tina m. grieco-calub, maya-simone collins, hillary...

1
Figure 4: Experiment 2, Effect of speech-shaped noise A. Span scores (mean + SE) of children who completed the MST in quiet (black bars) and in the presence of speech-shaped noise (SSN, blue bars). As a group, children performed similarly in both conditions [t(1,14) = 0.36, p = 0.73]. B. Children in 4A were split into two groups based on the first condition that they completed (e.g., quiet or SSN). The numbers in white indicate presentation order. A mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) failed to show main effects of condition and order, but revealed a significant condition x order interaction [F(1,13) = 5.2, p<0.05]. This finding suggests that children always performed better on the first task, regardless of condition. Span scores in quiet or in the presence of speech-shaped noise did not correlate with receptive vocabulary scores. Figure 2. The Missing Scan Task Introduction Children often find themselves needing to listen and learn in noisy environments, including home and the classroom. These adverse listening environments not only interfere with access to the target speech signal (i.e., “energetic” masking), but they also tax a listener’s cognitive resources (i.e., “informational masking”). Young children experience greater masking than older children and adults, consistent with the idea that performance in background noise has a slow developmental time course (e.g., Wightman & Kistler, 2005; Leibold & Buss, 2013; Jones, Moore, & Amitay, 2015). The mechanisms underlying this development, however, are unclear. Recent work suggests that background noise directly interferes with auditory working memory, which is a key cognitive function that subserves speech perception in noise (e.g., Osman & Sullivan, 2014). This work, however, has largely focused on recall tasks (e.g., digit span) which may be too complex for younger children. The present study aimed to test young children’s working memory capacity in the presence of background noise, including speech babble (Experiment 1) and speech-shaped noise (Experiment 2). We implemented the Missing Scan Task which requires children to retrieve items in immediate memory but only verbally identify the “missing” item from a set of objects that were previously viewed. We hypothesized that young children will retrieve fewer items in the presence of background noise. References Buschke, H. (1963a). Relative retention in immediate memory determined by the missing scan method. Nature, 200, 11291130. Jones, P. R., Moore, D. R., & Amitay, S. (2015). Development of auditory selective attention: Why children struggle to hear in noisy environments. Developmental Psychology, 51(3), 353-369. Leibold, L., & Buss, E. (2013). Children’s identification of consonants in a speech-shaped noise or a two-talker masker. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(4), 1144-1155. Osman, H., & Sullivan, J. R. (2014). Children's auditory working memory performance in degraded listening conditions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(4), 1503-1511. Roman, A., Pisoni, D., & Kronenberger, W. (2014). Assessment of working memory capacity in preschool children using the missing scan task. Infant and Child Development, 23(6), 575-587. Rothauser, E. H., Chapman, W. D., Guttman, N., Nordby, K. S., Silbigert, H. R., Urbanek, G. E., and Weinstock, M. (1969). “IEEE recommended practice for speech quality measurements,” IEEE Trans. Audio Electroacoust. 17, 227–246. Wightman, F. L. & Kistler, D. J. (2005). Informational masking of speech in children: Effects of ipsilateral and contralateral distracters. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118(5), 3164-3176. Speech babble interferes with verbal working memory in 5-year-old children Tina M. Grieco-Calub, Maya-Simone Collins, Hillary Snyder Northwestern University, The Roxelyn & Richard Pepper Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders Summary 1.Speech babble interferes with working memory in 5-year-old children. This is consistent with recent data collected from school- age children (Osman & Sullivan, 2014). 2.The condition x order interaction observed in Experiment 2, with the speech-shaped noise condition, was an unexpected finding. Children who completed in the MST in quiet first performed similarly to the children in the quiet conditions of Experiment 1, suggesting that the sample of children in Experiment 2 are representative of typical 5-year-old children. However, children who completed the MST in speech-shaped noise first scored lower in quiet (completed after the noise condition). One possibility is that, unlike the babble, the speech-shaped noise had a longer-lasting impact on cognitive processing. In other words, the speech-shaped noise had a forward masking effect on the quiet condition. Additional studies are necessary to investigate this finding. 3.The MST likely engages the phonological loop of working memory, or verbal working memory, due to retrieval of lexical items. The MST may also, however, engage the visual sketchpad (Roman et al., 2014). It is unclear which strategies children used to identify the “missing” animal, and therefore it is unclear which part of working memory was engaged by the background noise. Additional studies are necessary to disambiguate this issue. 4.In conclusion, acute exposure to background noise, regardless of its content, interferes with cognitive processing in 5-year-old children. This has implications for both speech perception in noise as well as performance on cognitively-taxing tasks, like those commonly encountered in school. Methods Participants : Forty-three (N = 26 females) 5-yr-old children (range: 58.6 - 66.2 months) were recruited to participate in the study. Children were native English speaking and had normal medical and otological histories, per parent report. Background noise stimuli : Experiment 1: Two-talker male babble. Pairs of sentences from the IEEE corpus (Rothauser et al., 1969) spoken by an adult male were randomly chosen from a list of pre-recorded sentences and digitally mixed. Sentences were RMS-averaged and scaled to yield an overall intensity level of 60 dB SPL. Experiment 2: Speech-shaped noise. Gaussian noise was multiplied by the spectral envelope of the two-talker male babble and scaled to yield an overall intensity level of 60 dB SPL. Procedure : Children participated in an adapted version of the Missing Scan Task (MST; Buschke, 1963; Roman, Pisoni, & Kronenberger, 2014; Figure 2). Twelve children performed the task in quiet only, and thirty-one children performed the task twice, once in quiet and once in the presence of background noise (babble or speech-shaped noise). Conditions were counterbalanced to minimize an order effect. Children were also administered the Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROW-PVT). Acknowledgments We would like to acknowledge Tiffany Fang for her assistance in participant recruitment and data collection. We would also like to acknowledge Kristi Ward for her help with data analysis. This project was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Northwestern University. This project was supported by an Undergraduate Research Grant from Northwestern University. Which animal is missing? Animals hide behind barn one by one. All but one animal returns. Children view a set (2-10) of animals and name them. Familiarization phase: - Children are introduced to 34 animals plastic animals. Animals that children cannot name are removed during the test phase. - The procedure is illustrated to the right. Children are shown two animals, are asked to name them, and then watch them hide behind the barn. One animal returns, and children are asked to name the “missing” animal (i.e., the one still hiding). The researcher repeats the procedure until the perform the task. Test Phase: - On each trial, children are shown a set of animals, starting with a set size of 3 objects. - The researcher conducts the procedure as illustrated to the right, and incrementally increases the set size after correct responses. - The test phase continues until children are unable to identify the missing animal on 2 consecutive trials. - The span score is the longest set size that the child correctly recalls without errors. Figure 1. Spectrum of background noise in Experiment 1 (red line) and Experiment 2 (blue line). Results Figure 3: Experiment 1, Effect of two-talker male babble A. Span scores (mean + SE) of children who completed the MST in quiet only. B. Span scores of children who completed the MST in quiet (black bars) and in the presence of two-talker male babble (red bars). Children had statistically significantly greater span scores in quiet [t(1,15) = 2.83 , p < 0.05; red asterisk]. C. Children in 3B were split into two groups based on the first condition that they completed (e.g., quiet or babble). The numbers in white indicate presentation order. A mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a main effect of condition [F(1,14) = 7.5, p<0.05]. There was no main effect of order, and no condition x order interaction. These results suggest that children consistently performed poorer in the presence of babble, regardless of presentation order. D. Children’s span score in quiet was positively correlated to their receptive vocabulary score [r(28) = 0.471, p < 0.05]. This relation was not significant for the babble condition. 0 2 4 6 8 0 2 4 6 8 0 2 4 6 8 0 2 4 6 8 0 2 4 6 8 C A D B Span score Span score (Quiet) Receptive vocabulary (standard score) Span score Span score * Quiet only N=12 Quiet Babble Quiet, Babble Babble, Quiet Order N=16 N=8 N=8 Quiet SSN Quiet, SSN SSN, Quiet Order N=15 N=8 N=7 Span score Span score A B 1 1 2 2 1 1 2 2 1 3 5 7 9 85 95 105 115 125 135

Upload: others

Post on 07-Jul-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Tina M. Grieco-Calub, Maya-Simone Collins, Hillary Snyderhll.northwestern.edu/assets/auddev-poster-2015-tgc_rev.pdf · Tina M. Grieco-Calub, Maya-Simone Collins, Hillary Snyder Northwestern

Figure 4: Experiment 2, Effect of speech-shaped noise

A. Span scores (mean + SE) of children who completed the MST in quiet (black bars) and in the presence of speech-shaped noise (SSN, blue bars). As a group, children performed similarly in both conditions [t(1,14) = 0.36, p = 0.73]. B. Children in 4A were split into two groups based on the first condition that they completed (e.g., quiet or SSN). The numbers in white indicate presentation order. A mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) failed to show main effects of condition and order, but revealed a significant condition x order interaction [F(1,13) = 5.2, p<0.05]. This finding suggests that children always performed better on the first task, regardless of condition. Span scores in quiet or in the presence of speech-shaped noise did not correlate with receptive vocabulary scores.

Figure 2. The Missing Scan Task

Introduction Children often find themselves needing to listen and learn in noisy environments, including home and the classroom. These adverse listening environments not only interfere with access to the target speech signal (i.e., “energetic” masking), but they also tax a listener’s cognitive resources (i.e., “informational masking”). Young children experience greater masking than older children and adults, consistent with the idea that performance in background noise has a slow developmental time course (e.g., Wightman & Kistler, 2005; Leibold & Buss, 2013; Jones, Moore, & Amitay, 2015). The mechanisms underlying this development, however, are unclear. Recent work suggests that background noise directly interferes with auditory working memory, which is a key cognitive function that subserves speech perception in noise (e.g., Osman & Sullivan, 2014). This work, however, has largely focused on recall tasks (e.g., digit span) which may be too complex for younger children. The present study aimed to test young children’s working memory capacity in the presence of background noise, including speech babble (Experiment 1) and speech-shaped noise (Experiment 2). We implemented the Missing Scan Task which requires children to retrieve items in immediate memory but only verbally identify the “missing” item from a set of objects that were previously viewed. We hypothesized that young children will retrieve fewer items in the presence of background noise.

References

Buschke, H. (1963a). Relative retention in immediate memory determined by the missing scan method. Nature, 200, 1129–1130.

Jones, P. R., Moore, D. R., & Amitay, S. (2015). Development of auditory selective attention: Why children struggle to hear in noisy environments. Developmental Psychology, 51(3), 353-369.

Leibold, L., & Buss, E. (2013). Children’s identification of consonants in a speech-shaped noise or a two-talker masker. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(4), 1144-1155.

Osman, H., & Sullivan, J. R. (2014). Children's auditory working memory performance in degraded listening conditions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(4), 1503-1511.

Roman, A., Pisoni, D., & Kronenberger, W. (2014). Assessment of working memory capacity in preschool children using the missing scan task. Infant and Child Development, 23(6), 575-587.

Rothauser, E. H., Chapman, W. D., Guttman, N., Nordby, K. S., Silbigert, H. R., Urbanek, G. E., and Weinstock, M. (1969). “IEEE recommended practice for speech quality measurements,” IEEE Trans. Audio Electroacoust. 17, 227–246.

Wightman, F. L. & Kistler, D. J. (2005). Informational masking of speech in children: Effects of ipsilateral and contralateral distracters. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118(5), 3164-3176.

Speech babble interferes with verbal working memory in 5-year-old children Tina M. Grieco-Calub, Maya-Simone Collins, Hillary Snyder Northwestern University, The Roxelyn & Richard Pepper Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Summary 1. Speech babble interferes with working memory in 5-year-old children. This is consistent with recent data collected from school-age children (Osman & Sullivan, 2014).

2. The condition x order interaction observed in Experiment 2, with the speech-shaped noise condition, was an unexpected finding. Children who completed in the MST in quiet first performed similarly to the children in the quiet conditions of Experiment 1, suggesting that the sample of children in Experiment 2 are representative of typical 5-year-old children. However, children who completed the MST in speech-shaped noise first scored lower in quiet (completed after the noise condition). One possibility is that, unlike the babble, the speech-shaped noise had a longer-lasting impact on cognitive processing. In other words, the speech-shaped noise had a forward masking effect on the quiet condition. Additional studies are necessary to investigate this finding.

3. The MST likely engages the phonological loop of working memory, or verbal working memory, due to retrieval of lexical items. The MST may also, however, engage the visual sketchpad (Roman et al., 2014). It is unclear which strategies children used to identify the “missing” animal, and therefore it is unclear which part of working memory was engaged by the background noise. Additional studies are necessary to disambiguate this issue.

4. In conclusion, acute exposure to background noise, regardless of its content, interferes with cognitive processing in 5-year-old children. This has implications for both speech perception in noise as well as performance on cognitively-taxing tasks, like those commonly encountered in school.

Methods Participants: Forty-three (N = 26 females) 5-yr-old children (range: 58.6 - 66.2 months) were recruited to participate in the study. Children were native English speaking and had normal medical and otological histories, per parent report. Background noise stimuli: Experiment 1: Two-talker male babble. Pairs of sentences from the IEEE corpus (Rothauser et al., 1969) spoken by an adult male were randomly chosen from a list of pre-recorded sentences and digitally mixed. Sentences were RMS-averaged and scaled to yield an overall intensity level of 60 dB SPL. Experiment 2: Speech-shaped noise. Gaussian noise was multiplied by the spectral envelope of the two-talker male babble and scaled to yield an overall intensity level of 60 dB SPL. Procedure: Children participated in an adapted version of the Missing Scan Task (MST; Buschke, 1963; Roman, Pisoni, & Kronenberger, 2014; Figure 2). Twelve children performed the task in quiet only, and thirty-one children performed the task twice, once in quiet and once in the presence of background noise (babble or speech-shaped noise). Conditions were counterbalanced to minimize an order effect. Children were also administered the Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (ROW-PVT).

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge Tiffany Fang for her assistance in participant recruitment and data collection. We would also like to acknowledge Kristi Ward for her help with data analysis. This project was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Northwestern University. This project was supported by an Undergraduate Research Grant from Northwestern University.

Which animal is missing?

Animals hide behind barn one by one.

All but one animal returns.

Children view a set (2-10) of animals and name them.

Familiarization phase: -  Children are introduced to 34 animals plastic

animals. Animals that children cannot name are removed during the test phase.

-  The procedure is illustrated to the right. Children are shown two animals, are asked to name them, and then watch them hide behind the barn. One animal returns, and children are asked to name the “missing” animal (i.e., the one still hiding). The researcher repeats the procedure until the perform the task.

Test Phase: -  On each trial, children are shown a set of

animals, starting with a set size of 3 objects. -  The researcher conducts the procedure as

illustrated to the right, and incrementally increases the set size after correct responses.

-  The test phase continues until children are unable to identify the missing animal on 2 consecutive trials.

-  The span score is the longest set size that the child correctly recalls without errors.

Figure 1. Spectrum of background noise in Experiment 1 (red line) and Experiment 2 (blue line).

Results

Figure 3: Experiment 1, Effect of two-talker male babble

A. Span scores (mean + SE) of children who completed the MST in quiet only. B. Span scores of children who completed the MST in quiet (black bars) and in the presence of two-talker male babble (red bars). Children had statistically significantly greater span scores in quiet [t(1,15) = 2.83 , p < 0.05; red asterisk]. C. Children in 3B were split into two groups based on the first condition that they completed (e.g., quiet or babble). The numbers in white indicate presentation order. A mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed a main effect of condition [F(1,14) = 7.5, p<0.05]. There was no main effect of order, and no condition x order interaction. These results suggest that children consistently performed poorer in the presence of babble, regardless of presentation order. D. Children’s span score in quiet was positively correlated to their receptive vocabulary score [r(28) = 0.471, p < 0.05]. This relation was not significant for the babble condition.

0

2

4

6

8

0

2

4

6

8

0

2

4

6

8

0

2

4

6

8

0

2

4

6

8

C A

D

B

Span

sco

re "

Span

sco

re"

(Qui

et) "

Receptive vocabulary"(standard score)"

Span

sco

re "

Span

sco

re "

*"

Quiet only"N=12 "

Quiet" Babble" Quiet, Babble" Babble, Quiet"

Order!N=16 " N=8 " N=8 "

Quiet" SSN" Quiet, SSN" SSN, Quiet"

Order!N=15" N=8 " N=7 "

Span

sco

re "

Span

sco

re "

A B

1 1 2 2

1 1 2 2

1

3

5

7

9

85 95 105 115 125 135