 1970; Silberman, 1970).

    In the area of physical education, the outcome of one educator's

    desire to improve teaching methodology was the emergence of a spectrum

    of teaching styles. Developed by Mosston (1972), the spectrum of

    teaching styles is a model that offers educators a range of alter-

    native teaching methods. According to Mosston (1972) each teaching

    style on the spectrum has a unique approach to the teaching-learning

    process, and each style is built upon the axiom that teaching behavior

    is a chain of decision making. In each teaching style decisions con-

    cerning the teaching-learning process are separated into three stages:

    pre-impact or planning decisions; impact or execution decisions; and

    post-impact or assessment decisions. The actual structure of each

    teaching method on the spectrum is determined by who, the teacher or

    the learner, makes which decisions.

    l

  • 2

    Teaching methods at one end of the spectrum are characterized by

    complete teacher decision making during each stage. Hoffman (1971)

    and Hellison (1973) maintain that such methods have a long history of

    use in physical education. At the other end of the spectrum, the

    teaching methods are characterized by complete learner decision making

    during each stage. Instructional strategies associated with these

    methods have been referred to as individualized instruction. Accord-

    ing to Mosston (1972), decision making progressively shifts along the

    spectrum from the teacher to the learner to fonn seven distinct teach-

    ing styles.

    Regardless of the teaching style, it is apparent that the value of

    a style lies in its ability to facilitate learning. Mosston (1972)

    postulates that the value of each style on his spectrum can be evalu-

    ated by the extent of its contribution to four developmental channels:

    physical development, emotional development, social development, and

    intellectual development.

    A number of physical educators have chosen to use teaching styles

    which are learner centered while others are comfortable utilizing teach-

    ing styles which are teacher centered. Although present educational

    trends tend to stress the basic concept of individualized attention

    and student involvement, the large size of most physical education ac-

    tivity classes seem to encourage the more teacher centered styles of

    teaching (Hoffman, 1971). According to Mosstonrs (1972) assessment,

    the teaching styles on his spectrum characterized by complete teacher

    decision making contribute less to the four developmental channels

  • 3

    than those characterized by complete learner decision making.

    The purpose of this study therefore was to compare two of Mosston•s

    (1972) teaching styles and their effect on learning along the physical

    developmental channel. Specifically, this study investigated the

    effects of the command and reciprocal teaching styles, as conceptua-

    lized by Mosston (1972), on the acquisition of twelve tumbling skills

    by college level students.

    Statement of the Problem

    The problem under investigation was to compare the effects of

    command and reciprocal teaching styles on the acquisition of tumbling

    skills. The specific question addressed in this study was: Does the

    command style, in comparison to the reciprocal style, more effectively

    facilitate the learning of tumbling skills?

    The problem of selecting the most effective teaching style for

    teaching motor skills is not, a new one. Valid concerns for student

    betterment have been raised by dedicated physical educators for a

    number of years. However, a satisfactory resolvement still remains.

    This study is one effort toward a satisfactory resolvement.

    Justification

    Although innovative approaches to education appear to have sig-

    nificant implications for motor skill instruction, the justification

    for the present study is substantiated in the need to evaluate the

    merits of these approaches. There is also a need for answers toques-

    tions concerning the extent of effectiveness found in innovative

  • 4

    approaches to motor skill instruction.

    A number of physical educators have suggested there is a need for

    research that focuses directly on the teaching-learning environment

    (Anderson, 1975; Siedentop, 1976; Locke, 1977). These educators postu-

    late that increased infonnation on teaching and learning can be helpful

    in the future preparation of physical education teachers. In support

    of more research in the area of teaching methodology, Milton (1973)

    states: 11It is research that can guide us out of the overgrown

    thicket of dogma which surrounds current ideas about instruction and

    1earning 11 (p. ix). This study should contribute, therefore, to bring-

    ing about new insights concerning approaches to the teaching of tumb-

    ling skills at the college level.

    Statement of the Hypothesis

    The null hypothesis with respect to the purpose of this study was:

    There will be no significant difference between the mean tumbling

    skill test scores achieved by the two experimental treatment groups.

    Limitations

    This study was limited to the following:

    l. The subjects were 28 female and 23 male undergraduate students

    enrolled in two sections of G.E. 165--Fundamentals of Physical Educa-

    tion, Fall semester, 1981, at Virginia State University, Petersburg,

    Virginia.

    2. Treatments were administered in two fifty-minute sessions per

    week for a period of six weeks.

  • 5

    3. The investigator was the regular classroom teacher assigned

    to both sections of G.E. 165--Fundamentals of Physical Education.

    4. The test used to measure tumbling skill acquisition was a

    modification of a skill test used by Jarvis (1967).

    Assumptions

    The following assumptions were made with regard to the purpose of this study:

    1. It was assumed that the subjects did not participate outside

    of class in activities similar to those being taught and tested in

    this study.

    2. It was assumed that the subjects performed to the best of

    their abilities on the skill tests.

    Definition of Terms

    For the purpose of clarification, the following terms are defined

    as they relate to this study:

    Spectrum of Teaching Styles. This is a theoretical model of seven

    teaching styles developed by Mosston (1972). For more specific in-

    formation about each teaching style in the spectrum, consult Appen-

    dix A.

    Command Teaching Style. This is the first teaching style on Moss-

    ton's (1972) spectrum. It is the teachinq style in which the teacher

    is the central dominant figure. Command style instruction assumes

    that learning takes place under a series of directions given by the

    classroom teacher.

  • 6

    Reciprocal Teachina Stvle. This is the third teaching style on

    Mosston's (1972) spectrum. In this style, the classroom teacher

    determines and designs specific tasks in the content to be learned.

    The tasks are then presented to the students in the form of task

    cards (see Appendix B). Following the teacher's explanation and

    demonstration of a task, the students form dyads. One member of a

    dyad (the doer) performs the task, while the remaining member (the

    teacher-partner) offers feedback based on criteria established and

    outlined by the teacher.

    Teacher-partner. The member of a reciprocal teaching style dyad

    who offered feedback to the doer. The teacher-partner was required

    to watch the performance of a partner, compare the performance to

    specific criteria, draw conclusions about the quality of the perfor-

    mance, and communicate the information back to the partner who per-

    formed the task (Mosston, 1972).

    Doer. The member of a reciprocal teaching style dyad who was --performing a task (Mosston, 1972).

    Feedback. This term was defined as the information a doer received

    from a teacher-partner during the reciprocal teachinq process (Mosston,

    1972).

    Teacher-centered Teaching Style. This includes any pedagogical

    strategy in which the teacher instructed the same skills to all stu-

    dents at the same time, by the same method, and required all students

    to practice in the same way, at the same pace, and for the same length

    of time (Siedentop, 1976).

  • 7

    Student-centered- Teaching Style. This included any pedagogical

    strategy which provided opportunities for students to progress at

    their own rate as they acquired various skills (Siedentop, 1976).

  • Chapter II

    Review of Related Literature

    The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of corranand

    and reciprocal teaching styles on the tumbling skill acquisition of

    college students. The literature found to be pertinent to this study

    was reviewed in the following areas:

    1. Research on the spectrum of teaching styles.

    2. Research on motor skill learning.

    3. Research on the effects of feedback on motor skill learning.

    4. Research comparing teacher-centered and student-centered

    styles of teaching.

    Research on the Spectrum of Teaching Styles

    Teaching styles included in Mosston's (1972) spectrum have been

    investigated in several studies utilizing a variety of motor skills.

    Mariani (1970), using two groups of male college students, found the

    task teaching style superior to the command teaching style in the

    teaching of the backhand tennis stroke, but found no significant

    difference between the two styles in the teaching of the forehand

    stroke. Two treatment groups were given 6 weeks of tennis instruc-

    tion. One group was instructed by the task method and the other group

    was instructed by the command method. Both groups met for 1 hour of

    tennis instruction twice a week for a total of 12 hours. Subjects in

    both groups were given a tennis skills test at the beginning and at

    8

  • 9

    the end of the course. Mariani (1970) administered a posttest sixty

    days later to measure the subjects• retention of forehand and backhand

    tennis achievement. Both groups showed significant improvement from

    the beginning of the tennis course to the end; however, the group in-

    structed by the task method reported retention that was significantly

    greater at the .01 level than did the command instructed group.

    Dougherty (1970) studied the effects of corrnnand, task and indi-

    vidual program styles on the development of physical fitness and motor

    skill. Six classes of male college students served as subjects for

    his investigation. Dougherty randomly assigned two classes to each of

    the three teaching styles. The classes were instructed using their

    respective teaching styles during their bi-weekly meetings for a period

    of fourteen weeks. A five item fitness test and a test on movement

    skills were administered to all subjects during the 1st, 7th, and 14th

    week of the treatment period. Interaction analysis observations were

    also made on the subjects during the 2nd, 6th, and 12th weeks of the

    treatment period. In general, no significant differences were found

    between the three teaching styles. However, the interaction analysis

    observations revealed that the task and individual program styles de-

    veloped student independence and allowed for more teacher attention

    than did the command style. Dougherty concluded by suggesting that

    the choice of a teaching style should depend on factors such as per-

    sonality, time, and objectives and goals sought.

    Boschee (1972) also compared the effects of command, task and

    individual program styles of teaching. Unlike Dougherty (1970),

  • 10

    Boschee used fifth grade students as his subjects. In the Boschee

    study. 221 subjects were divided into six treatment groups. The sub-

    jects were evaluated on acquisition of specific skills in alley soccer,

    a game knowledge test on alley soccer, and personal and social adjust-

    ment. Each of the six treatment groups participated in 25 minutes of

    activity, three times weekly, for a period of 8 weeks. Boschee re-

    ported that the styles of teaching he compared were not significantly

    different from one another in terms of overall improvement between the

    pre- and post-battery of alley soccer skill tests, nor in terms of

    personal and social adjustment.

    Another study comparing spectrum teaching styles was conducted

    by Bryant (1974). Bryant studied the effects of the practice and

    reciprocal teaching styles using fifth and sixth grade males as sub-

    jects. He trained two groups of the subjects, one group to each

    teaching style, for a period of 9 weeks. The groups were compared in

    three areas: (1) ability to perform seven vaults to evaluate skill

    development; {2) a tumbling task to evaluate cooperation between the

    subjects; and (3) knowledge of gymnastics. Bryant reported no sig-

    nificant difference between the groups on motor skill development or

    the cooperation they exhibited. He did note a significant difference

    in knowledge gained in favor of the practice teaching style.

    Jacoby (1975) investigated the effects of command, reciprocal and

    individual program styles of teaching on knowledge,skill and attitude

    in the area of softball. The population included three sequentially

    taught intact classes of ten to thirteen year olds in each of six

  • 11

    schools in Ohio. Student-teachers from Ohio University and their

    cooperating teachers instructed the classes. At the end of five

    weeks, tests on knowledge of softball, skiil in pitching, accuracy

    in throwing, and attitude towards physical education were adminis-

    tered. A posttest only research design was utilized. Jacoby reported

    no significant differences between the teaching styles he investi-

    gated as measured by the subjects' sport skill, knowledge and atti-

    tude.

    Gerney (1979) researched the effects of the practice style and

    the reciprocal style on skill acquisition and social development of

    fifth grade students. Skill acquisition was measured by the Skinner

    Hockey Task and social development was measured by an adaptation of

    Interaction Analysis. Gerney randomly assigned 16 males and 16 fe-

    males to each of two treatment groups. Subjects in each group were

    instructed during their regular physical education period in the

    teaching style to which they were assigned. A pre- post test proce-

    dure was utilized. Results revealed no significant difference between

    the two teaching styles with regard to skill acquisition.; however,

    the reciprocal treatment group proved significantly superior to the

    practice style group in the area of social development as defined by

    Gerney.

    Virgilio (1979) tested command and reciprocal teaching strategies

    to determine their effect on cognitive, affective, and psychomotor

    behavior in beginning archery. The subjects were forty-six fifth

  • 12

    grade pupils. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of four groups

    and the groups to one of two treatments. Subjects were instructed by

    each teaching style for a period of six weeks, meeting three times per

    week for thirty minutes each session. The results of the study indi-

    cated there were no significant differences between the teaching

    styles in developing archery knowledge, archery skill, or self-concept.

    Experimental studies previously conducted which compared teaching

    styles on Mosston's (1972) spectrum tend to provide little evidence

    that one style is more effective than another with regard to motor

    skill acquisition. While these studies fail to make a favorable case

    for the efficiency of any specific teaching style over another, they

    do reveal the inconclusive nature of much of the evidence obtained.

    Research on Motor Skill Learning

    Scholars representing diverse professional backgrounds have made

    contributions to the body of knowledge concerning the nature of motor

    skill learning. Lawther (1968), for example, included the following

    among his principles of motor learning: a) skill teaching should

    begin at whatever level the student has already attained and proceed

    from that point; and b) the skill assignments must be neither too

    easy nor too difficult.

    The importance of reinforcement in promoting motor skill learning

    has been the focus of a number of experiments conducted by Skinner

    {1969). Skinner's theory of operant conditioning requires i:hat rein-

    forcement be given to a subject for a general response in the direc-

    tion of a desired response. According to Skinner, the learner, instead

  • 13

    of achieving a desired skill by trial and error, should be guided

    toward the correct response through constant reinforcement.

    Robb (1972) has identified three phases of skill learning she

    considers important to optimal motor skill perfonnance: 1) plan for-

    mation or the executive plan, 2) practice, and 3) automatic execution.

    In the first phase the learner gets a broad picture of the skill.

    According to Robb, this should be followed by objectives, goals, and

    a proper demonstration of the task. The second phase consists of

    meaningful practice with appropriate feedback. The final phase of

    learning is an automatic execution of the first two phases. Robb

    stresses the importance of instructors being aware of all three phases

    and what effect the phases have on the learner's perfonnance.

    Gentile (1972) maintained that in motor skill learning, the ap-

    propriate teaching strategy should be matched with the nature of the

    skill. Gentile found that characteristics of motor learning directly

    influence stragegies employed by the teacher. For example, a teacher

    may use one strategy to facilitate motor skill acquisition in gymnas-

    tics and use a completely different strategy when teaching soccer.

    According to Gentile, the nature of a teacher's behavior is very much

    detennined by the type of skill to be learned.

    Based on their findings in the area of learning and perfonnance

    of motor skills, Singer and Gaines (1975) suggested two major cate-

    gories of variables that may generally be thought of as affecting the

    status of the learner: (1) personal, and (2) environmental and in-

    structional influences. With regard to the personal variable, Singer

  • 14

    and Gaines argue that body build, early childhood experiences, and

    specific skill practice all have a high correlation with success in

    motor skill learning. Other critical qualities of success in motor

    skill learning noted by the authors inc1ude personality, heredity and

    environment, sensory abilities, intelligence, attitudes, emotions, and

    aspirations.

    Singer (1975) refers to any task a learner is confronted with as

    that learner's display. He indicated that the teacher is external to

    the learner's display, but serves as a potential display manipulator.

    The teacher, according to Singer, has the potential to make the display

    easier to master for the learner by manipulating the display in one or

    all of the following ways: (1) change the atmosphere to enhance

    variety and cut down on boredom; (2) offer visual cues, especially

    during early stages of skill learning; (3) motivate, reinforce and

    direct behavior through the use of visual feedback; and (4) adapt

    displays to artificial situations or simulated equipment.

    In a discussion of the effects of anxiety and tension on motor

    skill learning, Singer (1975) argues that moderate stress, anxiety,

    and tension are necessary factors for optimal motor skill perfonnance.

    However, he further notes that too much of these factors tend to

    impede learning and performance of motor skills. Singer also main-

    tains that desirable arousal conditions are different from task to

    task and from learner to learner.

    The literature reviewed in the area of motor skill learning pro-

    vided theory and opinion. The review did not, however, result in any

  • 15

    conclusive findings with regard to the relationship between motor

    skill learning and styles of teaching.

    Research on the Effect of Feedback on

    Motor Skill Learning

    The reciprocal teaching style investigated in the present study

    is structured in a manner which provides for immediate feedback and

    knowledge of results. Therefore, a review of literature in this area

    seemed warranted.

    Bilodeau and Bilodeau (1961) reported that feedback is one of the

    strongest and most important variables controlling performance and

    learning. This finding was supported by Robb (1968) when she found

    that the key to effective learning of a specific movement pattern was

    practice, coupled with feedback information. Robb also suggested that

    concurrent visual feedback was the most important variable for learn-

    ing the movement response. Thompson (1969) also garnered support for

    Bilodeau and Bilodeau's (1961) assertion when he reported that immedi-

    ate feedback through the use of a graph check sequence method of

    teaching golf, facilitated the learning of selected golf skills.

    Penman (1969) reported results that conflict with those of Bilo-

    deau and Bilodeau, 1961; Robb, 1968; and Thompson, 1969. Penman

    studied the relative effectiveness of teaching beginning tumbling with

    and without feedback in the form of an instant replay videotape re-

    corder. The subjects in his study were 130 college freshmen. Groups

    of 25 students were randomly assigned as the control and experimental

    groups. These groups were taught in the same room, with the same

  • 16

    instructor, and at the same hour of the day. Both groups were taught

    the same content; however, the experimental group had use of an instant

    replay videotape recorder. The study lasted for 12 weeks or 24 in-

    struction periods of approximately 35 minutes each. The posttest only

    design Penman employed, required that each student made three passes

    on a tumbling mat executing the tumbling stunts learned. The perfor-

    mances were evaluated by a four-man jury. Results revealed there was

    no benefit in providing feedback in the form of an instant replay

    videotape recorder to teach beginning tumbling skills to Washington

    State University freshmen students. A slight difference in mean per-

    formance scores was noted in favor of the control group but that dif-

    ference was not significant.

    Meyers (1972) reported results similar to those of Penman (1969).

    Meyers compared four methods of feedback in programmed instruction

    for teaching archery. The subjects were 100 male high school freshmen.

    Each subject was randomly assigned to a treatment group. The methods

    of feedback used were: (1) basic program with no teacher assistance;

    (2) basic program with verbal feedback; (3) basic program with video

    feedback; and (4) basic program with verbal and video feedback. The

    AAHPER Archery Skills Test was administered before and after a three-

    week practice period. The results indicated no significant differences

    among the improvement scores of the groups.

    Sinclair (1972) investigated the effect of immediate visual feed-

    back upon the learning of a selected track and field skill. His sub-

    jects were 78 seventh grade boys. The subjects were randomly assigned

    to three treatment groups and were administered treatment for six

  • 17

    class sessions. One group was designated the control group and re-

    ceived conventional instruction. The second group (EGl) received

    videotape-no replay treatment. The third group (EG2) received com-

    plete visual feedback of performance to complement the conventional

    verbal analysis and correction. Experimental group one (EGl)

    was videotaped but visual feedback was withheld from the instruc-

    tional process. Although no significant differences were found between

    group performances, Sinclair reported that the group which received

    the videotape-with replay treatment, realized quicker progress between

    sessions 2 and 6 than did the group taught in the conventional manner.

    Leah's (1973) findings lend no support to Meyer's (1972) or Sin-

    clair1s (1972) study. Leah studied the effects of instant vid~otape

    feedback in learning target archery. Ninety female volunteers from

    Glassboro State College served as subjects. The subjects formed three

    beginning archery classes that met twice a week for seven weeks. The

    subjects were assigned at random to three treatment groups. One group

    received standard lecture and demonstration, another group received

    standard lecture and demonstration with videotape feedback immediately

    following their performance. A third group received standard lecture

    and demonstration with feedback after every other class period. Leah

    used the Modified Chicago Round as his pre- and posttest measure.

    His findings indicated all three methods increased skill level and

    form however, the videotape immediate feedback group showed the high-

    est gains.

    Tobey (1974) studied feedback in order to determine what type of

  • 18

    feedback actually occurs in physical education activity classes. He

    described feedback to be mostly verbal, directed toward a single student

    and often non-specific. He also indicated that patterns of feedback

    varied with the teacher, the age of the learner, and the skills being

    taught.

    Fuey, Sandargas, and Bushell (1975) investigated two types of

    feedback in the teaching of two swimming skills to handicapped children.

    The types of feedback they studied were non-task specific praise and

    task specific praise plus correction. The subjects were grouped in

    pairs, and each member of a pair received the alternate type of feed-

    back for the same skill. In all cases, the researchers noted faster

    and more complete acquisition resulted from task specific praise plus

    correction.

    Catano (1975) used sixty volunteer undergraduates from St. Mary's

    University as subjects when he investigated the relation of improved

    perfonnance through verbal praise to source of praise. The three

    sources of praise used in the study were: (1) peer praise, (2) experi-

    menter praise, (3) control condition or no praise. The results indi-

    cated that the use of verbal praise can improve perfonnance in a

    mirror tracing task when it is administered by a source of authority.

    Newell and Chew (1975) found, in a study involving visual feed-

    back and positioning. movements, that visual distance infonnation seems

    more useful than location infonnation. They concluded that the more

    visual feedback a subject is given, the more accurate his perfonnance

    will be.

  • 19

    Feedback has received attention from researchers concerned with

    variables that can affect learning. There is, however, conflict among

    the literature with regard to the impact feedback has on the learning

    of a motor task. In general, the literature reviewed in the area of

    feedback suggested that feedback can be valuable in the learning of a

    motor task. However, the literature clearly suggests that feedback

    must be immediate and specific to the skills involved in order to be

    of any significant value to the learner. The literature also points

    out that visual feedback has a greater effect on motor achievement

    than verbal feedback.

    Research Comparing Teacher-centered and

    Student-centered Teaching Styles

    Studies investigating teaching methods in physical education have

    generally compared a teacher-centered style of teaching with a student-

    centered style. Keller (1963), for example, used secondary school

    males in a comparison of teaching styles. In Keller's study, the

    student-centered style was described as a program consisting of one

    instructional class and two laboratory periods. During the laboratory

    periods, the student was expected to achieve specific goals without

    teacher assistance. The teacher was available for advice and guidance

    during the laboratory periods in Keller's teacher-centered style.

    Keller reported no significant difference between the two methods

    with respect to physical fitness or skill development.

    Schlott (1970) also compared a teacher-centered and a student-

    centered style of teaching physical education. Schlott's teacher-

  • 20

    centered style consisted of explanation and demonstration by the

    teacher and practice of the skills by the students. After the skills

    of a particular activity were presented and practiced, they were used

    in lead up games. The student-centered style in Schlott's study was

    defined in terms of a problem-solving technique. The teacher, in the

    student-centered style, conducted class activity by verbally struc-

    turing problems to which each student was expected to react on an

    individual basis. Students were engaged in self-testing situations

    during the second half of the problem-solving lesson. Schlott reported

    no difference between the two methods with regard to acquisition of

    field hockey skills for college females.

    In a study which utilized female college freshmen as subjects,

    Croom (1971) compared a teacher-centered style of instruction with a

    movement exploration style in non-major, physical education skills

    classes. In Cream's movement exploration style, students were en-

    couraged to self-discover the best means of moving their bodies to

    accomplish certain goals. Croom described her teacher-centered teach-

    ing style as being traditional in nature, characterized by explanation,

    demonstration, and execution. Considering the results of the Scott

    Motor Ability test, Croom concluded that the teacher-centered style she

    employed was more effective in improving general motor ability than

    was the movement exploration style.

    Slooten and Kneer (1976) conducted a study which compared handball

    knowledge and skill of students taught by three different teaching

    styles. How each student was to learn, to practice, and to be evaluated

  • 21

    were the variables for determining the different styles of teaching.

    Two of the tested styles were teacher-centered, differing only with

    respect to the students' ability to contract for the grade of their

    choice. The third style was student-centered and was described as an

    independent study which permitted students to set their own goals as

    well as contract for the grade of their choice. Slooten and Kneer

    found no significant differences in either knowledge or skill acqui-

    sition between the three classes of freshmen male students when taught

    by the different methods.

    Thaxton and others (1977) reported that the student-centered group

    they studied was significantly better when compared to the teacher-

    centered group in selected skills of gymnastics and tumbling. The

    researchers used fourth grade girls for a period of four weeks to

    compare the effectiveness of a teacher-centered, explanation, demon-

    stration, execution style of teaching and a student-centered, self-

    directed style.

    Empirical research reviewed in the area of styles of teaching

    physical education activities has provided varied results. Some results

    lend support to the contention that no one style of teaching is more

    effective than another while other results support the antithesis of

    that contention.

  • Chapter III

    Methods and Procedures

    The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of command

    and reciprocal teaching styles on the acquisition of twelve tumbling

    skills by college level students. Procedures used in the collection

    and treatment of the data for this study will be described under the

    following headings: 1) subjects; 2) experimental period; 3) experi-

    mental treatments; 4) instructional procedures; 5) evaluation instru-

    ment; 6) testing procedures; 7) design of the study; and 8) statistical

    procedures.

    Subjects

    The subjects for this study were fifty-one coeds from Virginia

    State University in Petersburg, Virginia. These students were enrolled

    in two sections of G.E. 165--Fundamentals of Physical Education during

    the Fall semester of the 1981-82 academic year. G.E. 165--Fundamentals

    of Physical Education is an elective course that offers one academic

    credit. The subjects were assigned to either one or the other sections

    of the class according to their choice during pre-registration or

    registration for the Fall semester. Permission to use the students as

    subjects and to conduct the research was granted by Dr. James E. Hunter,

    Chairman of the Department of Health, Physical Education and Recrea-

    tion at Virginia State University.

    22

  • 23

    A background data form, constructed by the investigator (see

    Appendix C), was completed by the students in both class sections prior

    to the experimental period. The information gathered from the form

    aided the investigator in determining how each student rated himself

    or herself as a tumbler and the amount of tumbling instruction each

    subject had previously encountered. Students who had previously re-

    ceived tumbling instruction and who indicated a rating of very good

    or excellent were eliminated from the study.

    Experimental Period

    The study was conducted during the F_a 11 semester of the 1981-82

    academic year. The experimental period consisted of twelve, fifty-

    minute class meetings over a period of six weeks. Treatments were

    administered during regularly scheduled class meetings for both class

    sections of G.E. 165--Fundamentals of Physical Education. Section

    one met on Mondays and Wednesdays at 8:00 a.m. and section two met

    on Mondays and Wednesdays at 9:00 a.m. Both classes were conducted

    in the East Wing of Daniel Gymnasium. The experimental period fol-

    lowed the schedule outlined and included as Appendix o.

    Experimental Treatments

    The experimental groups used in this study were two intact physi-

    cal education activity classes. Popham and Sirotnik (1973), in sup-

    port of intact groups, pointed out the following consideration:

    Disregarding the fact that often the only available source for the appropriate student sample will be found in school classrooms, there is a decided advantage in using realistic school situations to investigate relationships between

  • 24

    educational variables. Typically, an investigator wishes to generalize research findings to real school situations, so his research is frequently (but not always) most genera-lizable when the investigation is conducted in the milieu of an authentic school environment. (p. 204)

    The use of intact classes precluded the random assignment of

    subjects to the two treatment groups. Therefore, the investigator

    designated the class section which met at 8:00 a.m. as treatment group

    one and the section which met at 9:00 a.m. as treatment group two.

    Following the designation of class sections to groups, either the

    command or the reciprocal treatment was randomly assigned to a group

    by the toss of a coin. Group one subjects (8:00 a.m.) were assigned

    the command style treatment and group two subjects -(9:00 a.m.) were

    ~signed the reciprocal style treatment.

    Students in both treatment .groups were instructed during their

    regular class period, in the teaching style to which they were as-

    signed. The content for implementing the two teaching styles was

    specified in twelve daily lesson plans for each group. The lesson

    plans contained time allotments for each segment of a lesson, as well

    as, exact procedures to be followed for preparation, warm-up, skill

    instruction and skill application (see Appendices E and F).

    Instructional Procedures

    Both experimental classes of G.E. 165--Fundamentals of Physical

    Education were taught by the investigator utilizing the exact proce-

    dures specified in the daily lesson plans. The investigator was also

    the regular classroom teacher assigned to those sections by the

    department chairman at Virginia State University. In support of

  • 25

    regular personnel conducting research in education, Campbell and

    Stanley (1963) advocate the following:

    ... An alternative model is for the idea for classroom research to originate with teachers and other school personnel, with designs to test these ideas worked out cooperatively with specialists in research methodology, and then for the bulk of the experimentation to be carried out by the idea producers themselves. (p. 21)

    Both classes included in the study met two times for fifty minutes

    per class meeting prior to the experimental period. The subjects had

    no knowledge of the experimental nature of the classes. The instruc-

    tional procedures during the two class meetings prior to the experi-

    mental period followed the outline below:

    1st Meeting: Course orientation including a discussion of teacher

    and student expectations, course requirements and classroom procedures.

    2nd Meeting: Review of course orientation; completion of back-

    ground data form; description of procedures to following during pre-

    testing.

    The two initial class meetings for both sections were not in-

    cluded in the experimental period in an effort to allow class enroll-

    ments to stabilize. Instruction after the two initial meetings fol-

    lowed the schedule outlined in Appendix D.

    Evaluation Instrument

    The tumbling skills test used in this study was devised by the

    investigator (see Appendix G). It is a modification of the rating

    scale used by Jarvis (1967) in a study of self-instructive materials

    and motor skills.

  • 26

    The test utilized two gymnastic judges to rate twelve tumbling

    stunts on a 0-5 point scale. A subject was given one point for each

    component of a skill that was performed correctly. The subjects in

    this study could receive a maximum of two points for each component of

    a skill they correctly performed, as the two judges• ratings were

    combined. Therefore, the highest score possible for each student was

    the total of the five segments times two or ten points. Subjects

    were allowed one attempt to perform each stunt according to the pre-

    determined criteria. The judges independently scored videotaped per-

    formances of the students using the point criteria designated for each

    stunt on separate score sheets (see AppendixH ).

    Face validity was claimed for each of the twelve items contained

    in the tumbling skills test used in this study. For example, the

    headstand was a test that measured one's ability to stand on one's

    head. As a result, no statistical analysis using a criterion measure

    was employed to establish validity for the twelve test items. The

    performance criterion was reviewed by a panel made up of one gymnastic

    instructor and three collegiate gymnasts (see Appendix I). After

    minor adjustments and revisions, the panel agreed that the skills test

    would measure what it purported to measure.

    Reliability of the tumbling skills test was determined by ad-

    ministering the test to fourteen college students enrolled in Gymnas-

    tics and Tumbling class during the Spring quarter, 1981 at Virginia

    Polytechnic Institute and State University in Blacksburg, Virginia.

    Each student was videotaped while performing the twelve tumbling

  • 27

    stunts included in this study. Two collegiate gymnasts not involved

    with the complete study, rated the performances of the students using

    criteria identical to that \'lhich was used in the completed study.

    The Pearson Product-Moment correlation technique was utilized in

    establishing an interrater reliability coefficient of +.82 for the

    test-retest scores of the tumbling skills test. Interrater reliabil-

    ity coefficients were also calculated for each of the twelve stunts

    included in the completed investigation utilizing the same correla-

    tion technique. The reliability estimates calculated on the two

    raters I scores used in the completed study, ranged from a pas ft i ve

    .58 to a positive .96 (see Appendix J).

    Testing Procedures

    The pretest was administered to the subjects in both treatment

    groups during the two class meetings that immediately preceded the

    experimental period. The pretests were administered to determine

    initial homogeneity of skill level between the two groups. A posttest

    was administered to both groups during the two class meetings imme-

    diately following the experimental period. The posttests were ad-

    ministered to determine the tumbling skills acquisition of the

    subjects in each group. Subjects absent during either the pre- or

    posttesting session were eliminated from the study.

    Both pre- and posttesting sessions were supervised by the inves-

    tigator with the assistance of two physical education instructors,

    two video equipment operators and three physical education major stu-

    dents. The instructors assisted the investigator by making sure all

  • 28

    subjects understood and followed the testing procedures. The two video

    equipment operators assembled and operated all of the video taping

    equipment. The three students assisted by spotting and demonstrating

    the stunts during the testing sessions.

    Stunt performances by all subjects during the pre- and posttesting

    sessions were recorded on videotapes. Two portable television cameras

    and two videotape recorders were utilized. The sessions were video-

    taped in order to permit the judges to rate the subjects' performances

    in each group without having any knowledge of the group's experimental

    treatment.

    All subjects were tested during the time allotted for testing.

    The subjects in each group were alphabetically assigned a number. The

    subjects were then separated into two columns according to odd or even

    numbers. Subjects in the column with even numbers were instructed to

    assemble adjacent to camera one. Subjects in the column with odd

    numbers assembled adjacent to camera two. All subjects simultaneously

    observed a demonstration of each stunt before being allowed to at-

    tempt it during the pretests, but were not provided with demonstra-

    tions during the posttests. Following each demonstration, subjects

    in numerical order according to columns were given one attempt to

    perform the stunt. A diagram of the testing procedures is included

    as Appendix K.

    Two physical education teachers with experience in teaching and

    coaching gymnastics served as raters for both the pretest and the

    posttest (see Appendix L). Both raters participated in a rating

  • 29

    practice session held one week prior to the administration of the pre-

    tests. During the practice session, each rater was given an oppor-

    tunity to discuss the performance criteria established by the investi-

    gator for each stunt. Details with regard to testing and scoring

    procedures were delineated, but details relative to experimental

    treatments were withheld. The rating practice was administered col-

    lectively to the judges and consisted of a videotape replay of fourteen

    students performing the twelve stunts included in the skills test·

    for the completed study. The videotape used in the rating practice

    session was recorded during a pilot study of the evaluation instru-

    ment in the Spring, 1981. Stop action of the camera enabled analysis

    of specific faults, and a discussion of the raters' opinions of the

    quality of the performances. The raters viewed the performances in the

    same order and at the same time. Each rater was given separate copies

    of score sheets on which to independently rate the performances.

    The raters followed the same procedures used in the rati.ng prac-

    tice session when they rated the pre- and posttest performances for

    the completed study. The only exception was that each rater was

    instructed not to confer with one another with regard to scoring the

    subjects.

    Study Design

    The following diagram illustrates the design employed in this

    study:

  • 30

    Group N Pretest

    I

    Command Style

    II

    Reciprocal Style

    25

    26

    Definition of Symbols:

    OTsT1

    OTsT l

    N = number of subjects

    TST = Tumbling Skills Test

    c = command style treatment

    r = reciprocal style treatment

    Treatment Pos ttes t

    0rsr 2

    OrsT2

    The command and reciprocal styles of instruction were randomly

    assigned by the toss of a coin and they were the independent variables

    indicated by Xe and Xr in the diagram. The dependent variable was

    the tumbling skill test scores on the posttest. The equivalence of

    two treatment groups was checked by using the Analysis of Variance

    statistics on the pretest scores. Observations on the pretest are

    indicated in the above diagram by OTST and observations on the post-1

    test are indicated by OTsT2. The broken line refers to the non-random

    assignment of subjects to treatment groups.

    Kerlinger (1973) refers to the design used in this study as a

    "compromise design" and points out that even though the design lacked

    the assurance provided by randomization, if treatment equivalence is

  • 31

    established, one can proceed with the study without violating the

    equivalence assumption (p. 341).

    Statistical Procedures

    Raw data were obtained from the pretest and posttest scores on

    the tumbling skills test for subjects in both treatment groups. An

    initial analysis of pretest scores utilizing the Analysis of Variance

    statistics was conducted to ascertain if there were differences in

    tumbling skill level between the two groups before they were sub-

    jected to the experimental treatments. To gain differential values

    among the groups, Analyses of Variance were calculated on the twelve

    stunts included in the study for each treatment group. The .05 level

    was utilized to determine significance for all statistical analyses

    conducted in this study. Analysis of Variance summary tables were

    established to indicate the effects of command and reciprocal teaching

    styles on the acquisition of each of the twelve tumbling skills.

    Tables were also established to indicate pretreatment and post-

    treatment group mean scores on the twelve tumbling skills.

  • Chapter IV

    ANALYSIS AND DISCUSSION OF RESULTS

    The purpose of this study was to compare the effects of command

    and reciprocal teaching styles on the acquisition of twelve tumbling

    skills by college students. This chapter contains the statistical

    analysis and discussion of the results of the investigation.

    The data were obtained from fifty-one college students enrolled

    at Virginia State University, Petersburg, Virginia. Two intact

    classes were randomly assigned a treatment number and were subjected

    to different types of treatment as follows:

    Group I------------------ Command style treatment

    Group II----------------- Reciprocal style treatment

    Statistical Analysis

    Since the two treatment groups were composed of intact classes

    predetermined by patterns of registration rather than being assigned

    at random, it was necessary to test for homogeneity among the groups.

    The mean pretreatment scores on each tumbling skill for both groups are

    presented in Table 1. According to pretreatment mean scores, both

    treatment groups were similar in tumbling skill level. As seen in

    Table 1, the command treatment group (Group I) reported mean scores

    slightly higher than the reciprocal treatment group (Group II) in

    eight of the twelve tumbling skills. The preliminary analysis for

    homogeneity consisted of twelve one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAS)

    32

  • 33

    Table ·1

    Group Means for Pretreatment Scores on Twelve Skills

    for Groups I and II

    Skills

    Headstand

    Forward Roll

    Forward Roll Without Hands

    Handstand

    Backward Roll

    Dive Roll

    Chest Roll

    Cartwheel

    Back Extension

    Limbre

    Back Walkover

    Roundoff

    Total Mean Score

    Group I (Command)

    1. 76

    5.92

    1.36

    1.80

    5.16

    1.12

    .32

    1. 76

    0

    .24

    0

    .48

    1. 66

    Note: Means are reported as points out of a total of 10.

    Group II (Reciprocal)

    1.46

    5. 15

    .96

    1.53

    4.46

    .76

    .34

    1.53

    0

    0

    . 15

    .53

    1.40

  • 34

    comparing pretreatment scores of the two groups of subjects on the

    twelve tumbling skills previously listed in Table 1.

    The results of the preliminary analyses showed no significant

    differences in pretreatment performance scores of the two groups.

    Specifically, the two treatment groups appeared to be initially homo-

    geneous in all twelve tumbling skills. Summary ANOVAS of the pre-

    liminary analyses are displayed in Tables 2 through 13. The F ratios

    given in Tables 2 through 13 were not significant at the .05 level,

    thereby indicating no significant performance differences on either of

    the twelve skills between the two groups before treatments were ad-

    ministered.

    Because homogeneity was established for the twelve tumbling

    skills, one-way classification ANOVAS were utilized in analyzing the

    posttreatment mean performance scores for each group. The mean post-

    treatment performance scores on all twelve skills for both groups are

    shown in Table 14. As shown in Table 14, the command treatment group

    reported mean scores slightly higher than the reciprocal treatment

    group on nine of the twelve tumbling skills. This investigator noted

    that-the roundoff was the only tumbling skill in which the corrrrnand

    treatment reported a lower mean score than the reciprocal treatment

    group on both pretreatment and posttreatments.

    Posttreatment tumbling skill tests results of the treatment groups

    were compared with pretreatment results to determine if learning had

    occurred during the experimental treatment period. This comparison

    revealed similar mean gain scores between the two groups.

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    35

    Table 2

    ANOVA Summary Table of Headstand Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    1.13 1 1.13

    267.03 49 5.44

    268. 16 50

    * F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .2077

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    36

    Table 3

    ANOVA Summary Table of Forward Roll Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    7.48 1 7.48

    121 . 23 49 2.47

    128. 71 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * 3.02

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    37

    Table 4

    ANOVA Summary Table of Forward Roll Without Hands

    Pretreatment Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    2.02 1 2.02

    82.73 49 1.68

    84.75 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    1.20*

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    38

    Table 5

    ANOVA Summary Table of Handstand Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    .87 l .87

    380.47 49 7.76

    381.34 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .112

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    .Total

    *

    39

    Table 6

    ANOVA Summary Table of Backward Roll Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    6.21 1 6.21

    359.83 49 7.34

    366.04 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .846

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    40

    Table 7

    ANOVA Summary Table of Dive Roll Pretreatment

    Scored for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    1.57 1 1.57

    129.26 49 2.63

    130.83 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .596

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    41

    Table 8

    ANOVA Summary Table of Chest Roll Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    • 01 1 • 01

    99.33 49 2.02

    99.34 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .004

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    42

    Table 9

    ANOVA Summary Table of Cartwheel Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    .62 l .62

    327.03 49 6.67

    327.65 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .092

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    43

    Table 10

    ANOVA Summary Table of Back Extension Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    0 1 0

    0 49 0

    0 50

    *F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * 0

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    44

    Table 11

    ANOVA Summary Table of Limbre Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    .735 1 .735

    34.560 49 .705

    35.295 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * 1.04

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    45

    Table 12

    ANOVA Summary Table of Back Walkover Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    .302 1 .302

    15.385 49 .313

    15. 687 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .964

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    46

    Table 13

    ANOVA Summary Table of Roundoff Pretreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    .04 l .04

    76. 71 50 1.56

    76.75 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04

    F

    * .025

  • 47

    Table 14

    Group Means of Posttreatment Scores on Twelve Tumbling

    Stunts for Groups I and II

    Skills

    Headstand

    Forward Roll

    Forward Roll Without Hands

    Handstand

    Backward Roll

    Dive Roll

    Chest Roll

    Cartwheel

    Back Extension

    Limbre

    Back Walkover (

    Roundoff

    Total Mean Score

    Group I (Conunand)

    5.04

    7.52

    5.56

    6.60

    6.80

    3.44

    4.60

    5.52

    2.08

    3.48

    3.32

    4.28

    4.85

    Note: Means are reported as points out of a total of 10.

    Group II (Reciprocal)

    4.69

    8.11

    5.19

    6.19

    6. 61

    3.11

    3. 61

    6.53

    1.80

    2.34

    2.19

    5.69

    4.67

  • 48

    Presented in Tables 15 through 26 are the results of the one-way

    analysis of variance concerning group differences in mean scores for

    the twelve tumbling skills. The F ratios given in Tables 15 through

    26 were not statistically significant, thereby indicating no signifi-

    cant performance differences in the twelve tumbling skills between

    the command treatment group and the reciprocal treatment group. A

    difference between the two treatment groups would have been signifi-

    cant at the .05 level if the F ratio for an analysis of variance on

    either of the tumbling skills had exceeded 4.04. Since neither group

    performed significantly better on the tumbling tests than did the

    other, the null hypothesis was not rejected, indicating that the skill

    acquisition of college students taught by the reciprocal style would

    probably not be different from the performance of college st~dents

    taught by the command style.

    An illustration of group means and mean gains for the pretreatment

    and posttreatment scores for both groups on the tumbling tests-is located

    in Table 27. This table indicates there were mean score improvements

    by both treatment groups on the tumbling test from the pretreatment to

    the posttreatment. As seen in Table 27, the command treatment group

    progressed from a mean score of 1.66 before experimental treatment was

    administered to a mean score of 4.85 after experimental treatment

    was administered. The reciprocal treatment group progressed similarly,

    from a mean score of 1.40 before treatment to 4.67 after treatment.

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    49

    Table 15

    ANOVA Summary Table of Headstand Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    1.54 l 1.54

    552.50 50 11. 27

    554.04 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .1366

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    50

    Table 16

    ANOVA Summary Table of Forward Roll Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df i~S

    4.52 1 4.52

    168. 90 49 3.44

    173.42 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * 1.31

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    51

    Table 17

    ANOVA Summary Table of Forward Roll Without haTids

    Posttreatment Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    1. 73 1 1.73

    206.20 49 4.20

    207.93 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * • 411

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    52

    Table 18

    ANOVA Summary Table of Handstand Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    2.12 1 2. 12

    202.04 49 4.12

    204.16 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * . 514

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    53

    Table 19

    ANOVA Summary Table of Backward Roll Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    .43 1 . 43

    1410.16 49 28.77

    1410.59 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04

    F

    * .0149

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    54

    Table 20

    ANOVA Summary Table of Dive Roll Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    1.34 1 1.34

    336.82 6.87

    338 .16 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .1950

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    55

    Table 21

    ANOVA Summary Table of Chest Roll Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    12.35 l 12. 35

    676.16 49 13.79

    688. 51 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .895

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    56

    Table 22

    ANOVA Summary Table of Cartwheel Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    13. 22 1 13.22

    656. 71 49 13.40

    669.93 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .986

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    57

    Table 23

    ANOVA Summary Table of Back Extension Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    .95 1 .95

    563.88 49 11. 50

    564.83 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    * .0826

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    *

    58

    Table 24

    ANOVA Sununary Table of Limbre Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    16.38 l 16.38

    881.13 49 17. 98

    897. 51 50

    F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    . 911 o*

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    59

    Table 25

    ANOVA Summary Table of Back Walkover Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    16. 21 l 16. 21

    483.48 49 9.86

    499.69 50

    * F for P of .05 = 4.04.

    F

    1.64 *

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    60

    Table 26

    ANOVA Summary Table of Roundoff Posttreatment

    Scores for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    25.42 25.42

    329.58 49 6.72

    355.00 50

    *F for P of .05 = 4.04

    F

    3.78*

  • 61

    Table 27

    Comparison of Group Mean Scores on Pretreatment and Posttreatment Tumbling Tests for Groups I and II

    Pretreatment Posttreatment

    Group I (Command) l.66 4.85

    Group II (Recriprocal) 1.40 4.67

    Note: Means are reported as points out of a total of 10.

    Mean Gain

    3. 19

    3.27

  • 62

    If the posttreatment mean performance scores significantly dif-

    fered between the two treatment groups, it would have been evident

    that the two teaching styles under investigation had a differential

    effect on tumbling skill acquisition. In order to determine if such

    a difference existed, a one-way Analysis of Variance was conducted

    on the posttreatment mean performance scores of both groups. The

    one-way ANOVA summary table for posttreatment mean scores is shown in

    Table 28. The lack of significance of the F ratio in Table 28 sug-

    gests that both teaching styles were equally effective in oromoting

    tumbling skill acquisition.

    The command treatment proved to be an effective instructional

    style (Pre !i_ = 1.66, Post !i = 4.85, p < .05). This group recorded slightly higher mean scores on the pretest and on the posttest than

    did the reciprocal treatment group. The mean differences between the

    two groups were not statistically significant.

    The reciprocal treatment group developed the tumbling skills

    equally well when compared with the command treatment group. This

    finding indicates that reciprocal teaching is also an effective in-

    structional style (PreM= 1.40, Post!i= 4.67, p < .05).

    The subjects in the command and reciprocal treatment groups im-

    proved on the tumbling skill tests from pretreatment to posttreat-

    ment with mean gain scores of 3.19 and 3.27, respectively. According

    to pretreatment mean scores of 1.66 and 1.40, both groups were similar

    in tumbling skill level. The groups were also similar in terms of

    variability. Mean and standard deviation scores indicating these

    findings are presented in Table 29.

  • Source of Variance

    Between

    Within

    Total

    63

    Table 28

    ANOVA Summary Table of Posttreatment Mean Scores

    for Groups I and II

    ss df MS

    . 06 l .06

    117. 49 49 2.39

    117. 55 50

    *F for P of .05 = 4.04

    F

    .0251*

  • Group

    Command

    Reci proca 1

    64

    Table 29

    Mean and Standard Deviation Scores on Tumbling

    Skill Tests for Command and Reciprocal Groups

    Pretreatment Posttreatment

    N

    25

    26

    M

    1. 66

    1.40

    SD

    . 73

    .83

    N

    25

    26

    M

    4.85

    4.67

    SD

    1.58

    1.50

  • 65

    Discussion

    The results of this investigation, which failed to reject the

    null hypothesis of no difference between the command and the recipro-

    cal styles of teaching, support earlier findings in some respects

    but not in others. This discussion will focus on previous studies

    and will point out how the completed investigation supports or con-

    tradicts the results reported in earlier studies.

    The results of the completed investigation are consistent with

    earlier studies which compared styles of teaching on Mosston's (1972)

    spectrum with only one exception. Mariani· (1970) found the task

    teaching style superior to the command teaching style when he taught

    the backhand tennis stroke to his subjects. There are a number of

    similarities between the Mariani study and the completed investigation.

    The task teaching style used in the Mariani study was similar to the

    reciprocal style used in the completed investigation. Mariani also

    used college age students as his subjects and conducted his experi-

    ment for a period of six weeks. In spite of the structural similari-

    ties, findings of the completed investigation garner no support for

    the earlier findings of Mariani.

    The results of other studies comparing teaching styles on the

    spectrum are consistent with the findings of the completed investiga-

    tion. The only other spectrum study reviewed which utilized college

    students as subjects was conducted by Dougherty (1970). Dougherty

    found no significant differences in physical fitness and motor skill

    performance between three groups of male college freshmen when he

  • 66

    investigated command, task, and individual program teaching styles.

    Boschee (1972), Bryant (1974), Jacoby (1975), Gerney (1979), and

    Virgilio (1979) also reported non-significant differences between

    spectrum teaching styles when they used various motor skills as de-

    pendent variables. The results of the completed investigation, coupled

    with the findings of earlier studies on Mosston1 s (1972) spectrum of

    teaching styles, tend to suggest that while all the styles investi-

    gated facilitate motor skill acquisition, evidence to reach a strong

    and valid conclusion regarding superiority of either style remains

    lacking.

    Several earlier studies compared a teacher-centered method of

    teaching with a student-centered method (Keller, 1963; Schlott, 1970;

    Croom, 1971; Slooten and Kneer, 1976; Thaxton and others, 1977). The

    teacher-centered method as described in these studies was similar to

    the command style treatment used in the completed investigation.

    The student-centered methods were similar to the reciprocal style

    treatment to the extent that students were encouraged to learn skills

    with little or no assistance from the classroom teacher. Three of

    these studies reported findings consistent with the results of the

    completed investigation, while two seem to be in conflict.

    Slooten and Kneer 0976) found no significant differences in

    skill acquisition between the three classes of male students when

    taught by two teacher-centered methods and one student-centered method.

    The findings of Slooten and Kneer supported the earlier findings of

    Schlott (1970) and Keller (1963) who also studied the differential

  • 67

    effects of teacher-centered and student-centered teaching methods on

    motor skill acquisition. The resuits of the above three studies were

    consistent with the findings of the completed investigation.

    The two studies which reported conflicting and contradictory

    findings were conducted by Croom (1971) and Thaxton and others (1977).

    These researchers reported evidence to support the superiority of

    either a teacher-centered or a student-centered teaching method they

    investigated. Croom (1971) found a teacher-centered method she de-

    scribed as being characterized by explanation, demonstration and execu-

    tion more effective in teaching physical skills than the student-

    centered method of self-discovery she employed. Thaxton and others

    (1971) reported results that were the antithesis of what Croom (1971)

    found.

    The literature reviewed in the area of motor skill learning tended

    to suggest that motor skills are more readily learned through teaching

    methods that consider the differences in abilities and interests of

    the learner. If this suggestion is accepted, then it would seem likely

    that the reciprocal teaching style would be better suited than the

    command teaching style for the improvement of skill learning. No

    evidence to support that suggestion was found in the completed inves-

    tigation.

    The research previously reviewed in the area of feedback suggested

    two important considerations with regard to its impact on motor skill

    learning: 1) feedback should be given immediately following the com-

    pletion of a task; and 2) feedback should be specific to the skill

  • 68

    involved. Although the reciprocal teaching style used by Bryant

    (1974), Jacoby (1975), Gerney (1979) and this investigator provided

    for immediate feedback and specific skill corrections, none of these

    studies reported evidence of those factors having a statistically

    significant effect on performance scores. This observation does not

    support the position taken by Fuey, Saudergas, and Bushell (1975),

    Catano (1975), Newell and Chew (1975), Thompson (1969), and Robb

    (1968) who contend that motor skill acquisition is significantly en-

    hanced when feedback, praise and correction is used.

  • Chapter V

    Summary, Conclusions, Recommendations

    Summary

    The purpose of this investigation was to compare the effects of

    command and reciprocal teaching styles on the tumbling skill acquisi-

    tion of college students in beginning tumbling. Fifty-one coeds from

    Virginia State University in Petersburg, Virginia served as subjects.

    Each subject completed a background data form prior to the study.

    Only those subjects who perceived themselves as poor or fair tumblers

    and who indicated little or no prior tumbling instruction were included

    in the investigation. The subjects were assigned to one of two

    treab1ient groups by their choice during registration or pre-

    registration for the Fall semester of the 1981-82 academic year. The

    groups were then assigned one of two treatments (command or reciprocal)

    by the toss of a coin.

    Subjects were pretested during the two class periods immediately

    preceding the first week of the experimental period and posttested the

    two class periods that immediately followed the sixth week of the ex-

    perimental period. Subjects were instructed in each group for a period

    of six weeks meeting twice a week for fifty minutes each session

    during the Fall semester, 1981. Instruction was given under condi-

    tions appropriate to each of the teaching styles investigated.

    The evaluation instrument used to assess the tumbling skill

    acquisition of the subjects was a modification of a rating scale used

    69

  • 70

    by Jarvis (1967), designed by the investigator specifically for this

    study. The test utilized two gymnastic judges to rate twelve tumbling

    skills on a 0-5 point scale. The raters independently scored video-

    taped performances of the stunts using performance criteria set up for

    each stunt on separate score sheets. Reliability coefficients between

    the two raters' scores were computed. These interrater correlations

    ranged from +.58 to +.96.

    The subjects in the command treatment group (Group I) learned the

    tumbling skills by following commands and directions given solely by

    the classroom teacher. Each skill was taught using a 311 x 511 card

    which contained specific performance criteria. After a skill was ex-

    plained and demonstrated by the teacher, the entire group was allowed

    to execute the skill.

    The reciprocal treatment group (Group II) learned the tumbling

    skills in dyads. One student in a dyad served as doer, while the

    other performed the role of teacher-partner. After a skill was exe-

    cuted by the doer and evaluated by the teacher-partner, the roles were

    exchanged. Students worked in different dyads each week of the experi-

    mental period. Each member of a dyad was supplied one task card for

    each of the twelve tumbling skills. Each task card contained the exact

    performance criteria for each skill used by the classroom teacher to

    instruct the command treatment group.

    One-way classification analysis of variance were utilized to

    determine initial homogeneity among the two treatment groups. The

    same statistics were also used to determine which teaching style was

  • 71

    more effective in promoting tumbling skill acquisition. The .05 level

    of significance was utilized to determine acceptance or rejection of

    the null hypothesis. Mean and standard deviation tables, group pre-

    and posttreatment mean score tables, and analysis of variance summary

    tables were used to present the analysis of data.

    The findings of this investigation revealed the following:

    1. no significant difference was found between the command treat-

    ment group and the reciprocal treatment group with regard to initial

    homogeneity.

    2. both treatment groups showed significant improvement in tumb-

    ling skill level based on a comparison of pretreatment and posttreatment

    mean performance scores.

    3. no significant difference was found between the command

    treatment group and the reciprocal treatment group in mean tumbling

    performance scores as a result of the experimental treatments.

    Conclusions

    Within the limitations imposed by the design and procedures of

    this investigation, the following conclusions were reached:

    1. Both command and reciprocal teaching styles promote tumbling

    skill acquisition of college age students in beginning

    tumbling.

    2. Neither teaching style used in this investigation is more

    effective than the other in promoting tumbling skill

    acquisition for college students over a six week instruc-

    tional period.

  • 72

    On the basis of the findings and conclusions reported in this

    study, it is apparent that college level students are capable of teaching

    each other beginning tumbling skills. The nonsignificant difference

    between treatment group performances tends to suggest that practitioners

    could utilize the reciprocal style equally effectively as the command

    style while at the same time furthering each student's ability to learn

    in alternative modes.

    Although no significant differences were found between the two treat-

    ment groups, the investigator observed and received feedback with regard

    to how much the students "enjoyed" the reciprocal process. This observa-

    tion suggests the possibility that the subjects in the reciprocal treat-

    ment group achieved positive affective results beyond tumbling skill

    acquisition.

    Recommendations

    As a result of this investigation, the following recommendations for

    future research are offered:

    1. Further studies should be conducted which investigate the ef-

    fects of the reciprocal teaching style on college age students who are

    experienced tumblers. Such studies should incorporate advanced tumbling

    skills as measurement criteria. It might be found that teaching tumbling

    utilizing the reciprocal style can effectively facilitate advanced skill

    learning.

    2. Further studies, similar in design to the completed investiga-

    tion, should be conducted which include the subject's gender as a variable.

  • 73

    Tumbling coaches and instructors tend to group students according to their

    gender for instruction. Perhaps an optimal teaching style exists between

    instructors and students of the same gender.

    3. Follow-up studies should be conducted utilizing a larger sample

    of subjects for data analysis. It is not atypical for one physical edu-

    cator to be expected to instruct students far exceeding the number used

    in this investigation within a given period of time. It is possible that

    a study which utilizes a larger sample might reveal significant results

    with regard to the superiority of a teaching style which allows students

    to teach themselves.

    4. Further studies should be conducted to compare the effects of

    additional teaching styles identified by Mosston (1972) on tumbling skill

    acquisition of college age students. At the time of this writing, only

    two completed studies were found which investigated styles on Mosston's

    spectrum with college age students as subjects. Neither of these