psychological processes in cooperative language learning

12
Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning: Group Dynamics and Motivation ZOLTAN DORNYEI Department of English Applied Linguistics Eiituos University Ajtdsi Durer sor 19 Budapest 1 146, Hungary Email: [email protected]. hu Cooperative learning (CL) has been found to be a highly effective instructional approach in education in general and this has been confirmed with regard to second language (L2) learn- ing as well. This article investigates reasons for the success of CL from a psychological per- spective,focusing on two interrelated processes: the unique group dynamics of CL classes and the motivational system generated by peer cooperation. It is argued that the affective domain of CL plays a crucial role in the educational potential of the method. This paper summarizes the specific factors that contribute to the promotion of learning gains. While the analysis con- cerns cooperatively structured learning only, it is assumed that the processes described have a broader relevance to understanding the success of peer collaboration in general. COOPERATIVE LEARNING (CL), THE IN- structional use of small groups in order to achieve common learning goals via cooperation, has made an almost unprecedented impact in ed- ucation during the last two decades. According toJohnson, Johnson, and Smith (1995),CL is one of the most thoroughly researched areas in edu- cational psychology. As they assert, We know more about cooperative learning than we know about lecturing, age grouping, departmental- ization, starting reading at age six, or the 50-minute period. We know more about cooperative learning than about almost any other aspect of education. The explanation for this interest in CL is very simple: Investigations have almost invariably in- dicated that CL is a highly effective classroom in- tervention, superior to most traditional forms of instruction in terms of producing learning gains and student achievement, higher-order thinking, (p. 4) The Modern LanguageJournal, 81, iv, (1997) 0 1997 Tiu ModPrn I.anguage Journal 002S-7902/97/482-49S $1.50/0 positive attitudes toward learning, increased mo- tivation, better teacher-student and student-stu- dent relationships accompanied by more devel- oped interpersonal skills and higher self-esteem on the part of the students. Furthermore, CL ap- pears to be applicable “with some confidence at every grade level, in every subject area, and with any task” (Johnson et al., 1995, p. 4). Although there were attempts to introduce CL principles in language instruction over 15 years ago (Gunderson &Johnson, 1980), CL has only recently become an area of major interest in the L2 field. By now a fairly solid body of literature has accumulated, including two edited volumes containing a rich selection of conceptual and re- search studies (Holt, 1993; Kessler, 1992),a num- ber of journal articles (e.g., Bejarano, 1987; Chang & Smith, 1991; Jacob, Rottenberg, Pat- rick, & Wheeler, 1996; Milleret, 1992; Szostek, 1994), as well as some practical language teach- ing materials specifically developed for the pur- pose of CL (e.g., Coelho, Winer, & Olsen, 1989). In addition, in her well-known book on small group work, Cohen (1994) devotes a whole chap- ter to discussing the bilingual classroom, which includes foreign language classes.

Upload: others

Post on 01-May-2022

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning: Group Dynamics and Motivation ZOLTAN DORNYEI Department of English Applied Linguistics Eiituos University Ajtdsi Durer sor 19 Budapest 1 146, Hungary Email: [email protected]. hu

Cooperative learning (CL) has been found to be a highly effective instructional approach in education in general and this has been confirmed with regard to second language (L2) learn- ing as well. This article investigates reasons for the success of CL from a psychological per- spective, focusing on two interrelated processes: the unique group dynamics of CL classes and the motivational system generated by peer cooperation. It is argued that the affective domain of CL plays a crucial role in the educational potential of the method. This paper summarizes the specific factors that contribute to the promotion of learning gains. While the analysis con- cerns cooperatively structured learning only, it is assumed that the processes described have a broader relevance to understanding the success of peer collaboration in general.

COOPERATIVE LEARNING (CL), THE IN- structional use of small groups in order to achieve common learning goals via cooperation, has made an almost unprecedented impact in ed- ucation during the last two decades. According to Johnson, Johnson, and Smith (1995), CL is one of the most thoroughly researched areas in edu- cational psychology. As they assert,

We know more about cooperative learning than we know about lecturing, age grouping, departmental- ization, starting reading at age six, or the 50-minute period. We know more about cooperative learning than about almost any other aspect of education.

The explanation for this interest in CL is very simple: Investigations have almost invariably in- dicated that CL is a highly effective classroom in- tervention, superior to most traditional forms of instruction in terms of producing learning gains and student achievement, higher-order thinking,

(p. 4)

The Modern LanguageJournal, 81, iv, (1997)

0 1997 Tiu ModPrn I.anguage Journal 002S-7902/97/482-49S $1.50/0

positive attitudes toward learning, increased mo- tivation, better teacher-student and student-stu- dent relationships accompanied by more devel- oped interpersonal skills and higher self-esteem on the part of the students. Furthermore, CL ap- pears to be applicable “with some confidence at every grade level, in every subject area, and with any task” (Johnson et al., 1995, p. 4).

Although there were attempts to introduce CL principles in language instruction over 15 years ago (Gunderson &Johnson, 1980), CL has only recently become an area of major interest in the L2 field. By now a fairly solid body of literature has accumulated, including two edited volumes containing a rich selection of conceptual and re- search studies (Holt, 1993; Kessler, 1992), a num- ber of journal articles (e.g., Bejarano, 1987; Chang & Smith, 1991; Jacob, Rottenberg, Pat- rick, & Wheeler, 1996; Milleret, 1992; Szostek, 1994), as well as some practical language teach- ing materials specifically developed for the pur- pose of CL (e.g., Coelho, Winer, & Olsen, 1989). In addition, in her well-known book on small group work, Cohen (1994) devotes a whole chap- ter to discussing the bilingual classroom, which includes foreign language classes.

Page 2: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

7d tan Dilnya

An interesting question is why L2 researchers have only started to discover CL relatively re- cently. One reason is that small group work has been part of 1 2 methodology for a long time and, therefore, CL may have appeared to offer noth- ing startlingly new. However, typical group work activities associated with communicative Ian- guage teaching are not equivalent to CL, because the small group format is not the essence of CL. While it is true that communicative group work (such as role play or problem-solving tasks) is a prerequisite to CL and frequently embodies cer- tain CL principles, small group activities in L2 classes often are not cooperative in nature, or they underutilize CL principles.

There are many ways of looking at cooperative language learning. The focus of this paper is not the analysis of how group interaction inherent to CL promotes the acquisition of the L2. For this, the reader is referred to the excellent summaries by McGroarty (1993) and Kagan and McGroarty (1993), and the seminal paper of Long and Porter (1985) that describes how the increased amount and variety of target language output and input in group work facilitates L2 develop- ment. Rather, I argue that by focusing on the psy- chological dimension of CL, we can find the key to its effectiveness in the affective domain. I highlight two interrelated psychological processes underlying CL which, I propose, contribute sig- nificantly to the outstanding learning potential of the method: (a) the unique group dynamics inherent to the CL process that generate a sup- portive learning environment characterized by strong cohesiveness among learners, and (b) the motivational basis of CL which underlies student achievement gains.

It is interesting to note that the approach I have taken in this paper bears a close resemblance to a framework recently set up by Slavin (1996), who identified three major theoretical perspectives to explain the achievement effects of CL: motiva- tional, social cohesion, and cognitive. The first two perspectives are a direct match for the two psychological processes in this article. The cog- nitive perspective “holds that interactions among students will in themselves increase student achievement for reasons which have to do with mental processing of information rather than with motivations” (Slavin, 1996, p. 48). Thus, the cognitive perspective is analogue to increased language learning through interaction, cited above.

The focal issue of this article-the psychologi- cal processes forming the affective foundation of cooperative language learning-is relatively

483

“subject-matter-free,’’ which means that these processes are not so much dependent on the ac- tual target of learning (i.e., the mastery of the L2) as on more basic components of the learning process such as the relationships and interactions among learners and the psychological processes involved. Much of the research support for the arguments made here is drawn from first lan- guage ( L l ) contexts, without conclusive evi- dence that the results are directly transferrable to the L2 field. Although the increasing amount of knowledge about CL in L2 classrooms (see ref- erences above) has generally confirmed the va- lidity of the claims made in nonlanguage class- rooms with regard to language learning, more focused research is needed to decide to what ex- tent nonnative speakers from different cultures and with different cultural expectations about student and teacher roles, group work, and inter- personal communication respond to CL in the same way as the (primarily North American and Israeli) first language users among whom the ap- proach has been developed. It would also be in- teresting to see whether cooperative language learning works equally well with, for example, adults in evening language courses as with pupils in ordinary school contexts.

COOPERATIVE LEARNING IN A NUTSHELL

Cooperative learning has been defined in dif- ferent ways and implies several related methods of organizing and conducting classroom instruc- tion. However, three key components of CL make a learning approach “cooperative.” First, learners spend most of the class time working in small groups of between 3 and 6 students. Sec- ond, learning is structured so that group mem- bers are motivated to ensure that their peers have also mastered the material or achieved the in- structional goal, and therefore an intensive process of cooperation is generated, involving various creative collaborative learning strategies. Third, evaluating and rewarding the group’s achievement in a CL class becomes as important as or more important than evaluating and re- warding individual achievement.

A good way to understand the essence of CL is by contrasting it with competitive and individualis- tic classroom structures. In the former, only the best students are rewarded so that students are forced to work against each other in an attempt to outdo their classmates. This situation encour- ages the survival of the fittest. Competitive learn- ing can be characterized by a negative interde-

Page 3: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

484

pendence among students. The learners’ goals are “so linked that there is a negative correlation among their goal attainments” (Deutsch, 1962, p. 276). In an individualistic classroom structure, by contrast, there is no interdependence. Students are required to work independently and the probability of achieving a goal or reward is nei- ther diminished nor enhanced by the presence of a capable other. The cooperative classroom, on the other hand, is characterized by a positive in- terdependence of the students. As Johnson et al. (1995) summarize, positive interdependence oc- curs ”when one perceives that one is linked with others in a way so that one cannot succeed unless they do (and vice versa) and/or one must coor- dinate one’s efforts with the efforts of others to complete a task” (p. 31). In other words, positive interdependence is the belief that students “sink or swim together” (p. 31).

Positive interdependence results in promotive interaction, which can be defined as “individuals encouraging and facilitating each other’s efforts to achieve and complete tasks, and produce in order to reach the group’s goals” (Johnson et al., 1995, p. 20). Students are divided into small groups and learning takes place in these basic so- cial units through peer teaching, joint problem solving, brainstorming, and varied interpersonal communication, as well as through individual study monitored by peers. Thus, in a cooperative learning situation, as the name indicates, every- thing is centered around the process of coopera- tion, that is, giving and receiving ideas and clarif- ication, providing task-related help and assistance, exchanging needed resources (e.g., information or materials), and providing constructive feed- back (Johnson et al., 1995; Sharan, 1995).

The key question is, how can positive interde- pendence be achieved? That is, how can learners be “motivated to cooperate? Olsen and Kagan (1992) list five principal ways to accomplish CL structures:

The Modern Language Journal 81 (1 997)

which need to be fitted together (i.e., the jig- saw procedure).

5. Structuring rules Setting rules that emphasize the shared nature of responsibility for the group product (e.g., no one can proceed to some new project or material before every other group member has completed the previ- ous assignment).

Besides positive interdependence, Johnson and Johnson (1995) mention three other condi- tions necessary for the effectiveness of CL: indi- vidual accountability, mastery of social skills, and regular group processing. They argue that CL works best when the group rewards for learning are combined with individual accountability in order to ensure that participants perform their share of the work. In these CL formats, students are individually quizzed and receive recognition based on the sum of all team members’ scores.

Social skills may need to be taught; simply plac- ing students in a learning group and expecting them to cooperate effectively may not be success- ful. As Johnson and Johnson state, “We are not born instinctively knowing how to interact effec- tively with others. Interpersonal and group skills do not magically appear when they are needed” (1995, p. 122). This is particularly true in ethno- linguistically heterogeneous L2 classrooms, where the cultural dissimilarity among the students is a further source of divergence in the skills necessary for high quality cooperation, such as leadership, decision making, trust building, communication, and conf lict-management skills. (For classroom recommendations on how to teach these skills to students by open modeling and controlled prac- tice, see Cohen, 1994; Ehrman 8c DGrnyei, in press).

Finally, according to Johnson and Johnson (1995), effective group work is influenced by whether groups regularly reflect on how they are functioning, what has been conducive to com- pleting the tasks, and how they should continue or change-thus engaging in group processing. Such processing enhances group maintenance, facilitates acquisition and practice of social skills, reminds members of the group norms, and gives members feedback on their participation.

1. Structuring the goal: Groups work towards a sin- gle team product (e.g., joint performance).

2. Structuringthe reuards: In addition to individual scores or grades, some sort of team score is also calculated and joint rewards or grades are given for the group’s overall production.

3. Structuring student roles: Assigning different roles to every group member so that everybody has a specific responsibility (e.g., “explainer,” “summarizer,” or “note- taker”).

4. Structuring materials. Either limiting resources so that they must be shared (e.g., one answer sheet for the whole group) or giving out re- sources (e.g., worksheets, information sheets)

GROUP DYNAMICS IN COOPERATIVE LEARNING

CL has been rooted in a social psychological approach to the study of small groups. Its inno- vation and strength lies almost entirely in the conscious and systematic exploitation of the principles of group dynamics to enhance student

Page 4: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

Zoltan Dornyei

learning outcomes. In this section, a group dy- namics-based analysis of CL is applied in order to see how the necessary conditions of interaction and cooperation develop.’

Group dynamics concerns the analysis of the behavior of small groups, generally about 4 to 20 members (for more detailed overviews of the prin- ciples of group dynamics from a L2 perspective, see Dbrnyei & Malderez, 1997; Ehrman & Dbrnyei, in press).‘ The educational applicabil- ity of group dynamics rests on three factors:

1 . Most organized learning occurs in some kind of group (e.g., classes, seminars, workshops, discussion groups).

2. Group characteristics and group processes sig- nificantly contribute to success or failure in the classroom and directly effect the quality and quantity of learning within the group.

3. Theoretical and practical knowledge about group dynamics might assist teachers to create learning environments where learning is a re- warding and efficient experience. An aware- ness of the principles of group dynamics can also help teachers to make classroom events less threatening, develop more efficient class- room management, and develop creative, well balanced, and cohesive groups.

485

Croup Cohesiueness and Instructed Ixnguage Learning

One concept central to the explanation of many group-related phenomena is group cohe- siveness, or “the strength of relationship linking the members to one another and to the group it- self‘ (Forsyth, 1990, p. 10). It is an index of the level of group development, directly related to within-group cooperation and to both the qual- i ty and quantity of group interaction (see Bar-Tal & Bar-Tal, 1986; Greene, 1989; Shaw, 1981). Three recent meta-analyses of past studies addressing the relationship between group cohesiveness and group performance found a significant positive relationship between the two variables, indicat- ing that cohesive groups, on average, tend to be more productive than noncohesive groups (Evans & Dion, 1991; Gully, Devine, & Whitney, 1995; Mullen & Copper, 1994).

The cohesiveness-performance effect can be particularly strong in language classes in which the learners’ communicative skills are developed primarily through participatory experience in real world language tasks. In these contexts, com- munication is unfolded and enlivened in positive relationships, and the warm, cohesive group cli-

mate significantly enhances peer interaction. In- deed, Levine and Moreland’s (1990) review of the literature confirms that members of a cohe- sive group are more likely than others to partici- pate actively in conversations and engage in self- disclosure or collaborative narration, which are student behaviors necessary for efficient com- municative task involvement. In addition to pro- moting interaction, cohesiveness also mediates the effects of CL on achievement because “stu- dents will help one another because they care about one another and want one another to suc- ceed” (Slavin, 1996, p. 46).

The Development of Group Cohesiueness

In view of the arguments above, group cohe- siveness is one of the most important attributes of the successful communicative language class. It is therefore particularly critical for language teach- ers to understand how it evolves among learners.

Group cohesiveness develops gradually through- out the existence of the group. The amount of time spent together and the shared group history are key factors that tend to develop stronger in- termember ties. By far the most crucial ways of consciously fostering cohesiveness is to help stu- dents learn about each other by sharing genuine personal information. Acceptance of another person does not occur without getting to know that person well; enemy images and a lack of tol- erance often stem from insufficient information about the other party.

In addition to getting to know one another, more concrete factors can also enhance affilia- tion (see Dornyei & Malderez, 1997; Ehrman & Dornyei, in press; Johnson & Johnson, 1995; Levine & Moreland, 1990; Shaw, 1981; Turner, 1984; for practical group-building tasks for the L2 classroom, see Hadfield, 1992):

1. Proximity, or physical closeness (e.g., sitting next to each other), which is a necessary con- dition for the formation of relationships.

2. Contact in situations where individuals can meet and communicate (e.g., cafeterias and other relaxation areas, outings and other ex- tracurricular activities, as well as in class op- portunities).

3. Interaction in which the behavior of each per- son influences the others’ (e.g., group activi- ties, project work).

4. Cooperation between members for common goals (e.g., to accomplish group tasks).

5. The rmarding nature ofgroup expm’ence for the individual; rewards may involve the enjoy- ment of the activities, approval of the goals,

Page 5: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

486

\uccess in goal attainment, and personal in- strumental benefits.

6. Succmjid completzon of whole grmp tasks and a sense of group achievement.

7. Joznt hardshzp that group members have expe- rienced (e.g., carrying out some difficult task together).

8. Intmgroup competataon (e.g., games in which groups compete); this has been found to bring together members of small groups.

9. Common threat, which can involve, for example, the feeling of fellowship before a difficult exam.

10. Group legends, which are an efficient way of “pumping up group pride” (Mullen & C o p per, 1994, p. 224); these may involve building up d kind of group mythology, giving the group d name, and inventing characteristics for the group.

11. Initesting tn the group to create cohesiveness. 12. Publzc commitment to the group to strengthen a

Sense of belonging. 13. Definzng the group agaznst another, that is, em-

phasiLing the distinction between “us” and “them,” a powerful but potentially dangerous aspect of cohesiveness.

The Modern Language Journal 81 ( 1 997)

should adopt a more “democratic” teaching style and be “prepared to step aside to give the learner a meaningful role” (McDonell, 1992, p. 169), only intervening when necessary. As Ehrman and Dornyei (in press) summarize, democratic lead- ers involve the group members in decision mak- ing about their own functioning, share with them the long-term goals and steps to be taken to achieve these, and take part in the activities themselves. That is, they consciously distribute influence and promote learner autonomy: Stu- dents are given positions and tasks ofgenuine au- thority, are invited to design and prepare activi- ties themselves, and are encouraged to take part in project work and peer teaching.

Group cohesiveness is also fostered by leader- ship and teaching styles. The way leaders live out their role and encourage a feeling of warmth and acceptance can also enhance group cohesive- ness. Kellerman (1981) argues that a prerequisite tor any group with a high level of cohesiveness is a leader whose presence is continuously and strongly felt: “highly cohesive groups are those in which the leader symholiLes group concerns and identity and is personally visible to the member- ship” (p. 16). Indeed, one of the surestways ofun- dermining the cohesiveness of a group is for the leader to be absent, either physically or psycho- logically (the latter idea referring to insufficient care for the group and its goals).

In addition, an efficient group leader’s task is riot so much to lead the group but rather to facil- itate it, that is, to create the right conditions for development, in particular a safe and accepting climate, and to enable the group to do away with any emerging obstacles. Seen from this perspec- tive, the traditional autocratic teaching style, whereby the teacher makes virtually all the deci- +ns, dictating policy and actions, never dis- cussing the schedule or asking for input from the members, is an obstacle to group development because it does not allow for the group to struc- ture itself organically, or for the members to share increasing responsibility. The instructor who aims to be conscious of group dynamics

Cohesiveness und the CL Process

Nearly all reports on CL projects highlight the improved interrelations among students and be- tween the students and the teacher. Researchers assert that CL is particularly effective in creating cohesive groups (see Johnson &Johnson, 1995). This increased group cohesiveness can be ex- plained by three main reasons.

First, CL methodology consciously recognizes the importance of team building, emphasizing the necessity of spending initial time training CL skills such as building trust, providing leader- ship, and managing conflicts. CL also contains regular self-evaluation, which ensures that any potential intermember tensions are properly processed.

Second, the emerging cohesiveness in CL class- rooms is also the function of the special dynam- ics of the CL process, which organically includes several of the cohesiveness-promoting factors listed above. The small group format and the positive interdependence among students pro- vide proximity, contact, and interaction. Promo- tive interaction through coordination and com- munication requires understanding each participant’s needs, interests, and abilities, and results in knowing each other on a personal level, “rather than as complexes of performances (what persons do)” (Johnson &Johnson, 1995, p. 104). Although there is usually no joint hardship or common threat, CL formats often include inter- group competition. Because of the supportive environment and a lack of face-threatening com- petition, group experience for learners is typi- cally rewarding. Because the CL process centers on the successful completion of group tasks, stu- dent satisfaction is further enhanced. Further- more, as Johnson and Johnson argue, promotive interaction includes the public commitment to

Page 6: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

Zoltan Diirnyei

accomplish the group goals as well as consider- able investment of time and energy toward this, which results in a growing attachment to the task and the group.

Third, students in cooperatively structured classes are in control of organizing their own learning, that is, there is considerable learner au- tonomy. The dominant small group format sim- ply excludes the teacher from the primary stu- dent communication networks and considerably decentralizes the decision-making process in the classroom, providing learners with an opportu- nity for self-regulation (Sharan & Shaulov, 1990). Learner autonomy is also ensured by the teacher’s democratic teaching style, mentioned earlier.

In sum, based on the numerous factors pro- moting cohesiveness listed above, we may con- clude that the dynamics of the CL process s u p port the main conditions for organic group development and the emergence of a mature, well-balanced internal class structure, character- iLed by strong student cohesiveness. This cohe- siveness is a strong mediator of CL processes on learning outcomes because it is a necessary re- quirement for communicative task involvement, and, as discussed below, it also fosters student mo- tivation to learn.

487

important impact of the CL process on learner motivation occurs at the learning situation level, but continuous exposure might influence motiva- tional processes at the learner level as well.

THE MOTIVATIONAL BASIS OF STUDENT ACHIEVEMENT IN COOPERATIVE LEARNING

The superior task performance and learning achievement repeatedly observed with students in cooperatively structured classrooms (for re- views, see Cohen, 1994; Johnson & Johnson, 1995) would not occur without a powerful moti- vational basis energizing the CL process. Coop- erative goal structure and the learning format that characteriLe CL generate a special motiva- t ional system, which is largely responsible for the efficiency of CL.

Dornyei (1994) argues that the motivational complex underlying instructed L2 learning is a multidimensional construct comprising at least three fairly independent levels: (a) the language level (concerning ethnolinguisl ic, cultural-affec- tive, intellectual, and pragmatic values and atti- tudes attached to the target language and its speakers); (b) the learner level (concerning vari- ous fairly stable personality traits that the learner has developed in the past); and (c) the learning situation level (concerning situation-specific mo- t ives rooted in various aspects of language learn- ing in a classroom setting) (see Table 1). The most

Motivational Components at the Learning Situa- tion Level: GroupSpeciific Motives

Swezey, Meltzer, and Salas (1994) point out that most theories of motivation attempt to explain motivational processes at the individual level, even though action conducted within groups might show motivational characteristics that stem from the group as a social unit rather than from the individual members. Because in educa- tional contexts this claim appears to be particu- larly valid, Dornyei’s (1994) model of L2 motiva- tion includes a set of group-specific motivational components related to four aspects of group dy- namics: classroom goal structures, group cohe- sion, goal-orientedness, and the norm and reward system. Let us examine how these group proper- ties affect motivation in cooperative classrooms.

Classroom Goal Structure. The classroom goal structure in CL is centered around positive inter- dependence and the resulting process of cooper- ation. L2 studies investigating the motivational role of cooperativeness confirm the positive ef- fect found in L1 classrooms. In her conceptual analysis, Ushioda (1996) concludes that collabo- rative learning in itself can create the appropri- ate psychological conditions for intrinsic moti- vation. Julkunen’s (1989) investigation of the effects of competitive, individualistic, and coop erative goal structures on L2 motivation sup- ports the superiority of CL. Julkunen and Bor- zova’s (1996) results indicate that students tend to prefer CL situations to individualistic and competitive ones, and the researchers also found a significant positive relationship between coop- erative goal structures and various aspects of L2 learning motivation.

Group Cohesiveness. The fact that group cohe- siveness has a positive impact on further motiva- tion to learn was made explicit by Deutsch (1962) in his initial theory of cooperation, which stated that promotively oriented groups would show more achievement pressure. In a summary of re- search on the effect of cohesiveness on learner dispositions and behaviors, Johnson and Johnson (1995) verified this assumption. Furthermore, in their study of classroom motivation to learn a L2, Clement, Dornyei, and Noels (1994) confirmed that group cohesiveness is indeed an important component of L2 motivation. This might be due

Page 7: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

488

TAR1.E 1 Components of Foreign Language Learning Motivation

IANMJAGE LEVEL Integrative Motivational Subsystem

IXARNER LEVEL Need for Achievement

The Modern Language Journal X I (1 997)

Instrumental Motivational Subsystem

Self-confidence” - Language Use Anxiety* - Perceived L2 Competence - Causal Attributions* - Self-Efficacy*

LEARNING SITUATION LEVEL Course-SpeciJic Motivational Cornfionents

Interest‘ Relevance Expectancy* Satisfaction*

T?iic)m-L~peczjic Motivational Components

Affiliative Drive Authority Type* Direct Socialization of Motivation

- Modeling -Task Presentation -Feedback

Croup-SpeciJir Motavatzonal Goal-Orientedness* Components Norm & Reward System*

Group Cohesion* Classroom Goal Structure’

Not?. Adapted from Diirnyei, 1994, p. 280. * Components assumed to be affected by the CL-generated motivational system.

t o a sense of obligation and moral responsibility to the group. The group’s goal-oriented norms have a strong influence on the individual. In co- hesive groups, the likelihood of “social loafing” and “free-riding’’ (i.e., doing very little actual work while still reaping the benefits of the team’s performance) decreases. In addition, positive re- lations make the learning process more enjoy- able, thus promoting intrinsic motivation.

Goal-OriPntedness. The extent to which the group is attuned to pursuing its goal (in our case, L2 learning) is referred to as “goal-orientedness.” In school contexts the “official group goal” (mas- tering the L2) may not be a goal at all. Further- more, members may not show the same degree of commitment to the group goal. However, due to the positive interdependence among students in CL, we can expect individual and group goals to converge more than in other educational con- texts. This has been supported by Nichols and Miller (1994), who found that students in CL classes were more goal-oriented than students participating in traditional instruction.

Norm and Reward System. One aspect of positive interdependence is the fact that rewards are con- tingent upon group performance. As a result, be- cause the whole group benefits from a member’s high academic achievement, we do not find the common peer pressure against doing academic work in cooperatively structured classrooms. The “norm of mediocrity,” which exists in many edu- cational contexts, results in learners suffering so- cial consequences for academic success, which is reflected in labeling hard-working students “teacher’s pet,” “nerd,” or “brain” (Daniels, 1994, p. 1011). In contrast, Daniels found that the norm system of CL results in learners gaining so- cial approval for academic excellence and help- ing each other to achieve this. This type of norm system exerts a powerful influence on group members’ attitudes, values, and actions. Students in cooperatively structured classrooms are moti- vated to excel by their need for social approval and by the wish to avoid negative sanctions for not doing their fair share in working towards group success (Ames & Ames, 1984; Johnson & Johnson, 1995).

Page 8: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

Zoltan Dornyn

Teacher-Specific Motives

The primary teacher-specific motive in CL concerns the teacher’s authority type. As de- scribed before, CL is typically accompanied by a democratic leadership style that fosters learner autonomy. Research shows that although control- ling classroom contexts may result in higher short-term productivity, autonomy-supporting classroom contexts lead to a higher level of long- term, intrinsic motivation (see, for example, Ames, 1992; Deci, 1992; Klein, Erchul, & Pride- more, 1994; Swezey et al., 1994; Sharan & Shaulov, 1990). Indeed, many researchers share Paris and Turner’s (1994) assertion that “The essence of motivated action is the ability to choose among alternative courses of action, or at least, to choose to expend varying degrees of ef- fort for a particular purpose” (p. 222).

The claim that autonomy is at the core of the motivation to learn is also central to Deci and Ryan’s (1985) influential “self-determination” theory. According to Deci and Ryan, the need for autonomy is an innate human need, referring to the desire to be self-initiating and self-regulating of one’s actions. Therefore self-determination, that is, engaging in an activity “with a full sense of wanting, choosing, and personal endorse- ment” (Deci, 1992, p. 44), is a prerequisite for any behavior to be intrinsically rewarding. CL pro- vides a learning environment that fully supports self-determination on the part of the students. The following summary of the educational rele- vance of self-determination by Deci, Vallenard, Pelletrier, and Ryan (1991) applies almost liter- ally to C L

The specific supports for self-determination we sug- gest include offering choice, minimizing controls, ac- knowledging feelings and making available informa- tion that is needed for decision making and for performing the target task. With a general attitude of valuing children’s autonomy and by providing the type of autonomy support just mentioned, we stand the greatest chance of bringing about the types of ed- ucational contexts that facilitate conceptual under- standing, flexible problem solving, personal adjust- ment, and social responsibility. (p. 342)

Learner autonomy has been shown to exert a significant positive impact on motivation in L2 contexts as well. In her extensive discussion of learner autonomy and L2 motivation, Ushioda (1996) concludes that autonomy and motivation go hand in hand: “Autonomous language learn- ers are by definition motivated learners” (p. 2). In another recent study, Dickinson (1995) also makes the initial assumption that an aclive, inde-

489

pendent attitude to learning and personal in- volvement in decision making leads to increased L2 motivation. Dickinson’s review of relevant mo- tivational studies confirms this view:

It has been shown that there is substantial evidence from cognitive motivational studies that learning suc- cess and enhanced motivation is conditional on learners taking responsibility for their own learning, being able to control their own learning and perceiv- ing that their learning successes and failures are to be attributed to their own efforts and strategies rather than to factors outside their control. Each of these conditions is a characteristic of learner autonomy as it is described in applied linguistics. (pp. 173-174)

Course-Speczfic Motives

The CL process has been found to increase three of the four components associated with course-specific motivation in Dhrnyei’s (1994) construct: intrinsic interest, expectancy, and sat- isfaction. The increased student interest in the learning process stems from several sources, many of which have been discussed earlier: more varied and dynamic tasks, greater task involve- ment, the pleasure of working in a cohesive group, the self-determined nature of learning, and the informational feedback received from the peers. The expectancy of successful task ful- filment is enhanced by the group serving as a “re- source pool that is greater in any given area than the resources possessed by any single member” (Douglas, 1983, p. 189)-that is, students know that they can count on their peers when in trou- ble. The satisfaction that students experience after they complete a task successfully is in- creased by the shared experience and the joint celebration. This has been confirmed by Van Oostrum and Rabbie’s (1995) experiment, in which cooperative groups reported higher satis- faction about their obtained results and perfor- mance than learners in competitively structured classes. In addition, Szostek (1994) has found in a L2 context that a great deal of the satisfaction group members feel comes from the success of coaching, teaching, drilling, and helping each other to learn.

Motivational Processes at the Learner Level: Self-Confidence

Almost every report on the outcomes of CL highlights some kind of improvement in the learners’ self esteem, self-efficacy, and confidence, often as the result of changes in the learners’ at- tributional system or a decrease in the language

Page 9: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

490

anxiety they experience. Following C l h e n t (1980; Clkment & Kruidenier, 1985; Labrie & C l h e n t , 1986; Cli.ment et al., 1994), these factors have been subsumed under a broad motivational process, linguistic self-confidence, in Dhrnyei’s (1994) model.

According to Covington’s self-worth theory of achievement motivation (Covington & Roberts, 1994), the highest human priority is the need for self-acceptance and therefore “in reality, the dy- namics of school achievement largely reflect at- tempts to aggrandize and protect self-percep- tions of ability” (p. 161). A primary feature of CL is that i t avoids any social comparison of individ- uals and thus students do not equate their worth with the ability to achieve competitively. Rather, as Ames and Ames (1984) argue, the student’s willingness to put forth effort serves as a primary criterion in the evaluation of behavior. The ab- sence of the detrimental effects of social com- parison, accompanied by the increased peer ac- ceptance and support that exists in cohesive groups, is expected to result in higher percep- tion of ability, self-worth, and academic self-es- teem (ix., self-efficacy) in CL groups than in tra- ditional classes. This was demonstrated by Johnson, Johnson, and Taylor’s (1993) and Nichols and Miller’s (1994) empirical studies.

Enhanced self-confidence in cooperatively structured classroonis is also a function of the at- tributional focus of such environments. The guid- ing principle in the attribution theory of motiva- tion is that people search for understanding, asking “why” questions to explain their past suc- cesses and failures. These explanations, in turn, play a central role in the determination of future achievement. Failure that is ascribed to stable and uncontrollable factors such as low ability decreases the expectation of future success more than fail- lire that is ascribed to controllable factors such as cffort ( Weiner, 1979). Ames and Ames (1984) argue that whereas in a competitive classroom structure the focus is on ability, in a coop- erative setting the attributional focus is on effort and intent. This enhances achievement behavior through the learners’ increased self-confidence.

A final reason for increased self-confidence is the Fact that cooperation typically generates less anxiety and stress than other learning formats (Deci & Ryan, 1985). This is partly caused by the positive emotional tone that characterizes CL. and partly by the increased level of self-determi- nation. In addition, Johnson et al. (1995) point out that cooperation also produces more effec- tive coping strategies to deal with anxiety than does competition.

The Modern Language Journal 81 (1 997)

A Summary of Motivation in Cooperative Language Learning

The overview of the motivational basis of co- operative language learning presented above helps us understand the consistently favorable af- fective impact of CL on L2-related attitudes and motivation. This positive influence was the cen- tral theme of the first L2 study on CL (Gunder- son &Johnson, 1980), in which the authors con- cluded:

Perhaps the most basic instructional objective in a foreign language class is to send students away with at least as favorable an attitude toward learning the lan- guage as they had when they first arrived in the class- room. Certainly, students who finish one foreign lan- guage course should wish to take the second. While competitive and individualistic learning do have their place, the use of cooperative learning groups is an im- portant teaching strategy for promoting positive atti- tudes toward learning a foreign language. (p. 43)

As argued earlier, the consistency of improved student attitudes and motivation observed in CL contexts suggests that the CL process generates a specific motivational system that energizes learn- ing. In Table 1 the components activated by this system are marked with an asterisk. The un- marked components are not necessarily irrele- vant, but they are not assumed to be enhanced by CL in particular; for example, learners in a CL class may or may not be instrumentally moti- vated. The number of different motivational as- pects which CL significantly affects explains the remarkable results obtained in a major study on the role of motivation in CIA by Sharan and Shaulov (1990), who found that more than half of the variance in achievement in three academic subjects was caused by the “motivation to learn” factor. Such a substantial impact is very rare in motivation studies in general and is due to the fact that the motivational system promoted within cooperative situations (a) mediates be- tween achievement and several significant inde- pendent variables related to the unique social structure of the CL classroom, and (b) consider- ably enhances the learners’ achievement-related self-concept. Thus, we may conclude that from a motivational point ofview CL is undoubtedly one of the most efficient instructional methods.

CONCLUSION

As stated earlier, the strength of CL lies in the small group learning format accompanied by positive interdependence among the learners, re- sulting in intensive interaction and a process of

Page 10: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

Zoltdn DiLnya’

cooperation. CL, in fact, can be seen as the learn- ing process which best maximizes the beneficial effects of peer collaboration. By emphasizing the students’ active participation in constructing their own knowledge, CL signifies a major depar- ture from traditional educational contexts where “instruction is still largely viewed as a vast delivery service whose task is to deliver a completed man- ufactured product to the consumer” (Sharan & Shaulov, 1990, p. 196). Indeed, CL can also be viewed as an instructional approach that fully re- alizes the principles of an emerging student-cen- tered teaching paradigm (Johnson et al., 1995).

This article examined the psychological foun- dations of the success of cooperatively structured learning. Two interrelated processes, the group dynamics of CL classes and the motivational sys- tem generated by peer collaboration, were dis- cussed. It was argued that CL tends to produce a group structure (including peer relationships and learning norms) and a motivational basis that provide excellent conditions for L2 learning. In a CL class we would see motivated students en- gaged in varied interactions while working inten- sively towards completing group tasks-features that are considered crucial for efficient commu- nicative L2 classes. Indeed, Coelho (1992) draws attention to the striking similarities between co- operative group skills emphasized by CL and L2 functions emphasized by communicative lan- guage teaching and argues that CL can provide the foundation for communicative language cur- riculum design.

Finally, I emphasize that although the discus- sion in this article focused entirely on coopera- tively structured learning, the processes de- scribed are not restricted to CL but are also characteristic of any student collaboration, group work, or team work in general, as CL is only viewed as the learning format which maximizes student collaboration. Therefore, an understand- ing of the “deep structure” of CL can help us un- derstand some of the fundamental processes and concepts underlying modern language teaching methodology.

49 1

4 Group dynamics is a relatively young field in the so- cial sciences, overlapping with disciplines such as so- cial, industrial, organizational and clinical psychology, psychiatry, anthropology, sociology, and social work, since all these fields involve groups as focal points around which human relationships are organized.

NOTES

I In this article I follow the use of the term “group dy- namics” as established in social psychology and include the entire language class under ‘‘group.” Therefore, “group dynamics” in this paper concerns not only small group work within the class hut also whole classroom dynamics.

REFERENCES

Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms, goals, structures, and stu- dent motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology,

Ames, C., & Ames, R. (1984). Systems of student and teacher motivation: Toward a qualitative defini- tion. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 535- 556.

Bar-Tal, Y., & Bar-Tal, D. (1986). Social psychological analysis of classroom interaction. In R. S. Feld- man (Ed.), The social psychology of education: Cur- rent research and theory (pp. 132-149). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Bejarano, Y. (1987). A cooperative small-group meth- odology in the language classroom. TESOL @ar-

Chang, K.-Y. R., & Smith, W. F. (1991). Cooperative learning and CALL/IVD in beginning Spanish: An experiment. Modern Language Journal, 75,

Clement, R. (1980). Ethnicity, contact and communica- tive competence in a second language. In H. Giles, W. P. Robinson, & P. M. Smith (Eds.), Lan- guage: Social psychologtcal perspectives (pp. 147- 154). Oxford, England: Pergamon.

Cltment, R., Dornyei, Z., & Noels, K. (1994). Motiva- tion, self-confidence and group cohesion in the foreign language classroom. Language Learning,

Clement, R., & Kruidenier, B. G. (1985). Aptitude, atti- tude and motivation in second language profi- ciency: A test of Clement’s model.Journa1 of Lan- guage and Social Psychology, 4, 21-37.

Coelho, E. (1992). Cooperative learning: Foundation for a communicative curriculum. In C. Kessler (Ed.), Cooperative language learning: A teacher’s re- source book (pp. 31-49). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Coelho, E., Winer, L., & Olsen, J. W.-B. (1989). All sides of the issue: Activities for cooperative jigsaw groups. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Alemany Press.

Cohen, E. (1994). Designing groupwork (2nd ed.). New York: Teachers College Press.

Covington, M. V., & Roberts, B. W. (1994). Self-worth and college achievement: Motivational and per- sonality correlates. In P. R. Pintrich, D. R. Brown, & C. E. Weinstein (Eds.), Student motivation, cog- nition, and learning (pp. 157-187). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Daniels, R. (1994). Motivational mediators of coopera- tive learning. Psychologzcal Reports, 74, 101 1-1022.

84,267-271.

terly, 21, 483-504.

205-211.

44,417-448.

Page 11: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

492

Deci, E. L. (1992). The relation of interest to the moti- vation of behavior: A self-determination theory perspective. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and d e velopmat (pp. 43-70). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

I>eci, E. L., &Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and sel/dtermination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.

Ileci, E. L., Vallenard, R. J., Pelletrier, L. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). Motivation and education: The self- determination perspective. Educational Psycholc-

Deutsch, M. (1962). An experimental study of the ef- fects of cooperation and competition upon group process. Human Relations, 2, 199-231.

Dickinson, I.. (1995). Autonomy and motivation: A lit- erature review. System, 23, 165-174.

IXirnyei, 2. (1994). Motivation and motivating in the foreign language classroom. Modern Language Journal, 78,273-284.

Dornyei, Z., & Malderez, A. (1997). Group dynamics and foreign language teaching. System, 25,65-81.

I)ouglas, T. (1983). Groups: Understandingpeople gathered together. London: Fdvistock Press.

Ehrman, M. E., & Dornyei, Z. (in press). The visible and invisible classroom: Interpersonal and group dynamics in second language education. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Evans, C. R., & Dion, K. I.. (1991). Group cohesion and performance: A meta-analysis. Small Group fi- search, 22, 175-186.

Forsyth, D. R. (1990). Group dynamics (2nd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Greene, C. N. (1989). Cohesion and productivity in small groups. Small Group Behaviol; 20, 70-86.

(hlly, S. M . , Devine, D. J., & Whitney, D. J. (1995). A meta-analysis of cohesion and performance: Ef- fects on level of analysis and task interdepen- dence. Small Group Research, 26,497-520.

Gunderson, B., &Johnson, D. W. (1980). Building posi- tive attitudes by using cooperative learning groups. Foreign Language Annals, 13,39-43.

Hadfield, J. (1992). Classroom dynamics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

I-lolt, D. D. (Ed.). (1993). Cooperative learning. Washing- ton, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics & ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics.

Jacob, E., Rottenberg, L., Patrick, S., & Wheeler, E. (1996). Cooperative learning: Context and op- portunities for acquiring academic English. TESOI. Quarterly, 30,253-280.

Johnson, D. W., &Johnson, R. T. (1995). Cooperative learning and nonacademic outcomes of school- ing. In J. E. Pedersen & A. D. Digby (Eds.), Sec- ondary schools and roqberative learning (pp. 81- 150). New York: Garland.

Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Smith, K. A. (1995). Cooperative learning and individual student achievement in secondary schools. In J. E. Peder- sen & A. D. Digby (Eds.), Secondary schools and ro- operative learning (pp. 3-54). New York: Garland.

$st, 26, 325-346.

The Modern Language Journal 81 (1 997)

Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Taylor, B. (1993). Im- pact of cooperative and individualistic learning on high-ability students’ achievement, self-esteem, and social acceptance. Journal of Social Psychology,

Julkunen, K. (1989). Situation- and task-specific motivation in fweign language learning and teaching. Joensuu, Finland: University of Joensuu.

Julkunen, K., & Borzova, H. (1996). English language learning motivation in Joensuu and Petrozavodsk. Joensuu, Finland: University of Joensuu.

Kagan, S., & McGroarty, M. (1993). Principles of c o o p erative learning for language and content gains. In D. D. Holt (Ed.), Cooperative learning (pp. 47- 66). Washington, DC: Center for Applied Lin- guistics & ERIC Clearinghouse on Languages and Linguistics.

Kellerman, H. (1981). The deep structures of group co- hesion. In H. Kellerman (Ed.), Group cohesion: Theoretical and clinical perspectives (pp. 3-21). New York: Grune & Stratton.

Kessler, C. (Ed.). (1992). Cooperative language learning: A teach’s resource book. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Pren- tice Hall Regents.

Klein, J. D., Erchul, J. A,, & Pridemore, D. R. (1994). Ef- fects of individual versus cooperative learning and type of reward on performance and continu- ing motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychol-

Labrie, N., & CICment, R. (1986). Ethnolinguistic vital- ity, self-confidence and second language profi- ciency: An investigation. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 7, 269-282.

Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. I,. (1990). Progress in small group research. Annual h i m ojPsychology,

Long, M. H., & Porter, P. A. (1985). Group work, inter- language talk, and second language acquisition. TESOL Quarterly, 19,207-228.

McDonell, W. (1992). The role of the teacher in the co- operative learning classroom. In C. Kessler (Ed.), Cooperative language learning: A teacher’s resource book (pp. 163-174). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

McGroarty, M. (1993). Cooperative learning and sec- ond language acquisition. In D. D. Holt (Ed.), Co- operative learning (pp. 19-46). Washington, DC: Center for Applied Linguistics & ERIC Clearing- house on Languages and Linguistics.

Milleret, M. (1992). Cooperative learning in the Por- tuguese for Spanish speakers classroom. Foreign Language Annals, 25,435-440.

Mullen, B., & Copper, C. (1994). The relationship be- tween group cohesiveness and performance: An integration. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 210-227.

Nichols, J. D., & Miller, R. B. (1994). Cooperative learn- ing and student motivation. Contemporary Educa- tional Psychology, 19, 167-178.

Olsen, R. E. W.-B., & Kagan, S. (1992). About coopera- tive learning. In C. Kessler (Ed.), Cooperative lan- guage learning: A teacher’s resource book (pp. 1-30). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

133,839-844.

OD, I9,24-32.

41,585-634.

Page 12: Psychological Processes in Cooperative Language Learning

Zoltrin Dornyei

Paris, S. G., &Turner, J. C. (1994). Situated motivation. In P. R. Pintrich, D. R. Brown, & C. E. Weinstein (Eds.), Student motivation, cognition, and learning (pp. 213-237). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Sharan, S. (1995). Group investigation: Theoretical foundations. In J. E. Pedersen & A. D. Digby (Eds.), Secondary schools and cooperative learning (pp. 251-277). New York: Garland.

Sharan, S., & Shaulov, A. (1990). Cooperative learning, motivation to learn, and academic achievement. In S. Sharan (Ed.), Coqberative learning: Theory and research (pp. 173-202). New York: Praeger.

Shaw, M. E. (1981). Ckmp dynamics: The psychology o j smallgroup behavior (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw- Hill.

Slavin, R. E. (1996). Research on cooperative learning and achievement: What we know, what we need to know. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21,43- 69.

Swezey, R. W., Meltzer, A. L., & Salas, E. (1994). Some is- sues involved in motivating teams. In H. F. O'Neil

493

& M. Drillings (Eds.), Motivation: Theory and re- search (pp. 141-169). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Szostek, C. (1994). Assessing the effects of cooperative learning in an honors foreign language class- room. Foreign Language Annals, 27,252-261.

Turner, J. C. (1984). Social identification and psycho- logical group formation. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), The social dimension: European studies in social psychology (pp. 518-538). Cambridge: Cambridge Univer- sity Press.

Ushioda, E. (1996). Learner autonomy 5: The role ofrnoli- vation. Dublin: Authentik.

Van Oostrum, J., & Rabbie, J. M. (1995). Intergroup competition and cooperation within autocratic and democratic management regimes. Small Group Research, 26, 269-295.

Weiner, B. (1979). A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational Psy- chology, 71, 3-25.

National Foreign Language Center Announces Advanced Studies/Mellon Fellowship Competition ~

THE NATIONAL FOREIGN LANGUAGE CENTERS (NFLC) INSTITUTE OF ADVANCED STUDIES invites proposals for a limited number of fellowships for the 1998-99 academic year. Proposals may be for quantitative or qualitative research documenting the pedagogical, cognitive, and behavioral as- pects of language learning; the effectiveness of curricular designs, materials, delivery modes, and ped- agogies; and the methods of teacher preparation. The six thematic areas that constitute the research emphases for the 1998-99 academic year fellowships are:

-Technology and Language Learning -Foreign Language Aquisition and Teaching Methodologies -Language Learning in Immersion Environments -Testing and Assessment -Heritage Language Learners -Culture and Language Learning

Post-l)octorulFeZlows in the following categories: (a) analysis and write-up of existing data; (b) data col- lection during the time of the fellowship; (c) team-based research projects, with one or two of the re- searchers working at the Center while collaborating with their institutional-based researchers; and (d) Faculty development grants to undertake research that addresses an aspect of an institutional language program.

StudentFellow for doctoral candidates at the dissertation stage or immediate post-doctoral students who seek to complete and/or continue their dissertation research.

For details of residency requirements, stipends, and application procedures, write to:

Institute of Advanced Studies National Foreign Language Center, Suite 400 1619 Massachusetts Avenue, NW Washington, DC 20036

Proposals must be postmarked no later than January 31, 1998.