risk protection preschool final 130207

Upload: salvador-david-godoy

Post on 17-Feb-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    1/31

    1RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Risk and protective factors among preschool children for future psychosocial problems

    What we know from research and how it can be used in practice

    Anna-Karin Andershed

    rebro University

    Henrik Andershed

    rebro University

    David P. Farrington

    University of Cambridge

    Contact: Dr. Anna-Karin Andershed, rebro University. E-mail: [email protected]

    Summary

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    2/31

    2RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Despite extensive knowledge from research telling us which traits, behaviors, relations and

    circumstances that increases (risk factors) and decreases (protective factors) the risk for long

    lasting problems, the actual practical use of this knowledge by professionals working with

    children seems very limited. The purpose of this paper is twofold: 1) To review previous

    reviews and meta-analyses in order to identify from empirical research risk and protective

    factors in preschool age (up to age six) for psychosocial problems, specifically externalizing

    and internalizing problems. 2) To discuss how risk and protective factors can be used by

    professionals working with children and their families in the framework of concepts such as

    evidence based practice, risk focused prevention and treatment, and the Risk, Need, and

    Responsivity principles. The review shows that quite many risk and protective factors that are

    potentially changeable and thus practically meaningful have been identified via empirical

    research. It is concluded that there is an extensive amount of research on risk and protective

    factors in preschool age but that this knowledge is not complete or perfect. However, this

    extensive amount of research make up the best available evidence and it should be used by

    well trained professionals so that they can work in an evidence based way to help more

    children to a better life.

    How to refer to this paper:

    Andershed, A-K., Andershed, H., & Farrington, D. P. (2012).Risk and protective factors

    among preschool children for future psychosocial problems.What we know from research

    and how it can be used in practice.Report written on the commission of the Nordic Welfare

    Center.

    Introduction

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    3/31

    3RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    What happens in the lives of young children can influence their future development,

    health and well-being throughout the life span. We now from research that many of the

    psychosocial problems of adolescence and adulthood are associated with risk and protective

    factors that are evident much earlier in life, in the preschool years. Despite this knowledge

    from research telling us which traits, behaviors, relations and circumstances that increases

    (risk factors) and decreases (protective factors) the risk for long lasting psychosocial

    problems, the actual practical use in health care, preschool, social work, and psychiatry of the

    research based knowledge concerning risk and protection has thus far been very limited. The

    purpose of this paper is twofold: 1) To review previous reviews and meta-analyses in order to

    identify from empirical research risk and protective factors in preschool age (up to age six) for

    psychosocial problems, specifically externalizing and internalizing problems. 2) To discuss

    how risk and protective factors can be used by professionals working with children and their

    families in the framework of concepts such as evidence based practice, risk focused

    prevention and treatment, and the Risk, Need, and Responsivity principles.

    The Importance of Focusing Externalizing and Internalizing Problems

    Two of the most common forms of mental health problems in youths are externalizing

    problems, here defined as disruptive, oppositional, defiant, aggressive, and criminal behavior

    and substance using/abusing behavior and internalizing problems, here defined as anxious and

    depressive symptoms or behaviors. This is the case in many western countries (American

    Psychiatric Association, 2000; Kopp & Gillberg, 2003). Both of these problems are associated

    with a range of negative outcomes in adulthood. They not only predict future externalizing

    and internalizing problems, but also substance abuse and dependence, psychiatric illnesses,

    and various types of other psychosocial problems (e.g., Kim-Cohen et al., 2003; Murray &

    Farrington, 2010; Rutter et al., 2006). Hence, it is important to identify the risk and protective

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    4/31

    4RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    factors for these psychosocial problems in research and then to use this knowledge in practice

    with children and their families. Furthermore, it is important to underscore that studies show

    that it is very common among adults with externalizing problems (e.g., criminal behavior) to

    also exhibit other problems, for example substance abuse and mental health problems (Moffitt

    et al., 2001; Wngby et al., 1999). A Swedish study, for example, of about 500 young adult

    women showed that it is statistically typical (significantly more commonly observed than

    what would be expected by chance) to simultaneously exhibit criminal behavior, substance

    abuse, and mental health problems (Wngby et al., 1999). The same study showed that it in

    fact is extremely uncommon (i.e., anti-typicalless commonly observed than expected by

    chance) to exhibit criminality only, (i.e., without substance abuse and mental health problems)

    in young adulthood (Wngby et al., 1999). The implication of this is that the factors

    considered as risk factors externalizing problems can often tend to be risk factors for other

    psychosocial problems as well.

    What are Risk and Protective Factors?

    A risk factor is something, for example a characteristic, relationship, trait, behavior,

    event or circumstance that increases the probability for a certain outcome. When researchers

    label something as a risk factor, this usually means that there is a statistical relationship

    between the factor and the outcome, showing that the two can be observed to exist at the same

    time or change in a parallel fashion. A limitation of most risk factor research is that we cannot

    be sure whether the risk factor will cause the outcome or vice versa. Thus, we cannot be sure

    based on research that an identified risk factor is a cause of externalizing or internalizing

    problems. We can usually only tell that the factor and the outcome in some way co-vary or are

    correlated. A statistically significant association may indicate that the risk factor and the

    outcome indeed are dependent of each other, but, in fact, the risk factor and the outcome may

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    5/31

    5RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    also be related to each other because they both are caused by a third factor and actually are

    not at all causally related to each other.

    A protective factor is something, for example a characteristic, relationship, trait,

    behavior, event or circumstance that decreases the probability for a certain outcome in the

    presence of risk. Thus, the presence of one or several protective factors can make the child

    more resilient against risk factors, i.e., can make it possible for the individual to develop well

    despite the presence of risks. Many, perhaps most children are likely to experience at least one

    risk factor during development. Children growing up in adverse environments will experience

    many risk factors. Thus, strengthening and maximizing protective factors in children is a very

    important task both for parents and professionals working with children. One can also talk

    about promotive factors, which are usually defined as factors being associated with positive

    development regardless of the level of risk of the child. In this paper, the focus will be on

    protective rather than promotive factors.

    Risk and Protective Factors May Exist in Many Layers

    To understand the development of behaviors in general, a perspective that encompasses

    the individual in his or her context is necessary. The holistic-interactionistic perspective

    provides such a point of view (see e.g., Stattin & Magnusson, 1996). It implies that behavior

    develops through continuous interactions between the individual and his or her social

    environment. Hence, characteristics, experiences and conditions of the individual, as well as

    circumstances and conditions of the environment need to be considered when we want to

    explain the development of psychosocial problems. This can be illustrated via a model such as

    the one presented in Figure 1, which shows the different levels /layers in which risk and

    protective factors potentially can be found affecting the development of externalizing and

    internalizing problems. The arrows show that factors on different levels can affect each other

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    6/31

    6RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    (e.g., that the child can affect the family/care-givers and that the care-givers in turn can affect

    the child, etc.).

    Figure 1. An ecological model of reciprocal effects between levels or layers in which risk and

    protective factors can exist.

    Di ff erent types of risk and protective factors

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    7/31

    7RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    There are several ways of categorizing risk- and protective factors and these ways of

    categorizing can have practical implications. First of all, there are factors that are changeable

    (dynamic) and factors that cannot be changed (static). Dynamic factors such as certain

    behaviors of the child or certain relationship qualities, can be affected via interventions

    whereas static factors such as gender or ethnicity cannot. Second, there are direct (proximal)

    and indirect (distal) factors. Direct factors are more directly and causally related to the

    outcome (here defined as externalizing or internalizing problems), while indirect factors are

    more likely to be related to the outcome through their relation to the direct factors. For

    example, a childs problems with disinhibition could be directly related to a childs

    externalizing behavior problems, while young motherhood probably is more indirectly related

    to the childs problem behavior through its potential relation to problems with parenting

    skills, low education, low socioeconomic status, single motherhood, etcetera. Third, there are

    initiating and maintaining factors, where the initiating factors can cause the first appearance

    of externalizing or internalizing problems, while the maintaining factors make the behavior

    continue over time. These factors can be, but are not necessarily always the same. For

    example, shop lifting can be initiated as a consequence of peer pressure, but maintained

    through individual needs of thrill seeking activities.

    The first purpose of the present paper is to review previous reviews and meta-analyses

    in order to identify from empirical research risk and protective factors in preschool age (up to

    age six) for psychosocial problems, specifically externalizing and internalizing problems. The

    second purpose is to discuss how risk and protective factors can be used by professionals

    working with children and their families in the framework of concepts such as evidence based

    practice, risk focused prevention and treatment, and the Risk, Need, and Responsivity

    principles.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    8/31

    8RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Method

    The present review of risk and protective factors for externalizing and internalizing

    problems is a review of previous reviews (i.e., narrative compilations, systematic reviews, or

    meta-analyses). A number of concepts have been used in these publications to describe

    externalizing problems: Delinquency, antisocial behavior, offending, arrests, convictions,

    police contacts, aggression, alcohol use/abuse, conduct disorder oppositional behaviors,

    disruptive behavior disorder, comorbid ADHD and conduct problems. A number of concepts

    have been used in these publications to describe internalizing problems: Anxiety, anxiety

    disorders, depression, mood disorder, poor psychiatric adjustment, internalizing problems. We

    have included clinical (diagnosis) as well as subclinical levels of these problems in our

    review.

    The main literature searches were conducted in PsycINFO, the Campbell Collaboration and

    Cochrane Collaboration libraries, Biological Abstracts and Medline looking for papers classified as

    literature reviews, systematic reviews, or meta-analyses published from 1990 an onward. To be as

    inclusive as possible in this first stage, the search terms were built on overarching and common

    descriptions, rather than specific concepts and narrow syndromes. Consequently, the terms internaliz*,

    externaliz*, conduct*, crim*, antisocial*, delinquen*, depress*, or anxi*were included to capture

    potential outcomes, in combination with the terms risk*,protect*, or resilien*to capture the area of

    research, and child*orpreschool* to capture research on children in the target age period. The terms

    were not restricted to titles or keywords, but could be present anywhere in the database records.

    However, we added the restriction that the papers had to be classified either as reviews or meta-

    analyses, and be written in English. Based on title information, the records were more closely

    scrutinized, and the potentially relevant titles were retrieved in full text. Additional papers were added

    through suggestions from participants of a reference group of researchers in the field.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    9/31

    9RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    We have included recent literature reviews, published since 2006, and meta-analyses

    (no time limit) focusing on prospective, longitudinal research (follow up periods > 2 years) on

    risk and protective factors among children up to age 6 for long-lasting psychosocial problems,

    with a specific focus on externalizing and internalizing problems. The research presented here

    is based exclusively on empirical quantitative scientific studies of risk and protective factors.

    We also use individual, original empirical studies when necessary, to exemplify certain

    important points (e.g., showing that several risk factors are worse than a few).

    It should be noted that studies specifically studying or separating out children in this

    particular age group are not very common, especially in comparison to research on older

    children and adolescents. Actually, the primary reason for exclusion of papers that were

    relevant in topic, was the sample used or the way information was presented. Mostly, samples

    included older children or the papers did not differentiate between children of different ages

    making it impossible to determine whether a certain risk- or protective factor could be

    considered valid for the preschool population or not. Most studies are initiated when children

    are starting to be able to self-report on their feelings, thoughts and behaviors through

    questionnaire responsesusually at school age (approx. age 7 or later). In addition, as

    research is being compiled in reviews or meta-analyses, there is often a lack of differentiation

    between children of different ages. Associations between risks and outcomes for preschoolers

    are rather rarely kept separate from similar associations for older children. For example, a

    common distinction is that of childhood, which usually encompasses all children up to the age

    of 12. In this review, such studies, when the preschool population has not been separated out

    from children of other ages, have not been included.

    A second common reason for excluding a review or meta-analysis from our review was

    the lack of prospective analyses, or lack of possibilities to disentangle cross sectional from

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    10/31

    10RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    longitudinal research due to insufficient presentation of data. We required a minimum follow

    up time of 2 years, since the scope of our review was to describe research on risk and

    protective factors that are related to future psychosocial problems. The entire search

    procedure resulted in that 31 reviews or meta-analyses that met our criteria were included in

    the present review.

    Results

    Which are the Risk and Protective Factors Among Preschool Children?

    Table 1 displays the risk and protective factors identified in our present review of

    previous reviews and meta-analyses. Important to note is that the table in no way rank the risk

    and protective factors. This was not possible to do. Thus, Table 1 is merely a list of risk and

    protective factors that have been shown to significantly be associated with future

    externalizing or internalizing problems.

    What is clear from Table 1 is that quite many factors have been identified as risk and

    protective factors in the preschool years, and that the factors exists primarily in the child and

    the closest social environment, e.g., in the family and among peers. Clear is also that many

    but not all identified factors seem practically useful in that they seem to be potentially

    dynamic/changeable. Moreover, an important finding to highlight is that the opposite side of a

    risk factor is not always a protective factor. This is clear in the results because we do not

    identify with the included reviews and meta-analyses, the same amount of protective factors

    as risk factors.

    Table 1.Risk and Protective Factors among Children up to 6 Years of Age for Future

    Externalizing and Internalizing Problems.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    11/31

    11RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Risk factors inpreschool age for

    externalizingproblems in:a

    Risk factors inpreschool age for

    internalizingproblems in:b

    Risk factorsChildhood/

    Adolescence

    Adulthood

    Childhood/

    Adolescence

    Adulthood

    Adoptee2, 30 Sleep problems, malnutrition19, 31 Cognitive factors , , , ,

    Neurological abnormalities, low verbal and performance IQ, low

    IQ, low cognitive ability

    Conduct problems14, 22, 23, 29Externalizing problems, behavior problems, conduct disorder,

    oppositional behaviors, disruptive behavior disorder

    ADHD-symptoms20, 22, 28, 29, 31Hyperactivity, attention problems,impulsivity, restlessness, poor motor coordination, delayed motor

    development

    Temperamental factors20, 22, 29, 31Difficult temperament,irritability, disinhibition, undercontrol, aggression, sensation

    seeking, fearlessness, low harm avoidance

    Early behavioral inhibition, Oppositional defiant disorder , , ,12

    Internalizing problems, depression , , Marital conflict, home discord, family stress , Abuse and neglect1, 7, 10, 11, 14, 16, 19, 22, 29Maltreatment, neglect,

    physical punishment, physical abuse, family violence, witnessingviolence

    Demographic factors7, 10, 14, 21, 22, 29

    Family structure, large family size, young motherhood, marital

    disruption, early broken home, single parenthood, parental

    separation, foster care

    Socioeconomic factors10, 21, 22, 27, 29

    Low SES, low income, overcrowding, social dependency

    Parents characteristics and behaviorsMothers low IQ, parents low education

    7, 22

    Mothers alcohol use, parents alcoholism, parents criminality7,

    22, 31

    Mothers depression, parents psychopathology6, 7

    Parent-child relationship factors4, 6, 7, 8, 26, 13, 15, 19, 22, 24, 31

    Poor parenting, deviant parent-child interaction, coerciveinteractions, insecure attachment, poor monitoring/supervision,

    low involvement, mothers negative control, low affect, low

    responsiveness, rejection, low warmth

    Protective factorsChild factors5, 9, 10, 15, 17, 18, 27

    Effective self-regulation, emotion regulation, easy temperament, high IQParent-child relationship factors4, 5, 9, 15, 17, 18, 27

    Parental acceptance and responsivity, warmth, secure attachment, positive family relationships, few infant-

    caregiver separationsEnvironmental factors

    Physical aspects of caregiving environment (safety, stimulation)ae.g., Delinquency, antisocial behavior, offending, arrests, convictions, police contacts, aggression, alcohol

    use/abuse, conduct disorder oppositional behaviors, disruptive behavior disorder, comorbid ADHD andconduct problems,be.g., Anxiety, anxiety disorders, depression, mood disorder, poor psychiatric adjustment, internalizing problems

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    12/31

    12RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    The number given at the risk and protective factors in the table means that the corresponding review has shownthat this factor is a risk or protective factor.

    1Bedi & Goddard, 2007; 2Bimmel et al., 2003; 3Boylan et al., 2007; 4Brumariu & Kerns, 2010 ; 5Condly, 2006; 6Degnan etal., 2010; 7Derzon, 2010; 8Epkins & Heckler, 2011; 9Eriksson et al., 2011; 10Farrington et al., in press; 11Gewirtz & Edleson,2007; 12Kertz & Woodruff-Borden, 2011; 13Kroneman et al., 2009; 14Leschied et al., 2008; 15Luthar, 2006; 16Maas et al.,2008; 17Masten & Obradovi, 2008; 18Masten & Wright, 2010; 19Mendes et al., 2009; 20Moffitt, 2006; 21Murray &Farrington, 2010; 22Murray et al., 2010; 23Petitclerc & Tremblay, 2009; 24Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994; 25Tandon et al., 2009;26van IJzendoorn et al., 1999; 27Vanderbilt-Adriance & Shaw, 2008 ; 28Waschbusch, 2002; 29Welsh & Farrington, 2007;30Wierzbicki, 1993; 31Zucker et al., 2008

    Several Perhaps Many Risk Factors of Limited Predictive Value Before Age 3

    An important observation we make via some of the reviews and their included

    longitudinal studies is that predictions are difficult from observations and assessments of very

    young children, below 2-3 years of age (Moffitt, 2003; Rutter et al., 2006) and the predictions

    become increasingly reliable with age (Leschied et al., 2008; Rothbaum & Weisz, 1994). For

    example, behavior problems assessed before the childs age of 3 are less enduring than when

    assessments are made between ages 3 to 6 (Cai, 2004). Thus, this indicates that one should be

    careful in using these kinds of risk factors for risk assessment purposes before age 3.

    Generalizability of the Risk and Protective Factors?

    Most of the research included in the reviews summarized in Table 1 has been conducted

    in the United States. There are, however, few reasons to believe that the underlying processes

    connecting risk and protective factors to outcomes are substantially different between children

    in different western countries. This is probably particularly true for risk and protective factors

    on the individual- and family levels. Contextual factors, partly dependent on differences in

    conditions applicable to for example availability of health care and social security systems,

    may be more of subject to differences between countries.

    Single Factors are Generally Weak but Some Risk And Protective Factors Seem

    Stronger and More Important Than Others

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    13/31

    13RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    The single risk and protective factors in preschool children, identified in research and

    shown in Table 1 are significantly (i.e., are not due to chance), but generally quite weakly

    associated with a higher (risk factors) or lower (protective factors) risk for externalizing or

    internalizing problems in adolescence and/or adulthood. It was not possible to calculate the

    actual effect sizes for the protective factors. However, for risk factors, it is clear that the effect

    sizes (when presented in the reviews or meta-analyses) are small for the majority of the single

    factors (i.e., that most children exhibiting only one of these individual risk factors will have a

    very good chance of not developing externalizing or internalizing problems in the future).

    Very few studies have investigated the differential and unique effects of risk and

    protective factors in preschool children. However, a British large-scale longitudinal study

    The 1970 British Cohort Studydid just this and followed about 16.000 preschool children

    (assessment of risk factors at age 5) into early adolescence (age 10) and adulthood (age 30-34)

    (Murray et al., 2010). Among the factors identified as risks (see Table 1) and assessed at age

    5, conduct problems/externalizing problems was clearly the single most important risk factor

    for conduct problems at age 10. Conduct problems at age 5 increased the risk about four times

    for conduct problems at age 10. The other risk factors at age 5 increased the risk about 1.5-2.0

    times for conduct problems at age 10. This was true among both boys and girls. In terms of

    the risk for adult criminality, conduct problems did not stand out as much and all risk factors

    including conduct problems increased the risk for adult criminality about equally much (1.5-

    2.0 times each) (Murray et al., 2010). Thus, in preschool age, conduct problems of the child

    may be the single most important risk factor for adolescent problem behavior. Other risk

    factors tend to be quite similar in their unique predictive power of long-lasting behavior

    problems.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    14/31

    14RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    The More Risk Factors the Higher the Risk and the More Protective Factors the Better

    the ProtectionThe Cumulative/Additive Effects of Risk and Protective Factors

    Both risk and protective factors tend to have cumulative effects according to research,

    i.e., the more risk factors the higher the risk and the more protective factors the stronger the

    protection (Appleyard et al., 2005; Eriksson et al., 2010; for a review of cumulative effects).

    For example, in the 1970 British Cohort Study, with an increasing number of risk factors at

    age 5, the more the children developed conduct problems in adolescence. Prevalences

    increased from 2% and 5% (with 0 risk factors) to 38% and 54% (with 5 or more risk factors)

    for girls and boys, respectively. In terms of predicting adult criminality, the tendency was the

    same with the number of children developing adult criminality increasing from 3% and 17%

    (with 0 risk factors) to 11% and 44% (with 3 or more risk factors) for girls and boys,

    respectively (Murray et al., 2010). Similarly, average scores on the Child Behavior Checklist

    (CBCL) externalizing and internalizing indices at ages 7 and 16 were found to increase with

    accumulation of risk factors assessed at 4.5 years among the participants in a longitudinal

    study of at-risk urban children (Appleyard et al., 2005).

    Protective Factors Not Always the Opposite Sides of Risk Factors

    A protective factor is, according to the definition used here, something that decreases the

    probability for a certain outcome in the presence of risk. Thus, the presence of one or several

    protective factors can make the child more resilient against risk factors, i.e., can make it

    possible for the individual to develop well despite the presence of risks. When we define

    protective factors in this way it is clear that the opposite sides of risk factors not always are

    protective. This is clearly shown in the list of factors identified in the present reviewsee

    Table 1. This is in fact an important conclusion because to our experience, a common opinion

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    15/31

    15RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    among professionals working with children is that the opposite side of risk always is

    protective.

    The Complexity and Heterogeneity of Risk and Protective factors

    Research provides us with a rather extensive list of risk and protective factors as shown in

    Table 1. A complexity and limitation of this research is that we based on research, as of yet,

    do not know which of these factors that have causal relations to externalizing or internalizing

    problems. We know that the identified risk and protective factors co-vary with higher and

    lower risk respectively for future externalizing or internalizing problems. Co-variation

    between a potential cause and the effect is necessary but not sufficient to conclude causality.

    Furthermore, different risk and protective factors will apply in different ways to different

    children, and the same factor can vary in importance between individual children. Two

    processes, equifinality and multifinality, are of special importance to mention in this context.

    Equifinality (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996) refers to the fact that a certain outcome can develop

    through several different developmental pathways and have different causal backgrounds for

    different children. Behaviors can have many different roots, and the patterns of the most

    salient and important risk factors will vary between individuals. Thus, individuals developing

    externalizing or internalizing problems will often do so with different risk factors.

    Multifinality (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996) refers to the process through which the same risk

    factors are associated with different outcomes. Hence, the same kinds of risk factors can lead

    to different things.

    More Similarities Than Differences Between Boys and Girls Concerning Risk and

    Protective Factors

    Even though boys are clearly overrepresented when it comes to externalizing problems, and

    girls are overrepresented with internalizing problems, the same risk factors seem relevant

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    16/31

    16RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    regardless of gender (see Moffitt et al., 2001. Neither are there any clear or strong indications

    of that protective factors would be crucially different between boys and girls (Eriksson et al.,

    2010). There are, however, indications that boys are often exposed to a higher number and

    greater level of risk for externalizing problems than girls, even though the risks in and of

    themselves are basically the same (see Moffitt et al., 2001). The practical implication of this is

    that an assessment of risk and protective factors does not need to be different for boys and

    girls.

    Heritability and Upbringing and Risk and Protective factors

    Several studies show that genetic as well as environmental factors are important for the

    development of externalizing and internalizing problems (see e.g., Arseneault et al., 2003;

    Eley et al., 2003; Kendler et al., 2006). One way of understanding this is that the genetic

    makeup of the individual affects the early development of the nervous system of that

    individual which makes up the disposition, vulnerability or sensitivity of the individual for

    developing various genetically underbuilt risk factors. Several of the risk factors listed in

    present article such as for example; hyperactivity, attention problems, restlessness, are at least

    partly genetically underbuilt (e.g., Larsson et al., 2004). Important to note here is that these

    partially genetically underbuilt risk factors can of course be intervened with effectively

    although they are in part genetically underbuilt.

    Discussion

    The first purpose of this paper was to identify risk and protective factors in preschool age for

    externalizing and internalizing problems. Our review of previous reviews and meta-analyses

    of preschool risk and protective factors showed that quite many potentially changeable and

    thus practically useful risk and protective factors have been identified via empirical research.

    Because most reviews identified in the present review have focused on risk factors for

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    17/31

    17RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    externalizing problems in adolescence or adulthood it is important reiterate that several,

    perhaps many, of the identified risk factors also are risk factors for other problems as well,

    such as substance use problems and other mental health problems (see e.g., Moffitt et al.,

    2001; Wngby et al., 1999). A purpose of the present paper was also to discuss how this

    knowledge of risk and protective factors can be used in social work practice with children and

    their families. Therefore, we now move on to discuss some of the complexities and nuances of

    risk and protective factors and then move on to discuss how this knowledge can be used in

    practice.

    Using Knowledge about Risk and Protective Factors in PracticeA Concrete Way of

    Working Evidence Based

    Evidence based practice concerns from which sources professionals gather information to

    handle their roles as professional decision makers and important people in interventions. In

    order to work in an evidence based manner, the professional must use the best available

    evidence gathered from research when making decisions about how to approach and intervene

    with the problems of children and their families. The professional must also integrate his or

    her professional expertise and the perspective of the individual child and the family into their

    decision making process (see Sackett et al., 1996). This integration of sources of knowledge

    can be illustrated by the Evidence Based Practice triad presented in Figure 1.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    18/31

    18RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Figure 2. The Evidence Based Practice (EBP) -triad.

    This review can be seen as a source of available evidence concerning risk and protective

    factors in preschool age for externalizing and internalizing problems. This evidence, gained

    through longitudinal empirical research concerning risk and protective factors, is however

    based on groups of children. A risk factor is thus a factor that generally means a bit higher

    risk for externalizing or internalizing problems. This means that many children exhibiting this

    risk factor will never develop externalizing or internalizing problems. Here is where the

    professional and his or her expertise and capability to analyze the risk and protective factors

    in adequate ways comes in and becomes essential for the plan for interventions. The essential

    analyses that the professional need to learn to master to carry out adequately are the ones that

    deals with the extent a particular risk factor present in a specific case really is a crucial and

    causal risk factors for that specific individual and whether the weak protective factor observed

    has the potential to make the individual more resilient to risks.

    Best

    evidence

    from

    research EBP

    Professionals

    expertise

    Childrens and

    families expectationsand values

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    19/31

    19RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Risk-Focused Prevention and Treatment

    A very concrete way of integrating the best available evidence into practice is to make

    use of existing research on risk and protective factors in tailoring interventions. This can be

    done within the framework of risk-focused prevention and treatment. A risk-focused approach

    in prevention and treatment is based on the idea that modification of key risk factors for a

    certain problem will decrease the problem. Similarly, strengthening key protective factors for

    a certain problem will buffer against or modify the effects of risk for the problem (Farrington

    & Welsh, 2007). The risk focused approach then basically builds on two steps: 1) Identify

    children with risk factors and perhaps also with weak protective factors. Assess risk and

    protective factors of the child and family in detail if possible. 2) Aim interventions to reduce

    the risk factors and to strengthen the protective factors. Adhering to this approach is a very

    hands-on way to link research, practice and policy making (Farrington et al., in press) that has

    been considered by several governments as both plausible and practical (Farrington & Welsh,

    2007).

    Three Useful Guiding Principles in Risk Focused Prevention and TreatmentThe Risk,

    Need, and Responsivity Principles

    When applying risk-focused prevention or treatment in practice, there are three guiding

    principles that are very useful: the principles of Risk, Need, and Responsivity (e.g., Andrews

    et al., 1990; Andrews & Bonta, 2006; Dowden & Andrews, 1999, 2002, 2003). Actually,

    several meta-analyses have shown that the use of these principles will increase the possibility

    for implementing effective interventions among children as well as adults (Andrews et al.,

    1990; Dowden & Andrews, 1999, 2002, 2003; Lipsey, 2009).

    The Risk principle implies that an intervention will be more effective if the most

    intensive efforts are focused on children at high risk. Hence, the most ambitious and intensive

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    20/31

    20RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    interventions should be directed to children at high risk for future problems. As described

    above, children with several risk factors are generally at higher risk than children with fewer

    risk factors. The Need principle implies that interventions are more effective if they are

    tailored to focus on the specific childs most relevant needs, that is , the factors that explain or

    cause the problems of the particular child at hand. The Responsivity principle says that

    interventions need to be tailored, in terms of both content and delivery, to suit the abilities,

    inclination, and motivations of the specific child and family. This principle says that the

    professional must think about how the interventions should be planned and implemented so

    that the child and the family respond to the intervention. Clearly, a reliable and valid

    assessment of risk and protective factors is essential for professionals to conduct to be able to

    work in accordance with these three important principles because one needs to know things

    about risk and protective factors to be able to do a risk assessment (the Risk principle) and to

    identify the needs of the child and the family (and follow the Need principle).

    Structure is Important in Assessments

    Thus, when working practically and concretely according to risk-focused prevention and

    treatment and according to the Risk, Need, and Responsivity principles, it is essential to

    assess risk and protective factors of the child and in his/her surroundings. This can be done in

    at least two main ways: With or without structure/instrument. Structure is here referred to as

    clear definitions of the risk and protective factors to be assessed, as well as clear and well-

    defined rating scales of the risk and protective factorswhich are typical characteristics of

    structured assessment instruments. Examples of structured instruments that involve risk and

    protective factors identified in this review are the Early Assessment Risk List for Boys under

    age 12 (EARL-20B; Augimeri, Webster, Koegl, & Levene, 1998), the Early Assessment Risk

    List for Girls under age 12 (EARL-21G; Levene et al., 2001), and the Evidence-based

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    21/31

    21RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    STructured assEssment instrument of Risk and protective factors (ESTER-assessment;

    Andershed & Andershed, 2010). A structured method or instrument will provide clear

    definitions and a set way of conducting the assessment and the assessment will also be

    documented in a systematic, structured and standardized manner. Achieving high so called

    inter-rater agreement is a basic feature of legal security in assessments. This means that two

    independent raters agree to a high extent in the assessment of for example a child and his or

    her family. This is easier to achieve when using a structured assessment instrument (see e.g.,

    Andershed et al., 2010; Enebrink, Lngstrm, Hultn, & Gumpert, 2006). In terms identifying

    risk and protective factors, the use of a structured instrument is also a good thing. A recent

    study showed that professionals trained in and using a structured instrument (ESTER-

    assessment) identified significantly more risk and protective factors in a case, as compared to

    professionals not trained in nor using a structured instrument (Andershed & Andershed, in

    prep.). Thus, if correctly used, there are many advantages of using structured instruments to

    assess risk and protective factors. Thus, we would argue based on research that professionals

    should use structured instruments when assessing risk and protective factors in children and

    their families.

    Risk Focused Prevention Needs to be Carried Out With Care by Well Trained

    Professionals

    Professionals working the approach and principles described here need appropriate

    training/education to do so. Such training need to include basic schooling in risk and

    protective factors, the research underlying these factors and its strengths and limitations. It

    also need to include training in how to communicate this information to the family and, not

    the least, how to use the information about risk and protective factors in making effective

    plans of interventions.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    22/31

    22RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Using research, based on groups of children, such as the research presented here, as a

    means for identifying individual children at risk will always be a difficult act of balance. For

    example. some high risk children and families will not develop long lasting problems, while

    others will. Unfortunately, they will not necessarily appear to be very different at the time of

    assessment. Hence, there is no definite way for a professional to determine who the children

    and families are that will fare well in spite of risk. Professional will likely identify and

    intervene with a larger number of children than what is necessary. In the same fashion

    however, we will also fail to detect some children and families who need professional help.

    These limitations of risk-focused prevention and treatment need to be known by the involved

    professionals. A very important aspect of applying general risk and protective factors on

    individual children and families has to do with the professionals communication with the

    care givers. Professionals have to be able to communicate riskwhy certain factors are being

    assessed, what high risk means, etcin a very nuanced way, to avoid potential negative

    effects in terms labeling of the child or in terms of poor relations between the family and the

    professional.

    What Professions Should Work with Risk Focused Prevention and Treatment?

    All professionals working with preschool children, given the adequate training and

    supervision can apply the concrete thinking of risk focused prevention and treatment. It is a

    very concrete way of working in an evidence based way. It is however likely that the concrete

    assessments and interventions will be carried out in different ways depending on where we

    are (e.g., primary child and family health care, psychiatry, preschool, social work, etc)

    because of differences in amount of time for assessments and interventions and because of

    differences in the purpose of work of the specific profession, but with the same focus and on

    the same risk and protective factors.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    23/31

    23RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    A Process of Assessment, Intervention, and Follow-Up Involving Risk and Protective

    Factors

    In Figure 3 below we propose a process of assessment, intervention, and follow-up which

    involves the practical use of risk and protective factors. First, a structured assessment is

    carried out, preferably using a structured checklist, instrument, or questionnaire. This is done

    by a trained professional or professionals. This results in a list of risk factors that are present

    and that needs to be reduced or ultimately eliminated and protective factors that are weak and

    that needs to be strengthened in the specific child and his/her family. Now, the professional or

    professionals carries out an analysis of these risk and protective factors using the Risk, Need,

    and Responsivity principles. The Risk principle: How high is the risk for future problems and

    of high priority is this child and family for the most intensive interventions? The Need

    principle: Which are the needs of the child and the family, e.g: which of the risk factors

    present are the most important to intervene with for this particular child and family? Which of

    the risk factors are causing the problem behavior/s of the child? The Responsivity Principle:

    Which interventions can be effective to reduce the most important risk factors and strengthen

    the most important protective factors and howshould they be delivered to the child and the

    family to be effective? Then, interventions are carried out and after some time, as follow-up

    assessment is conducted and a new analysis starts, that can result in that the interventions can

    stop because they have been very successful or that the same interventions that have been

    carried out need to continue, or that some or all interventions need to be changed in order to

    gain positive changes in the child. We would argue that this kind of circular process should be

    structurally undertaken in practice until the final goals of the interventions have been reached.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    24/31

    24RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Figure 3. A Process of Assessment, Intervention, and Follow-Up Involving Risk and

    Protective Factors.

    Final Comments and Conclusions

    There is an extensive amount of research on risk and protective factors in preschool age for

    future problems. This knowledge is not complete or perfect. There are many things we need to

    know more about concerning risk and protective factors. However, this extensive amount of

    research make up the best available evidence for the time being and it should be used by well

    trained professionals so that they can work in an evidence based way to help more children to

    a better life.

    Observed factors

    Pronounced Risk

    factors: E.g:

    Aggression

    Hyperactivity

    Poor parenting

    Rejection

    Weak Protective

    Factors: E.g:

    Warmth

    Safety

    Available interventions

    Intervention A

    Intervention B

    Intervention C

    Intervention D

    Intervention E

    Structured

    assessmentby

    trained

    professional/s of

    risk- and protective

    factors

    Analysisby

    trained

    professional/s

    concerning

    Risk? Need?

    Responsivity?

    Analyses to tailor

    a plan for

    intervention/s

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    25/31

    25RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    References

    American Psychiatric Association (2000).Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

    disorders (4th ed., Text Revision). Washington, DC: Author.

    Andershed, H., & Andershed, A-K. (2010). Risk-need assessment for youth with or at risk for

    conduct problems: Introducing the assessment system ESTER.Procedia Social and

    Behavioral Journal, 5,377-383.

    Andershed, H., Fredriksson, J., Engelholm, K., Ahlberg, R., Berggren, S., & Andershed, A-K.

    (2010). Initial test of the new risk-need assessment instrument for youths with or at risk

    for conduct problems: ESTER-assessment.Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences, 5,

    488-492.

    Andershed, A-K., & Andershed, H. (in prep.).Are Structured Assessments of a Youths Risks

    and Needs Really Better Than Unstructured?A Case Vignette Study.Manuscript in

    preparation.

    Appleyard, K., Egeland, B., van Dulmen, M. H. M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2005). When more is

    not better: the role of cumulative risk in child behavior outcomes.Journal of Child

    Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 235-245.

    Augimeri, L. K., Webster, C. D., Koegl, C. J., & Levene, K. S. (1998).Early Assessment Risk

    List for Boys: EARL-20B. Version 1: Consultation edition.Toronto, Canada: Earlscourt

    Child and Family Centre.

    Bedi, G., & Goddard, C. (2007). Intimate partner violence: What are the impacts on children ?

    Australian Psychologist, 42, 66-77.

    Bimmel, N., Juffer, F., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2003). Problem

    behavior of internationally adopted adolescents: A review and meta-analysis.Harvard

    Review of Psychiatry, 11, 64-77.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    26/31

    26RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Boylan, K., Valliancourt, T., Boyle, M., & Szatmari, P. (2007). Comorbidity of internalizing

    disorders in children with oppositional defiant disorder.European Child & Adolescent

    Psychiatry, 16, 484-494.

    Brumariu, L. W., & Kerns, K. A. (2010). Parent-child attachment and internalizing symptoms

    in childhood and adolescence: A review of empirical findings and future directions.

    Development and Psychopathology, 22, 177-203.

    Condly, J. S. (2006). Resilience in children. A review of the literature with implications for

    education. Urban Education, 41, 211-236.

    Connell, A. M. & Goodman, S. H. )2002). The association between psychopathology in

    fathers versus mothers and childrens internalizing and externalizing behavior problems:

    A meta-analysis.Psychological Bulletin, 128, 746-773.

    Degnan, K. A., Almas, A. N., & Fox, N. A. (2010). Temperament and the environment in the

    etiology of childhood anxiety.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 497-

    517.

    Derzon, J. H. (2010). The correspondence of family features with problem, aggressive,

    criminal, and violent behavior: A meta-analysis.Journal of Experimental Criminology,

    6, 263-292.

    Enebrink, P., Lngstrm, N., Hultn, A., & Gumpert, C. H. (2006). Swedish Validation of the

    Early Assessment Risk List for Boys (EARL 20B), a decision aid for use with children

    presenting with conduct-disrordered behaviour.Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 60,438-

    446.

    Eriksson, I., Cater, ., Andershed, A-K., & Andershed, H. (2011). What protects youth from

    externalising and internalizing problems? A critical review of research findings and

    implications for practice.Australian Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 21, 113-125.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    27/31

    27RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Epkins, C. C., & Heckler, D. R. (2011). Integrating etiologial models of social anxiety and

    depression in youth: Evidence for a cumulative interpersonal risk model. Clinical Child

    and Family Psychology Review, 14, 329-376.

    Evans, S. E., Davies, C., & DiLillo, D. (2008). Exposure to domestic violence: A meta-

    analysis of child and adolescent outcomes.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13, 131-

    140.

    Farrington, D. P., Loeber, R., & Ttofi, M. M. (in press). Risk and protective factors for

    offending. In B. C. Welsh & D. P. Farrington (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of crime

    prevention. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Farrington, D. P., & Welsh, B. C. (2007). Saving children from a life of crimeEarly risk

    factors and effective interventions.New York: Oxford University Press.

    Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A-M., &

    Roisman, G. I. (2010). The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in

    the development of childrens externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child

    Development, 81, 435-456.

    Gewirtz, A. H., & Edleson, J. L. (2007). Young childrens exposure to intimate partner

    violence: Towards a developmental risk and resilience framework for research and

    intervention.Journal of Family Violence, 22, 151-163.

    van IJzendoorn, M. H., Schuengel, C., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (1999). Disorganized

    attachment in early childhood: Meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae.

    Development and Psychopathology, 11, 225-249.

    Kendler, K. S., Gatz, M., Gardner, C. O., & Pedersen, N. L. (2006). A Swedish National Twin

    Study of Lifetime Major Depression.American Journal of Psychiatry, 163,109-114.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    28/31

    28RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Kertz, S. J., & Woodruff-Borden, J. (2011). The developmental psychopathology of worry.

    Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 14, 174-197.

    Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Harrington, H., Milne, B. J., & Poulton, R. (2003).

    Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental disorder: developmental follow-back of a

    prospective-longitudinal cohort.Archives of General Psychiatry, 60,709-17.

    Kitzmann, K. M., Gaylord, N. K., Holt, A. R., & Kenny, E. D. (2003). Child witnesses to

    domestic violence: A meta-analytic review.Journal of Consulting and Clinical

    Psychology, 71, 339-352.

    Kopp, S., & Gillberg, C. (2003). Swedish child and adolescent psychiatric outpatients. A five-

    year cohort.European Child and Adolescent Psychiaty, 12,30-35.

    Kroneman, L. M., Loeber, R., Hipwell, A., E., & Koot, H. M. (2009). Girls disruptive

    behavior and its relationship to family functioning: A review.Journal of Child and

    Family Studies, 18, 259-273.

    Larsson, J-O., Larsson, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2004). Genetic and Environmental

    Contributions to Stability and Change of ADHD Symptoms Between 8 and 13 Years of

    Age: A Longitudinal Twin Study.Journal of the American Academy of Child &

    Adolescent Psychiatry, 43,1267-1275.

    Leschied, A., Chiodo, D., Nowicki, E., & Rodger, S. (2008). Childhood predictors of adult

    criminality: A meta-analysis drawn from the prospective longitudinal literature.

    Canadian Journal of Criminology and Criminal Justice,50, 435-467.

    Levene, K. S., Augimeri, L. K., Pepler, D. J., Walsh, M. M., Webster, C. D., & Koegl, C. J.

    (2001).Early Assessment Risk List for Girls (EARL-21G). Version 1 - Consultation

    Edition.Toronto, Canada: Earlscourt Child and Family Centre.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    29/31

    29RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Luthar, S. S. (2006). Resilience in development: A synthesis of research across five decades.

    In D. Cicchetti & J. D. Cohen (Eds.). Developmental psychopathology (2nded). Volume

    3, Risk, disorder, and adaptation (pp. 739-795). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Inc.

    Maas, C., Herrenkohl, T. I., & Sousa, C. (2008). Review of research on child maltreatment

    and violence in youth. Trauma, Violence, & Abuse, 9, 56-67.

    Masten, A. S., & Obradovi, J.(2008). Disaster preparation and recovery: lessons from

    research on resilience in human development.Ecology and Society, 13, 9.

    Masten, A. S. & Wright, M., OD. (2010). Resilience over the lifespan: Developmental

    perspectives on resistance, recovery, and transformation. In J. W. Reich, A. J. Zautra, &

    J. S. Hall (Eds.).Handbook of adult resilience(pp. 213-237). New York: Guilford.

    Mendes, D. D., de Jesus Mari, J., Singer, M., Machado Barros, G., & Mello, A. F. (2009).

    Study review of the biological, social and environmental factors associated with

    aggressive behavior.Rev Bras Psiquiatr, 31 (suppl II), S77-85.

    Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Life-Course-Persistent and Adolescence-Limited antisocial behavior. A

    10-year research review and a research agenda. In B. B. Lahey, T. E. Moffitt, & A.

    Caspi (Eds.), Causes of conduct disorder and juvenile delinquency(pp. 49-75). New

    York: Guilford Press.

    Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Life-course-persistent versus adolescence-limited antisocial behavior. In

    D. Cicchetti & D. J. Cohen (Eds).Developmental psychopathology, Vol 3: Risk,

    disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed.) (pp 570-598). Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons Inc.

    Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, a., Rutter, M., & Silva, P. A. (2001). Sex differences in antisocial

    behavior. Conduct disorder, delinquency, and violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal

    Study.Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    30/31

    30RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Murray, J., & Farrington, D. P. (2010). Risk factors for conduct disorder and delinquency:

    Key findings from longitudinal studies. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 55, 633-

    642.

    Murray, J., Irving, B., Farrington, D. P., Colman, I., Bloxson, A. J. (2010). Very early

    predictors of conduct problems and crime: results from a national cohort study.Journal

    of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1198-1207.

    Petitclerc, A., & Tremblay, R. E. (2008). Childhood disruptive behaviour disorders: Review

    of their origin, development, and prevention. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 54,

    222-231.

    Rothbaum, F., & Weisz, J. R. Parental caregiving and child externalizing behavior in

    nonclinical samples: A meta-analysis.Psychological Bulletin, 116, 55-74.

    Rutter, M., Kim-Cohen, J., & Maughan, B. (2006). Continuities and discontinuities in

    psychopathology between childhood and adult life.Journal of Child Psychology and

    Psychiatry, 47, 276-295.

    Sackett, D. L., Rosenberg, W. M. C., Gray, J. A. M., Haynes, R. B., & Richardson, W. S.

    (1996). Evidence based medicine: what it is and what it isnt.BMJ, 312, 71-72.

    Tandon, M., Cardeli, E., & Luby, J. (2009). Internalizing disorders in early childhood: A

    review of depressive and anxiety disorders. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of

    North America, 18, 593-610.

    Tatano-Beck, C. (1999). Maternal depression and child behavior problems: A meta-analysis.

    Journal of Advanced Nursing, 29, 623-629.

    Vanderbilt-Adrience, E., & Shaw, S. D. (2008). Conceptualizing and re-evaluating resilience

    across levels of risk, time and domains of competence. Clinical Child an dFamily

    Psychology Review, 11, 30-58.

  • 7/23/2019 Risk Protection Preschool FINAL 130207

    31/31

    31RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS IN PRESCHOOL AGE

    Waschbusch, D. A. (2002). A meta-analytic examination of comorbid hyperactive-impulsive-

    attention problems and conduct problems.Psychological Bulletin, 128, 118-150.

    Wierzbicki, M. (1993). Psychological adjustment of adoptees: A meta-analysis.Journal of

    Clinical Child Psychology, 22, 447-454.

    Wngby, M., Bergman, L. R., & Magnusson, D. (1999). Development of Adjustment

    Problems in Girls: What Syndromes Emerge? Child Development, 70,678-699

    Zucker, R. A., Donovan, J. W., Masten, A. S., Mattson, M. E., & Moss, H. B. (2008). Early

    developmental processes and the continuity of risk for underage drinking and problem

    drinking.Pediatrics, 121 (suppl 4), S252-272.