recognition of emotional facial expressions in...

144
RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF SOCIAL SCIENCES OF MIDDLE EAST TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY BY PINAR DURSUN IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE IN PSYCHOLOGY JUNE 2007

Upload: others

Post on 13-Jul-2020

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS

A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF SOCIAL SCIENCES

OF MIDDLE EAST TECHNICAL UNIVERSITY

BY

PINAR DURSUN

IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR

THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE IN

PSYCHOLOGY

JUNE 2007

Page 2: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

Approval of the Graduate School of (Name of the Graduate School) Prof. Dr. Sencer Ayata

Director

I certify that this thesis satisfies all the requirements as a thesis for the degree of Master of Science

Prof. Dr. Nebi Sümer Head of Department This is to certify that we have read this thesis and that in our opinion it is fully adequate, in scope and quality, as a thesis for the degree of Master of Science.

Assoc. Prof. Faruk Gençöz Supervisor

Assoc. Prof. Adnan Cansever (GATA)

Assoc. Prof. Faruk Gençöz (METU, PSY)

Dr. Özlem Bozo (METU, PSY)

Page 3: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

iii

I hereby declare that all information in this document has been obtained and presented in accordance with academic rules and ethical conduct. I also declare that, as required by these rules and conduct, I have fully cited and referenced all material and results that are not original to this work.

Name, Last name: Pınar Dursun

Signature :

Page 4: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

iv

ABSTRACT

RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS

Dursun, Pınar

M.S., Department of Psychology

Supervisor: Assoc. Prof. Faruk Gençöz

June 2007, 130 pages

The ability to recognize emotional facial expressions (EFE) is very critical for

social interaction and daily functioning. Recent studies have shown that alcohol

dependent individuals have deficits in the recognition of these expressions. Thereby,

the objective of this study was to explore the presence of impairment in the decoding

of universally recognized facial expressions -happiness, sadness, anger, disgust, fear,

surprise, and neutral expressions- and to measure their manual reaction times (RT)

toward these expressions in alcohol dependent inpatients. Demographic Information

Form, CAGE Alcoholism Inventory, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Beck

Depression Inventory (BDI), The Symptom Checklist, and lastly a constructed

computer program (Emotion Recognition Test) were administered to 50 detoxified

alcohol dependent inpatients and 50 matched-control group participants. It was

hypothesized that alcohol dependents would show more deficits in the accuracy of

reading EFE and would react more rapidly toward negative EFE -fear, anger, disgust,

sadness than control group. Series of ANOVA, ANCOVA, MANOVA and

MANCOVA analyses revealed that alcohol dependent individuals were more likely

Page 5: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

v

to have depression and anxiety disorders than non-dependents. They recognized less

but responded faster toward disgusted expressions than non-dependent individuals.

On the other hand, two groups did not differ significantly in the total accuracy

responses. In addition, the levels of depression and anxiety did not affect the

recognition accuracy or reaction times. Stepwise multiple regression analysis

indicated that obsessive-compulsive subscale of SCL, BDI, STAI-S Form, and the

recognition of fearful as well as disgusted expressions were associated with

alcoholism. Results were discussed in relation to the previous findings in the

literature. The inaccurate identification of disgusted faces might be associated with

organic deficits resulted from alcohol consumption or cultural factors that play very

important role in displaying expressions.

Keywords: Emotion; Emotional Facial Expression; Recognition Accuracy;

Alcoholism

Page 6: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

vi

ÖZ

ALKOL BAĞIMLISI OLARAK YATAN HASTALARDA YÜZDEKİ DUYGU İFADELERİNİN ALGILANMASI

Dursun, Pınar

Master, Psikoloji Bölümü

Tez Yöneticisi: Doç. Dr. Faruk Gençöz

Haziran 2007, 130 sayfa

Yüzdeki duygu ifadelerini doğru bir biçimde tanıyabilmek, sosyal etkileşim ve

gündelik işlevsellik adına çok önemlidir. Son yıllarda yapılan çalışmalar, alkol

bağımlılarının yüzdeki duygu dışavurumlarını algılamalarında sorun yaşadıklarını

göstermektedir. Böylece, bu çalışmanın amacı, alkol bağımlılarında evrensel olarak

kabul edilen yüzdeki duygu dışavurumlarının- mutluluk, üzüntü, kızgınlık, iğrenme,

korku, şaşırma ve nötr- algılanmasında bir bozukluk olup olmadığını saptamak ve bu

dışavurumların algılanmasındaki reaksiyon zamanlarını ölçmektir. Bu amaçla, 50

arındırılmış alkol bağımlısı yatan hasta ve eşleştirilmiş 50 kontrol grubu

katılımcılarına, Demografik Bilgi Formu, CAGE Alkol Bağımlılığı Skalası,

Durumluk-Süreklilik Kaygı Envanteri (DSKE), Beck Depresyon Envanteri (BDE),

Ruhsal Belirti Tarama Testi (SCL-90-R) ve son olarak araştırmacılar tarafından

geliştirilmiş “Duygu Tanıma Testi” uygulanmıştır. Hipotezlerimiz; alkol

bağımlılarının kontrol grubuna göre bu dışavurumları algılarken daha fazla hata

yapacağı ve negatif dışavurumlar olan korku, kızgınlık, iğrenme ve üzüntüyü

algılarken daha çabuk reaksiyon göstereceği yönündedir. Uygulanan bir çok

Page 7: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

vii

ANOVA, ANCOVA, MANOVA ve MANCOVA analizleri, alkol bağımlılarının

bağımlı olmayanlara göre daha fazla depresyon ve anksiyete bozukluklarına eğilimi

olduğunu ortaya koymuştur. Bağımlılar, iğrenme ifadesini daha az ancak daha hızlı

algılamışlardır. Öte yandan, iki grup, doğru yanıtlarının toplamında istatistiksel

olarak anlamlı bir farklılık göstermemişlerdir. Ayrıca, depresyon ve anksiyete

düzeylerinin algılama doğruluğunda ya da reaksiyon zamanlarında bir etkisi olmadığı

görülmüştür. Stepwise Regresyon Analizi, SCL’nin obsesif-kompülsif alt ölçeğinin,

BDE’nin, SDKE-D Formunun ve korku ve ek olarak iğrenme duygu

dışavurumlarının algılanmasının alkolizm ile ilişkili olduğunu göstermiştir.

Çalışmanın sonuçları literatürdeki bulgular bağlamında tartışılmıştır. İğrenme

ifadesinin yanlış algılanması, alkol tüketiminin sonucu olan organik bir bozuklukla

ya da ifadelerin yanlış yorumlanmasında rol oynayan kültürel faktörlerle

ilişkilendirilebilir.

Anahtar Sözcükler: Duygu; Yüzdeki Duygu Dışavurumları; Algılama Doğruluğu;

Alkol Bağımlılığı

Page 8: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

viii

To my parents İnci-Ahmet Dursun, my sister Ekin Dixmier

and my dearest friends,

for their love and inspiration…

Page 9: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

ix

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study is the first achievement of my life. Sometimes, I felt very hopeless

and anxious in order to complete this study properly. I really appreciate to some

people who always supported me and believed in me.

First, I would like to express my deepest gratitude to my supervisor Assoc.

Prof. Faruk Gençöz for his guidance, advice, criticism, encouragements throughout

the study. Also, I would like to thank his beautiful and wise wife, Assoc. Prof. Tülin

Gençöz for her valuable comments and statistical help. It was really pleasure to work

with them and to have been exposed to their original ideas to progress this study.

I would like to thank my commitee members Assoc. Prof. Adnan Cansever,

and Dr. Özlem Bozo for their help, trust, and sincerity.

I would like to express my deepest gratitude to Prof. Dr. Arşaluys Kayır for

her continued warm and supports as well as encouragement that make me feel more

self-confident and relieved.

I am very grateful to Kayhan İnce for his help to construct Emotional

Recognition Test and patience to help me. Additionally, I wish to express my

gratitude to Dr. Refhan Balkan Öztürk for her kind and support and help at collecting

data at Bakırköy Mental and Neurological Diseases Hospital AMATEM clinic, as

well as the clinic staff there.

I would like to thank all the alcohol dependent inpatients and volunteers for

their participation, trust and sincerity.

I owe many thanks to my dearest friends Şafak Çakmak, Irmak Odabaş,

Seyha Hocaoğlu, my editor Evrim Sabak and S. Murat Direr for their love, altruism,

endless encouragement, support, sincere understanding, valuable suggestions, and

finally tolerance as well as patience that helped me to reduce my anxiety and

motivated me even in the hardest time of the thesis process.

Finally, I am forever indepted to my family for their unconditional love, care,

understanding, and trust when I needed most.

Page 10: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

x

TABLE OF CONTENTS

PLAGIARISM iii ABSTRACT iv ÖZ vi DEDICATION viii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ix TABLE OF CONTENTS x LIST OF TABLES xiii LIST OF FIGURES xiv CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

1.1. Emotion 1

1.1.1. Philosophical Background of Emotion 1 1.1.2. Perspectives on Emotion 3 1.1.2.1. Evolutionary Perspective and Charles Darwin 3 1.1.2.2. The Psychodynamic Perspective 5 1.1.2.3. Peripheral Perspective and James-Lange Theory of Emotion 5 1.1.2.4. Central Perspective and Cannon-Bard Theory of Emotion 7 1.1.2.5. Cognitive Perspectives 8 1.1.3. Emotion and Related Concepts: Feeling, Affect, Emotional States, Mood, and Emotional Disorders

10

1.1.4. Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE) 13 1.1.5. Basic or Discrete Emotions 15 1.1.5.1. Basic or Discrete Emotions: Definitions 17 1.1.5.1.a. Happiness 17 1.1.5.1.b. Sadness 19 1.1.5.1.c. Fear 20 1.1.5.1.d. Anger 21 1.1.5.1.e. Surprise 23 1.1.5.1.f. Disgust 25 1.1.6. Universality of Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE) 25 1.1.7.The Neuropsychology of Emotion 28 1.1.7.1. The Hypothalamus 28 1.1.7.2. The Limbic System 28 1.1.7.3. The Cortex 29 1.1.8. The Development of Emotions 30 1.1.8.1. The Development of the Expression of Emotions 31 1.1.9. The Emotional Facial Expression (EFE) and Cognition 32 1.1.9.1. Affective/Mood Disorders, Anxiety Disorders and Recognition of Facial Expressions (EFE): The Literature Review

32

1.1.9.2. Affective/Mood Disorders, Anxiety Disorders and Reaction Times towards Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE): The Literature Review

35

1.1.9.3. Other Psychopathological Disorders, Recognition Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE) and Reaction Times towards EFE: The

39

Page 11: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

xi

Literature Review 1.2. Alcohol Dependence or Alcoholism 42 1.2.1. Ethyl Alcohol 42

1.2.2. Alcohol and Harm 43 1.2.3. Alcohol Dependence or Alcoholism 43 1.2.4. History of Classification 44 1.2.5. Current Diagnostic Classification 45 1.2.6. Epidemiology 47 1.2.7. Alcohol Dependence and Co-morbid Disorders 48 1.2.8. Alcohol Dependence and Cognition 49 1.2.9. Alcohol Dependence and Recognition of Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE): The Literature Review

50

1.3. Purposes of the Present Study 52 1.4. Hypotheses of the Present Study 53 2. METHOD 54 2.1. Participants 54 2.2. Materials 54 2.2.1. Demographic Information Form 55 2.2.2. CAGE Alcoholism Inventory 55 2.2.3. The Symptom Distress Checklist (SCL 90-R) 56 2.2.4. State- Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) 58 2.2.5. Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) 58 2.2.6. Emotion Recognition Test 59 2.3. Procedure 60 2.4. Data Analyses 60 3. RESULTS 63 3.1. Demographic Characteristics of Participants 63 3.2. CAGE Alcoholism Inventory, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), State- Trait Anxiety Inventory, (STAI) and Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R) Scores

65

3.3. Decoding Accuracy in General 67 3.3.1. Decoding Accuracy of Each Facial Expression 68 3.4. Manual Reaction/Recognition Times for Accurate Responses in General 71 3.4.1. Reaction Times for Accurate Responses towards Each Facial Expression

71

3.5. Covariates with Accuracy Decoding in General 73 3.5.1. Covariates with Decoding Accuracy in Each Facial Expression 74 3.6. Covariates with Manual Reaction Times for Accurate Responses 76 3.6.1. Covariates with Reaction Times for Accurate Responses toward Each Facial Expression

76

3.7. Misinterpretations of Emotional Facial Expressions 78 3.8. Stepwise Multivariate Regression 81 4. DISCUSSION 84 4.1. Alcohol Dependence and Co-morbid Disorders 84 4.2. Decoding Accuracy of Emotional Facial Expressions 85

Page 12: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

xii

4.3. The Effects of Depression, State-Trait Anxiety, and Symptom Checklist on the Decoding Accuracy of Emotional Facial Expressions

87

4.4. Reaction Times for Accurate Responses toward Emotional Facial Expressions

89

4.5. The Effects of Depression, State-Trait Anxiety, and Symptom Checklist on the Reaction Times for Accurate Responses toward Emotional Facial Expressions

90

4.6. Misinterpretations of Emotional Facial Expressions 91 4.7. The Roles of BDI, STAI-S, SCL-90, Recognition of Disgust and Fear Emotional Expressions in the Prediction of Alcoholism

94

4.8. General Discussion and Conclusions 98 4.9. Limitations of the Present Study 100 4.10. Clinical Implications 102 4.11. Directions for Future Research 103 REFERENCES 105 APPENDICES 117 Appendix A: Demographical Information 117 Appendix B: CAGE Alcohol Use Inventory 118 Appendix C: Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) 119 Appendix D: State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) 122 Appendix E: The Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R) 126 Appendix F: Basic Emotional Facial Expressions 130

Page 13: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

xiii

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1 Demographic Variables 64 Table 2 Demographic Variables 64 Table 3 Descriptive Statistics of BDI, STAI, CAGE and SCL-90-R Results 66 Table 4 Descriptive Statistics of Subscales of SCL-90-R Results 67 Table 5 ANOVA Results of Recognition of Accuracy 68 Table 6 Descriptive Statistics of Accuracy Scores towards all Emotional

Expressions 68

Table 7 Descriptive Statistics of Accuracy Scores towards Negative Emotional Expressions

69

Table 8 Descriptive Statistics of Accuracy Scores towards the Expressions of Happiness, Surprise, and Neutral

70

Table 9 ANOVA Results of Happy Emotional Expressions 70 Table 10 ANOVA Results of Surprised Emotional Expressions 70 Table 11 ANOVA Results of Neutral Expressions 70 Table 12 ANOVA results of Reaction Times towards all Expressions 71 Table 13 Descriptive Statistics of Reaction Times towards Negative

Expressions 72

Table 14 ANOVA results of Reaction Times towards Happy Expressions 72 Table 15 ANOVA results of Reaction Times towards Surprised Expressions 73 Table 16 ANOVA results of Reaction Times towards Neutral Expressions 73 Table 17 Descriptive Statistics of Reaction Times towards the Expressions of

Happiness, Neutral and Surprise 73

Table 18 ANCOVA Results of Accuracy Decoding 74 Table 19 MANCOVA with Decoding Accuracy in Each Facial Expression 75 Table 20 MANCOVA with Reaction Times for Accurate Responses towards

Each Negative Facial Expressions 77

Table 21 Descriptive Statistics of Accurate Responses of All Participants towards All Emotional Expressions

80

Table 22 Variables in each step for Stepwise Multiple Regressions using Demographics, BDI, STAI, SCL-90, Facial Expressions and Reaction Times to predict Alcoholism Level

82

Table 23 Summary of Stepwise Multiple Regressions using demographics, BDI, STAI, SCL-90, Recognition of Facial Expressions due to predict alcohol dependence based on CAGE

83

Page 14: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

xiv

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Episodes of Emotion. A spectrum of affective phenomena in terms of the time course

12

Page 15: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

1

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

In this chapter, firstly description, perspectives of emotion and emotional facial

expressions are explained. Then, inaccurate recognition of facial expressions in the

literature review is presented. Secondly, general information regarding alcohol

dependence and related literature review about impairment in decoding emotional facial

expression are mentioned. Finally, the purposes and the hypotheses of the present study

are explained.

1. 1. EMOTION

Generally, in Western culture, emotions are always seen as the enemies of

rationality. Mostly, they are considered as primitive or childish rather than mature or

civilized. Tears are stupid, tears are childish, and tears are sign of weakness. However,

as Ekman and Friesen says “… emotions are the most private, personal, and unique part

of ourselves. They are parts of ourselves that have enormous power over our lives”

(1975, p. 5). If emotions had not been existed, we would not have felt proud when our

loved ones do something worthy, we would not have experienced joy at the birth of our

children, anxiety when threatened, and grief at the loss of our loved ones (Lazarus,

1991, p. 3; Planalp, 1999, p. 10). Actually, without emotion, nothing makes any

difference, we become totally indifferent. They determine the quality of our lives.

1. 1. 1. Philosophical Background of Emotion

Emotions have always been central concern to human beings. Almost every

great philosopher from the periods of BC to contemporary ages had been concerned

with the nature of emotion and had theorized about its origins and expressions.

According to one of the well-known philosophers, Democritus (500 BC)

Page 16: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

2

happiness or positive emotion was characterized by a state of mental and physical

equilibrium (Wikipedia, 2007). Similarly, Hippocrates, coeval of Democritus and

almost regarded as the founder of modern medicine, claimed that emotional states were

characterized by brain temperature, aridity, and moisture and he was the first

philosopher to establish a relationship between brain functioning and emotion. For

instance, according to him grief and anxiety arise from brain cooling, fear arises from

brain overheating (Wikipedia, 2007).

Plato (427-347 BC) was also one of the pioneers in proposing the interaction

between soul and body. He proposed a “Three State Theory.” These states were

Pleasure, Pain, and Neutral. Plato described pain as “the destruction of organic

harmony, pleasure as the recovery of this destruction, and neutral state as the harmony

itself” (Wikipedia, 2007).

However, according to Aristoteles (384-322 BC) who was a student of Plato,

pleasure was a normal state of a living and conscious being. Pain arose if any

obstruction occurred in this normal state. In other words, pain was the contrary of

nature. Clearly, Aristoteles distinguished emotional mental processes in three parts.

First one was potentialities of experiencing passions, capacities or predispositions;

second one was formed habits, namely repeated experience of Passions, disposition or

character; and third one was passions which are states accompanied by pain and

pleasure. According to Aristoteles, there were opposite feelings such as anger and

mercifulness, and love and fear. Additionally, in “rhetoric”, which is one of Aristoteles’

famous books, emotions were described as only biological products such as sneezing,

and they were contrary of thoughts and they affect the judgments (Oatley & Jenkins,

1996, p.12). In short, Aristoteles suggested that emotions had effects on cognition and

actually, they were based on our interpretation of events. Namely, Aristoteles

unwittingly claimed that emotions had cognitive roots.

In Patristic, Medieval, and Renaissance periods, emotion was a popular issue

among philosophers and literacy authors. Later, 17th century became more significant

than previous periods. For instance, Descartes, the founder of modern philosophy was

the first philosopher to propose the separation of mind and body. In his most well

Page 17: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

3

known book “The Passions of the Soul”, he discussed the relationship between emotion

and neurophysiology. In addition, he claimed that there were six fundamental emotions,

which were love, hatred, sadness, joy, desire, and lastly wonder. And he discussed these

emotions in terms of neurophysiological dimensions (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p.15).

According to Descartes, emotions that were related to our physiological structure

resulted from soul, in which thinking takes place and he concluded that emotions could

be controlled by our thoughts. Moreover, Descartes agreed with Aristoteles’

assumptions that emotions rely on our evaluations of events. However, in his attempt to

improve upon Aristoteles and Thomas Aquinas, who saw emotion as experiencing and

evaluating stimuli in terms of their potential for gain and pleasure, Descartes’ research

led writers to confuse the mind-body problem with a soul-body problem (Candland et

al., 1977, p. 21).

Lastly, one of the most important 17th century philosophers Spinoza, who is

considered as a rationalist philosopher assumed that, the universe was a reflection of the

perfect God and human beings were a part of this great reflection. We human being,

God and this universe were all one. According to him, love was the main emotion;

however, there were other subsequent emotions such as envy, resentment, or passion

(Frijda, 1986, p. 265). Similar to Aristoteles, Spinoza seemed to have a cognitive view

toward emotions, as Spinoza also proposed that although emotion and idea had different

characteristics; emotion was determined by ideas (Deleuze, 2000, p. 12-20). Indeed,

more or less this vision constitutes a basis of cognitive approach.

1. 1. 2. Perspectives on Emotion

1. 1. 2. 1. Evolutionary Perspective and Charles Darwin

The evolutionary perspective on emotion derives from Charles Darwin the father

of modern biology. At the beginning of 20th century, in “the expression of the emotions

in man and animals”, that is one of the most important books of Darwin; he (1934)

stated that expressions of emotions were heritages from our past evolutionary habits.

Generally, emotions were derived from habits in which our evolutionary or individual

past had once been useful. However, some expressions of emotions still have served an

Page 18: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

4

adaptive purpose. Darwin stressed their communicative function, animals including

humans, signal their readiness to fight, run, or attend to each other’s needs through a

variety of postural, facial, and other nonverbal communication. For instance, a baby’s

cry sends a signal to its parents, just as bared teeth display anger (Kowalski & Westen,

2005, p. 363). Additionally, “when infants feel hunger or discomfort or suffer, they

scream and in that case, their eyes are firmly closed so that their round skin becomes

wrinkled. The mouth is widely opened, with the lips retracted in a strange manner,

which makes it as a squarish form. The breath is inhaled inconsistently. This expression,

the firm closing eyelids and compression of the eyeball serve to protect the eyes from

becoming gorged with blood” (Darwin, 1934, p. 74). However, Darwin (1934) proposed

that some expressions of emotions did not have any function today, for example,

sneering is undeveloped remain of snarling that was functional in previous stage of

development of human beings (cited in Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 2). Darwin gave

concrete examples such as weeping that indicates an extreme form of pain, as shown by

the writhing of the whole body and teeth clenching that are accompanied by sweating

and trembling (1934, p. 72).

Moreover, Darwin (1872) defined and arranged expressions of emotions based

on their original functions. He thought that emotions resembled reflex-like mechanisms

(cited in Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p.2). He claimed that emotions were similar to reflex

actions that appeared due to the excitement of the peripheral nerve, which transmitted

its influence to certain neurons, and these transmissions, excited certain muscles or

glands to action. For example, coughing and sneezing were familiar instances of reflex

actions (Darwin, 1934, p.10).

According to Darwin, facial expressions of emotions were universal, not

learned, same in all cultures, biologically determined and occurred in many animal

species (1934, p.1; Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p.23; Stein & Oatley, 1992, p. 161). He

strongly emphasized the similarities between nonhuman and human. For instance,

animals also showed wonder, curiosity, imitation, attention, memory, and reasoning just

like human beings. Darwin (1934) concluded that facial expressions were important

tools for communication and recognition of emotions served a healthy relationship

Page 19: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

5

between both humans and nonhumans (cited in Lazarus, 1991, p. 71).

1. 1. 2. 2. The Psychodynamic Perspective

Indeed, Sigmund Freud (1856-1939) the founder of classic psychoanalytic

theory did not directly propose a theory of emotion. He had little to say about the

structure of emotions. Instead, he emphasized determinable relationship between

perception and sensation (Candland et al., 1977, p. 62). His views on the nature of

emotions are complex and primarily on anxiety. Generally, he focused on emotional

traumas, inner conflicts, and personality. He considered emotions to be “archaic

discharge syndromes”. He thought that emotions are not always simple. Often they are

felt obscurely, some emotions and their meanings become clear only by expressing

them, or in talking them to another person or in reflecting upon them. In his famous

book on emotions “Inhibitions, symptoms and anxiety,” Freud formulated a new

conception of nature of anxiety that is considered as the result of an evaluation by the

ego of dangerous aspects of external or internal environment (Candland et al., 1977, p.

63).

Briefly, psychoanalysis has been primarily concerned with certain classes of

danger situations: birth, hunger, and absence of mother, loss of love, fear of castration,

fear of death, and fear of conscience. Namely, anxiety is not the result of repression that

is used in order to cope with dangerous situations, on the contrary, the reason for

repression (Candland et al., 1977, p. 193; Lazarus, 1991, p.235). According to

psychodynamic theorists, people can be unconscious of their own emotional experience,

that unconscious emotional processes can influence thought, behavior, or even health.

This perspective also suggests that we regularly delude ourselves about our own

abilities and attribute these abilities due to avoid unpleasant emotional consequences

(Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 360).

1. 1. 2. 3. Peripheral Perspective and James-Lange Theory of Emotion

Contemporary wisdom claims that William James’ great achievement was to

introduce psychology to American people. As a psychologist, one of his intellectual

Page 20: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

6

achievements was to emphasize the peripheral component of the emotional experience,

an emphasis James made explicit in what has come to be called the James-Lange theory

of emotion over a century ago. In 1884, W. James proposed the first important

physiological theory of emotion. He suggested that bodily changes directly follow the

perception of an exciting “fact” and that “our feeling of the same changes as they occur

is the emotion”. In other words, James argued that emotion is rooted in the bodily

experience. According to him, first, we perceive the object then bodily response occurs

and lastly emotional arousal appears (Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 347). For instance,

when we see a stimulus such as a bear, we have a ponding heart, we begin to run and

than we fear. We do not run because of fear, we fear because of running. We feel sorry

because we cry, angry because we strike, afraid because we tremble (Candland et al.,

1977, p. 25). When his Danish colleague Carl Lange independently proposed a similar

view in 1885, since then this theory has been known as James-Lange theory of emotions

(Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 348). Lange proposed that vasomotor disturbances

following environmental events constitute emotional reactions, that the cognitive

qualities of emotion are secondary to the physiological qualities (Candland et al., 1977,

p. 87). According to Candland et al. although these two theories have obvious

differences, the two theoretical positions have historically been grouped together into

one theory (1977, p. 87).

In detail, James described two kinds of emotions, the coarser, and the subtler:

the former means that “one recognizes a strong organic reverberation”, the second one

is “those whose organic reverberation is less obvious and strong”. He implied an

acceptance of the evolutional nature of emotion: there are many emotions, they are

changeable, not stable, and they shade the coarser emotions into one another (Candland

et al., 1977, p. 24). Specifically, the subtle emotions are learned or acquired ones. For

example, resentment is learned through association with past events. However, the

coarser emotions are emotions that all human beings experience identically such as fear.

To sum up, the most influential writers on emotion were Aristoteles and

Descartes who were able to look toward the body and used physiological functioning of

the human beings. Continental theories from Descartes to James were primarily

Page 21: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

7

centralistic, and they posited some form of a mind that based on the interpretation of

events, this interpretation provided us emotions or feelings. Undoubtedly, James

changed the emphasis from the central mind to periphery organs. Thanks to James, he

did not only make physiological researches but also he shifted attention from the central

mind to the peripheral structures of the body (Candland et al., 1977, p. 20).

1. 1. 2. 4. Central Perspective and Cannon-Bard Theory of Emotion

James-Lange theory of emotion could not be replicated and this theory met with

a great deal of criticism. Generally, these criticisms included the secondary position of

cognition when compared to physiology; it was questioned that the same environmental

stimulus did not elicit identical emotions in different people. Among these criticisms,

perhaps the most ardent one belong to Walter B. Cannon (1927-1931). Cannon

proposed an alternative theory suggesting that emotions are cognitive rather than

physiological state of arousal. He perceived the sequence of events as external

stimulation followed by neural processing followed by physiological reactions. Philip

Bard expanded Cannon’s theory by showing the thalamic structures for the expression

of emotion, this general theoretical position came to be referred to as the “Cannon-Bard

Theory”. This novel theory included that emotion-inducing stimuli simultaneously elicit

both an emotional experience, such as fear, and bodily responses such as sweating

(Candland et al., 1977, p. 87-88; Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 348). In the central view,

overt response follows upon brain activity that produces experience; response thus

follows upon experience. In other words, we run and tremble because we feel frightened

(Frijda, 1986, p. 177).

To conclude, indeed only certain aspects of James-Lange and Cannon-Bard

theories of emotion are opposite. The major point of disagreement between these

positions really concerns the point in time or in the sequence of events when

physiological functioning is an important concept in explaining emotions. James

stressed the importance of precognitive or determining physiological states, whereas

Cannon emphasized the importance of post-cognitive or response-type physiological

states (Candland et al., 1977, p. 88). In fact, these theories can be assumed as

Page 22: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

8

complementary rather than opposites.

1. 1. 2. 5. Cognitive Perspectives

During the past decade, interest to the role of emotion in cognition and in

behavior has increased dramatically. In fact, it can be assumed that there have been two

approaches to the issues concerning cognition and emotion. The first was initiated by

Charles Darwin and was concerned with emotional expression and behavior. The

second approach was associated primarily with the work of William James. Indeed, both

Darwin’s and James’ writings indicated that they took for granted the assumption that

some kind of perception and evaluation must precede an emotional response (Candland

et al., 1977, p. 192). However, the relationship between cognition and emotion seemed

to be based on the 5th century, for instance Plato believed that reason must direct the

passions. Nowadays, contemporary theories are predominantly based on cognitive

principles to explain both the causes and consequences of emotions.

In terms of the nature of emotions, many cognitive theorists believe that emotion

depends on the interpretation or appraisal system. Appraisal can be defined as a kind of

personal meaning of an event, which includes the evaluations of the significance of the

facts for personal well-being (Leon & Hernandez, 1998). Similarly, Eich et al.

suggested that people have a perceptual-interpretive system that analyzes and evaluates

environmental stimuli for their emotional significance. This environmental stimulus is

interpreted based on cognitions and then the appropriate emotion arises (2000, p. 88).

First, the facts must be appraised for personal benefit and harm, and then an emotion

occurs. The way people respond emotionally depends on the appraisals they make, in

other words, their inferences about causes of the emotion and surely, their own bodily

sensations are crucial in emotional experience (Leon & Hernandez, 1998).

According to Stanley, Schachter and Jerome Singer (1962) (Schachter- Singer

theory) emotion involves two factors, first is the physiological arousal and second is the

cognitive interpretation of this arousal (cited in Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 361).

Specifically, when people experience a state of nonspecific physiological arousal, which

could be anger, happiness, or others, they attempt to figure out what these arousals

Page 23: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

9

mean for their own sake. Meanwhile, although facial expressions are major source of

information about people’s emotions, knowledge about the situation can influence or

sometimes override information from the face (Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 362). In

order to interpret the arousal, people generally use situational cues. They respond as

what the situational cues suggest. Shortly, cognitive processes play a central role in

interpreting other people’s emotions. However, numerous studies support some degree

of Schachter-Singer theory. According to many cognitive theorists, people’s emotions

also reflect their judgments and appraisals of the situations or stimuli that confront them

not only their appraisal mechanisms (Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 361).

Specifically, in terms of the antecedents of emotions, basic emotions are

typically caused by the perceptions of general categories of events: happiness arises

with a perception of the progress towards a goal; sadness arises when a goal is lost or

when major loss of an anticipated pleasure; anger occurs when a plan is blocked;

perceived threats to one’s body and self-esteem leads to anxiety; a goal conflict or a

threat to self-preservation leads to fear; a perception of something to reject leads to

disgust; and desire is produced by a perception of something to approach and so on.

These emotion production rules are also central to various appraisal theories of emotion

(Eich et al., 2000, p. 88; Stein & Oatley, 1992, p. 209).

Some cognitive theorists prefer to explain emotion profoundly as a process that

includes five basic components; first is objects or precipitating events, second is

appraisal, third is physiological changes, fourth is action or expression, and final

component is regulation (Planalp, 1999, p. 11). According to their point of view, this

process begins with a precipitating event. It seems better to explain process theories of

emotion by giving a specific example on sadness. For instance, sadness is generally felt

when an undesirable event happens, such as loss of loved one or separation or being

rejected. Surely in order to generate sadness, this event must be appraised not only as

negative but also as feeling of weakness. Otherwise anger or fear can be felt. And

physiologically one can be tired, has low in energy. In action stage, withdrawing from

social contact or talking less cause sadness, regulation stage includes talking to someone

closer about sad feelings or events, or trying to act happily (Planalp, 1999, p. 12).

Page 24: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

10

To sum up, there are many perspectives and theories about the nature or causes

of emotions. Some has been able to replicated; some has been rearranged based on

contemporary findings.

1. 1. 3. Emotion and Related Concepts: Feeling, Affect, Emotional States, Mood,

and Emotional Disorders

The phenomena to which the label “emotion” or “emotional” is a difficult

matter, also there has not been an agreement due to distinguish them. For example,

some people consider hunger as an emotion; others do not (Frijda, 1986, p. 1). For this

reason, to begin with, it is very important to define some terms.

Literally, the word emotion and its derivates have passed through identifiable

stages. In 17th and18th centuries, written language remained faithful to the Latin

derivation of emotion, namely, emovere “to move away from”.

Emotion refers to in:

1695: a moving out, a migration

1735: causing movement

1822: a physical moving, stirring, or agitation

The application of the word to mental states developed simultaneously:

1660: a vehement or excited mental state

1735: tending or able to excite emotion

1808: a mental feeling or affection (for example, of pain, desire, hope)

1847: connected with the feelings or passions (Candland et al., 1977, p. 4).

By the late 19th century, it was accepted to distinguish emotion from cognition

(reasoning) and volition (willing). This separation still continues. Emotion is commonly

thought to be a separate faculty, but it interferes with rational aspects of our minds, and

subverts or distorts our motivation or values. In sum, neither common usage nor the

definitions offered by theorists clarify the problem of defining emotion because the

meanings of the term continue to evolve.

To begin with, emotion can be defined as an internal reaction of feeling, which

Page 25: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

11

may be either positive (such as joy) or negative (such as anger), and may reflect a

readiness for action (Vasta, Haith & Miller, 2000, p. 448).

According to Oatley and Jenkins, emotion is generally caused by a person

consciously and unconsciously evaluating an event as relevant to a goal that is

important; the emotion is felt as positive when a goal is reached, as negative when a

goal is obstructed. Another definition of these authors is that, “an emotion is usually

experienced as a distinctive type of mental state, and sometimes is accompanied or

followed by bodily changes, expressions and actions” (1996, p. 96). On the other hand,

Eich and et al. stated that “an emotion has properties of a reaction: it often has an

identifiable cause such as a stimulus or antecedent thought, intense experience of short

duration, emotions usually have high cognitive involvement and the person is aware of

them” (2000, p. 89).

Probably, the most comprehensive definition belongs to Ekman who defines

emotion as a whole process, “a particular kind of automatic appraisal is influenced by

our evolutionary and personal past, and then a set of physiological changes and

emotional behaviors begin to deal with the situation”. Usually, they occur in a response

to a social event that can be real, remembered, or imagined (Ekman, 1993). Surely

words are important in terms of defining and dealing with our emotions however, as

Ekman says “emotions are not so simple to reduce into words” (2003, p. 13).

In general, many terms are used to illustrate emotion such as mood, affect, and

feeling, which are generally used interchangeably; however, there are some distinctions

between these terms. Contemporary American and English oral usage often

differentiates these terms: for instance, primarily emotion is used to refer to observable

behavior, and feelings to inferred states. Thus, emotion is assumed as intense but

temporary, and feelings as weak but more permanent (Candland et al., 1977, p. 4). On

the other hand, Lazarus defines feeling as a sensory perception, such as feelings of pain,

pleasure, and distaste, rather than as emotion (1991, p. 57). Indeed, in many studies, the

term “feeling” is used as the synonym of emotion.

The term affect is more general term is used any phenomena related with

emotion such as moods and disposition or both (Eich et al., 2000, p. 89). Lazarus

Page 26: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

12

defines affect as subjective quality of an emotional experience (1991, p. 57).

Alternatively, Vasta et al. define affect as the outward expression of emotions through

facial expressions, gestures, and innotation etc (2000, p. 448). In addition, the terms

“emotion” and “emotional episode” are used for states that endure a limited amount of

time (see, Figure 1).

Figure 1. Episodes of Emotion . (A spectrum of affective phenomena in terms of the time course, Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p.124).

-Expressions-

-Autonomic

Changes-

__Self-reported

Emotions____

____Moods___

____Emotional disorders___

___Personality traits__

Seconds Minutes Hours Days Weeks Months Years Lifetime

One of the important concepts that is often confused with emotion is mood.

Actually, mood differs from emotion in that the feelings or emotions involved last over

a longer period (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p.12). Moods have a tendency to be diffuse

rather than directed toward individuals, events, or objects in the environment. Yet

moods have dramatic effects on our behavior, they affect the intensity of our reactions

to emotion and provoke stimuli (Canland et al., 1977, p. 108). According to Eich et al.

mood tends to be more subtle, longer lasting, less intense and nonspecific compared to

emotions. In contrast, people may not be aware of their mood (2000, p. 89). For

instance, a feeling of anger lasting for just a few minutes, or even for an hour, is called

Page 27: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

13

an emotion. However, if the person remains angry all day or becomes angry for days,

then it is called mood (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p.12). On the other hand, Oatley &

Jenkins (1996) define moods as an emotional state that endures for hours, days and

sometimes weeks and generally, they do not have any intention. However, emotions

have an intention or have an object. The best way to discriminate mood and emotion is

that, emotion tends to change more easily than mood, which is more resistant to change

(p. 125). Despite these differences, apparently, it is not easy to discriminate mood and

emotion.

If emotional states last longer than moods, then this state is called as emotional

disorders that refer to mood disorders on the basis of currently used classification

system DSM-IV (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, APA, 1994).

One step ahead from emotional disorders is called as personality disorders which last a

lifetime (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 127). It is found that people with personality

disorders have more emotional intensity than others do (Linehan, 1987). Therefore,

people can differ in terms of subjective experience of emotion that refers to what the

emotion feels like to the individual. For example, when we have a blue mood, we feel

sadness for hours or days; a melancholic personality is prone to feeling sad; depression

is the mental disorder in which sadness and agony are central (Ekman, 2003, p. 93).

For instance, in terms of subjective experience, one of the most prominent

psychological disorders is Alexithymia refers to difficulty in distinguishing emotions

(Helmers & Mente, 1999). Literally, “A-lexi-thymia” means “without language for

emotions” (Westen, 1999). In other words, alexithymic individuals have difficulty

telling their emotions; mostly it is believed that they misinterpret their emotional arousal

as symptoms of physical illness (Helmers & Mente, 1999).

1. 1. 4. Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE)

Human emotions are the language of human social life. They provide the outline

patterns that relate people each other. For instance, the smile; the best-established

universal signal of emotion, happiness is the emotion of cooperation; anger is the

emotion of interpersonal conflict (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p.87). According to Oatley

Page 28: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

14

and Jenkins, “emotions are heuristics”. Emotional states organize ready repertoires of

action. In other words, they occur without our need to say to ourselves, “this situation is

dangerous”, instead we simply feel frightened and we take action (1996, p. 258).

Specifically, facial expressions are the readouts of these internal emotional states

(Lazarus, 1991, p. 70).

Actually, emotional facial expression (EFE) refers to the overt behavioral signs

of emotion or it indicates something within that is externalized (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996,

p.124). In detailed, emotional facial expressions can be defined as a means of

communication that are more rapid than language and they facilitate communication in

everyday life in order to interpret the intentions, goals, opinions and attitudes of others

(Erickson & Schulkin, 2003; Batty & Taylor, 2003).

In daily life, generally, we monitor the emotional reactions of others and prefer

reacting and regulating our behaviors based on them. Thus, they constitute very

powerful tools in social coordination (Batty & Taylor, 2003). According to Erickson &

Schulkin (2003) “as human beings, we construct and interpret the world around us into

useful and understandable categories in order to predict and understand events central to

human survival and experience”. Similarly, Ekman postulated that emotional

expressions were crucial to the development and regulation of interpersonal

relationships (1992, p. 177).

Surely, when interpreting facial expressions of others, we consider the

situational and social settings in which expression is produced. Our main tool for

reading expressions is usually a “face”. It is assumed that emotions are often revealed

on the face that can display more than ten thousand expressions (Ekman, 2003, p. 14;

Ekman, 1993; Lazarus, 1991, p. 70). Kowalski & Westen support Ekman that, face is

the primary center of emotion (2005, p.351). The face is not only a means of

communicating emotions, but also is a vehicle to communicate intentions (...) (Erickson

& Schulkin, 2003). Face is a very important source of social information and it appears

as if we are biologically prepared to perceive and respond to faces in a universal manner

(Eastwood & Smilek, 2005). Also, it is known that specific signals in each part of the

face that convey the messages of fear, surprise, sadness, happiness or enjoyment, anger,

Page 29: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

15

disgust, and combinations of these emotions (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p.14). Even

though, some people believe that they can read attitudes, personality, moral character,

and intelligence from face, it is certain that sex, age and ethnicity may be read more

accurately and easily than all above (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p.13). More specifically, a

person is most easily identified by his face than his body. For instance, when we are told

a person that we have not met yet, usually we want to see the photograph of his face.

According to Ekman and Friesen, problems in understanding facial expressions arise

because most of the time people do not watch each other’s faces (1975, p.13). In fact,

watching someone’s face is intimate, thereby, we generally do not want to be intrusive

or rude by starring at people’s faces.

Ekman and Friesen pointed out that some facial expressions are extremely rapid,

brief, and lasts merely a fraction of a second. These are called micro-expressions. It is

rare for a facial expression of emotion to last as long as five or ten seconds, very long

facial expressions are not genuine expressions, these exaggerated form of the

expressions are called as mock expressions (1975, p.14; Ekman, 1993).

Due to test their assumptions, Ekman and Friesen constructed Facial Atlas of the

face, which is aimed to portray photographically each of the universal facial expressions

of emotion. For this, they first constructed a table which all the facial muscles and six

emotions including famous theorists such as Darwin, Duchenne, Huber and Plutchik

and their proposals about the relationship between emotion and muscles. And they filled

the gaps based on their deep experiences. Then, they took of photos of the models and

each of the facial areas (brow/forehead; the eyes/lids, and the root of the nose, and the

lower face including the cheeks, mouth, most of the nose, and chin). In conclusion, the

completed Atlas consists of series of photographs of the three different areas of the face,

each photograph keyed to one of the six emotions (Ekman, 1993).

1. 1. 5. Basic or Discrete Emotions

According to Frijda, emotions can be characterized in terms of action readiness

mode corresponds to what are often called “primary”, “elementary”, “basic” or

“fundamental” emotions. For instance, anger is the urge to attack or urge to regain

Page 30: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

16

freedom of action and control. Fear is the urge to separate oneself from aversive events

(1977, p. 72). On the other hand, Lazarus claimed that primary or basic emotions

express universal biological rules handed down genetically through evolution because

they have proved adaptionally useful (1991, p. 70).

Ekman postulated that each of the emotions is not a single affective state but a

“family” of related states. Each member of an emotion family shares a certain

characteristics, such as commonalities in expression, in physiological activity, in nature

of the antecedent events and appraisal processes. And these shared characteristics are

the products of our evolution. According to him basic emotions arise from an inherited

neural structure, involve a characteristic neuromuscular response pattern, and are

correlated with distinctive subjective qualities for each emotion. He distinguished basic

emotions from one another and from other affective states, such as moods, emotional

traits, etc. on the basis of these criteria (1992, p. 173-175).

Recently, there are six universally accepted basic emotions; fear, surprise,

sadness, happiness, anger, disgust. Each emotion has its own characteristics and

appearance figures. In fact, Ekman (1993) emphasized that, there is no single expression

for each emotion. For instance, there are 60 anger expression variations in intensity

stretching from annoyance to rage. These emotion families distinguish from the family

of other expressions.

Furthermore, there are other emotions such as love, jealousy, hatred, envy,

regret, interest, guilt or despair that are spoken in some societies but not in others. The

elaborations or combinations of these basic emotions are called as social emotions

(Stein & Oatley, 1992, p. 162). These social emotions are not primarily of survival or

biologically based but very critical for social interaction of individuals. For instance,

pride has been proposed to be a combination of joy and anger; love a combination of joy

and acceptance; envy or hatred arise from a comparison of oneself with another person

(Stein & Oatley, 1992, p. 209; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 88). Indeed, as Stein and

Oatley emphasized that it is more common to experience the mixed feelings instead of

single ones in almost all cultures (1992, p. 209).

Page 31: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

17

1. 1. 5. 1. Basic or Discrete Emotions: Definitions

Basic emotions are distinguished as negative and positive. Happiness is a

positive emotion. Fear, anger, disgust, and sadness are negative emotions and most

people do not enjoy them. Surprise is neither positive nor negative (Ekman & Friesen,

1975, p. 99). Ekman (1992b) stated that all negative emotions differ in their appraisal,

antecedent events, behavioral responses, physiology, and other characteristics.

Generally, people strive to attain emotions if pleasant, to avoid them if unpleasant (Stein

& Oatley, 1992, p. 203). People differ in whether they can enjoy, tolerate of surprise,

fear, disgust, happiness, anger, or sadness. No one feels emotions in the same way.

People also differ tremendously in experiencing the emotional intensity. For instance,

the extent to which people experience happiness is relatively stable across age and

gender but differs substantially across cultures. More specifically, cultural traditions and

upbringing within a culture play a role in shaping one’s attitude about feeling or

displaying emotions.

1. 1. 5. 1. a. Happiness

Happiness is the emotion that most people want to experience. People like being

happy. It makes feel good. People choose conditions in which happiness are

experienced and almost organize their lives based on feeling good. Oatley and Jenkins

defined happiness as the emotion or mood of achieving subgoals and of being engaged

in that one is doing (1996, p. 259). It is observed that when people are happy, they are

more cooperative and altruistic (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 87).

In English, happiness is used almost synonymous with the pleasure and

excitement. Pleasure is defined as a product of positive physical sensations that is

opposite of the physical sensation of pain. Usually people feel happy when they

experience pleasurable sensations, unless they get punishment for these feelings or feel

guilty about them. Excitement is defined as the opposite of boredom. People become

easily excited when something arouses their interests. However, in boredom nothing is

new and attracts attention you. Certainly, people can feel happy without excitement or it

Page 32: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

18

is also possible to be excited without happiness. Instead of happiness, excitement can

blend with fear or anger (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 100). Briefly, excitement and

pleasure are different experiences, which often involve happiness. Therefore, it seems

that there are four routes of happiness; pleasure-happiness, excitement-happiness, relief-

happiness or happiness involving self-concept. When pain stops, people get happy.

When people do not feel negative emotions such as fear, anger, or disgust, commonly

they feel happy. This kind of happiness is the relief-happiness. Fourth kind of happiness

is about self-concept. Something, which enhances your view of yourself happens, this

situation affirms or further elaborates a favorable self-concept. Praise, friendship, the

esteem of others is rewarding and makes you feel happy. This kind of happiness

originally is developed from experiences in which people get approval. It is a more

contented, smiling happiness, rather than laughing.

Happiness varies not only in type, but also in intensity. It can vary from a smile

to a broad grin, in the extreme form, laughter with tears. Surely, the presence of laughter

does not always indicate the intensity of the happiness. People can extremely feel happy

and may not laugh. Smiles that are a part of the happiness facial expression often occur

when a person is not happy. People can smile in order to mask their emotions.

Happiness is exhibited in the lower face and lower eyelids. The intensity of a

happy expression is primarily determined by the lip position. The mouth may or may

not be parted, with teeth exposed or not. Additionally, a happy person shows wrinkle

lines running from the nose out and down to the area beyond the corners of the mouth.

These naso-labial folds occur. In addition, the cheek becomes raised when there is a

pronounced smile or grin. The more extreme the smile or grin, the more pronounced

will be the naso-labial folds, the raising of the cheek, the crow's feet, and the lines under

the eyes (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 110).

Happiness often blends with surprise, which is the shortest emotion. This blend

lasts only for a moment. Happiness also blends with contempt, producing a smug,

scornful, or superior expression. Additionally, it can blend with anger. Most commonly,

a smile or slight grin is used to mask anger, in which case the person looks happy, not

angry. Interestingly, a person can feel both happy and angry at the same time, enjoying

Page 33: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

19

his/her anger. Lastly, happiness blends with fear. Indeed, the expression is not a blend,

but either a comment or a mask. It can be felt at the same time for instance in a roller

coaster. Happy- sad emotions can also blend. This kind of expressions generally occurs

in bittersweet experiences or when the happy expression is being used as a mask-

laughing on the outside, crying inside (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 112).

1. 1. 5. 1. b. Sadness

There are many words to describe sad feelings: distraught, disappointed,

dejected, blue, depressed, despairing, grieved, helpless, miserable, and sorrowful.

According to Oatley and Jenkins, sadness is the opposite of happiness. It can be

described simply as the emotion of losing a goal or social role (1996, p. 259).

Specifically, anything can make you sad, but the most often you are sad about losses.

Many types of loss can trigger sadness. For instance; loss through death or rejection by

a loved one or loss of an opportunity or reward through your own effort, or another's

disregard. As compared with fear that looks toward future, sadness seems to look

toward the past (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 260; Ekman, 2003, p. 83; Ekman & Friesen,

1975, p. 114).

Ekman distinguished sadness from agony. According to him, in sadness there is

more resignation and hopelessness whereas in agony there is a protest. Therefore, agony

attempts to deal actively with the source of the loss, it seems to include rebellion.

However, sadness in which suffering is muted is a passive feeling. People silently

endure their distress. This suffer is not like physical pain; it is the suffering of loss,

disappointment, or hopelessness. Thus, sadness is rarely a brief feeling. It is one of the

long-lasting emotions. People are usually for at least minutes and more typically for

hours or even days (2003, p. 84).

Even in such intense grief, there are moments when other emotions may be felt.

For instance, we feel angry toward the person who is responsible for a loss, while we

feel sadness and agony about the loss itself (Ekman, 2003, p. 85; Ekman & Friesen,

1975, p. 114). Or grieving person may feel fear in terms of how s/he will survive

without her/his loss. Moreover, when recalling some funny moments with the deceased,

Page 34: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

20

amusement can be felt.

There is a distinctive appearance in each of the three facial areas during sadness.

The inner corners of the eyebrows are drawn up and the inner corner of the upper eyelid

is raised, and the corners of the lips are down.

Sadness blends with fear and anger mostly. Blends of sadness with disgust and

blends of sadness with happiness are also common types.

1. 1. 5. 1. c. Fear

Fear is the emotion of anticipated danger (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p.260). In

other words, the antecedent event for fear is physical or psychological harm (Ekman,

1992, p. 184). Survival depends on learning to avoid or escape from situations that

cause severe pain and the physical injury (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 47). Namely, fear

renders a mode of readiness to cope with danger. Thus, it promotes vigilance for the

feared event that can be an imagined or real. Even though fear is accepted as unpleasant,

it has function to protect the body from further injury (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p.260).

According to Darwin, fear is the most depressing of all the emotions, it lowers the

temperature, induces utter, and might be associated with the most violent and prolonged

attempts to escape from the danger. Nevertheless, extreme fear often acts as a powerful

stimulant (1934, p.32).

Fear differs from surprise in three ways. Firstly, fear is a horrible experience,

surprise is not and fear is unpleasant, moreover can be traumatic, but surprise can be

pleasant or unpleasant. Secondly, you can be afraid of something familiar that you know

very well is going to happen. For example, although it is not surprised for you to go to

the dentist, you can still be frightened. Thirdly, in terms of duration, surprise has always

a short duration, but fear can also occur gradually (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 48-49).

Even danger disappears; you may still feel fear depending upon your appraisal of the

feared situation.

Just like surprise, fear may be followed by any of the other emotions or by no

emotion at all. You may become angry or disgusted or sad or even happy if threat has

been avoided (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 50). Nevertheless, the most common blend

Page 35: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

21

with fear is surprise. Because usually fearful events are unexpected and it is common to

be both surprised and frightened at the same moment or nearly at the same moment.

The appearance of full-face fear is characterized as; the eyebrows are raised and

drawn together, the eyes are open, and the lower lid is tensed, and the lips are stretched

back unlike in surprise expression (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 50). Actually, there are

two-way fear expressions; each of these expressions has a slightly different meaning.

One is apprehensive fear that refers to an impending a harmful event. In this expression,

fear is shown only in brow and eyes, not in lower face (mouth), another is frozen or

horrified fear. Unlike apprehensive fear, the expression of horrible fear does not include

a brow component. Mouth is more stretched (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 55-60).

According to Frijda who considered fear as a protective response, extreme fear is

expressionless (1986, p. 18).

On the other hand, Frijda describes the fear expression as “forceful eye closure,

frowning by drawning the eyebrows together, bending the head, hunching the shoulders,

bending the trunk, and knees” (1986, p. 16).

1. 1. 5. 1. d. Anger

Ekman and Friesen claimed that anger is very likely the most dangerous

emotion. When people are angry, they hurt others purposefully (1975, p. 76). However,

according to Lazarus, although anger is commonly classified as negative emotion,

people often report feeling good about their anger. Nevertheless, he added that when

anger is acted out, it can have harmful social or physiological consequences, especially

when it is not managed (1991, p. 5).

Oatley & Jenkins defined anger as asserting ourselves in dominance. In other

words, according to them, it is the emotion of frustration with anything we are trying to

do, or with anyone who shows lack of consideration or who obstructs us (1996, p. 260).

They confirmed the frustration-aggression hypothesis of Berkowitz (1989) including

that aggressive behavior arises from frustrated needs or desires. The frustrating obstacle

may not be a person, you can be angry at an object or natural event that frustrates you,

even though you may feel a bit embarrassed or less justified in your anger (Ekman &

Page 36: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

22

Friesen, 1975, p. 79). Surely, your anger will be more likely and more intense if you

believe that the agent of the interference acted arbitrarily. As a result of this feeling, you

may express your anger directly, indirectly, symbolically or displacing it onto a safer or

more convenient target, such as a scapegoat.

Beside frustration, a second major provocation to anger is a physical threat.

However, you may feel fear if the threatening injury is much more powerful than you

are, if this injury is insignificant or unable to hurt then you feel more contempt.

Third major source of anger is someone’s action or statement that causes you to

feel psychologically rather than physically hurt, such as a rejection, an insult.

Doubtlessly, the more you care the person that rejects you, the more you get angry. An

insult from someone you have little regard for, a rejection by someone, whom you never

consider as a friend or lover, may make you feel contempt or surprise rather than anger.

On the other hand, if hurt comes from the person you care greatly about, you may feel

sadness instead of anger or both. This type of source, at the same time may cause you to

rationalize this anger feeling and make you feel guilty.

Fourth major source of anger is someone’s action or statement toward you or

someone that violate your moral values. If someone treats another person in a manner

you consider immoral you may become angry even though you are not directly affected.

For instance, sexual or physical abuse or anything that is opposite to your values,

clearly, if you have a value about marriage like “until death us a part” when a husband

leaves his wife, this situation may lead to anger you.

Absolutely, there are other sources of anger, such as a person’s failure to meet

the expectations, and another person’s anger are two of the causes of anger.

Anger varies in intensity from slight irritation or annoyance to rage or fury.

People differ not only in terms of what makes them angry or in terms of what they do

when they are angry but also in terms of how long it takes them to become angry

(Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 81).

Also, boys and girls differ in showing anger, girls are taught to control or not to

show anger toward anyone but boys are encouraged to express their anger (Ekman &

Friesen, 1975, p. 76).

Page 37: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

23

Similar to other emotions, anger also blends with other emotions; a person may

also feel anger-sadness, anger-fear, and anger-disgust at the same time. Some people

even like being angry. They enjoy argument. Moreover, intimacy can be established or

reestablished by an angry exchange between two people.

Human anger has a large variety of manifestations especially is manifested in

each of the three areas; the eyebrows are lowered, and drawn together, the eyelids are

tensed, and the eye appears to stare in a hard fashion. The lips are either tightly pressed

together or opened in a square. Actually, the nostrils may be widened, but this is not

essential to the anger facial expression and also may occur in sadness (Ekman &

Friesen, 1975, p. 76; Frijda, 1986, p. 19).

Generally, the blended expressions are accomplished by two blended emotions

registering in different areas of the face. However, with anger unless the expression is

registered in all three areas of the face, the message becomes ambiguous. Anger

message is overwhelmed by the other emotion in blend. In other words, anger is easily

masked except two expressions in which anger message remains salient. One is anger-

disgust blend expression. The other is the blend need not require different facial areas

show the different emotions. There are other blends in which the blending is

accomplished by separate facial areas rather than mixing within facial areas, contempt-

anger blend, anger-surprise blend, happiness-anger blend, and sadness-anger blend

(Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 92-94). One of the most common mixtures is sadness and

anger, which may be caused by a loss that also frustrates some plan (Stein & Oatley,

1992, p. 209).

1. 1. 5. 1. e. Surprise

Ekman and Friesen indicated that surprise is the most brief emotion (1975, p.

34). It is sudden. If you have a time to think about that event, you will not feel surprised.

Also, it does not take long time unless the surprising event unfolds new surprising

elements. Correspondingly, Frijda defined surprise as a response to sudden stimuli that

consists of widening of the eyes, brief suspension of breathing, and general loss of

muscle tone. The loss of muscle tone causes the mouth to fall open, and may make the

Page 38: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

24

subject stagger or force him to sit down (1986, p. 18). The more unexpected event, the

greater the surprise you feel (Teigen & Keren, 2002). Specifically, Ekman and Friesen

postulated that surprise is triggered by both unexpected and misexpected events.

Unexpected events can be defined as an unusual event, which is unanticipated. At that

moment, the surprised person does not expect anything in particular to happen. In

misexpected surprise, there is an aroused specific anticipation for something different to

happen at that moment. In this situation the event needs to be usual but unexpected for

surprising (1975, p. 34).

Almost anything can be surprising; a sight, smell, taste, touch and surely, the

greater the unexpected thing, the more surprised you will be. In other words, surprise

has different intensity levels from mild to extreme, depending upon the event itself.

Because of the brevity of the experience of surprise, another emotion quickly follows it,

the face generally shows a blend of surprise, and subsequent emotion depending upon

how you feel about what surprised you. Therefore, surprise may turn to pleasure or

happiness, or disgust, or aggression, or fear depending on the event. Ekman (1992)

states that fear is the most common subsequent emotion to surprise, because unexpected

events are usually dangerous, and mostly people associate surprise with fear. While

some people seek novelty, love being surprised, and organize their lives, moreover

some people cannot stand being surprised (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 36).

The appearance of full-face surprise is characterized as; the curved and high

eyebrows, stretched skin below the brow, opened eyelids, and opened mouth with

parted lips and teeth, dropped jaw. The mouth may be just slightly open, moderately

open or widely open depending upon the intensity level of surprise (Ekman & Friesen,

1975, p. 39-40).

There are four types of surprise; first is questioning or uncertain surprise

includes only the eyes and brow movements of the full-face surprise expression. Second

is astonished or amazed surprise that is characterized only the eyes and mouth

movements. Third is dazed surprise or less interested surprise. Dazed surprise occurs

when only the brow and mouth movements appear. The last type is the combination of

these three facial areas. Its message is clear that is surprise (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p.

Page 39: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

25

43).

1. 1. 5. 1. f. Disgust

Disgust is a feeling of aversion. Specifically, disgust is the emotion of revulsion

and avoidance of anything that makes one sick (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 66). People

can feel disgust from any taste, a smell, a sight, a touch or a sound or even an idea.

According to Oatley and Jenkins, disgust may be an innate rejection of substances

because they are decayed, infectious, or toxic (1996, p.262). However, Ekman and

Friesen argued that what is repulsive to one culture might be attractive to another

culture. Because, culture usually expresses itself in eating habits as well. For instance, a

popular food can be an aversive food in other cultures.

Disgust usually involves getting-rid-of and getting-away-from responses.

Removing the object or oneself from that object is a goal. Generally, nausea and

vomiting accompany with disgust. Nausea and vomiting occur without disgust and

likewise disgust occurs without nausea or vomiting (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 66).

Disgust can blend with surprise, fear, happiness and also with sadness but mostly

disgust blends with anger. The appearance of full-face disgust is characterized as; the

upper lid is raised, the lower lip is raised and pushed up to the upper lip, or is lowered

and the nose is wrinkled, the cheeks are raised, lines show below the lower lid is pushed

up but not tense, the brow is lowered, lowering the upper lid (Ekman & Friesen, 1975,

p. 76).

1. 1. 6. Universality of Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE)

Are facial expressions of emotion the same for people everywhere? Are they all

cultural products? Are facial expressions universal or specific to each culture?

Alternatively, are facial expressions an accurate reflection of emotional experience?

These questions have played a central role in many studies.

In fact, the innateness of an expression was one of the widespread questions that

Darwin had curiosity. In 1872, Darwin sent questionnaires to 36 observers inquiring the

way of expressions of emotions such as astonishment by eyes and mouth being opened

Page 40: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

26

widely etc. As a result of this survey, he found impressive agreement among observers

(Frijda, 1986, p. 67). Definitely, even though this survey had many methodological

problems, it gave considerable strength to the global conclusion, which has been

confirmed by recent studies.

In terms of universality of expressions of emotions, especially Izard in 1969 and

1971, Ekman and his colleagues in 1972 and 1982 made major studies concerning

Western, Japan and African cultures.

As a result of these major studies, Paul Ekman in 1972 proposed a Two-factor

theories of emotion (one factor is universal/biological, other is cultural) or

Neurocultural theory that refers to an innate neural patterning of expressions is

accompanied by culturally variable display rules that regulate when each expression can

be made. Ekman and his colleague Wallece V. Friesen (1975) postulated that six basic

emotional categories are universally recognized. Namely, these are; happiness, sadness,

surprise, anger, fear, and disgust (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 67; Abboud, Davoine, &

Dang, 2004). However, Izard (1977) adds interest, shame, guilt, and contempt to this list

of universal expressions (cited in Frijda, 1986, p. 68). Yet, there has not been

confirming studies of Izard’s list of emotions.

Briefly, in a study of Ekman and his colleagues, they displayed to the

participants some photographs, which include the faces of North American actors that

express fear, anger, happiness, surprise, disgust, sadness. Different cultural groups,

ranging from Swedes and Kenyans to the members of a preliterate tribe in New Guinea

(Ifaluk tribe) with minimal Western contact, participated to this study. Consequently,

results revealed that all groups recognized six emotions correctly. Then these emotions

have been accepted as universal (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 23). Shame and excitement

or interest may be considered as universal but they have not been firmly established yet

(Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 22; Westen, 1999, p. 489). On the other hand, Lewis &

Haviland-Jones (2000, p. 239) mentioned that the literature recently focuses on seven

emotions: happiness, fear, sadness, disgust, anger, surprise, and nowadays contempt.

Contempt is defined as the emotion of interpersonal rejection especially toward

members of out-groups (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 262). With regard to Lewis &

Page 41: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

27

Haviland-Jones, Ekman stated that contempt facial expression which is expressed as a

variation on the closed-lips disgust mouth were not included in preliterate culture

studies (1992, p. 176). Therefore, contempt can not be assumed as one of the basic

emotions. Because, the universal feature of basic emotions is based on the appearance

of the face for each of the primary emotions. To conclude, there are still debates on the

universality of contempt emotion.

Furthermore, it was found that all facial expressions could vary from one culture

to another. People in different cultures may change in what they have been taught about

managing or controlling their facial expressions of emotions or display rules (Ekman &

Friesen, 1975, p.24; Malatesta and Haviland, 1982). People learn to control the way

they express many emotions by using different patterns of emotional expression that are

considered appropriate within their cultures. For instance, in another study of Ekman

and Friesen (1975), findings revealed that, although Japanese and Americans had

virtually identical facial expressions, in terms of display rules Japanese masked their

facial expressions of unpleasant emotions more than Americans did (p. 24).

Corresponding to this study, another study proved that the same facial expressions were

judged as showing the same emotions in all the countries like US, Japan, Chile,

Argentina, and Brazil regardless of language or culture (Ekman & Friesen, 1975, p. 24).

Shortly, people in two different cultures may feel sadness at the death of a loved

one, however, one culture may prescribe that they mask their facial expression with a

mildly happy appearance. More specifically, if a language does not have any words for

describing an emotion, it does not mean that emotion does not occur in this culture. For

example, Ekman (1993) argued that although the Tahitians have no word for describing

sadness, sad expressions were observed in Tahitian people who had experienced a loss.

In sum, emotions and emotional expressions are strongly affected by cultural

ideas. The unwritten codes or display rules are internalized as a function of an

individual’s culture, gender, and family background. Nevertheless, the questions of how

far emotions are universal, how far they are socially constructed by cultures are still

very difficult to answer.

Page 42: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

28

1. 1. 7. The Neuropsychology of Emotion

Doubtlessly, physiological dimension of emotion is very critical, although there

has not been generally accepted theory to date about emotional processing and

associated neural systems (Herba & Phillips, 2004). On the other hand, neuroscience

studies have demonstrated that emotion like cognition is distributed throughout the

nervous system and not located in any particular region. Also, selective impairments in

recognizing facial emotions without a deficit in facial identity, and conversely have

been reported indicating that different aspects of faces are processed in separate neural

subsystems. Three areas of the brain in forebrain however, are particularly important:

the hypothalamus, limbic system, and the cortex (Batty & Taylor, 2003; Kowalski &

Westen, 2005, p. 357; Frijda, 1986, p. 379; Candland et al., 1977, p. 152).

1. 1. 7. 1. The Hypothalamus

The hypothalamus that is situated in front of the midbrain and adjacent to the

pituitary gland helps regulate behaviors ranging from eating and sleeping to sexual

activity and emotional experience. In terms of emotion, this tiny structure becomes a

central link in a central circuit that converts emotional signals generated at higher levels

of the brain into autonomic and endocrine responses. Especially in anger or rage attacks,

hypothalamus is stimulated particularly in nonhumans (Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p.

79). Furthermore, because of its characteristics of producing hormones that affect other

endocrine glands, the hypothalamus is often thought as a part of the endocrine system.

As it is known that, the hormonal state of the organism contributes to the determination

of the individual’s reactivity to emotional stimuli or to the intensity of the response

(Candland et al., 1977, p.106). Even though hormones may not induce emotions, they

certainly seem to affect them.

1. 1. 7. 2. The Limbic System

It is a set of interrelated structures with diverse functions involving emotion,

motivation, learning, and memory. The limbic system includes the septal area, the

amygdala, and the hippocampus that is important especially for storing new

Page 43: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

29

information. The role of septal area is newly understood and it appears to be involved in

some forms of emotionally significant learning such as avoiding aversive experiences

(Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 81). Stimulation of the septal area elicits pleasure

(Candland et al., 1977, p. 156). The amygdala, which is a complex of nuclei, resides in

the anterior temporal lobe involved in many emotional processes, especially learning

and remembering emotionally significant events. One of its primary roles is to attach

emotional significance to events (Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 81; Frijda, 1986, p.

381).

1. 1. 7. 3. The Cortex (Neocortex)

The cortex that reaches its largest development in human beings plays several

roles with respect to emotion. It allows people to consider whether a stimulus is safe or

harmful. People with damage to the regions of the frontal cortex that receive input from

the amygdala have difficulty making choices guided by their emotions. One of its roles

is interpreting the meaning of peripheral responses, for instance, a person’s shaky knees

and dry throat while speaking in front of a group gives a clue that she is anxious

(Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 81; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 144). Most of the

researches have shown that the right and the left hemispheres of the cortex appear to be

specialized, with the right hemisphere is dominant in processing emotional cues from

others and producing facial displays of emotion (Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p. 144). In

other words, the right side of the cortex has been found to be more closely associated

with the processing of emotional events.

Furthermore, in terms of emotional facial expression, recent neuroscience

studies have demonstrated that particular brain regions are responsible in processing of

faces such as fusiform gyrus and superior temporal sulcus. Especially cortical

(prefrontal, frontal, and orbito-frontal cortices, occipito-temporal junction, cingulate

cortex, and secondary somatosensory cortex) and subcortical structures such as

amygdala, basal ganglia, and insula are important structures (Damasio et al., 2000).

Among these structures, the amygdala has often been linked with the mediation of

emotional behavior (Batty & Taylor, 2003; Amaral, 2002). Particularly processing of

Page 44: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

30

fearful faces and sad faces are associated with amygdala, happy faces are linked with

cingulate sulcus, and orbital frontal regions are activated by angry faces. Disgust seems

to activate basal ganglia and insula (Batty & Taylor, 2003). Kowalski and Westen

mentioned that amygdala is associated with the expression of rage, fear and calculation

of the emotional significance of a stimulus (2005, p. 81). In other words, it is involved

in detecting others’ emotions from their facial expressions and vocal tone (Kowalski &

Westen, 2005, p. 359). It was found that impairment in the recognition of facial

expressions of fear is commonly found in patients with damage to amygdala (Adolphs

et al., 1994).

To sum up, emotional processes are distributed throughout the nervous system.

The emotional reaction to a stimulus appears to occur through two distinct neural

pathways: a quick response based on circuit from the thalamus to the amygdala, and a

slower response based on a more through cognitive appraisal, based on thalamus-to-

cortex-to-amygdala circuit. In both cases, the amygdala passes information to the

hypothalamus that is involved in regulating autonomic responses. The cortex plays

multiple roles such as interpreting the meaning of events and translating emotional

reactions into socially desirable behaviors (Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 360; Oatley &

Jenkins, 1996, p. 158).

1. 1. 8. The Development of Emotions

Facial expressions are very important for children to regulate their social

behaviors as well as for the quality of parent-child relationships. The abilities to

accurately interpret and express emotions through facial expressions develop during

infancy and childhood. As children mature, they learn to modify their emotional

expression in socially appropriate ways (Erickson & Schulkin, 2003; Dunn & Hughes,

1998). Around the age of 10 years, the ability to categorize facial expressions of basic

emotions reaches an almost adult level (Gosselin & Larocque, 2000).

Emotions are assumed as the first language that parents and infants

communicate with before the infant acquires speech. In terms of communication, faces

are not only interesting stimuli to view; they are also sources of social information.

Page 45: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

31

Infants react to their parents’ facial expressions and tones of voice. In addition, this

communicative and social role of facial expression is the result of extended use of basic

survival responses (Erickson & Schulkin, 2003).

The sensitivity of babies to emotional expressions in faces grows slowly over the

first two years of life. Even three month-old babies may look longer at faces as the

intensity of the smile increases (Vasta et al., 2000, p.215; Santrock, 1997, p. 199).

Infants use eye gaze direction, along with facial expression, to gather information about

the environment by the end of the first year of life (Erickson & Schulkin, 2003).

By far the most important form of communication for the newborn is “crying”.

Crying is a part of the infant’s larger affective communication system. Darwin believed

that crying in newborns evolved as a means of providing the mother with information

about the baby’s state or condition (1934, p. 118). Caregivers should understand

whether baby is saying for example “I am hungry”, “I am wet” or “I am frightened” on

the basis of crying style that can be interpreted based in part on experience.

Newborns do not only use crying as a communication tool but also can

communicate “likes” using behaviors such as smiling, vocalizing, and gazing at an

object that they find interesting (Vasta et al., 2000, p.448; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996,

p.163). Many theorists believe that initially a close correspondence exists between what

babies feel and what they express (Vasta et al., 2000, p.449). Researchers have found

that even newborn babies possess all the facial muscle movements necessary to produce

virtually any adult emotional expression (Vasta et al., 2000, p.449; Oatley & Jenkins,

1996, p.163). For instance, facial responses to sweet that elicit positive facial responses

and bitter tastes that elicit aversive responses are expressed by infants and adults of

primate species (Erickson & Schulkin, 2003).

1. 1. 8. 1. The Development of the Expression of Emotions

Infants express some emotions earlier than other emotions. At birth, babies can

display “distress” by crying and “interest” by staring attentively. The facial expression

of “disgust” has been observed in newborns in response to sour tastes or odors

(Santrock, 1997, p. 200; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996, p.163). By 10 to 12 weeks of age,

Page 46: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

32

smiling (reflecting pleasure) appears in response to the human voice or moving face.

Sadness and anger are first evidences in facial expressions at 3 or 4 months. Fearful

expressions do not appear until about 7 months. Guilt, shame, and embarrassment,

which are some of the complex affective responses, are not apparent until near the end

of the baby’s first year (Santrock, 1997, p. 200; Vatsa et al., 2000, p. 449; Oatley &

Jenkins, 1996, p.164). By four years of age, children can infer desires of others through

direction of eye gaze (Erickson & Schulkin, 2003).

In terms of the recognition of facial expressions, previous studies have

consistently shown that expression of happiness and sadness are more recognizable than

fear and disgust (Gosselin & Larocque, 2000). Gosselin & Larocque (2000) suggested

that expressions of anger and surprise were easier to identify than disgust but harder

than happiness and sadness. Nelson and Dolgin (1985) found that even 7 months old

infants were able to categorize between happiness-fear and happiness-surprise by

looking at the faces of different people.

In sum, even though infants can able to display expression of emotions, their

accurate recognition of expressions improves sooner. As they mature they able to

discriminate better. Thus, gradually they learn to express their emotions in a socially

appropriate way and identify facial expressions, which contribute to the appraisal of

other people’s emotions in order to organize social interactions.

1. 1. 9. Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE) and Cognition

1. 1. 9. 1. Affective/Mood Disorders, Anxiety Disorders and Recognition of

Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE): The Literature Review

It is apparent that emotional or affective states can influence cognitive processes

such as memory, learning, and judgments. Most of the studies have demonstrated that

mood or emotional states influence the way people make judgments, inferences, and

predictions.

Early theories such as Beck’s (1976) schemata theory and Bower’s (1981)

network theory proposed that in both anxiety and depression, cognitive biases operate

through information processing. The main difference between these two is about the

Page 47: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

33

content of bias. (cited in Persad & Polivy, 1993). Specifically, anxious individuals

selectively perceive threatening information, whereas depressed individuals have a bias

for information related to sadness, loss and failure (Mogg, Millar, & Bradley, 2000;

Mogg & Bradley, 2006; Rohner, 2002). Several authors suggested that biases in

selective attention play a causal role in the onset and maintenance of anxiety and mood

disorders. In addition, during social interactions, misinterpreting emotional expressions

may interrupt the flow of conversation, may cause discomfort, and confusion both in the

speaker and the listener. Hence, these biases of facial expressions constitute one of our

purposes in this study.

From the research carried out so far, there appears to be substantial support that

individuals with affective disorders show some mild impairment, because it is accepted

that cognitive processes in depression is characterized by the negative schemata that

distort one’s world, the self and the future (Cavanagh et al., 2005). The negative

perceptual bias may be consistent with this negative cognitive triad. In other words,

depressed individuals tend to underestimate the probability of their own success and

overestimate the probability of bad events occurring in the future. Their negative

perceptions toward the world, self and future also affect their social interactions. They

are more likely to infer negative conclusions or appraisals from their spouses or friends’

behaviors (Persad & Polivy, 1993). Hence, these patients are usually expected to judge

facial stimuli more negative than do non-depressed individuals.

In order to examine the attributional style of depressed patients, Leppanen et al.

(2004) conducted a study in which recognition of different facial expressions were

compared in patients with moderate to severe depression. In this study, also reaction

times were measured. Totally, 18 depressed and 18 matched healthy controls were

presented neutral, happy, and sad expressions. As a result of this study, it was found that

depressed individuals and controls were equally accurate at recognizing happy and sad

faces. However, depressed individuals attributed neutral faces to sad faces and they

recognized neutral expressions both more slowly and less accurately than healthy

participants did. This result confirms that depressed individuals have some impairment

on recognition of neutral expressions due to their attentional bias towards negativeness.

Page 48: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

34

The findings of reaction times will be mentioned in the next part in this study.

Similarly to the findings above, according to Hale (1998) depressed individuals

tended to perceive negative emotional states in others improperly. Specifically, these

individuals were more likely to attribute sadness to neutral faces, and neutral faces to

happy faces.

On the other hand, Mendlewicz and her colleagues (2005) designed a study in

which they aimed to investigate the recognition of facial expressions among female

adolescent inpatients with major depression and female adolescents with eating

disorders. They also compared these two inpatients groups with the healthy control

group. As a result of this design, they did not find any significant differences between

eating disorder group and controls in their facial expression recognition. As expected,

depressed patients demonstrated less accuracy rates in decoding angry expression than

inpatients with eating disorders and control group. This study also supports the

existence of decoding impairment in depression.

Furthermore, Surguladze et al. (2005) examined the neural basis of attentional

biases toward happy and sad facial expressions in major depressed and healthy

individuals. They found that preferential increases in neural response to sad but not

happy facial expressions were observed in depressed individuals. The results indicated

the existence of association of attentional biases and neural basis for the negative

cognitions and social dysfunction in depressed individuals.

There are also other studies, which are designed to compare major depression

with other disorders in terms of facial expression impairments. For instance, Weniger et

al. (2004) examined facial expression recognition abilities in subjects with various

schizophrenia subtypes and subjects with major depression. As predicted, disorganized

and paranoid schizophrenic subjects showed strong impairments, whereas depressive

subjects demonstared only minor impairments. Surely, schizophrenia is one of the

disorders, which has deleterious effect on the mind. The authors found that both

schizophrenia groups and depressed individuals identified happy expressions at higher

rates than negative facial expressions, interestingly depressed individuals rated

expressions less aroused than other groups.

Page 49: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

35

In sum, because of the exhibition of poor social communication skills of

depressed individuals, our one of the hypotheses was that these interaction problems

might be associated with deficits to recognize facial expressions.

1. 1. 9. 2. Anxiety Disorders, Affective/Mood Disorders and Reaction Times

towards Emotional Facial Expressions (EFE): The Literature Review

There has been extensive research about anxiety, which is supposed to interfere

with cognitive and emotional processing. It is observed that anxious individuals are

more sensitive to threatening information and they are more likely to exhibit attentional

bias or hypervigilance and evaluative bias to these threatening-content stimuli. It is

assumed that attentional vigilance for threat may be an important figure in both causing

and maintaining anxiety (Bradley et al., 2000; Mogg et al., 2000; Mogg & Bradley,

2006). In each anxiety disorders, vigilance appears according to the domain concern of

the individuals. Specifically, in panic disorders physical threats, in social anxiety

socially threatening situations, in generalized anxiety disorders idiosyncratic worries

constitute the subjects of biases (Rossignol et al., 2005; Mansell et al., 2002).

Several studies about the existence of attentional biases, generally used Stroop

task in which participants are asked to name the colors of the words as faster as possible

without ignoring the content. The findings of these studies usually manifested that

participants who had anxiety, expressed greater interference in color naming tasks,

which were associated with their fears. Nevertheless, when Stroop task method was

found as questionable, recently probe detection task has been developed. Moreover,

nowadays in order to obtain ecological validity, facial expressions have begun to use.

(Mogg & Bradley, 2006; Bar-Haim et al., 2005; D’Argembeau et al., 2003).

With respect to facial expressions, there have been discrepant findings of the

studies so far. For instance, Bradley et al. (2000) proposed that anxious individuals were

faster in responding to probes that replace threat rather than neutral stimuli. Accordant

with the study of Bradley et al. (2000), Mogg and Bradley (2006) found consistent

findings with their primary hypothesis, which was initial and rapid attentional bias for

fear-related stimuli in spider-fearful individuals. In other words, high fear is associated

Page 50: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

36

with an enhanced initial attentional bias for fear-relevant stimuli. However, in a study of

Mogg et al. (2000), findings revealed that there were no significant differences between

patients with generalized anxiety disorders and normal control individuals in terms of

reaction times toward threat, sad and happy faces. Also, they did not find any significant

results in individuals with depressive disorder in terms of showing an attentional bias or

processing bias for sad faces. However, Leppanen et al. (2004) found that depressed

individuals were slower to react toward neutral faces than sad faces, whereas control

individuals recognized neutral faces faster than sad ones. They found that healthy

individuals recognized happy expressions faster and more accurately than sad

expressions.

Furthermore, Surcinelli et al. (2006) investigated the relationship between trait

anxiety and recognition of emotional facial expressions. They found that individuals

with high-trait anxiety recognized only fear faces significantly better than individuals

with low-trait anxiety. And they were more likely to classify ambiguous faces as

expressing fear. The two groups did not differ in terms of the recognition of anger,

sadness, happiness, surprise, disgust, and neutral faces. As it is known, a primary

function of fear is considered a facilitation of the detection of danger in the

environment. Participants with high anxiety may perceive many daily situations as

threatening and they organize their lives based this fear expectation.

On the other hand, Philippot and Douilliez (2005) found that a group of anxiety

disorders such as generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, panic disorder with

agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder did not cause any attentional or evaluative

biases in the process of threatening facial expressions. It was assumed that especially

socially anxious individuals would evaluate facial expressions more negatively.

However, in three studies, they presented a set of photographs of EFE to patients with

generalized anxiety disorder, patients with social phobia, patients suffer from panic

disorder with agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder and surely to a control group.

Their decoding accuracy was measured based on participants’ ability to infer the

emotion that was presented on computer screen. As a conclusion, no significant

differences could be found between anxiety disorders and control group in terms of EFE

Page 51: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

37

decoding accuracy. Indeed, the authors believed that these unexpected results were due

to several methodological limitations.

In the study of D’Argembeau et al. (2003) which was designed to examine

identity and expression memory in social anxiety, results showed that individuals with

high and low socially anxiety did not differ on total correct responses and this

expression memory was better for happy than angry expressions in both groups.

However, they added that high anxious participants remembered happy faces less than

low anxious participants did. This decreased encoding of positive information in

memory may take a role in maintenance of social anxiety.

On the other hand, Cooper et al. (2006) who contribute new dimensions to the

issue of attentional bias in anxiety investigated the existence of an attentional bias to

angry faces in non-anxious populations by using the dot-probe task in 100 ms and 500

ms conditions. They expected the inability to find any evidence for attentional cues.

They used thirty pictures from Ekman and Friesen (1976) set of emotional expressions.

This set includes neutral, happy, and angry expressions. Participants were asked to press

the keyboard button when they saw a dot. There were eight conditions: two emotional

expressions (angry or happy), two probe locations, and two gaze directions. The

analysis was based on the reaction times for correct responses. Results revealed that at

100 ms there was an attentional bias towards the location of the relatively threatening

stimulus; the angry face in angry/neutral pairs, the neutral face in neutral face in

neutral/happy pairs. In sum, Cooper et al. (2006) examined whether previous attempts

to observe an attentional bias towards angry expressions in the general populations

using the dot-probe task would fail when they used 500 ms dot-probe task. However, in

100 ms condition, the findings were interpreted as vigilance to the angry faces for

angry/neutral pair, as avoidance of the happy faces for neutral/happy pair. The authors

elucidated these results as an attentional bias towards threat, which is a universal feature

of human cognitive processing, and this feature is not only specific to anxiety. Instead,

evolutionarily, humans detect threat from their environment in order to fight or flight.

Angry facial expression is one of the threat signals for survival.

Page 52: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

38

Furthermore, recent studies have focused on the differing brain regions for

different emotions, however little attention has been paid to the timing of this emotional

processing. For this aim, Batty and Taylor (2003) used event-related potentials (ERPs)

in their study in order to find out latency and amplitude differences in timing of

processes between fearful, disgusted, sad, surprised, happy, and neutral faces.

According to them, six basic facial emotional expressions are processed very quickly in

normal individuals suggesting that facial details, reflecting emotional content, are

included in rapid processing. Their study concluded that positive emotions (happy and

good surprise) evoked the measure component-N170 significantly earlier than negative

emotions (fear, disgust, and sadness) and the amplitude of N170 evoked by fearful faces

was larger than neutral or surprised faces. In other words, negative emotions were

processed later than positive emotions according to the localization in the brain.

Similar to the study of Batty and Taylor, Rossignol (2005) examined whether

there was a non-clinical anxiety bias toward facial expressions, based on STAI scores.

For this aim, twenty students were distinguished as high or low anxious group. By using

event-related potentials, they investigated neurophysiological dimension of non-clinical

anxiety toward fearful and happy expressions. Consequently, the amplitude of N170

evoked by fearful faces was larger than happy expressions. Even though there was no

significant difference between anxiety groups, fearful expression was processed earlier

than happy expression. This increase was found only for the expression of fear. This

outcome was interpreted, as it might be the result of unconscious attention.

To conclude, even though still no firm conclusion can be drawn from the

existing studies, it is obvious that anxiety and depression affect the processing of facial

expressions in terms of reaction times or accurate decoding. However, there are still

debates in the determination of the most affected expression and it is still difficult to

find the clear-cut results of information processing systems in depression and anxiety.

Page 53: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

39

1. 1. 9. 3. Other Psychopathological Disorders, Recogntion of Emotional Facial

Expressions (EFE) and Reaction Times towards EFE: The Literature Review

The deficits in social interactions are shown in some psychopathological

disorders may be partly related to difficulties in the recognition of facial expressions.

Such deficits have been demonstrated in various clinical populations such as

schizophrenia, personality disorders especially psychopathic and borderline personality

characteristics, Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, autism and attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder (Shayegan and Stahl, 2005; Dujardin et al., 2004; Mendlewicz et

al., 2005; Roudier et al., 1998; Blair et al., 2004).

The empirical evidences so far have displayed that individuals with

schizophrenia have stronger deficits in the recognition of facial expressions. These

deficits may prevent them to interpret others’ intentions, goals, and desires. In one of

the most prominent studies, Weniger et al. (2004) compared different subtypes of

schizophrenia (disorganized, paranoid, and residual types). The results indicated that

particularly individuals with disorganized schizophrenia were the most impaired

subtypes in all schizophrenia spectrums. Whereas individuals with paranoid

schizophrenia were significantly impaired in identifying sadness and fear, disorganized

individuals had impairment in all emotions. All subtypes were better on happy than

disgusted, angry and sad faces. The expressions of surprise and fear were found as the

most difficult expressions to identify than other expressions for all groups. This study

has confirmed that one of the markers in schizophrenia can be a deficit in reading facial

expressions.

Similar to Weniger et al., Martin et al. (2005) examined the abilities of patients

with schizophrenia in recognition of facial expressions and identity matching. Totally

20 patients and a control group that was composed of 20 healthy individuals

participated in the study. They were asked to choose the correct emotion or correct

person of the photos of five persons expressing five different emotions (happiness,

sadness, fear, anger, and disgust). Consequently, performance in patients with

Page 54: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

40

schizophrenia was more impaired than controls. As predicted, they were found as less

successful on both identity-matching and reading facial expressions.

Exner et al. (2004) investigated the volume of amygdala in patients with

schizophrenia in their recognition of emotional expressions. 9 patients with paranoid

schizophrenia, 6 undifferentiated subtypes, 1 disorganized subtype and 16 matched

control individuals were asked to match the corresponding emotion on a probe stimulus

of a face exhibiting one of the six expressions. As expected, individuals with paranoid

schizophrenia showed a deficit in recognizing all emotional expressions. Also, this

study has confirmed that cognitive deficits are more likely to appear in disorganized

symptoms. As hypothesized, the findings have proved that there was a reduced volume

in right amygdala in schizophrenic individuals. That decrease can be associated with the

impairment in the recognition of facial expressions.

From the research carried out so far, there appears to be a substantial support

that individuals with schizophrenia have cognitive impairments in terms of recognition

facial expressions. That impairment has been linked to the serious interruption in their

social relationships.

Besides schizophrenia, recently, most studies have demonstrated that in

Parkinson’s disease (PD) because of the enormous loss of dopaminergic neurons,

cognitive deficits arise. Mainly, these deficits concern tasks including planning or

organizing the sequential actions. (Dujardin et al., 2004). Dujardin et al. (2004)

examined whether PD has an effect on any impairment in reading facial expressions. 18

unmedicated PD patients and healthy individuals participated in this study, as a

conclusion, they found that untreated PD patients were significantly impaired in the

recognition of all facial expressions.

Likewise, Roudier et al. (1998) investigated the recognition of facial expressions

and processing faces identity in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Based on

assumption, which is that specific brain lesions affect the ability to discriminate

unfamiliar faces, they compared 31 AD patients with 14 control group. These

participants were administered to tasks of discrimination of faces and emotions by

presenting emotional faces test of Ekman and Friesen (1975). The findings

Page 55: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

41

demonstrated that AD patients were significantly impaired in discrimination of facial

identities, and in naming and pointing emotions. However, unexpectedly, discrimination

of facial expression was preserved in AD patients, only verbal identification of

emotions significantly impaired. These results suggested that the operations of facial

discrimination and facial discrimination are separate issues. Surely, these findings bring

new debates on brain structures and damages of Alzheimer’s disease especially on

linguistic area. Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s Diseases are relatively recent studies that

still have no firm conclusions.

Nowadays, there have been studies with the individuals of personality disorders.

It is accepted that one of the symptoms of borderline personality disorder is their

significant problems in interpersonal relationships that may be associated with facial

expression decoding. Based on this assumption, Renneberg et al. (2005) investigated the

expressiveness of facial reactions to emotion-eliciting film material among 30 female

Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) inpatients, depressed inpatients, and non-clinical

control group. They expected depressed patients to display less facial expression of

emotions than normal control group, and BDP patients to show more facial reactions

than non-clinical group. As expected, the results indicated that depressed individuals

showed less happy expressions when compared to control group, whereas clinical

groups were not found as different from each other in the reading of facial expressions

of happiness. There was almost no difference between BDP patients and control group.

Interestingly, regarding surprise expression, BDP patients displayed no surprise

expressions while watching the film.

Similarly, psychopathic individuals are accepted as having low level of fear

emotion and empathy dysfunction (Patrick, 1994). Because of these features, they

display emotional dysfunctions and relationship problems. In order to clarify this

assumption, Blair et al. (2004) investigated the ability of psychopathic individuals to

process emotional expressions. They expected a recognition deficit particularly on

negative expressions. As predicted, the results displayed that psychopathic individuals

showed selective impairment for the recognition of fearful expressions than control

individuals. In other words, they did more errors for fearful expressions than other

Page 56: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

42

expressions. In addition, these individuals considered that fearful faces were the most

difficult expression to recognize. Researchers proposed that this impairment could be

associated with the amygdala dysfunction.

In support of Blair et al., Montagne et al. (2005) studied on the accuracy of

perception of emotional faces in normal individuals with scoring high or low on

psychopathic personality characteristics that are characterized by lack of fear. As

expected, the results showed that participants with high scores on psychopathic

personality characteristics were significantly less accurate in recognizing the expression

of fear when compared to participants with low scores on psychopathic personality

characteristics. These two studies have suggested that individuals with psychopathic

tendencies have difficulty to distinguish fearful expression. This inability may be related

with amygdala dysfunction that can be responsible from their antisocial behavior and

their inability to perceive of victim’s distress.

To sum up, it is proved that identification difficulty of emotional facial

expressions is not only specific to alcohol dependence. There are many disorders, which

have proved serious impairments in their decoding of expressions.

1. 2. ALCOHOL DEPENDENCE or ALCOHOLISM

1. 2. 1. Ethyl Alcohol

Literally, alcohol has been derived from an Arabic word al kihl that refers to the

essence of something and it is accepted as the first psychopharmacological agent

(Ceylan & Türkcan, 2003. p.1). Anthropological evidences have suggested that use of

alcohol has been widespread throughout history. The origins of alcohol use pre-date

recorded history and it is believed that ethanol was known even by Neolithic man. For

instance, in China archaeological evidence dates the origin of alcohol from fermented

grain to some 6000-7000 years ago (Heather, Peters & Stockwell, 2001, p. 16). Alcohol

can be described as the world’s favorite substance. While the pains of alcohol are the

numerous and diverse, its popularity continues.

Page 57: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

43

1. 2. 2. Alcohol and Harm

In fact, use of alcohol and related problems were encountered even in ancient

ages such as Ancient Greek, Roman, or Egypt. Hippocrates, the Greek physician

described symptoms such as nausea, insomnia, palpitations and delirium which,

centuries later, became a familiar part of the clinical picture of “alcoholism”. However,

scientific attention to alcohol problems has accelerated during the past 30 years, when

substantial advances have occurred in understanding of drinking problems as well as

their prevention and treatment. It has been known that alcohol consumption is

responsible for increased illness and death (Room, Babor & Rehm, 2005). However,

alcohol and health outcomes are complex and multidimensional. Alcohol has been

shown to be causally related to more than 60 different medical –physical and mental-

conditions. It causes many disruptions in social, occupational, and family life (Miller,

1990; Hodgson et al.).

Furthermore, gender differences in the consequences of drinking alcohol are

large and consistent. Generally, these studies suggest that women appear to suffer

serious negative consequences of alcohol consumption earlier and to a greater degree

than men do. Specifically, they suffer more cognitive and motor impairment due to

heavy alcohol than men (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2004). These negative effects on cognitive,

motor, and reproductive health as well as social norms pressures on women mostly

discourage women from excessive alcohol intake when compared to men. Therefore, in

our study merely males were used.

1. 2. 3. Alcohol Dependence or Alcoholism

Specifically, alcohol can be a problem, but identifying who is an alcoholic, who

is a problem drinker, and who is a responsible drinker can be difficult at times. A person

is identified as having alcoholism when s/he is physiologically dependent on alcohol,

and therefore shows withdrawal symptoms when no alcohol has been consumed.

Problem drinkers are not physiologically addicted to alcohol, but still have a number of

problems stemming from alcohol consumption, including problems with work and

family, and health-related complications (Kowalski & Westen, 2005, p. 391).

Page 58: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

44

Alcoholism or alcohol dependence is a deeply pervasive, malignant, long-term,

and multifaceted problem (Hoes, 1997). It can be defined as a condition in which

individuals drink alcohol habitually and excessively and individuals continue to drink

even though this results in serious harm to their physical, mental health and family and

school or occupation (Berger, 1993, p.11). In other words, alcoholics have problems to

keep their drinking habit under control. On the other hand, some theorists suggest that

alcoholism is a family disease because the entire family hurts when one or more

members are alcohol dependent. A serious drinking problem mostly lead to economic

problems, a loss of self-respect, psychological and emotional difficulties such as

divorce, suicide, delinquency (Berger, 1993, p.11).

1. 2. 4. History of Classification

Various classification systems were published by medical writers in US and

Europe. Bruhl-Cramer introduced the concept of dipsomania or drink seeking. In 1849,

the term alcoholic was coined by Magnus Huss in Sweden to describe people who

suffered negative consequences of alcohol use. Between 1850 and 1941, totally 39

classifications of alcoholism were developed around the world. Generally, these

classification systems were rooted on excessive drinking (Ceylan & Turkcan, 2003, p.

2; Heather, Peters & Stockwell, 2001, p. 49).

In US from the 19th century to 1933 alcohol was seen as a dangerous substance

that should be avoided. When alcohol was legalized in 1933, American disease model

of alcoholism has started. In 1941, Bowman and Jellinek published an elaborate

classification scheme for the disease, based on the pattern of drinking frequency,

etiology, co-morbid disorders, and ability to abstain (Heather, Peters & Stockwell, 2001,

p. 49). For instance, Jellinek postulated five species of alcoholism: alpha, beta, gamma,

delta, and epsilon. His classification became the most popular alcohol typology for the

next 20 years. However, these classification systems were not found as sufficient to be

used currently.

Page 59: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

45

1. 2. 5. Current Diagnostic Classification

Current nosology for alcohol use disorders and dependence relies on the

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM; American Psychiatric

Association, 1952, 1968, 1980, 1987, 1994) and the International Statistical

Classification of Diseases, Injuries, and Cause of Death (ICD; World Health

Organization, 1967, 1978, 1989, 1992). Current Diagnostic criteria for both the DSM

and ICD are based on years of evolution of the concept of excessive use, associated

negative consequences, and physical dependence. However, as illustrated below using

DSM-IV one can be diagnosed as Alcohol Dependent without having physiological

dependence. The Alcohol Abuse diagnosis is more detailed and better operationalized,

and the threshold is lower, that is, only one of the four symptoms must be positive in

order to get a diagnosis. Diagnosis of Alcohol Dependence and Alcohol Abuse are

mutually exclusive. A patient who has ever been diagnosed with Alcohol Dependence

cannot be diagnosed with Alcohol Abuse (Heather, Peters & Stockwell, 2001, p. 57).

Based on DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000) Alcohol use disorders are the alcohol-

related psychiatric disorders. These include alcohol abuse, which involves persistent

drinking behavior in the face of repeated social, interpersonal, and occupational

problems that are due to excessive alcohol consumption. Alcohol dependence includes

these psychosocial problems, but can also involve physiological dependence on alcohol,

such as tolerance and withdrawal symptoms. This syndrome is often referred to as

alcoholism. Heavy drinking and binge drinking are operationalized in different ways

across different studies. Alcohol-related problems will be used to refer scores on

measures of negative psychosocial consequences of excessive alcohol consumption

such as arrest for drunken driving. Finally, social drinkers is the label for people who

drink alcohol at least occasionally, but do not meet the criteria for alcohol abuse or

dependence (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2004).

Page 60: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

46

DSM-IV Criteria for Alcohol Dependence (APA, 1994)

A. A maladaptive pattern of substance use, leading to clinically significant impairment

or distress as manifested by three or more of the following occurring at any time in the

same 12-month period.

(1) Tolerance, as defined by either of the following:

a. Need for markedly increased amounts of a substance to achieve intoxication

or desired effect.

b. Markedly diminished effect with continued use of the same amount of the

substance.

(2) Withdrawal, as manifested by either of the following: (a) characteristic withdrawal

syndrome for the substance, or (b) the same (or a closely substance is taken to relieve or

avoid related) withdrawal symptoms.

(3) The substance is often taken in larger amounts or over a longer period than was

intended.

(4) There is a persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control substance

use.

(5) A great deal of time is spent in activities necessary to obtain the substance, use the

substance, or recover its effects.

(6) Important social, occupational, or recreational activities are given up or reduced

because of substance use.

(7) The substance use is continued despite knowledge of having a persistent or recurrent

physical or psychological problem that is likely to have been caused or exacerbated by

the substance.

Specifiers;

*With a physiological dependence

*Without a physiological dependence

Page 61: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

47

1. 2. 6. Epidemiology

Nowadays the most common substance-related disorder has been alcoholism

around the world. Surely, a number of genetic and environmental variables contribute to

the development of alcohol abuse.

• As in other Western countries, alcoholism is the third largest health problem in

the US, following heart disease and cancer.

• There is a sex difference (male/female) of 17.7 % vs. 6.3 %.

• In the US, an estimated 13 million or 7.4 percent of the population meet the

diagnostic criteria for alcohol abuse or alcoholism. When alcoholism is an index

disorder, its lifetime prevalence in males under the age of 60 years varies

between 9.8% (USA) to 43.8% (Canada).

• The highest prevalence is in 14-19 to 40-59 year-old.

• More than one half of American adults have a close family member who has or

has had alcoholism (Hoes, 1997; Health Risk, 2000, cited in Kowalski &

Westen, 2005, p. 391).

A research indicating that alcohol use in Turkey is increasing, particularly in the

last twenty years, has been recently carried out by Ministry of Health. The alcohol use

prevalence was found to be 42.6% and the rate of regular alcohol use was 20.5% in

young population. Additionally, Ceylan and Türkcan (2003) reported that in a sample of

1550 individuals, aged between 12 and 65, representing İstanbul population in terms of

age, gender and districts, prevalence of alcohol use was 33.5%. Furthermore, 25.6% of

the sample stated that they currently use alcohol, 7.9% stated they cut down alcohol use

and those who use alcohol, 12.6% stated that they use alcohol more than once during a

day.

To sum up, accordant with research carried out in abroad, studies on alcohol

prevalence in Turkey displayed that alcohol use has been increasing among population,

particularly in young adults. Moreover, they demonstrated that prevalence of alcohol

dependence disorders have been also increasing.

Page 62: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

48

1. 2. 7. Alcohol Dependence and Co-morbid Disorders

Alcohol dependence is usually exhibited other forms of mental disorder

particularly mood disorders, anxiety disorders and occasionally antisocial personality

disorders (Oltmanns & Emery, 2001, p.382; Preisig et al., 2001).

The relationships between alcohol dependence and mood or anxiety disorders

have been well documented in many studies. The associations between alcoholism,

mood, and anxiety disorders have not only been found in clinical settings, but also in

community samples. It has been suggested that individuals with alcohol dependence

show moderate to severe depressive symptomatology and they are more prone to

anxiety symptoms than non-dependent individuals do (Tedstone et al., 2004; Gratzer et

al., 2004, Terra et al. 2006). Grant and Harford (1995) have suggested that dependent

individuals up to 70% meet criteria for a lifetime diagnosis of depression. Preisig et al.

(2001) postulated that in alcohol dependent individuals the frequency of depressive

symptomatology ranged from 16% to 59%.

Some researchers believe that alcohol misuse especially in men is a mask for

depression (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2004). Similarly, regarding the anxiety disorders, Gratzer

et al. (2004) found that 35-54% of individuals with an anxiety disorder had a lifetime

diagnosis of an alcohol use disorder. Additionally, their findings revealed that

depression had a stronger relationship with alcohol dependence than anxiety. In another

study, again it was found that there was a stronger association between alcohol misuse

and negative affect such as anxiety and depression (Berger and Adesso, 1991).

Ceylan and Türkcan (2003) pointed out on gender differences about the

relationship between alcohol use and some mental disorders. They proposed that in men

mostly antisocial personality disorder and anxiety disorders-phobia and panic disorder

(15%), depression (5%) however in women mostly anxiety disorders-phobia and panic

disorder (38%), depression (19%), antisocial personality disorder (10%) displayed co-

morbidity with alcoholism.

Possible sources of the associations between depression, anxiety disorders, and

alcoholism have been studied extensively. Generally, it is assumed that alcohol is used

in order to manage or regulate the affects of drinkers, to increase positive affect when

Page 63: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

49

people are fatigue, decrease when they are anxious, or over aroused. However, the

evidence regarding the nature of these associations remains vague.

In sum, the above studies provide strong evidences that both anxiety disorders

and depression are associated with high rates of alcohol dependence or alcohol abuse.

Hence, in this study one of the aims is to find the accordant findings with previous

studies.

1. 2. 8. Alcohol Dependence and Cognition

In recent years, substantial evidences have shown that alcohol dependent

individuals suffer some degree of brain dysfunction. It has been proved that chronic

alcohol dependence has deleterious effects on central nervous system functioning

(Chelune and Parker, 1981). Long-term chronic alcohol misuse causes disturbances just

like brain aging.

Neuropsychological studies have revealed that dependence causes essential

cognitive impairments such as on neocortex or on hippocampus that decrease in abstract

concept-formation or abstract reasoning ability, cognitive flexibility, problem-solving

ability, new learning, perceptuomotor speed, accuracy processes and lastly

visuoperceptual area and surely memory processes (Miller, 1990; Brandt & Provost,

1985; Garcia-Moreno et al., 2002; Chelune and Parker, 1981; Loas et al., 2000).

Chelune and Parker (1981) proposed that alcohol dependent individuals were

good at on familiar settings but they had troubles on complex or new settings. As a

result of their study, whereas the verbal skills remained same, performance and adaptive

abilities significantly disrupted when compared to non-dependent individuals.

Although Chelune and Parker (1981) did not find any significant results

regarding verbal skill impairment, Monnot et al. (2001) found that on affective prosody

functioning (nonlinguistic feature of language such as pitch, innotation patterns, stress,

timing, rhythm etc) which, is an essential element in social interaction localized to the

right hemisphere, detoxified alcoholics demonstrated significant deficits in their ability

to identify emotion in the voices of others.

Page 64: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

50

Accordant with previous studies, Obernier et al. (2002) postulated that alcohol

dependent individuals displayed decreased neuropsychological performance on the tests

of learning, memory, abstract thinking, problem solving, visuospatial and perceptual

motor functioning, and information processing. In addition, they suffered damage to the

hippocampal formation, frontal cortex, and grey and white matter loss.

As conclusion, most of the literature up to now has focused on traditional

intellectual cognitive skills. However, other skills may be impaired in alcohol abusers or

in alcohol dependent individuals. For instance, emotional processing abilities in alcohol

dependent individuals have been studied less extensively with a few notable exceptions.

Thus, in this study the relationship between emotional facial expression and alcohol

dependence was focused.

1. 2. 9. Alcohol Dependence and Recognition of Emotional Facial Expressions

(EFE): The Literature Review

It is apparent that alcohol dependent individuals frequently have severe

interpersonal difficulties. It has been also proved that accurate recognition of facial

expressions enables healthy social relationships. Numerous studies have been designed

to examine the association of alcohol dependence with the recognition of facial

expressions especially towards negative emotions. Some studies have confirmed this

assumption and some have not.

In order to find out any deficit in the perception of alcohol dependent

individuals, Philippot et al. (1999) conducted a study. As a result of this study, they

found that recognition of facial expressions seemed to be severely impaired in

recovering alcoholics. They suggested that particularly alcoholic dependents had a bias

in the recognition of angry and contempt. Specifically, according to these authors,

alcoholic participants had a systematic bias in interpreting faces expressing disgust as

showing anger or contempt.

In accordance with the findings of Philippot et al., the findings of Frigerio et al.

(2002) study manifested that alcohol dependent individuals did more errors in the

recognition facial expressions. However, it was found that alcohol dependent

Page 65: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

51

individuals had a specific deficit in recognizing sad faces. Based on the study of Frigero

et al. (2002), alcoholics had a tendency to interpret sad faces as anger or disgust. If so,

they may be more sensitive to feelings to threat from faces looking at them and they are

more likely to interpret facial expressions as hostile than as sadness.

Furthermore, according to the study of Philippot et al. (1999), alcohol dependent

individuals also showed overattribution of anger and contempt and they systematically

overestimated the intensity of all emotions. However, Kornreich et al. (2001) found that

recovering alcoholics tended to overestimate the intensity of the EFE for only happiness

and anger. Similarly Kornreich et al. (2001) found that recovering alcoholics displayed

accuracy deficits especially for happiness, anger, disgust, and sadness but not for fear.

Both studies did not find any gender differences in decoding EFE (Philippot et al.,1999;

Kornreich et al., 2001).

However, inconsistent with the findings of both Philippot et al. (1999) and

Kornreich et al. (2001), Townshend & Duka (2003) displayed that alcoholics showed

different patterns in responding on anger and disgust but they did not find any

differences between experimental and normal control groups in responding on happy,

sad and surprised expressions.

Kornreich et al. (2003) replicated their studies in order to explore that whether

impairment in EFE decoding is specific to alcoholism compared with opiate

dependence. They found that accuracy scores were significantly lower in alcoholics

rather than opiate dependents. Surprisingly, they noted that opiate dependence was also

associated with an impaired EFE decoding however less than alcoholism.

That is to say, substantial evidences have presented the deficiency in the

recognition of facial expressions in alcohol dependent individuals whereas still there

have been no stable conclusions yet.

To sum up, alcohol perhaps more than any other substances, touches the lives of

most people dramatically in the whole world as well as in Turkey. The long-term use of

alcohol has devastating impact on many areas of a person’s life. Besides the effect on

organ systems, especially the disruption of relationships with family and friends can be

very painful. On the other hand, one of the crucial sources in regulating interpersonal

Page 66: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

52

relationships in daily life depends on the ability to perceive the emotional state of other

people. However, it seems obvious that alcoholic individuals frequently have severe

interpersonal difficulties that may be related to poor or inaccurate perception of

emotional facial expressions (EFE).

1. 3. Purposes of the Present Study

Emotional expressions are important sources in terms of social communication

in daily life. Therefore, any confusion or any impairment in understanding emotional

facial expression may interrupt daily relationships. A growing number of studies have

demonstrated that individuals with some disorders such as affective disorders, anxiety

disorders, personality disorders, psychotic disorders, and substance disorders have some

deficits in reading these expressions. Some may have attentional bias in reading or

reacting toward sad expressions, some in angry or fearful that they assume as

threatening (Mogg et al., 2000; Rohner, 2002; Blair et al., 2004).

Numerous studies have shown that long-term alcohol consumption can lead to

cognitive and neurological impairments. So far, little attention has been given to studies

that are related with the recognition deficits in alcohol dependent individuals. These

rarely studies consistently have proved that individuals with alcohol dependence have

some impairments in recognizing emotional expressions, which may be associated with

interpersonal conflicts (e.g. Garcia-Moreno et al., 2002; Frigerio et al., 2002; Kornreich

et al., 1998, 2001, 2002; Philippot, 1999).

Thereby, this study aimed to investigate the accuracy of recognition of

universally recognized Emotional Facial Expression (EFE) -happiness, sadness, anger,

disgust, fear, surprise, and neutral expressions- and to measure the manual reaction

times (RT) towards these expressions in alcohol dependent inpatients and in normal

control group. Depression and anxiety levels were measured that may influence the

results in terms of attentional or evaluative biases. Besides the differences of two

groups, some similarities would be predicted in terms of the tendency of expressions.

Page 67: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

53

1. 4. Hypotheses of the Study

1. Alcohol dependent group would score higher than normal control group on the

CAGE inventory, Beck Depression Inventory, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory,

and Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R) and its nine subscales as compared to

normal control group.

2. Alcohol dependent group would show more deficits in total emotional

recognition accuracy than normal control group.

3. Alcohol dependent group would show more deficits especially in the

recognition of negative emotional expressions, which are anger, fear, sad, and

disgust, than non-dependent individuals. In other words, it was predicted that

alcohol dependents would display less accurate scores than normal control

group.

4. Alcohol dependent group would react faster to the negative expressions as

compared to normal control group.

5. When Beck Depression Inventory, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, and SCL-90

were accepted as covariates, alcohol dependent group would show more

evaluative bias (misinterpretation) especially toward negative expressions as

compared to normal control group.

6. When Beck Depression Inventory, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, and SCL-90

were accepted as covariates, alcohol dependent group would show more

attentional bias/hypervigilance and thus they would react faster to the negative

expressions as compared to normal control group.

7. The most correct recognition would be for happy facial expression than other

expressions for both groups.

Page 68: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

54

CHAPTER II

METHOD

2. 1. Participants

In order to test the proposed hypotheses, totally 101 volunteers participated in

the current study. From Alcohol and Substance Treatment Center (AMATEM) of

İstanbul Mazhar Osman Bakırköy Education and Research Hospital for Psychiatry and

Neurological Diseases, 51 (50%) detoxified alcohol dependent inpatients who were

treated with antidepressants or anxiolytics at the time of the study and 50 normal control

group (49%) that were match for age, sex and years of education participated. Inpatients

who were treated with antipsychotics were excluded from the study. At least 10 years of

dependence was the inclusion criterion for the inpatient participants. All the participants

were males because it was easier to access. The presence of a psychotic disorder,

antisocial personality disorder, or dementia led to exclusion from the study.

Approximately, inpatients with 15 days participated in the study.

As a control group, volunteer judges, prosecutors, clerks, and other employees in

İstanbul Beyoglu Courthouse; teachers and other personnels in a private education

centre; volunteers in ordinary cafés participated in the present study. While selecting the

participants for control group, first they were administered CAGE inventory to detect

possible alcohol dependence.

The mean age of all participants was 46.21 (SD = 7.19, range = 30-67). The

mean age of alcohol dependent group was 45.50 (SD = 7.56), control group’s mean age

was 46.94 (SD = 6.79).

2. 2. Materials

Both groups were administered a battery of self-report measures including

Demographic Information Form, CAGE Alcoholism Questionnaire, The Symptom

Checklist (SCL-90-R), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), and State-Trait Anxiety

Page 69: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

55

Inventory (STAI) and finally a Emotion Recognition Test that was constructed by using

a set of photographs from Ekman and Friesen’s (1976) “Pictures of Facial Affect”.

2. 2. 1. Demographic Information Form

It was developed by the authors in order to collect some information including

gender, age, educational level, marital status, occupational status, years of alcohol use,

hospitalization number, use in family, and reason of use. A copy of this form was

presented in Appendix A.

2. 2. 2. CAGE Alcoholism Inventory (1968)

The CAGE (Cut Down, Annoyed, Guilty, Eye Opener) inventory that is quite

simple and rapid was originally developed by Ewing and Rouse in 1968. It was

designed to detect current alcoholism. It consists of four items that are asked to with

respect to an occurrence during the previous one year. Each item can have either yes or

no response. Two or more positive answers are the common cut-off for detecting

alcoholism. The items are:

1. Have you ever felt you should cut down on your drinking?

2. Have people annoyed you by criticizing your drinking?

3. Have you ever felt bad or guilty about your drinking?

4. Have you ever had a drink in the morning? (eye opener)

It has proven to be a valid instrument (Bisson et al., 1999, Ewing, 1984, Bühler,

et al. 2004). It shows high internal consistency with a Cronbach alpha coefficient of

.85. Furthermore, it shows some similarities with DSM-IV dependence items such as

cut-down attempts in CAGE and quit/cut-down attempts in DSM-IV; annoyed in

CAGE, social problems in DSM-IV; eye-opener in CAGE, tolerance and withdrawal

in DSM-IV (Bühler et al., 2004). Although this simple questionnaire is used in

medical area, its reliability and validity coefficients could not have been found for

Turkey. A copy of the Turkish version of CAGE was presented in Appendix B.

Page 70: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

56

2. 2. 3. The Symptom Checklist (SCL 90-R, 1977)

SCL-90-R was developed by Derogatis and Clearly (1977) in order to assess the

symptoms and these symptoms’ severity in preceding one week. Kılıç (1987) translated

it into Turkish and by using 416 university students he found .91 as the internal

consistency value of the scale (Öner, 1997, p.463). Then Dağ (1991) investigated the

psychometric properties of Turkish version of the Symptom Checklist- Revised (SCL-

90-R) by using 532 students from Hacettepe University and reported that SCL-90- R is

a reliable and valid instrument. It includes totally 90 items under 10 subscales

respectively:

1. Somatization (SOM) that consists of 12 items concerning with somatic distress

of the body;

2. Obsessive-Compulsive (O-C) that consists of 10 items related with distress of

unwanted and uncontrollable thoughts and behaviors (compulsions);

3. Interpersonal Sensitivity (INT) consists of 9 items that is associated with

personal distress about feeling inferiority and incompetency when meeting with people;

4. Depression (DEP) consists of 13 items that is related with the distress of

general pessimism, anhedonia, hopelessness, lack of motivation, suicidal ideations and

other cognitive & somatic symptoms;

5. Anxiety (ANX) consists of 10 items that concerns with the distress of high

level of anxiety and irritable mood;

6. Hostility (HOS) consists of 6 items that is related with distress of

aggressiveness, being oppositional, hostile.

7. Phobic Anxiety (PHOB) consists of 7 items that is associated with the distress

of having specific phobia;

8. Paranoid Ideation (PAR) consists of 6 items that is concerning with the

distress of projective thoughts, persecutions, grandiosity feelings and delusions;

9. Psychotism (PSY) consists of 10 items that is related with the distress of

socially introversion, schizoid style of living or having hallucinations or delusions;

10. Additional Items consist of 7 items that are associated with the distress of

sleep and eating disturbances, guilty feelings.

Page 71: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

57

SCL-90-R is 5-point Likert type checklist. By asking “how much below

problems or symptoms affected you for last one week?” participants sign the most

appropriate choice based on their conditions (never, a little, half, very much, completely)

and take respectively 0, 1, 2, 3, or 4 scores. For the calculation of the score of each

subscale, total subscale scores are added and are divided into the number of the items in

that subscale. SCL-90-R was used is three ways: Global Symptom Index (GSI), Positive

Symptom Total (PST), and Positive Symptom Distress Index (PSDI). In order to

determine Global Symptom Index (GSI) score, except unanswered ones, all subscale

scores are added, and after total score is found, this score is divided to 90. The possible

score ranged between 0 and 4. For each subscale validity, at least 40% of the checklist

should have been completed. For the interpretation of scores, scores from 0 to 1.5

accepted as normal, scores between 1.51- 2.50 were considered as high distress, scores

between 2.50-4.00 were considered as a very high distress.

Originally Derogatis et al. (1980) found that reliability coefficients of the scale

ranged from .77 to .90 (.86 for SOM subscale, .86 for O-C subscale, .86 for INT

subscale, .90 for DEP subscale, .85 for ANX subscale, .84 for HOS subscale, .82 for

PHOB subscale, .80 for PAR subscale, .77 PSY subscale).

In Dağ (1991) study, test-retest reliability of the scale was found as .90 (n=99),

for each subscale test-retest reliability ranged from .65 to .87, (.75 for SOM subscale,

.87 for O-C subscale, .84 for INT subscale, .87 for DEP subscale, .73 for ANX subscale,

.70 for HOS subscale, .65 for PHOB subscale, .73 for PAR subscale, .79 PSY subscale)

and internal consistency was found as (Cronbach Alpha) .97. Pearson correlation

coefficients between MMPI and SCL-90-R and BDI were .10 - .77 and .78,

respectively. Convergent and discriminant validities were tested by correlations between

SCL-90-R, MMPI, and BDI and it was found as satisfactory. Briefly, results showed

that the SCL-90-R is a reliable and valid instrument for normal university students in

Turkey. Higher scores on this scale indicate greater symptoms. A copy of the Turkish

version was presented in Appendix C.

Page 72: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

58

2. 2. 4. State- Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI, 1970)

STAI is a 40-item self-report scale designed by Spielberger et al. in 1970 in

order to measure the level of state (situational- how a person feels at that moment) and

trait (continual- how a person feels in general independently from any specific situation

or time) anxiety of individuals. It consists of two parts; State and Trait Anxiety

Inventories, and each one consists of 20 items. In practice, state anxiety inventory is

given first, so that the individual could declare her/his anxiety because of seeking help

from mental health professional. In State Anxiety Inventory, by asking “how do you

feel now” to participants, the items are rated on a 4-point scale with 1 standing for not at

all, 2 for a little, 3 for very much, and 4 for completely. In Trait Anxiety Inventory, by

asking "how do you feel in general" to participants, the items are rated on a four-point

scale with 1 standing for almost never, 2 for sometimes, 3 for mostly and 4 for almost

always.

Originally, test-retest reliability of the scale ranged from .16 to .54 for state

anxiety inventory and from .73 to .86 for trait anxiety inventory. The internal

consistency of the former one varied from .83 and .92, and between .86 and .92 for

latter one. Construct and criterion validity values were reported to be satisfactory

(Spielberger et al. 1970).

Öner and Le Comte (1985) performed adaptation study of STAI by examining

both normal and psychiatric patients. Test-retest reliability was found to be ranging .71

to .86 for trait anxiety inventory, from .26 to .68 for state anxiety inventory. Internal

consistency of trait anxiety inventory ranged from .83 to .87, while that of state anxiety

inventory ranged from .94 to .96. Criterion and construct validities were demonstrated

to be satisfactory. A copy of the Turkish version of STAI was presented in Appendix D.

2. 2. 5. Beck Depression Inventory (BDI, 1972)

Depressive symptomatology was assessed by using BDI that is a 21-item self-

report inventory of depression and its levels. It was designed by Beck in 1972.

Participants were asked to circle the most appropriate choice by considering last week.

Each choice has its own score and as a result of these scoring the level of depression is

Page 73: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

59

determined. The highest score on this scale is 63. Scores less than 10 refer to no

depression, 11 to 19 mild depression, 20 to 25 to moderate depression, and scores 26

and higher to severely depression.

Beck (1982) found split-half reliability as .74, and Miller & Seligman (1973)

found test-retest reliability as .74. Also, when compared to Hamilton Depression Scale,

validity coefficient was found to be as .75.

In Turkey, Teğin translated BDI into Turkish in 1980 and found test-retest

reliability as .65 on University students. Split-half reliability coefficient was found for

inpatients as .61, for university students as .78. Lastly, Turkish adaptation of BDI has

been developed by Hisli (1998; cited in Şahin & Savaşır, 1997) will be used in the

study. Internal consistency of the Turkish version BDI has been found as .74 and test-

retest reliability coefficient was between .74 and .86 (Şahin & Savaşır, 1997). A copy of

the Turkish version of BDI was presented in Appendix E.

2. 2. 6. Emotion Recognition Test

This computer-based test was constructed by the authors by using a set of

photographs from Ekman & Friesen’s (1976) “Photos of Facial Affect”. It included the

photos of four male and four female models (totally 56 photos) with happy, surprised,

fearful, sad, disgusted, angry, and neutral facial expressions that were selected from

Ekman & Friesen’s (1976) series. The mixed photos were digitized on a computer

presentation via JAVA application developed for the presentation on a portable

computer (ASUS XP 2800 2 Ghz processor, 512 MB main memory, 15” LCD screen

with 1024x768 resolution). It had a trial section that was composed of the first seven

photos. The trial section included every emotional facial expressions (anger, sadness,

happiness, neutral, fear, disgust and surprise) that was presented in the same order for

each participants. Rest of the 49 photos was used for the analyses in the study. There

were equal numbers of happy, sad, surprised, fearful, disgusted, angry, and neutral

expressions overall ensuring that participants did not become familiarized to one

specific emotional category. These 49 photos were presented randomly for each

participant. There were 20-second intervals for each photo. On the monitor, participants

Page 74: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

60

saw both photos and colorful seven options in the below of these photos and they were

asked to push the same color button option on the keyboard that match on the monitor.

On the keyboard, D button represents happiness, F button represents sadness, G button

represents fear, H button represents disgust, J button represents anger, K button

represents surprise, and L button represents neutral facial expressions.

2. 3. Procedure

The volunteer inpatients that were under the treatment of alcohol dependence

were asked to participate in the study. Then, they were asked to read and sign the

informed consent form before starting to fill the questionnaires and conducting of

Emotion Recognition Test.

For the control group, predetermined places (courthouses, private schools, and

ordinary cafés) were visited and asked for permission to administer the questionnaires

and Emotion Recognition Test. Differently from experimental group, they were not

asked to fill some questions such as hospitalization number and reasons of drinking in

the demographic information form.

After all participants (N = 101) were assessed with CAGE, SCL-90-R, BDI and

STAI, two groups were administered on an Emotion Recognition Test. All participants

were tested individually in a quiet room. They were instructed to respond as accurately

and as quickly as possible. All stimulus material took approximately 2 or 3 minutes to

complete. Instructions were given in the same way for two groups. No feedback was

given regarding the appropriateness of any response. After completing the

questionnaires and administration of computer based Emotion Recognition Test,

participants were debriefed and thanked for their participation.

2. 4. Data Analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted with the Statistical Package for Social

Sciences for Windows (SPSS) version 13.00. Demographic information was analyzed

through descriptive statistics.

Page 75: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

61

For the analyses of Beck Depression Inventory, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory,

Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R), and CAGE Alcohol Inventory, two-tailed t-test

analyses were applied in order to compare group differences.

In order to examine the decoding accuracy of emotional facial expressions in

both groups, first of all, for each participant the number of correct responses for each

type of facial expression was calculated and an accurately identified expression

received 1 score, and misidentified expression received 0 score. Same procedures were

carried out in order to examine the reaction times; each participant’s

recognition/reaction times toward each expression were calculated. Only accurate

responses were considered for the analysis of reaction times. To sum, each participant

obtained a total reaction time score and a recognition accuracy score. Additional to

these total scores, accuracy scores and reaction times scores toward each facial

expression were also calculated.

A series of 2 (group: dependents vs. non-dependent individuals) *4 (negative

emotional expressions: sadness, fear, anger, disgust) multivariate analysis of variance

(MANOVA) were performed in order to find out group differences in the recognition

accuracy and reaction times. In the analyses of decoding accuracy and reaction times for

happy, surprised and neutral expressions, a series of one-way between subjects

ANOVAs were conducted because of the different natures of emotions.

In order to determine whether there was a group difference on accuracy scores

and general reaction times when depression, state-trait anxiety, and general symptom

distress levels were taken as covariates, a series of one way with covariates between

group designs (ANCOVA) were conducted. To find out the effects of four covariates

(BDI, STAI, SCL-90-R) on each expression, all facial expressions again were analyzed

based on their characteristics. For negative facial expressions 2*4 MANCOVA analysis

was run for the comparison of group differences in the recognition accuracy and reaction

times. For happiness, surprise, and neutral expressions, a series of one-way between

subjects with covariates -ANCOVAs were conducted.

Misinterpretations of emotional facial expressions were found by calculating the

frequencies of each participant’s incorrect responses. The most frequent inaccurate

Page 76: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

62

responses were revealed and their percentages were calculated for both groups.

Furthermore, in order to find out whether alcoholism would be predicted by

predictors, we conducted a stepwise multiple analysis.

Page 77: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

63

CHAPTER III

RESULTS

First of all, data was examined for missing values, univariate and multivariate

outliers, and 3 cases were deleted. All analyses were carried out by totally 101 cases.

3. 1. Demographic Characteristics of Participants

Based on demographic information form, education levels, marital status,

occupational status, and lastly the duration of alcohol use were determined.

General results revealed that, as illustrated in Table 1 and 2, in terms of

educational level among the all participants (N = 101), 4 individuals (4%) were literate,

27 individuals (26.7%) were graduated from primary school graduated, 23 individuals

(22.8%) were secondary school graduated, 26 individuals (25.7%) had a high school

degree and lastly 21 individuals (20.8%) university graduates.

In terms of occupational status; among the all participants (N = 101), 7

individuals (6.9%) were workers, 45 individuals (44.6%) worked as a state officer, 30

individuals (29.7%) were employers, 15 individuals (14.9%) were retired and finally

only 4 individuals (4%) were unemployed (see Table 2).

Among the all participants (N = 101), 6 individuals (5.9%) were single, 79

individuals (78.2%) were married, 14 individuals (13.9%) were divorced, and 2

individuals (2%) were separated (see Table 2).

Finally, our experimental group (n = 51), in terms of the duration of alcohol use;

their mean is 22 years. Specifically, 23 individuals (46%) reported their use from 10 to

20 years, 20 individuals (40%) reported that from 21 to 30 years, and 7 individuals

(14%) reported more than 31 years (see Table 2).

Page 78: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

64

Table 1. Demographic Variables Alcohol Dependence N Mean S.D. t (df)

Age dependent 51 45.49 7.56 -1.013(99) non-dependent 50 46.94 6.79 Education dependent 51 3.29 1.17 -1.276(99) non-dependent 50 3.36 1.22 * p < .001

Table 2. Demographic Variables

N

%

Education

Literate Dependent Non-dependent

1 3

2% 6%

Primary S. Graduation Dependent Non-dependent

16 11

31% 22%

Secondary S. Graduation Dependent Non-dependent

11 12

21% 24%

High School G. Dependent Non-dependent

13 13

25% 26%

Undergraduate or above Dependent Non-dependent

10 11

19% 22%

Occupational Status Worker Dependent Non-dependent

5 2

9.8% 4%

State Officer Dependent Non-dependent

11 34

21.6% 68.4%

Employer Dependent Non-dependent

17 13

33.3% 26%

Retired Dependent Non-dependent

14 1

27.5% 2%

Unemployed Dependent Non-dependent

4 0

7.8% 0

Marital Status Single Dependent Non-dependent

5 1

9.8% 2%

Page 79: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

65

Table2.Demographic Variables (Contiuned) n %

Non-dependent 48 96.2% Divorced Dependent Non-dependent

13 1

25.5% 2%

Separated Dependent Non-dependent

2 0

3.9% 0

Alcohol Use in Family

Yes Dependent Non-dependent

8 3

15.7% 6%

No Dependent Non-dependent

43 47

84.3% 94%

Duration of Alcohol Use 10-20 years 23 46% 21-30 years 20 40% ≥ 31 years 8 14%

3. 2. CAGE Alcoholism Inventory, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), State-Trait

Anxiety Inventory, (STAI) and Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R) Scores

In order to compare experimental and control group in terms of their depression,

state-trait anxiety, alcohol dependence, and psychopathological symptoms, two-tailed t-

test analyses were conducted on the CAGE, BDI, STAI, and SCL-90-R scores. As

hypothesized, the two groups differed significantly in all measures except state anxiety as

illustrated in Table 3. Inpatients were significantly more alcohol dependent than control

group indicating that CAGE is an appropriate instrument to classify the two groups in

terms of dependency, t (99) = 27.32, p <.001. In terms of BDI scores, the results have

displayed that alcohol dependent individuals obtained significantly higher scores on BDI

than non-dependent individuals, t (99) = 4.340, p <.001; they had significantly higher

trait anxiety levels indicating that these inpatients were generally but not currently more

anxious than control group, t (99) = 4.169, p <.001. Finally they had significantly more

psychopathological symptoms on total psychopathology checklist t (99) = 4.814, p <.001.

As illustrated in Table 5, when 9 subscales of SCL-90-R were considered, results

revealed that alcohol dependent group significantly scored higher on all subscales; t-test

Page 80: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

66

values were respectively; for somatization t (99) = 3.657, p < .001; for obsessive thoughts

t (99) = 4.867, p < .001; for interpersonal sensitivity t (99) = 4.052, p < .001; for

depression t (99) = 3.926, p < .001; for anxiety t (99) = 2.898, p < .001; for hostility t (99)

= 2.898, p < .05; for phobic anxiety t (99) = 5.213, p < .001; for paranoid thoughts t (99)

= 3.075, p < .05; and finally for psychotism t (99) = 5.036, p < .001. In other words,

alcohol dependent individuals have had significantly more discomfort on these nine

subtypes than non-dependent individuals (see Table 4).

Table 3. Descriptive Statistics of BDI, STAI, CAGE and SCL-90-R Results Measures Alcohol

Dependence N Mean

SD t-test (df)

BDI Dependent 51 13.96 7.94 4.340(99)* Non-dependent 50 7.72 6.40 Total 101 10.87 7.84 STAI-S Dependent 51 37.58 9.02 1.639(99)* Non-dependent 50 34.68 8.80 Total 101 36.14 8.99 STAI-T Dependent 51 46.52 8.25 4.169(99)* Non-dependent 50 40.08 7.26 Total 101 43.33 8.39 CAGE Dependent 51 3.49 .78 27.32(99)* Non-dependent 50 .12 .35 Total 101 10.87 1.80 SCL-90-R Dependent 51 73.50 52.35 4.814(99)* Non-dependent 50 33.28 27.36 Total 101 53.59 46.41 * p < .001

Page 81: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

67

Table 4. Descriptive Statistics of Subscales of SCL-90-R Results Subscales of SCL-90-R

Alcohol Dependence

N Mean

SD t-test (df)

Somatization Dependent 51 8.45 7.81 3.657(99)** Non-dependent 50 3.76 4.65 Total 101 6.12 6.83 Obs.Com. Dependent 51 10.37 7.07 4.876(99)** Non-dependent 50 4.66 6.83 Total 101 7.54 6.52 Inter.Sens. Dependent 51 8.60 11.43 4.082(99)** Non-dependent 50 4.42 3.76 Total 101 6.53 5.54 Depression Dependent 51 11.84 9.30 3.926(99)** Non-dependent 50 5.92 5.26 Total 101 8.91 8.10 Anxiety Dependent 51 6.74 6.63 3.768(99)** Non-dependent 50 2.76 3.38 Total 101 4.77 5.62 Hostility Dependent 51 3.66 3.92 2.898(99)* Non-dependent 50 1.82 2.22 Total 101 2.75 3.31 Phobia Dependent 51 4.17 3.92 5.213(99)** Non-dependent 50 1.02 1.72 Total 101 2.61 3.41 Paranoia Dependent 51 5.81 4.71 3.075(99)* Non-dependent 50 3.46 2.90 Total 101 4.67 4.08 Psychotism Dependent 51 6.74 6.63 5.036(99)** Non-dependent 50 2.76 3.38 Total 101 4.77 5.62 ** p < .001, * p < .01 3. 3. Decoding Accuracy in General

Decoding accuracy was defined as the participants’ ability to infer the presented

emotion correctly on the monitor. An accurately identified expression received 1 score,

misidentified expression received 0 score. Each participant had his own total scores. To

investigate whether there was the group differences on accuracy scores, one way

ANOVA was run with group on total accuracy scores. The results indicated that

although the mean scores of non-dependent individuals were higher than alcohol

dependent individuals, they did not differ significantly from each other in terms of

Page 82: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

68

accurate recognition toward scores of all seven-expression scores. In other words, it was

found that alcohol dependent individuals did not perceive the expressions significantly

worse than non-dependent individuals. F (1, 99) = 3.34, p > .05 (ns.) (see Table 5 and

6).

Table 5.ANOVA Results of Recognition of Accuracy Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Between Gr. 161.33 1 161.33 3.34 Within Gr. 4789.83 99 48.38 Total 4951.16 100

.071

Table 6. Descriptive Statistics of Accuracy Scores toward all Emotional Expressions Alcohol Dependence N Mean SD Accuracy Scores dependents 51 32.39 7.41 non-dependents 50 34.92 6.45 Total 101 33.64 7.03

3. 3. 1. Decoding Accuracy of Each Facial Expression

In order to assess whether there was a group difference between alcohol

dependent individuals and control participants’ performances in their ability to decode

emotional facial expression, a between subject multivariate analysis of variance

(MANOVA), 4 (negative facial expressions: anger, sadness, disgust, fear) * 2 (group:

alcohol dependent individuals vs. control) was carried out. The results showed that

according to Wilks’ Lambda criterion, these 4 expressions were significantly affected by

group, F (4, 96) = 3.50, p < .05. Surprisingly, univariate analyses demonstrated that

alcohol dependent individuals and non-dependent individuals did not differ from each

other in the recognition of angry, fearful, and sad expressions. The only significant

difference was observed in the recognition of the expression of disgust. F(1, 99) =

10.424, p .< .05. As presented in Table 7, alcohol dependent individuals had lower

accuracy scores in the recognition of disgusted expressions than other expressions when

Page 83: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

69

compared to non-dependent individuals.

Accuracy scores for happy (a positive emotion), neutral and surprised (neither

positive nor negative emotions) were treated separately because of their different nature

from negative emotions; they were analyzed by univariate analysis of variance (One-Way

ANOVA). As expected, findings have suggested that no expression exhibited a

significant difference between groups (see Tables 7, 8, 9, 10, 11).

In sum, it was found that the performances of alcohol dependent inpatients were

similar to control group’s performances in the recognition of six expressions- happy, sad,

angry, surprised, and neutral faces. Surprisingly, the results displayed that true scores of

alcohol dependent individuals towards fearful faces were higher than the scores of control

group. However, this difference did not reach a statistical significance (see Table 7).

Table 7. Descriptive Statistics of Accuracy Scores towards Negative Emotional Expressions Facial Expressions

Alcohol Dependence

N Mean

S.D Univariate F (1,99)

Angry Dependents 51 4.68 1.66 n.s Non-dependent 50 4.90 1.63 Total 101 4.79 1.64 Fearful Dependents 51 3.00 1.57 n.s Non-dependent 50 2.72 1.77 Total 101 2.86 1.67 Disgusted Dependents 51 4.01 1.81 10.42*

Non-dependent 50 5.14 1.66 Total 101 4.57 1.82 Sad Dependents 51 4.23 1.97 n.s Non-dependent 50 4.72 1.75 101 4.47 1.87 * p < .05

Page 84: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

70

Table 8. Descriptive Statistics of Accuracy Scores towards the Expressions of Happiness, Surprise, and Neutral

EFE Alcohol Dependence

N Mean

S.D

Happiness Dependent 51 6.78 .576 Non-dependent 50 6.82 .481 Total 101 6.80 .529 Surprise Dependent 51 5.33 1.43 Non-dependent 50 5.68 1.30 Total 101 5.50 1.37 Neutral Dependent 51 4.33 2.46 Non-dependent 50 4.94 1.94 Total 101 4.63 2.30

Table 9.ANOVA Results of Happy Emotional Expressions Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Between Gr. .032 1 .032 .114 Within Gr. 28.07 99 .283 Total 28.04 100

.737

Table 10.ANOVA Results of Surprised Emotional Expressions Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Between Gr. 3.03 1 3.03 1.613 .217 Within Gr. 186.21 99 1.88 Total 189.25 100

Table 11. ANOVA Results of Neutral Expressions

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. GROUP 9.292 1 9.292 1.885 .173 Error 488.15 99 4.93 Total 2666.0 101

a R Squared = .019 (Adjusted R Squared = .009)

Page 85: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

71

3. 4. Manual Reaction/Recognition Times for Accurate Responses in General

Due to investigate whether there was a group difference on reaction times

toward all accurately recognized expressions, one way ANOVA was run with group

(dependents vs. non-dependent individuals) on total reaction time scores. Unexpectedly

statistical analysis did not manifest any significant differences between alcohol

dependent individuals and non-dependent individuals in terms of reaction times towards

all seven expressions. In other words, alcohol dependent individuals did not react

quicker or slower toward expressions than non-dependent individuals, F (1, 99) = 2.20,

p > .05 (ns.) (see Table 12).

Table 12. ANOVA results of Reaction Times towards all Expressions

Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. GROUP 9625663785 1 9625663785 2.203 .141 Error 432518541237 99 4368874153 Total 3210788526867 101

a R Squared = .022 (Adjusted R Squared = .012)

3. 4. 1. Reaction Times for Accurate Responses towards Each Facial Expression

In order to find out the group differences on the reaction times towards negative

expressions, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted on 4

negative expressions (anger, fear, disgust and sadness). The findings displayed that

alcohol dependent individuals did not perform significantly different from non-

dependent individuals in any of the negative expressions (see Table 13). According to

Wilks’ Lambda criterion, F (4, 96) = .872, p . > .05 (ns.).

Page 86: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

72

Table 13. Descriptive Statistics of Reaction Times towards Negative Expressions Reaction Times Alcohol

Dependence N Mean

(seconds) SD

RT Sadness Dependent 51 24.36 17.29 Non-dependent 50 27.33 20.04 Total 101 25.83 18.67 RT Fear Dependent 51 17.83 14.36 Non-dependent 50 18.09 15.55 Total 101 17.96 14.87 RT Anger Dependent 51 24.96 14.00 Non-dependent 50 30.87 22.45 Total 101 27.89 18.81 RT Disgust Dependent 51 21.74 10.85 Non-dependent 50 24.60 13.41 Total 101 23.16 12.21

For examining the reaction times towards happy, neutral, and surprised

expressions, each of facial expressions was calculated separately on univariate analysis

of variance (One way ANOVA). As expected, no significant results were found between

alcohol dependent individuals and non-dependent individuals in terms of the reaction

times toward happy, surprised, and neutral expressions. F(1, 99) = .319, p . > .05 (ns.);

F(1, 99) = .569, p . > .05 (ns.); F(1, 99) = .1.620, p . > .05 (ns.) respectively (see Tables

14, 15, 16, 17).

Table 14. ANOVA results of Reaction Times towards Happy Expressions Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Between Gr. 124930496.9 1 124930496.9 .319 Within Gr. 38736661107.3 99 391279405.1 Total 38861591604.3 100

.573

Page 87: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

73

Table 15. ANOVA results of Reaction Times towards Surprised Expressions Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. Between Gr. 74694218.6 1 74694218.6 .569 Within Gr. 13001339299.3 99 131326659.5 Total 13076033517.9 100

.453

Table 16. ANOVA results of Reaction Times towards Neutral Expressions Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig GROUP 324358034 1 324358034 1.620 .206 Error 00.853 99 200214387 Total 65110990639 101

a R Squared = .016 (Adjusted R Squared = .006)

Table 17. Descriptive Statistics of Reaction Times toward the Expressions of Happiness, Neutral and Surprise Reaction Times Alcohol

Dependence N Mean

(seconds) SD

RT Happiness Dependent 51 22.74 9.18 Non-dependent 50 24.91 26.54 Total 101 23.84 19.71 RT Neutral Dependent 51 19.32 13.41 Non-dependent 50 22.90 14.86 Total 101 21.09 14.19 RT Surprise Dependent 51 24.91 11.43 Non-dependent 50 26.63 11.48 Total 101 25.76 11.44

3. 5. Covariates with Accuracy Decoding in General

In order to examine whether there was a group difference on accuracy scores

when depression, state-trait anxiety and general symptom distress levels were taken into

the account, one-way variance analyses with covariates between group design

(ANCOVA) was conducted with group (dependents vs. non-dependent individuals) and

covariates as BDI, STAI and SCL on total accuracy scores. The findings yielded that no

significant difference was observed. F (1, 95) = 1.50, p > .05 (ns.) (see Table 18).

Page 88: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

74

Covariates did not cause any significant differences on the recognition of all facial

expression scores.

Table 18. ANCOVA Results of Accuracy Decoding Measures Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. SCL-90-R 38.65 1 38.65 .789 .377 SAI 31.65 1 31.65 .646 .424 TAI 55.62 1 55.62 1.135 .289 BDI 35.63 1 35.63 .727 .396 GROUP 73.350 1 73.350 1.497 .224 Error 4653.3 95 48.98

a R Squared = .060 (Adjusted R Squared = .011)

3. 5. 1. Covariates with Decoding Accuracy in Each Facial Expression

Group differences for the accuracy of 4 negative emotions: fear, disgust, anger,

sadness were examined by taking the scores Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), State-

Trait Anxiety (STAI), and Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R) as the covariates. Results of

MANCOVA revealed significant group differences. On the basis of Wilks’ Lambda

criterion F (4,92) =5.35, p.< .001. As presented in Table 19, however, analyses indicated

that alcohol dependent group had lower accuracy scores than non-dependent group only

for the recognition of disgusted expression, F (1,95) = 12.32, p.< .01.

The separate analyses of one way with covariate design (ANCOVA) for the

recognition of happy, surprised, and neutral expressions have exhibited that groups did

not differ from each other when trait and state anxiety, depressive symptomatology and

general psychopathological distress were considered as covariates. In other words,

accuracy of other emotions did not indicate a significant group differences, F values

respectively; F (1,95) =.161, p. > .05 (ns.); F (1,95) = .828, p. > .05 (ns.); F (1,95) =

7.69, p. > .05 (ns.).

Page 89: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

75

Table 19. MANCOVA with Decoding Accuracy in Each Facial Expression

Measures Dependent V. Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig. SAI

SAD 3.517 1 3.517 .988 323 FEAR 3.947 1 3.947 1.419 236

ANGER .099 1 .099 .037 .849 DISGUST .084 1 .084 .028 .867 TAI SAD .567 1 .567 .159 .691 FEAR 2.416 1 2.416 .869 .354 ANGER 10.728 1 10.728 3.994 .049

DISGUST 4.448 1 4.448 1.477 .227 BDI SAD .808 1 .808 .227 .635 FEAR 3.714 1 3.714 1.335 .251 ANGER .009 1 .009 .003 .955 DISGUST 2.586 1 2.586 .859 .356 SCL-90-R SAD .755 1 .755 .21 .646 FEAR .037 1 .03 .013 .909 ANGER 2.281 1 2.281 .849 .359 DISGUST 4.378 1 4.378 1.454 .231 GROUP SAD 4.643 1 4.643 1.305 .256 FEAR 8.105 1 8.105 2.914 .091

ANGER .013 1 .013 .005 .945 DISGUST 37.108 1 37.108 12.322 .001*

Error SAD 338.075 95 3.559 FEAR 264.218 95 2.781 ANGER 255.145 95 2.686 DISGUST 286.096 95 3.012 Total SAD 2374.000 101 FEAR 1107.000 101 ANGER 2590.000 101 DISGUST 2446.000 101

* p < .01

Page 90: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

76

3. 6. Covariates with Manual Reaction Times for Accurate Responses

In order to find out any group difference on general reaction times toward

accurately recognized expressions by taking the scores of Beck Depression Inventory

(BDI), State-Trait Anxiety (STAI), and Symptom Checklist (SCL-90-R) as the

covariates, one-way univariate analysis with covariates (ANCOVA) was conducted.

However, this analysis did not reveal any significant findings, F (1,95) = 1.905, p. > .05

(ns.)

3. 6. 1. Covariates with Reaction Times for Accurate Responses towards Each

Facial Expression

Due to prove the effect of state-trait anxiety, depression level, and general

psychopathology scores on reaction times toward each expression, firstly a multivariate

analysis with covariances (MANCOVA) was conducted. Multivariate analysis of 2

(group: dependents vs. controls) * 4 (negative emotions: fear, disgust, anger, sadness)

between group design with covariates as STAI, BDI and SCL-90-R was carried out. As

illustrated in Table 20, the results manifested that alcohol dependent group differed

significantly from non-dependent group only in the recognition time of disgusted faces,

Wilks’ Lambda criterion F (1,95) = 4.289, p. < .05. In other words, alcohol dependent

group recognized disgusted expression significantly faster than non-dependent

individuals.

As predicted, the results of separate univariate analyses with the same covariates

(ANCOVA) for the recognition time of happy, surprised and neutral expressions yielded

that alcohol dependent group did not differ significantly from non-dependent group, F

(1,95) = .541, p. > .05 (ns.); F (1,95) = .925, p. > .05 (ns.); F (1,95) = 1.555, p. > .05

(ns.) respectively.

Page 91: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

77

Table 20. MANCOVA with Reaction Times for Accurate Responses towards Each Negative Facial Expressions

Measures DV Sum of Squares df Mean Square F Sig.

State Anxiety

RT Sad 29597151.1 1 29597151.1 .084 .773

RT Fear 54020495.1 1 54020495.1 .237 .628 RT Anger 483183705.9 1 483183705.9 1.378 .243 RT Disgust 230652923.3 1 230652923.3 1.571 .213 Trait Anxiety

RT Sad 835960005.4 1 835960005.4 2.367 .127

RT Fear 236971463.4 1 236971463.4 1.038 .311 RT Anger 135484868.6 1 135484868.6 .386 .536 RT Disgust 8046209.2 1 8046209.2 .055 .815 BDI RT Sad 374122208.0 1 374122208.0 1.059 .306 RT Fear 27168709.6 1 27168709.6 .119 .731 RT Anger 19168517.3 1 19168517.3 .055 .816 RT Disgust 265481125.0 1 265481125.0 1.809 .182 SCL-90-R RT Sad 20129804.5 1 20129804.5 .057 .812 RT Fear 2712591.0 1 2712591.0 .012 .913 RT Anger 4105242.5 1 4105242.5 .012 .914 RT Disgust 61053171.0 1 61053171.0 .416 .521 GROUP RT Sad 199821735.8 1 199821735.8 .566 .454 RT Fear 48366281.9 1 48366281.9 .212 .646 RT Anger 315380593.0 1 315380593.0 .899 .345 RTDisgust 629605221.2 1 629605221.2 4.289 041* ERROR RT Sad 33553524302.1 95 353194992.6 RT Fear 21679128275.5 95 228201350.2 RT Anger 33318829809.9 95 350724524.3 RT Disgust 13944454432.6 95 146783730.8 * p <.05. a. R Squared = .038 (Adjusted R Squared = -.013 b. R Squared = .020 (Adjusted R Squared = -.031) c. R Squared = .058 (Adjusted R Squared = .009. d. R Squared = .065 (Adjusted R Squared = .016)

Page 92: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

78

3. 7. Misinterpretations of Emotional Facial Expressions

In our study, we aimed to find out a group difference in the recognition of facial

expressions between alcohol dependent and non-dependent group. Namely, we expected

that alcohol dependent group would have less accuracy scores than non-dependent

group. However, our results failed to reveal an accuracy deficit in the identification of

happy, sad, fearful, disgusted, surprised, and neutral expressions except disgusted ones

in alcohol dependent group. Therefore, we decided to concentrate on the number of

inaccurate responses of both groups in order to find out the existence of common

misinterpretations of facial expressions (see Table 21). The total scores of accurate

responses were higher than the scores of inaccurate responses of both groups. Moreover,

general accurate scores of non-dependent individuals ranged from 39% (fear) to 88%

(happy).

As expected, the findings have suggested that both alcohol dependent and non-

dependent groups recognized happy faces more accurately than other expressions at 87%

and 88% respectively. Additionally, it was obtained that two groups tended to display

same misjudgments toward fearful, angry, surprised, and neutral faces except sad and

disgusted faces.

Firstly, the analyses demonstrated that there was a significant difference between

alcohol dependent group and non-dependent group in the recognition of disgusted

expressions. Alcohol dependent individuals recognized disgusted faces at the ratio of

58%. The rest, 42%, inaccurately responded to the disgusted faces, and 46% of this

percentage (42%) perceived the expression of disgust as anger. On the other hand, in the

group of non-dependent individuals, the ratio of properly understanding was 74%.

Within the incorrect responses (26%), 36% of them failed to perceive disgusted

expression and they misunderstood them as surprised ones. This difference may be

explained by the effect of alcohol abuse.

In terms of sad expressions, 68% of control group responded accurately. When

we consider their inaccuracy scores, it was found that non-dependent group perceived

sadness as neutral in the rate of 36%. However, differently from non-dependent

Page 93: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

79

individuals, 60% of alcohol dependent individuals decoded sadness correctly and they

tended to misjudge sad faces as surprised faces with the percentage of 35%.

The results exhibited that 70% of non-dependent group evaluated angry faces

properly, whereas this ratio declined to 67% for alcohol dependent group. When we

focused on their incorrect responses, it was obtained that 33% of alcohol dependent

individuals and 32% non-dependent individuals misevaluated angry faces as surprised

faces.

Similarly, it was observed that 69% of non-dependent individuals judged neutral

faces exactly however this rate decreased to 62% for alcohol dependent individuals. In

addition, both groups misinterpreted neutral faces as sad faces. Their inaccurate response

rates were 34% in the group of non-dependent individuals and 36% in the group of

alcohol dependent individuals.

Interestingly, the findings postulated that non-dependent individuals recognized

fearful faces with the minimum scores when compared to all emotional faces. Their

percentage of correct responses was only 39%. This ratio was higher for alcohol

dependent individuals as 42%. When we consider their misjudgments, it was revealed

that both groups tended to misinterpret fearful faces as surprised faces. The inaccurate

response rates were 65% in non-dependent individuals and 63% in alcohol dependent

individuals. In terms of surprised expression, control group seemed to get the highest

score except happy with the ratio of 82%. Just like non-dependent individuals, the

accuracy scores of alcohol dependent group were also higher. Their percentage of

accuracy scores was 77%. Moreover, two groups’ error rates were the same. In both

groups, 51% of inaccurate responses exhibited that alcohol dependent and non-

dependent individuals tended to perceive surprised faces as fearful faces.

These shared misinterpretations of facial expressions in alcohol dependent and

non-dependent groups could be associated with cultural factors. These findings would be

discussed in the next chapter.

Page 94: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

80

Table 21. Descriptive Statistics of Accurate Responses of All Participants towards All Emotional Expressions

EFE Alcohol

Dependence N % Mean SD Min. Max.

Happiness Dependent 51 87 6.78 .576 4 7 Non-dependent 50 88 6.82 .418 5 7 Total 101 6.80 .052 Sadness Dependent 51 60 4.23 1.75 0 7 Non-dependent 50 68 4.72 1.97 1 7 Total 101 4.47 1.82 Fear Dependent 51 42 3.00 1.77 0 5 Non-dependent 50 39 2.72 1.57 0 7 Total 101 2.86 1.67 Anger Dependent 51 67 4.68 1.66 1 7 Non-dependent 50 70 4.90 1.63 1 7 Total 101 4.79 1.64 Surprise Dependent 51 77 5.33 1.43 0 7 Non-dependent 50 82 5.68 1.30 2 7 Total 101 5.50 1.37 Disgust Dependent 51 58 4.01 1.81 0 7 Non-dependent 50 74 5.14 1.66 0 7 Total 101 4.57 1.82 Neutral Dependent 51 62 4.33 2.46 0 7 Non-dependent 50 69 4.94 1.94 0 7 Total 101 4.57 2.23

Page 95: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

81

3. 8. Stepwise Regression Analysis

In order to estimate whether alcohol dependence on the basis of CAGE scores

would be predicted by demographical variables such as age and education; by

psychological symptoms scores obtained from Beck Depression Inventory, State-Trait

Anxiety Inventory, and Symptom Checlikst and its nine subscales-anxiety, depression,

phobic anxiety, hostility, interpersonal sensitivity, obsessive-compulsion, somatization,

psychotizm, and paranoid ideation; by recognition scores from each seven emotional

facial expressions; and lastly by reaction time scores toward accurately recognized facial

expressions, a stepwise multiple regression analysis was run. CAGE scores were entered

as the Dependent Variable. The results of stepwise multiple regression analyses were

presented in Table 24, including multiple R (R), R square (R2), adjusted R2 (∆R2), the

standardized regression coefficients-Beta (β), semipartial correlations (sp²) and t values.

In the first step, age and educational levels of both groups were submitted;

however, they did not predict a significant degree of variance in the alcohol dependence.

In the next step, BDI, STAI, and SCL-90-R scores were entered and the results revealed

that only obsessive-compulsion, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), State-Trait Anxiety

Inventory State Form (STAI-S) scores predicted alcohol dependence significantly, R2=

.22, F (1,99) = 27.716, p. < .001; R2= .25, F (2,98) = 16.555, p. < .001; R2= .28, F (3,97)

= 12.765, p. < .001 respectively. Findings indicated that %22 of variance in alcohol

dependence or CAGE scores was explained by obsessive-compulsion characteristics of

participants; %25 of variance in alcohol dependence was explained by depression scores;

%28 of variance in alcohol dependence was accounted for state anxiety scores of groups.

In the third step, accuracy scores from seven emotional facial expressions were submitted

due to predict alcohol dependence. The results suggested that accurate scores in the

recognition of disgusted and fearful faces predicted alcohol dependence significantly,

R2= .33, F (4,96) = 11.998, p. < .00; R2= .37, F (5,95) = 11.247, p. < .001 respectively

(see Table 25). In the last step, reaction time scores were submitted whether they would

predict alcohol dependence, nevertheless, the findings did not demonstrate an association

between reaction times and CAGE scores.

In sum, stepwise regression analyses indicated that obsessive-compulsive subscale

Page 96: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

82

in other words, distress of unwanted and uncontrollable thoughts and behaviors

(compulsions), depression level obtained from Beck Depression Inventory, and

recognition of fearful facial expressions predicted positively the existence of alcoholism.

Nevertheless, recognition accuracy scores of disgusted facial expression and scores from

State Anxiety Inventory predicted negatively the presence of alcohol dependence on the

basis of CAGE.

Table 22. Variables in each step for Stepwise Multiple Regressions using Demographics, BDI, STAI, SCL-90, Facial Expressions and Reaction Times to predict Alcoholism Level Variables

Step I Age Education Step II STAI BDI 9 subscales of SCL-90-R Step III 7 Facial Expressions Step IV Reaction Times to 7 Facial Expressions Dependent Variable Scores on CAGE Alcoholism Inventory

Page 97: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

83

Table 23. Summary of Stepwise Multiple Regressions using demographics, BDI, STAI, SCL-90, Recognition of Facial Expressions due to predict alcohol dependence based on CAGE Model R R² ∆R²

(Adjusted) Β (Beta)

sp² (semi-partial)

t

1. Obs-Com. .468 .22 .21 .47 .47 5.25***

2. Obs.Com., BDI

.503 .25 .23 .33 .23

.30 .21

3.058** 2.106*

3. Obs. Com., BDI, State Anxiety

.532 .28 .26 .38 .34 -.23

.33 .28 -.20

3.465** 2.841** -.2032*

4. Obs. Com., BDI, State Anxiety, Disgust

.577 .33 .31 .34 .35 -.20 -.23

.31 .30 -.19 -.27

3.187**

3.020**

-1.856 -2.690**

5. Obs. Com., BDI, State Anxiety, Disgust, Fear

.610 .37 .33 .34 .35 -.20 -.27 .20

.32 .33 -.21 -.32 .24

3.187**

3.020** -1.856

-3.243**

2.415*

* p < .05., ** p < .01, *** p < .001.

Page 98: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

84

CHAPTER IV

DISCUSSION

4. 1. Alcohol Dependence and Co-morbid Disorders

One of the purposes in our study was to measure the levels of depression, state-

trait anxiety, the existence of alcoholism, and the presence of general

psychopathological distress of both groups (nine subscales -anxiety, depression, phobic

anxiety, hostility, interpersonal sensitivity, obsessive-compulsion, somatization,

psychotizm, and paranoid ideation). Most of the studies up to now have shown that

anxiety disorders and depression are associated with high rates of alcohol dependence

both in nonclinical and clinical populations even though the exact nature of these

associations remains unclear (e.g. Gratzer et al., 2004; Ceylan & Turkcan, 2003, p.59-

63; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2004; Hoes, 1997; Merikangas et al., 1998; Wetterling and

Junghanns, 2000; Preisig et al., 2001; Devanand, 2002; Tedstone & Coyle, 2004; Terra

et al., 2006). It was observed that in the alcohol dependence population, the probability

of developing of a unipolar mood disorder is four times more than healthy population

(Ceylan, Turkcan, 2003, p.63). In addition, Ceylan, Turkcan suggested that alcohol

dependence is accompanied with mostly agoraphobia, social anxiety and PTSD among

all anxiety disorders (2003, p. 63). A prevalence of 30.6% was found for specific phobia,

24.7% for social phobia, 22.2% for anxiety disorder induced by alcohol, 19.3% for

generalized anxiety disorder, 5% for obsessive-compulsive disorder, 4.6% for

posttraumatic stress disorder, and 2% for panic disorder with agoraphobia (Terra et al.,

2006). Merikangas et al. (1998) who investigated co-morbidity between specific

subtypes of anxiety and alcoholism postulated that the association was greater for phobic

disorders than for panic and generalized anxiety disorder. In the study of Gratzer et al.

(2004), their findings exhibited that a significantly higher rate of alcoholism was linked

to depression-anxiety rather than only anxiety.

Page 99: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

85

In accordance with previous studies, the results of this study yielded that alcohol

dependent group scored higher than normal control group on the CAGE Alcohol

Inventory, Beck Depression Inventory, Trait Anxiety Inventory, and Symptom Checklist

(SCL-90-R) and on its nine subscales. However, no significant difference was found in

State Anxiety Inventory between our experimental and control group. Thus, as expected,

alcohol dependent individuals were found as more depressed, had higher scores of

psychopathological distress in terms of nine measures, and had higher trait anxiety than

non-dependent individuals.

4. 2. Decoding Accuracy of Emotional Facial Expressions

In our study, it was predicted that alcohol dependent individuals would have

cognitive impairments in the recognition of universal Emotional Facial Expression

(EFE) -happy, sad, angry, disgusted, fearful, surprised, and neutral expressions.

However, the results indicated that there was no significant difference between alcohol

dependent individuals and non-dependent individuals in the total accurate recognition

scores toward seven expressions-happiness, sadness, fear, anger, disgust, surprise, and

neutral. Even though alcohol dependent individuals did more errors in the total accuracy

scores than non-dependent individuals did, this difference did not reach a statistical

significance. On the other hand, their pattern of answers to emotional expressions differs

from each other. The findings revealed that alcohol dependent individuals and non-

dependent individuals did not differ from each other in their recognition of happiness,

sadness, fear, anger, surprise, and neutral except disgusted faces. In other words, alcohol

dependent individuals did more errors in their recognition of disgusted expression

significantly, than control participants did. Additionally, the findings also suggested that

alcohol dependent individuals perceived disgusted faces as angry faces. Almost half of

the inaccurate responses showed that they misinterpreted the expression of disgust as

angry.

Interestingly, even though there was not any significant difference between two

groups in terms of the number of accurate responses, alcohol dependent individuals were

found as better in the recognition of fearful expression than non-dependent individuals.

Page 100: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

86

Non-dependent individuals did more errors than alcohol dependent individuals in the

decoding of fearful faces. These inaccurate response tendencies will be discussed in Part

4.6.

In sum, although our study failed to manifest total accuracy deficit between

experimental and normal control groups, some of the results were in line with previous

studies showing a deficit in the recognition of emotional expression in alcohol

dependent individuals with a particular bias in disgust and their misinterpretation of

disgusted expression as angry. The findings of Philippot et al. (1999) exhibited that

recognition of facial expressions seemed to be severely impaired in recovering

alcoholics. The authors suggested that particularly alcohol dependent individuals had a

bias in their recognition of angry and contempted faces. In other words, it was found

that alcohol dependent participants had a systematic bias in interpreting faces

expressing disgust as anger or contempt similar to our findings.

Furthermore, Townsend and Duka (2003) postulated that alcohol dependent

inpatients tended to perceive angry faces when they were shown to disgusted faces. This

result suggests that alcohol dependent individuals may have a difficulty in

distinguishing these two emotional expressions. It was also found that alcohol

dependent individuals also overestimated the pictures of disgust when compared to

angry ones. Townshend and Duka (2003) were not able to clarify this finding; at last,

they interpreted this confusion as the damage of long-term alcohol abuse. They

concluded that in the processing of anger and disgust, orbitofrontal cortex plays a

remarkable role. These authors suggested that there might have been a reduction in the

functioning of these critical areas. However, even though alcohol has deleterious effects

on brain functioning, there has not been any distinct explanation yet for the impairment

of facial expressions. Nevertheless, our findings highlight that alcohol dependent

individuals seem to be more sensitive to the feelings of threat on the faces looking at

them and they are more likely to interpret those facial expressions as hostile rather than

disgusted or angry.

Page 101: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

87

4. 3. The Effects of Depression, State-Trait Anxiety, and Psychopathological

Symptoms on the Decoding Accuracy of Emotional Facial Expressions

In particular, previous studies have indicated that there is a general tendency to

find emotion-congruent effects when people interpret the ambiguity. That is, if an

individual experiences a negative emotion, s/he is more likely to adopt the negative

interpretation of an ambiguous stimulus than other individuals. For instance, anxiety is

known to be associated with biases towards threat related information and with a bias

towards the threatening interpretation of ambiguous stimuli. In other words, it was

observed that anxious individuals selectively perceived threatening information such as

misunderstanding of negative emotions and depressed individuals having a bias for

information related to sad expression. Based on this assumption, in this study, we

expected alcohol dependent individuals to display both attentional and evaluative biases

towards negative emotional expressions because of their higher levels of depression and

higher scores on trait anxiety. Also, by taking these symptoms as covariates, we

attempted to take out their roles in the information processing system. However, our

findings yielded that when state-trait anxiety level, depression level and nine

psychopathological symptoms were taken as covariates, alcohol dependent individuals

had impairment only in the recognition of the expression of disgust. This finding can be

interpreted by explaining the nature of the emotion of disgust.

Disgust is one of the basic emotions, which is characterized by a distinctive

facial expression, specific cognitive, physiological, and behavioral components. It has an

evolutionary dimension and it motivates behavior leading to an avoidance of infection

(Rubio-Godoy et al., 2007). It was observed that even in primates, bitter stimuli elicit

disgusted expression like as three-day-old baby displays disgusted when a stinky odor

was added to breast milk (Erickson and Schulkin, 2003). As can be seen, emotional

expressions serve a communicative role that human beings and animals use in order to

survive. By means of the emotional expression of disgust, animals learn to give aversive

responses from a poisonous food. Therefore, a communicative and social function of

emotional expressions causes animals to learn what is safe to eat by observing others’

reactions to food. As far as it is known, chronic alcohol consumption leads to brain

Page 102: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

88

dysfunction that makes individuals suffer from learning and especially spatial memory

impairments (Chelune and Parker, 1981; Obernier et al., 2002). It was found that in the

limbic system, septal area takes a major role in emotionally significant learning, such as

avoiding aversive experiences. Similar to the interpretations of Townshend and Duka,

perhaps the toxic effect of alcohol may decrease the related functioning of brain.

One possible explanation about the confusion of disgusted expressions with

angry ones may include the morphological characteristics of two expressions rather than

decreased functioning of brain areas. In terms of appearance characteristics, anger is the

emotion most often confused with disgust (Ekman, 2003, p.184). Both have the lowered

eyebrows. Also, anger is usually masked except anger-disgust expression. However,

while looking at closely, there are much more differences than similarities. For instance;

eyebrows are not drawn together, upper eyelids are not raised, eyelid muscles are not

tense in disgusted faces. Upper lip is raised in disgusted faces like u shape however, in

angry expression, lips are pressed together (narrowing the lips).

Furthermore, there has been a growing interest in the role of disgust in

psychopathology especially among anxiety disorders, such as Obsessive-Compulsive

Disorder-fear of contamination and Specific Phobias, even in Eating Disorders-food

rejection (Davey et al., 2006; Rossignol et al., 2007).

Our research has failed to show the effect of state-trait anxiety on the

identification of all emotional expressions. Our results are not consistent with those

already reported in the literature that especially social anxious people show evaluative or

attentional bias towards the processing of social threat. Because angry expression is

universally assumed as the salient signal of threat and danger, numerous findings

revealed that sub-clinically or clinically anxious individuals misinterpret angry

expression (Surcinelli et al., 2006; Ioannou et al., 2004; Rossignol et al., 2007).

However, there are also contrary findings in the literature. For instance, Mohlman et al.

(2007) found that anxious individuals recognized the expression of anger more easily

and accurately than other expressions. On the other hand, Philippot and Douilliez (2005)

demonstrated that individuals with generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, panic

disorder with agoraphobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder did not exhibit any attentional

Page 103: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

89

or evaluative biases in the process of threatening facial expressions. When we consider

the previous studies, it is difficult to draw clear-cut conclusions about evaluative or

attentional bias in anxiety.

Similarly, we hypothesized that when Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) scores

were accepted as covariates, alcohol dependent group would show more evaluative bias

(misinterpretation) especially toward negative expressions when compared to normal

control group. However, we did not find any significant differences between our two

groups. In this respect, our study did not confirm the existence of perceptual bias in

depression. Indeed, so far there have been discrepant findings of the previous studies

about the presence of impairments in the decoding of facial expressions. Some studies

revealed that depressed patients exhibited a general deficit in recognizing negative

emotional expressions mainly toward sadness, fear, and anger (Surguladze et al.,

Mendlewicz et al., 2005). However, similar to our findings, Weniger et al. (2004) found

that depressive individuals were able to recognize all of the facial expressions as

correctly as normal individuals were.

In conclusion, more or less our findings add to a growing body of evidence

demonstrating an important effect of emotion on different aspects of cognitive

processing.

4. 4. Reaction Times for Accurate Responses towards Emotional Facial Expressions

One of our purposes was to examine the quickness in their recognition of all

seven expressions in alcohol dependent individuals and healthy control individuals.

Contrary to our hypothesis, the results did not manifest any significant differences

between alcohol dependent individuals and non-dependent individuals in terms of total

reaction time scores toward all seven expressions. This result is not in line with previous

studies (e.g. Tedstone & Coyle, 2004; Foisy, 2007). This discrepancy between the

present results and those of former studies may be explained by some methodological

limitations.

Page 104: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

90

4. 5. The Effects of Depression, State-Trait Anxiety, and Psychopathological

Symptoms on the Reaction Times for Accurate Responses towards Emotional

Facial Expressions

It has been well established that clinically or non-clinically anxious individuals

display an attentional bias toward the stimuli that they perceived to be threatening.

Therefore, we expected them to react more rapidly than normal control group. Similarly,

depression is counted as one of the disorders that affect the individuals’ positive

perception. Depressed individuals’ negative points of view toward self, world, and

future make them recognize expressions in a more negative manner, which might cause

an attentional bias especially toward sadness. Based on these perspectives, we

hypothesized that alcohol dependent individuals would perceive negative emotional

expressions, anger, fear, sadness, and disgust more rapidly than non-dependent

individuals. Nevertheless, the results demonstrated that alcohol dependent group

recognized only disgust facial expression significantly faster than non-dependent

individuals did. Additionally, findings indicated that BDI, STAI, and SCL-90-R did not

affect reaction time scores toward happiness, surprise, and neutral facial expressions.

These results are somehow inconsistent with prior findings. There is a

considerable amount of evidence have shown that especially clinically anxious

individuals, particularly individuals with social phobia and generalized anxiety disorders

were hyper-vigilant to threatening stimuli (e.g. Mansell et al., 2002; Mohlman, et al.,

2007; Mogg et al., 2000). In our study when state and trait anxiety levels were taken

under control, two groups differed in the reaction times only towards disgusted faces.

Alcohol dependent individuals perceived disgusted faces more quickly than non-

dependent individuals when their depression, anxiety levels were controlled.

The rapid recognition of disgusted faces of alcohol dependent inpatients may be

related to their high exposure of disgusted faces from the faces looking at them rather

than an existence of organic deficits. It is well documented that familiar faces are more

accurately recognized than non-familiar ones (e.g. Gallegos & Tranel, 2005; Elfenbein

& Ambady, 2003). Therefore, we may assume that familiar disgusted faces are more

recognizable than other faces so that they are reacted faster than others. Also, one of our

Page 105: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

91

findings concluded that disgusted faces were confused with angry ones by alcohol

dependent inpatients. Specifically, alcohol dependent inpatients reacted faster towards

disgusted expressions which were perceived as angry because of their high familiarity

and high exposure levels of disgusted faces from the faces looking at them. This

misperception may cause such interpersonal conflicts and also social isolation. This

inference should be clarified by further research.

4. 6. Misinterpretations of Emotional Facial Expressions

In the current study, it was observed that both alcohol dependent individuals and

non-dependent individuals tended to respond all expressions almost similarly.

Specifically, only the group difference was obtained in the recognition of disgusted

faces. Then, we decided to find out their misinterpretation of responses towards all facial

expressions. Even though the total scores of inaccurate responses were less than accurate

ones for both groups, unfortunately accurate scores of control group were not high as

expected.

Results indicated that alcohol dependent individuals and non-dependent

individuals displayed similar misjudgments toward fearful, angry, surprised, and neutral

expressions except sad and disgusted. Happy expressions were not considered in this

context because of their obtaining of highest recognition scores for both groups. In

addition, the causes of misinterpretation of disgusted expression in alcohol dependent

individuals had already been discussed. Interestingly, it was observed that non-

dependent individuals perceived disgusted faces as surprised faces.

Differently from other emotional expressions, it was found that non-dependent

individuals perceived sad as neutral expression. On the other hand, alcohol dependent

individuals tended to misjudge sad faces as surprised faces.

Generally, the findings suggested that alcohol dependent individuals and non-

dependent individuals displayed similar tendencies toward neutral faces. Both groups

misjudged neutral faces as sad faces. Likewise, both groups failed to interpret angry

expression correctly and they assumed them as surprised expression.

Page 106: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

92

Additionally, the most confusion between emotional expressions was observed in

the responses of surprised and fearful expressions for both groups. Both groups

misinterpreted fearful faces as surprised faces. The lowest scores belonged to fear

expressions for both groups. They exhibited a tendency to perceive surprised faces as

fearful faces. However, this error rate was less than the errors of fear expressions. In

other words, all participants were better in reading of surprised faces rather than fearful

faces.

The misinterpretations of facial expressions of both groups and unexpected poor

performance of non-dependent individuals may be explained by cultural differences.

Recently, substantial cross-cultural studies have pointed out the role of culture in the

identification of emotional expressions. Previous studies in which Japanese and

Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion (JACFEE) photo set were used showed that

especially Japanese individuals were different from Americans in terms of recognition of

fearful faces. These studies exhibited that Japanese individuals mistook fearful faces for

surprised ones same as our findings (Russell et al., 1993; Shioiri et al., 1999). Komaki et

al. (2006) found that Japanese participants were not able to discriminate fearful faces

properly from surprised faces. Additionally these individuals could not perceive fearful

faces as a threatening stimulus. On the other hand, non-Japanese individuals easily

recognized fearful expressions. In conclusion, Komaki et al. (2006) discussed their

findings and explained their doubts about the universality of facial expressions.

Likewise, Shioiri et al. (1999) postulated that Japanese participants had lower scores

than American participants in the recognition of all universally accepted expressions that

are anger, contempt, disgust, fear, happiness, sadness, and surprise. In addition, they did

more errors in the expressions of anger, disgust, fear, and sadness. Surprised and happy

are the easiest expressions to recognize similar to our findings. Authors explained the

distinctions that Japanese people tend to mask their negative emotional expressions

when the presence of an authority figures than Americans do. In this context, Japanese

and Turkish cultures may share common characteristics such as collectivism. Collective

cultures emphasize group cohesion, solidarity, and harmony unlike individualistic

cultures, which foster autonomy, separateness, and individuality (Matsumoto et al.,

Page 107: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

93

2002). People in individualistic cultures are more likely to show their emotions and they

feel more freely to express their emotions than people of collectivistic cultures. This

may help to explain the recognition differences between American and Japanese people

who display similarities with our culture.

On the other hand, the commonality of alcohol dependent individuals and non-

dependent individuals may be explained by familiarity issue. As it is known, people

have more positive beliefs and attribute more positive traits toward the members of their

own group. This in-group bias effect or in-group advantage or in other words, ethnic

bias can be responsible for these discrepant results. Most of the studies about culture-

specific recognition of facial expressions have proved that participants are more accurate

in the judgment of emotional expressions in which they have greater familiarity

(Elfenbein & Ambady, 2003; Beaupre & Hess, 2003). Gallegos & Tranel (2005)

demonstrated that personally familiar faces were recognized both accurately and faster

even they were neutral than other faces. In this respect, unfamiliar facial expressions that

were used in this study might have affected the accuracy of interpretations.

Furthermore, these observed culture specific rules of decoding may be resulted

from morphological characteristics of pose of faces. Namely, disgusted may be more

recognizable expression than others because of its salient nose wrinkle pose. However,

fearful and surprised expressions may be more difficult to categorize because of their

shared pose characteristics of a face. According to Gosselin and Larocque (2000) the

reason of misinterpretations are that, the more similarities one emotion shares with

another, the more likely to be perceived inaccurately and to be confused with each other.

This explanation can be conceivable in order to explain the confusion of fearful and

surprised faces, such as raising the eyebrows together; however, this approach does not

explain the confusion of sad and neutral expressions.

One of the interesting findings in our study was the confusion of the facial

expressions of neutral and sadness. Specifically, both alcohol dependent and non-

dependent healthy group misidentified neutral faces as sad faces. One possible

explanation of this finding includes the negative perceptual bias of depression.

Cognitive theories of depression proposed that depressed individuals tend to interpret

Page 108: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

94

ambiguous information in a negative manner. Hence, these patients are usually expected

to judge facial stimuli more negative than do controls. Mogg et al. (2006) suggested that

depressed patients made more negative interpretations and they showed more negative

recall than non-depressed patients did. Similarly, Leppanen et al. (2004) found that

depressed individuals attributed neutral faces to sad faces and recognized neutral

expressions both more slowly and less accurately than healthy participants did.

However, even though we obtained that, alcohol dependent individuals scored higher in

Beck Depression Scale, non-dependent individuals were not counted as depressed. Then

we interpret this common point as also cultural. An alternative explanation for this

outcome can be found in the literature about socio-economic status of participants.

Herba & Phillips (2004) exhibited that in their study, which preschool children were

used; deprived socio-economic groups were significantly at more risk for emotional

difficulties. Their findings indicated that socio-economically disadvantaged children

were more accurate on fearful expression. The authors suggested that exposure of high

stress living conditions may cause an improvement of a bias which has a survival value.

Perhaps, high stress, low income, low life standards, and decreased expectations might

have caused our sample to attribute neutral stimuli to sadness. Future studies should

take into account such factors.

4.7. The Roles of BDI, STAI-S, SCL-90, Recognition of Disgust and Fear Emotional

Expressions in the Prediction of Alcoholism

Eventhough, we do not have enough number of participants, we examined

whether alcohol dependence based on CAGE scores would be predicted by

demographical variables such as age and education; by psychological symptoms scores

obtained from Beck Depression Inventory, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, and Symptom

Checlikst (SCL-90) and its nine subscales-anxiety, depression, phobic anxiety, hostility,

interpersonal sensitivity, obsessive-compulsion, somatization, psychotizm, and paranoid

ideation; by recognition scores from each seven emotional facial expressions; and by

reaction time scores toward accurately recognized facial expressions. As a result

stepwise regression analyses, whereas obsessive-compulsive subscale of SCL-90,

Page 109: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

95

depression level obtained from Beck Depression Inventory, and recognition accuracy

scores of fearful facial expressions positively associated with the level of alcohol

consumption, recognition accuracy scores of disgusted facial expression and scores from

STAI- State Anxiety Form negatively associated with the presence of alcohol

dependence.

These findings are in agreement with previous studies. Several studies have

reported that individuals who are dependent on psychoactive substances have a higher

prevalence of anxiety disorders (e.g. Gratzer et al., 2004; Ceylan & Turkcan, 2003,

p.59-63; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2004; Hoes, 1997; Merikangas et al., 1998; Wetterling and

Junghanns, 2000; Preisig et al., 2001; Devanand, 2002; Tedstone & Coyle, 2004; Terra

et al., 2006). There is a hypothesis, which includes that alcoholism is a result of self-

medication or anxiety management technique. Specifically, this hypothesis suggests that

alcohol is used to reduce the anxiety levels of individuals. Although it has not been

proved, previous studies revealed that there was a prevalence of 30.6% was for specific

phobia, 24.7% for social phobia, 19.3% for generalized anxiety disorder, 5% for

obsessive-compulsive disorder, 4.6% for posttraumatic stress disorder, and 2% for panic

disorder with agoraphobia among alcohol dependent patients (Terra et al., 2006).

However, none of the studies up to now has established a direct relationship between

obsessive-compulsive disorder and alcoholism. Among anxiety disorders, social phobia

plays a remarkable role than obsessive-compulsive disorder. Nevertheless, it is also

noteworthy that alcoholism has some similarities between some aspects of obsessive-

compulsive disorder in terms of craving features in alcoholism. The concept craving

can be defined as “a compelling urge” intruding into thoughts and changing both mood,

and behavior of an addicted individual (Janiri et al., 2004). It concerns drinking

intention, and feelings in the event of conflict between desire and abstinence. The

symptom checklist (SCL-90) that was given to participants has totally 10 items for

measuring obsessive-compulsive characteristics of an individual. These items generally

focus on the difficulty on performing daily chores and making decisions, concentration

problems, and distress of unwanted and uncontrollable thoughts and behaviors

(compulsions). Therefore, alcohol dependent individuals might have filled these items

Page 110: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

96

while considering their severity of withdrawal, problems in the performance of daily

duties during alcohol intake. The present findings highlight the need for further studies

to determine the relationship between OCD and alcoholism.

Another finding of our regression analysis was that state anxiety played a

negative role in the prediction of the existence of alcoholism. Most of the studies have

pointed out the relationship between alcoholism and self-medication hypothesis, which

is mentioned above. Briefly, this hypothesis includes that the reason of using alcohol is

to decrease the tension of individual (Kushner et al., 2001). However, in our study we

found an opposite direction in the relationship between alcoholism and state anxiety

level of an individual. In our study, it was also found that alcohol dependent individuals

did not get higher scores from STAI-S significantly from non-dependent individuals.

These both findings can be resulted from medicational treatment of our alcohol

dependent inpatients, since anxiolytic drugs helps to diminish situational anxiety of

patients. However, since there are studies, which failed to find such a relationship

between decreased State Anxiety level and increased alcohol consumption (vice versa)

should be replicated by other researchers.

One of the findings in stepwise regression analysis was that depression scores

predicted positively the presence of alcoholism. This result is consistent with several

lines of previous work. Devanad et al. (2002) suggested that in depressed individuals,

the prevalence of alcohol use/abuse is three to four times greater than in non-depressed

individuals. Similarly, in a study of Gratzer et al. (2004), their findings revealed that

there was a significantly higher rate of alcohol abuse or dependence in the co-morbid

depression and anxiety group than in the pure anxiety disorder group. In this respect, the

co-morbid depression and anxiety group seem to have the greatest risk of alcohol abuse

or dependence in both females and males. Of these participants, 45% reported anxiety

as the first disorder, 30% reported alcohol dependence as the first disorder, while 10%

reported depression as the first disorder, and rest of 15% reported all three begun

simultaneously. Briefly, depression seems to have a stronger association with alcohol

misuse or alcoholism than anxiety disorders especially for females and all age groups.

Page 111: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

97

However, it remains unclear whether alcoholism occurs secondary to mood and anxiety

disorders or is a primary cause of these disturbances.

Fourth step in our regression analysis revealed that impaired recognition of

disgusted faces predicted significant but modest (R2=.30) the existence of alcoholism.

Over the past decades, there has been a growing interest of in the role of disgusted facial

expression in psychopathology. Kornreich et al. (2001) indicated that alcoholic patients

had significantly less accuracy scores than obsessive-compulsive patients and normal

individuals. Alcohol dependent individuals had impaired recognition especially towards

the expressions of happiness, anger, disgust, sadness, but not fear. In another study of

Kornreich et al. (2003) their results revealed that accuracy scores were significantly

lower in alcohol dependent individuals than normal individuals. In line to previous

studies, Townshend et al. (2003) found that alcohol dependent individuals exhibited

inappropriate responses in the identification of angry and disgusted facial expressions.

Accordant with the existing findings, our study confirms that alcohol dependent

individuals tend to show a deficit in the recognition of disgusted expression. And

inaccurate identification of disgusted facial expression seems to be contributed

significantly to the explanation of alcoholism.

Final step of the regression analysis revealed that accurate recognition of fearful

faces significantly predicted the presence of alcoholism. In this study, other analyses

had revealed that alcohol dependent inpatients had responses that were more accurate in

the identification of toward fearful expressions when compared to other expressions and

non-dependent individuals. However, this difference was not statistically significant.

Indeed, this finding seems to be parallel only to the findings of Townshend and Duka

(2003). Their study manifested that alcohol dependent inpatients showed an enhanced

intensity of fear recognition. The authors interpreted this increased fear perception may

be the result of a hyperactivity of the amygdala, since neuro-psychological data suggests

that fearful expressions activate amygdala (Blair et al., 2004). That interpretation may

highlight the efficacy of detoxification and medical treatment on the amygdala, which

had become damaged due to long-term alcohol dependence. Surely, further studies are

Page 112: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

98

required to clarify the association of the dysfunction of amygdala with increased fear

recognition.

To sum up, this regression analysis provided further evidences in the explanation

of alcoholism. More or less, these findings can highlight new treatment models and can

imply further research issues.

4. 8. General Discussion and Conclusions

The current study examined the presence of any impairment in the recognition of

emotional facial expressions in alcohol dependence. It was also investigated the

existence of attentional and evaluative bias of state-trait anxiety and depression by

measuring the reaction times of facial expressions. At the end of study, we concentrated

on what kind of variables would predict an existence of alcoholism.

To begin with, our findings have confirmed the existing literature that alcohol

dependence co-morbids to anxiety disorders and depression. We found that alcohol

dependent individuals obtained higher scores on both State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and

Beck Depression Inventory.

On the other hand, the general results did not support our hypothesis that alcohol

dependent group would have less accurate scores than non-dependent group in the

recognition of all emotional expressions. However, it was found that alcohol dependent

group differed significantly from non-dependent group only in the identification of

disgusted expression. Specifically, alcohol dependent group did more errors in decoding

of disgusted faces, which were confused with angry ones. This misinterpretation is

consistent with several studies (e.g.Townshend & Duka, 2003; Philippot et al., 1999).

These authors interpreted this confusion as the damage or a reduction in the functioning

of orbitofrontal cortex, which plays the major role in discriminating of these emotions

because of long-term alcohol misuse. The misjudgment of anger expression may

suggest that alcohol dependent individuals feel more sensitive to threat from faces

looking at them and they are more likely to judge facial expressions as hostile rather

than disgusted or angry faces. Thus, inaccurate decoding of these expressions may cause

Page 113: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

99

alcohol dependent individuals to misunderstand their friends or relatives’ intentions and

feelings, which interrupt the flow of conversation and may result more social-isolation.

Interestingly, findings revealed that both alcohol dependent and non-dependent

individuals discriminated fearful faces less than other emotional expressions. Both

groups had the minimum scores toward fearful faces when compared to other

expressions. Instead, they misinterpret fearful faces as surprised faces. However, it is

difficult to explain why non-dependent individuals showed a similar tendency.

Additional to this confusion, both groups had similar misjudgments toward anger and

neutral expressions except sadness and disgust. As predicted, happy expressions were

the most recognizable expression for both groups. This finding is in agreement with

previous studies.

On the other hand, our regression analysis demonstrated that correct recognition

of fearful faces was one of the predictors in the presence of alcohol dependence. One

possible explanation depends on a neuro-psychological approach, which is that the

increased fear perception is a kind of overcompensation of the amygdala (Townshend &

Duka, 2003; Blair et al., 2004). This interpretation may highlight the efficacy of

detoxification and medical treatment of the amygdala. An alternative explanation for

this finding includes socio-economic status of alcohol dependent inpatients. According

to Herba & Phillips (2004) deprived socio-economic level significantly causes greater

risk for emotional difficulties. The authors stated that exposure of high stress, low

income, low life standards, and decreased expectations may play a role in the

improvement of the perception of fearful expressions. Further studies may focus on the

socio-economic status of participants in terms of emotional perception.

Furthermore, we tested the impacts of depression, state-trait anxiety levels, and

nine psychopathological symptoms (Somatization, Obsessive-Compulsions,

Interpersonal Sensitivity, Depression, Anxiety, Hostility, Psychotism, Paranoid Ideation,

and Phobic Anxiety) on the recognition of facial expressions and reaction times toward

these facial expressions to point out the presence of attentional or evaluative bias of

depression and state-trait anxiety. Surprisingly, our findings manifested that alcohol

dependent group differed only in the identification of disgusted faces. And they reacted

Page 114: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

100

towards disgusted faces more rapidly than non-dependent groups. In other words,

alcohol dependent individuals recognized less but responded faster towards disgusted

expression than non-dependent individuals did when depression, state-trait anxiety

levels were controlled. Therefore, our findings have suggested that there has an impact

of depression and state-trait anxiety levels on the identification of facial expressions.

The literature up to now has contained discrepant findings about selective

attention or misinterpretation of depression and state-trait anxiety in the recognition of

facial expressions. Whereas, it has been difficult to infer stable conclusions from both

our study and previous studies, the impairment and rapid reactions to disgusted faces in

alcohol dependence should be reconsidered.

To conclude, this study has provided further evidences for impaired recognition

of emotional facial expressions in the alcohol dependent group when compared to the

control group. In addition, it implies the roles of depression and state-trait anxiety in

information processing system.

4. 9. Limitations of the Present Study

Surely, some limitations need to be acknowledged in our study.

For both alcohol dependent and non-dependent samples, besides using self-

reported measures, a use of a computerized Emotional Recognition Test might constitute

one of the methodological limitations. Because, computerized material was novel and its

psychometric properties were not documented. Also, it may need more sophistication

than just filling in the self-report inventories. Therefore, some of the participants may

not feel familiar to computerized material, which may affect the performances.

One of the important limitations may be that all data were collected during

business hours for the control group, even though interruptions were prevented, this

factor may cause a decline in their performances. This might have restricted the validity

of our findings.

The selection of the control group may constitute a weakness of our study. They

were chosen mostly from courthouses, from different positions; nevertheless, they may

not be a representative of a population.

Page 115: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

101

Additionally, the control group of this study represented non-clinical individuals.

Thus, they should have been asked whether they were in treatment for any psychological

disorder, if so they should have been excluded from the study. Only potential alcohol

dependent individuals and psychotics were excluded.

One of the important shortcomings of our study may be a low number of all

participants, even though experimental studies require low number of participants when

compared to general studies, this situation inevitably may reduce the ecological validity.

Another potential problem is related to medication of alcohol dependent

inpatients. All of the patients were under treatment when they were administered to all

measures. The impact of medication (anxiolytics or antidepressants) could reduce

particularly the state anxiety levels of patients and affect the reaction time scores. As a

result, this might have affected their ratings of their feelings of anxiety on both State

Anxiety Inventory and the accuracy level on recognition of facial expressions. It could

have been better to study the first time alcohol dependent patients who were not under

medication. However, as this condition was rare, the patients under medication were

conveniently included to the study.

One of the limitations is related with the time of administration of all measures to

alcoholic inpatients. Indeed, it could have been better to test them after their

hospitalization process is over. Because, their anxiety levels resulting from withdrawal

of alcohol abuse could have affected their scores on State –Trait Anxiety Inventory and

Symptom Checklist. Nevertheless, it would be more difficult to reach the individuals

after their hospitalization are ended because of their settlements out of İstanbul.

Finally, one of the critical weaknesses of our study is about gender issue. Hence,

in almost all societies to reach female alcohol dependent individuals is very difficult,

therefore we decided to use only male individuals. The present study should have rather

included a mixed-gender sample of both groups. A large body of evidences has shown

that females are more skilled in emotion processing because of their empathy, emotional

understanding abilities (e.g. Herba & Phillips, 2004; Thayer & Johnsen, 2000).

Therefore, in order to test this assumption and to determine whether there would be

Page 116: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

102

accuracy differences in the identification of facial expressions and their pattern of errors,

it is prominent to carry out a study in which females will participate.

4. 10. Clinical Implications

Emotion identification is very critical for social interaction and daily functioning.

The ability to recognize facial expressions is an important component of social

interaction in order to interpret the intentions, goals, opinions and attitudes of people

(Erickson & Schulkin, 2003; Batty & Taylor, 2003). It is very important to know how

people experience emotions in terms of the quality of relationship. However, any small

impairment in the recognition of facial expressions may interrupt the flow of

conversation and hence may cause interpersonal conflicts.

Alcoholism is characterized by multiple neuropsychological dysfunctions and by

profound interpersonal relationship problems and social isolation (Kornreich, et al.,

2001). As predicted, alcohol dependent participants were found as less accurate in

identifying seven emotional expressions (happiness, sadness, fear, anger, surprise,

disgust, and neutral) although we failed to reach statistical significance. Specifically,

alcohol dependent individuals showed a significant error rate in decoding of disgusted

expression. Instead, they misjudge disgusted faces as angry ones. This can be a toxic

result of heavy alcohol consumption. The misjudgment may suggest that alcohol

dependent individuals are more sensitive to the feelings of threat on faces looking at

them and they are more likely to interpret facial expressions as hostile rather than

disgusted or angry. Also, the number of fear recognition in alcohol dependents was

much more than non-dependents, this issue should be replicated.

Furthermore, stepwise multiple regression analysis indicated that obsessive-

compulsive subscale of symptom checklist, Beck Depression Inventory, STAI-State

Form, and the recognition of fearful as well as disgusted expressions were associated

with alcoholism. Briefly, this analysis highlight that the increased fear recognition,

decreased disgust recognition and the existence of depression and obsessive-compulsive

features, and reduced state anxiety levels are related with the alcohol dependence. In

order to find out most appropriate treatment model of alcoholism, it is also important to

Page 117: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

103

understand the nature of alcoholism. Thereby, the implications of regression analysis

may be considered.

Similar findings were obtained when state-trait anxiety levels, depression, and

other psychopathological symptoms were taken as covariates. In other words, alcohol

dependent individuals recognized less but responded faster toward disgusted expressions

than non-dependent individuals when covariates were considered. Surely, it has been

well established that anxious individuals exhibit an attentional bias toward threat cues.

This bias may play an important role in the development and maintenance of anxiety. If

these biases provoke anxiety states, then removal of biases can be a target of treatment.

However, when we consider our findings that failed to indicate the existence of any bias,

then treatment can be regenerated through this novel finding.

Specifically, our general findings provide more information to the clinicians in

order to realize the co-morbid disorders, cognitive impairments, and social isolation of

long-term alcohol abuse in a more comprehensive manner. Consequently, new treatment

techniques can be produced through decreased inaccurate decoding and reduced

functioning of brain areas that are responsible for the process of disgusted expression.

As conclusion, most of the literature up to now has focused on cognitive

deficiencies of long-term alcohol abuse. In the current study, the main objective was to

determine whether the alcohol dependence would decrease the ability of emotional facial

expressions. In terms of the design and objectives, this is the first study in Turkey on the

identification of emotional facial expressions in alcohol dependent individuals.

4. 11. Directions for Future Research

The results of the present study in general did not support our hypothesis, which

was that alcohol dependent individuals would show inaccurate identification of facial

expressions. Additional studies are needed to clarify the nature of this relationship.

Future research should examine the presence of any impairment both in a larger sample

and with a mixed-gender sample. Because considerable amount of evidences have

proved that females are better in detecting and decoding facial expressions. They have

been also found as more emotionally expressive than males (e.g. Herba & Phillips, 2004;

Page 118: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

104

Thayer & Johnsen, 2000). Moreover, it may be advisable for future studies to use

instrument, which are as ecologically valid as possible.

In order to find out the relationship between interpersonal problems and

impairments in the recognition of facial expressions properly, it will be better to use a

measure such as Inventory of Interpersonal Problems. Thus, if interpersonal difficulties

are reported, these difficulties can be correlated with facial decoding problems. Then, it

will be proved that non-verbal emotional cues play an important role in maintenance of

interpersonal communication.

Furthermore, common identifications of facial expressions of both groups let us

consider some cultural elements. The confusion of fearful and surprised faces, the

misjudgment of neutral faces as sad ones can be counted as the examples of the

commonalities. Perhaps, environmental conditions and socio-economic status may take

role in these misinterpretations. Definitely, concept of the universality of facial

expressions and their recognitions may need to be reconsidered. Similarly, our findings

yielded that there was an increased recognition in fearful expression in alcohol

dependent individuals, eventhough this difference did not reach a statistical significance.

Therefore, further research is needed to resolve these issues.

Page 119: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

105

REFERENCES

Abboud, B., Davoine, F., & Dang, M. (2004). Facial expression recognition and synthesis based on an appearence model. Signal Processing: Image Communication, 19, 723-740.

Adolphs, R.,Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1994).Impaired recognition of

emotion in facial expressions following bilateral damage to the human amygdala. Nature, 372, 669-672.

Amaral, D. (2002). The primate amygdala and the neurobiology of social behavior:

implications for understanding social anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 51, 1, 11-17. Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Glickman, S. (2005). Attentional bias in anxiety: A

behavioral and ERP study. Brain and Cognition, 59, 11-22. Batty, M., Taylor, M.J. (2003). Early processing of the six basic facial emotional

expressions. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 613-620. Beaupre, M.G., Hess, U. (2003). In my mind, we all smile: A case of in-group

favoritism. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 371-377. Berger, G. (1993). Alcoholism and The Family. New York: Library of Congress

Cataloging-in-Publication Data. Berger, B. D., Adesso, V. J. (1991). Gender differences in using alcohol to cope with

depression. Addictive Behaviors, 16, 315-327. Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination & reformulation.

Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59-73. Bisson, J., Nadeau, L., & Demers, A. (1999). The Validity of the CAGE Scale to screen

for Heavy Drinking and Drinking Problems in a General Population Survey.

Page 120: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

106

Addiction, 94, 715-722. Blair, R.J.R, Mitchell, D.G.V., Peschardt, K.S., Colledge, E., Leonard, R.A., Shine, J.H.,

Murray, L.K., Perrett, D.I. (2004). Reduced sensitivity to others’ fearful expressions in psychopathic individuals. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1111-1122.

Bradley, B.P., Mogg, K. & Millar, N.H. (2000). Covert and Overt Orienting of Attention

to Emotional Faces in Anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 14 (6), 789-808. Brandt, J., Provost, D. G. (1985). On the dissimilar effects of alcohol and aging on the

perception of cognitive failings, Alcohol, 2, 633-635. Bühler, A., Kraus, L., Augustin, R., Kramer, S. (2004). Screening for Alcohol-related Problems in the General Population using CAGE and DSM-IV: Characteristics of Congruently and Incongruently Identified Participants. Addictive Behaviors, 29,

867-878. Candland, D.K., Fell, J. P., Keen, E., Leshner, A.I., Tarpy, R. M. & Plutchik, R. (1977).

Emotion. USA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company. Carroll, J., Russell, J.A. (1996). Do Facial Expressions Signal Specific Emotions?

Judging Emotion From the Face in Context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 70, No.2, 205-218.

Cavanagh, J., Geisler, M.W. (2005). Mood effects on the ERP processing of emotional

intensity in faces: A P3 investigation with depressed students. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72, 118-125.

Ceylan, M.E., Türkcan, A. (2003). Araştırma ve Klinik Uygulamada Biyolojik

Psikiyatri Alkol ve Madde Kullanım Bozukluklukları.Genişletilmiş 2. Baskı. İstanbul.

Chelune, G., Parker, J. G. (1981). Neuropsychological deficits associated with chronic

alcohol abuse. Clinical Psychological Review, 1, 181-195. Cooper, R.M., Langton, S.R.H. (2006). Attentional bias to angry faces using dot-probe

Page 121: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

107

task? It depends when you look for it. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1321-1329.

Dağ, İ. (1991). Belirti Tarama Listesi (SCL-90-R)’nin Üniversite Öğrencileri İçin

Güvenilirliği ve Geçerliği. Türk Psikiyatri Dergisi, 2: (1). Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T.J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L.L., Parvizi, J.

(2000). Subcorticol and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions.National Neuroscience, 3, 1049-1056.

Darwin, C. (1934). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: Watts &

Co. Davey, G.C.L., Bickerstaffe, S., MacDonald, B.A. (2006). Experienced disgust causes a negative interpretation bias: A causal role for disgust in anxious psychopathology. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1375-1384. Deleuze, G. (2000). Spinoza Üstüne On Bir Ders. Ankara: Öteki Yayınları. Devanand, D. P. (2002). Comorbid Psychiatry Disorders in Late Life Depression.

Biological Psychiatry, 51, 236-242. Dujardin, K., Blairy, S., Defebvre, L., Duhem, S., Noel, Y., Hess, U., Destée, A. (2004).

Deficits in decoding emotional facial expressions in Parkinson’s Disease. Neuropsychologia, 42, 239-250.

Dunn, Y., Hughes, C. (1998). Young Children’s Understanding of Emotions within

Close Relationships. Cognition and Emotion, 12 (2), 171-190. Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D. (2005). Functional consequences of perceiving facial

expressions emotions without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 12, 18-23.

Eich, E., Kihlstrom, J. F., Bower, G., Forgas, J. P. & Niedenthal, P. M. (2000).

Cognition and Emotion. USA: Oxford University Press.

Page 122: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

108

Ekman, P. (1992b). Are there basic emotions? Psychological Review, 99, 550-553. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. In .L. Stein, & K. Oatley (Eds.), A

Special Issue of Cognition and Emotion; Basic Emotions, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. publishers, p. 169-200.

Ekman, P. (1999). Basic Emotions. In T. Dalgleish, & M.Power (Eds.), Handbook of

Cognition and Emotion. Sussex, UK.: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Ekman, P. (1993). Facial Expression and Emotion. American Psychologist, 48, 4, 384-

392 Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions Revealed: Recognizing faces and feelings to improve communication and emotional life. New York: Times Books Ekman, P.& Friesen, W.V. (1976). Pictures of Facial Affect. Consulting Psychologists

Press, Palo Alto, CA. Ekman, P. & Friesen, W.V. (1975). Unmasking The Face: a guide to recognizing

emotions from facial expressions. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Elfenbein, H.A., Ambady, N. (2000). When Familiarity Breeds Accuracy: Cultural

Exposureand Facial Emotion Recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 85, No.2, 276-290

Erickson, K., Schulkin, J. (2003). Facial expression of emotion: A cognitive

neuroscience. Perspective. Brain and Cognition, 52, 52-60. Exner, C., Boucsein K., Degner, D., Irle, E., Weniger, G. (2004). Impaired emotional

learning and reduced amygdala size in schizophrenia: 3 month follow up. Schizophrenia Research, 71, 493-503.

Ewing, J.A. (1984). Detecting Alcoholism. The CAGE Questionnaire. Journal of

American Medical Association (JAMA), 252 (14) , 1905-1907.

Page 123: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

109

Frigerio, E., Burt, M.D., Montagne, B., Murray, L.K. & Perrett, D.I. (2002). Facial Affect

Perception in Alcoholics. Psychiatry Research, 113, 161-171. Frijda, N.H. (1986). The emotions. USA: Cambridge University Press. Gallegos, D.R., Tranel, D. (2005). Positive facial affect faciliates the identification of

famous faces. Brain and Language, 93, 338-348. Garcia-Moreno, L.M., Conejo, N.M., Capilla, A., Garcia-Sanchez, O., Senderek,

K., Arias, J.(2002). Chronic ethanol intake and object recognition in young and adult rats. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 26, 831-837.

Gosselin, P., Larocque, C. (2000). Facial Morphology and Children’s Categorization of

Facial Expressions of Emotions: A Comparison Between Asian and Caucasian Faces, The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 161(3), 346-358.

Grant, B.F., Harford T.C.(1995). Comorbidity between DSM-IV alcohol use disorders

and major depression: results of a national survey. Drug Alcohol Dependence, 39 (3), 197-206.

Gratzer, D., Levitan, R.D., Sheldon, T., Toneatto, T., Rector, N. A., Goering, P. (2004).

Lifetime rates of alcoholism in adults with anxiety: a community survey of Ontario, Journal of Affective Disorders, 79, 209-215.

Hale, W.W.(1998). Judgment of Facial Expressions and Depression Persistence.

Psychiatry Research, 80, 265-274. Helmers, K. F., Mente, A. (1999). Alexithymia and health behaviors in healthy male

volunteers. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47, (6), 635-645. Herba, C., Phillips, M. (2004). Annotation: Development of facial expression

recognition from childhood to adolescence: behavioral and neurological perspectives. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45 (7), 1185-1198.

Page 124: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

110

Hodgson, R., Alwyn, T., John, B., Thom, B., & Smith, A. (2002). The fast alcohol

screening test. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 37 (1), 61-66. Hoes, M. (1997). Alcoholism and drug addictions. European Psychiatry,12, 102-103. Hoes, M. (1997). Alcoholism: new aspects in epidemology, diagnosis, and research. European Psychiatry,12, 374-377. Ioannou, M.C., Mogg, K., Bradley, B.P. (2004). Vigilance for threat: effects of anxiety

and defensiveness. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1876-1891. Jackson, K.M.& Sher, K. (2003). Alcohol Use Disorders and Psychological Distress:

A Prospective State-Trait Analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology Vol.112, No.4, 599-613.

Janiri, L., Calvosa, F., Dairo, T., Pozzi, G., Ruggeri, A., Addolotaro, G., Di

Giannantonio, M. De Risio, S. (2004). The Italian version of Obsessive-Compulsive Drinking Scale: validation, comparison with the other versions, and Difference between type 1- and type 2-like alcoholics. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 74, 187-195.

Jones, K. L., Shainberg, L. W., Byer, C. O. (1969). Drugs and Alcohol. New York:

Harper & Row. Kontsevich, L.L., Tyler, C.W. (2004). What makes Mona Lisa Smile? Vision Research,

44, 1493-1498. Kornreich, C., Blairy S., Philippot, P., Dan, B. Foisy, M.L., Hess, U., Le Bon, O., Pelc, I.,

Vernback, P. (2001). Impaired Emotional Facial Expression Recognition in Alcoholism Compared with Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder and Normal

Controls. Psychiatry Research, 102, 235-248. Kornreich, C., Foisy, M.L., Philippot, P., Dan, B., Tecco, J., Noel, X., Hess, U., Pelc, I.,

Page 125: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

111

Vernback, P. (2003). Impaired Emotional Facial Expression Recognition in Alcoholics, Opiate Dependence Subjects, Methadone Maintained Subjects and Mixed Alcohol- Opiate Antecedents Subjects Compared with Normal Controls. Psychiatry Research, 119, 251-260. Kornreich, C., Philippot, P., Foisy, M. L., Blairy, S., Raynaud, E., Dan, B., Hess, U.,

Noel, X. Pelc, I., Verbanck, P. (2002). Impaired emotional facial expression is associated with interpersonal problems in alcoholism. Alcohol and Alcoholism, 37 (4), 394-400.

Kornreich, C., Philippot, P., Verpoorten, C., Dan, B., Baert, I., Le Bon, O., Vernback, P.,

Pelc, I. (1998). Alcoholism and Emotional Reactivity: More Heterogeneous Film- Induced Emotional Response in Newly Detoxified Alcoholics Compared to Controls- A Preliminary Study. Addictive Behaviors, Vol. 23, No. 3, 413-418.

Kowalski, R. & Westen, D. (2005). Psychology (4th ed.). USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Kushner, M.G., Thuras, P., Abrams, K., Brekke, M., Striar, L. (2001). Anxiety mediates

The association between anxiety sensitivity and coping-related drinking motives in alcoholism treatment patients. Addictive Behaviors, 26, 869-885.

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. USA: Oxford University Press, Inc. Leon, I., Hernandez, J.A. (1998). Testing the Role of Attribution and Appraisal in

Predicting Own and Other’s Emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 12 (1), 27-43. Leppanen, J. M., Milders, M., Bell, S., Terriere, E., Hietanen, J., K. (2004). Depression

biases the recognition of emotionally neutral faces. Psychiatry Research 128, 123-133.

Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. M. (2000). Handbook of Emotions (2nd ed). New York:

Guilford Press. Linehan, M. (1987). Dialectical behavior therapy for borderline personality disorder:

Theory and method. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 51, 261-276. Loas, G., Otmani, O., Lecercle, C., Jouvent, R. (2000). relationships between the

Page 126: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

112

emotional and cognitive components of alexithymia and dependency in alcoholics. Psychiatry Research, 96, 63-74.

Malatesta, C.Z., Haviland, J.M. (1982). Learning Display Rules: The Socialization of

Emotion Expression in Infancy. Child Development, 53, 991-1003. Mansell, W., Ehlers, A., Clark, D.M., Chen, Y.P. (2002). Attention to positive and

negative social-evaluative words: Investigating the effects of social anxiety, trait anxiety, and social threat. Anxiety Stress and Coping, 15, 1, 19-29.

Martin, F., Baudouin, J.-Y., Tiberghien, G., Franck, N. (2005). Processing emotional

expression and facial identity in schziophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 134, 43-53.

Matsumoto, D., Consolation, T., Yamada, H., Suzuki, R., Franklin, B., Paul, S., Ray, R., Uchida, H. (2002). American-Japanese cultural differences in judgements of emotional expressions of different intensities. Cognition and Emotion, 16 (6),

721- 747. Mendlewicz, L., Linkowski, P., Bazelmans, C., Philippot, P. (2005). Decoding

emotional facial expressions in depressed and anorexic patients. Journal of Affective Disorders, 89,1-3, 195-199.

Merikangas, K.R., Stevens, D.E., Fenton, B., Stolar, M., O’malley, S., Woods, S.W.,

Risch, N. (1998). Co-morbidity and familial aggregation of alcoholism and anxiety disorders. Psychological Medicine, 28, 773-788.

Miller, L. (1990). Neuropsychodynamics of alcoholism and addiction: personality,

Psychopathology, and cognitive style. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 7, 31-49.

Mogg, K., Bradbury, K.E., Bradley, B.P. (2006). Interpretation of ambiguous

information in clinical depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1411-1419.

Mogg, K., Bradley, B.P. (2006). Time Course of attentional bias for fear-relevant

pictures in spider-fearful individuals. Behavior Research and Therapy, 44, 1241-1250.

Mogg, K., Millar, N., Bradley, B.P. (2000). Biases in eye movements to threatening

Page 127: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

113

facial Expressions in generalized anxiety disorder and depressive disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 695-704.

Monnot, M., Nixon, S., Lovallo, W., Elliott, R. (2001). Altered emotional perception in

Alcoholics: deficits in affective prosody comprehension. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 25(3), 362-369.

Montagne, B., Van Honk, J., Kessels, R. P. C., Frigerio, E., Burt, M., Van Zandvoort,

M., Perrett, E., de Han, E. H. F. (2005). Reduced efficiency in recognizing fear in subjects scoring high on psychopathic personality characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 5-11.

Nelson, C.A., Dolgin, K. G. (1985). The generalized discrimination of facial expressions

by seven-month-old infants. Child Development, 56, 58-61. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Gender differences in risk factors and consequences for alcohol use

and problems, Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 981-1010. Oatley, K. & Jenkins, J.M. (1996).Understanding Emotions. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Blackwell Publishers Inc. Obernier, J.A., White, A.M., Swartzwelder, H.S., Crews, F.T. (2002). Cognitive deficits

and CNS damage after a 4-day binge ethanol exposure in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 72, 521-532.

Oltmanns, T.F., Emery, R.E. (2001). Abnormal Psychology. (3rd ed.). New Jersey:

Prentice Hall. Öner, L. (1997). Türkiye’de Kullanılan Psikolojik Testler, Bir Başvuru Kaynağı. 3rd ed. İstanbul: Boğaziçi Üniversitesi Yayınları. Patrick, C.J. (1994). Emotion and psychopathy: Startling new insights. Psychophysiology,

31, 319-330. Persad, M.S., Polivy, J. (1993). Differences Between Depressed and Nondepressed

Page 128: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

114

Individuals in the Recognition of and Response to Facial Emotional Cues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, Vol.102, No.3, 358-368.

Philippot, P., Douilliez, C. (2005). Social phobics do not misinterpret facial expression

of emotion. Behavior Research and Therapy, 43, 5, 639-652. Philippot, P., Kornreich, C., Blairy, S., Baert, I., Dulk, D., Le Bon, O., Streel, E.,Hess,

U., Pelc, I., Verbanck, P. (1999). Alcoholics’ Deficits in the Decoding of Emotional Facial Expression. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 23, 1031-1038.

Planalp, S. (1999). Communicating Emotion: Social, Moral, and Cultural Processes. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Preisig, M., Fenton, B.T., Stevens, D.E, Merikangas, K. R. (2001) Familial Relationship

Between Mood Disorders and Alcoholism. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42, 2, 87-95.

Renneberg, B., Heyn, K., Gebhard, R., Bachmann, S. (2005). Facial expression of

emotions in borderline personality disorder and depression. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 36, 183-196.

Rohner, J.C. (2002). The time-course of visual threat processing: High trait anxious

individuals eventually avert their gaze from angry faces. Cognition and Emotion, 16, (6), 837-844. Room, R., Babor, T., Rehm, J. (2005). Alcohol and public health, The Lancet, 365, 519-

530. Rossignol, M., Anselme, C., Vermeulen, N., Philippot, P., Douilliez, C., Campanella, S.

(2007). Categorical perception of anger and disgust facial expression is affected by non-clinical social anxiety: An ERP study. Brain Research, 1132, 166-176.

Rossignol, M., Philippot, P., Douilliez, C., Crommelinck, M., Campanella, S. (2005).

The perception of fearful and happy facial expression is modulated by anxiety: an event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 377, 115-120.

Rubio-Godoy, M., Aunger, R., Curtis, V. (2007). Serotonin- A link between disgust and

immunity? Medical Hypotheses, 68, 61-66.

Page 129: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

115

Russell, J., Suzuki, N., Ishida, N. (1993). Canadian, Greek and Japanese freely produced

emotion labels for facial expressions. Motivation and Emotion, 17, 337-351. Santrock, J.W. (1997). Life-span Development. (6th ed.). USA: McGraw-Hill

Companies Inc. Shayegan, D. K., Stahl, S. M. (2005). Emotion processing the amygdala, and outcome in

Schizophrenia, Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry,29, 840-845.

Surcinelli, P., Codispoti, M., Montebarocci, O., Rossi, N., Baldaro, B. (2006). Facial

emotion recognition in trait anxiety. Anxiety Disorders, 20, 110-117 Stein, N.L. & Oatley, K. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. In L.N. Stein, & K.

Oatley (Eds), A Special Issue of Cognition and Emotion; Basic Emotions, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd. publishers, p. 161-168.

Surguladze, S., Brammer, M. J., Keedwell, P., Giampietro, V., Young, A., W., Travis,

M. J., Williams, S., C.R., Phillips, M., L. (2005). A differential pattern of neural response toward sad vs. happy facial expressions in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 201-209.

Swendsen, J. D, Tennen, H., Carney, M.A., Affleck, G., Willard, A., Hromi, A. (2000).

Mood and Alcohol Consumption: An Experience Sampling Test of the Self-Medication Hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Vol.109, No.2, 198-204.

Şahin, N. H., & Savaşır, I. (Eds.). (1997). Bilişsel-davranışçı terapilerde değerlendirme:

Sık kullanılan ölçekler. Ankara: Türk Psikologlar Derneği. Tedstone, D., Coyle, K. (2004). Cognitive Impairments in Sober Alcoholics: Performance

on Selective and Divided Attention Tasks. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 75, 277-286.

Teigen, K.H., Keren, G. (2002) When are successes more surprising than failures?

Cognition and Emotion, 16 (2), 245-268.

Page 130: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

116

Terra, M.B., Barros, H.M.T., Stein, A.T., Figueira, I., Jorge, M.R., Palermo, L.H., Athayde,

L.D., Gonçelvas, M.S., Spanemberg, L., Possa, M.A., Filho, L.D., Da Silveira, D.X. (2006). Social anxiety disorder in 300 patients hospitalized for alcoholism in Brazil: high prevalence and undertreatment. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47, 463-467.

Thayer, J.F., Johnsen, B.H. (2000). Sex differences in judgments of facial affect: A

multivariate analysis of recognition errors. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 41, 243-246.

Townshend, J.M., Duka, T. (2003). Mixed Emotions: Alcoholics’ Impairments in the Recognition of Specific Emotional Facial Expressions. Neuropsychologia, 41, 773- 782. Vasta, R., Haith, M.M., Miller, S.C. (2000). Child Psychology, the modern science (3rd

ed.) USA: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Weniger, G., Lange, C., Rüther, E. & Irle, E. (2004). Differential Impairments of Facial

Affect Recognition in Schizophrenia Subtypes and Major Depression. Psychiatry Research, 128, 135-146.

Westen, D. (1999). Psychology; Mind, Brain, and Culture.(2nd ed.). USA: John Wiley

& Sons, Inc. Wetterling, T., Junghanns, K. (2000). Psychopathology of alcoholics during withdrawal

and early abstinence, European Psychiatry, 15, 483-488.

Wikipedia, Inc. (2007). http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Democritus Wikipedia, Inc. (2007). http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Hippocrates Wikipedia, Inc. (2007). http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Plato

Page 131: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

117

APPENDICES

Appendix A

DEMOGRAPHICAL INFORMATION

1. ID No: ............ 2. Tarih: .... 3. Ad ve Soyadınız: 4. Yaşınız: ........... 5. Oturduğunuz şehir: ........... 6. Mesleğiniz:

a. İşçi b. Memur c. Serbest Meslek d. Emekli e. Sanatçı f. Ev hanımı g. İşsiz h. Diğer ................

7. Eğitiminiz:

a. Okuma yazma biliyorum b. İlkokul mezunu c. Ortaokul mezunu d. Lise mezunu e. Üniversite mezunu f. Yüksek lisans ya da doktora

8. Hayatınızda en uzun süre oturduğunuz yer:

a. Büyükşehir b. Şehir c. Kasaba d. Köy 9. Medeni Durumunuz:

a. Bekar b. Evli c.Boşanmış d. Ayrı Yaşıyor

10. Çocuğunuz var mı? a. Var ............tane b. Yok

11. Ailenizde sizden başka alkol kullanan var mı? Var ise kaç yıldır devam ediyor?

...................................................................

12. Alkol sorununuz kaç yıldır devam ediyor? .................................................

13. Alkol sorunu nedeniyle kaç kez hastaneye yattınız? .................................................

14. Sizce alkol sorununuzun nedeni nedir? ……………………………………………………….

Page 132: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

118

Appendix B

CAGE ALCOHOL USE INVENTORY

1. Şimdiye kadar içmeyi kesmeniz ya da azaltmanız gerektiğini hissettiniz mi?

a. Evet b. Hayır

2. İçmenizle ilgili olarak başkalarının eleştirilerinden sıkıldığınız oldu mu? a. Evet b. Hayır 3. Hiç şimdiye kadar içmenizden dolayı kendinizi kötü ya da suçlu hissettiniz mi? a. Evet b. Hayır 4. Hiç şimdiye kadar sinirlerinizi yatıştırmak ya da akşamdan kalma halinizi gidermek amacıyla sabah ilk iş olarak içtiniz mi? a. Evet b. Hayır

Page 133: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

119

Appendix C

BECK DEPRESSION INVENTORY (BDI)

YÖNERGE: Her gruptaki cümleleri dikkatle okuyunuz ve BUGÜN Dahil, GEÇEN HAFTA içinde kendinizi nasıl hissettiğinizi en iyi anlatan cümleyi seçiniz. Seçmiş olduğunuz cümlenin yanındaki numarayı daire içine alınız. 1. 0 Kendimi üzüntülü ve sıkıntılı hissetmiyorum.

1 Kendimi üzüntülü ve sıkıntılı hissediyorum. 2 Hep üzüntülü ve sıkıntılıyım. Bundan kurtulamıyorum. 3 O kadar üzüntülü ve sıkıntılıyım ki artık dayanamıyorum.

2. 0 Gelecek hakkında umutsuz ve karamsar değilim. 1 Gelecek hakkında karamsarım. 2 Gelecekten beklediğim hiçbir şey yok. 3 Geleceğim hakkında umutsuzum ve sanki hiç bir şey düzelmeyecekmiş gibi geliyor. 3. 0 Kendimi başarısız bir insan olarak görmüyorum. 1 Çevremdeki bir çok kişiden daha çok başarısızlıklarım olmuş gibi hissediyorum. 2 Geçmişime baktığımda başarısızlıklarla dolu olduğumu görüyorum. 3 Kendimi tümüyle başarısız bir kişi olarak görüyorum.

4. 0 Birçok şeyden eskisi kadar zevk alıyorum. 1 Eskiden olduğu gibi her şeyden hoşlanmıyorum. 2 Artık hiçbir şey bana tam anlamıyla zevk vermiyor. 3 Her şeyden sıkılıyorum. 5. 0 Kendimi herhangi bir şekilde suçlu hissetmiyorum. 1 Kendimi zaman zaman suçlu hissediyorum 2 Çoğu zaman kendimi suçlu hissediyorum.

3 Kendimi her zaman suçlu hissediyorum. 6. 0 Kendimden memnunum. 1 Kendimden pek memnun değilim. 2 Kendime çok kızıyorum. 3 Kendimden nefret ediyorum. 7. 0 Başkalarından daha kötü olduğumu sanmıyorum. 1 Zayıf yanlarım ya da hatalarım için kendi kendimi eleştiririm. 2 Hatalarımdan dolayı her zaman kendimi kabahatli bulurum. 3 Her aksilik karşısında kendimi kabahatli bulurum.

Page 134: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

120

8. 0 Kendimi öldürmek gibi düşüncelerim yok 1 Zaman zaman kendimi öldürmeyi düşündüğüm oluyor fakat yapmıyorum 2 Kendimi öldürmek isterdim. 3 Fırsatını bulsam kendimi öldürürüm. 9. 0 Her zamankinden fazla içimden ağlamak gelmiyor.

1 Zaman zaman içimden ağlamak geliyor. 2 Çoğu zaman ağlıyorum. 3 Eskiden ağlayabilirdim şimdi istesem de ağlayamıyorum.

10. 0 Şimdi her zaman olduğumdan daha sinirli değilim. 1 Eskisine kıyasla daha kızıyor ya da sinirleniyorum. 2 Şimdi hep sinirliyim. 3 Bir zamanlar beni sinirlendiren şeyler şimdi hiç sinirlendirmiyor. 11. 0 Başkaları ile görüşmek, konuşmak isteğimi kaybetmedim. 1 Başkaları ile eskisinden daha az konuşmak, görüşmek istiyorum. 2 Başkaları ile konuşma ve görüşme isteğimi kaybettim. 3 Hiç kimseyle görüşüp, konuşmak istemiyorum. 12. 0 Eskiden olduğu kadar kolay karar verebiliyorum. 1 Eskiden olduğu kadar kolay karar veremiyorum. 2 Karar verirken eskisine kıyasla çok güçlük çekiyorum. 3 Artık hiç karar veremiyorum. 13. 0 Aynada kendime baktığımda bir değişiklik görmüyorum. 1 Daha yaşlanmışım ve çirkinleşmişim gibi geliyor 2 Görünüşümün çok değiştiğini ve daha çirkinleştiğimi hissediyorum. 3 Kendimi çok çirkin buluyorum. 14. 0 Eskisi kadar iyi çalışabiliyorum 1 Bir şeyler yapabilmek için gayret göstermek gerekiyor. 2 Herhangi bir şeyi yapabilmek için kendimi çok zorlamam gerekiyor. 3 Hiçbir şey yapamıyorum. 15. 0 Her zamanki gibi iyi uyuyabiliyorum 1 Eskiden olduğu gibi iyi uyuyabiliyorum. 2 Her zamankinden 1-2 saat daha erken uyanıyorum ve tekrar uyuyamıyorum. 3 Her zamankinden çok daha erken uyanıyorum ve tekrar uyuyamıyorum. 16. 0 Her zamankinden daha çabuk yorulmuyorum. 1 Her zamankinden daha çabuk yoruluyorum. 2 Yaptığım hemen herşey beni yoruyor. 3 Kendimi hiçbir şey yapamayacak kadar yorgun hissediyorum

Page 135: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

121

17. 0 İştahım her zamanki gibi 1 İştahım eskisi kadar iyi değil. 2 İştahım çok azaldı 3 Artık hiç iştahım yok 18. 0 Son zamanlarda kilo vermedim 1 İki kilodan fazla verdim 2 Dört kilodan fazla kilo verdim 3 Altı kilodan fazla kilo verdim. Daha az yiyerek kilo vermeye çalışıyorum. Evet ........... Hayır.......... 19. 0 Sağlığım beni fazla endişelendirmiyor. 1 Ağrı, sancı, mide bozukluğu veya kabızlık gibi rahatsızlıklar beni endişelendiriyor. 2 Sağlığım beni endişelendirdiği için başka şeyler düşünmek zorlaşıyor. 3 Sağlığım hakkında o kadar endişeliyim ki başka hiçbir şey düşünemiyorum. 20. 0 Son zamanlarda cinsel konulara olan ilgimde bir değişme fark etmedim. 1 Cinsel konularla eskisinden daha az ilgiliyim. 2 Cinsel konularla şimdi çok daha az ilgiliyim 3 Cinsel konulara olan ilgimi tamamen kaybettim 21. 0 Bana cezalandırılmışım gibi gelmiyor. 1 Cezalandırılabileceğimi seziyorum

2 Cezalandırılmayı bekliyorum 3 Cezalandırıldığımı hissediyorum.

Page 136: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

122

Appendix D

STATE-TRAIT ANXIETY INVENTORY (STAI)

Yönerge: Aşağıdaki kişilerin kendilerine ait duygularını anlatmakta kullandıkları bir

takım ifadeler verilmiştir. Her ifadeyi okuyun, sonra da on anda nasıl hissettiğinizi

ifadelerin sağ tarafındaki seçeneklerden uygun olanını daire içine alın. Doğru ya da

yanlış yanıt yoktur. Herhangi bir ifadenin üzerinde fazla zaman harcamadan anında nasıl

hissettiğinizi gösteren yanıtı işaretleyin.

Hiç

Biraz

Çok

Tamamıyla

1. Şu an sakinim 1 2 3 4

2. Kendimi emniyette hissediyorum. 1 2 3 4

3. Şu an sinirlerim gergin. 1 2 3 4

4. Pişmanlık duygusu içindeyim 1 2 3 4

5. Şu anda huzur içindeyim 1 2 3 4

6. Şu anda hiç keyfim yok 1 2 3 4

7. Başıma geleceklerden endişe ediyorum. 1 2 3 4

8. Kendimi dinlenmiş hissediyorum.

1 2 3 4

9. Şu anda kaygılıyım. 1 2 3 4

10. Kendimi rahat hissediyorum. 1 2 3 4

11. Kendime güvenim var 1 2 3 4

12. Şu anda asabım bozuk. 1 2 3 4

13. Çok sinirliyim 1 2 3 4 14. Sinirlerimin çok gergin olduğunu hissediyorum.

1 2 3 4

15. Kendimi rahatlamış hissediyorum 1 2 3 4

16. Şu anda halimden memnunum 1 2 3 4

Page 137: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

123

Hiç

Biraz

Çok

Tamamıyla

17. Şu anda endişeliyim. 1 2 3 4

18. Heyecandan endimi şaşkına dönmüş hissediyorum.

1 2 3 4

19. Şu anda sevinçliyim 1 2 3 4

20. Şu anda keyfim yerinde 1 2 3 4

Page 138: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

124

Yönerge: Aşağıdaki kişilerin kendilerine ait duygularını anlatmakta kullandıkları bir

takım ifadeler verilmiştir. Her ifadeyi fazla zaman harcamadan okuyun, sonra da genelde

nasıl hissettiğinizi ifadelerin sağ tarafındaki seçeneklerin uygun olanını daire içine alın.

Doğru ya da yanlış yanıt yoktur.

Hiçbir zaman

Bazen

Çoğu zaman

Her zaman

21. Genellikle keyfim yerindedir. 1 2 3 4

22. Genellikle çabuk yorulurum. 1 2 3 4

23. Genellikle kolay ağlarım. 1 2 3 4

24. Başkaları kadar mutlu olmak isterim. 1 2 3 4

25. Çabuk karar veremediğim için fırsatları kaçırırım.

1 2 3 4

26. Kendimi dinlenmiş hissederim 1 2 3 4

27. Genellikle sakin, kendime hakim ve soğukkanlıyım

1 2 3 4

28. Güçlüklerin yenemeyeceğim kadar biriktiğini hissederim.

1 2 3 4

29. Önemsiz şeyler hakkında kaygılanırım. 1 2 3 4

30. Genellikle mutluyum. 1 2 3 4

31. Herşeyi ciddiye alır ve etkilenirim. 1 2 3 4

32. Genellikle kendime güvenim yoktur.

1 2 3 4

33. Genellikle kendimi emniyette hissederim. 1 2 3 4

34. Sıkıntılı ve güç durumlarla karşılaşmaktan çekinirim.

1 2 3 4

35. Genellikle kendimi hüzünlü hissederim. 1 2 3 4

36. Genellikle hayatımdan memnunum. 1 2 3 4

Page 139: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

125

Hiçbir zaman

Bazen

Çoğu zaman

Her zaman

37. Olur olmaz düşünceler beni rahatsız eder. 1 2 3 4

38. Hayal kırıklıklarını öylesine ciddiye alırım ki hiç unutamam.

1 2 3 4

39. Aklı başında ve kararlı bir insanım. 1 2 3 4

40. Son zamanlarda kafama takılan konular beni tedirgin eder.

1 2 3 4

Page 140: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

126

Appendix E

THE SYMPTOM CHECKLIST (SCL-90-R)

Yönerge: Aşağıda zaman zaman herkeste rastlanılabilecek şikayetlerden oluşan bir liste

verilmiştir. Her soruyu dikkatle okuyunuz. Sözü geçen problemlerin son 1 Hafta içinde

sizi ne ölçüde rahatsız ettiğini göz önünde tutarak, yandaki sütundaki rakamlardan birini

işaretleyiniz. Hiçbir soruyu atlamayınız.

0 Hayır hiç 1 Biraz 2 Orta Derecede 3 Fazla 4 Çok Fazla 1. Baş Ağrıları 0 1 2 3 4

2. Sinirlilik veya içinizin titrediği hissi 0 1 2 3 4

3. Kafanızdan atamadığınız, tekrarlayan, hoşa gitmeyen düşünce ve sözcükleri

0 1 2 3 4

4. Baygınlık hissi veya baş dönmesi 0 1 2 3 4

5. Cinsel ilgi, istek ya da hazda azalma 0 1 2 3 4

6. Başkalarını eleştirmeye yatkınlık 0 1 2 3 4

7. Herhangi birinin düşüncelerini yönetecebileceğiniz hissi 0 1 2 3 4

8. Zorluk ve sıkıntılarınızdan başkalarından sorumlu olduğu duygusu

0 1 2 3 4

9. Hafıza zayıflığı, hatırlamada güçlük 0 1 2 3 4

10. Sakarlık, dikkatsizlik veya ihmallerin sizi rahatsız etmesi

0 1 2 3 4

11. Kolayca sinirlenme veya huzursuz olma 0 1 2 3 4

12. Kalp veya göğüs üzerinde ağrı 0 1 2 3 4

13. Cadde veya açık alanlarda korku duyma 0 1 2 3 4

14. Enerji, güç azalması, hareket ve düşüncede yavaşlama 0 1 2 3 4

15. Yaşamınıza kendi elinizle son verme düşüncesi 0 1 2 3 4

16. Başkalarının duymadığı sesler işitme 0 1 2 3 4

17. Titreme 0 1 2 3 4

18. İnsanların çoğuna güvenilemeyeceği duygusu 0 1 2 3 4

Page 141: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

127

19. İştahsızlık 0 1 2 3 4

20. Ağlamaya yatkınlık 0 1 2 3 4

21. Karşı cinsle ilişkilerde çekingenlik, çaresizlik 0 1 2 3 4

22. Tuzağa düşürülme, kapana kıstırılma duygusu 0 1 2 3 4

23. Nedensiz ani korkular 0 1 2 3 4

24. Kontrol edemeyeceğiniz öfke nöbeti ve duygusal patlamalar

0 1 2 3 4

25. Yalnız olarak evden çıkmaktan korku duyma 0 1 2 3 4

26. Bazı konularda kendini suçlama eğilimi 0 1 2 3 4

27. Bel ağrıları 0 1 2 3 4

28. Herhangi bir işe başlamada ve sürdürmede zolanma hissi

0 1 2 3 4

29. Yalnızlık hissi 0 1 2 3 4

30. Hüzün, iç sıkıntısı 0 1 2 3 4

31. Gereğinden çok tasalanma ve endişelenme 0 1 2 3 4

32. Hiçbir şeye ilgi duymama 0 1 2 3 4

33. Ürkeklik, korku duyma 0 1 2 3 4

34. Duygularınızın kolayca incinebilmesi ve alınganlık 0 1 2 3 4

35. Özel ve gizli düşüncelerinizin başkaları tarafından bilindiği hissi

0 1 2 3 4

36. Başkalarının sizi anlamadığı ve size ilgisiz olduğu duygusu

0 1 2 3 4

37. Başkalarının size dostça davranmadığı, sizden hoşlanmadığı duygusu

0 1 2 3 4

38. Doğru ve eksiksiz olmasını garantilemek için herşeyi çok yavaş yapma gereksinimi

0 1 2 3 4

39. Kalbinizin çok hızlı atması veya çarpması 0 1 2 3 4

40. Midede nahoş duygular ya da bulantı 0 1 2 3 4

41. Başkaları karşısında aşağılık duygusu 0 1 2 3 4

42. Kas ağrı ve sızıları 0 1 2 3 4

43. Başkalarının sizi gözlediği veya hakkınızda konuştuğu duygusu

0 1 2 3 4

44. Uykuya dalmakta güçlük çekme 0 1 2 3 4

45. Yaptığınız işleri tekrar tekrar kontrol etme zorunluluğu hissetme

0 1 2 3 4

Page 142: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

128

46. Karar vermede güçlük çekme 0 1 2 3 4

47. Otobüs, trende yolculuk etmekten korkma 0 1 2 3 4

48. Nefes almada güçlük çekme 0 1 2 3 4

49. Nöbetler şeklinde ateş basması veya buz kesmesi 0 1 2 3 4

50. Sizi korkuttuğu için belirli olay, yer ve nesnelerden uzak durma

0 1 2 3 4

51. Zihinde boşluk duygusu 0 1 2 3 4

52. Bedeninizin çeşitli yerlerinde hissizlik, uyuşma veya karıncalanma

0 1 2 3 4

53. Boğazınızda yumru tıkandığı hissi 0 1 2 3 4

54. Gelecekle ilgili umutsuzluk 0 1 2 3 4

55. Dikkatinizi toplamada güçlük çekme 0 1 2 3 4

56. Bedeninizin bazı kısımlarında güçsüzlük 0 1 2 3 4

57. Gerginlik veya tedirginlik hissi 0 1 2 3 4

58. Kol ve bacaklarda ağırlık hissi 0 1 2 3 4

59. Ölüm veya ölmekle ile ilgili düşünceler 0 1 2 3 4

60. Aşırı yemek yeme 0 1 2 3 4

61.Başkaları size bakarken, hakkınızda konuşurken huzursuzluk ve rahatsızlık duyma

0 1 2 3 4

62. Aklınıza size ait olmayan düşüncelerin gelmesi 0 1 2 3 4

63. Birisine zarar ve acı verme, dövme, yaralama isteği 0 1 2 3 4

64. Sabahları erken uyanma 0 1 2 3 4

65.Dokunma, sayma ve yıkama gibi davranışları zorunluluk hissederek tekrarlama

0 1 2 3 4

66. Huzursuz, rahatsız uyku veya uykunuzun bölünmesi 0 1 2 3 4

67. Birşeyleri kırmak veya parçalamak için dayanılmaz istek duyma

0 1 2 3 4

68.Başkalarının paylaşmadığı düşünce, görüş ve inançlarının olması

0 1 2 3 4

69. Başkaları ile birlikteyken konuşma ve davranışlarınıza dikkat etme zorunluluğu hissetme

0 1 2 3 4

70. Sinema ve alışveriste olduğu gibi kalabalıktan

huzursuzluk duyma ve kaçınma

0 1 2 3 4

71. Herşeyin çok zor ve yorucu olduğu duygusu 0 1 2 3 4

Page 143: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

129

72. Dehşet ve paniğe kapılma nöbetleri 0 1 2 3 4

73. Topluluk içinde yiyip içerken husursuzluk duyma 0 1 2 3 4

74. Sık sık tartışmalara girme ya da iddialaşma 0 1 2 3 4

75. Yalnız kaldığınızda sinirlilik veya huzursuzluk hissi 0 1 2 3 4

76. Başarılarınızın başkaları tarafından yeterince takdir edilmediği hissi

0 1 2 3 4

77. İnsanlarla birlikteyken bile yalnızlık duyma 0 1 2 3 4

78. Yerinizde duramayacak ölçüde huzursuzluk hissi 0 1 2 3 4

79. Değersizlik duyguları 0 1 2 3 4

80. Başınıza kötü birşey geleceği hissi 0 1 2 3 4

81. Yüksek sesle bağırma veya birşeyler fırlatma hissi 0 1 2 3 4

82. Topluluk içinde bayılmaktan korkma 0 1 2 3 4

83. Eğer fırsat verirseniz insanların sizi kullanacağı duygusu

0 1 2 3 4

84. Cinsellikle ilgili oldukça rahatsız edici hayal, düşünce ve duygularınızın olması

0 1 2 3 4

85. Suç ve günahlarınızdan dolayı cezalandırılmanız gerektiği düşüncesi

0 1 2 3 4

86. Dehşet veya korku uyandıran düşünce ve hayaller 0 1 2 3 4

87. Bedeninizde ciddi bir bozukluk olduğu düşüncesi 0 1 2 3 4

88. Başka birine karşı gerçek bir yakınlık duymama hissi 0 1 2 3 4

89. Suçluluk duygusu 0 1 2 3 4

90. Aklınızda herhangi bir bozukluk olduğu düşüncesi 0 1 2 3 4

Page 144: RECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS IN …etd.lib.metu.edu.tr/upload/12608450/index.pdfRECOGNITION OF EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSION IN ALCOHOL DEPENDENT INPATIENTS Dursun, Pınar

130

Appendix F

SAMPLE of BASIC EMOTIONAL FACIAL EXPRESSIONS

ANGRY SAD NEUTRAL

FEARFUL HAPPY DISGUSTED

SURPRISED