on free word order phenomena in czech as compared to ... · p. ko'il~. on free word order...

15
ZfSl 51 (200!» 3. 3116-320 Peter Kosta On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to German: Is Clause Internal Scrambling A-Movement, A-Bar-Movement or Is It Base Generated?* Summary This article deals \\l1h the i,sue of Scralllhlillg as a syntactic phenomenon \\hlcl1 imol\es the hasL' gL'nera- tion of scrambled elements in their positions ami their Lf mO\Tmenl 10 positions \\here they recci\c thcta-roles, In the jirsl "ccl/oli thc hasic dichotomy in the eunent t(1\\ards SClWllhlilig \\"i1l be analyzcd. namely (i) the movement and (ii) the hase generation According tCl lil. there is one un- derlying word order amI the \'ariety of alternate \\"md order arrangel1lents in a clause is thL' rewlt 01' A- \'S, A-har l1lo\ement. ACCl1rding to (ii), there is l1(1t one hasic order for constitLICl1h anci the \anahle \\llrd l1rcier is the result 01' frec generation 01' constituents in an arhitrary order. In the ,,'c,'ulld I'0rl we adopt thc idea (lI' Bayer Kornlilt (1994) that Scramhling 01' unrocused "lPs is l1(1t due to mon:mcnt ((lr Attracr alpha) a! hut rather is base generated ami a subreet NP can also he Iiccnseci Insidc 01' a VI', I. Il1lrodlicliol1 The present artiele is dedicatcd to a phenomcnon whieh 1.'\ Cl' since Ross (1967) has bcen refcrrcd to as S'cl'ilmhlillg. Ross proposcd that I'rcc \\ord ordcr in languages such as Warlpiri. Latin. Gcrman. Japanese. Korean or Russian is brought about hy a S'cl'ilmhlillg rulc that was orden:d late in the hloek oftransformations and I'ollowed the ordinary phrase strueture rules. case marking. agreement. retlexiviLation and pronominalization transfor- mations that free and fixed word order langllages \\cre assumed to have in COmml1/l. Czech is a language that exhihits a rieh morplllllogical system 01' ease Illarking. Eaeh grammatical relation in a sentcnee is usually encoded by a partieular case: subjeets are most otien in the Nominati\ e. direct ohjccts in thc Acellsative. and indireet ohjccts in thc Dative. As a result. arguments ean uSlIally he 1'reely reordcrcd i1'they arc clausc mates: ( I) a. ... I'rolo::e l1ikdo :Jejll/(; knihu l1ektilfpil hceause nohody(nom) prohahly book(aee) not bought b.. , prolo::e knihu lIikdo ::I'cjlll(; I IIcko/lpil bceause hook(ace) nobody(nolll) prohably not bought In languagcs with free word order the frccdom 01' \\ord order is assuilled tn rcllcct thc in- formation structure nfa sentcnee on a eommunieative level (cr. Mehlhnrt1 2002: Kosta and Sehüreks 2006/in prep,l. In traditional dcscriptive 01' 1'unetional grammars the most otien diseusscd factor in dctermining Russian \\ord order is the so-ealled Theme/Rhemc- distinetion (or Given/Ne\\ information: ef c,g, Adamee 1966: Km tun()\ a 1976: Krylova and Chavronina 1984). The ThemciRheme-distinetion plays a major role in explaining the Dieser Artikel ist dem Andenken \on Prof. PhDr, Pr-cmysl Adame<:. I)rSc .. gc\\idmer. der mich durch seine theoretische Arhell auf dem Gebiet der und funktionalen Syntax der russischen Sprache stark geprägt hat. Seine persönliche Ausstrahlung. mcnschlichc Wiirrne und freundschaft \\erden mir killen,

Upload: others

Post on 01-Sep-2019

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

ZfSl 51 (200!» 3. 3116-320

Peter Kosta

On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to German: Is Clause Internal Scrambling A-Movement, A-Bar-Movement or Is It Base Generated?*

Summary

This article deals \\l1h the i,sue of Scralllhlillg as a syntactic phenomenon \\hlcl1 imol\es the hasL' gL'nera­tion of scrambled elements in their surt~lce positions ami their Lf mO\Tmenl 10 positions \\here they recci\c thcta-roles, In the jirsl "ccl/oli thc hasic dichotomy in the eunent appro~1ches t(1\\ards SClWllhlilig \\"i1l be analyzcd. namely (i) the movement and (ii) the hase generation ~1pproach, According tCl lil. there is one un­derlying word order amI the \'ariety of alternate \\"md order arrangel1lents in a clause is thL' rewlt 01' A- \'S, A-har l1lo\ement. ACCl1rding to (ii), there is l1(1t one hasic order for constitLICl1h anci the \anahle \\llrd l1rcier is the result 01' frec generation 01' constituents in an arhitrary order. In the ,,'c,'ulld I'0rl we adopt thc idea (lI' Bayer Kornlilt (1994) that Scramhling 01' unrocused "lPs is l1(1t due to mon:mcnt ((lr Attracr alpha) a! ~Jil

hut rather is base generated ami a subreet NP can also he Iiccnseci Insidc 01' a VI',

I. Il1lrodlicliol1

The present artiele is dedicatcd to a phenomcnon whieh 1.'\ Cl' since Ross (1967) has bcen refcrrcd to as S'cl'ilmhlillg. Ross proposcd that I'rcc \\ord ordcr in languages such as Warlpiri. Latin. Gcrman. Japanese. Korean or Russian is brought about hy a S'cl'ilmhlillg rulc that was orden:d late in the hloek oftransformations and I'ollowed the ordinary phrase strueture rules. case marking. agreement. retlexiviLation and pronominalization transfor­mations that free and fixed word order langllages \\cre assumed to have in COmml1/l. Czech is a language that exhihits a rieh morplllllogical system 01' ease Illarking. Eaeh grammatical relation in a sentcnee is usually encoded by a partieular case: subjeets are most otien in the Nominati\ e. direct ohjccts in thc Acellsative. and indireet ohjccts in thc Dative. As a result. arguments ean uSlIally he 1'reely reordcrcd i1'they arc clausc mates:

( I) a . ... I'rolo::e l1ikdo :Jejll/(; knihu l1ektilfpil

hceause nohody(nom) prohahly book(aee) not bought b.. ,prolo::e knihu lIikdo ::I'cjlll(; I IIcko/lpil

bceause hook(ace) nobody(nolll) prohably not bought

In languagcs with free word order the frccdom 01' \\ord order is assuilled tn rcllcct thc in­formation structure nfa sentcnee on a eommunieative level (cr. Mehlhnrt1 2002: Kosta and Sehüreks 2006/in prep,l. In traditional dcscriptive 01' 1'unetional grammars the most otien diseusscd factor in dctermining Russian \\ord order is the so-ealled Theme/Rhemc­distinetion (or Given/Ne\\ information: ef c,g, Adamee 1966: Km tun()\ a 1976: Krylova and Chavronina 1984). The ThemciRheme-distinetion plays a major role in explaining the

Dieser Artikel ist dem Andenken \on Prof. PhDr, Pr-cmysl Adame<:. I)rSc .. gc\\idmer. der mich durch seine theoretische Arhell auf dem Gebiet der f()rm~I\en und funktionalen Syntax der russischen Sprache stark geprägt hat. Seine persönliche Ausstrahlung. mcnschlichc Wiirrne und freundschaft \\erden mir killen,

Page 2: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

307 P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared t() Gennan

syntactic properties of Russian ward order in recent approaches \\ithin the generative Principles-and-Parameters theory and its extension. Minimalism (cr. King 1995: Bailyn 1995: Kondrashova 1994: Junghanns and Zybatow 1997: Sekerina 1995: Kosta 1997; Kosta 2002: Kosta and Schürcks 2006/in prep.: Mehlhom 2002). This article deals with the issue of Scrumh/ing as syntactic phenomenon which involves thc base generation of scrambled elements in their surface positions and their LF move­ment to positions where they receive theta-roles. In thefirst section the basic dichotomy in the current approaches towards Scrumh/ing will be analyzed. namely (i) the movcment and (i i) the base generation approach. According to (i). there is one underlying word order and the variety 01' alternate word order arrange­ments in a clause is the result 01' A- vs. A-bar movemenl. According to (ii). there is not one basic order for constituents and the variable word order is the result of free generation 01' constituents in an arbitrary order.

I. I. Scramh/ing os A-har-m01'emcnt

Some standard approaches on Scrambling assume that Scrambling is an installCe of op­tional ovcrt A' -movemenl. Thus. there is one underlying \\ ord order and the variety 01' al­ternate structures is thought to be the result 01' Move-Alpha by adjunction an XP (NP. pp. AP or ADY) to IP 01' YP. In the abo\e sentences (1 (1) would be eonsidered to be the base­generated word order whilc (I b) is assumed to be derived by so me type of Move-Alpha. The hase-generation analysis generatcs both constituents orders in the förmer approaches at the level of D-structure. in other words the major constituents do not have '1 lixed syn­taetic position at D-structure (cr Corver/van Riemsdijk 19(4).

I. I. I. Scr(//lIhling ond Islonc!\'

Under a movement analysis. it is generally assumed lhat the direcl objecl NP is adjacent to the verb at D-structure. ti'om whieh il reeeivcs a theta roJe under sisterhood. The basic word order and the derivcd word order are derived by some senten ce internal movement operation Icading to a syntactic chain. The relevant question lhat ariscs is whcther the scrambled eonstitucnt hcads an A-chain or an A' -chain. Under a movement approach. one would expeet scrambling to display propcrlics generally associated with movement­derived struelures. There must be an antecedent-trace relation: this relation is apparently unbounded: Scrambling obeys island constraints. Webelhuth (1989) has shown that Scrambling in German are sensitive lo Ross's (1967) island constraints on movement transformations. The following ill-formed German sentences illustrate the sensitivity 01' Scrambling to island effeets such as the Left Branch Condition (2'1). the Coordinate Struc­lure Constraint (2b). thc PP-island condition (cxamples takcn from Webelhulh 1989: cL also Corvcr/van Riemsdijk 1994:3passim):

(2a) *... weil meines Bmders gestern (- - Auto! gestohlen l\'lInle ... because my brother' s yesterday car stolen was

(2b) *... \\'eil Hansjemand [- -lind .'vfario! angemeldet hat ... because Hans somebody and \1ary regislered

(2c) *... \I eil ihre Freiheit die Lellte lange [li'ir - - jgekiilllpji hahen bccause their freedom the people long for fought have

Page 3: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

308 ZfSI 51 (2006) 3

Ifwe compare Czech with German the same sensitivity to island effects arises:

(3a) * prolo::'e meho bratra vcera bylo ukradeno [aUIO - -] (3b) * proloie Petra nekdo [ - - a Marii} phhläsil (3c) * proloie svou H'obodu lide dlouho [pro - -] bojol'Oli

J. /.2. Wh-movemenl, Topicali:.aliol11'S. Scrambling

Ross's (1967) initially 1'ormulated dcscriptive generalization that Scrambling is clause bound exhibits for example Gennan. Thus, in contrast to Wh-movemcnt or topicalization. a finite CP may never be crossed by a scrambled constituent in languages like German:

(4a) Wen, glaubl Hans, dass Maria lieh I I,. (Wh-movement) Who believes Hans that Mary love

(4b) DEN HANS, glaubl Maria:'11 liehen I, (Topicalization) It is Hans, that believes Mary to love

(4c) *... weil Hans den Wagen versprochen hOl, dass er /- - reparieren 'würde] (Scrambling) ... because Hans thc car pramissed has that hc repair would

The same distribution of clause-intemal Serambling vs. clause unbound movel11ent exhib­its Czech:

(5a) Koho, Pell' 1II1·sli,::'e .\forie miluje I, (K-movement) (Sb) PETRA, Maric m\'Sli, ze mil1lje I, (Topicalization) (Sc) * .. .proloze Pclr di:. slihi!. ze hl' /opral'il - -] (Scrambling)

J. /.3. Long Dislancc S'cl'i/mh!ing (LDS)

In German, howevcr, therc is a construction in which the object-NP can bc scrambled 01' infinitival :'/I-eomplements into thc matrix-IP:

(6) ... ,reil Heinrich dcn Wagen, n'l'sprochcn haI [PRO C,:'1I repariacn! ... because Heinrich the car promised has to \\ ash (LOS)

A sil11ilar elTect can be seen in (7) with an embedded intinitival complement and a matrix contral verb in Czech:

(7) .. proloze Pclr vii:., slibil [PRO c, oprm'ill(LOS)

If one compares thc German data in (4) vs. (6) or the Czeeh data in (5) \'s. (7) one is likely to say that in special sentences (with a limited lexical class of contra I \ erbs) Gerlllan and Czech exhibit somcthing Iike Long Oistance Scrambl ing (LOS) \\here an object- NP can be scrambled 01' infinitival complement clauses. To show that an analysis of LOS as A-bar-l11ovement cannot be thc appropriate one let us eonsider the 1'ollowing data trom Baycr/Komtilt (1994:26) O~ a thraugh c) and its Czech equivalenls (9a thraugh cl.

Page 4: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

309 P. KOST~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to German

First, consider the German case in which we move an adverb of quantification from its D­structure position in the complement into the matrix c1ause (8a through 8c(

(8a) ... weil Heinrich versprochen hat [PRO dreimal den Rosenkranz ZII beten) because Heinrich promissed has three-times the rosary to pray

(8b) [Dreimal], hat Heinrich (e,J versprochen [PRO e, den Rosenkranz zu beten) (ambiguous) A' -movement

(8c) ... weil Heinrich [dreimal], versprochen hat [PRO (*e,J den Rosenkranz zu be­ten) (unambiguous) LDS

Let (8a) be the underived D-structure where the adverb of quantification takes scope only over the act of praying, thus having narrow scope. Bayer/Komfilt (1994:26) takes (8b) to be a clear case of A' -movement The adverb of quantification dreimal can bind a trace in­side the complement, and we thus get the reading of "three prayers", which (8b) shares with (8a). The second alternative structure is that dreimal is binding a trace inside the ma­trix IP; following this structure we would get a reading according to which there are "three promises". This creates the ambiguity. Assuming now that Scrambl ing is adjunction to YP. the same ambiguity should be available in (8c). However, the example (8c) is clearly unal1lbiguous. providing us only with the interpretation of three promiscs. Following Bayer's and Kornfilt's (1994:26) explanation we can conclude that LOS is very unlikely to bc a case of A' -movemenL Gur argument is the following: Adverbs of quantification can move to an A' -position. with all the conscquences of A' -movemcnt as in (Sb). If LDS werc A' -movement, these adverbs should behave syntactically as in A' -movement How­ever. they do not as illustrated in (Sc). In thc next exal1lplc I would like to show that the same holds true for the analogous Czech sentences (9a through cl:

(9a) .. .proto::e Petr s/ihil [PRO tNkrät se modlit rli::enecj (9b) .. .[TNkrät], Petr (e,) slihil jPRO e, modlit sc I'li::enec] A'-movcmcnt (9c) .. proto::e Petr tNkrät slihil [PRO (*e,) modlit sc rzi::cnecj LOS

/./.4 BOI/nd pronollns

Mahajan (1990) has shown that Scrambling in Hindi (unlike Quantifiel' Raising (QR) 01'

instances of Wh-movement that applics only at LF) does not exhibit the weak crossover eflects (WCG) that are typical for A' -movcment, and that Scral1lbling does not allow tor reconstruction. Note that reconstruction is usually possible in instances of A' -movement, thus avoiding apparent violations of the binding theory. Consider thc tollowing cxamples from German and Czech which involvc a co indexation between a definite NP and a pos­sessIve pronoun:

Adjllnction to VP (name-like binder)

(IOa) Wir wollten jdem ProFessor}, seine, Sekretärin vorstellen (lOb) Wir wollten *seine, Sekretärin j dem ProFessor}, vorstellen (I Oe) [Seine, Sekretärin}1 hahen 'wir [dem ProFessor}, CI noch nicht vorgestellt

All (Jennan examples laken rann Bayer!Komtllt (1994: 17-(0).

Page 5: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

310 ZfSl51 (2006)3

In (1 Oa) the Dative NP (being in its base-generated position) can bind the possessive pra­noun seine. (lOb), however. which is a nonnal case of (sentence internal) object scram­bling. does not a110w this binding. (I Oe) is a clear case of A' -movement yielding a well­fonned sentence. In this sentence the NP, seine Sekretärin has moved to SpecCP. which is always an operator position in Gennan. In contrast to (lOb). in (1 Oe) the moved NPi seine

can be bound becausc it can rcconstruct into the trace position at LF. The binding NP i can c-command NPi (cr. Bayer/Cornfilt 1994: 19). If thc case of Scrambling in (lOb) were an instance of A' -movemcnt. wc would wrangly predict that rcconstruction is possible here.

ctoo . The same contrast betwecn Scrambling and A' -movemcnt is shown undcr ( 11 b) vs. (1Ic) in Czech:

(11 a) Ch!eli/11J11' [projesorm'i],/JI'l'dl!Q\'i!jelw, sekrl'!äi'ku

(11 b) Ch!Nijsml' *jl'ho, sl'krl'/(/i'ku [PI'O!E'.I·OI'O\'i, JIJI'I'c!.I!(I\'I·! (I I c) [Jeho, sl'krl'!(i,'klllJI'1J11' pl'Ojesol'O\'i, ::u!lmjl"i!c' l1epI'e(iI'!{/\'ili 1';

1.1. 5. A 11 Upl10 rs

Considcr ncxt thc binding propcrtics of anaphors (in Czcch C.g. S\'llj.I\,(1. S\'(; and its in­flected forms):

(12a) ...pl'O!o::e Pdr, \'/'(/!il svemll;/*;!Jiillm'i psaj Peter gavc back to his land lord the dog

(12b) .. PI'O!o::1' PI'!r, \ni!il psaj sl'emu;/; p(il1m., Pcter gave back the dog tCl his land lord

Thc cxample (12ab) shows that DO-argument shi ft alters binding possibil ities and the landing sitcs ror argumcnt shift arc rclc\ ant to binding thcory. As alrcady dcmonstratcd. rellexive binding in (12b) can only be possiblc ifthe 00 moves into a position that allows thc anaphor (10) to be bound by its antecedent (00). c. g. under C-command. This seems to be an A-position rat her than an ;\. -position. This asymmetry between 10 anci 00 can be accounted 1'01' by assUll1ing that thc DO C-coll1ll1ands the 10. thus creating the inter­pretation that thc dog bclongcd to the landlord. Thus. DO fronting can ll1ake reflexivc binding possible. Rellexive binding in (I 2b) can only be possible if the 00 is in an A­relatcd position.' The same pattern scems to hold in Gcrman:

( I3a) \\'I'il PI'!l'r, Sl'il1I'III, '; Herl'l7 dl'n ! IUl1d; ::uriick guh

(13b) \\'eil PI'!l'r, dl'l1 Hund; Sl'iI1I'III,; Herl'l7 ::uriick guh

Baycr/Komiilt ( IlJ1J4:S0. Fn. 4) strcss that there is a crucial distinclion hcn: hetwccn "moycmcnt to the preverhal position in V2-struUures" (as in Ic. our IOc) ami standard Scramhling construction. The formcr type is not a type 01' a gcnuine Scramhl ing construction. rathcr it shows movcmcnt 10 an A'­position in SpecCP and hence is an inslance 01' \10ve-!\lpha. Anoop MahaJall ( IlJlJ430 I) assulllcs that this position mthcr than hcing a pureA-position is a L-rclated position. These are ... "speciiier and cOlllplement positions 01' a lexical itcm and 1'unclional heads lexically related to it. Within the c1ausal system it inclll(ks SPFC and Complement positions 01' V. A(iR and T." Non-L-rL'iated po,itions are "all other pusitions that are not-L-related. These will include SPEC ep and all adjunction positions."

Page 6: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

311 P K, >\T '\. On Free Ward Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to German

The same seems to hold true tor Hindi and other languages whieh obey Serambling 01'00 . ~

as 0rlion,

I, 1.0. SCreJlI7hling os adjunct;on

As a contrast to the above mentioned examrl~s 01' DO-shit! notiee that PP-adjunets ean freely adjoin to VI' or 11' as demonstrated under (14a, ISa through 14c ISc), The adjunc­tion 01' the moved PP to these A' -positions is able to reconstruct into a trace-position as indicated in the following representations:

(14a) ... doss /in sc;ner, Wohnung]; .I!ar;a den Proji.;ssor,c; schon o/i hcsucht hat that in his arartment Maria the professor alre<1dy orten visited has "that Maria has ottcn visited the rrofessor in hIS apartment"

( 14b) , " dass iv!ar;a (111 seincr, Wohnung); dcn Proji.;s.wr, CI schon o/i nesucht llOt (14c) .. dass /;n sciner, WOII/71l11g!1 der Pmji.;I·S(i/', schon o/i C; \'On .\!aria Iwsucht

11'lIrde

(ISa) .. , :':e /1' jcllO, InNJ; Petr IJroji.;,\01'O, CI 11:': nl;koliknltnU\'(th'il (15b) ... :':c Petr (''jeho, !J,}e'}l!Jro/i.'soreJ, C; 11::' lIL'kolikrcitna"I'rh'il (I Sc) ". :':c hd */"jeho, hl't(i}; i/l'c .1\'(:111, h,'ti"I prok.lor, 11:': lIe'ko!ikrät n(/\',I't/I'CIl

Perrelll

As indicated by the traces. there is al\\ <1ys a \\ay 01' reeonstructing the PP into <1 rosition \\here the prol\olln scilll'r1jcl1O is C-coll1manded by thc NP Professor. Une dilTerence that dcscends from idiosyneratie rropertics 01' C7ech rronollns is the t~let that an antecedent in nominative has to bind its anaphor .I\"'j, S\"(I, sl'e; in its intleclL'd forms from the subjeet po­sition. The reading l'jef/() would in this case only refer to Peter ur to an antecedent outside the sentence.'

1.1. 7 fJmso(!I'

An ub\iollS surt~lce (PF) rellex 01' the difference het\\cen SCr<Jmbling as substitution and Scrambling as adjunction tu 11' has been ohsel'\ed by Bayer/Kornfilt (}994:23 forthcom­ingl. They statcd that an adjunction to 11' creates a rrosodic brcak that is clearly absent in thc shirt uf an objcct-NP, Thcy present sumc clear cxamrlcs \\hich show the ditTcrcnce bct\\ccn German Serambling and English adjunctioJ\ to IP:

( 16a) dass dCII ! [c;nrh'h niellland olisstelll'1I kOlln that the Heinrich nobody stand can "that nohody ean stand Ilcinrich'"

(16h) :ndws dCII HeilIrich # nielllolld ausstehcn konll ( 16c) :):)tllOt lIelln' nolJOdl' can stand (16d) tllOt !fclln' # lIo!Jod,' COI/ stand

er. ,ilso Vi, lane: Deprc/ (]'i'i-!:] ill rmthe:oming) \\ ho states thm the si l11ilaritie:; hct\\ een languages öhihiting oble:e:1 shirt are thc !()lIo\\ ing: the: creatioll 01" ne\\ -bil1ding po\sibilities by Ih~ mme:d obicl"l: the ahsene:e ami thc rcpair <)1" WCU ,!olations: L'ull1patibilit) \\ith lloating Ljuantitiers, For the dilTerences cl'. ihldcll1. For further ""amplcs cf. Bavcr'Kornlilt (] 'i9-!:22I"orthcoll1int'I,

Page 7: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

312 ZfSI 51 (2006) 3

(17a) ze Pefra nikdo nemuze 'vJstat ( 17b) :) ?ze Pefra # nikdo nemtiie lystat

As (17a, b) show, Czeeh is a language that obeys the option of German rather than of Eng­lish, e.g. (17a) is a wel1-formed sentenee with shift of the DO into a position before the subjeet (there is no prosodie break) \vhile (17b) with an artifieially ereated prosodie break is rather weird or awkward. In English the pattern seems to be just the opposite, If there is no prosodie break, Serambling is almost impossible, as the eontrast in (16e, d) shows. Aeeording to this intuition, the eases of Serambling as adjunetion whieh allow for LF­reeonstruetion in (14, 15) are most naturally pronouneed with a prosodie break after the serambled PP. Thus, it might be a eorrelation between Serambling as adjunetion and intonational phras­ing ami Serambling as substitution or objeet shift without it. This observation made by Bayer/Korntilt (1994:24) suggests that "only adjoined XPs allcm ror a prosodie break".

1,1.8. Parasific gaps

A test to prove whether Serambling behaves likc A-bar-movement or A-mO\ement has been first undertaken by Webelhuth (1989), He gives ample cvidenee for eases where 'I

fronted DO phrase ean at the same time bind thc pronoun and a parasitie gap in an adjunet elause. Thus, it seems that Serambling displays both A- and A' -properties.

( 18a) :)Pefcr haf jeden Gust; !ohnc e an::uschallcn} scincm, Nachbarn t m"gcsfcllt Peter has every guest without to-Iook-at his neighbor introdueed "Peter introdueed cvery gllest to his neighbor without looking at"

(18b) Pefer haf die Gä,~te; lohnc e on:u,lchaucn]einunder; f m"gesfellf Peter has thc gucsts without looking-at eaeh other introdueed-to "Peter introdueed the guests to eaeh other without loooking at them"

( llle) ? Pet,. pi'edsfm'il kuf,diho hostu; Inehledc no ncho!.ieho, ,I'ousedm'i Peter has introdlleed every gllest without looking at his neighbor

(1IId) Pet,. PI'CdSfcl\'il hos/)'; {nehledc na nc}sol7L~ /lo\':ajem, Peter introdueed all guests not looking at them each other

( llle) :):JPet,. pt'ecA/o\'il sobc nm':ajem/ {nehledc na /lc} hos/I' , Petcr introdueed eaeh other guests

The serambled strueture in (18ab) manifest both an anti-weak erossc)Vcr elleet (an A­property) and parasitie gap lieensing (an A' -propcrty). This paradox raises the question whether the standard diehotomy A- vs. A-bar-movement is suffieient to adequately ehar­aeterize thc array of properties displayed by serambled struetures. Aeeording to Webel­huth (1989), the standard portion ing of phrase strueture positions in A- and A-bar posi­tions is too rough. Therefore. he proposes an intermediate mixed position. Serambling in German is then eonsidered a unitary proeess in whieh thcre is a single derived landing po­sition (a VO or IP adjoined position) t()r the serambled phrasc whieh simultaneously ex­hibits A- and A-bar propcrties.

Page 8: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

313 P. KU'd\. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to German

1.2. Scrambling os ,4-m01'cmel1l

The following arguments have been provided to support the A-movement analysis. Firsl, the fact that a finite clause boundary may not be crossed by a scrambled constituent as in German and Czech. We repeat the examplcs here:

(4a) Wen,glaubl Hans. dass .\tarioliehll,. (Wh-movement) Who believes Hans that Mary love

(4b) DEN HANS, glauhl .\torio ::uliehenl, (Topica1ization) Jt is Hans, that believes Mary to love

(4c) *' .. \I'eil Hans den Wagen l'ersproc!7el1 haI, dass CI' (- - reparierel1 lI'iirde] (Scrambling) ... becausc Hans the car promissed has that he repair would

The samc distribution 01' clause-internal Scralllbling vs. clause unbound Illovement cxhib­its C.lcch:

(5a) KollO j Pell' mn-Ji, :e Marie mi//lje I j (K-movement) (5b) PErRA j A1(/l'ie I17n·li,':e miluje Ij (Topicalization) (5c) *.. ,prolo':e Pell' vu:. slihil, ':e hl' jOfJ/,{llil- -J (Scrambling)

This rcmainds 01' thc clause boundncss 01' NP-movemcnt in passive constructions or ana­phoric binding in German and Czech that can be explained in terms 01' principlc ;\ of thc Binding Thcory (cl'. Fansclc)\\ ILJ90 and Corver/van Ricmsdijk ILJ94:7). Se(()/7(II!', Scrambling may givc rise to new A-binding propcrties, as dcmonstrated in (12b)

(12a) .. ,proto:e Pell', 1'I'C/lil.\'I'e,tlllil*jIJill1o\·i psaj Peter gavc b~ck to his landlord thc dog

(12b) .. .proto:e Pelr j lHIlil psaj svemllili IJiiI1Ol'i Peter gavc back the dog to his I~ndlord

Thc examples (12b) vs. (12a) show th~t DO-argument shit! alters binding possibilities and thc landing sites for argumcnt shirt are rclcvant to binding theory. As already dClllon­strated, reflexivc binding in (12b) can only bc possible if thc DO moves into a position that allows the anaphor (10) to bc bound by its antccedcnt (DO), e. g. under C-command. This scems to be an A-position rather th~n an A·-position. This asymmctry bctwecn 10 and 00 can bc accOLmted for by assuming that the 00 C-commands the 10, thus creat­ing the interprct~tion that the dog belonged to the landlord. Thus, OO-fronting can make rcllc,<;ive binding possiblc. Rdlcxivc binding in (12b) can only be possible ifthe 00 is in a-\-rclated position. The same pattern seellls to hold in Cicrman:

( 13a) weif PeleI', seil1em j *; Herrn den H/lnd/ ::1In'ick :;,oh (13b) ~rcif Pelcr j dcn Hund/ seinem, / f{crm ::uriick gah

rhirdf!', Scrambling does not cxhibit wcak cross()\'er etlects, as demonstrated undcr ( 19ab):

( ]LJa) \l'ei/jedel1, seinc j i'vfllller I //lag ( 19b) prolo':e ka':dcho,)f:'ho, malka l//lifujc

Page 9: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

ZfSI 51 (2006) 3

(20a) Werl [tl liebt seinel Muttel):) (20b) Kdol [tl miluje smjil matku}? (20c) *We11 1 [fiebt seinel A1utter tJ? (20d) *KohoJmilujejehol matka tJ?

The generally accepted explanation 01' the contrast between the subject Wh-movement and object Wh-movement is that in the grammatical examples thc traee of Wh-mo\ement is locally A-bar-bound by the Wh-phrase which is in SpecComp and the pronoun or anaphor are locally A-bound by the trace, while in the ungrammatieal examples (20cd) both the pronoun and the trace are locally A-bar-bound by the Wh-phrase in SpeeComp. It is gen­erally assumed that the WCO effect arises \\henever a single operator loeally A-bar-binds both a pronoun and a traee. a eontext 01' so called multiple \ariable binding (er. Kosta 199511996:23). 11' ( 19ab) were an instance 01' A-bar-movement the same e1'1'ect should bc expected: thus, the sentences \\ith direct object shirt should be ungrammatical. But this is not the case.

FO/lrthly. Serambl ing does not allow lor reconstruction. This \\ as demonstrated under (8.9).

There are also several l~1Cts that militate against taking Serambling as A-mo\cment. One serious taet is that as opposed to NP-mO\ement Scrambling lacks the functional motiva­tion 01' NP-movement. NP-movemcnt is classically triggered by the interplay of Theta­theory and Case-Theory. Serambling on the opposite does not exhibit such a trigger to let it movc (laI' an interpretation o1'Scrambling as A-movement see, e. g.. Witkos 2(06).

The fJrofJosal h,' Bm-erIKol"lljilt 1994

I1'Scrambling is neither an instance 01' A"-mO\emenl. nor 01' A-movement. what is it then'? We would like to adopt the idca 01' ßayer/Kornfilt ( 1994:35passim) that Scrambling 01' un­1'ocuscd NPs is not due to movement (01' Attract alpha in the ne\\ terminology) at all but rather is base generated in the 1'ollowing \\ay: Bayer and Korntilt (ibidem) argue that a subjeet NP, marked fur nominative ease. ean also be lieensed inside 01' a YP. This is because INFL in German ean be viewed as a morpho­logical category that attaches to Y rather than being a terminal syntactie catcgory whieh takes YP as its complement. In this sense (following Abney 1986) Y is the semantie head 01' a complex eategory bui Iding a complex category fonnation as 10rmu lated under (21 ):

(21) Complex Category Formation (CCF) In a strueture [... X" y" ... ] where X" is a raising category that governs y", (0 <::: n <::: max). X"will project into the complex category IX" (Baycr Korntilt 1994:36) ,yn,

"In German. i1' Xe> is an inllectional affix I. the Y-stem will attach to I in morphosyntax i.e .. "'betore" it heads a YP. In English. however. due to the presence 01' adesignated aux­iliary system ( ... ) - I is a tcnninal syntactie node." (Bayer/Kornfilt. ibidem). In Reuland

Page 10: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

31SP. KOSlA. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to Geman

(1990) a similar analysis has been given, with the difference that he takes thc inflected verb to be [X"/I'']:

(22) English German

I' [I"/VP]

~ ~ '0 VP [I' V"]

~ ~ V" V" 1"

J. /. F.rgatil'e I'erhs I'S. lI/1ergati1'L' 1'erhs i/1 GermCln

The structure fo1' German is motiv akd by the presence 01' ergati ve verbs whieh otten show an unmarked constituent ordcr dative-nominative-Verb. Furthermore. scra1l1bled structurcs with an direct objcct in accusative beforc a nominative subject can bc dcri\'cd in thc 1'01­10\\ ing way: sincc thc verb is at thc same time an J' category it can assign the nominativc case under the standard condition of Spcc-head-Agree1l1ent. This makcs it possiblc to base-generate a scrumblcd chluse such as dass de/1 Posthoten-ACC der /I/lIlI/-NOM hei.lst ("'that the dog bites the post1l1an") as in (23):

(23) IP/VP

~ NPace IP V"

den Postboten /~

NPnom l' V"

dCI'Hl\ll~

V" I" beiß -t

Llndcr this analysis it scems that somc rroblems 01' Case assignment need to be elaritieel: First Rayer/Korntilt assul1le that "Case assigment docs not take place only under strict string-adjacency. Olherwise V would be able to to assigll accusati\e Case only to its sister node NP," In their analysis, howcVl:r, the go\erning force ami hence Case-assigning abil­ity 01' V is kept intact in the projections 01' V. Seconu. the nominative NP in (23) is governed by V. This eloes notl1lean. howe\ec that it is also licensed by V. 11' it were the case that V licenscs the subjcct. thc markcd Scral1l­bling order in (22) would be indistinguishable from the unmarked dative-nominative order of Ger1l1an ergative verbs or psych-verbs. Accord ing to standard assumptions. an unerga­ti\e V cannot take a ddinite subject NP as an argument. The subjcct NP is (in the acti\ e clausc) licensed by [ tor Case and by VP tor its theta-role. AdJoining thc nominative NP to

V-zero. the adjoined position must be licensed as the specilier 01' J. Thus. the on!y differ­ence between scramblcd [P Iike (23) anu an unscrambled cannonical IP is that in the Scrambling constructions. the VP remains unsaturated until the Specl P position is li­

Page 11: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

316 ZfSl 51 (2006) 3

censed. That the nominative NP in Scrambling constructions is not licensed by Y is shown by the fact that the Nominativodparticiple sequence cannot be moved together to first posi­tion in German Y2-clauses:

(24) * [Der Hund-NOM gebissen] hat den Postboten-ACC erst einmal the dog bitten has the mai Iman only once

Since Y does not license the nominative NP in (24) it also does not fonn a eonstituent with it. These sentences with unergative verbs contrast sharply with eorresponding eonstruc­tions including ergative verbs as in (25):

(25) [Die Luft-NOM ausgegangen] ist dem Taueher-OAT erst einmal the air ran out is the diver only once

Because the nominative subject 01' an ergative verb is a genuine internal argument of the verb. it is Iicensed by it and fOrlns a eonstituent \\ith it. Compared to Czech we cannot ex­clude sentences like (24) and it seems contraintuitiv to deseribe the mechanisms in the same way as in (24) vs. (25). Thcre are. however. facts 01' binding properties that allow us to ex plain the meehanisms of object shi nas an operation of base-generation.

2.2. Scoj!c lind hinding

Thc CCF proposal 01' Bayer/Kornti It ( 1994:40) seems to allow to base generate the OSY­word order as demonstrated above. cf. ex. (26a):

(26a) *ChlL;li/III1C [11' fjcho, sckrctä,.-kll); [ka::'dJmll proj'csorol'i J, pl'edl·tl/vit c,]

As a consequence. the serambled objeet NP will be in a regular A-position. This yields the results 01' the NP-movement theory which are desirable with respect to binding and the abO\e demonstrated weo eflects \vithout requiring the funetional trigger of NP­movel11ent or attraet Alpha. Recall no\\ that with respect to a quantified binder there is a signifieant difference bctween apparent adjunction to YP (11 b) and apparcnt adjunetion to IP:

(2CJb) .. proto::'c /11' /1'\"(;, rodh'c}1 ka::'ch"-, ::'I'cjmc CI miluje] because his parents( ace) everyone(nom) probably loves

Imagine now a representation without traces of 1110vement. Thanks to the CCF mechanism the constituent strueture of (26b) is obvious. The structure of double object construction is less clear. Let us hypothctically (and ncglecting YP-internal subjects) assume that in the canonieal [0 00 order the dative NP is attaehed to an A-position outside YP tor reasons ofthe thematic structurc ofthe verb. i.e .. in h" 10 [VI' 00 Y]] the dative 10 would still be in an A-position. because the lexical entry 01' the verb requircs such a position. One could argue that the 10 is Iieensed as an argument 01' the verb by virtue of its lexical case. while the DO is licenscd by virtue ofthe structural object position provided by the X-bar­syntax. Attachment 01' 10 to Y would yield the scrambled order in (26a). In order to dis­tinguish the resulting structure from a pure head category we use thc superscript "S". i.e. Y~ rcfcrs to a phrase that is not induced by thc X-bar-system but by the satisfaction of the theta grid orthe ditransitive verb. (26ab) would then have the following structure:

Page 12: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

317 p, KOSTA. On Free Ward Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to German

(27a) VP

/~, NPacc V'

*jeho sekretarku, ~

NPdat V kazdemu, profesorovi pfedstavit

If we invoke unrestricted movement of the qllantitied NP to an operator position by QR. we can accollnt tor the il1-fonnedness of (27a), (27'1) \\'()lIld callse a ease of WCO, 13l1t so would (27b) which however is welJ-formed, What could be the rcasoll that QR does not apply in clallses with scrambled \\ord order')

(27b) IPIYP

~ NPaec IP/VP

sve, rodice ~ N Pnom !'IV

kazdy', miluje

Kiss (19X7) has suggested that LF-mo\el1lent occurs in languagcs \\ith a rather lixed order of eonstituents. while languages like HlIngarian. \\hich can reorder their constituents at S­structure more h'eely make LF-movel1lent sllperlluos, According to Kiss. Hungarian has ~1

tlat 11'. but an articulated pre-lP structllrc into which operators mo\e at S-structure, Thus. HlIngarian is said to encode scope relations syntaetically. \\hile English Iemes score as­signl11ent to LF, With respect to word order freedol11. (Jerl11an amI C/ech are sOl1le\\hat bet\veen English and HlIngarian, Deviations frol1l the lInmarked order ofa quantitied /0 and qllantitied 00 leads to a loss of the othem ise observed scope al1lbigllity, If LF­l1lo\el1lcnt could ti'eely undo the S-Structure score relations cxhibited by scramhled sen­lcnces. the semantic cffect 01' Scrambling \\ould be destroyeel. anel Scral1lbling would loose its function. Therefore \\e take the follO\\ing thesis as grantcd:

(2X) Scral1lbling bleeels LF-mO\cl1lenl

Obsef\e now how can \\e predict the faets ShO\\l1 in (27ab). (27'1) is a case ofScrambling, Thlls (2X) applies and the Q-NP cannot move, As a conseqllence. the Q-NP will not C­coml1land anel thus fail to bind the possesive pronolln je!w!\cil7(" The same is true for (27b), Thc only difference is that here the pronoun ami its potential binder are dominated b~ an identical segment. namely IP. We ,1SSllme that the seope of a Q-NP \\ill srread to the highest eommon node that dominates iL As a consequence. the pronolln in (27b) \\ ill be in the score 01' the Q-NP without LF-movement to !Ja\e taken place, Thus. the CCF­mechanisl1l will guarantee the bound variable reading,

Page 13: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

31R ZfSI 51 (2006) 3

.:'.3. Unsoh'cd proh/cms: LDS (lI1d LF

Independently. however. there seem to be an ample evidence that cases of LDS have to be explained with another mechanism than short Scrambling within the clausal boundary. Besides examples Iike

(7) . ..proto::c Petr vti:, s/ihi/ (PRO c, opravit](LDS)

there seems to bc evidence at least for Czech that there are also examples with LDS com­parable to the LDS in Japanese and Serbo-Croatian:

(29) Tuto knihu Petr a Pavcl vedi kdy Mojmir pfecetl

(30) *') Jakou knihu Petr a Pavcl vedi kdy Mojmir pfecetl

These cxamples show. ho\Vcver. that LDS and Wh-movement also behave different with respcet to thc type 01' Serambling (A-bar vs. A-movement). In the former cases LDS can be trcatcd as basc-gencratcd in A-positions with LF-movemcnt to Theta-positions. It can bc demonstrated that LDS in Czeeh \\hieh treat Scrambling as an instance of eostless overt movemenl. run into an ovetgeneration problem. In particular. they cannot account for the contrast bCl\\Cem scrambied and Wh-movcd elements when they are extraeted out 01' a Wh-island. Boskovie and Takahashi's theory. which involves thc base-gencration of seramblcd elemcnts in thcir surface positions and thcir LF movcment to positions where thcy receivc theta rolcs. does not run into these problems. For the sake of time and lack 01' space wc eannot go more dccply into this problcm.

RekrCIlCC.I

/\HR.\fl.·\\I, W. S. [H. \11'1.1 tcds.) 19i1h.lol,ic. hlCUS ami Conligurationality. Amskrdal11: .lohn Benjamins. ;\ll..\\IH. I'. 1966. Proj~ldllk ,10\ \ SO\Tcl11ennom russkol11ja7yke. Praha. [l·\1'I.I".I. I'. 199~. A eonligurational appro~lch tu russian .Jrec" word ordcr. Ph.D. Disscrtatlon. Cornell

Univcrsity. Ithaea. New Y(1rk. B.\ HI',.J. F. ::00 I. On scramhling: a rcply to BoskO\ic and Takahashi. Linguistie Inquirv 32, 635-6~X.

B\YLI'';'.I. F. ::003 [)oes Russian scral11hling exist" In: K. K\RIVl[ (ed.) 20m. Word order and scramhling. Oxford: Blaek\\ eiL I~6-176.

B,\1'1 R. .J. .I. KOR"FII T. 1<)94. Against seramhlltlg as an inswnec 01' Moyc-alpha. in: CORVFR, N.! 11. VA, R[I\IS[lI.lK (eds.). 1994. Studies on Seral11hling. :vIoyement and Non-Moyement Approaehes 10 Free Wmd-Order l' henolllena. Berlin ~e\\' '{ork : Mouwn de Gruyter (Studies in Cicneratiye Grammal' 41 ). 17-60.

B[SKl 1'. P. 2006a. Seral11hling in C7eeh: syntax. sCl11anlics, and informatil1Tl strueture. Proeeedings 01' ~Wl.C ::1. LJBC Worklng Papers in Linguistics. 1-15.

BISKl 'I'. P. ::006b. Seralllhiing in c,'ech. Procecdings 01' CamLing ::005. Camhridge. BISKI 'I'. I' ::006e. Phase Featuring-dri\ en EPP-I'catures ami EPP-driven Suhjaceney in c,'eeh. ['0 appeal' in:

KllSL\,I'. L. Still 1<1KS (eds.), Conlrlhutions 01' thc Sl\th European Conference on I'ormal Descrip­lion 01' Sta\ic LanguagL's FDSL VI held at Polsdam University. Novemher :10-December ::, 2005. Frank,'url am Main etc.: Peter Lang Verlag (Plllsdam Linguistic Investigations: I).

lloSKO\ 1(, 1.. ::003. On Left Hranch t\traction. in: KOST\. P. .I. BLASZCZAK J. FRASEK H AL. (eds.), In­\esllgalions inl(1 Formal Sla\ ie LlI1guistics. Contrihutions 01' the Founh European Conference on For­mal I)eseription 01' Sla\ ie Languages - FDSL IV held al Potsdam University. Novemher 2~-30. ::00 i, Pan 11. Fran"rUrl am Main Cle., 549-577 (Linguistik InternationaL 10.2).

IloSKO\ll, Z. ::004. TopicaliLation. Foe'lliLation. Ll'\leal Insertion. and Scramhllng. Unguislic fJ1(fliin 35. 613-63i1.

BOSKO\ [c.l.. \"IJ D T\K.-\I[A~II[ 199X. Scrambling ami Last Resort. Lingllil/ic fl1ljllirl' 29.347-366.

Page 14: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

319 P. K, 'S 11. On Free W()rd Order Phelwmena in Czech as Compared to Germall

CHO\lSKY. N. 1981. Lectures on GOlernment and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris. CIIO\lSKY, N. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Camhridge, "lA: MIT Press. CHmlSKY. N. 19lJ9. Derilation hy Phase. in: MIT Occasional Papers in Ll11gllistics 18 [also appeared in:

KE:\STO\IICZ. M. 2001 (ed.), Ken Haie: A Life in Langllage. I-52. Cambridge. MA MIT Press]. CHOMSI'Y. N. 2000. Mll1imalist Inqlliries: The Framework. in: MARTI:\. R. D. MIOIIELS J. URIACiEREKA

(eds.). Step hy Step. Essays on Minimalism in Honor 01' Howard Lasnik. 89- 155, Cambridge. MA: MIT Press.

C(lRI'ER, N. H. VA'\ RIEMSDIJI'. 19lJ4. Introduction: approaches to and properties 01' scramhling, in: CORITK N. / H. I'A, RII\ISDIJK (eds.). 1<)<)4. Studies on Scramhling. MOlement ami Non-Movement Appmaches to Free Word-Ord"r Phenomena Fkrlin / New York: MOllIon de Gruyter (Studies 111 Gen­erative Cjramlllar 41), 1-15.

01:\ BlcSTEN, H. J. Rt TllN. 1<)89. On Verh Raising, Extraposition and Free Word Order in Dutch. in: D. JASI'[I{S / W. KIOOSTER Y. PL ISEYS P. SIUREn (eds.). Sententlai Compkmentation and the Lexicon. Studies in Honour 01' Wim de Gcest. Dordrecht: Foris, 41-5h.

DII'RI/, V. 1<)94: Parameters ofoblect movement. in: (()J(\ER. N. H. I~:\ Rlr\lSDIJI' leds.) 1994: Studies on Scramhling. Mo\ement ami "Jon-Mo\emcnt approaches 10 Free Word Order-Phenomena. Berlin !

Ncw York: Mou1on de Gruyter (Studies in Generatile Cjrammar 41). jOI-152. FA:\SII Cl\\. G. IlJ90. Seramhling as NP-Movement. in: G. GRE\II.'\DORI - W SrlR,HELD (eds.). Scram­

hling and ßaniers. Amsterdalll: Benjamins. I 13-140. (jlll)l(o\\. W. P. KOSIA. IlJlJlJ. S)ntax und SyntaxkonL(:ptionen. in: H. J·\(II;\O\\ (ed.}, Handbuch der

sprachwissenschaftlichen Russillk und ihrer Grenzdis71pllnen. Wieshaden: Ilarrassowitz. 386-424. CiRIIII'llORI. G. W. SHR".H.1D leds.) IlJ90. Scramhling ami Harriers. Amstcrdam: Benjamins. HIIDI\(. H. IlJ88. O-Tracking Systems - [vidcnce lrom German. in: L. M.~RVI / P. MLYSKIN (cds.), Con­

ligurationality. Dordrecht: Foris. 185-20h. III )11 11. 198h. Logieal Form Con,trainls and Conligurational Structure 111 Japanese. Doc1oral Dissertation.

Univer,ity 01' Washll1g1on. Ilo\\lllU(I;. !\. 1986. Word Order and Syntactic Features in the Scanclinavian Languages and English. Doc­

toral Dlsscrtation. University "I' Stockholm. JI '\(,[ II:\NS. L. Cj. 1YI1.\ 1'0\\. IlJln. Syntax and Information Structure 01' Russian Clauscs. 111: BRO\\,r·, F.

W.I I AL. (eds.). Annual Workshop on Formal Appro;lches 10 Sialic Linguistics. The Corn.:11 Meeting 1995 (- 'v1ichigan Slavic Materials; :;lJ). Ann Arhor. Michlgan: Michigan Sialic Puhlicatlons. 28<)­319.

KI'1 ,. Tlnl'Y III )LU )\11'1. 1995. Conl"lgunng Topic ami Focus in Russian. PhD Diss. Stanrord, California Kiss. K. (. 1987. Conligurationality in Hungarian. Dordrechl: Rciclel. KI)'\[lR ISIIOI··I. 1\. 1994. Agre.:ment ami Datil': Subjects in Rus,i~lIl. in: Proceeding, "rthc Second ;\nnual

Workshop on Formal Appro,lches 10 Sial\( Linguistics: the M.Ll. Meeting I l)IJJ, cd. hy SICRI;L't \ I RI i 11:\, Sil\'l '\ FRI ,I'S, \'\11 1.11 .I.I:\A PRO(;IlI v, \1ichigan Sial ie PuhliCdtlOns. Ann Arhor, 255­2X'i.

KI IS I I, P. Il)ln. Syntaktisch.: Prin:ripien und Inrornwtionsstruktur in sogenannten "nichtkonligurationellcn" Sprachen, in: Kos 1\, P. b:. \tl:\:\ (cds.). Sla\ Istische Linguistik 19lJh. Referate des XXII. Konstanzer Sial iSlischen Arhcitstreffens. 1'\1\sdam. 17.-20.(Jl). 1lJlJh. München: Sagner IlJl)7, IO'i-1.15.

KI)S\ I. f'. IlJ95!]l)l)h. Empty Calegoncs. l\ull-Subjects ami 1\ull-Obj.:cts and Ihm lo lreat Ih.:m in the \1inimalist Program. in: Linguistics inl'otsd'lm2.3 (1')lJ5199h). 7-38. lJnil. ofPotsdam.

Kosl I. P. 2002. Minimalism and hee Constituent Order (in Russlan as Compared In (icrman). in: KIlSII. P. J. Flnsr" (eds.). Current Approaches to Formal Sialic Linguistics. Contrihutions 01' the Seconcl Euwpc<ln Conk'rcnce on Formal Dcseription orSlal'ic Languages FDSL 11 held at Polsdam Univcrsity. No\cmher 20-22. jlN7. Frankfurt am Main dc. : Peter Lang Verlag 2002 (Linguislik Intcrnational; lJ), 25J-272.

Kos 1\, f'. L. SIIIL RII'S. 2006 In prep. rhe Focus F.:arure Rn lSited. To appear 111: KOSTA. P. L. SI 111 RCI'S (cds.). Contrihutions or th.: Sixth Furopean Conference Formal Description 01' Slavic Lan­guages FDSL VI hcld at Potsdam University. NO\ember 30-Deeemher 2. 2005. Frankfurt am Main .:IC .. Pc'ler Lang V.:rlag (Potsdam Linguistic Im estigations; 1).

Page 15: On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared to ... · P. Ko'il~. On Free Word Order Phenomena in Czech as Compared 307 . t() Gennan . syntactic properties of Russian ward order

320 ZfSl 5 J (2006) 3

KO\'Tl'''O\'A, I. 1976. SO\Temennyj russkij jazyk. Porjadok slov i aktual"noe clenenie. Mosb a: Prosvesce­nie.

KRYLO\A, O. / S. CHA V·RO"I".·V. 1984. Porjadok slo\ \ rLlsskom jazyke. Moskva: Russkij jazyk. MAILVJAN, A. 1990. On the A-A' Distinction. Doctoral dissertation. MIT, Cambridge, Mass. MEHLHOR". B. 2002. Kontrastierte Konstituenten im Russischen: experimentelle Untersuchungen zur In­

formationsstruktur. rhO. Diss. Uni \. Leipzig. Frankfurt am Main etc.: Peter Lang (Europäischc Hochschulschriftcn, Rcihe 16, Slawische Sprachen und Literaturen: (5).

Ru I". 'D, E. 1990. Head 'vlovemcnt and the Relation Betwcen Morphology and Syntax. in: Ci. 80011 / 1. VA" MARIE (eds). Morphology Yearbook 3. 129-161.

RIEMSDI1K, 11. V".N 1989. 'v1ovement and Regeneration. in: P. BL'\I"C.A (cd.), D\alect Variation and the Thc­ory of Grammar. Dordrecht: Fons, 105-135.

Ro~s, J. R. 1%7. Constraints on Variables in Syntax. Doctoral Dissertation, MIT. S·\BIl, J. / M. SAlTO. 2005. The Free Word Order rhenomcnon. Its Syntactic Sourccs and Diversity. Berlin

Nc\\' '1'01'),.: Mouton de (iruyter (Studics in Cienermive Grammal' (9) SAlTO. M. 19X5. Some Asymllletries in Japanese and Their Theoretical Consequenees. J)octoral Disserta­

tion, MIT. SLKFRINA. L A. 1995. Scramhling ami ('ontigurationality: Evidence from Russlan Syntactic Processing. in:

BI((lW'd:, E. W. iT AL. (eds.), Annual Workshop on Formal Approaches to Slavic Linguistics. The Cornell 'vkcting 1995 (~ Michigan Slavic rvlaterials: 39), Ann Arhor. Michigan: Michigan Slavic Puh­lications, 433-463.

Wr'!lI'IHi'l H, Ci. 19X9. Syntactic Saturation rhenomena and thc Modern (jermanie Languages. Doctllral Dissertation, lJniverslty of Massachusdts, Amhcrst.

WII KOS, .I. 2006/ in prep. Polish and A-tvpe SCJ'<llllhling. To ilppear in: Kos I.v, P. L. S('rl(RcK~ (eds.). ('on­trihutions of thc Sixth European Cl1Jllc'rence Formal [)escription of Slavic Langu,lges FDSL VI held at I'otsdalll Lniverslty, Novemher 30-December 2, 2005. Frankfurt am Main etc.: I'der Lang Verlag (Potsdanl Linguistic InYestig,rtions: I).

Prof Dr. Petn Kosta. Universität Potsdam. Institut /Ur Slavistik. Slavischc Sprachwissen­schaft. Postfach 60 1553. 14415 Potsdam, Deutschland (pkosta(alll/i-potsdOlI/.dc)