janis groupthink

7

Click here to load reader

Upload: a280872

Post on 01-Dec-2015

110 views

Category:

Documents


5 download

DESCRIPTION

groupthink according to author Janis

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Janis Groupthink

t.

84

Groupt....-..-...-.. ........-.by Irving L. Janis

c

"HOW COULD WE HAVE BEEN' so STUPID?" Presi­dent John F. Kennedy asked after he and aclose group of advisers had blundered into the

Bay of Pigs invasion. For the last two years I havebeen studying that question, as it applies not only tothe Bay of Pigs decision-makers but also to those wholed the United States into such other major fiascos asthe failure to be prepared for the attack on Pearl Har­bor, the Korean War stalemate and the escalation ofthe Vietnam WaI,.

Stupidity certainly is not the explanation. The menwho participated in making the Bay of Pigs decision,for instance, comprised one of the greatest arrays ofintellectual talent in the history of American Gov­ernment-Dean Rusk, Robert ~lcNamara, DouglasDillon, Robert Kennedy, ~lcGeorge Bundy, ArthurSchlesinger Jr., Allen Dulles and others.

It also seemed to me that explanations were in­complete if they concentrated only on disturbancesin the behavior of each individual within a decision­making body: temporary emotional states of elation,fear, or anger that reduce a man's mental efficiency,for example, or chronic blind spots arising from aman's social prejudices or idiosyncratic biases.

I preferred to broaden the picture by looking atthe fiascos from the standpoint of group dynamics asit has been explored over the past three decades, firstby the great social psychologist Kurt Lewin and laterin many experimental situations by myself and otherbeha~oralscientists. My conclusion after poring overhundreds of relevant documents-historical reportsabout formal group meetings and informal conversa­tions among the members-is that the groups thatcommitted the fiascos were victims of what I call"groupthink.,.

IIGrcupy." In each case study, I was surprised todiscover the extent to which each group displayedthe typical phenomena of social conformity that areregularly encountered in studies of group dynamicsamong ordinary citizens. For example, some of thephenomena appear to be completely in line withfindings from social-psychological experiments SllOW­

ing that powerful social pressures are brought to bear

by the members of a cohesive group whenever a dis­sident begins to voice his objections to a group con­sensus. Other phenomena are reminiscent of theshared illusions observed in encounter groups andfriendship cliques when the members simultaneouslyreach a peak of "groupy" feelings.

Above all, there are numerous indications pointingto the development-of group norms that bolster mo­rale at the expense of critical thinking. One of themost common norms appears to be that of remainingloyal to the group by sticldng with the policies towhich the group. has already committed itself, evenwhen those policies are obviously working out badlyand have unintended consequeaces that disturb theconscience of each member.. This is one of' the keycharacteristics of groupthink.

1984. I use the term groupthink as a quick and easyway to refer to the mode of thinking that persons en-:">.gage in when concurrence-seeking becomes so dominant in a cohesive ingroup that it tends to overriderealistic appraisal of alternative courses of action.Groupthink is a term of the same order as the wordsin the newspeak vocabulary George Orwell used inhis dismaying world of 1984. In that context,.groupthink takes on an invidious connotation.Exactly such a connotation is intended, since theterm refers to a deterioration in mental efficiency,reality testing and moral judgments as a result ofgroup pressures.

The symptoms of groupthink arise when the mem­bers of decision-making groups become motivated toavoid being too harsh in their judgments of theirleaders' or their colleagues' ideas. They adopt a softline of criticism, even in their O\VIl thinking. At theirmeetings, all the members are amiable and seek com­plete concurrence on every important issue, with nobickering or conflict to spoil the cozy, "we-feeling"atmosphere,

Kill. Paradoxically, soft-headed groups are oftenhard-hearted when it comes to dealing with outgroupsor enemies. They find it relatively easy to resort to de­humanizlng solutions-they will readily authorizebombing attacks that kill large numbers of civiliansi~

REPRINTED FROM PSYCHOLOGY TODAY MAGAZlrreCopyright © 1971 by Ziff-Davis Publishing Company

Page 2: Janis Groupthink

the name of the noble cause of persuading an un­friendly government to negotiate at the peace table.They are unlikely to pursue the more difficultand con­troversial issues that arise when alternatives to a harshmilitary solution come up for discussion. Nor are theyinclined to raise ethical issues that carry the implica­tion that this fine group of ours, Ivith its human­itarianism and its high-minded principles, ~night becapable of adopting a course of action that is in­humane and immoral.

Norms. There is evidence from a number of social­psychological studies that as the members of a groupfeel D10re accepted by the others, which is a centralfeature of increased group cohesiveness, they displayless overt conformity to group norms. Thus we wouldexpect that the more cohesive a group becomes, theless the members will feel constrained to censor whatthey say out of fear of being socially punished for an­tagonizing the leader or anyof their fellow members,

In contrast, the groupthink type of conformity tendsto increase as group cohesiveness increases. Group­think involves nondeliberate suppression of criticalthoughts as a result of internalization of the group'snorms, which is quite different from deliberate sup- .pression on the basis of external threats of social pun­ishment. The more cohesive the group, the greater theinner compulsion on the pa.rtof each member to avoidcreating disunity, which inclines him to believe in thesoundnessof whatever proposals are promoted by theleader or by a majority of the group's members.

In a cohesive group, thedanger is not so much thateach individual will fail toreveal his objections towhat the others proposebut that he will think theproposal is a good one, without attempting to carryout a careful, critical scrutiny of the pros and cons ofthe alternatives. When groupthink becomes domi­nant, there also is considerable suppression of deviantthoughts, but it takes the form o( each person's decid­ing that his misgivings are not relevant and should beset aside, that the benefit of the doubt regarding anylingering uncertainties should be given to the groupconsensus.

Stress. I do not mean to, imply that ail cohesivegroups necessarily suffer from groupthink. All in­groups may have a mild tendency toward groupthink,displaying one or another of the symptoms from timeto time, but it need not be so dominant as to influencethe quality of the group's final decision. Neither do Imean to imply that there is anything necessarily in­efficient or harmful about group decisions in general.On the contrary, a group whose members have prop­erly defined roles, with traditions concerning theprocedures to follow in pursuing a critical inquiry,probably is capable of making better decisions than

85

any individual group member workingalone. .'~,

The problem is that the advantages of having decfsions made by groups are often lost because of PO\v­erful psychological pressures that arise when themembers work closely together, share the same set ofvalues and, above all, face a crisis situation that putseveryone under intense stress.

The main principle of groupthink, which I offer inthe spirit of Parkinson's Law, is this: The moreamiability and esprit de corps there is alnong themembers of a policy-nlaking ingroup, the greater thedanger that independent critical thinking tviII be re...placed by groupthink; which is likely to result in ir...rational and dehumanizing actions directed againstoutgroups.

Symptoms. In my studies of high..level govern­mental decision-makers, both civilian and military, Ihave foundeight mainsymptoms ofgroupthink.

1 INVULSERA.BILITY. ~'Iost or all of the members ofthe ingroup share an illusion of invulnerability that

provides for them some degree of reassurance aboutobvious dangers and leads them to become over­optimistic and willing to take extracrdtnary risks. Italsocauses them to fail to respond to clear warnings ofdanger.' .

The Kennedy ingroup, which uncritically accepted-«,the Central Intelligence Agency's disastrous Bay cPigs plan, operated on the false assumption that theycould keep secret the fact that the United States wasresponsible for the invasion of Cuba. Even after newsof the plan began to leak out, their belief remained un­shaken. They failed even to consider the danger that

awaited them: a worldwiderevulsion against the U.S.

A similar attitude ap­peared among the membersof President Lyndon B.Johnson's ingroup, the

"Tuesday Cabinet," which kept escalating the Viet­nam War despite repeated setbacks and failures."There was a belief," Bill Moyers commented afterhe resigned, "that if we indicated a willingness to useour power, they [the North Vietnamese] would getthe message and back away from an all-out confronta­tion.... There was a confidence-it was never braggedabout, it was just there-that when the chips werereally down, the other people would fold."

A most poignant example of an illusion of in­vulnerability involves the ingroup around Admiral H.E. Kimmel, which failed to prepare for the possibilityof a Japanese attack on Pearl Harbor despite repeated-«,warnings. Informed by hisintelligence chief that radcontact with Japanese aircraft carriers had been lost,Kimmel joked about it: "What, you don't knowwherethe carriers are? Do you mean to say that they couldbe rounding Diamond Head (at Honolulu) and youwouldn't know it?U The carriers were in fact moving

Page 3: Janis Groupthink

86

full-steam toward Kimmel's command post at thetime. Laughing together about a danger signal, whichlabels it as a purely laughing matter, is a characteristicmanifestation of groupthink.

2 RATIO~ALE. As ,ve see, victims of groupthinkignore warnings; they also collectively construct

rationalizations in order to discount warninzs andother forms of negative feedback that, taken seriously,might lead the group members to reconsider their as­sumptions each time they recommit themselves topast decisions. Why did the johnson ingroup avoidreconsidering its escalation policy when time andagain the expectations on which they based their deci­sions turned out to be wrong? James C. Thompson Jr.,a Harvard historian who spent five years as an observ­ing participant in both the State Department and theWhite House, tells us that the policymakers avoidedcritical discussion of their prior decisions and contin­uallv invented new rationalizations so that thev couldsincerely recommit themselves to defeating tb~ NorthVietnamese.

In the .fall of 1964, before the bombing of NorthVietnam began, some of the policymakers predictedthat six weeks of air strikes would induce the NorthVietnamese to seek peace talks. When someone asked,"What if they don't?" the answer was that anotherfour weeks certainly would do the trick.

Later, after each setback, the ingroup agreed thatby investing just a bit more effort (bystepping up thebomb tonnage a bit, for instance), their course of ac­tion would prove to be right. The Pentagon Papersbear out these observations.

In The Limits of Interoention, Townsend Hoopes,who VIas acting Secretary of the ...ur Force under John­son, says that Walt \V.Bestow in particularshowed a remarkable ca-pacity for what has beencalled "instant rationali-zation." According toHoopes, Rostow buttressed the group's optimismabout being on the road to victory by culling selectedscraps of evidence from news reports or, if necessary,by inventing "plausible" forecasts that had no basisin evidence at all.

Admiral Kimmel's group rationalized a\~lay theirwarnings, too. Right up to December 7, 1941, theyconvinced themselves that the Japanese would neverdare attempt a full-scale surprise assault againstHawaii because Japan's leaders would realize that itwould precipitate an all-out war which the UnitedStates would surely win. They made no attempt tolook at the situation through the eyes of the Japaneseleaders-another manifestation of groupthink.

3 l\1oRALITY. Victims of groupthink believe un­questioningly in the inherent morality of their in­

group; this belief inclines the members to ignore

the ethical or moral consequences oftheir decisions.Evidence that this symptom is at workusually is ofa ~

negative kind-the things that are left unsaid in groupmeetings. At least two influential persons had doubtsabout the morality of the Bay of Pigs adventure. Oneof them, Arthur Schlesinger Jr., presented his strongobjections in a memorandum to President Kennedyand Secretary ofState Ruskbut suppressed them whenhe attended meetings of the Kennedyteam. The other,Senator J. William Fulbright, was not a member of thegroup, but the President invited him to express hismisgivings in a speech to the policymakers. However,when Fulbright finished speaking the Presidentmoved on to other agenda items without asking for re-

.actions of the group.David Kraslow and Stuart H. Loory, in The Secret

Search for Peace in Vietnam, report that during 1966President Johnson's ingroup was concerned primarilywith selecting bomb targets in North Vietnam. Theybased their selections on four factors-the military ad­vantage, the risk to American aircraft and pilots, thedanger of forcing other countries into the fighting, andthe danger of heavy civilian casualties. At their regu-lar Tuesday luncheons, they weighed these factors theway school teachers grade examination papers, aver­aging them out. Though evidence on this point isscant, I suspect that the group's ritualistic adherence J~.

to a standardized procedure induced the members tofeel morally justified in their destructive \vay of deal-ing with the Vietnamese people-after all, the dangerof heavy civilian casualties from U.S. air strikes wastaken into account on their checklists.

4 STEREonPES. Victims of groupthink hold stereo­typed views of the leaders of enemy groups: they

are so evil .that genuine attempts at negotiatingdifferences with them are unwarranted, or they are

too weak or too stupid todeal effectively with what­ever attempts the ingroupmakes to defeat their pur­poses, no matter how riskythe attempts are.

Kennedy'sgroupthinkers believed thatPremier FidelCastro's air"force was so ineffectual that obsoleteB-26s could knock it out completely in a surprise at­tack before the invasion began. They also believedthat Castro's army was so weak that a small Cuban­exile brigade could establish a well-protected beach­head at the Bayof Pigs. In addition, they believed thatCastro wasnot smart enough to put down any possibleinternal uprisings in support of the exiles. They werewrong on all three assumptions. Though much of tbe··"---'­blame was attributable to faulty intelligence, the.point is that none of Kennedy's advisers even ques­tioned the CIA planners about these assumptions.

The Johnson advisers' sloganistic thinking about..the Communist apparatus" that was "working all

Page 4: Janis Groupthink

around the world" (as Dean Rusk put it) led them tooverlook the powerful nationalistic strivings of theNorth Vietnamese government and its efforts to wardoff Chinese domination. The crudest of all stereotypesused by Johnson's inner circle to justify their policieswas the domino theory ('

1I1£ we don't stop the Reds inSouth Vietnam, tomorrow they will be in Hawaii andnext week they will be in San Francisco," Johnsononce said). The group so firmly accepted this ster­eotype that it became almost impossible for any ad­viser to introduce a more sophisticated viewpoint.

In the documents on Pearl Harbor, it is clear to seethat the Navy commanders stationed in Hawaii had anaive image of Japan as a midget that would not dare

tostrike a blowagainst a powerful giant.

5 P!lESSCRE. Victims of groupthinkapply direct pressure to any indi..

vidual who momentarily expressesdoubts about any of the group's sharedIllusions or who questions the validityof the arguments supporting a policyalternative favored by the majority.This gambit reinforces the con­currence-seeking norm that loyalmem­bers are expected to maintain.

President . Kennedy probably wasmore active than anyone else in raisingskeptical questions during the Bay ofPigs meetings, and yet he seems to haveencouraged the group's docile, uncriti­cal acceptance of defective argumentsin favor of the CIA's plan. At everymeeting, he allowed the CIA represen­tatives to dominate the discussion. Hepermitted them to give their immedi­ate refutations in response to each ten­tative doubt that one of the othersexpressed, instead of asking whetheranyone shared the doubt or wanted topursue the implications of the newworrisome issue that had just beenraised...And at the most crucial meeting.when he wascalling on each member togive his vote for or against the plan, hedid not call on Arthur Schlesinger, theone man there who was known by thePresident to have serious misgiVings.

Historian Thomson informs us thatwhenever a member of johnson's in­group began to express doubts, thegroup used subtle social pressures to"domesticate" hitn. To start with, thedissenter was made to feel at home,provided that he lived up to two re­strictions: 1) that he did not voice hisdoubts to outsiders, which would playinto the hands of the opposition; and 2)that he kept his criticisms within thebounds of acceptable deviation, whichmeant not challenging any of the fun­damental assumptions that went into

the group's prior commitments. Onesuch "domesticated dissenter" was BillMoyers. When Moyers arrived at ameeting, Thomson tens us, the Presi­dent greeted him with,. "Well, herecomes Mr. Stop-the-Bombing."

6 SELF-CENSORSHIP. Victims ofgrbupthink avoid deviating from

what appears to be group consensus;they keep silent about their misgivingsand even minimize to themselves theimportance of their doubts.

Aswe have seen, Schlesingerwas notat all hesitant about presenting hisstrong objections to the Bay of Pigsplan in a memorandum to the Presidentand the Secretary of State. But he be...came keenly aware of his tendency tosuppress objections at the \Vhite Housemeetings. "In the months after the Bayof PigsI bitterly reproached myselfforhaving kept so silent during those eru..cial discussions in the cabinet room,"Schlesinger writes in A ThousandDays. "I can only explain Iny failure todo more than raise a few timid ques­tions by reporting that one's impulse toblow the whistle on this nonsense wassimplyundone by the circumstances ofthe discussion."

7 UNANI~fJTY. Victims of groupthinkshare an illusion of unanimity

within the group concerning almost alljudgments expressed by members whospeak in favor of the majority view.This symptom results partly. from thepreceding one, whose effects are aug­mented by the false assumption thatanyindividualwho remains silent dur­ing any part of the discussion is in fullaccord with what the others are saying.

When a group of persons who re­spect each other's opinions arrives at aunanimous view, each member is likelyto feel that the belief mustbe true. Thisreliance on consensual validationwithin the group tends to replace indi­vidual critical thinking and reality test­ing, unless there are clear-cutdisagreements among the members. Incontemplating a course of action suchtIS the invasionof Cuba, it is painful forthe membersto confront disagreementswithin their group, particularly if it be­comes apparent that there are widelydivergent views about whether the pre­ferred course of action is too risky toundertake at all. Such disagreementsare likely to arouse anxieties ·aboutmaldng a serious error. Once the senseof unanimity is shattered, the membersno longer can feel complacently con-

87

,--......

Page 5: Janis Groupthink

8a

fident about the decision they are in..clined to make. Each man must thenface the annoying realization that thereare troublesome uncertainties and hemust diligently seek out the best infer..mation he can get in order to decide forhimself exactly how serious the risksmight be. This is one of the unpleasantconsequences of being in a group ofhardheaded, critical thinkers.

To avoid such an unpleasant state,the members often become inclined,without quite realizing it, to prevent la..tent disagreements from surfacingwhen they are about to initiate a risk)'course of action. The group leader andthe members support each other inplaying up the areas of convergence intheir thinking, at the expense of fullyexploring divergencies that might re­veal unsettled issues"

1C0ur meetings took place in acurious atmosphere of assumed con­sensus," Schlesinger writes. His addi­tional comments clearly show that,curiously, the consensus was an illu­sion-an illusion that could be main­tained only because the majorparticipants did not reveal their ownreasoning or discuss their idiosyncraticassumptions and vague reservations.Evidence from several sources makes itclear that even the three principals­President Kennedy. Rusk andMcNamara-had widely differing as..sumptions about the invasion plan.

8 MJNDCUARDS. Victims of group­think sometimes appoint them­

selves as mindguards to protect theleader and fellow members from ad­verse information that might break'the complacency they shared about theeffectiveness and morality of pastdecisions. At a large birthday party forhis wife, Attorney General Robert F.Kennedy, who had been constantly in­formed about the Cuban invasion plan,took Schlesinger aside and asked himwhy he was opposed. Kennedy listenedcoldly and said, "You may be right oryou may be wrong, but the Presidenthasmadehismind up. Don't pushit anyfurther. Now is the time for everyoneto help him all they can,"

Rusk also functioned as a highly ef­fective mindguard by failing to trans­mit to the group the strong objectionsof three "outsiders" who had learned ofthe invasion plan-Undersecretary ofState Chester Bowles, USIA DirectorEdward R. Murrow, and Rusk's in­telligence chief, Roger HUsman. Had

Rusk done so, their warnings mighthave reinforced Schlesinger's memo­randum and jolted some of Kennedy'singroup, if not the President himself,into reconsidering the decision.

.Products. When a group of execu­tives frequently displays most or all ofthese interrelated symptoms, a detailedstudy of their deliberations is likely toreveal a number of immediate con­sequences. These consequences are, ineHect, products of poor decision-mak­ing practices because they lead to in­adequate solutions to the problemsunder discussion.

FIRST, the group limits its discussionsto a few alternative courses of action(often only two) without an initial sur­vey of all the alternatives that might beworthy of consideration.

SECOND, the group fails to reexaminethe course of action initially preferredby the majority after they learn of risksand drawbacks they had not consideredoriginally. ,;

THIRD, the members spend little orno time discussing whether there arenonobvious gains they may have over­looked or ways of reducing the seem­ingly prohibi tive costs that maderejected alternatives appear unde..sirable to them.

FOt.,~TH, members make little or noattempt to obtain information from ex­perts within their own organizationswho might beable to supply more pre­cise estimates of potential losses andgains.

FIFTH. members show positive in­terest in facts and opinions that supporttheir preferred policy; they tend to ig­nore facts and opinions that do not.

SIXTH, members spend little time de­liberating about how the chosen policymight be hindered by bureaucratic in­ertia, sabotaged by political opponents,or temporarily derailed by common ac­cidents. Consequently, they fail towork out contingency plans to copewith foreseeable setbacks that couldendanger the overall success of theirchosencourse.

Support. The search for an explana-. tion of why groupthink occurs has ledme through a quagmire of complicatedtheoretical issues in the murky area ofhuman motivation. My belief, based onrecent social psychological research, isthat we can best understand the varioussymptomsof groupthink as a mutual ef­fort among the group members tomaintain self-esteem and emotional

equanimitybyprovidingsocial supportto each other, especially at times\yhe~they share responsibility for making vtal decisions.

Even when no important decision ispending, the typical administrator willbegin to doubt the wisdom and moral­ity of his past decisions each time he re-

-ceives information about setbacks,particularly if the information is ac­companied by negative feedback fromprominent men who originally hadbeen his supporters. It should not besurprising, therefore, to find that indi­vidual members strive to develop unan­imity and espri t de corps that will helpbolster each other's morale, to createan optimistic outlook about the successof pending decisions, and to reaffirmthe positive value of past policies towhich all of them are committed.

Pride. Shared Illusions of invulner­ability, for example, can reduce anxietyabout taking risks. Rationalizationshelp members believe that the risks arereally not so bad after all. The assump­tion of inherent morality helps themembers to avoid feelings of shame orguilt. Negative stereotypes function asstress-reducing devices to enhance,~.sense of moral righteousness as wellpride in a lofty mission.

The mutual enhancement of self-es­teem and morale mav have functionalvalue in enabling the members to main...tain their capacity to take action, but ithas maladaptive consequences insofaras concurrence-seeking tendencies in­terfere with critical, rational capacitiesand lead to serious errors of judgment.

While I have limited Inystudy to de­cision-making bodies in Government,groupthink symptoms appear in busi­ness, industry and any other field wheresmall, cohesive groups make the deci­sions. It is vital, then, for all sorts of"people-and especially group leaders­to know what steps they can take toprevent groupthink.

Remedies. To counterpoint my casestudies of the major fiascos, I have alsoinvestigated two highly successfulgroup enterprises, the formulation ofthe Marshall Plan in the Truman Ad­ministration and the handling of theCuban mtssile crisis by Presiaent Ken­nedy and his advisers. I have fo~r'-­

instructive to examine the steps Knedy took to change his group's deci­sion-meking processes. Tllese changesensured that the mistakes made by hisBay of Pigs ingroup were not repeated

Page 6: Janis Groupthink

· .

by the missile-crisis ingroup, eventhough the membership of both groupswas essentially the same.

The following recommendations forpreventing groupthink incorporatenlany of the good practices I discov­ered to be characterlstic of the ~1aT­

shall Plan and missile-crisis group~:

1 The leader ofa policy-forminggroupshould assign the role of critical eval­uator to each member, encouraging thegroup to give high priority to open air­ing of objections and doubts. This prac­tice needs to be reinforced by theleader's acceptance of criticism of hisown judgments in order to discouragemembers from soft-pedaling their dis­agreements and from allowing theirstriving for concurrence to inhibit criti-'cal thinking.2 When the key members of a hier­archy assign a policy-planning missionto any group within their organization,they should adopt an impartial stanceinstead of stating preferences and ex­pectations at the beginning. This willencourage open inquiry and impar..tial probing of a wide range of policyaltematiYes.3 The organization routinely shouldset up several outside policy-planningand evaluation groups to work on thesame policy question, each deliberatingunder a different leader. This can pre­vent the insulation of an ingroup.4 .\t intervals before the group reachesa final consensus, the leader should re­quire each member to discuss thegroup's deliberations with associates in

89

his own unit of the organization-as- entire issue before making a definitivesurning that those associates can be choice. -~

trusted to adhere to the same security How. These recommendations havregulations that govem the policy- their disadvantages. To encourage themakers-sand then to report back their open airing of objections, for instance,reactions to the group. might lead to prolonged and costly de­S The group should invite one or more bates when a rapidly gro\ving crisis re­outside experts to each meeting on a quires iInmecliate solution. It alsocouldstaggered basis and encourage the ex- cause rejection, depression and anger.perts to challenge the views of the core A leader's failure to set a norm Jl1ightmembers, create cleavage between leader and6 At every general meeting of the members that could develop into a dis­group, whenever the agenda calls for ruptive power struggle if the leaderan evaluation of policy alternatives, at looks on the emerging consensus as ana..least one member should play devil's thema. Setting up outside evaluationadvocate, functioning as a good lawyer groups might increase the risk of secu..in challenging the testimony of those rity leakage. Still, inventive executiveswho advocate the majority position. who know their way around the organi­7 Whenever the policy issue involves zational maze probably can figure outrelations with a rival nation or organi- how to apply one or another of the pre..zation, the group should devote a siz- scriptions successfully, without harm­able block of time, perhaps an entire ful sideeffects.session, to a survey of all warning sig- They also could benefit from the ad..nals from the rivals and should write vice of outside experts in the adminis­alternative scenarios on the rivals' trative and behavioral sciences.intentions. .. . Though these experts have much to of-a When the group is surveying policy fer, they have had few chances to workalternatives for feasibility and effec- on poticy-making machinery withintiveness, it should from time to time di- large organizations. As matters n~.vide into two or more subgroups to stand, executives innovate only wlmeet separately. under different chair- they need new procedures to avoid re-men, and then come back together to peating serious errors that have de-hammer out differences. ftated their self-Images,9 After reaching a preliminary con- In this era of atomic warheads, urbansensus about what seems to be the best disorganization and ecocatastrophes, itpolicy, the group should hold a "sec- seems to me that policymakers shouldend-chance" meeting at which every collaborate with behavioral scientistsmember expresses as viviclly as he can and give top priority to preventingall his residual doubts, and rethinks the groupthink and its attendant 6ascos.

Page 7: Janis Groupthink

Summary

This article described three heuristicsthat are employed in making judamentsunder uncertainty: (i) representativeness,which is usually employed when peo­ple are asked to judge the probabilitythat an object or event A belongs toclass or process B; (ii) availability of in­stances or scenarios, which is often em­played when people are asked to assessthe frequency of a class or the plausibU­ity of a particular development; and(illl adjustment from an anchor, whichis usually employed in numerical predic­tion when a relevant value is available.These heuristics are highly economical

and usually effective, but they lead tosystematic and predictable errors. Abetter understaJ1dinl of these heuristicsaad of the biases to which they leadcould improve judgments and decisionsin situations of uncertainty.

Refereaces IDCI NOI"

t. D. KahDeman and It. Tversky, PsYchoL RIV.10, 237 (1973).

2. -, COf'llitiv. '"cnol. 3, 430 (972).3. W. Edwards, ill Fomuzl R.lrMu1IUUion of

RIUJUIJI lu.df'1'llelll,. B. Kleinmuntz.. Ed. (Wney,New York. 1961). pp. 17-52-

•• P. Slovic and S. Lidlcaa.st.eift, Of'ftUL B,iuJv.BIAlf&. PrrjamIlJlII:. " 649 (1971).

,. A. Tvenky aDd D. lCahaemlD, Pr'/~hol. Bull.". 105 (1971).

6. -. Cornitiv. Pryr:hol. 5. :D7 (19;3'.1. It C. Oalbraidl and B. 1. Underwood..

.,,~'".. Co,"itlo" 1, 56 (1973).

8. L. 1. Qa~an aDd 1. P. Ch:&~man. J.Abno"". Prycftol. ~. 193 (l961); ibid.., ~.at

Z11 (1969t9. M. Bar-HiUcL Orr41l.. S.h"v. Hum.. p,r­

fomtlDlc, 9. 396 (lSi]).10. J. Coheu. :£. L Cesa.icL D. H~t3Jl, Sr. J~

Prycho/. 63. .41 (1972).11. ~f. Alpert :lJ1d H. .R.a.ma. unpubli5hcd man\.

SCnpt: C. A. S. von Holstein. ACt.2 PS}·~ir.QI.

31. 478 (1911l; R. L. Wu:lQer, J. Am. Stal.A.ssoc. 6%. ':"'16 (1967).

12. L. J. Sav&:lc, Til. FouncWrtons of SUUini($('Niley, Nc'''' YOrk, 19'4).

13. B. De fm.~ai. in IrrtnruuiolUJl EIlC)'clofHdlaof Ih. SociAu Sci'"c,~. D. E. Sills. Ed. (~b.c·mil1aD. New' Yotk, 1961), yolo 11. PI'. 01.96-'04.

14. 'I'bis re.sca.n:h was supported by the Adv3nc:eaJleseard1 Projects AllucY of the Oep:1ttme::tof Defense md. wu monitored by the Om;:of Naval Research under contract 1"0100014­73.c.04J8 to the Ore.oD R.ese:U'~n lnsuture,Euccne. Aclditional sU~l'Clrt ~or this rese:u';~

was provu:lc:ci by the Re-se:lrc~ .tnd Develop­ment AutQioritY of the Heorew Universi.y.1erusalem, :tsr1eL