measuring intelligibility in children: why and how

26
MEASURING INTELLIGIBILITY IN CHILDREN: WHY AND HOW Peter Flipsen Jr., PhD, S-LP(C), CCC-SLP Idaho State University [email protected] A S H A 2 0 1 0 P h i l a d e l p h i a , P A

Upload: fruma

Post on 23-Feb-2016

63 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How. Peter Flipsen Jr., PhD, S-LP(C), CCC-SLP Idaho State University [email protected]. Intelligibility. Making yourself understood. The ultimate goal of human communication. Usually the long-term goal for most clients we work with. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

MEASURING INTELLIGIBILITY IN CHILDREN: WHY AND HOW

Peter Flipsen Jr., PhD, S-LP(C), CCC-SLPIdaho State University

[email protected]

Page 2: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

INTELLIGIBILITY Making yourself understood.

The ultimate goal of human communication. Usually the long-term goal for most clients

we work with.

IF IT’S THE GOAL, LET’S MEASURE IT! Measure it at baseline to know where we start. Measure it regularly to monitor progress.

Page 3: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

INTELLIGIBILITY AND ACCURACY Related but NOT THE SAME THING.

Typically-developing 4 year old is fully intelligible (100%). But still makes some speech sound errors.

Many children with SSD present with much reduced intelligibility. But they may have enough sounds correct in

single words that they DON’T qualify for services.

Page 4: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

BEING INTELLIGIBLE Means:

Choosing the best words to convey intentions Formulating the syntax correctly Knowing the correct sounds to produce Including the appropriate prosody Staying on topic Having the physical skill to produce the entire

message fluently.

Page 5: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

INTELLIGIBILITY More than just speech sound accuracy!

Can NOT be adequately measured with an articulation test! Ertmer (2010) and others have shown that single

word artic scores rarely account for more than 25% of the variability.

Need to measure it directly by asking: “how much can be understood?”

Page 6: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

MEASURING INTELLIGIBILITY

If you want to know how well someone is being understood,

YOU HAVE TO MEASURE HOW MUCH IS BEING UNDERSTOOD DIRECTLY!

Page 7: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

MEASURING INTELLIGIBILITY How do we do it?

Many clinicians make informal estimates.

Demo – clinicians listened to a sample and rated.

Page 8: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

INFORMAL ESTIMATES? Here’s a sampling of values the clinicians came up

with: 20%, 65%, 45%, 40%, 55%, 35%, 15%, 70%, 60%, 40%,

40%, 50%, 30%, 65%, 15%, 55%, 25%, 45%, 60%, 35%

Probably no better than pulling numbers out of a hat.

Highly variable across listeners. Rely on memory. Not valid for monitoring progress as SLP becomes a

“more familiar listener”. Calibration changes over time.

Page 9: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

RATING SCALES – AN ALTERNATIVE? Many available. For example:

1. completely intelligible 2. mostly intelligible 3. somewhat intelligible 4. mostly unintelligible 5. completely unintelligible

Schiavetti (1992) identified several problems: 1. listeners don’t treat all parts of the scale equally. 2. high variability in scores across listeners

especially in the middle of the scale. 3. they don’t allow us to identify possible sources of

intelligibility breakdowns.

Page 10: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PARATING SCALES – PRACTICAL PROBLEMS

1. How much change does it take to move from one level to the next? Not clear. Not consistent across points on the scale.

2. Such scales are NOT sensitive enough to track change in individuals over time.

Page 11: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

WRITE-DOWN PROCEDURES Generally considered the best approach.

Listener tries to identify exactly what the intended message was.

We calculate actual % understood.

Page 12: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

WRITE-DOWN PROCEDURES Possible at several linguistic levels.

1. Single words. Spontaneous or imitated.

2. Sentences. Spontaneous or imitated.

3. Conversation.

Page 13: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

WRITE-DOWN PROCEDURES Which level to use?

Speakers with less than perfect speech likely to perform differently at different levels. Milder cases do better in connected speech

because context helps listener to “fill in the blanks”.

More severe cases do better in single words because high level of errors overwhelm the listener.

Ideally do more than one level.

Page 14: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

SINGLE WORD PROCEDURES Several possible approaches.

Most flexible procedure available is:

Children’s Speech Intelligibility Measure (CSIM) By Wilcox and Morris Published by Psych Corp in 1999.

Page 15: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

CSIM 50 item test. NOT norm-referenced. Can be administered multiple times because

unique test is created each time. Each item has 12 “similar sounding” words.

E.g., tall, stall, wall, shawl, call, all, fall, ball, hall, crawl, mall, Paul

E.g., Tanner, planner, matter, manner, mother, banner, mother, brother, sander, bother, batter, other

Page 16: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

CSIM Pick 1 item at random before administering.

Child imitates examiner saying the word (can read it if they are old enough).

Record child’s productions and give to unfamiliar listeners (preferably at least 2). Listener either writes word down with no reference or

circles word on blank test form (multiple choice option).

Page 17: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

SENTENCE LEVEL PROCEDURES Several possible have been proposed.

For very young or very unintelligible children one quite flexible one is:

The Beginner’s Intelligibility Test (BIT). Originally designed for the hearing impaired but

can be used with any population.

Osberger, M. J., Robbins, A. M., Todd, S. L., & Riley, A. I. (1994). Speech intelligibility of children with cochlear implants. The Volta Review, 96(5), 169-180.

Page 18: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

BIT NOT norm-referenced. 4 sets of 2-5 word sentences.

E.g., 1. The bear sleeps. 2. Mommy sits. 3. The rabbit hops. 4. The cowboy jumps. 5. Grandma falls. 6. That is a black hat. 7. The boy is under the table. 8. My airplane is small. 9. He is painting the chair. 10. She is cooking dinner.

Page 19: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

BIT Use one set each time so it can be

administered at regular intervals.

Child imitates or reads the sentences which are recorded.

Give recording to 2 unfamiliar listeners who write down what they hear.

Calculate % of words correctly identified.

Page 20: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

FOR OLDER CHILDREN (7 YEARS +) Could use sentence portion of: Assessment of the Intelligibility of Dysarthric

Speech. By Yorkston & Beukelman. Published by Pro-Ed. NOT norm-referenced. Select 5 sentences from each list of 5-8 word

sentences (20 total sentences, 130 total words). Child imitates or reads the sentences which are

recorded. Give recording to 2 unfamiliar listeners who write

down what they hear. Calculate % of words correctly identified.

Page 21: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

CONVERSATION Most ecologically valid approach (i.e., this is

what we do most of the time). Record true interaction. Avoid narratives as they may induce abnormal

prosody which may influence scores. Have unfamiliar listeners transcribe what they

hear (regular spelling!).

% understood = words understood ------------------------- (words understood + words not

understood)

Page 22: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

CONVERSATION – BIG CHALLENGE Unlike CSIM or BIT we won’t know what the

intended targets are.

How many words are present in the parts we don’t understand? With occasional unintelligible parts, listeners can

easily guess how many words are there. They use context to help.

Page 23: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

CONVERSATION – BIG CHALLENGE With longer stretches no context to help.

Listeners can count syllables! Each syllable contains a vowel and vowels are louder. Have them put X down for each syllable heard.

Use the syllable counts to estimate the number of unknown words. For most young children about 75% of words are 1

syllable long. Most of the rest are 2 syllables long. 3:1 ratio = 1.25 syllables per word on average. Count syllables heard. Divide by 1.25 to get words

not understood.

Page 24: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

WHO SHOULD BE JUDGES? Unfamiliar = best test.

Don’t do it yourself because you will become familiar very quickly (and biased!).

Preferably use inexperienced listeners but use colleagues who don’t know the child if necessary. Do it for each other to spread the work around.

Page 25: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PA

WHO SHOULD BE JUDGES? Don’t use parents or siblings.

Tend to be highly familiar so you get over-estimate of intelligibility.

Reference: Flipsen, P., Jr. (1995). Speaker-listener familiarity: Parents as judges of delayed speech intelligibility. Journal of Communication Disorders 28(1), 3-19.

Page 26: Measuring Intelligibility in Children: Why and How

ASHA 2010 Philadelphia, PACLINICAL DECISIONS:HOW INTELLIGIBLE? WHAT CRITERIA? No known valid norms available.

Too many variables involved to develop such norms.

Best index = age in years / 4 = % understood in conversation.

2 years old = 2/4 or 50% 3 years old = 3/4 or 75% 4 years old = 4/4 or 100% *

* speech sound errors still quite possible.

Reference: Coplan, J., & Gleason, J. R. (1988). Unclear speech: Recognition and significance of unintelligible speech in preschool children. Pediatrics, 82, 447–452.