no milk today? challenges of maintaining a vegan diet in

82
I Master Programme in Sustainable Management Class of 2015/2016 Master Thesis 15 ECTS No Milk Today? Challenges of Maintaining a Vegan Diet in Germany Uppsala University Campus Gotland Yasmin Emre Supervisors: Tina Hedmo & Matilda Dahl

Upload: others

Post on 14-Feb-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

I

Master Programme in Sustainable Management

Class of 2015/2016

Master Thesis 15 ECTS

No Milk Today?

Challenges of Maintaining a Vegan Diet

in Germany

Uppsala University Campus Gotland

Yasmin Emre

Supervisors:

Tina Hedmo

& Matilda Dahl

II

Abstract

Purpose: The aim of this research is to identify the variables influencing the maintenance of a

vegan diet in Germany. The Theory of Planned Behaviour is used as a theoretical framework in

order to analyse the intention of vegan consumers who try to maintain their dietary behaviour.

Background: Animal agriculture has a number of negative consequences on the environment,

such as accelerating climate change and water pollution, and is further linked to world hunger.

Consumers’ dietary behaviour can thus make a significant difference, especially when opting for

a strict vegan diet. However, research found that about 70 % of vegan consumers abandon this

dietary choice. By studying German vegan consumers (recent and long-term), this thesis will ex-

amine the challenges and potential barriers those individuals encounter when trying to uphold their

vegan diet.

Method: An explanatory strategy with a deductive, quantitative approach was applied. The theo-

retical framework is based on the Theory of Planned Behaviour by Ajzen (1991). Additional con-

structs based on previous findings of research on food and meat were added to the original model,

and six hypotheses were developed and tested. The data was collected at first through formative

research, followed by an online questionnaire, based upon self-selection and snowballing sampling

in various German vegan groups on Facebook.

Findings and conclusion: Maintaining a vegan diet in Germany is not easy. The study identified

a number of challenges, amongst which a lack of perceived behavioural control and an attachment

for cheese have been identified as the biggest ones. Especially these two factors lead to a strong

ambivalence among vegans on whether to maintain their dietary behaviour or not. Education

campaigns could foster general knowledge on the preparation and nutritive value of plant-based

diets and address common myths regarding meat production and consumption.

III

Acknowledgements

First of all, I would like to thank my supervisors at Uppsala University for giving critical feed-

back. I also want to express my gratitude to my fellow students for constructive advice through-

out the different steps of creating this thesis.

A big thank you to the many participants of this study, without whom the study would not have

been possible. Thanks a lot, vegan guys and girls! Tofu on.

My thanks also go to the Berlin crowd: the science department of the Albert Schweitzer Stiftung,

and to Stefan for being the best roommate I could imagine.

Furthermore, I would like to express my appreciations to my friends, and especially to Alex and

Natacha, who convinced me during the statistical methods that moving to a sunny island in the

Carribean and living there as an illegal (but happy) immigrant was not (yet) an option.

I also thank Stinki the Cat for being very fluffy.

Finally, I am thankful for my friend and love, Gregor, for his inspiration support (even though I

was not very fond of you practising the trumpet in our apartment.) Thank you for being there.

Visby & Cologne, August 2016

Yasmin

Introduction 1

Table of Content

Abstract...................................................................................................................... II

Table of Content ........................................................................................................ 1

Index of Abbreviations .............................................................................................. 3

List of Figures ........................................................................................................... 3

List of Tables ............................................................................................................. 4

1 Introduction ......................................................................................................... 5

1.1 Background ............................................................................................................ 6

1.2 Research Question ................................................................................................. 7

2 Theoretical Framework ....................................................................................... 8

2.1 Consumer Behaviour ............................................................................................. 8

2.2 Theory of Planned Behaviour ................................................................................ 8

2.3 Theory of Planned Behaviour: Additional Constructs ........................................ 13

2.4 Summary of the Model and Criticism ................................................................. 15

2.5 Hypotheses .......................................................................................................... 16

3 Method............................................................................................................... 19

3.1 Research Philosophy and Approach .................................................................... 19

3.2 Research Strategy ................................................................................................ 19

3.3 Sampling .............................................................................................................. 20

3.4 Formative Research and Pre-Test ........................................................................ 22

3.5 Questionnaire Design .......................................................................................... 22

3.6 Operationalisation ................................................................................................ 23

3.7 Limitations ........................................................................................................... 29

Introduction 2

4 Empirical Findings ............................................................................................ 30

4.1 Descriptive Statistics ........................................................................................... 30

4.2 Analysis of the Theoretical Model ...................................................................... 30

4.3 Hypotheses Testing ............................................................................................. 35

5 Discussion ......................................................................................................... 41

5.1 Characteristics of the Sample .............................................................................. 41

5.2 Intention ............................................................................................................... 41

5.3 Attitudes and Attitudes 4N .................................................................................. 42

5.4 Subjective Norm .................................................................................................. 43

5.5 Perceived Behavioural Control ........................................................................... 44

5.6 Meat Attachment ................................................................................................. 45

5.7 Cheese Attachment .............................................................................................. 47

5.8 Ambivalence ........................................................................................................ 48

6 Conclusion ......................................................................................................... 49

6.1 Limitations and Strength ..................................................................................... 50

7 References ......................................................................................................... LI

8 Appendix .......................................................................................................... LX

Introduction 3

Index of Abbreviations

df Degrees of Freedem

FB Facebook

IIC Inter-Item-Correlation

M Mean

PCA Principal Component Analysis

PCB Perceived Behavioural Control

SD Standard Deviation

SN Subjective Norm

t Value of the t-Statistic

TIB Theory of Interpersonal Behaviour

TPB Theory of Planned Behaviour

List of Figures

Figure 1 - Theory of Planned Behaviour (Ajzen, 1991) ................................................................. 9

Figure 2 - Aligned Model of TPB (Ajzen, 1991) .......................................................................... 18

Figure 3 - Gender… ..................................................................................................................... LX

Figure 4 - Age…… .................................................................................................................... LXI

Figure 5 - Former Dietary Style ................................................................................................ LXII

Figure 6 - Education ................................................................................................................. LXIII

Figure 7 - Net Income ............................................................................................................. LXIV

Figure 8 - Residential Area ....................................................................................................... LXV

Figure 9 - Children in Household ............................................................................................ LXVI

Figure 10 - Facebook Post Example ....................................................................................... LXIX

Introduction 4

List of Tables

Table 1 - Scale Creation ................................................................................................................ 24

Table 2 - Correlation of Original Constructs ................................................................................ 31

Table 3 - Correlation of Original Constructs and Intention .......................................................... 31

Table 4 - Correlation of All Variables .......................................................................................... 32

Table 5 - Hierarchical Regression for Intention ............................................................................ 33

Table 6 - Hypothesis H2 - Multiple Regression ............................................................................ 36

Table 7 - Descriptive Statistics of PBC Scale ............................................................................... 37

Table 8 - Descriptive Statistics of Attitudes 4N Scale .................................................................. 38

Table 9 - Gender… ....................................................................................................................... LX

Table 10 - Age…... ..................................................................................................................... LXI

Table 11 - Former Dietary Style ............................................................................................... LXII

Table 12 - Education level ....................................................................................................... LXIII

Table 13 - Net Income ............................................................................................................. LXIV

Table 14 - Residential Area ....................................................................................................... LXV

Table 15 - Children in Household ........................................................................................... LXVI

Table 16 - Reasons Participants Turned Vegan ..................................................................... LXVII

Table 17 - Reliability after Item-Correction .......................................................................... LXVII

Table 18 - Means for Variables ............................................................................................. LXVIII

Table 19 – Complete Survey in English .................................................................................... LXX

Table 20 – Complete Survey in German ................................................................................ LXXV

Introduction 5

1 Introduction

"The fridge had been emptied of all Dudley's favourite

things — fizzy drinks and cakes, chocolate bars and burgers

— and filled instead with fruit and vegetables and the sorts of

things that Uncle Vernon called "rabbit food."

J.K. Rowling (2000), Harry Potter and the Goblet of Fire

Consumers´ (dietary) behaviour play an important role in sustainable development (Geeraert,

2013). The growing demand for meat and other animal-based products has led to a form of ani-

mal agriculture which, according to the United Nations, is one of the main causes for climate

change (Steinfeld et al., 2006). Further environmental consequences include the loss of biodiver-

sity and the pollution of both air and water. Current forms of animal agriculture are “one of the

most relevant topics to be addressed if Western consumers are to shift towards a more sustaina-

ble diet” (Schösler et al., 2012, p. 39).

Veganism, the diet with the highest scores with respect to sustainable consumption (Springmann

et al., 2016), has created some awareness in Germany in recent years: plant-based diets are being

promoted by the media as healthy and modern (O’Riordan and Stoll-Kleemann, 2015), Berlin

has been voted the No. 1 vegan-city by one of the most popular international vegan websites

(HappyCow, 2015) and even the meat industry has acknowledged an apparent shift in consumers

eating behaviour and started offering vegan meat substitutes, aimed at the mass-market (Michail,

2015). A transformation slowly seems to be taking place. Yet, eating vegan is far from being the

norm - Germany is still one of the countries with the highest meat consumption in the world (ap-

prox. 60kg per person and year), and only about 1.1% of the population is vegan (VEBU, 2015).

So what are the challenges for vegan consumers in a society where they are the minority? The

aim of this study is to identify factors influencing the maintenance of a vegan diet. The potential

barriers of maintaining a vegan diet that recent research has identified will be examined, along

with other factors identified by formative research.

The Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) by Ajzen (1991) will serve as a theoretical framework

for this study. The TPB is one of the most influential models for predicting and influencing con-

sumer behaviour (Armitage and Conner, 2001), and is based on the assumption that behaviour is

formed by conscious decision-making. The continuing maintenance of a vegan diet in a society

where consuming animal-based product is the norm requires such a conscious (‘planned’) behav-

Introduction 6

iour - vegan consumers have restricted possibilities for spontaneous food-related decisions, espe-

cially when eating out. The TPB has already been tested for predicting food consumption in vari-

ous contexts (Conner and Armitage, 1998; Godin and Kok, 1996; Tobler et al., 2011), and hence

seems like an appropriate choice for this study.

The findings, specific to the German market, will enrich research on consumer behaviour, and

more specifically, the growing body of research on sustainable consumption and veganism. It

will also lead to a better understanding of the challenges of sustainable yet non-conformist con-

sumption behaviour. Furthermore, the study could provide valuable insights for vegan companies

and policy makers to design effective marketing campaigns in Germany with the purpose of in-

fluencing consumer´s dietary behaviour in favour of veganism, thus implicitly promoting a more

sustainable consumption.

The thesis is written during an internship at the charitable Albert Schweitzer Foundation for Our

Contemporaries in Berlin, Germany. The non-profit organization offers insights and access to

vegan consumers’ communication networks.

1.1 Background

Sustainability and the consumption of animal-based products

The United Nations find that “the consumption of meat and dairy products (…) cause a dispro-

portionately large share of environmental impacts” (UNEP, 2010, p. 51). Not only has the inter-

national livestock sector a bigger impact on climate change impact than the transportation sector

worldwide (Steinfeld et al., 2006), the water usage of animal agriculture is considerably higher

than the same amount of plant-based calories (Baroni et al., 2007; Chemnitz et al., 2016; Stein-

feld et al., 2006). The livestock industry is also mainly responsible for “habitat change like de-

forestation, destruction of riparian forests or the drainage of wetlands, be it for livestock produc-

tion itself or for feed production” (“FAO,” 2008, p. 187). And since approximately 70% of the

worldwide agricultural land is being used for livestock production, “representing the largest of

all anthropogenic land uses”(“FAO,” 2008, p. 270), Foley (2011) points out how “(…) grain-fed

meat production systems are a drain on the global food supply” (p. 62).

Because of these facts, there exists a vast consensus amongst researchers that for a more sustain-

able consumption, diets in Western Europe need to shift away from animal-based products to-

wards plant-based foods (Baroni et al., 2007; Berners-Lee et al., 2012; Chemnitz et al., 2016; El-

zen et al., 2009; Geeraert, 2013; Hallström et al., 2014; Hedenus et al., 2014; Kahiluoto et al.,

2014; Koneswaran and Nierenberg, 2008; Meier and Christen, 2013; van Dooren et al., 2014).

Introduction 7

Some authors find that to mitigate the climate change effects a transition to a vegetarian diet

would be sufficient (Hedenus et al., 2014; Kahiluoto et al., 2014). Yet, most agree that the

strongest positive effects on the environment comes with a transition towards a vegan diet

(Baroni et al., 2007; Berners-Lee et al., 2012; Meier et al., 2014; van Dooren et al., 2014).

Previous Research on Sustainable Food Consumption

In the past, many researchers of sustainable food consumption focused on the subject of meat re-

duction (e.g. Ifat Zur and Christian A. Klöckner, 2014; Saba and Di Natale, 1998), thereby often

neglecting dairy, which also plays an important part in animal agriculture. Moreover, most stud-

ies emanated from the perspective of consumers who still eat meat- and dairy products.

Even if they differentiated between meat-eaters and vegetarians, in many cases they did not

clearly distinguish between vegetarian and vegan consumers (Povey et al., 2001; Ruby, 2012).

Vegans might have been included in former vegetarian studies but were most often not marked

accordingly. While there has been a number of studies about what drives people to become vege-

tarians (Greenebaum, 2012; Radnitz et al., 2015), research solely focusing on vegans is still rela-

tively scarce.

1.2 Research Question

According to the Humane Research Council (Asher et al., 2014), 86% of vegetarians and 70% of

vegans abandon their diet and go back to eating animal-based products. Being vegan is appar-

ently not easy within a society where the consumption of animal-based products like meat and

dairy is the norm.

One way to understand why so many consumers give up the more sustainable vegan consump-

tion is by analysing the challenges the vegan consumers are facing in the daily maintenance of

the diet. The research question is thus:

What are the barriers vegan consumers in Germany are facing in maintaining the more

sustainable vegan dieting behaviour?

Theoretical Framework 8

2 Theoretical Framework

2.1 Consumer Behaviour

Consumer behaviour is about understanding, predicting and explaining consumers´ behaviour, in

order to identify patterns of behaviour. Behaviour refers to both external (which can be observed)

and internal (non-observable) behaviours. In general, consumer behaviour is not limited to con-

crete products but also incorporates the behaviour of people towards immaterial goods inside a

society.

In the last years transformative consumer research has evolved, “a movement (…) to encourage,

support, and publicize research that benefits consumer welfare and quality of life for all beings

affected by consumption across the world” (Mick, 2006, p. 1). The difference to traditional con-

sumer behaviour is that transformative consumer research seeks to develop solutions to consumer

problems which are often originated in (over-)consumption. The findings of this study are related

to transformative consumer research.

2.2 Theory of Planned Behaviour

A well-known model for explaining and predicting consumer behaviour is the Theory of Planned

Behaviour (TPB) by Ajzen (1991). The TPB is an extension of the Theory of Reasoned Action

(Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980), which had the limitation that it could only explain behaviours over

which consumers have full volitional control.

The model has been applied in a variety of studies concerned with sustainable consumption

(Cook et al., 2002; Dunn et al., 2011; Hung‐ Yi Lu et al., 2010; Sparks and Shepherd, 1992; Zur

and Klöckner, 2014), and “has been shown to be a useful predictor of food choice intentions”

(Armitage and Conner, 1999, p. 263), including studies on meat consumption or veganism and

vegetarianism (Graça et al., 2015a; Povey et al., 2001).

In it, Ajzen argues that behaviour is best determined by behavioural intention. ‘Behavioural in-

tention’ refers to the intention to perform a behaviour, and for reasons of better legibility, is

phrased throughout this text as ‘intention’. Without the intention to engage in a behaviour, a con-

sumer is unlikely to carry out a specific behaviour.

For this study, the intention in question is the consumer´s willingness to maintain a vegan diet.

Ajzen (1991) argues that intention is determined by three variables: “attitudes”, “subjective

norms” and “perceived behavioural control”.

Theoretical Framework 9

Figure 1 - Theory of Planned Behaviour (Ajzen, 1991)

These three variables have antecedent salient beliefs: behavioural beliefs which are assumed to

influence attitudes toward the behaviour, normative beliefs which constitute the underlying de-

terminants of subjective norms, and control beliefs, which provide the basis for perceptions of

behavioural control.

Ajzen (1991) claims that, “as a general rule, the more favorable the attitude and subjective norm

with respect to a behavior, and the greater the perceived behavioral control, the stronger should

be an individual’s intention to perform the behavior under consideration” (p. 188).

Behaviour

Behaviour is defined as an action that can be observed (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1977). Ajzen and

Fishbein (1980) argue that behaviour is based upon controlled decision making, and that it can be

best predicted through an individual’s intention: without the intention to eat a vegan diet there will

be no corresponding behaviour.

Actual behaviour cannot be measured in a cross-sectional study: for that, the participants needed

to be asked again after a specific time if they indeed engaged in the behaviour expressed through

the intention. Instead, this study can only measure the expressed intention of participants.

Theoretical Framework 10

Attitude

Attitudes are typically explained by the term “evaluations”: they consist of how a person thinks

about (evaluates) a specific behaviour, topic, idea or object (Augoustinos et al., 2006).

The TPB also refers to attitudes as evaluations (Ajzen, 1991). There, the attitude variable is

based upon behavioural beliefs: being an expectancy-value model, according to the TPB atti-

tudes are evaluations built on information-based behavioural outcomes - at a first glance without

an affective component that is included in other definitions of attitude. Ajzen (2001) responded

to its apparent lack by explaining that the beliefs the attitude is based on are not necessarily only

cognitive, but can also be influenced by emotions (Ajzen, 2011).

Still, the absence of a distinct affective component in the model has been criticized by some au-

thors (Conner and Armitage, 1998; Wolff et al., 2011), who suggest that adding affects as an ad-

ditional variable could enhance the predictability of the model in some contexts. Especially with

regard to food consumption, Graça et al. (2015a) identified a strong affective component to-

wards the enjoyment of meat. This will be explained later in chapter 2.3.

Finally, Povey et al. (2001) found that the most salient beliefs towards eating a vegan diet from

vegans were humane, healthy, environmentally friendly and restrictive.

Subjective Norm

Norms are the typically unwritten rules of a society on how people are expected to behave, given

their social surroundings and circumstance (Hechter and Opp, 2001). Since Cialdini´s Focus

Theory of Normative Conduct (1990), norms are often distinguished in descriptive (the level of

representations of what other do) and injunctive (the levels of others’ disapproval) norms.

The second variable of the TBP, subjective norm, consists of the normative belief and the moti-

vation to comply with it. Both originate from the consumers´ perceived social pressure from his

environment to perform or not perform a certain behaviour, which is in turn influencing the con-

sumers decision making process and the intention towards a specific behaviour (Ajzen, 1991).

According to the TPB, individuals experience social pressure from either persons closely related

to them or being significantly important to them. Ajzen (1991) argues that the bigger the per-

ceived pressure, the more a consumer will act upon it.

Cialdini et al. (1990) find that social norms are able to have a stronger influence on intentions

than attitudes. This might be likely for the context of this study, as eating is also often a social

event. Beverland (2014) refers to Pollan (2011) when explaining that “being a vegetarian can be

Theoretical Framework 11

alienating and people must accommodate their choices or vice versa” (p. 375). Research found

that social influences are especially important for consumers who changed their dietary habits

(Asher et al., 2014; Steptoe et al., 2004). This coincides with Haverstock et al. (2012); who

found that being a member in a social networks is a common trait for vegetarians and vegans, as

they offer social support in order to maintain the diet.

Originating from these earlier findings, it is hypothesised that the influence of the subjective

norm is especially relevant for consumers who ate meat and dairy before going vegan and be-

longed to the societal norm. Vegetarians, on the other hand, are already used to stand out due to

their eating behaviour.

H11: Former meat-eaters feel a significantly stronger influence from their social environment

than former vegetarians.

Perceived Behavioural Control

The perceived behavioural control (PBC) refers to a consumer´s perceived control over the per-

formance of a behaviour (Ajzen, 2002) – how easy or difficult a consumer finds a certain behav-

iour, in this case the maintenance of a vegan diet.

Like the attitude and the subjective norm, is built upon control beliefs1. Ajzen emphasizes that

the PBC might vary across situations and actions, which makes it quite suitable for this study,

since the eating behaviour is not performed in one single setting or situation. The PBC incorpo-

rates two elements found in earlier studies (Zolait, 2014): the first one refers to external re-

sources, the "controllability”, sometimes also referred to as “facilitating conditions" (Triandis,

1979), like time or money.

The second element refers to internal resources, the self-efficacy, like knowledge and skills. Ac-

cording to Ajzen (1991), the PBC has similarities with Bandura´s concept of perceived self-effi-

cacy (Bandura, 1982, 1977), which “is concerned with judgments of how well one can execute

courses of action required to deal with prospective situations” (Bandura, 1982, p. 122). While

some researchers find that the PBC should be divided into self-efficacy and perceived control

1 According to the model, the PBC – in contrast to the two constructs attitude and subjective norm - can be used to

directly to predict behavioural outcomes (Ajzen, 1991, p. 184), as “the effort expended to bring a course of be-

havior to a successful conclusion is likely to increase with perceived behavioral control” (ibid). Since due to the

cross-sectional design of the study this study focuses only on the intention, the predictability of actual behaviour

of the PBC can be neglected.

Theoretical Framework 12

over the behaviour, Armitage and Conner (1999) found that the PBC variable is a robust predic-

tor of dietary behaviour in itself. Richetin et al. (2011), who tested the prediction of cognitions in

attitudinal models, also find that the PBC “(…), empirically (…) has been shown to be a very

strong predictor of behaviour” (p. 45). Moreover, it has been found that between all three varia-

bles of the model, the PBC has the largest influence on the intention (Armitage and Conner,

2001; Graça et al., 2015a). Thus, it is hypothesised that in the case of vegan consumers, the PBC

will also have the biggest influence on the intention.

H12: Of all the variables of the original constructs (attitude, subjective norm and perceived be-

havioural control), the perceived behavioural control has the biggest influence on the inten-

tion.

Furthermore, previous studies in the context of food consumption found that the preparation of

vegetarian food was often not perceived as an easy task (Lea et al., 2006), and a lack of practice

and knowledge about plant-based dishes are obvious barriers for the maintenance of a vegan diet

(Aiking et al., 2006; Beverland, 2014; Schösler et al., 2012). For this study it is hypothesised that

with ongoing time, the PBC will increase and participants will find the maintenance of the vegan

diet easier.

H13: The PBC value is significantly higher for participants who have been vegan for a longer

time.

Behavioural Intention

Ajzen (1991) defines behavioural intentions as following: “Intentions are assumed to capture the

motivational factors that influence a behavior; they are indications of how hard people are will-

ing to try, of how much of an effort they are planning to exert, in order to perform the behaviour”

(p. 182). He argues that intentions are the most important predictor of behaviour, and that “in

combination, attitude toward the behavior, subjective norm, and perception of behavioral control

lead to the formation of a behavioral intention” (Ajzen, 2002, p. 665).

In the context of meat consumption, Saba and Di Natale (1998) find strong evidence that inten-

tions have a significant effect on actual meat consumption. Yet Richetin et al. (2011), who also

studied intentions in the context of meat, find the TPB should include intentions about both per-

forming and not performing an action, as they point out that “strong intention of not performing

a certain action is not the same as a weak intention of performing this action” (p.52). Sheeran

(2002) finds that this would enhance “both consistency and discrepancy between intentions and

subsequent action” (p. 6). For better predictability, Sutton (1998) proposes in his seminal paper

Theoretical Framework 13

about intentions that they “should be measured proximally, after rather than before people have

made a real decision” (p. 1334). This fits well with this research, since it studies the intention of

consumers who have already made the decision to change their behaviour and act accordingly.

Wegner and Wheatley (1999) however question the intention-behaviour link, arguing that inten-

tions in general do not predict behaviour, as "(...) the real causal mechanisms underlying behav-

ior are never present in consciousness”. While this might be true for many behaviours, in this

context the critique can be neglected for two reasons: for once, the participants of this study al-

ready show the desired behaviour, and secondly, this new behaviour is based upon conscious de-

cision-making.

Another critique may be more relevant: Sheeran (2002) summarizes that behaviour may not only

be predicted by intention, but also by automatic processes – habits. The next chapter will illus-

trate findings of previous research – like habits - that are important in the meat/vegan/food con-

text and which are not included in the original TPB. For this study, some of them will therefore

be added as additional constructs in order to increase the predictability of the model.

2.3 Theory of Planned Behaviour: Additional Constructs

Attitudes 4N

Piazza et al. (2015) have analysed attitudinal barriers for plant-based diets, which they summa-

rized as the 4Ns of meat consumption: natural, nice, normal and necessary. They found that ap-

preciation and enjoyment (“nice”) of meat are highly influential barriers in attitude, and that

meat-eaters “endorsed the 4Ns at a significantly higher rate” (p. 119) than vegetarians/vegans.

They further observed that beliefs about the naturalness, normalness and necessity of consuming

animal-based products is also found – albeit to a lesser degree – in vegan consumers. It will

hence be tested if the intention to maintain the vegan diet might be lower with participants who

show beliefs of the Attitudes 4Ns.

H14: Participants who show high results in the 4N´s have a significantly lower intention to

maintain the vegan diet in the next 6 months than participants with low results.

Meat Attachment

A prime example for extending the TPB with respect to the present study can be found in Graça

et al. (2015a), who identified a strong affective connection towards meat consumption. They la-

belled it “meat attachment” and suggest to include as an additional TPB variable in further stud-

ies on plant-based diets. The “Meat Attachment Questionnaire” by Graça et al. (2015a) is in line

Theoretical Framework 14

with various other research that emphasises the enjoyment of meat as one of the major chal-

lenges towards the maintenance of a plant-based diet (Köster, 2009; Lea and Worsley, 2003; Pasi

Pohjolainen et al., 2015; Rothgerber, 2014). For this study, it is hypothesised that meat attach-

ment influences the intention to maintain the vegan diet.

H15: Meat attachment negatively correlates with the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

Cheese Attachment

Research suggests that cheese, like many processed foods with high levels of sugar and fat, can

lead to addictive behaviour (Gearhardt et al., 2011) – especially since cheese contains casomor-

phins, which stimulate the brain´s reward system (Schulte et al., 2015). Hence, further develop-

ing the rationale of the meat attachment towards a construct labelled “cheese attachment” seems

like a promising research approach, which to the author´s knowledge has not been done yet.

H16: Cheese attachment negatively correlates with the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

Ambivalence

As mentioned above, attitudes (one of the core-constructs in the TPB) are evaluations that are

either positive or negative. In the social sciences they are typically measured on (uni- or bipolar)

scales like “agree” or “do not agree”. Yet, sometimes people might have both positive and

negative beliefs about a topic – in that case, they are ambivalent. The traditional scaling - even

with middle-points - is not suited to capture ambivalence beliefs (Breckler, 1994). This research

hence tries to mitigate this (methodological) weakness by adding a concrete ambivalence item to

the study.

Sparks et al. (2001), who applied ambivalence in the domain of dietary choices to the TPB,

found that ambivalence might influence the “predictive ability of attitude-intention-models,

especially when applied to health-related behaviours [such as diets] that are characterized by

motivational conflicts” (p. 54). And Povey et al. (2001) found in the context of vegan diets that

higher ambivalence weakens the relation between attitude and intention to follow the diet.

Theoretical Framework 15

Other important findings

Previous research has identified more variables influencing food behaviour which this research -

limited in time and scope – cannot incorporate. For completeness’ sake however they shall be

mentioned shortly.

Since food preparation is usually performed frequently, habits were found to be influencing die-

tary behaviour (Armitage and Conner, 2001; Godin and Kok, 1996; Graça et al., 2015a; Zur and

Klöckner, 2014). Health beliefs play an ambivalent role in the discussion of dietary choices. On

one hand, health beliefs can act as a powerful driver (Lea et al., 2006; Povey et al., 2001; Ruby,

2012; Sabaté, 2003; Zur and Klöckner, 2014), on the other hand, health concerns, originating

from the perceived nutritional necessity of meat (Pohjolainen et al., 2015; Piazza et al., 2015),

are often a strong barrier preventing the maintenance of plant-based diets (Lea and Worsley,

2003; Povey et al., 2001). With regard to veganism, the matter of moral beliefs is naturally im-

portant: previous research found that moral reasons and the concern about animal welfare are the

major causes for people to adopt plant based-diets (Beardsworth and Keil, 1991; Cordts et al.,

2014; Fox and Ward, 2008; Tobler et al., 2011; Zur and Klöckner, 2014).

Socio-demographic factors like gender and education are also highly influential for maintaining

plant-based diets (de Boer et al., 2014; Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002; Lea and Worsley, 2001;

Pasi Pohjolainen et al., 2015; Schösler et al., 2012). Previous research also identified environ-

mental awareness (Cron and Pobocik, 2013; Haverstock and Forgays, 2012; Macdiarmid et al.,

2016) and self-identity (Armitage and Conner, 1999; Sparks and Shepherd, 1992) as important

variables in the food context.

2.4 Summary of the Model and Criticism

The Theory of Planned Behaviour is especially relevant for this study as dietary choice is volun-

tary, and dietary decisions are to a large extent under the consumer´s control: consumers make

conscious choices to adopt such behaviours based on information, motivations, and knowledge

they have. Being an expectancy-value model, the TPB is based on the assumption that behaviour

is formed by rational decision making, and as such on expected utility. Utility in the context of

consumer behaviour can best be described as the degree of contentment, happiness or personal

benefit. Yet, its underlying assumption that people act rational has earned the model some criti-

cism by a number of authors (Kollmuss and Agyeman, 2002; Scheibehenne et al., 2008; Valle et

al., 2005).

Theoretical Framework 16

While the rationality of the model can be questioned for many behaviours, it is argued here that

the continuous maintenance of a vegan diet within a daily routine is to a certain extent based

upon rational ‘planned behaviour’. Nonetheless, Köster (2009) attests the TPB and all other ra-

tional decision-making models in food behaviour “low predictive validity (…)” (p. 70). He

claims that they do not take certain important factors like “past behaviour, habit and hedonic ap-

preciation” (ibid) into account. To overcome these shortcomings, he recommends to extend the

TPB and add these variables as extra-constructs.

The extendibility of the core TPB model with further variables has been advocated by Ajzen

himself (2011) and is one of its strongest features. Being able to include “missing” variables –

such as habits – in a controlled fashion makes up for its shortcomings when compared to more

complex models like Triandis' (1979) “Theory of Interpersonal Behaviour” (TIB), which in cer-

tain contexts has been found to have a higher predictive ability (Bamberg and Schmidt, 2003;

Valois et al., 1988). Therefore, this study will mitigate some of the above mentioned shortcom-

ings of the core TPB model by extending it with the already discussed additional constructs in

chapter 2.3.

In the end, and despite all criticism, the TPB seems like a good fit for this study: it has already

been tested for explaining and predicting food consumption in various contexts (Conner and

Armitage, 1998; Godin and Kok, 1996; Tobler et al., 2011), including meat and dairy (Graça et

al., 2015a; Hung‐ Yi Lu et al., 2010), and it is flexible enough to be extended with required vari-

ables.

2.5 Hypotheses

Summarized in Figure 2 below are salient variables which research identified to have an influ-

ence on maintaining a vegan diet, mapped to the TPB framework.

For this study, the intention refers to the intention of consumers to maintain the vegan diet. “Atti-

tude” refers be a person´s positive or negative feelings toward veganism, the “subjective norm”

to much the person wishes to respect and follow the opinions of people important to him, and the

“perceived behavioural control” refers to a person’s perceived belief of how easy or difficult it

will be to stick to the vegan diet.

Based upon the review of relevant theories and findings, and in combination with the research

question, the following hypotheses have been developed and will be tested:

Theoretical Framework 17

H11: Former meat-eaters feel a significantly stronger influence from their social environment

than former vegetarians.

H12: Of all variables of the original constructs (attitude, subjective norm and perceived behav-

ioural control), the perceived behavioural control has the biggest influence on the intention.

H13: The PBC value is significantly higher for participants who have been vegan for a longer

time.

H14: Participants who show high results in the 4N´s have a significantly lower intention to

maintain the vegan diet in the next 6 months than participants with low results.

H15: Meat attachment negatively correlates with the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

H16: Cheese attachment negatively correlates with the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

To the author´s best knowledge, no research has been conducted yet to identify the variables in-

fluencing the maintenance of a vegan diet in Germany. It is the aim of this study to fill this gap.

Furthermore, special attention will be paid if with “cheese attachment” there exists a concept

similar to the already identified “meat attachment”.

Theoretical Framework 18

Figure 2 - Aligned Model of TPB (Ajzen, 1991)

H14 H15

Additional Constructs

Original

Constructs

Attitude toward the behaviour

Perceived behavioural

control

Subjective norm

H11

H12

2 H13

Intention to

maintain the

vegan diet

H16

Cheese attachment

Ambi- valence

Attitudes 4N

Behaviour

Meat attachment

Method 19

3 Method

3.1 Research Philosophy and Approach

Which variables are influencing the maintenance of a vegan diet in Germany? The aim of this

research has been approached with a subjective ontology and a positivist philosophical perspec-

tive on the epistemology. Positivism is a philosophical position linked to empirical research

based on the assumption that reality can be objectively perceived, described and apprehended.

Common methodologies include the testing of a theory, such as in a survey (Sobh and Perry,

2006).

The aim of a deductive approach is to affirm different assumptions based on a theory, which

might explain relationships between the variables, and to further question these objectively by

developing hypotheses (Saunders et al., 2016). This study has thus applied a deductive approach

based on the already existing Theory of Planned Behaviour. Different variables influencing con-

sumer behaviour have already been identified by researchers, and this study tested the relation-

ships between those.

3.2 Research Strategy

This thesis has further applied the single (mono-method) data collection technique in form of an

online questionnaire with quantitative data analysis procedures. According to Groves et al.

(2009), surveys in general are “one of the most commonly used method in the social sciences

(…) to test theories of behavior” (p. 3). Furthermore, quantitative surveys allow for the collec-

tion of a large amount of data from a considerably large population in very time- and cost-eco-

nomical ways (Saunders et al., 2012).

Benefits of online surveys are that they are not bound to geographical or chronological con-

straints - they are available 24/7 and can be taken by participants wherever and whenever they

have sufficient time. Having an online connection is a requirement, which according to a study

conducted in 2015 is the case for about 80% of the Germans population (Tippelt and Kup-

ferschmitt, 2015). Furthermore, online distributed questionnaires are usually perceived as author-

itative, trusted and popular by people, and are usually easy to explain and to understand by par-

ticipants (Casler et al., 2013; Davidov and Depner, 2011; Saunders et al., 2012).

Method 20

Some disadvantages of data elicitation via self-administered surveys have been described by

Wray and Bloomer (2006). They point out that, due to the static nature of online questionnaires,

there is no way to clarify potential misunderstandings, which can lead to implausible results.

Furthermore, with self-administered surveys there is always the possibility of manipulation - par-

ticipants are i.e. able to provide wrong demographic data and deliberately skew their answers.

Another issue is a low overall response rate or a high number of incomplete questionnaires.

Most of these shortcomings, however, hold true for offline surveys as well and can be mitigated

by careful survey design.

3.3 Sampling

Two types of sampling techniques exist: in probability (representative) sampling, the entire pop-

ulation is known and can theoretically (‘probably’) be selected, while in non-probability sam-

pling that is not the case (Saunders, 2016). The latter is true for this study: the numbers of vegan

consumers in Germany are estimated, but not precisely known. It is also not known who exactly

of the German population is vegan. Hence, the data for this study was collected through non-

probability sampling.

It is estimated that only about 1.1% of the consumers are vegan in Germany, and due to the low

number they belong to the ‘hard-to-reach population’ (Marpsat and Razafindratsima, 2010).

Haverstock et al. (2012) found that a group membership in social networks seems to be common

among vegans. Hence, this research has focused on reaching vegans through social networks.

The biggest social network in Germany (and worldwide) is Facebook (FB): approximately 42

million people are using FB in Germany, and about 23 million of them use it on a daily basis

(Tippelt and Kupferschmitt, 2015). A search for German vegan groups in Facebook resulted in

more than 100 groups with approximately more than 100.000 members. And whereas usually in-

ternet users tend to be younger and over-proportionally male (Saunders, 2012), in the case of

German Facebook the users age and gender groups are normal distributed within the range of 14-

49 year olds (Tippelt and Kupferschmitt, 2015). The age range of people older than 50 years was

underrepresented, as they are usually underrepresented in social media.

In Facebook, users create profiles and can join groups of interest. By posting a link with a de-

scription of the survey to such a groups, their members have been able to take part in the re-

search. This sampling type is called self-selection (Saunders et al., 2012). Furthermore, the social

Method 21

network facilitates the sharing of content, which made it well-suited for the snowballing tech-

nique: vegan group members can share the link with vegan friends that are not part of the group

or even have no Facebook account.

While internet samples in general have found to be consistent with findings from traditional

methods (Gosling et al., 2004) it is important to note that self-selection and snowballing sam-

pling also have methodological weaknesses. Self-selection is more likely to elicit data from par-

ticipants who have an especially strong interest or opinion on the topic. With snowballing it is

also likely that the sample was not representative of the target population.

However, Baltar and Brunet (2012), who specifically analysed Facebook as a sampling method,

conclude that it is an appropriate tool for hard-to-reach populations. The applicability of Face-

book as a valid tool for sampling in research has also been recognized by a number of different

authors (Thomson and Ito, 2014; Wilson et al., 2012). Other researchers with a focus on vegetar-

ians/vegans studies have used these sampling methods on Facebook as well (Beardsworth and

Keil, 1991; Haverstock and Forgays, 2012).

Approach on Facebook

The link to the questionnaire was published on different German Facebook vegan groups for

nine days, from the 17th – 26th of April. Furthermore, since the research is done during an intern-

ship at the charitable Albert Schweitzer Foundation for Our Contemporaries in Berlin, the organ-

isation also posted a link of the online survey to Facebook users who have liked their page (ap-

prox. 80.000 users). An example of a group posting on FB is added in the Appendix.

It should be emphasized that not all users were able to see the post – Facebook has its own secret

algorithm on who sees which post, and accurate information on how often the post will be dis-

tributed is not available. The same applies to the postings in groups – it is uncertain how many

people have actually seen it. Yet, the website Unipark, where the online survey was hosted, pro-

vided information on how many people clicked on the link and thus supplied numbers of com-

pletion rates.

As an incentive the users were offered to participate in a voucher raffle with an overall value of

30 EUR from different companies in Germany. Ideally, the survey would have been administered

to a target group of more than 200 participants.

Method 22

Sampling Summary

To summarize the sampling technique: it is impossible to enumerate the entire population of

German vegan consumers, not only because of economic reasons but also because the precise en-

tirety of the vegan population in Germany is unknown.

Since vegan consumers however can readily be accessed in special-interest groups on Facebook,

and as vegan Facebook users can forward the survey link to other vegans they know, the non-

probability sampling techniques self-selection and snowballing were chosen for this study.

Even though the sampling techniques are linked with self-selection biases and the findings of the

data cannot be generalized to the whole population, these sampling types have enabled to reach a

population that might be difficult to reach otherwise.

3.4 Formative Research and Pre-Test

The Theory of Planned Behaviour requires formative research, as the beliefs the model is based

on (attitude, normative and control beliefs) need to be tested for every new behaviour. Ajzen rec-

ommends that at least five (to nine) salient beliefs should be identified, which are specific towards

a population and a context (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980). Ajzen (2002) emphasizes that “to secure

reliable, internally consistent measures [of belief], it is necessary to select appropriate items in the

formative stages of the investigation”. This process is also defined as ‘eliciting beliefs’ (p. 4).

Eliciting beliefs were ensured by asking participants to generate a list of factors they believe could

hinder them to perform the behaviour: the maintenance of their vegan diet. The 15 participants for

the formative research were chosen through voluntary participation during vegan ‘cracker-barrel’

in Cologne, Germany. The salient findings were then utilized in the final questionnaire.

A pre-test of the questionnaire was run to make sure the length of time and the wording is appro-

priate. Furthermore, the findings of the pre-test were tested upon reliability for the final question-

naire. The items were then selected depending on how high or low they scored in the total score.

The pre-test was conducted on 10 vegan people who are either friends or acquainted with the

researcher.

3.5 Questionnaire Design

The nature of this survey has been cross-sectional, meaning that data was only collected at one

point in time. This is a disadvantage for studies on behaviour, as longitudinal studies enable to

not only measure the intention but also actual behaviour. Since the time frame for this research

Method 23

was limited, however, a longitudinal study is not feasible and would have to be conducted in fu-

ture research.

This study has followed Ajzen´s (2006) recommendations in ‘Constructing a theory of planned

behaviour questionnaire’ on how to develop questions for the TPB. In addition, further variables

from previous research were included, together with some self-developed items.

Table 2 is providing an outline of the different variables this study has examined with one or

more survey items. The full questionnaire with all items is included in the Appendix.

3.6 Operationalisation

To verify the hypotheses, it was necessary to operationalise the different variables of the model

into measurable items. For that the constructs of the model (core and additional) were divided

into dependent and independent variables. The empirically collected data was then tested by

means of different analyses.

Regarding the measures, Ajzen (2002) comments that “attitude, subjective norm and perceived

behavioral control can be measured by asking direct questions about the capability to perform a

behaviour, or indirectly on the basis of beliefs about the ability to deal with specific inhibiting or

facilitating factors” (p. 668). He explains that it is sufficient to assess the direct measures if the

purpose of the research is to predict intentions and behaviour, while indirect beliefs should be

assessed when planning more extensive research, i.e. for behavioural change interventions. Since

the scope of this research was limited, only direct measures were assessed.

Dependent and Independent Variables

A dependent variable (often called ‘outcome’ variable) is being affected by changes of the inde-

pendent variables (Field, 2013). Since the intention is being influenced by different variables, in

this study it was the dependent variable. It consisted of one item that retrieved a self-stated inten-

tion of participants.

For the analyses, the independent variables had to be determined. These independent variables

(also called ‘predictor’ variables) cause an effect on the dependent variable (Field, 2013). As de-

fined by Ajzen (1991), the independent variables in this study were the attitude, the subjective

norm and the PBC, as well as the additional constructs Attitudes 4N, meat and cheese attachments

and ambivalence.

The model has an equitation on how to measure the intention (Ajzen, 1991):

Method 24

It illustrates how the intention is composed through the total values of each of the three original

TPB constructs: attitude (‘AB’), subjective norms (‘SN’) and perceived behavioural control

(‘PBC’).

Scaling

The scales express a level of agreement/disagreement, and are essential for measuring attitudes.

Ajzen recommends using any attitude scaling procedure (Ajzen, 2002), either unipolar (Likert)

or bipolar (semantic differential), but does not give further instructions if a 5-point or a 7-point

scale yields higher results for unipolar scales.

Nunnally and Bernstein (1994), who tested different scales, find that the differences in how 5-

point and 7-point scales score are relatively small. Since 85% of the German Facebook users surf

Facebook on mobile devices (Wiese, 2016) it was argued that the usability of a 5-point scale

might be higher for those participants. There is also some discussion in research if Likert scales

should be measured on interval or ordinal levels (Jamieson, 2004; Knapp, 1990). This study has

treated them as ordinals, as stated by Pallant (2011).

The scales in this study thus ranged from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Some items

have been asked reversely to counteract answer-tendencies, which were later recoded for the

analysis.

3.6.1 Scale Creation and Analysis Method for Testing

Table 1 lists the included items as well as the scale creation method and which statistical anal-

yses for all relevant scales were applied.

Table 1 - Scale Creation

Variable Items used Scale

crea-

tor

Source Hypo-

thesis

Analysis method

for testing

Independent Variables - Original constructs Attitude 1. good – bad

2. harmful – beneficial

3. unpleasant – pleasant

4. unenjoyable - enjoyable

Mean - Ajzen (2006) -

“Constructing a TpB

Questionnaire: Con-

ceptual and Methodo-

logical Considerations”

&

/ Hierarchical

Multiple Regres-

sion

Pearson´s Corre-

lation

Method 25

- Povey et al., (2001) -

“Attitudes towards fol-

lowing meat, vegetar-

ian and vegan diets: an

examination of the role

of ambivalence”

Subjective Norm

1. My family thinks I should

eat a vegan diet

2. My friends think I should

eat a vegan diet

3. Regarding my diet, I

comply with what my

family thinks

4. Regarding my diet, I

comply with what my

friends thinks.

Mean - Ajzen (2006) -

“Constructing a TpB

Questionnaire: Con-

ceptual and Methodo-

logical Considerations”

&

- Povey et al., (2001) -

“Attitudes towards fol-

lowing meat, vegetar-

ian and vegan diets: an

examination of the role

of ambivalence”

& self-developed

H11

Hierarchical

Multiple Regres-

sion

t-test

PBC 1. It depends mostly on

myself if I maintain my

diet.

2. I am confident I will

maintain the vegan diet in

the following six months.

3. I find eating vegan easy.

Possible answers on a Likert

scale for the question

“Which influence have the

following factors on your

intention to stay vegan in

the next six months?”:

4. Higher costs

5. Change of eating habits

6. Low availability of vegan

substitute products in

supermarkets

7. Low availability of vegan

foods when eating out

5. Low knowledge about

vegan recipes and cooking

Mean - Ajzen (2006) -

“Constructing a TpB

Questionnaire: Con-

ceptual and Methodo-

logical Considerations”

&

- Povey et al., (2001) -

“Attitudes towards fol-

lowing meat, vegetar-

ian and vegan diets: an

examination of the role

of ambivalence”

&

- Graça et al, (2015) -

"Attached to meat?

(Un)Willingness and

intentions to adopt a

more plant-based diet"

& self-developed

H12

H13

Hierarchical

Multiple Regres-

sion

Pearson´s Corre-

lation

Method 26

Dependent variable

Intention Score - Fishbein & Ajzen

(1975) - "Belief, atti-

tude, intention and be-

havior: an introduction

to theory and research”

H12

H14

H15

H16

Hierarchical

Multiple Regres-

sion

Pearson´s Corre-

lation

Independent variables - Additional constructs

Attitudes

4N

1. Human beings are natural

meat-eaters.

2. A healthy diet requires at

least some meat.

3. It is abnormal for humans

not to eat meat.

4. Meat is delicious

Mean - Piazza et al. (2015) -

"Rationalizing meat

consumption. The 4Ns”

H14

Hierarchical

Multiple Regres-

sion

Pearson´s Corre-

lation

Meat

Attach-

ment

1. In the long term, I find not

eating meat challenging.

2. Since I´m not eating meat

anymore I feel weak.

3. I don´t mind not eating

meat.

Mean - Graça et al, (2015) -

"Attached to meat?

(Un)Willingness and

intentions to adopt a

more plant-based diet"

H15

Hierarchical

Multiple Regres-

sion

Pearson´s Corre-

lation

Cheese

Attach-

ment

1. In the long term, I find not

eating cheese challenging.

2. The prospect of a life

without cheese makes me a

little sad

3. I don´t mind not eating

cheese.

Mean - Self-developed (based

on Graça et al. (2015) -

"Attached to meat?

(Un)Willingness and

intentions to adopt a

more plant-based diet)

H16 Hierarchical

Multiple Regres-

sion

Pearson´s Corre-

lation

Ambiva-

lence

1. I feel torn between vegan

and non-vegan food

Mean - Berndsen et al.

(2004) “Ambivalence

towards meat”

/ Hierarchical

Multiple Regres-

sion

Pearson´s Corre-

lation

Socio-demographics

Socio-

De-

mographics

*not

mandatory

1. Age

2. Gender

3. Household size

4. Residential area*

5. Education

6. Income*

Valid

%

self-developed

/ Descriptive

statistics

Method 27

The complete survey in English (Table 19) and in its original language German can be found in

the Appendix (Table 20).

3.6.2 Preliminary Tests

The data set was checked for errors by inspecting the frequencies of each variable and by recoding

reversely phrased items. The different procedures for categorical variables and continuous variable

were considered before the total scores of the variable scales were calculated, so the data was not

distorted (Pallant, 2011). To get an overview of the data and information about the distribution

and frequencies of the participant´s responses, descriptive statistics were applied. Furthermore, the

mean for the variables was computed. The data was also screened for outliers. The 5% Trimmed

Mean showed no high differences between the original and the new trimmed mean for the varia-

bles.

It is a standard to test items for normality in order to check the assumptions of linear statistical

procedures, i.e. by using the The Kolmogorov-Smirnov. Significant results would indicate that the

data does not follow a normal distribution, as required by the statistical methods used in this paper

(i.e the t-test, multiple regression analysis). However, tests for normality will not be performed in

this work: The reason being that Lumley et al. (2002) emphasise that most tests do not require any

assumption of normal distribution if the sample size is sufficiently large (n = 500) (p. 166)

The negligibility of normality in large sample sizes is also shared by other scholars (Field, 2013;

Pallant, 2011). Since the sample size of this study is quite large (n = 2847), non-normality of the

data can be ignored.

3.6.3 Reliability

The reliability of a scale indicates how consistent the measures are (Saunders, 2016). Ajzen (2006)

emphasizes that for the TPB, the set of items to be used need to correlate highly with each other.

In order to test the internal consistency (the degree to which the items of the scale are all measuring

the same underlying attribute) (Pallant, 2011), Cronbach’s coefficient alpha was used (Ajzen,

2006). Field (2013) argues that the Cronbach Alpha-values should be interpreted with caution, as

with a higher number of items also the α-value increases, which does not necessarily mean that the

items display a high internal consistency. Negatively coded items were reversed for the alpha-

analysis.

Method 28

Some of the Cronbach Alpha scores were below .7, as indicated by Kline (2000) for psychological

constructs such as the TPB’s. However, since all constructs consisted of less than ten items, Pal-

lant´s (2011) recommendation to measure the mean inter-item-correlation was (IIC) utilized. The

items showed an acceptable consistency within the optimal range between .2 to .4, sometimes

higher, which indicates good results.

Some scales, however, needed adjustments:

SN: In the Subjective Norm scale the items “I comply with what my family do” and “I comply

with what my friends does” had low IIC of .217 to .220 and were deleted. Even though according

to Pallant (2011) these items showed an acceptable internal consistency, it was decided to remove

them from the scale, since the Cronbach-Alpha value then increased to .715 and the IIC to .577.

PBC: While the Cronbach Alpha on the compounded PBC scale had a good value of .73, the item

“It depends mostly on myself if I maintain my diet” showed a weak IIC of .155 and had to be

deleted, after which the Cronbach Alpha value increased to .76.

Meat Attachment: The item “Since I am not eating meat anymore I feel weak” had a low IIC of

.160 and was deleted. The Cronbach Alpha value increased to .732 then.

Table 17 in the Appendix shows the reliability after item-correction.

3.6.4 t-tests, Pearson´s Correlation and Multiple Regression

A t-test allowed to look at differences between two groups. The two means of the scales of former

meat eaters versus former vegetarians were compared. For each test prior to looking at the p-

value, a Levene test for homogeneity of variances was carried out.

To test linear relationships between two metric variables, correlations analyses using Pearson’s r

were performed. In order to identify influences on the dependent variable, multiple regression

analyses were the statistical tool of choice. To be able to interpret the correlation strength, Cohen

(1988) gives some indications: values of 0 indicate no correlation at all, while small correlations

are between r = .10 to .29 and medium correlations between r= .30 to .49. Large correlations are

between .50 to 1.0, with 1 indicating a perfect correlation.

The multiple regression analysis enabled exploration about the relationships between the depend-

ent and independent variables, and illustrated the strengths and direction of the correlations be-

tween each variable. Since the sample size is large, the model was evaluated using the R2. The R2

value shows how much of the variance in the dependent variable intention is explained by the

Method 29

model. In order to know which of the variables contributes the most to the prediction of the de-

pendent variable, the Standardized Beta values were compared. The data was also checked for

multicollinearity. The tables showed no bivariate correlations of .7, which would indicate that

correlations between the independent variables are too high. The tolerance and VIF values also

indicated no multicollinearity, with values less than .10 and above .1.

P-values lower than α = 5% were considered statistically significant and marked accordingly.

3.7 Limitations

Internet as a sampling technique is typically connected with the selection bias problem (Baltar

and Brunet, 2012). It is important to acknowledge that findings through web-based sampling

methods often cannot be generalized to the whole population. Furthermore, elderly people were

underrepresented in the data, as they are usually underrepresented in social media, too. Also,

consumers that either do not use the internet or are not on Facebook were not included. Thus,

findings of the study were not representative to all vegan consumers in Germany. Another limita-

tion is that the ‘intention’-variable was retrieved through only one item, whereas it is recom-

mended to use a set of different questions.

Empirical Findings 30

4 Empirical Findings

According to the survey hosting software Unipark, the link to the survey was clicked 5715 times

and resulted in 3519 participants who completed the questionnaire. This is a completion rate of

62%. The data was then cleaned by deleting all participants who indicated to be non-vegans (n =

401). Furthermore, missing data sets (participants who had not filled out all mandatory fields)

were deleted. From 3519 participants who completed the survey this led to a cleared data set of n

= 2847 participants, representing 49,8% of all participants.

4.1 Descriptive Statistics

The demographics (Table 9 to Table 16) are illustrated in the Appendix. As can be seen in Table

9, the majority of the participants was female (80,3%) and relatively young – the majority of the

participants ranged between 18 to 35 (Table 10). ‘Animal welfare’ achieved the highest mean as a

reason to turn vegan amongst the reasons animal welfare, health, environment and social environ-

ment (Table16).

4.2 Analysis of the Theoretical Model

Correlation of Variables

At first, to explore the relationship of the original variables of the models amongst themselves, a

multiple regression analysis was conducted. As can be seen in the correlation matrix in Table 2,

there were medium and small correlations: a significant, medium sized and positive correlation

existed between the attitude and the perceived behavioural control (r = .358, p < .001). The positive

correlation between the attitude and the subjective norm was also significant, but quite small

(r = .036, p = .01). The positive significant correlation between the subjective norm and the per-

ceived behavioural control was also small (r = .046, p = .05).

Empirical Findings 31

Table 2 - Correlation of Original Constructs

Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation of the Original Constructs of the TPB

Variables 1 2 3

Attitude - .036* .358***

Subjective Norm - .046*

PBC -

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, n = 2847

In the next step, the intention variable was added.

Table 3 - Correlation of Original Constructs and Intention

Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation of the Original Constructs of the TPB

and the Intention to Maintain the Vegan Diet

Variables 1 2 3 4

Intention - .319*** .036* .403***

Attitude - .036* .358***

Subjective Norm - .046*

PBC -

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, n = 2847

Table 3 shows that the strongest correlation here was between the intention to maintain the vegan

diet and the perceived behavioural control (r = .403, p < .001). According to Cohen´s effect size,

a medium correlation was at hand. The positive correlation between the intention and the attitude

also was of medium size (r = .319, p < .001), while the subjective norm and the intention only

correlated weakly (r = .036, p < .001).

In the next step, the additional independent variables (attitudes 4N, meat attachment, cheese at-

tachment and ambivalence), were entered.

Empirical Findings 32

Table 4 - Correlation of All Variables

Pearson’s Product Moment Correlation of the Original Constructs of the TPB

and the Intention to Maintain the Vegan Diet

Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

Intention - .319*** .036* .403*** -.186*** -.225*** -.337*** -.366***

Attitude - .036* .358*** -.358*** -.211*** -.298*** -.281***

Subjective Norm - .046* .015 -.022 -.034* -.038*

PBC - -.226*** -.312*** -.503*** -.507***

Attitudes 4N - .353*** .238*** .231***

Meat Attachment - .267*** .306***

Cheese Attachment - .485***

Ambivalence -

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, n = 2847

The output (Table 4) revealed that all the additional constructs were negatively influencing the

intention, and that several small to large effects existed.

The largest negative correlations on the intention were medium-sized: ambivalence (r = -.366, p <

.001) and cheese attachment (r = -.337, p < .001). This indicates that amongst all the additional

variables, ambivalent feelings among the vegan diet and an attachment for cheese negatively cor-

relate the most negatively with the intention to maintain the plant-based diet. Meat attachment (r

= -.225, p < .001) and the Attitudes 4N (r = -.186, p < .001) also negatively influenced the inten-

tion, indicating that participants who endorse the Attitudes 4N seem to have a strong attachment

to meat.

A large effect of a negative correlation existed between the perceived behavioural control and

ambivalence (r = -.507, p < .001), showing that if participants who had low perceived behavioural

control had the highest ambivalent feelings towards the vegan diet. Cheese attachment also nega-

tively correlated with the ambivalence (r = -.485, p < .001), thus indicating that the higher an

attachment for cheese, the more ambivalent the participants are.

Empirical Findings 33

By adding the variables step-wise into the multiple regression, the procedure enabled to see how

the additional variables increased the predictability of the intention (Table 5).

Table 5 - Hierarchical Regression for Intention

Summary of Hierarchical Regression Analysis for Variables predicting Intention

β t R R2

∆R2

.444 .197

Attitude .200 11.10***

Subjective Norm .014 .837

PBC .331 18.35***

Step 2 .446 .199 .002**

Attitude .185 9.788***

Subjective Norm .016 .925

PBC .325 17.96***

Attitudes 4N -.047 -2.61**

Step 3 .453 .205 .006***

Attitude .183 9.73***

Subjective Norm .014 .858

PBC .306 16.45***

Attitudes 4N -.023 -1.230

Meat Attachment -.083 -4.460***

Step 4 .468 .219 .014***

Attitude .169 9.01***

Subjective Norm .013 .779

PBC .246 12.20***

Attitudes 4N -.012 -.663

Meat Attachment -.070 -3.82***

Cheese Attachment -.140 -7.15***

Step 5 .484 .234 .015***

Attitude .161 8.66***

Subjective Norm .011 .680

PBC .199 9.52***

Attitudes 4N -.007 -.350

Meat Attachment -.053 -2.89***

Cheese Attachment -.098 -4.82***

Ambivalence -.154 -7.58***

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, n = 2847

Step 1

Variable

Empirical Findings 34

The hierarchical multiple regression revealed that at step one the variables attitude and PBC both

contributed significantly to the regression model, F (3, 2842) = 232.91, p < .001) and together

accounted for 19.6% of the variation, while the subjective norm was not significant.

In the second step, the Attitudes 4N explained additional 0.2% of the intention’s variance, which

was significant despite the small amount, F (1, 2841) = 6.80, p = .009.

In the third step, the variable meat attachment explained additional 0.6%, F (1, 2840) = 19.90, p <

.001, while in the fourth step, cheese attachment added 1.4%, F (1, 2839) = 51.11, p < .001.

Lastly, in step 5, ambivalence explained 1.5 %, F (1, 2838) = 57.40, p < .001. All variables together

finally accounted for 23.2%, the changes however were so small that they did not substantially

contribute to explaining the variance in intention.

Empirical Findings 35

4.3 Hypotheses Testing

H11 – Subjective Norm

Former meat-eaters feel a significantly stronger influence from their social environment than

former vegetarians.

To test this hypothesis, a group variable was formed, including former vegetarians (group A; n =

1633) and former meat eaters (group B; n = 998). Some participants (n = 215) had stated that

they were pescetarians, meaning that they did eat fish but no meat. They were excluded from the

analysis as they are an in-between both groups.

An independent-samples t-test was then conducted. Since Levene´s p-value was .076 and hence

larger than .050, equal variances were assumed. The test indicated that there was a statistically

significant (t(2629) = 2.52, p = .012) higher influence of the subjective norm in the former vege-

tarian-group (M = 1.59, SD = 0.62) compared to the former meat-eater group (M = 1.52, SD =

0.60).

The magnitude of the mean differences was explained by the effect size (d = 0.10 and r = .005).

This indicates that former meat eaters – albeit the effect is small - feel a stronger pressure from

their social environment than former vegetarians.

The hypothesis was confirmed.

Empirical Findings 36

H12 – Perceived Behavioural Control (PBC) 01

Of all variables of the original constructs (attitude, subjective norm and perceived behavioural

control), the perceived behavioural control has the biggest influence on the intention.

The influence of the original variables attitude, subjective norm and perceived behavioural con-

trol on the intention was tested through a standard multiple regression analysis. The preliminary

analyses ensured that the assumptions of multicollinearity were not violated: the VIF values were

near 1, showing that the data was adequate for multiple regression analysis. The adjusted R2-

value explained 19.6% of variance on the intention, and the PBC item showed its largest contri-

bution with β = .331 (see Table 6).

The hypothesis was thus confirmed.

Table 6 - Hypothesis H2 - Multiple Regression

Standard Multiple Regression of the Original Variables

Scale b SE b β

Attitude .371 .033 .200***

Subjective Norm .015 .018 .014

Perceived Behavioural Control .362 .020 .331***

R2 = .196, *p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001, n = 2847

Empirical Findings 37

H13 – Perceived Behavioural Control (PBC) 02

The PBC value is significantly higher for participants who have been vegan for a longer time.

The assumed relationship, conducted through Pearson´s product-moment correlation, was con-

firmed and significant (r = .435, p < .001). The strength of the positive correlation, illustrated

with the r-value, is of medium-to-strong quality, thereby demonstrating that the perceived behav-

ioural control is higher for participants who have been vegan for a longer time.

The hypothesis was thus confirmed.

To get a more detailed understanding Table 7 illustrates which items of the compounded PBC

scale received the highest agreement. The items “Change of eating habits” (M = 4.70, SD = .759)

and “Low knowledge about vegan recipes and cooking” (M = 4.65, SD = .900) received consid-

erably high agreements.

Table 7 - Descriptive Statistics of PBC Scale

Descriptive Statistics of the PBC Scale

Min. Max. Mean SD

It depends mostly on myself if I maintain my diet. 1 5 4.80 0.59

I am confident I will maintain the vegan diet in

the following six months.

1 5 4.84 0.52

I find eating vegan easy. 1 5 4.25 0.90

Higher costs 1 5 4.51 0.94

Change of eating habits 1 5 4.70 0.76

Low availability of vegan substitute products 1 5 4.46 .097

Low availability of vegan foods when eating out 1 5 3.98 1.25

Low knowledge about vegan recipes and cooking 1 5 4.65 0.90

n = 2756

Empirical Findings 38

H14 – Attitudes 4N

Participants who show high results in the 4N´s have a significantly lower intention to maintain

the vegan diet in the next 6 months than participants with low results.

The relationship between the Attitudes 4N and the intention variable was measured through Pear-

son´s product-moment correlation coefficient. There was a significant negative correlation be-

tween the intention to maintain the vegan diet and the Attitudes 4N (r = -.184, p < .001). The size

of the correlation coefficient indicated that a negative, linear correlation of small strength was at

hand.

The hypothesis was thus confirmed.

Table 8 illustrates in detail which item of the compounded Attitude 4N scale received the highest

agreement: it was “A healthy diet requires at least some meat” (M = 4.89, SD = .085), which re-

fers to the “necessary” of the 4N.

Table 8 - Descriptive Statistics of Attitudes 4N Scale

Descriptive Statistics of the Attitudes 4N Scale

Min. Max. Mean SD

(Natural) Human beings are natural

meat-eaters.

1 5 4.50 0.85

(Neccessary) A healthy diet requires at

least some meat.

1 5 4.89 0.43

(Normal) It is abnormal for humans not

to eat meat.

1 5 4.75 0.70

(Nice) Meat is delicious. 1 5 3.73 1.34

n = 2847

Empirical Findings 39

H15 – Meat Attachment

Meat attachment negatively correlates with the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

To analyse the influence of meat attachment on the intention, a Pearson´s product-moment corre-

lation analysis was conducted. The analysis revealed a significant negative correlation (r = -.225,

p < .001). According to Cohen´s effect size (1988), this constitutes as a small-to medium effect.

The hypothesis was thus confirmed. The intention to maintain a vegan diet was lower if the meat

attachment increased as well.

Empirical Findings 40

H16 - Cheese Attachment

Cheese attachment negatively correlates with the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

The same procedure as with the meat attachment was carried out for this analysis. A Pearson’s

Product Moment Correlation analysis was conducted to test the influence of cheese on the inten-

tion. The correlation revealed a negative linear relation of medium strength (r = -.337, p < .001).

The hypothesis was thus confirmed. An attachment for cheese negatively correlates with the in-

tention.

Discussion 41

5 Discussion

The purpose of this research was to analyse the challenges influencing the maintenance of a vegan

diet in Germany. As discussed earlier in the theoretical framework, the applied model, based on

the Theory of Planned Behaviour, assumes that intention is an immediate antecedent of behaviour

and based on the three different psychological constructs: attitude, subjective norm and perceived

behavioural control. The TPB is analysed by means of a t-test, correlations and standard and hier-

archical multiple regressions.

In this chapter, the results for each formulated hypothesis will be discussed. The variables analysed

are subjective norm, perceived behavioural control, attitudes 4N, meat attachment, cheese attach-

ment and ambivalence. All the hypotheses were confirmed.

5.1 Characteristics of the Sample

Before the discussion of the hypotheses, the characteristics of the sample shall be mentioned. As

can be seen in Table 4 in the Appendix, the majority of the participants was female. This can be

due for various reasons: first, previous research found that survey respondents in general are more

likely to be female (Porter and Whitcomb, 2005; Sax et al., 2003). Second, findings in the context

of meat consumption/avoidance report a similar gender skewness (Haverstock and Forgays, 2012;

Mooney and Walbourn, 2001). Finally, the majority of vegans in Germany seems to be female,

too (VEBU, 2015), which makes the gender distribution of the sample plausible.

With regard to the sample selection and sample size it should be mentioned that the study does not

claim to be representative for all vegan consumers in Germany - due to the sampling method it

cannot be. The large sample size (n = 2847) however, suggests that it is typical for vegan consum-

ers in Germany who use Facebook as a communication tool.

5.2 Intention

The intention variable in this study consisted of only one item, which is a weakness, as Ajzen

(1996) recommends that a set of different questions should be used for this latent (not directly

observable) variable. This might explain why only 23.2% of the variance in intention was ex-

plained, which is considerably lower than what Sutton (1998) found in his meta-analysis of inten-

tions regarding the TBP. He showed that models using the TPB typically explain on average be-

tween 40% and 50% of the variance in intention. Godin and Kok (1996) have found different

values in their review of the TPB to health-related behaviours: they find that 66.2% of variance is

Discussion 42

explained by the intention, yet exempt this finding from “behavioural categories where the per-

ceived behavioural control plays a more important role than the intention” (p. 93). It could be

argued that for maintaining a diet the perceived behavioural control is of relative importance, since

the behaviour in question is carried out on a daily basis, in varying locations. This might illustrate

why in this study the explained variance is lower than in their meta-analyses, since the mentioned

PBC value explained provided a fairly large amount of prediction on the intention variable

It is important however to acknowledge that intentions can be measured differently: this study

applied the standardized regression analysis. Another possibility to process the intention is through

structural equitation. There, however, due to the formative nature of the model, it is necessary to

measure behaviour in a test-retest as well, which is not possible in a cross-sectional study.

5.3 Attitudes and Attitudes 4N

H14: Participants who show high results in the 4N´s have a significantly lower intention to

maintain the vegan diet in the next 6 months than participants with low results

An attitude in general explains how a consumer thinks about (evaluates) a particular behaviour.

The results were heavily skewed: the participants had a very positive attitude, which is not sur-

prising – asking vegans about their vegan attitude would naturally obtain rather positive scores,

such as analysed by Povey et. al. (2001), who found that participants had the most positive feelings

for their own dietary behaviour. The attitude contributed to the prediction of the intention, which

is in line with previous research (Cron and Pobocik, 2013).

More interesting in this context however were the more specific Attitudes 4N. They refer to the

belief that eating meat is either natural, necessary, normal or nice. Piazza et al. (2015), who devel-

oped the 4Ns and conducted different studies, including vegans, aimed to test the 4Ns with regard

to different aspects like animal welfare or meat justifications, while in this study the scale was

used as an auxiliary dimension to study its impact on the intention to maintain the vegan diet.

Generally, it can be argued that obtaining the Attitude 4Ns from participants who are already ve-

gan is futile. The aim of this hypothesis was however to test whether the endorsement of the 4Ns

influences the maintenance of the vegan diets. The results showed that this was indeed the case:

The Attitudes 4N negatively influenced the intention.

Another interesting finding is that in this study the highest item mean for the 4N constructs stems

from the item “A healthy diet requires at least some meat”, with a margin of almost one point. This

contradicts the findings of Piazza et al. (2015), where the highest endorsement from vegans was

Discussion 43

that meat is natural. A possible reason for this deviation might be due to the questionnaire design:

unlike Piazza´s study, each ‘N’ was elicited with only one item. So whether German vegans, unlike

the Australian vegans in Piazza´s study, indeed subscribe more to necessity of meat consumption

instead of its naturalness could be clarified in a future study where the scale consists of more items.

With regards to the model however, the Attitudes 4N provided a significant, yet only very small

amount of prediction on the intention variable. Since the Attitude 4N-construct was not tested on

the TPB before, a comparability with other studies is not possible. It might be that the construct

is not well suited for this kind of model, or that the low predictability indicates that for vegan

participants, the Attitudes 4N are not a very important consideration in their intention formation.

5.4 Subjective Norm

H11: Former meat-eaters feel a significantly stronger influence from their social environment

than former vegetarians.

Subjective norms (SN) refers to the perceived social pressure that is put upon the participants to

perform (or not perform) a certain behaviour. It was hypothesised that that former meat-eaters

would feel a stronger pressure from their social environment than former vegetarians. Partici-

pants who were vegetarians before turning vegan are typically used to being commented on their

dietary behaviour from their social environment, since abstaining from meat is not the norm in

Western societies. It was hence assumed that former meat-eaters might feel a higher social pres-

sure, as they are not used to stand out. The hypothesis was confirmed: former meat eaters do feel

a higher pressure - but only very slightly. The study did not assess the length people were vege-

tarian before – if participants were vegetarian only for a short duration it might have influenced

the results.

Interesting in this context was also that the SN construct had the lowest influence on the inten-

tion, for both former meat-eaters and vegetarians. There are different possibilities how this can

be interpreted. One way would be to assume that former meat eaters indeed feel only little pres-

sure from their social environment, and that it also does not influence their intention. Since the

majority of the participants however stated that their environment is not supportive of their diet,

and in regard that Steptoe (2004) and Paisley et al. (2008) found that the social influence and the

‘significant other’ are important factors when changing dietary habits, this interpretation should

be regarded with care.

Discussion 44

Another and maybe more plausible possibility to explain the low scores is the low predictability

of the SN-construct itself. In the present study the SN-variable provided only a very small and

insignificant amount of prediction on the intention variable, compared with the other two TPB-

variables attitude and PBC. The low overall predictability was also supported by the other analyses

carried out.

The low values for the SN construct however are in line with previous research: while Graça et al.

identified the SN as the weakest predictor in the TPB model (2015a), Povey´s et al. (2001) even

found the SN to be non-significant to predict intentions. Armitage and Conner (2001), who con-

ducted a meta-analysis on the TPB, also found the SN to have poor predictive power. Ajzen (1991)

himself explains that according to the theory, none of the three original constructs have to make a

significant contribution to the prediction of the intention, since the importance of each construct

varies from behaviour to behaviour.

A possible way to overcome the poor predictability of the SN and to analyse the social influence

of the vegan diet would be to conduct a qualitative study instead. Since the TPB is based on self-

reported answers, it might be more insightful to observe actual behaviour of vegans.

5.5 Perceived Behavioural Control

H12: Of all variables of the original constructs (attitude, subjective norm and perceived behav-

ioural control), the perceived behavioural control has the biggest influence on the intention.

The perceived behavioural control (PBC) variable refers to how easy or difficult participants find

the implementation of a certain behaviour. In this study, the PBC variable provided the largest

amount of prediction on the intention variable between all original constructs.

In other studies, the explanatory power of the PBC was similarly confirmed: Povey et al. (2001)

found that the PBC was the strongest sole predictor of intention for vegetarian and vegan diets

(Povey et al., 2001), and Armitage and Conner (2001) also found that the PBC usually adds more

prediction to the intention than attitude and SN.

Perceived behavioural control exerts a strong influence on participants’ intention to maintain a

plant-based diet and will be further analysed in the context of the next hypothesis.

H13: The PBC value is significantly higher for participants who have been vegan for a longer

time.

Discussion 45

The hypothesis was confirmed: time has an influence on the PBC values. Participants who are

vegan for a longer duration find the maintenance of a vegan diet easier. This finding is in line with

Cron and Pobocik (2013), who found that practicing vegetarianism for a longer time resulted in

higher perceived behavioural control.

In order to understand which are the factors that influence the PBC the strongest, the author com-

pared the single items that contributed to the PBC scale of the survey. Two items obtained the

highest agreement of the scale with regards to difficulties.

The first was “Change of eating habits”. While this study did not explicitly analyse habitual be-

haviour, a change of eating habits is an often-cited finding in previous research (Armitage and

Conner, 1999; Godin and Kok, 1996; Zur and Klöckner, 2014). Since the concept of habits – and

how to change them – is quite complex (Triandis, 1979) and would go beyond the scope of this

discussion, as a guide for future research Lewin´s (1997) three-stage model of change shall be

mentioned.

The second PBC item that requested which factor might influence the intention to maintain the

vegan diet was “little knowledge on how to prepare vegan foods”. This is an often cited finding

(Hoek et al., 2011; Lea and Worsley, 2001; Lea et al., 2006; Twine, 2014) - a lack of know-how

on vegan practice might act as a possible barrier to maintain the diet. Haverstock et al. (2012),

who studied participants who had been former vegetarians or vegans, found that one reason why

participants stopped their plant-based diet was because they had difficulties preparing vegetarian

or vegan food. The unfamiliarity with vegan substitutes is also reported in Hoek´s et al. (Hoek et

al., 2011) study, where they find that it acts as a key barrier for meat-eaters.

The overall findings suggest that both internal and external factors the PBC consists of should be

increased in order to help vegan consumers to maintain their diet. Internal resources refer to

skills about vegan food preparation, while external refer to the availability of vegan products. An

interesting area for future research would be to find out which specific measures and actions

might increase the perceived behavioural control.

5.6 Meat Attachment

H15: Meat attachment negatively correlates with the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

Meat attachment is an affective additional construct for the TPB developed by Graça et al.

(2015a), which measures if consumers have an affinity towards meat, hindering them from

adopting plant-based diets. For this study, the construct was used to measure if meat attachment

Discussion 46

might influence the maintenance of a vegan diet. It was only tested on participants who had

stated that they were meat eaters before.

Graça et al. (2015a) found that their developed scale predicted -1.03 of the variance for the inten-

tion towards meat substitution. In this study, the incremental validity of meat attachment was

lower. According to Cohen´s (1988) effect size, the unique effect of the meat attachment was

small. It could be that the vegan participants perceived the meat-related questions as disturbing

or sceptical towards their dietary style. It might have been useful if the data entry instruction on

the questionnaire would have been formulated more sensitively. Another possibility why the ad-

ditionally explained variance was lower than in the original study could be that this study used

less items than the original meat attachment scale. Furthermore, like with the cheese attachment,

it might be that the validity of the meat attachment construct would have increased if the partici-

pants would have been analysed in groups, giving the time they are vegan. According to Pro-

chaska and Velicer (1997), consumers get accustomed to change with the time they are perform-

ing a behaviour in question. The analyses in this study however did not differentiate between

participants who turned vegan recently and participants who are vegan for years.

While the added variance was low, meat attachment itself correlated with several other variables.

The results indicated that vegans due to the meat attachment had a lower intention to maintain

the vegan diet.

A possibility to counteract this is suggested by Lea and Worsley (2003), who identified affinity

for meat as the main barrier towards changing to a plant-based diet. They argue that the market-

ing of plant-based diets should focus on the tastiness of plant-based meals.

Since this study did not analyse the effects of meat substitutes to meat attachment or the mainte-

nance of the vegan diet, future research is necessary to study if vegan meat alternatives are able

to reduce meat attachment as a challenge for the vegan diet.

Discussion 47

5.7 Cheese Attachment

H16: Cheese attachment negatively correlates with the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

To measure if cheese attachment correlated with the intention to maintain a vegan diet, parts of

the meat attachment questionnaire were adopted for this study and replaced with cheese items.

Even though cheese attachment made the second strongest unique contribution to explaining the

dependent ‘intention’ variable, the incremental validity of cheese attachment in this study was low.

According to Cohen´s (1988) effect size, the unique effect of the cheese attachment was small. It

might be that the validity of the cheese attachment construct would have increased if the partici-

pants would have been analysed in groups, giving the time they are vegan. According to Prochaska

and Velicer (1997), consumers get accustomed to change with the time they are performing a

behaviour in question. The analyses in this study however did not differentiate between partici-

pants who turned vegan recently and participants who are vegan for years. Another possibility why

the additionally explained variance was low could be that the scale itself consisted of fewer items

than the original meat attachment scale in the study, or that linguistically they were were not di-

rectly transferable from the meat attachment scale.

The correlation results however were clear: cheese attachment correlates negatively with the in-

tention. Moreover, cheese attachment had the second highest influence on the intention of all var-

iables. Although earlier studies did not specifically analyse cheese attachment for vegans, the find-

ings are in line with previous research, who found that hedonic appreciation of food in general is

a barrier for the consumption of plant-based foods (Graça et al., 2015b; Lea and Worsley, 2003)

The results points to cheese attachment being an important variable influencing the maintenance

of a vegan diet, even though the validity is relatively low. The reason cheese poses a bigger

threat to the intention than meat might be explained by the scarcity of vegan cheeses on the mar-

ket. While there are many different meat substitutes from tofu, seitan, etc., the manufacturing of

vegan cheese is in the early stages of development, which can be easily seen in supermarkets,

where the sheer size of meat alternatives surmounts vegan cheese. Furthermore, the taste and

texture of animal-based cheese is harder to copy than meat, which might explain why abstaining

from cheese seems (and remains) to be a challenge for vegan consumers.

The findings suggest that there is a market niche for vegan cheese, which could be an opportunity

for businesses.

Future research is needed to analyse if the cheese attachment varies for participants depending on

the time they are vegan. Furthermore, research is needed to analyse the consumers´ acceptance of

Discussion 48

new vegan cheese substitutes. An interesting area of research would be to study if vegetarians –

who already omit some animal-based products – have a cheese attachment, and if it is hindering

them from becoming vegan. Furthermore, it should be established if the cheese attachment of ve-

gans is limited to German consumer preferences, or if cheese attachment is a transnational phe-

nomenon.

5.8 Ambivalence

Ambivalence refers to a person having mixed beliefs about a topic. It was measured directly in

this study with a single item, which can be as criticized for having too low a reliability. Berndsen

et al. (2004), who also assessed the ambivalence directly in their study, used five items instead,

which increases reliability and predictability. Yet, the ambivalence item made the strongest unique

contribution to explaining the dependent intention variable after the cheese attachment variable,

and its addition improved the predictive value of the TPB model. Still, the unique effect of the

ambivalence was small according to Cohen´s (1988) effect size.

The ambivalence item showed strong correlations with the other variables. Unsurprisingly, partic-

ipants having high scores in ambivalence had a lower intention to maintain the vegan diet. Sparks

et al. (2001), who analysed ambivalence in the context of on meat consumption, also find that a

higher ambivalence associates with a weaker intention. In a study they confirmed that ambivalence

has a moderating2 effect in attitude-intention-relationship models like the TPB (2004).

In a correlation matrix with the three core constructs of the TPB, attitude, subjective norm and

perceived behavioural control, the PBC had the highest negative influence on the ambivalence: it

seems that the maintenance of the vegan diet is perceived as difficult, and that participants thus

have ambivalent feelings towards vegan foods. Previous findings highlight that a lack of

knowledge and skills of vegetarian foods acts as a barrier for plant-based diets (Lea and Worsley,

2001; Pasi Pohjolainen et al., 2015). The results indicate that a lack of perceived behavioural con-

trol is also a challenge for participants already engaging in a vegan diet.

Furthermore, especially affective feelings for food seemed to protrude ambivalent feelings. This

finding is in line with Berndsen et al. (2004), who found that hedonic appreciation for food is a

main contributor to ambivalence, even though they measured it with meat and not with cheese.

2 A moderation effect refers to a variable altering the strength of the causal relationship.

Conclusion 49

6 Conclusion

The fact that consumers’ dietary behaviour can have a strong impact with regard to sustainability

has already been confirmed by previous research. But while adopting a vegan diet would be a

rational behaviour with respect to sustainability, research also found that its maintenance is often

not easy and thus discontinued.

This study showed several of the challenges which German vegans face while trying to maintain

their dietary behaviour, the biggest one being the hedonic appreciation for certain animal-based

foods. Interestingly, the product most dearly missed was not meat but cheese. And the attachment

to cheese is not a trivial matter: the study found that it elicits ambivalent feelings towards vegan-

ism. One reason helping to mitigate cheese attachment would be a broader supply of vegan sub-

stitute products: the results suggest that a bigger and better choice of vegan cheese substitutes

would be highly appreciated by vegans.

While cheese had a strongest impact on the decision to maintain a vegan diet, meat still plays a

role. The study found that some participants still hold beliefs that eating meat is either natural,

normal, necessary or nice, which also negatively influences the intention to maintain a vegan diet.

Here, education campaigns could foster general knowledge on the nutritive value of plant-based

diets and address common myths regarding meat production and consumption.

With respect to the applied theoretical framework, the results confirmed the importance of indi-

viduals perceived behavioural control when maintaining a certain behaviour. Vegan consumers’

(perceived) ability to overcome their acquired eating habits in a non-vegan society, the general

availability of desired substitute products at reasonable prices, as well as a general knowledge on

how to prepare plant-based dishes – they all strongly influence the intention to maintain a vegan

diet.

In line with most applications of the Theory of Planned Behaviour, this study found that a per-

ceived behavioural control is of crucial importance for the intention to maintain the vegan diet.

There are several ways how the PBC could be improved – starting with lessons about the prepa-

ration of foods in schools to a wider availability of vegan meals when eating out.

Finally, the study found that family and friends are mostly non-supportive of the change of diet –

which might not be surprising in a society where the majority of the population is consuming

animal-based products. The results indicate however that this perceived social pressure has only a

weak influence on the participants' intention on whether to maintain a vegan diet or not. While this

Conclusion 50

finding is in line with other applications of the TPB in western cultures, it would be interesting to

see if it holds true for research in more collective societies.

6.1 Limitations and Strength

The sampling technique applied in this study, using self-selection and snowballing approaches

within different vegan Facebook groups, resulted in a large sample size of a key demographic for

this study: German vegans that engage in an online community of practise. Naturally, this ex-

cludes German vegans who do not participate in said community and thus leaves room for stud-

ies with more permissive time constraints to research these harder to reach parts of the already

hard-to-reach vegan population.

In addition, this study defined membership of the vegan population based on dietary choice only.

While this classification is sensible for the research question at hand, it is not the only valid defi-

nition of a vegan lifestyle. The challenges of individuals who not only abstain from animal-based

foods, but from all animal-based products, are most likely different. If they are more difficult to

overcome, or if individuals who adopted their behaviour accordingly are actually less ambivalent

with respect to their decision could be the focus of another study.

Finally, with the application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB), this study was able to

build on a well-tested and extensible theoretical framework that had already been used in similar

studies. Drawing on the vast experience from previous work and its statistical findings helped to

mitigate the fact that the present study's design was cross-sectional only, for a topic where a lon-

gitudinal study would be better suited. While this design, owed to the practical constraints of a

master’s thesis, yielded valuable insights on the challenges of maintaining a vegan diet in Ger-

many, it would strongly benefit from a longitudinal counterpart in the future.

References LI

7 References

Aiking, H., Boer, J. de, Vereijken, J., 2006. Sustainable protein production and consumption:

pigs or peas?, Environment & policy. Springer, Dordrecht.

Ajzen, I., 2011. The theory of planned behaviour: Reactions and reflections. Psychol. Health 26,

1113. doi:10.1080/08870446.2011.613995

Ajzen, I., 2006. Constructing a TpB Questionnaire: Conceptual and Methodological Considera-

tions [WWW Document]. URL https://people.umass.edu/aizen/pdf/tpb.measurement.pdf

Ajzen, I., 2002. Perceived Behavioral Control, Self‐Efficacy, Locus of Control, and the Theory

of Planned Behavior. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 32, 665–683. doi:10.1111/j.1559-

1816.2002.tb00236.x

Ajzen, I., 2001. Nature and operation of attitudes. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 52, 27.

Ajzen, I., 1991. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 50, 179–

211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T

Ajzen, I., Fishbein, M., 1980. Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Prentice-

Hall, Englewood Cliffs, N.J.

Ajzen, I., Fishbein, M., 1977. Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of

empirical research. Psychol. Bull. 84, 888–918. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888

Armitage, C.J., Conner, M., 2001. Efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behaviour: a meta-analytic

review. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. Br. Psychol. Soc. 40, 471.

Armitage, C.J., Conner, M., 1999. Predictive validity of the theory of planned behaviour: the

role of questionnaire format and social desirability. J. Community Appl. Soc. Psychol. 9,

261–272.

Asher, K., Green, C., Gutbrod, H., 2014. Study of Current and Former Vegetarians and Vegans

[WWW Document]. URL https://faunalytics.org/feature-article/study-of-current-and-for-

mer-vegetarians-and-vegans/ (accessed 4.8.16).

Augoustinos, M., Walker, I., Donaghue, N., 2006. Social Cognition: An Integrated Introduction.

Pine Forge Press.

Baltar, F., Brunet, I., 2012. Social research 2.0: virtual snowball sampling method using Face-

book. Internet Res. 22, 57–74. doi:10.1108/10662241211199960

Bamberg, S., Schmidt, P., 2003. Incentives, Morality, Or Habit? Predicting Students’ Car Use

for University Routes With the Models of Ajzen, Schwartz, and Triandis. Environ. Behav.

35, 264–285. doi:10.1177/0013916502250134

Bandura, A., 1982. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Am. Psychol. 37, 122–147.

doi:10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.122

References LII

Bandura, A., 1977. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev.

84, 191–215. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191

Baroni, L., Cenci, L., Tettamanti, M., Berati, M., 2007. Evaluating the environmental impact of

various dietary patterns combined with different food production systems. Eur. J. Clin.

Nutr. 61, 279–286. doi:10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602522

Beardsworth, A., Keil, T., 1991. Health-related beliefs and dietary practices among vegetarians

and vegans: a qualitative study. Health Educ. J. 50, 38–42.

doi:10.1177/001789699105000111

Berndsen, M., Pligt, J. van der, 2004. Ambivalence towards meat. Appetite 42, 71–78.

doi:10.1016/S0195-6663(03)00119-3

Berners-Lee, M., Hoolohan, C., Cammack, H., Hewitt, C.., 2012. The relative greenhouse gas

impacts of realistic dietary choices. Energy Policy 43, 184–190. doi:10.1016/j.en-

pol.2011.12.054

Beverland, M.B., 2014. Sustainable Eating: Mainstreaming Plant-Based Diets In Developed

Economies. J. Macromarketing 34, 369–382.

Breckler, S., 1994. A Comparison of Numerical Indexes for Measuring Attitude Ambivalence.

Educ. Psychol. Meas. 54, 350–365. doi:10.1177/0013164494054002009

Casler, K., Bickel, L., Hackett, E., 2013. Separate but equal? A comparison of participants and

data gathered via Amazon’s MTurk, social media, and face-to-face behavioral testing.

Comput. Hum. Behav. 29, 2156. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2013.05.009

Chemnitz, C., Heinrich-Böll-Stiftung, Schmidt-Landenberger, E., Europe, F. of the E., Cornely,

B., Becheva, S., 2016. Fleischatlas 2016. Heinrich-Böll-Stiftung.

Cialdini, R.B., Reno, R.R., Kallgren, C.A., 1990. A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: Recy-

cling the Concept of Norms to Reduce Littering in Public Places. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.

58, 1015. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015

Cohen, J., 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2. ed. ed. L. Erlbaum As-

sociates, Hillsdale.

Conner, M., Armitage, C.J., 1998. Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior: A Review and Av-

enues for Further Research. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 1429–1464. doi:10.1111/j.1559-

1816.1998.tb01685.x

Cook, A.J., Kerr, G.N., Moore, K., 2002. Attitudes and intentions towards purchasing GM food.

J. Econ. Psychol. 23, 557–572. doi:10.1016/S0167-4870(02)00117-4

Cordts, A., Nitzko, S., Spiller, A., 2014. Consumer Response to Negative Information on Meat

Consumption in Germany. Int. Food Agribus. Manag. Rev. 17.

Cron, J., Pobocik, R., 2013. Intentions to Continue Vegetarian Dietary Patterns: An Application

of the Theory of Planned Behavior. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 113, A90.

doi:10.1016/j.jand.2013.06.317

References LIII

Davidov, E., Depner, F., 2011. Testing for measurement equivalence of human values across

online and paper-and-pencil surveys. Qual. Quant. 45, 375–390. doi:10.1007/s11135-

009-9297-9

de Boer, J., Schösler, H., Aiking, H., 2014. Meatless days“ or ”less but better"? Exploring strat-

egies to adapt Western meat consumption to health and sustainability challenges. Appe-

tite 76, 120–128. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2014.02.002

Dishman, R.K., 1994. Advances in Exercise Adherence. Human Kinetics Publishers, Champaign,

IL.

Dunn, K.I., Mohr, P., Wilson, C.J., Wittert, G.A., 2011. Determinants of fast-food consumption.

An application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Appetite 57, 349–357.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.06.004

Eagly, A.H., Chaiken, S., 1993. The psychology of attitudes. Harcourt, Fort Worth, Tex.

Elzen, M., Vuuren, van, D.., Kabat, P., Stehfest, E., Bouwman, A.F., 2009. Climate benefits of

changing diet. Clim. Change 95, 83–102. doi:10.1007/s10584-008-9534-6

FAO [WWW Document], 2008. . Glob. Farm Anim. Prod. Glob. Warm. Impacting Mitigating

Clim. Change. URL http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2367646/ (accessed

24.2.16).

Field, A.P., 2013. Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics: and sex and drugs and rock

“n” roll, 4th edition. ed. Sage, Los Angeles.

Fishbein, M., Ajzen, I., 1975. Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: an introduction to theory

and research, Addison-Wesley series in social psychology. Addison-Wesley, Reading,

Mass.

Foley, J.A., 2011. Can We Feed the World & Sustain the Planet? Sci. Am. 305, 60–65.

Fox, N., Ward, K., 2008. Health, ethics and environment: A qualitative study of vegetarian moti-

vations. Appetite 50, 422–429. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2007.09.007

Gearhardt, A.N., Grilo, C.M., DiLeone, R.J., Brownell, K.D., Potenza, M.N., 2011. Can food be

addictive? Public health and policy implications. Addiction 106, 1208–1212.

doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03301.x

Geeraert, F., 2013. Sustainability and dietary change: the implications of Swedish food consump-

tion patterns 1960–2006. Int. J. Consum. Stud. 37, 121–129. doi:10.1111/j.1470-

6431.2012.01100.x

Godin, G., Kok, G., 1996. The Theory of Planned Behavior: A Review of Its Applications to

Health-related Behaviors. Am. J. Health Promot. 11, 87–98. doi:10.4278/0890-1171-

11.2.87

References LIV

Gosling, S.D., Vazire, S., Srivastava, S., John, O.P., 2004. Should We Trust Web-Based Stud-

ies?: A Comparative Analysis of Six Preconceptions About Internet Questionnaires. Am.

Psychol. 59, 93–104. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.59.2.93

Graça, J., Calheiros, M.M., Oliveira, A., 2015a. Attached to meat? (Un)Willingness and inten-

tions to adopt a more plant-based diet. Appetite 95, 113–125. doi:10.1016/j.ap-

pet.2015.06.024

Graça, J., Oliveira, A., Calheiros, M.M., 2015b. Meat, beyond the plate. Data-driven hypotheses

for understanding consumer willingness to adopt a more plant-based diet. Appetite 90,

80–90. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2015.02.037

Greenebaum, J., 2012. Veganism, Identitx and the Quest for Authenticity. Food Cult. Soc. Int. J.

Multidiscip. Res. 15, 129–144. doi:10.2752/175174412X13190510222101

Groves, R.M., 2009. Survey methodology, 2 ed. ed, Wiley series in survey methodology. Wiley,

Hoboken, N.J.

Hallström, E., Röös, E., Börjesson, P., Lunds tekniska högskola, L., Department of Technology

and Society, Environmental and Energy Systems Studies, Lunds universitet, Institutionen

för teknik och samhälle, Miljö- och energisystem, Institutioner vid LTH, Lund Univer-

sity, Departments at LTH, Faculty of Engineering, L. at L.U., 2014. Sustainable meat

consumption: A quantitative analysis of nutritional intake, greenhouse gas emissions and

land use from a Swedish perspective. Food Policy 47, 81–90. doi:10.1016/j.food-

pol.2014.04.002

HappyCow, 2015. Top Vegan-Friendly Cities in the World [WWW Document]. URL

http://www.happycow.net/travel/top_five.html (accessed 29.12.15).

Haverstock, K., Forgays, D.K., 2012. To eat or not to eat. A comparison of current and former

animal product limiters. Appetite 58, 1030–1036. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2012.02.048

Hechter, M., Opp, K.-D., 2001. Social Norms. Russell Sage Foundation.

Hedenus, F., Wirsenius, S., Johansson, D., Chalmers University of Technology, Chalmers

tekniska högskola, Department of Energy and Environment, Physical Resource Theory,

Institutionen för energi och miljö, Fysisk resursteori, 2014. The importance of reduced

meat and dairy consumption for meeting stringent climate change targets. Clim. Change

124, 79–91. doi:10.1007/s10584-014-1104-5

Hoek, A.C., Luning, P.A., Weijzen, P., Engels, W., Kok, F.J., de Graaf, C., 2011. Replacement

of meat by meat substitutes. A survey on person- and product-related factors in consumer

acceptance. Appetite 56, 662–673. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.001

Horwath, C.C., 1999. Applying the transtheoretical model to eating behaviour change: chal-

lenges and opportunities. Nutr. Res. Rev. 12, 281–317.

doi:10.1079/095442299108728965

References LV

Hung‐Yi Lu, Hsin‐Ya Hou, Tzong‐Horng Dzwo, Yi‐Chen Wu, James E. Andrews, Shao‐Ting

Weng, Mei‐Chun Lin, Jun‐Ying Lu, 2010. Factors influencing intentions to take precau-

tions to avoid consuming food containing dairy products: Expanding the theory of

planned behaviour. Br. Food J. 112, 919–933. doi:10.1108/00070701011074318

Jamieson, S., 2004. Likert scales: how to (ab)use them. Med. Educ. 38, 1217–1218.

doi:10.1111/j.1365-2929.2004.02012.x

Kahiluoto, H., Kuisma, M., Kuokkanen, A., Mikkilä, M., Linnanen, L., 2014. Taking planetary

nutrient boundaries seriously: Can we feed the people? Glob. Food Secur. 3, 16–21.

doi:10.1016/j.gfs.2013.11.002

Kline, P., 2000. The Handbook of Psychological Testing. Psychology Press.

Kollmuss, A., Agyeman, J., 2002. Mind the Gap: Why do people act environmentally and what

are the barriers to pro-environmental behavior? Environ. Educ. Res. 8, 239–260.

doi:10.1080/13504620220145401

Koneswaran, G., Nierenberg, D., 2008. Global Farm Animal Production and Global Warming:

Impacting and Mitigating Climate Change. Environ. Health Perspect. 116, 578–582.

doi:10.1289/ehp.11034

Köster, E.P., 2009. Diversity in the determinants of food choice: A psychological perspective.

Food Qual. Prefer., European Conference on Sensory Science of Food and Beverages

2006European Conference on Sensory Science of Food and Beverages 2006 20, 70–82.

doi:10.1016/j.foodqual.2007.11.002

Lea, E., Worsley, A., 2003. Benefits and barriers to the consumption of a vegetarian diet in Aus-

tralia. Public Health Nutr. 6, 505–511. doi:10.1079/PHN2002452

Lea, E., Worsley, A., 2001. Influences on meat consumption in Australia. Appetite 36, 127–136.

doi:10.1006/appe.2000.0386

Lea, E.J., Crawford, D., Worsley, A., 2006. Public views of the benefits and barriers to the con-

sumption of a plant-based diet. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 60, 828–837.

doi:10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602387

Lewin, K., 1997. Resolving social conflicts: Field theory in social science. American Psycholog-

ical Association, Washington, DC.

Lumley, T., Diehr, P., Emerson, S., Chen, L., 2002. The Importance of the Normality Assumption

in Large Public Health Data Sets. Annu. Rev. Public Health 23, 151–169.

Macdiarmid, J.I., Douglas, F., Campbell, J., 2016. Eating like there’s no tomorrow: Public

awareness of the environmental impact of food and reluctance to eat less meat as part of

a sustainable diet. Appetite 96, 487–493. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2015.10.011

Marpsat, M., Razafindratsima, N., 2010. Survey Methods for Hard-to-Reach Populations: Intro-

duction to the Special Issue. Methodol. Innov. Online 5, 3–16.

doi:10.4256/mio.2010.0014

References LVI

Meier, T., Christen, O., 2013. Environmental impacts of dietary recommendations and dietary

styles: Germany as an example. Environ. Sci. Technol. 47, 877.

Meier, T., Christen, O., Semler, E., Jahreis, G., Voget-Kleschin, L., Schrode, A., Artmann, M.,

2014. Balancing virtual land imports by a shift in the diet. Using a land balance ap-

proach to assess the sustainability of food consumption. Germany as an example. Appe-

tite 74, 20–34. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2013.11.006

Mendes, E., 2013. An Application of the Transtheoretical Model to Becoming Vegan. Soc. Work

Public Health 28, 142–149. doi:10.1080/19371918.2011.561119

Michail, N., 2015. Meat-free alternatives - brought to you by the meat companies [WWW Docu-

ment]. URL http://www.foodnavigator.com/Market-Trends/Meat-free-alternatives-

brought-to-you-by-the-meat-companies (accessed 3.1.16).

Mick, D.G., 2006. Meaning and Mattering Through Transformative Consumer Research. Adv.

Consum. Res. 33, 1–4.

Mooney, K.., Walbourn, L., 2001. When college students reject food: not just a matter of taste.

Appetite 36, 41–50. doi:10.1006/appe.2000.0384

Nunnally, J.C., Bernstein, I.H., 1994. Psychometric Theory, 3rd edition. ed. McGraw-Hill, New

York.

O’Riordan, T., Stoll-Kleemann, S., 2015. The Challenges of Changing Dietary Behavior Toward

More Sustainable Consumption. Environ. Sci. Policy Sustain. Dev. 57, 4.

doi:10.1080/00139157.2015.1069093

Paisley, J., Beanlands, H., Goldman, J., Evers, S., Chappell, J., 2008. Dietary Change: What Are

the Responses and Roles of Significant Others? J. Nutr. Educ. Behav. 40, 80–88.

doi:10.1016/j.jneb.2007.04.374

Pallant, J., 2011. SPSS survival manual: a step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS. Allen

& Unwin, Crows Nest, N.S.W.

Pasi Pohjolainen, Markus Vinnari, Pekka Jokinen, 2015. Consumers’ perceived barriers to fol-

lowing a plant-based diet. Br. Food J. 117, 1150–1167. doi:10.1108/BFJ-09-2013-0252

Piazza, J., Ruby, M.B., Loughnan, S., Luong, M., Kulik, J., Watkins, H.M., Seigerman, M.,

2015. Rationalizing meat consumption. The 4Ns. Appetite 91, 114–128. doi:10.1016/j.ap-

pet.2015.04.011

Pollan, M., 2011. The Omnivore’s Dilemma: The Search for a Perfect Meal in a Fast-Food

World. Bloomsbury Paperbacks, New York.

Porter, S.R., Whitcomb, M.E., 2005. Non-Response in Student Surveys: The Role of De-

mographics, Engagement and Personality. Res. High. Educ. 46, 127–152.

doi:10.1007/s11162-004-1597-2

References LVII

Povey, R., Wellens, B., Conner, M., 2001. Attitudes towards following meat, vegetarian and ve-

gan diets: an examination of the role of ambivalence. Appetite 37, 15–26.

doi:10.1006/appe.2001.0406

Prochaska, J.O., Velicer, W.F., 1997. The Transtheoretical Model of Health Behavior Change.

Am. J. Health Promot. 12, 38–48. doi:10.4278/0890-1171-12.1.38

Radnitz, C., Beezhold, B., DiMatteo, J., 2015. Investigation of lifestyle choices of individuals fol-

lowing a vegan diet for health and ethical reasons. Appetite 90, 31–36. doi:10.1016/j.ap-

pet.2015.02.026

Richetin, J., Conner, M., Perugini, M., 2011. Not Doing Is Not the Opposite of Doing: Implica-

tions for Attitudinal Models of Behavioral Prediction. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 40–

54. doi:10.1177/0146167210390522

Rothgerber, H., 2014. Efforts to overcome vegetarian-induced dissonance among meat eaters.

Appetite 79, 32–41. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2014.04.003

Ruby, M.B., 2012. Vegetarianism. A blossoming field of study. Appetite 58, 141–150.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.09.019

Saba, A., Di Natale, R., 1998. A study on the mediating role of intention in the impact of habit

and attitude on meat consumption. Food Qual. Prefer. 10, 69–77. doi:10.1016/S0950-

3293(98)00039-1

Sabaté, J., 2003. The contribution of vegetarian diets to health and disease: a paradigm shift?

Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 78, 502S.

Saunders, M., 1959, Lewis, P., 1945, Thornhill, A., 2016. Research methods for business stu-

dents. Pearson Education, New York.

Saunders, M., Lewis, P., Thornhill, A., 2012. Research methods for business students. Pearson,

Harlow.

Sax, L.J., Gilmartin, S.K., Bryant, A.N., 2003. Assessing Response Rates and Nonresponse Bias

in Web and Paper Surveys. Res. High. Educ. 44, 409–432.

doi:10.1023/A:1024232915870

Scheibehenne, B., Miesler, L., Todd, P.., 2008. Fast and frugal food choices: Uncovering indi-

vidual decision heuristics. Appetite 50, 565. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2007.09.054

Schösler, H., de Boer, J., Boersema, J.J., 2012. Can we cut out the meat of the dish? Construct-

ing consumer-oriented pathways towards meat substitution. Appetite 58, 39–47.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.09.009

Schulte, E.M., Avena, N.M., Gearhardt, A.N., 2015. Which Foods May Be Addictive? The Roles

of Processing, Fat Content, and Glycemic Load. PLOS ONE 10, e0117959.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0117959

Sheeran, P., 2002. Intention-Behavior Relations: A Conceptual and Empirical Review. Eur. Rev.

Soc. Psychol. 12, 1–36. doi:10.1080/14792772143000003

References LVIII

Shumaker, S.A., Ockene, J.K., Riekert, K.A., 2009. The handbook of health behavior change,

3rd ed. ed. Springer Pub. Co, New York.

Sobh, R., Perry, C., 2006. Research design and data analysis in realism research. Eur. J. Mark.

40, 1194–1209. doi:10.1108/03090560610702777

Sparks, P., Conner, M., James, R., Shepherd, R., Povey, R., 2001. Ambivalence about health-re-

lated behaviours: an exploration in the domain of food choice. Br. J. Health Psychol. 6,

53–68. doi:10.1348/135910701169052

Sparks, P., Harris, P.R., Lockwood, N., 2004. Predictors and predictive effects of ambivalence.

Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 43, 371–83.

Sparks, P., Shepherd, R., 1992. Self-identity and the theory of planned behavior: assessing the

role of identification with “Green Consumerism.” Soc. Psychol. Q. 55, 388.

Steinfeld, H., Gerber, P., Wassenaar, T.D., Castel, V., Rosales M., M., Haan, C. de, 2006. Live-

stock’s long shadow: environmental issues and options. Food and Agriculture Organiza-

tion of the United Nations, Rome.

Steptoe, A., Perkins-Porras, L., Rink, E., Hilton, S., Cappuccio, F.P., 2004. Psychological and

Social Predictors of Changes in Fruit and Vegetable Consumption Over 12 Months Fol-

lowing Behavioral and Nutrition Education Counseling. Health Psychol. 23, 574–581.

doi:10.1037/0278-6133.23.6.574

Sutton, S., 1998. Predicting and Explaining Intentions and Behavior: How Well Are We Doing?

J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 28, 1317–1338. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1998.tb01679.x

Thomson, R., Ito, N., 2014. Facebook Advertisments for Survey Participant Recruitment: Con-

siderations from a Multi-National Study. Int. J. Electron. Commer. Stud. 5, 199–218.

doi:10.7903/ijecs.1175

Tippelt, F., Kupferschmitt, T., 2015. Ergebnisse der ARD/ZDF-Onlinestudie 2015 [WWW Doc-

ument]. URL http://www.ard-zdf-onlinestudie.de/fileadmin/Onlinestudie_2015/10-

15_Tippelt_Kupferschmitt.pdf (accessed 3.2.16).

Tobler, C., Visschers, V.H.M., Siegrist, M., 2011. Eating green. Consumers’ willingness to

adopt ecological food consumption behaviors. Appetite 57, 674–682. doi:10.1016/j.ap-

pet.2011.08.010

Triandis, H.C., 1979. Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. Nebr. Symp. Motiv. 27,

195–259.

Twine, R., 2014. Vegan Killjoys at the Table-Contesting Happiness and Negotiating Relation-

ships with Food Practices. Societies 4, 623–639.

doi:http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.its.uu.se/10.3390/soc4040623

UNEP, 2010. Assessing the environmental impacts of consumption and production. Int. J. Sus-

tain. High. Educ. 11. doi:10.1108/ijshe.2010.24911daf.001

References LIX

Valle, P., Rebelo, E., Reis, E., Menezes, J., 2005. Combining Behavioral Theories to Predict Re-

cycling Involvement. Environ. Behav. 37, 364–396. doi:10.1177/0013916504272563

Valois, P., Desharnais, R., Godin, G., 1988. A comparison of the Fishbein and Ajzen and the Tri-

andis attitudinal models for the prediction of exercise intention and behavior. J. Behav.

Med. 11, 459–472. doi:10.1007/BF00844839

van Dooren, C., Marinussen, M., Blonk, H., Aiking, H., Vellinga, P., 2014. Exploring dietary

guidelines based on ecological and nutritional values: A comparison of six dietary pat-

terns. Food Policy 44, 36–46. doi:10.1016/j.foodpol.2013.11.002

VEBU, 2015. Amount of Vegans and Vegetarians in Germany [WWW Document]. VEBU –

Veg. Dtschl. EV. URL https://vebu.de/veggie-fakten/entwicklung-in-zahlen/anzahl-ve-

ganer-und-vegetarier-in-deutschland/ (accessed 9.5.16).

Wegner, D.M., Wheatley, T., 1999. Apparent Mental Causation: Sources of the Experience of

Will. Am. Psychol. 54, 480–492. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.480

Wiese, J., 2016. Official Facebook statistics for Germany (Februar 2016) [WWW Document].

allfacebook.de. URL http://allfacebook.de/zahlen_fakten/erstmals-ganz-offiziell-face-

book-nutzerzahlen-fuer-deutschland (accessed 12.4.16).

Wilson, R.E., Gosling, S.D., Graham, L.T., 2012. A Review of Facebook Research in the Social

Sciences. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 7, 203–220.

Wolff, K., Nordin, K., Brun, W., Berglund, G., Kvale, G., 2011. Affective and cognitive atti-

tudes, uncertainty avoidance and intention to obtain genetic testing: An extension of the

Theory of Planned Behaviour. Psychol. Health 26, 1143–1155.

doi:10.1080/08870441003763253

Wray, A., Bloomer, A., 2006. Projects in linguistics: a practical guide to researching language,

2nd ed. ed. Hodder Arnold ; Distributed in the United States of America by Oxford Uni-

versity Press, London : New York.

Zolait, A.H.S., 2014. The nature and components of perceived behavioural control as an element

of theory of planned behaviour. Behav. Inf. Technol. 33, 65–85.

doi:10.1080/0144929X.2011.630419

Zur, I., Klöckner, C.A., 2014. Individual motivations for limiting meat consumption. Br. Food J.

116, 629–642. doi:10.1108/BFJ-08-2012-0193

Appendix LX

8 Appendix

1. Demographic Statistics

Table 9 - Gender

Gender of Participants

Frequency Valid Percent

Female 2269 80.3

Male 558 19.7

n = 2847

Figure 3 - Gender

Appendix LXI

Age

Table 10 - Age

Participants´ Age in Groups

Frequency Valid Percent

13 - 17 61 2.2

18 - 25 912 32.3

26 - 35 1005 35.6

36 - 45 469 16.6

46 - 55 302 10.7

56 and above 77 2.7

n = 2847

Figure 4 - Age

Age in Groups

13 - 17 18 - 25 26 - 35 36 - 45 46 - 55 56 and above

Appendix LXII

Dietary style before turning vegan

Table 11 - Former Dietary Style

Dietary style before turning vegan

Frequency Valid Percent

omnivorous (with meat and fish) 998 35.1

pescetarian

(with fish but without meat) 215 7.6

vegetarian (no fish and no meat, but

other animal-based products) 1633 57.4

n = 2846

Figure 5 - Former Dietary Style

Appendix LXIII

Education level

Table 12 - Education level

Education

Frequency Valid Percent

Lower Secondary School 33 1.2

Middle School 214 7.6

University Entrance Diploma 913 32.3

Vocational Education 483 17.1

University degree 1088 38.5

Promotion 43 1.5

No certificate 9 0.3

Other 43 1.5

n = 2847

Figure 6 - Education

Appendix LXIV

Net Income per Month

Table 13 - Net Income

Net Income per Month

EUR Frequency Valid Percent

under 1000 1002 35.4

1000 - 1500 453 16.0

1500 - 2500 603 21.3

2500 - 3500 227 8.0

more than 3500 117 4.1

Not stated 412 14.6

n = 2827

Figure 7 - Net Income

Appendix LXV

Residential Area

Table 14 - Residential Area

Residential Area

Frequency Valid Percent

Rural community (until 5.000 inhabitants) 402 14.2

Small town (until 10.000 inhabitants) 280 9.9

Town (until 100.000 inhabitants) 762 27.0

City (until 1 Mio. inhabitants) 965 34.1

Metropolis (over 1 Mio. inhabitants) 418 14.8

n = 2847

Figure 8 - Residential Area

Appendix LXVI

Children in Household

Table 15 - Children in Household

Children

Children Frequency Valid Percent

None 2232 79.0

1 299 10.6

2 212 7.5

3 and more 83 2.9

n = 2847

Figure 9 - Children in Household

Appendix LXVII

2. Descriptive Statistics – Variables

Table 16 - Reasons Participants Turned Vegan

Reasons Participants Turned Vegan

Min. Max. Mean SD

Animal Welfare 1 5 4.61 .845

Health 1 5 3.40 1.39

Environment 1 5 3.95 1.15

Social environment 1 5 1.62 .999

n = 2847

Table 17 - Reliability after Item-Correction

Cronbach´s Alpha (after correction)

Scale Min. Max. Mean SD

Internal

Consistency

(α)

N of

Items

Corrected Inter-

Item Correlation

(from-to)

Attitude 1 5 4.80 0.35 .679 4 .415 - .536

Subjective Norm 1 5 1.56 0.62 .715 2 .577

PBC 1 5 4.49 0.59 .758 7 .392 - .602

Attitude 4N 1 5 1.53 0.55 .472 4 .242 - .374

Meat Attachment 1 5 1.15 0.45 .732 3 .492 - .629

Cheese Attachment 1 5 1.97 1.03 .868 4 .684 - .792

n = 2847

Appendix LXVIII

Table 18 - Means for Variables

Means for all Variables

Scale Min. Max. Mean SD

Attitude 2 5 4.80 0.35

Subjective Norm 0,5 5 1.57 0.62

PBC 1,57 5 4.49 0.59

Attitude 4N 1 5 1.59 0.55

Meat Attachment 1 5 1.15 0.45

Cheese Attachment 1 5 1.97 1.03

Ambivalence 1 5 1.51 0.94

n = 2847

Appendix LXIX

3. Example of Survey Post in a Vegan Facebook Group

Figure 10 - Facebook Post Example

Appendix LXX

4. Complete Survey in English3

Table 19 – Complete Survey in English 0. Start page

Dear Sirs and Madams, thank you for taking the time to participate in this survey. Your participation is very valuable, and you are helping me collecting insights about vegan food consumption in Germany, for my Master´s thesis in Sustainable Management at Uppsala University. For the participation, it does not matter if you are eating vegan for a short or a long term.

The survey will take approx. 9 minutes. There are no wrong answers. The survey is completely anony-mous and the data will only be used for scientific purposes.

As a little ‘thank you’ on the 2nd of May vouchers for companies will be raffled among all participants

You can win vouchers from Amazon, H&M or a vegan online shop of your choice. 1x 20 EUR voucher 2x 10 EUR vouchers The winners can choose which company-voucher they want. As an alternative, the won amount can also be do-nated to a non-profit organisation. To get to the survey, please click ‘next’ in the very right corner. Do you have questions or comments? My contact email: [email protected] Thank you very much and with best regards Yasmin Emre

Question and data entry instruction Answers Scale

1a. Are you currently eating vegan?

(filter question)

o yes o no

Binary

1b. Non-vegan = End page

2. Since when do you eat vegan?

o less than 4 weeks o 1-3 month o 4-6 month o 7-12 month o 1-2 years o 3-5 years o over 5 years o over 10 years

Nominal

3. How influential were the following reasons for your decision to eat vegan?

o Animal Welfare o Health o Environment o Socio-environment

Ordinal 1: not at all influ-ential 2: rather not influ-ential 3: partly 4: rather influen-tial 5: very influential

4. The influential reason for you to eat vegan is not included?

Please state it here. free text

3 The red words were not included in the original survey. They illustrate which construct is behind the question.

Appendix LXXI

5. Vegan consumption is...

Please state your attitude towards vegan-ism. ATTITUDE

o good-bad o harmful-beneficial o unpleasant-pleasant o unenjoyable-enjoyable

Semantic differ-ential

6. Please state how much you agree with the following statements.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it. ATTITUDES 4N

o Human beings are natural meat-eat-ers

o A healthy diet requires at least some meat

o It is abnormal for humans not to eat meat

o Meat is delicious

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree 3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

7. Please state how much you agree with the following statements.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it. SUBJECTIVE NORM

o My family thinks I should eat a vegan diet

o My friends think I should eat a vegan diet

o Regarding my diet, I comply with what my family thinks

o Regarding my diet, I comply with what my friends thinks.

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree 3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

8. Strongest missed food item

As a vegan I find giving up these 3 foods the hardest: Please rank the three animal-based foods that you miss most, starting with your num-ber one. Example: you miss eggs the most, then fish and then milk. Eggs = 1 Fish = 2 3 = Milk The numbers are awarded by a click. With a second click, you unmark a food-item. MEAT & CHEESE ATTACHMENT

o Eggy o Fish & sea food o Sausages o Steak o Cheese o Cold cuts (i.e. salami, ham) o Milk o Cream o Milk products (i.e. yoghurt, butter

milk) o Butter o Bacon o None

Nominal 1 – 10

9. I am part of a vegan group (on Face-book, in forums or locally)

(filter question) SOCIAL SUPPORT

o yes o no

Binary

10. Please state how much you agree with the following statement.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it. SOCIAL SUPPORT

o Being part of a vegan group provides me with support and help.

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree 3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

11. Please state how much you agree with the following statements.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it. PERCEIVED BEHAVIOURAL CONTROL

o It depends mostly on myself if I main-tain my diet.

o I am confident I will maintain the ve-gan diet in the following six months.

o I find eating vegan easy.

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree 3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

Appendix LXXII

12. Please state how intention to main-tain the diet within the next six months.

The higher your intention, the higher the number should be. INTENTION

o I intend to eat a vegan diet in the next six months

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 9: strongly agree

13. Please state how much you agree with the following statement.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it. AMBIVALENCE

o I feel torn between vegan and non-ve-gan foods

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree 3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

14. Bevor I ate vegan I ate….

(filter question)

o omnivorous (with meat and fish) o pescetarian (without meat but with

fish) o vegetarian (neither fish nor meat, but

other animal-based products)

Nominal

15. How often did you eat meat before you became a vegan?

HABITS MEAT

o less than once per week o once or twice per week o three or four times per week o five times or more per week

Nominal

16. Please state how much you agree with the following statements.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it. MEAT ATTACHMENT

o In the long term, I find not eating meat challenging.

o Since I´m not eating meat anymore I feel weak.

o I don´t mind not eating meat.

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree 3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

17. How long were you a vegetarian be-fore you became vegan?

(filter question)

o less than 4 weeks o 1 - 3 month o 4 - 6 month o 7 - 12 month o 1 - 2 years o 3 - 5 years o 6 - 9 years o 10 (+) years

Nominal

18. How often did you eat cheese before you became a vegan?

(i.e. on top of bread or on a pizza?) HABITS CHEESE

o less than once per week o once or twice per week o three or four times per week

five times or more per week.

Nominal

19. Please state how much you agree with the following statements.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it.

CHEESE ATTACHMENT

o In the long term, I find not eating cheese challenging.

o The prospect of a life without cheese makes me a little sad

o I don´t mind not eating cheese.

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree 3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

20. Please state how much you agree with the following statements.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it.

o Killing animals for consumption is jus-tified.

o The meat industry is cruel.

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree

Appendix LXXIII

MORAL BELIEFS

3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

21. Which influence have the following factors on your intention to stay vegan in the next six months?

If a factor has no influence at all, please rate it with 1 star. The more stars you give, the higher you rate it. Example: because of health concerns you are unsure if you should continue eating vegan. = ‘Health concerns = 5 stars PBC

o Higher costs o Health concerns o Change of eating habits o Desiring meat o Desiring cheese o Missing acceptance in social environ-

ment o Low availability of vegan substitute

products in supermarkets o Low availability of vegan foods when

eating out o Low knowledge about vegan recipes

and cooking

Ordinal 1: not at all influ-ential 2: rather not influ-ential 3: partly 4: rather influen-tial 5: very influential

22. Please state how much you agree with the following statements.

The more stars you give, the higher you rate it. HEALTH BELIEFS

o Consuming high amounts of meat might cause serious health problems.

o The vegan diet is healthier than a diet with meat.

Ordinal 1: strongly disa-gree 2: rather disagree 3: partly agree 4: rather agree 5: strongly agree

23a. Environmental Awareness

Almost done! Only two more estimation questions about the environment and some general information, and the survey will be finished. Please estimate what most land globally is used for. ENVIRONMENTAL AWARENESS

o to grow food for humans o to grow food for animal agriculture o to grow plants for energy (i.e. ethanol)

Nominal

23b. Environmental Awareness

Please estimate what has a higher influ-ence on climate change. ENVIRONMENTAL AWARENESS

o cars o animal agriculture o industry o flying

Nominal

24. Socio-demographic information

In the end, some socio-demographic infor-mation for the statics. At the end of the page you can register for the raffle. Gender

o female o male

Binary

Age

o 13 - 65 and older Continuous

State

o Baden-Württemberg o Bayern o Berlin o Brandenburg o Bremen o Hamburg o Hessen o Mecklenburg-Vorpommern o Niedersachsen o Nordrhein-Westfalen o Rheinland-Pfalz o Saarland o Sachsen o Sachsen-Anhalt

Nominal

Appendix LXXIV

o Schleswig-Holstein o Thüringen

Residential area

o Rural community (till 5.000 inhabit-ants)

o Small town (till 10.000 inhabitants) o Town (till 100.000 inhabitants) o City (till 1 Mio. inhabitants) o Metropole (over 1 Mio. inhabitants)

Nominal

Household size

o 1 person o 2 persons o 3 persons o 4 persons o 5 persons and more

Nominal

Children o 0 children o 1 child o 2 children o 3 and more children

Nominal

Education o Lower school o Middle school o University Entrance Diploma o Vocational Education o University degree o Promotion o No educational certificate o Other:

Nominal & free text

Net Income per Month

o under 1000 EUR o 1000 EUR- 1500 EUR o 1500 EUR- 2500 EUR o 2500 EUR- 3500 EUR o more than 3500 EUR o I´d rather not say

Nominal

Do you have feedback regarding the sur-vey, or would you like to comment on something? If you are interested in the study results, please enter a short notice and your email address. Would you like to participate in the lottery?

o No o Yes, my email address is:

Binary & free Text

End page

Thank you very much for your participation! If you took part in the lottery, I will inform you via email on the 2nd of May in case you have won. Best regards & a sunny day Yasmin Emre

Appendix LXXV

5. Complete Survey in German4

Table 20 – Complete Survey in German

Frage & Beschreibungstext Antwortmöglichkeit Skala

1a. Vegan ja/nein

Ernähren Sie sich derzeit vegan?

o ja o nein

Binary

1b. Non-vegan = Endseite

2. Seit wann vegan?

Seit wann ernähren Sie sich vegan?

o weniger als 4 Wochen o 1 - 3 Monate o 4 - 6 Monate o 7 - 12 Monate o 1 - 2 Jahre o 3 - 5 Jahre o 6 - 9 Jahre o 10 (+) Jahre

Nominal

3. Wie ausschlaggebend waren die fol-genden Faktoren für Ihre Entscheidung, sich vegan zu ernähren?

Wenn ein Faktor gar nicht ausschlagge-bend war, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stär-ker ist Ihre Zustimmung. Weiter unten kön-nen Sie Gründe angeben, die nicht in der Liste enthalten sind.

o Tierschutz o Gesundheit o Umwelt o Soziales Umfeld

Ordinal 1: trifft gar nicht zu 2: trifft eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: trifft eher zu 5: trifft voll und ganz zu

4. Der ausschlaggebende Faktor für Ihre vegane Ernährung ist nicht dabei?

Hier können Sie ihn angeben.

[FREITEXT]

[free text]

5. Vegane Ernährung ist...

Bitte geben Sie Ihre Einstellung zu veganer Ernährung an. ATTITUDE

o schlecht - gut o schädlich - vorteilhaft o unangenehm - angenehm o ungenießbar - lecker

Semantic diffe-rential

6. Bitte geben Sie an, inwiefern Sie den folgenden Aussagen zu Fleisch zustim-men.

Wenn Sie einer Aussage gar nicht zustim-men, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stärker ist Ihre Zustimmung. ATTITUDES 4N

o Menschen sind von Natur aus Fleisch-esser.

o Für eine gesunde Ernährung ist etwas Fleisch notwendig.

o Es ist für Menschen unnormal kein Fleisch zu essen.

o Fleisch ist lecker.

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: stimme eher zu 5: stimme voll und ganz zu

7. Wie geht Ihr soziales Umfeld mit Ihrer veganen Ernährung um?

Bitte geben Sie an, inwieweit die folgenden Aussagen zutreffen. SUBJECTIVE NORM

o Meine Familie findet, ich sollte mich ve-gan ernähren.

o Meine Freunde finden, ich sollte mich vegan ernähren.

o Was meine Ernährung betrifft, richte ich mich nach den Vorstellungen mei-ner Familie.

o Was meine Ernährung betrifft, richte ich mich nach den Vorstellungen mei-ner Freunde.

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: stimme eher zu

4 The red words were not included in the original survey. They illustrate which construct is behind the question.

Appendix LXXVI

5: stimme voll und ganz zu

8. Am schwierigsten zu verzichten?

Als Veganer finde ich es am schwierigsten, auf diese 3 tierischen Lebensmittel zu ver-zichten: Bitte markieren Sie die 3 tierischen Le-bensmittel, die Ihnen am meisten fehlen. Vergeben Sie dabei die 1 für das Lebens-mittel, dessen Geschmack Sie am meisten vermissen, die 2 für das zweitliebste Le-bensmittel und die 3 für das drittliebste Le-bensmittel. Beispiel: Sie mochten am liebsten Eier, ge-folgt von Fisch und dann Milch: Eier = 1, Fisch = 2, Milch = 3. Die Zahlen werden durch einen einfachen Klick vergeben - durch einen zweiten Klick auf ein bereits markiertes Lebensmittel wird die Markierung wieder entfernt. MEAT OR CHEESE ATTACHMENT

o Eier o Fisch & Meeresfrüchte o Würstchen o Steak o Käse o Aufschnitt (z.B. Salami, o Schinken) o Milch o Sahne o Milchprodukte (z.B. o Joghurt, Buttermilch) o Butter o Speck o Keines der genannten

Nominal 1 – 10

9. Sind Sie Mitglied in einer veganen Gruppe (z.B. auf Facebook, in Internet-Foren oder lokal vor Ort)?

SOCIAL SUPPORT

o ja o nein

Binary

10. Bitte geben Sie an, inwieweit Sie der folgenden Aussage zustimmen.

Wenn Sie der Aussage gar nicht zustim-men, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stärker ist Ihre Zustimmung. SOCIAL SUPPORT

o Die Mitgliedschaft in der veganen Gruppe gibt mir Rückhalt und Hilfe.

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: stimme eher zu 5: stimme voll und ganz zu

11. Bitte geben Sie an, inwiefern Sie den folgenden Aussagen zustimmen.

Wenn Sie einer Aussage gar nicht zustim-men, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stärker ist Ihre Zustimmung. PERCEIVED BEHAVIOURAL CONTROL

o Es hängt zum großen Teil von mir selbst ab, ob ich mich vegan ernähre o-der nicht.

o Ich bin zuversichtlich, dass ich die ve-gane Ernährung in den nächsten 6 Mo-naten beibehalte.

o Mich vegan zu ernähren finde ich ein-fach.

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: stimme eher zu 5: stimme voll und ganz zu

12. Bitte geben Sie Ihre Intention an.

Mit einer höheren Zahl drücken Sie eine stärkere Absicht aus. INTENTION

o Ich habe die Absicht, mich in den nächsten sechs Monaten weiter vegan zu ernähren.

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 9: stimme voll und ganz zu

13. Bitte geben Sie an, inwiefern Sie der folgenden Aussage zustimmen.

o Ich fühle mich hin- und hergerissen zwischen veganem und nicht-veganem Essen.

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu

Appendix LXXVII

Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stär-ker ist Ihre Zustimmung. AMBIVALENCE

2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: stimme eher zu 5: stimme voll und ganz zu

14. Bitte geben Sie an, wie Sie sich er-nährten, bevor Sie vegan wurden.

o omnivor (mit Fleisch und mit Fisch) o pescetarisch (mit Fisch, aber ohne

Fleisch) o vegetarisch (weder mit Fisch noch mit

Fleisch, aber mit anderen Tierproduk-ten)

Nominal

15. Wie oft haben Sie vor der veganen Umstellung in einer durchschnittlichen Woche Fleisch gegessen?

HABITS MEAT

o weniger als 1x pro Woche o 1 - 2x pro Woche o 3 - 4x pro Woche o 5x pro Woche oder öfter

Nominal

16. Bitte geben Sie an, inwiefern Sie den folgenden Aussagen zu Fleisch zustim-men.

Wenn Sie einer Aussage gar nicht zustim-men, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stärker ist Ihre Zustimmung. MEAT ATTACHMENT

o Langfristig ohne Fleisch auszukommen ist eine Herausforderung für mich.

o Seitdem ich kein Fleisch mehr esse, fühle ich mich schwach.

o Auf Fleisch zu verzichten finde ich nicht schlimm.

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: stimme eher zu 5: stimme voll und ganz zu

17. Wie lang haben Sie sich vegetarisch ernährt?

o weniger als 4 Wochen o 1 - 3 Monate o 4 - 6 Monate o 7 - 12 Monate o 1 - 2 Jahre o 3 - 5 Jahre o 6 - 9 Jahre o 10 (+) Jahre

Nominal

18. Habits Cheese

Wie oft haben Sie vor der veganen Umstel-lung in einer durchschnittlichen Woche Käse gegessen? (z.B. überbacken oder auf Brot) HABITS CHEESE

o weniger als 1x pro Woche o 1 - 2x pro Woche o 3 - 4x pro Woche o 5x pro Woche oder öfter

Nominal

19. Bitte geben Sie an, inwiefern Sie den folgenden Aussagen zu Käse zustim-men.

Wenn Sie einer Aussage gar nicht zustim-men, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stärker ist Ihre Zustimmung. CHEESE ATTACHMENT

o Langfristig ohne Käse auszukommen ist eine Herausforderung für mich.

o Die Aussicht auf ein Leben ohne Käse finde ich etwas traurig.

o Auf Käse zu verzichten finde ich nicht schlimm.

o

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: stimme eher zu 5: stimme voll und ganz zu

20. Bitte geben Sie an, inwiefern Sie den folgenden Aussagen zustimmen.

Wenn Sie einer Aussage gar nicht zustim-men, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stärker ist Ihre Zustimmung.

o Tiere für den menschlichen Konsum zu schlachten ist gerechtfertigt.

o Die Fleischindustrie ist grausam.

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils

Appendix LXXVIII

MORAL BELIEFS

4: stimme eher zu 5: stimme voll und ganz zu

21. Inwieweit haben folgende Faktoren einen negativen Einfluss auf Ihre Ab-sicht, sich in den nächsten 6 Monaten weiterhin vegan zu ernähren?

Wenn ein Faktor überhaupt keinen Einfluss hat, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stärker ist der Einfluss. Beispiel: aufgrund von ge-sundheitlichen Bedenken sind Sie unsi-cher, ob Sie sich weiterhin vegan ernähren sollten. Gesundheitliche Bedenken = 5 Sterne. PBC

o Höhere Kosten o Gesundheitliche Bedenken/Defizite o Umstellung der bisherigen o Essgewohnheiten o Lust auf Fleisch o Lust auf Käse o Fehlende Akzeptanz im Umkreis o Schlechte Verfügbarkeit von pflanzli-

chen o Alternativprodukten o Geringe Auswahl beim Essen auswärts o Geringe Kenntnisse über Rezepte und o Zubereitung

Ordinal 1: gar keinen Ein-fluss 2: eher keinen Einfluss 3: teils-teils 4: eher einen Ein-fluss 5: sehr großen Einfluss

22. Bitte geben Sie an, inwiefern Sie den folgenden Aussagen zur Gesundheit zu-stimmen.

Wenn Sie einer Aussage gar nicht zustim-men, vergeben Sie bitte einen Stern. Je mehr Sterne Sie vergeben, desto stärker ist Ihre Zustimmung HEALTH BELIEFS

o Viel Fleisch zu konsumieren kann ernsthafte gesundheitliche Folgen ha-ben.

o Die vegane Ernährung ist gesünder als eine Ernährung mit tierischen Produk-ten.

o

Ordinal 1: stimme gar nicht zu 2: stimme eher nicht zu 3: teils-teils 4: stimme eher zu 5: stimme voll und ganz zu

23a. Environmental Awareness

Fast geschafft! Nur noch zwei Schätzfra-gen zur Umwelt und allgemeine Angaben zu Ihrer Person, dann ist die Umfrage be-endet. Bitte schätzen Sie, wofür Ihrer Meinung nach weltweit die meisten landwirtschaftli-chen Flächen genutzt werden. ENVIRONMENTAL AWARENESS

o Um Nahrungsmittel für die Menschen anzubauen

o Um Tierfutter anzubauen o Um Kraftstoffe anzubauen (z.B. Etha-

nol aus Mais)

Nominal

23b. Environmental Awareness

Was hat Ihrer Meinung nach den größeren negativen Einfluss auf den Klimawandel in Bezug auf CO2-Abgase? ENVIRONMENTAL AWARENESS

o Autoverkehr o Massentierhaltung o Fabriken o Flugzeuge

Nominal

24. Sozio-demographische Angaben

Zum Schluss einige Angaben für die Statis-tik-Auswertung. Am Seitenende können Sie sich für die Verlosung eintragen. Geschlecht

o weiblich o männlich

Binary

Alter

o 13 - 65 and older Continuous

Bundesland

o Baden-Württemberg o Bayern o Berlin o Brandenburg o Bremen o Hamburg o Hessen

Nominal

Appendix LXXIX

o Mecklenburg-Vorpommern o Niedersachsen o Nordrhein-Westfalen o Rheinland-Pfalz o Saarland o Sachsen o Sachsen-Anhalt o Schleswig-Holstein o Thüringen

Wohngegend

o Landgemeinde (bis 5.000 Einwohner) o Kleinstadt (bis 10.000 Einwohner) o Stadt (bis 100.000 Einwohner) o Großstadt (bis 1 Mio. Einwohner) o Metropole (über 1 Mio. Einwohner)

Nominal

Haushaltsgröße

o 1 Person o 2 Personen o 3 Personen o 4 Personen o 5 Personen und mehr

Nominal

Wieviel Kinder leben in Ihrem Haushalt?

o 0 Kinder o 1 Kind o 2 Kinder o 3 und mehr Kinder

Nominal

Was ist Ihr derzeit höchster Bildungsab-schluss?

o Hauptschule o Realschule o (Fach-) Abitur o Berufsausbildung o Fachhochschulabschluss/Universitäts-

abschluss o Promotion o Keinen Abschluss o Anderer, und zwar: o

Nominal

Wie hoch ist Ihr Nettoeinkommen?

o Unter 1000 EUR im Monat o 1000 EUR- 1500 EUR im Monat o 1500 EUR- 2500 EUR im Monat o 2500 EUR- 3500 EUR im Monat o Mehr als 3500 EUR im Monat o Dazu möchte ich keine Angabe ma-

chen.

Nominal

Haben Sie Feedback zur Umfrage, oder möchten Sie etwas loswerden, was der Fragebogen nicht umfasst hat? Falls Sie Interesse an den Umfrageergeb-nissen im Juni haben, geben Sie dies bitte mit einer kurzen Notiz und Ihrer Emailadresse hier an. Möchten Sie an der Verlosung teilnehmen?

o Nein o Ja, meine Email-Adresse lautet:

Binary

Endseite

Vielen lieben Dank für Ihre Unterstützung! Wenn Sie an der Verlosung teilgenommen haben, werde ich Sie am 02. Mai 2016 im Falle eines Gewinns per Email informieren. Freundliche Grüße & einen schönen Tag Yasmin Emre