stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a...

20
Stop or not? How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions A Deloitte series on behavioral economics and management

Upload: others

Post on 18-Oct-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Stop or not?How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

A Deloitte series on behavioral economics and management

Page 2: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Mark J. CotteleerDr. Mark J. Cotteleer is a research director with Deloitte Services LP. His research focuses on the application of advanced technology in pursuit of operational and financial performance improvement, and is widely published in top management and academic journals. Cotteleer has received multiple research and teaching awards and has led teams in technology-enabled reengineering, supply chain strategy, business analytics, and process design. His experience with clients includes work in manufacturing, supply chain, business analytics, health care, and service industries.

Elliot BendolyDr. Elliot Bendoly is a professor of management sciences at The Ohio State University Fisher College of Business. His current research interests include studies into the effectiveness of opera-tions/IT alignment as well as investigations in the behavioral operations domain: collaboration/group dynamics and work policies/task complexity/uncertainty. Bendoly serves as senior editor for the journal Production and Operations Management and as associate editor for the Journal of Operations Management. His 50-plus peer-reviewed articles also appear in Information Systems Research, MIS Quarterly, and Journal of Applied Psychology. His personal site can be viewed at www.bizbreed.com.

About the authors

Deloitte Consulting LLP’s Talent Strategies practice assists organizations with the activities, processes, and infrastructure related to developing, rewarding, and measuring employee performance. The practice offers services in areas including recruiting and sourcing, performance management, career development, learning, and succession planning. Learn more about our Talent offerings on www.deloitte.com.

Stop or not?

Page 3: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Contents

Work, interrupted | 2

Multitasking among students | 4

Outside the schoolhouse | 6

Effective work design strategies for multitasking and | 13 interrupted-knowledge work environments

Endnotes | 15

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

1

Page 4: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Work, interrupted

WHEN was the last time you were able to just sit down at your desk and complete

an entire task or project start to finish? No emails or texts, no checking of the Internet, no colleagues crashing through the door to report the latest unrelated challenge (or even gossip)?

In academic circles this is referred to as “focused work,” and according to Dave Coplin’s book, The Rise of the Humans: How to Outsmart the Digital Deluge, humans typi-cally only manage about 11 minutes of actual work at a stretch. And after an interruption from someone else, or taking a break to check email or send a quick text, it can take people

from 15 to 23 minutes to get back to where they were before.1 A potentially shocking loss of productivity!

The difficulty of performing sustained, focused work is well understood and has been studied across a variety of settings, including education, health care, and management. Here, we draw on that extensive body of research—focusing on two studies in particular—to examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiolo-gists. Our goal is to summarize findings related to the impact of interruptions of various

A DELOITTE SERIES ON BEHAVIORAL ECONOMICS AND MANAGEMENT Behavioral economics is the examination of how psychological, social, and emotional factors often conflict with and override economic incentives when individuals or groups make decisions. The field has its roots in the work of Nobel Prize winner Herbert Simon, who as far back as 1959 questioned the classical economic theory that individuals rationally maximize the outcomes they seek (that is, their “utility”) when making choices.2 Ever since, scholars have argued, and demonstrated, that in the face of uncertainty, humans employ all manner of simple, easy-to-use, but often inaccurate cognitive shortcuts. People are not maximizers, but rather muddlers, struggling to cope with a reality that is much less certain, much more complex, and much more variable than they admit. Worse, as has been shown repeatedly by another Nobel Prize-winning behavioral economist, Daniel Kahneman, “We can be blind to the obvious, and we are also blind to our blindness.”3

The implications of cognitive limitation and bias in decision making are as varied as they are vast. Consequences stretch across industries and applications. Managers therefore need to understand something about those aspects of human perception and cognitive processes that can lead us astray. In short, they need to understand something about the psychology of choice.

This paper is one in a series offered by Deloitte that is intended to address this need of managers attempting to improve the performance, growth, and innovative capabilities of their organizations.4 Each paper in this series examines the influence and consequences of behavioral principles on the choices people make related to their work. The reader can expect to see some common themes emerge that are related to the framing of choice, the uncertainty of outcomes, the influence of time (and timing), and a general inability to effectively process information. Each paper will provide a set of practical guidelines for recognizing, managing, and/or mitigating the effects of these very natural human limitations and biases in an applied setting.

Collectively, these papers illustrate how an understanding of biases and cognitive limitations is the first step in developing countermeasures that limit their impact on the organization. Alternatively, they may help organizations and individuals identify strategies that ethically exploit these inescapable elements of our humanity to the benefit of a company’s performance, growth, and innovation.

Stop or not?

2

Page 5: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Here, we draw on that extensive body of research to examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists.kinds—from switching among different proj-ects to interruptions of single projects—and to offer insights into effective strategies for com-batting the inevitable productivity losses that they impose. From this effort, we distill some key findings and prescriptions for managers

to move forward toward achieving higher-performing organizations.

This paper should take about 15 minutes for you to read. Turn off your smartphone, close your door, and see if you can do it in one sitting!

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

3

Page 6: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Multitasking among students

TODAY’S classrooms are filled with stu-dents surfing the Internet, posting social

media updates, and texting about upcoming weekend plans, all while listening to a lecture on macroeconomics. A recent study found that at any given point during class, 15 percent of students were off-task.5 Students’ multitasking is not limited to the lecture hall, but can be seen across dorm rooms, libraries, and cafes as students simultaneously process multiple streams of information and entertainment while learning. It’s no wonder that students’ inability to sustain focus is well documented within the literature.

Numerous studies show that students consistently choose multitasking—repeat-edly turning their attention to something else before returning to and completing the task at hand—over focused work. Even in cases where stu-dents are instructed to focus their attention on something impor-tant, they routinely succumb to the urge to switch between activities—even unaware of how often they turn their attention elsewhere. For example:

• In one study, third-year college students volunteered to have their laptops monitored during class, and yet still spent 42 percent of their time on non-course-related activities, generating on average 65 open and active windows on their laptop monitor dur-ing the lecture—63 percent of those open windows comprised of non-course-related

activities (gaming, emails, web surfing, and entertainment).6 Students were not just dis-tracted with one non-course-related activ-ity, but were frequently switching between multiple activities at any given time.

• This same study found that students on their laptops vastly underestimated their media use during class—underreporting instant messaging by 40 percent and email usage by 7 percent.7

• In another study of more than 1,000 college students, 80 percent admitted to texting during class; 15 percent admitted to send-ing 11 or more text messages during a single class session.8

In the context of student learning, it is important to remember that the digital activities in which students most commonly engage—Internet surfing, emails, texting—can compete for the same

mental bandwidth that is demanded by learn-ing.9 While students may vastly overestimate their ability to multitask and underestimate the frequency at which they do so, research demonstrates that individuals of any age have difficulty evaluating how well their own mental processes are operating at any given time, because most of these processes are uncon-scious.10 Our minds are simply not wired to simultaneously perform two intricate tasks.11

Under most conditions, the brain simply cannot simultaneously perform

two complex tasks.

Stop or not?

4

Page 7: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Interruption leads to forgetting

Any interruption, technology-induced or otherwise, can lead to forgetting. Forgetting requires relearning, and relearning harms pro-ductivity.12 Research shows that the longer an interruption lasts, the more information relat-ing to the task at hand will be forgotten. These costs accrue more rapidly where tasks require “significant concentration and attention.”13 As a result, interruption, forgetting, and the need to relearn can lead to a variety of negative out-comes, including:

• Work takes longer, driven by the dura-tion of the distraction and the time spent to re-familiarize oneself with a task when returning to work.

• The quality of the work suffers from the increased mental fatigue that results from repeatedly picking up and dropping a mental thread.14

• Retention falls as information is improp-erly “encoded” (that is, stored) in memory. Conditions at the time something is first learned are important when it comes to retention. One study found that multitask-ing during learning led to a whole letter grade drop (10 percent) in recognition and recall assessments.15

• Future application of knowledge is more difficult. The brain processes and stores information differently when distracted, making it more difficult to extend or extrapolate newly acquired learning to dif-ferent contexts. Therefore, even if someone can learn something while distracted, they may not be able to flexibly or effectively use that information.16

Educational research, therefore, clearly shows us the cost of distractions and other interruptions to learning and educational productivity.

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

5

Page 8: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Outside the schoolhouse

OF course, distractions do not end at the schoolhouse doors. They pervade

the workplace as well. We therefore ask the questions:

• Do distractions and interruptions have the same kinds of negative consequences on workers as they do on students? If so, what is a manager to do about it?

• How well do the strategies that workers typically rely on to manage distraction and interruption truly work?

• Can organizations create conditions that allow their workforces to ignore distrac-tions and resist a seemingly reflexive response to an unending barrage of external stimuli?

Fortunately, researchers are grappling with these questions. Studies take at face value the costs of interruption and examine the strategies employed by businesses and other organizations to deal with them. Strategies include such common-sense interventions as setting clear priorities, checking in (both on a schedule and unexpectedly), breaking up projects into smaller tasks, and “sequestering”

individuals to get high-priority work done. The results are sometimes surprising. In some cases, common sense prevails, while in others, it does not.

In the remainder of this article, we will focus on two well-crafted research studies that offer strategic insight to managers. We will describe the focus of each study and its findings. Following that, we will distill the managerial implications of both in an effort to offer practical guidance for managers seeking to improve performance across their teams and workplaces.

Priorities, checking in, and switching among tasks

The first study looked at whether project managers could influence project switch-ing behavior (and therefore organizational productivity) through the policies they set.17 Researchers performed a controlled experi-ment to evaluate the effects of prioritization and work monitoring on project switching behavior (that is, self-induced interruption in favor of a different task) in a distributed multi-project work environment (see sidebar, “The switching study,” for a more detailed descrip-tion of the study).

Strategies include such common-sense interventions as setting clear priorities, checking in (both on a schedule and unexpectedly), breaking up projects into smaller tasks, and “sequestering” individuals to get high-priority work done.

Stop or not?

6

Page 9: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Priorities matter—or do they?

Allocating resources in a multi-project environment is, in theory, facilitated by com-municating priorities to the worker in order to help them manage and focus efforts toward specified goals. When conflicts occur or resources are constrained, the highest-priority projects should generally be worked on first, and lower-priority projects should be set aside.18 Switching among tasks is discouraged in favor of focus on the most important task.

Researchers used the concept of “switch loss,” which refers to the time costs of alternat-ing between different types of work—includ-ing the cognitive effort required to disconnect from prior work and connect or reconnect with new work—to assess the impact on worker performance and productivity.19 Data showed a 3.5 percent decrease in total produc-tivity for every switch made. In a real-world knowledge work setting, researchers noted that they would expect higher productiv-ity losses, as the cognitive effort to disconnect/recon-nect with this type of work would be greater than that required by the project tasks tested.

Comfortingly, common sense seems to pre-vail when it comes to the setting of priorities.

Three key findings of the study can be summa-rized as follows:

• Workers who were provided with clear pri-orities switched tasks less often than those who did not receive priorities.

• Working with clear priorities, workers spent significantly more time on the highest-priority project.

• Given a greater focus on high-priority tasks, and a lower switch incidence, overall worker productivity for those with clear priorities was higher.

The contingent value of monitoring work

In addition to setting priorities, manag-ers also routinely monitor workers in order to check progress on assigned tasks. Researchers

considered a variety of monitor-ing approaches, and their timing, to understand the impact on worker switch-ing incidence and

overall productivity. For example, it is common in multi-project work environments to have scheduled progress updates in which workers

THE SWITCHING STUDY This study exposed working professionals to a series of computerized tasks that constituted multiple, parallel projects. In the experiment, half of the subjects were offered explicit project priorities for their work. The other half received the same work without stated priorities.

Across the two groups, subjects were exposed to one of the following monitoring conditions: 1) no reporting, 2) regularly scheduled progress updates, 3) early-stage managerial progress checks, and 4) late-stage managerial progress checks.

Researchers measured switch incidence by the total number of times each individual switched between projects before completing their full complement of work. In addition to the aggregate switching incidence, researchers observed the timing of the project switches relative to monitoring events to determine if the switch was “externally triggered” or not.

Data showed a 3.5 percent decrease in total productivity

for every switch made.

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

7

Page 10: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

know in advance when they are required to provide project progress updates. In other cases, managers might simply “stop by” for random or unscheduled status updates.

It is reasonable to ask whether one of these approaches is better than the other when it comes to driving worker performance. After all, the researchers reason, in multi-project environments, the “cultural value often asso-ciated with being a team player pressures workers to move all their projects along and distribute their efforts regularly.”20 Hence, the mere act of monitoring might drive project switching behaviors (and therefore reduce overall produc-tivity) as work-ers attempt to regularly show progress on each project.21 Then again, managers may be justifiably uncomfortable with foregoing monitoring alto-gether for fear that project issues may go unaddressed and progress might stall.

The research found that the “how” of moni-toring makes a difference. Specifically:

• Scheduled progress updates did not have a significant impact on worker switching incidence or overall productivity.

• Switching incidence was significantly higher when monitoring work progress took the form of unannounced visits, both early on and late in the project timeline.

We speculate from these findings that work-ers who were able to plan for project switch-ing addressed that need and made switches at natural project breaking points (more on this later), where rework or relearning effort was lessened.

As for the impact of unannounced moni-toring, several factors may be in play. Earlier

research asserts that when a higher-level manager arrives unannounced during a time when a worker is engaged in another manager’s project, the event can prove awkward. The visiting manager may make comments that may be interpreted as threatening, particularly in cases where the manager is in a position to evaluate the worker.22 This type of one-to-one interaction between worker and manager involves a stronger social component than the more general interactions required for sched-uled progress checks. Therefore, workers may feel more compelled to readily respond to this request from the manager.

Such unan-nounced prog-ress checks, often referred to as “drive-bys,” can cause costly operational dis-ruptions due to reactive project switching.23 When a worker feels pressured to

switch out of one project and into another, the potential result is an unnatural move or break in the worker’s overall progress plan—one that may result in a rapid return to earlier projects after the manager departs. Further, as these managerial progress checks can occur at any point, workers may feel obliged to frequently switch between projects even in the absence of a managerial progress check because they wish to maintain progress across all projects.

The importance of task completion

A final dimension of the study focused on where workers were in their progress toward completion of a task or project.24 Two key find-ings emerged from this angle of inquiry:

• Workers were more resistant to monitoring event-induced switching when the task they were working on was nearing completion. Project completion status did not have the same effect.

Unannounced progress checks, often referred to as

“drive-bys,” can cause costly operational disruptions due to

reactive project switching.

Stop or not?

8

Page 11: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

• Workers were more resistant to switching with each successive monitoring event, particularly when the monitoring event occurred later in the project timeline.

With respect to understanding why task completion turned out to be important, other research has shown that individuals tend to try to finish work close to completion before mov-ing on.25 According to the concept of psycho-logical ownership, individual workers develop a sense of ownership for projects that they work on.26 Uncompleted work creates “atten-tion residue” that engages cognitive resources and diminishes the ability to process new work.27 By completing work, workers are more likely to achieve cognitive closure and be able to take on new tasks.

Workers also seem to build up a toler-ance for monitoring, thereby becoming more resistant to switching over time. Researchers believe that workers actually “learned” from project switching. When a worker makes a switch, he or she must inevitably return to the dropped project. Over time, the worker learns that he or she must spend time relearning or reworking steps associated with the dropped project. This learning seemingly emboldens the worker to more firmly resist future switching. Further, resistance was notably greater when the monitoring occurred late in the project timeline. It is reasonable to speculate that when project time is dwindling, workers realize that there is less time available for relearning and rework on a dropped project. Therefore, they will likely be more resistant to switching in response to a managerial progress check.

As this study shows, management behaviors and monitoring can actually motivate workers to frequently switch between projects, nega-tively impacting worker and organizational productivity. However, a larger fraction of switching, even in this setting, appeared not to be externally triggered, but rather could be deemed purely discretionary (an estimated 60 percent of all switching observed).28

But what happens when workers no longer have the choice to switch? If organizational

policy provides some workers with permission to stay on task and ignore potential interrup-tions, what would be the impact to productiv-ity? What are the costs of such a strategy? A second study on interruption and forgetting in knowledge-intensive service environments offers some insight.

Handling intrusions and the cost/benefit of sequestering

Where interruptions are costly, it is rea-sonable to consider strategies for avoiding them. Be they emails and texts pouring in through a smartphone, or colleagues pouring in through the office door, interruptions give rise to the question of whether or not lock-ing oneself away from the world and letting someone else deal with those interruptions makes a difference. A second study looked at how this practice, known as sequestering, can impact productivity across knowledge-intensive service environments (see sidebar, “The sequestering study,” for a summary of the research approach).29 Researchers focused exclusively on intrusions, a form of interrup-tion in which a worker stops work on his or her primary task in order to spend time on unrelated activities. Intrusions are prevalent in knowledge-intensive service environments, and to some degree can be controlled through organizational policy, process design, and the

As this study shows, management behaviors and monitoring can actually motivate workers to frequently switch between projects, negatively impacting worker and organizational productivity.

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

9

Page 12: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

layout of the work environment.30 The question was, under what conditions do the benefits of sequestering outweigh the costs? Further, if sequestering is an effective strategy for miti-gating the impact of intrusions, what are its operational implications within knowledge-intensive service environments?

In “mixed services” environments, workers are tasked with completing their regular work while also attending to incoming customer requests, or intrusions. When these intrusions become too great, organizations sometimes de-couple the work both physically and proce-durally.31 De-coupling allows some workers to focus exclusively on one type of work without customer-induced interruptions.32 Enabling this type of focused work has been validated as an operations strategy in service organizations, including health care.33

The specific context of this second study was the radiology department of a large teach-ing hospital. Here, the radiologists’ primary responsibility is to read and interpret patient x-rays while also supporting hospital staff with their immediate treatment needs. For the radiologists, most of these interruptions simply delayed their ability to complete the daily flow of exams that needed processing before the end of their shift. However, in some

cases, an interruption might be so urgent that if the radiologist delayed handling it, there could be drastic consequences to the patient and hospital (for example, an emergent situa-tion in which a patient’s life depends on timely reading of an x-ray). In this study, researchers assessed how a strategy of partial sequestra-tion, in which one of the two department’s radiologists would be protected from incom-ing requests, would impact the department’s overall productivity. The study also evaluated the costs associated with a delay in responding to incoming requests.

Sequestering strategies, their benefits, and their costs

Following this line of inquiry, researchers tested multiple conditions—each unique in terms of the degree of sequestration, amount of forgetting, and frequency of interruption. Findings supported researchers’ primary hypothesis with respect to sequestration:

• Sequestration reduces both “average wait-ing time” and “average flow time” for the primary task.34

Findings supported this outcome in par-ticular when forgetting-induced rework was considered. With knowledge-intensive work

THE SEQUESTERING STUDY This study relates to the influence of an interruption on a single and possibly unique job rather than on a recurring, previously learned task. For example, consider a physician who is interrupted while reviewing a patient’s history and procedure results to decide on future treatment. Once the interruption has ended, the physician must retrace all his or her mental steps—adding rework to the original job—to re-attain knowledge achieved prior to the interruption. As the duration of the interruption increases, so does the amount of job-specific information that is forgotten. Similarly, the further along a worker gets prior to being interrupted, the greater the opportunity for forgetting during the interruption.

After gathering primary empirical data on arrivals and durations of radiology exam readings and interruptions, a series of simulations were performed to test the impact of different levels of sequestering, rework, and interruption arrival rate on performance. The simulation involved two radiologists processing both routine exams and interruptions, which arrived and lasted in accordance with data distributions collected through earlier observation. In total, 50 scenarios were tested, including five levels of forgetting, five levels of sequestering, and two levels of interruption arrival rates. The simulation results were augmented with a cost-ratio variable to examine sequestering effects on total system cost.

Stop or not?

10

Page 13: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

(for example, reading radiology exams), inter-ruptions extend the time it takes to process the work. The added time includes the duration of the interruption plus the rework/relearning time that is required when workers return to their in-process routine work—in this case, needing to re-familiarize themselves with the patient’s history and specifics of the exam in order to develop a reliable diagnosis.

Sequestration allows for some portion of the incoming interruptions to be directed to an unsequestered resource. This frees the sequestered resource to work unin-terrupted, or less often interrupted, on routine tasks, and hence reduces rework/relearning time. As a result, rou-tine tasks are processed more quickly.

But sequestration is not without its costs—in particular, to the speed with which interrup-tions are taken care of. Specifically:

• With sequestration, workers accommo-date interruptions less quickly, and the han-dling of interruptions requires more time.

With sequestration, there is some portion of resources that is dedicated to completing rou-tine jobs. Therefore, it follows that with those resources unavailable to attend to incoming interruptions, the work requested via inter-ruptions waits longer and is processed more slowly. This trade-off between higher through-put for routine work and lower throughput for work requested via interruptions requires a consideration of the overall productivity of the unit, for eventually, all work must be

completed. Findings in this regard supported sequestration strategies. Specifically:

• Sequestration led to overall gains in departmental productivity.

The trade-off in processing times (decrease in routine processing time versus increase in interruption processing time) revealed an average 75-second decrease in routine flow and waiting time, as compared to a 30-second increase in interruption flow and waiting time.

That is, the throughput improvement for routine work was 150 percent greater than the through-put loss for interruptions. Interestingly, this trade-off held, and even got better, when the interruption rate increased. For example, the researchers found that when interruptions doubled, the benefit ratio increased to 400 percent (that is, a five-minute flow and wait time savings for routine work versus a 75-second increase for interruption flow and wait times). In summary:

• With increasing amounts of sequestration, the time required to com-plete all work decreased. All work includes routine

tasks and work requested via interruptions.

Sequestering decreased the time required to complete the department’s work for two reasons. First, routine work accounts for the majority of the total work. And second, with sequestration, the increase in time required to handle an interruption is far less than the time saved by performing routine work under sequestered or partially sequestered condi-tions. Interestingly, a higher interruption rate

That is, the throughput

improvement for routine work was

150 percent greater than the loss for interruptions.

Interestingly, this trade-off held, and

even got better, when the interruption rate increased.

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

11

Page 14: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

made the time savings for the total shift even more pronounced.

An important caveat for sequestering strategy

Research demonstrates that sequester-ing can play an important role in improving productivity for knowledge-intensive work. It does so by disproportionately improving

throughput for routine work at the expense of work requested via interruptions. However, in some settings, there are metrics more useful than time for determining the value of seques-tration. To address this concern, the research-ers introduced differentiated costs of delay for routine and interruption types of work. They found that:

• When the costs of delaying an interruption are greater than the costs of delaying rou-tine work, overall system costs increase.

In the study on which we are reporting, the overall system was a hospital, where there are some potential costs that, if incurred, would outweigh any production efficiencies that could be gained through sequestration (for example, the loss of life that can result from delaying care to a critically ill patient). System costs, and whether these are acceptable costs, depend greatly on context. Therefore, management should evaluate any potential costs associated with implementing seques-tration that prioritizes routine work over incoming interruptions.

Stop or not?

12

Page 15: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Effective work design strategies for multitasking and interrupted knowledge work environments

WORKPLACE interruptions certainly have their costs. But simply know-

ing that doesn’t help, since having the ability to deal with workplace interruptions is key to the success of flexible, reactive organiza-tions. Fortunately, there may be intelligent approaches to reducing the worst kinds of interruptions, less disruptively timing neces-sary interruptions, and matching work assign-ments to those workers most resilient to lost productivity due to interruptions. Both of the studies on which we focus provide insights that knowledge-intensive service organizations can use to develop policies that can enhance pro-ductivity. Based on these insights, we provide recommendations for structuring, assigning, and overseeing projects for companies within knowledge-intensive service industries.

Structuring work

• Structure projects as many short tasks. A modular approach to project design can provide workers with convenient switch-ing points. A series of independent activi-ties that require little information transfer between tasks offers more instances of “near task completion” that may prompt work-ers to be more resistant to switching tasks in response to unanticipated managerial progress checks.

• Assign workers with a high rate of forget-ting to routine project work. When the costs of delaying interruptions are accept-able, free resources who are susceptible to

forgetting from interruptions and encour-age them to adopt a closed-door policy focused on routine work.

• Assign workers who suffer less from forgetting to handle incoming interrup-tions. Also consider shifting these workers to cover routine work that is detail-inten-sive and less conducive to modular design (that is, short tasks). These workers can work in less sequestered contexts to handle more interruptions.

• Determine appropriate sequestra-tion policy (zero, partial, complete) by department. Study both routine work and interruptions to determine the cost of delay for both (include costs for all downstream resources that could be rendered idle by vir-tue of a delay, and/or non-cost factors such as quality or safety). In high-interruption

Workplace interruptions certainly have their costs. But simply knowing that doesn’t help, since having the ability to deal with workplace interruptions is key to the success of flexible, reactive organizations.

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

13

Page 16: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

environments, or during periods where interruptions increase, consider strate-gies to dedicate resources to handling them, leaving others to stay focused on routine tasks.

Supervising work

• Create incentives for higher-priority proj-ects. Savvy managers may realize that they can influence workers to switch projects and that they can use this influence to their advantage, getting their lower-priority proj-ects greater focus. To counter this behavior, structure incentives for both managers and workers to resist switching to low-priority tasks and to focus on high-priority organizational efforts.

• Encourage a predictable approach to proj-ect monitoring. Reward disciplined project managers capable of resisting the urge to perform unannounced progress checks. Workers will be less likely to unnecessarily switch projects if they work with project leaders who refrain from (intentionally or not) intimidating and exerting pressure to influence progress across projects with varying priority levels.

• Communicate and intervene as necessary so workers understand when sequestra-tion is important to maintain. Workers are more likely to resist sequestration when interruptions are occurring in rapid

succession (ironically, when sequestra-tion may be most valuable).35 It is at this time that they will most need to be reas-sured by leaders to stay the course with routine work and let interruptions wait. Messaging should reinforce learning about how sequestration benefits the overall productivity of the collective department or organization.

For your 15-minute investment of seques-tered time, you have gained greater insight into the pervasiveness of interruptions, their costs, and the effectiveness of some common strate-gies for dealing with them. These insights serve as a starting point for management to begin evaluating the nature of project work and the resources employed to tackle it. As was exhib-ited by the radiology department referenced in the sequestration study, the effective use of these insights is highly dependent on context. Therefore, the derived value of these insights can only be realized when they are successfully integrated into a specific knowledge-intensive operating environment.

While the potential benefits suggest this is a worthwhile pursuit, even here we recommend caution. Efforts to restructure work using the guidance presented here should take place in the context of a well-structured process, be appropriately prioritized, and involve focused personnel subjected to regular, yet planned check-ins with higher-level managers. Therein lies the path to efficient execution of the orga-nization’s work effort.

Stop or not?

14

Page 17: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

1. Dave Coplin, The Rise of the Humans: How to Outsmart the Digital Deluge (Petersfield, Hampshire, UK: Harriman House, 2014).

2. Herbert A. Simon, “Theories of decision-making in economics and behavioral science,” American Economic Review 49, no. 3 (1959): pp. 253–283.

3. Daniel Kahneman, Thinking, Fast and Slow (New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux, 2011).

4. As used in this instance, “Deloitte” means De-loitte & Touche LLP, Deloitte Tax LLP, Deloitte Consulting LLP, and Deloitte Financial Advisory Services LLP. Please see www.deloitte.com/us/about for a detailed description of the legal structure of Deloitte LLP and its subsidiaries. Certain services may not be available to attest clients under the rules and regulations of public accounting.

5. R. Cismaru and M. Cismaru, “Laptop use during class: A review of Canadian Universities,” Journal of College Teaching & Learning (Online) 8, no. 11 (2011), pp. 22–28.

6. J.M. Kraushaar and D.C. Novak, “Examining the effects of student multitasking with laptops dur-ing the lecture,” Journal of Information Systems Education 21, no. 2, pp. 241–251.

7. Whittemore School of Business and Economics, University of New Hampshire, “In-class texting behaviors of college students: A university-wide study to determine the in-class texting attitudes and behaviors of students at the University of New Hampshire,” http://www.unh.edu/news/docs/UNHtextingstudy.pdf, accessed November 4, 2014.

8. Jeff Sovern, “Law student laptop use during class for non-class purposes: Temptation v. incen-tives,” University of Louisville Law Review 51, no. 3 (2013): pp. 483–534, http://www.louisvillelaw-review.org/printcontent/51/3/483/jeff_sovern-law_student_laptop_use_during_class_non-class_purposes_temptation_v_incentives.

9. Larry D. Rosen, L. Mark Carrier, and Nancy A. Cheever, “Facebook and texting made me do it,” Computers in Human Behavior 29, no. 3 (May 2013): pp. 948–958.

10. Ibid.

11. D. E. Meyer and R. W. Schvaneveldt, “Meaning, memory structure, and mental processes,” Science 192 (April 2, 1976): pp. 27–33.

12. C. M. Froehle and D. L. White, “Interruptions and forgetting in services,” Production and Operations Management 23, no. 4 (2014): pp. 704–722.

13. G. B. Davis and J. D. Naumann, “Knowledge work productivity,” Emerging Information Technologies: Improving Decisions, Cooperation, and Infrastructure, ed. K. Kendall (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1999), pp. 343–357.

14. The type of thinking required by the tasks that a person is switching to and from will also impact quality degradation. For example, switching from writing a comparative literature essay, which requires precise grammar and formal lan-guage, to sending a casual text message requires a significant change in cognitive demands that will likely result in greater “switching costs,” in this case mistakes in the essay.

15. H. Hembrooke and G. Gay, “The laptop and the lecture: The effects of multitasking in learning environments.” Journal of Computing in Higher Education, 15 (2003), pp. 1–19.

16. K. Foerde, B. J. Knowlton, and R. A. Poldrack, “Modulation of competing memory systems by distraction,” Proceedings of the National Acad-emy of Sciences of the United States of America 103, no. 31 (August 1, 2006): 11,778–83.

17. The full study is reported on in E. Bendoly, M. Swink, and W. P. Simpson, “Prioritizing and monitoring concurrent project work: Effects on switching behavior,” Production and Operations Management 23, no. 5 (2014): pp. 847–860.

18. K. Yaghootkar and N. Gil, “The effects of schedule-driven project management in multi-project environments,” International Journal of Project Management 30 (2012): pp. 127–140.

19. A. T. Jersild, “Mental set and shift,” Archives of Psychology 89 (1927): pp. 5–82; E. Bendoly, J. Perry-Smith, and D. G. Bachrach, “The percep-tion of difficulty in project-work planning and its impact on resource sharing,” Journal of Operations Management, 28, no. 5 (2010): pp. 385–397.

Endnotes

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

15

Page 18: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

20. W. P. Simpson, “Project planning and control when time matters: Focus on process to synchronize and drive results,” Production and Inventory Management Journal 46, no. 2 (2010): pp. 26–43.

21. K. Ball and D. C. Wilson, “Power, control and computer-based performance monitoring: Rep-ertoires, resistance and subjectivities,” Organiza-tion Studies 21, no. 3 (2000): pp. 539–565.

22. T. N. Gilmore, G. P. Shea, and M. Useem, “Side effects of corporate cultural transformations,” Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 33, no. 2 (1997): pp. 174–189.

23. F. Dibachi and R. L. Dibachi, Just Add Manage-ment: Seven Steps to Creating a Productive Workplace and Motivating Your Employees in Challenging Times (New York: McGraw-Hill, 2002).

24. For the purposes of this study, a “task” is defined as a discrete work-step in an overall “project.” Therefore, a project contains many distinguish-able tasks that are completed over time.

25. S. J. Payne, G. B. Duggan, and H. Neth, “Dis-cretionary, task, interleaving: Heuristics for time allocation in cognitive foraging,” Journal of Experimental Psychology 136, no. 3 (2007): pp. 370–388.

26. E. Bendoly and M. Prietula, “In ‘the zone’: The role of evolving skill and transitional workload on motivation and realized performance in operational tasks,” International Journal of Operations and Production Management 28, no. 12 (2008): pp. 1,130–1,152.

27. S. Leroy, “Why is it so hard to do my work? The challenge of attention residue when switching between work tasks,” Organizational Behavior

and Human Decision Processes 109, no. 2 (2009): pp. 168–181.

28. Bendoly, Swink, and Simpson,“Prioritizing and monitoring concurrent project work.”

29. A full write-up of this study can be found in C. M. Froehle and D. L. White, “Interruptions and forgetting in services,” Production and Opera-tions Management 23, no. 4 (2014): pp. 704–722.

30. Ibid.

31. R. Chase, “The customer contact approach to services: Theoretical bases and practical exten-sions,” Operations Research 29, no. 4 (1981): pp. 698–706; R. Metters and V. Vargas, “A typology of de-coupling strategies in mixed services,” Journal of Operations Management 18, no. 6 (2000): pp. 663–682; R. Metters and R. Verma, “History of offshoring knowledge services,” Journal of Operations Management 26, no. 2 (2008): pp. 141–147.

32. S. E. Sampson and C. M. Froehle, “Foundations and implications of a proposed unified services theory,” Production and Operations Management 15, no. 2 (2006): pp. 5–38.

33. N. L. Hyer, U. Wemmerat, and J. A. Morris, “Performance analysis of a focused hospital unit: The case of an integrated trauma center,” Journal of Operations Management 27, no. 3 (2009): pp. 203–219.

34. Waiting time is the total time a job spends in the system not being actively processed; waiting time is a subset of flow time, but only for those jobs that experience waiting. Duration is the time a job takes from the moment it enters the system to when it is completed and exits the system.

35. Froehle and White, “Interruptions and forget-ting in services.”

Stop or not?

16

Page 19: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

Contact

Mark CotteleerResearch directorDeloitte Services LP+1 414 977 [email protected]

How behavioral factors affect decisions related to work interruptions

17

Page 20: Stop or not?€¦ · examine how distractions and/or interruptions impact the productivity of a broad cross-section of workers, from students to radiologists. kinds—from switching

About Deloitte University Press Deloitte University Press publishes original articles, reports and periodicals that provide insights for businesses, the public sector and NGOs. Our goal is to draw upon research and experience from throughout our professional services organization, and that of coauthors in academia and business, to advance the conversation on a broad spectrum of topics of interest to executives and government leaders.

Deloitte University Press is an imprint of Deloitte Development LLC.

About this publication This publication contains general information only, and none of Deloitte Touche Tohmatsu Limited, its member firms, or its and their affiliates are, by means of this publication, rendering accounting, business, financial, investment, legal, tax, or other professional advice or services. This publication is not a substitute for such professional advice or services, nor should it be used as a basis for any decision or action that may affect your finances or your business. Before making any decision or taking any action that may affect your finances or your business, you should consult a qualified professional adviser.

None of Deloitte Touche Tohmatsu Limited, its member firms, or its and their respective affiliates shall be responsible for any loss whatsoever sustained by any person who relies on this publication.

About Deloitte Deloitte refers to one or more of Deloitte Touche Tohmatsu Limited, a UK private company limited by guarantee, and its network of member firms, each of which is a legally separate and independent entity. Please see www.deloitte.com/about for a detailed description of the legal structure of Deloitte Touche Tohmatsu Limited and its member firms. Please see www.deloitte.com/us/about for a detailed description of the legal structure of Deloitte LLP and its subsidiaries. Certain services may not be available to attest clients under the rules and regulations of public accounting.

Copyright © 2014 Deloitte Development LLC. All rights reserved. Member of Deloitte Touche Tohmatsu Limited

Follow @DU_Press

Sign up for Deloitte University Press updates at DUPress.com.