michal czaplinski (2012) enhancement in self-recognition: lying to yourself to better lie to others

Upload: michal-czaplinski

Post on 06-Apr-2018

220 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    1/23

    1

    Abstract

    People often have a tendency to acquire and retain false beliefs, despite available

    evidence to the contrary, especially when the belief concerns some positive description of

    the self. Such tendency has been termed self-deception. According to Robert Trivers, self-

    deception evolved in order for people to better deceive others, because when people

    believe the lie themselves, they are less likely to be caught lying to others. An oft-cited

    example of self-deception is peoples tendency to judge an attractively enhanced version

    of their photograph to be their actual image. The present study replicated such result but

    failed to find evidence to support the idea that enhancement in self-recognition is

    synonymous with self-deception. The idea that self-deception might have adaptive value

    was also not supported.

    The power of holding two contradictory beliefs in one's mind simultaneously, and

    accepting both of them...To tell deliberate lies while genuinely believing in them, to forget

    any fact that has become inconvenient, and then, when it becomes necessary again, to

    draw it back from oblivion for just as long as it is needed, to deny the existence of

    objective reality and all the while to take account of the reality which one denies - all this is

    indispensably necessary.

    George Orwell, Nineteen Eighty-Four

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    2/23

    2

    Introduction

    Self-deception is a notoriously difficult concept to define. Traditionally, scholars in

    psychology and philosophy have thought of self-deception as analogical to deception of

    others (DeWeese-Boyd, 2012). Hence, deception occurs when agent A convinces agent B

    of some proposition p, while simultaneously believing its negation, ~p. Self-deception, on

    the other hand, is when A and B are in fact the same agent (Haight, 1980). As such, self-

    deception is virtually equivalent to what Orwell had famously called doublethink.

    However, modelling self-deception after interpersonal deception results in at least two

    major problems.

    First, it gives rise to two paradoxes or puzzles of self-deception. The static puzzle

    concerns the alleged impossibility of holding two contradictory beliefs in ones mind. The

    dynamic puzzle concerns the impossibility of intentionally deceiving oneself how does

    the self-deceiver knowingly acquire the false belief ~p, if she was aware of its falsehood

    the whole time? (Mele, 2001).

    Second, it renders any empirical study of self-deception very difficult, as it is almost

    impossible to verify whether the individual in question is truly self-deceived, i.e. holding

    two contradictory beliefs at the same time. That said, there have been two studies to date,

    which claimed to demonstrate existence of self-deception using the dual-belief framework.

    The first study by Gur & Sackeim (1979) used the fact that people do not like the sound of

    their own voice. They asked their subjects to listen to a tape recording of different voices

    and report whenever they think they can hear their own voice. The subjects galvanic skin

    response was also measured. It was shown that while the subjects sometimes failed to

    report hearing their own voice, the skin always got it right. In another elegant experiment,

    Quattrone & Tversky (1984) demonstrated that people shift their tolerance to pain in order

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    3/23

    3

    to suit a favourable interpretation of such tolerance. The subjects were first asked to

    submerge their forearms in cold water until they could not bear it anymore. Subsequently,

    they were told that either increased or decreased tolerance to pain was indicative of a

    healthy heart. Most subjects denied consciously trying to adjust their tolerance, even

    though the results showed a clear shift in the desirable direction.

    While both aforementioned experiments demonstrate a discrepancy between the

    subjects overt behaviour and their awareness of such behaviour, it does not follow that

    they held two contradictory beliefs simultaneously. Even if we accept the notion that one of

    the beliefs could be stored in the unconscious, while the other was acted on consciously

    (Gur & Sackeim, 1979) neither of the studies offers direct evidence that this is indeed

    happening. Therefore many scholars have more recently preferred a version of what I call

    a motivational account of self-deception (Mele, 1997; Mele, 2001; Trivers, 2000; Van

    Leeuwen, 2007). On this reading, a number of varying definitions have been put forward

    such as active misrepresentation of reality to the conscious mind (R Trivers, 2000) or

    treatment of relevant data in a motivationally biased way (Mele, 1997). While, the

    nuances of various definitions of self-deception are outwith the scope of this article, I wish

    to emphasise their common underlying assumption that a sufficient condition for self-

    deception is favouring one kind of information (usually the favourable one) over another, in

    accordance with ones goals and motivations.

    Another longstanding tradition in psychology, which dates back to Freud is to study

    self-deception in intrapersonal terms. On such reading, self-deception is an expression of

    ego defence (Freud, 1946) or a form psychological immune system attuned to

    protecting ones mental wellbeing (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998).

    However, according to Robert Trivers (1976/2006; Trivers, 1985; Hippel & Trivers, 2011)

    an intrapersonal account of self-deception is incomplete and we should turn to studying

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    4/23

    4

    self-deception in interpersonal terms. His original hypothesis is that self-deception might

    have evolved as a means of deceiving others more efficiently. Put simply - if you were to

    tell a lie, it would be more convincing if you believed in it yourself. The rationale behind

    such explanation is threefold: First, people give away a number of cues associated with

    lying and deception such as overall nervousness, increased vocal pitch (DePaulo et al.,

    2003), pausing or simplified sentence structure (Vrij et al., 2006). However, when

    engaging in self-deception, the individuals believe the falsehood themselves and would

    not give off such cues (Hippel & Trivers, 2011). Second, upon discovering deception,

    attribution of deceitful intent is essential in determining who to seek retribution from

    (Schweitzer, Hershey, & Bradlow, 2006). Self-deceived individuals are by definition not

    aware of their intention to deceive, thus are less likely to be punished if the deception is

    exposed. Third, in addition to Trivers arguments, I believe there might be at least one

    more reason for why self-deception evolved for interpersonal purposes. Being unaware of

    ones own deception might serve as a buffer which protects from retribution. Challenging

    a deceiver might be costly if he is deeply self-deceived and the potential challenger might

    be aware of this cost and even withdraw from seeking retribution in order to avoid hearing

    the oft used accusation: Are you calling me a liar!?

    Self-deception manifests itself in a number of ways. People think that they are better

    than average and possess more positive than negative traits (Alicke, 1985); they claim

    responsibility for their own group success but attribute failure to external, temporary or

    specific causes (Mezulis et al., 2004); a vast majority of drivers think they are better than

    the average driver (Svenson, 1981); and as many as 94% of college professors claim they

    are better than the average teacher at their own institution (Cross, 1977). These are just

    some examples of what Taylor and Brown (1988) termed positive illusions

    unrealistically high or positive beliefs about oneself. This paper focuses on a particular

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    5/23

    5

    example of such a positive illusion peoples tendency to think they are more attractive

    than they really are (Epley & Whitchurch, 2008). In their study Epley & Whitchurch

    photographed faces of 23 participants and subsequently morphed them with a very

    attractive face (AF) and a very unattractive face (UF) to produce 10 composite images

    ranging from 50% AF to 50% UF in 10% increments. Next, the subjects in the study were

    invited back to the lab and presented with 11 randomly ordered pictures (the 10

    composites and the actual photograph) and asked to identify which one was their actual

    image. Subsequently, they were shown each picture in isolation (in random order) and

    asked to estimate (from 0% to 100%) the likelihood that it is their actual image. The results

    showed that people, on average, identified the face that was about 10% AF as their own

    (in 3 separate experiments) and tended to claim that the attractive faces are more likely to

    be their own. The participants also completed a Rosneberg self-esteem scale (Rosenberg,

    1965) and a test of implicit self-esteem using name letter effect (Nuttin, 1985).

    The methods used by Epley & Whitchurch in their study, although adequate, could still

    be improved at least two ways. First, the participants photograph was used in all three

    experiments described in their paper, instead of its mirror image, which is obviously more

    familiar to the subjects (Mita, Dermer, & Knight, 1977). Second, the method used to create

    both more and less attractive faces was somewhat arbitrary. The AF that they used was a

    composite of several photographs obtained from a third-party website which resulted in a

    highly average facial image. While average faces are universally held to be very attractive,

    other qualities such as symmetry, youthfulness, sexual dimorphism or pleasant expression

    are also important (Rhodes, 2006). Conversely, the UF was a picture of a sex-matched

    patient with craniofacial syndrome. Although their images are certainly unattractive there

    was no objective criterion, which their unattractiveness was judged against. Consequently,

    the facial images of the subjects did not differ from the UF in a systematic fashion and it is

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    6/23

    6

    possible that UF were objectively more different from the subjects own face than the

    unattractive morphs. Therefore, the first aim of the current study is to refine the work of

    Epley & Whitchurch by replicating parts of their study using improved materials and

    methods.

    Von Hippel and Trivers (2011) claim that the effects demonstrated in Epley &

    Whitchurchs study (as well other cognitive biases and positive illusions mentioned

    before) are an example of self-deception. However, according to the distinctist account,

    bias requires a one-level mental representation, whereas self-deception requires a two-

    level representation, since the self is both the deceived and the deceiver (Pinker, 2011),

    hence, self-deception and bias (or positive illusions) should be considered separate.

    Regardless of which account is correct however, this debate is outwith the scope of the

    current paper, as its subject matter is phenomenological (or merely semantic) rather than

    empirical.

    The aim of the present study is to investigate the relationship between inflated views of

    ones attractiveness, as studied by Epley & Whitchurch and self-deception. In classic

    psychological literature on self-deception, the concept of self deception was originally used

    to explain biased responding to psychometric questionnaires (Sackheim & Gur, 1979;

    Paulhus, 1984). This tendency to give exaggerated or overly positive self-reports was

    termed Socially Desirable Responding (SDR) and synonymous with self-deception. To

    control for SDR in psychometric assessment a number of self-deception questionnaires

    have been devised. Arguably, the most popular one is Balanced Inventory of Desirable

    Responding (BIDR) also known as Paulhus Deception Scale, currently in 7th version

    (Paulhus, 1998). The BIDR consists of two sub-scales: Self-Deceptive Enhancement

    (SDE) and Impression Management (IM). The two subscales were developed in order to

    differentiate between the more public tendency to enhance ones self-image (IM) and more

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    7/23

    7

    private tendency for ego defense (SDE) (Paulhus, 1984). There is an ongoing controversy

    about the meaning and validity of the constructs underlying these two factors. Among

    known effects, SDE (but not IM) has been linked to self-perception of mental health

    (Paulhus & Reid, 1991) as well as inflated self-ratings relative to ratings by fellow group

    members (Paulhus, 1998).

    Literature linking SDR to other positive illusions, however, is limited. It has been

    previously shown that high self-deception subjects exhibit more illusion of control, belief

    they are safe drivers and proneness to love (Paulhus & Reid, 1991) as well as higher

    intrinsic religiosity (Leak & Fish, 1989) but no research has looked at unrealistically

    positive evaluations of ones own attractiveness with connection to self-deception. Since

    BIDR measures a general trait-like tendency to give positive self-evaluations (Paulhus,

    2002) it can be used in conjunction with the face recognition task used by Epley &

    Whitchurch to test whether enhancement in self-recognition is part of the trait-like

    tendency to self-deceive. Since, Epley & Whitchurch found a relationship between

    enhancement in own-face recognition and implicit measures of self-esteem but not explicit

    measures of self-esteem, it is important to note that BIDR is a semi-implicit measure,

    because the scores from BIDR are not taken at face value (Paulhus, 2002). Therefore, the

    second aim of the study is to investigate whether the self-enhancing effect demonstrated

    by Epley & Whitchurch constitutes self-deception.

    Finally, the thirdaim of the current study is to test the idea that self-deception evolved

    in order to better deceive others byapplying it to the domain of sexual attraction. As von

    Hippel & Trivers put it themselves: To test these possibilities, one could assess whether

    the presence of attractive women causes men to show greater self-enhancement in their

    information-processing biases (von Hippel & Trivers, 2011, p.14). Thus, the argument is

    as follows - if men want to convince women of their attractiveness, they should believe in

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    8/23

    8

    their attractiveness themselves. To my best knowledge, the argument for adaptiveness of

    self-deception has never been thoroughly tested and this study would be one of the first to

    explore this theory. That said, a number of recent literature hints that self-deception might

    have adaptive value. First, a number of recent articles using agent-based and game-

    theoretic models have shown that self-enhancing perceptions of oneself can evolve and

    be beneficial to agents. Specifically, overconfidence in conflict is one such trait, which is

    suspected to confer adaptive benefits as long as the benefits of victory are sufficiently

    large relative to costs of competing (Johnson, Weidmann, & Cederman, 2011; Johnson &

    Fowler, 2011).

    Three hypotheses in the current study are as follows:

    1. People will show enhancement in the recognition of their own facial image.

    2. Enhancement in face recognition should correlate with measures of self-deception.

    3. In the presence of an attractive female confederate the male participants will be

    more prone to self-enhance on the face recognition task, but not score higher on

    measures of self-deception than in the presence of a male equivalent.

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    9/23

    9

    Methods

    Design

    The subjects in the study were randomly split into two groups - half of them completed

    the experiment in the presence of an attractive female confederate. The experimental task

    consisted of the face recognition task and BIDR questionnaire.

    Participants

    The subjects were 26 male Caucasian students of Glasgow University who self-

    identified as heterosexual. They volunteered to complete the experiment for either a

    financial reward or course credits. The mean age of participants was 19.6 years. All

    participants were clean-shaven on the day of the experiment.

    Materials

    All participants first had a photograph taken using a digital camera. The pictures of

    their faces were morphed in software based on Matlab and subsequently presented to

    them on the computer screen. Next, the subjects completed a 40-item Balanced Inventory

    of Desirable Responding (6th edition) (Paulhus, 1991). The 7th edition is commercial-only

    and only the previous edition is freely available for research purposes. The two subscales

    (SDE & IM) typically exhibit some inter-correlation (Paulhus, 2002) hence were collapsed

    into one measure of self-deception.

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    10/23

    10

    Procedure

    First, the participants arrived at the lab and had their faces photographed. Once they

    all had their photographs taken, the photographs were pooled together and the most

    representative image was selected for each participant (forward facing, focused and

    evenly lit). The image of each participant was subsequently separated into the texture and

    shape components. To create the shape component, each image was manually annotated

    using 41 landmark points corresponding to the overall shape of the face as well as outlines

    of other salient features: eyes, mouth and nose. By treating the landmark points as

    interconnected vertices, a mesh was created, which was the shape component of the

    facial image. The texture component was created by warping the pixel-by-pixel

    representation of the images onto the common reference shape. The pictures were also

    mirrored, because the mirror images of photographs are known to be significantly more

    familiar to participants than their true images (Mita et al., 1977).

    Next, after a period of about 2 to 3 weeks each of participants was invited back to the

    lab to complete the second part of the experiment. Depending on the condition, they were

    greeted by the male experimenter (whom they have met while taking the photograph) or

    the female confederate, whom they have not met before. In both conditions, the

    experimenter/confederate were present in the room for the duration of the experiment. The

    subjects were seated in front of the computer screen and asked to rate the facial

    photographs of all other participants in the study on a 7-point Likert scale ranging from

    very unattractive to very attractive.

    Using the ratings from each subject, the participants actual images were morphed to

    create both more and less attractive versions. The face was divided into principal

    components according to the mesh crated by the connecting the landmark points. Linear

    regression of the face shape and texture against the ratings of each face made by the

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    11/23

    11

    participant were performed. The regression coefficients for each principal component (the

    texture) and each vortex (the shape) were used to calculate the attractiveness dimension

    for each participant (Vetter & Troje, 1997). Both the shape and texture components for

    each participants own face were morphed in the direction of faces that they rated higher

    to create more attractive morphs and in the opposite direction to create less attractive

    morphs. On each trial the subject was presented with a lineup of 7 images containing the

    actual photograph, the 3 attractive morphs and the 3 unattractive morphs in random order.

    The morphs varied randomly in the amount of deviation from the actual image on each trial.

    The participants completed 50 of such trials, in which they were asked to choose the facial

    image that they thought was their actual image.

    Subsequently, the participants completed a pen-and-paper version of BIDR

    questionnaire and were debriefed with respect to the objectives of the study.

    Results

    1. Face recognition task

    As predicted, the participants tended to choose the attractive morphs more often the

    unattractive ones. The one-sample t-test shows that the difference was highly significant

    [t(25) = 5.19, p < 0.001, d= 1.02] with a mean of 0.24 and 95% CI of (0.15, 0.34). The

    histogram of mean morph level selected by all participants is presented in Figure 1. The

    mean morph level selected was above zero for all but 4 participants.

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    12/23

    12

    2. Correlation with BIDR

    The scores from face recognition task were correlated with scores from the BIDR

    questionnaire. However, since the SDE and IM factors of the BIDR showed no significant

    correlation, they were analysed separately. All of the correlations with the face recognition

    task were highly non-significant the table of results is presented in Table 1.

    Table 1. Relationships between SDE, IM, BIDR and face recognition task.

    SDE IM BIDR

    Face recognition

    task

    r= -0.08

    p = 0.69

    -0.15

    0.46

    -0.16

    0.43SDE 0.02

    0.93

    Values in cells correspond to: Pearsons correlation coefficientsp-values

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    13/23

    13

    3. Confederate vs. no confederate

    The group who completed the experiment in the presence of a female confederate

    showed no improvement in the face recognition task, SDE or IM over the group who

    completed the experiment in the presence of the male experimenter. The results of one-

    way MANOVA show that there was no significant between-group difference [F(3,22) =

    0.20, p = 0.90] Since the difference between groups was not significant, no follow-up

    comparisons were conducted.

    Discussion

    Hypothesis 1.

    The results of the face recognition task showed that the participants tended to select

    the more attractive faces as their own significantly more often than the unattractive ones.

    These results gives further support to the notion that people enhance while assessing their

    own attractiveness. Moreover, it shows that such enhancement can be implicit and

    automatic in nature and thus distinct from explicit measures of positive illusions (Taylor &

    Brown, 1988; Taylor & Brown, 1994).

    This finding also extends previous research, which used face morphing techniques to

    assess peoples self-image. In a similar study, Verosky & Todorov (2010) morphed their

    participants faces with either more or less trustworthy facial images generated by the

    computer. They found that the participants perceived the more trustworthy morphs as

    more similar to the self across all levels of morphing. Therefore, the current study adds to

    this literature. However, puzzlingly, such results are also partly at odds with literature on

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    14/23

    14

    body image. Both males and females of almost all ages show dissatisfaction with their own

    body in terms of both weight and size, more so among women (Feingold & Mazzella,

    1998), individuals with eating disorders (Skrzypek, Wehmeier, & Remschmidt, 2001) and

    especially during adolescence (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004; Powell, Matacin, & Stuart,

    2001). However, it must be noted that body image dissatisfaction is not the same as

    enhancement in the perception of ones body. In addition, subjective measures, which use

    body size or weight, are particularly ill-suited to test body perception in men. The reason is

    that the ideal male body is highly muscular, but low in body fat and the two measures are

    confounded - weight and size correlates positively with muscle mass but negatively with

    low body fat. Nevertheless, some recent studies have assessed the perception of own

    body using the method called somatoform matrix, which considers body fat and body

    muscle as separate factors. The method could be considered a whole-body analogue of

    the task conducted in the present paper (Cafri & Thompson, 2004). The somatoform

    matrix is a bidimensional measure, which consists of a computerised library of images

    varying in both muscle mass and body fat in uniform increments. In one study performed

    in three separate countries (the US, France and Austria) men were asked to select a

    computer generated image that would most closely resemble their actual body and the

    results showed that in all countries men tended to pick a slightly more muscular model

    (Pope et al., 2000). Such results are consistent with the findings of the current study as

    well as Epley and Whitchurch (2008).

    In a study of enhancement in self-perception cultural factors are important to consider.

    A number of studies in the recent decades uncovered a multitude of cultural differences

    ranging from social cognition through economic decision making to visual perception

    (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). For example, Westerners have been shown to

    score much higher on measures of explicit self-esteem than non-Westerners. However,

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    15/23

    15

    levels of implicit self-esteem have been shown to be comparable in both cultures

    (Yamaguchi et al., 2007). Since, the scores on face recognition task have correlated

    positively with implicit but not explicit self-esteem in Epley & Whitchurchs 2008 study, it

    can be concluded that inclusion of Caucasian participants only was justified.

    As second shortcoming of the current study was the necessary exclusion of female

    participants, in order to test Hypothesis 3. However, no sex difference is expected on the

    face recognition task as both males and females are presumed to enhance their

    perceptions equally. In fact, any potential difference between the sexes should, in principle,

    be an even bigger enhancement in females than males. As research in evolutionary

    psychology has shown, females more often than males put emphasis on their appearance

    and are aware of the fact that males seek attractiveness in a potential partner (Buss,

    1989). Nevertheless, no such sex difference was reported by Epley & Whitchurch, hence it

    is concluded that the lack of female participants was not a significant limitation of the

    present study.

    Many participants in the study reported dissatisfaction with the attractiveness of their

    actual photograph, after completing the study. They have also reported giving very low

    ratings to most images of other participants with the majority of faces receiving a rating of

    1, 2 or 3 on a scale between 1 and 7. Such reports indicate that the combination of

    unnaturally high luminance, en face orientation and cropping might have resulted in an

    image that was of inherently lower attractiveness than the participants actual, objective

    attractiveness level. If that had been the case the enhancement in self-recognition could

    have been driven by the fact that the actual photographs underestimate the subjects true

    attractiveness. To remedy such possible effect, the task could first be performed by a

    stranger in the presence of the actual participant. The stranger would be asked to select

    an image that she thinks belongs to the actual participant. Subsequently, the mean morph

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    16/23

    16

    level selected by her would be used as the true, actual image during the task involving the

    participant. Since, as Epley & Whitchurch (2008) have shown, there is enhancement in the

    recognition of a friends image, comparable to own-image recognition, using a friend of the

    participant could have potentially biased the procedure.

    Hypothesis 2.

    Contrary to predictions, no correlation was found between the measures of trait self-

    deception and the face recognition task. The result can be interpreted, as showing that

    enhancement in own-face perception is distinct from trait-like general tendency for self-

    deception. The result shows that the phenomenon measured by the face recognition task

    is domain specific and cannot be subsumed under the more domain-general construct of

    self-deception. The result is somewhat surprising, as both SDE and IM have been shown

    to correlate with various measures of positive illusions such as unrealistic optimism and

    illusory superiority (Hoorens, 1995) as well as more importantly to both implicit and explicit

    self-esteem (Greenwald & Farnham, 2000). Since implicit self-esteem has been shown to

    correlate with the face recognition task and BIDR is also a semi-implicit measure it is

    surprising that the correlations did not even approach significance.

    Hypothesis 3.

    Finally, contrary to predictions, the group who completed the experiment in the

    presence of a female confederate did not exhibit any more enhancement in self-

    recognition over the group who completed the task in the presence of the male

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    17/23

    17

    experimenter. In line with the predictions, however, there was no difference between the

    groups on measures of self-deception.

    Finding no effect of the presence of the female confederate on BIDR scores was of

    secondary importance and not particularly interesting in the light of lack of significance on

    the face recognition task. However, at least one conclusion can be drawn from such a

    result the tendency for self-deception is a stable personality trait is not influenced by the

    sex of the companion. This conclusion does unfortunately lose its power, when not

    contrasted with enhancement on the face recognition task, which was predicted to be a

    more malleable measure and activated by the cue of the presence of an attractive female.

    The most trivial explanation of the insignificance of the results could be that the female

    confederate simply did not exert a predicted effect on the participants. It might be the case

    that the confederate was simply not attractive enough to evoke the anticipated response.

    Although the choice of the confederate was arbitrary and mostly dictated by practical

    reasons, there is no reason to doubt that the confederate was attractive enough for the

    task, since she did not deviate strongly from the archetype any of the universals of female

    beauty such as waist-to-hip ratio of around 0.7, long hair, youthfulness, clear skin, facial

    symmetry, averageness and sexual dimorphism (Buss, 1994; Fisher & Voracek, 2006;

    Rhodes, 2006).

    Second, it might be the case that even though the attractiveness of the confederate

    was not a problem, the presence of a person in the room during the experiment created

    some confounding effects. The self-identification on the face recognition task might have

    been influenced by a number of factors unrelated to the experiment such as countless

    differences in individual personality characteristics of the experimenter or the discrepancy

    between the attractiveness level of the experimenter and the participants, who are likely to

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    18/23

    18

    feel more confident about their good looks in the presence of an unattractive experimenter

    than in the presence of an attractive one (Banaji & Prentice, 1994).

    Despite lack of statistical significance, the results are not worthless, but call for a

    revision of the experimental protocol and further inquiry. However, research that denies

    adaptive value of self-deception cannot be ignored, especially in the light of null results of

    the present study. For example, Peterson (2003) has found that self-deception predicts

    higher susceptibility to the gamblers fallacy in a simple card game and high self-deceivers

    were more prone to risk more money in the game and consequently lost more money than

    low-self-deceivers. A similar conviction was expressed by Johnson, Vincent, & Ross

    (1997) who gave their subjects negative feedback on a bogus IQ and subsequently

    measured their problem-solving ability. They showed that greater self-deception predicted

    worse post-failure problem solving and greater hostility. Finally, the only study to date

    which claims to have tested the theory of adaptive value of self-deception (McKay, Novello,

    & Taylor, in preparation) which was mentioned in McKay & Dennett (2009) has found

    preliminary evidence that high self-deceivers were less likely to be trusted in an

    anonymous cooperative exchange situation based on the Prisoners Dilemma paradigm.

    Directions for further research possible improvements.

    The current study can be improved in at least 5 following ways: (1) It should be

    ensured that none of the participants in the study knows each other. At least two

    participants in the present study knew each other, as many of them were first-year

    psychology students. The overall impact of such familiarity on the ratings of attractiveness

    in the present study should be minimal, but under ideal conditions such familiarity should

    be controlled for; (2) The first part of the experiment, when the participants photographs

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    19/23

    19

    are taken should be run by a different experimenter than the rest of the study in order to

    minimize any effect that familiarity with the experimenter might have on the face

    recognition task and BIDR. Ideally the during the second part of the experiment, each

    subject should be run by a different male or female confederate of matched attractiveness

    in order to minimize the impact of any personality characteristics on the task performance;

    (3) A third (control) condition should be added to the experiment in order to control for the

    effect of the mere presence of the experimenter/confederate in the room; (4) Unless

    suggestion #3 is fulfilled, a representative sample of potential confederates should be

    rated for attractiveness by a group of males in order to ensure that the most attractive

    confederate is chosen to take part in the study; (5) Finally, in order to make the

    experimental manipulation stronger, some additional measures could be employed such

    as priming the participants to think about sexual relationships or giving them a bogus

    questionnaire asking about their sex life. Additionally, a higher level of engagement

    between the female confederate and the participants would be desirable, for example, the

    aforementioned bogus questionnaire could be administered orally.

    Acknowledgements

    I would like to thank my supervisor Dr. Oliver Garrod for helpful comments, advice and

    support in performing the experiment. I would also like to thank Daria Merkoulova for her

    help in conducting the experiment.

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    20/23

    20

    References

    Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability andcontrollability of trait adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(6),

    16211630.

    Banaji, M R, & Prentice, D. A. (1994). The Self in Social Contexts.Annual Review ofPsychology, 45(1), 297-332. Annual Reviews.

    Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex Differences in Human Mate references: Evolutionary hypothesesTested in 37 Cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12(01), 1-14.

    Buss, D. M. (1994). Evolution of Desire. New York, NY: Basic Books.

    Cafri, G., & Thompson, J. K. (2004). Measuring Male Body Image: A Review of the Current

    Methodology. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 5(1), 18-29.

    Cross, P. (1977). Not Can But Will College Teaching Be Improved. New Directions forHigher Education, 17, 1-15.

    DePaulo, B. M., Lindsay, J. J., Malone, B. E., Muhlenbruck, L., Charlton, K., & Cooper, H.(2003). Cues to deception. Psychological Bulletin, 129(1), 74-118.

    Dawkins, R. (1976) The Selfish Gene. Oxford, Oxford University Press

    Deweese-Boyd, I. (2010). Self-Deception. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall2010 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.), Retrieved fromhttp://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2010/entries/self-deception/

    Epley, N., & Whitchurch, E. (2008). Mirror, Mirror on the Wall: Enhancement in Self-Recognition. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34 (9), 1159-1170.doi:10.1177/0146167208318601

    Feingold, A., & Mazzella, R. (1998). Gender Differences in Body Image Are Increasing .Psychological Science , 9 (3), 190-195.

    Fisher, M. L., & Voracek, M. (2006). The shape of beauty: determinants of female physicalattractiveness. Journal of Cosmetic Dermatology, 5(2), 190-194.

    Freud, A. (1946). The ego and the mechanisms of defence. (p. x, 196). Oxford, England:International Universities Press.

    Gilbert, D. T., Pinel, E. C., Wilson, T. D., Blumberg, S. J., & Wheatley, T. P. (1998).Immune neglect: A source of durability bias in affective forecasting. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 75(3), 617-638. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.3.617

    Greenwald, A. G., & Farnham, S. D. (2000). Using the Implicit Association Test to

    Measure Self-Esteem and Self-Concept. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 79(6), 1022-1038. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.79.6.1022

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    21/23

    21

    Gur, R. C., & Sackeim, H. A. (1979). Self-deception: A concept in search of aphenomenon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(2), 147-169.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.37.2.147

    Haight, M. (1980).A Study of Self-Deception. Sussex: Harvester Press.

    Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2-3), 61-83; discussion 83-135.

    Hippel, W. V., & Trivers, R. (2011). The evolution and psychology of self-deception.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(1), 1-16. doi:10.1017/S0140525X10001354

    Hoorens, V. (1995). Self-Favoring Biases, Self-Presentation, and the Self-OtherAsymmetry in Social Comparison. Journal of Personality, 63(4), 793-817.

    Johnson, Dominic D P, Weidmann, N. B., & Cederman, L.-E. (2011). Fortune favours the

    bold: an agent-based model reveals adaptive advantages of overconfidence in war.PloS one, 6(6), e20851. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0020851

    Johnson, Dominic D. P., & Fowler, J. H. (2011). The evolution of overconfidence. Nature,477(7364), 317-320. doi:10.1038/nature10384

    Johnson, E. A., Vincent, N., & Ross, L. (1997). Self-Deception versus Self-Esteem inBuffering the Negative Effects of Failure. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 385-405.

    Leak, G. K., & Fish, S. (1989). Religious Orientation, Impression Management, and Self-Deception: Toward a Clarification of the Link between Religiosity and SocialDesirability. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 28(3), 355-359.

    McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2004). Body image dissatisfaction among malesacross the lifespan: A review of past literature. Journal of Psychosomatic Research,56(6), 675-685.

    McKay, R., Novello, D. & Taylor, A. (in preparation) The adaptive value of self-deception.

    McKay, R. T., & Dennett, D. C. (2009). The evolution of misbelief. The Behavioral and

    Brain Sciences, 32(6), 493-510; d-61. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990975

    Mele, a R. (1997). Real self-deception. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(1), 91-102;discussion 103-36.

    Mele, A. R. (2001). Self-Deception Unmasked. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., & Hankin, B. L. (2004). Is there a universalpositive bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, andcultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychological Bulletin, 130(5),711747.

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    22/23

    22

    Mita, T. H., Dermer, M., & Knight, J. (1977). Reversed facial images and the mere-exposure hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35(8), 597-601.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.8.597

    Nuttin, J. M. (1985). Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: The name letter effect.

    European Journal of Social Psychology, 15(3), 353-361.

    Paulhus, D L. (1998). Interpersonal and intrapsychic adaptiveness of trait self-enhancement: a mixed blessing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5),1197-1208.

    Paulhus, D.L. (1998). Manual for the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding:Version 7. Toronto/Buffalo: Multi-Health Systems.

    Paulhus, D. L. (2002). Socially desirable responding: The evolution of a construct. The roleof constructs in psychological and educational measurement(pp. 4969). Mahwah,

    NJ: Erlbaum.

    Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 46(3), 598-609. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.598

    Paulhus, D. L, & Reid, D. B. (1991). Enhancement and denial in socially desirableresponding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(2), 307-317.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.307

    Peterson, J. B. (2003). Self-deception and failure to modulate responses despite accruing

    evidence of error. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(3), 205-223.doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00569-X

    Pinker, S. (2011). Representations and decision rules in the theory of self-deception.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(1), 35-37.

    Pope, H. G., Gruber, A. J., Mangweth, B., Bureau, B., DeCol, C., Jouvent, R., & Hudson,J. I. (2000). Body image perception among men in three countries. The AmericanJournal of Psychiatry, 157(8), 1297-301.

    Powell, J. L., Matacin, M. L., & Stuart, A. E. (2001). Body Esteem: An Exception to Self-

    Enhancing Illusions? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31(9), 1951-1978.

    Quattrone, G. A., & Tversky, A. (1984). Causal versus diagnostic contingencies: On self-deception and on the voters illusion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,46(2), 237-248. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.46.2.237

    Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty.Annual review ofpsychology, 57, 199-226. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208

    Sackeim, H. A., & Gur, R. C. (1978). Self-deception, other-deception and consciousness.In G. E. Schwartz & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances

    in research (Vol. 2) (pp.139-197). New York: Plenum Press.

  • 8/2/2019 Michal Czaplinski (2012) Enhancement in Self-Recognition: Lying to Yourself to Better Lie to Others

    23/23

    Schweitzer, M. E., Hershey, J. C., & Bradlow, E. T. (2006). Promises and lies: Restoringviolated trust. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 101(1), 1-19.

    Skrzypek, S., Wehmeier, P. M., & Remschmidt, H. (2001). Body image assessment usingbody size estimation in recent studies on anorexia nervosa. A brief review. European

    Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 10(4), 215-221. doi:10.1007/s007870170010

    Svenson, O. (1981). Are we less risky and more skillful than our fellow drivers?ActaPsychologica, 47, 143-151.

    Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and Well-Being: A social psychologicalperspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103(2), 193-210

    Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1994). Positive illusions and well-being revisited: separatingfact from fiction. Psychological Bulletin, 116(1), 21-7.

    Trivers, R. (2000). The elements of a scientific theory of self-deception.Annals of the NewYork Academy of Sciences, 907, 114-31.

    Trivers, Robert. (1985). Social Evolution. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings Pub. Co.

    Van Leeuwen, D. S. N. (2007). The Spandrels of Self-Deception: Prospects for aBiological Theory of a Mental Phenomenon. Philosophical Psychology, 20(3), 329-348. doi:10.1080/09515080701197148

    Verosky, S. C., & Todorov, A. (2010). Differential neural responses to faces physically

    similar to the self as a function of their valence. NeuroImage, 49, 1690-1698.doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.017

    Vetter, T., & Troje, N. F. (1997). Separation of texture and shape in images of faces forimage coding and synthesis. JOSA A, 14(9), 2152-2161.

    Yamaguchi, S., Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Murakami, F., Chen, D., Shiomura, K.,Kobayashi, C., et al. (2007). Apparent Universality of Positive Implicit Self-Esteem .Psychological Science, 18(6), 498-500.