information to users colored or poor quality illustrations

216
INFORMATION TO USERS The most advanced technology has been used to photo- graph and reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer. The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are re- produced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. These are also available as one exposure on a standard 35mm slide or as a 17" x 23" black and white photographic print for an additional charge. Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order. UMI University Microfilms International A Bell & Howell Information Company 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA 313/761-4700 800/521-0600

Upload: others

Post on 28-Feb-2022

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

INFORMATION TO USERS

The most advanced technology has been used to photo­graph and reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm master. UMI films the text directly from the original or copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type of computer printer.

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are re­produced by sectioning the original, beginning at the upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced form at the back of the book. These are also available as one exposure on a standard 35mm slide or as a 17" x 23" black and white photographic print for an additional charge.

Photographs included in the original manuscript have been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are available for any photographs or illustrations appearing in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly to order.

UMI University Microfilms International

A Bell & Howell Information Company 300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA

313/761-4700 800/521-0600

Page 2: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations
Page 3: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Order Number 8924824

Effect of rearing environment and environmental enrichment on behavior and neural development in young pigs

Grandin, Temple, Ph.D.

University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 1989

UMI 300N.ZeebRd. Ann Arbor, MI 48106

Page 4: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations
Page 5: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

EFFECT OF REARING ENVIRONMENT AND ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT ON BEHAVIOR AND NEURAL DEVELOPMENT IN YOUNG PIGS

BY

TEMPLE GRANDIN

B.A., Franklin Pierce College, 1970 M.S., Arizona State University, 1975

THESIS

Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Animal Science

in the Graduate College of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 1989

Urbana, Illinois

Page 6: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

U N I V E R S I T Y O F I L L I N O I S A T U R B A N A - C H A M P A I G N

T H E G R A D U A T E C O L L E G E

DECEMBER 1988

W E H E R E B Y R E C O M M E N D T H A T T H E T H E S I S BY

TEMPLE GRANDIN

F.NTTTT.FT) EFFECT OF REARING ENVIRONMENT AND ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT ON

BEHAVIOR AND NEURAL DEVELOPMENT IN YOUNG PIGS

B E A C C E P T E D IN P A R T I A L F U L F I L L M E N T O F T H E R E Q U I R E M E N T S F O R

T H E D E G R E E O F DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

W&^ Director of Thesis Research

Head of Department

Committee on Final Examinat ion!

Chairperson /7^<. _ _ _^

• Required for doctor's degree but not for master's

Page 7: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

As one gets to the end of the long road to a Ph.D., one

cannot help but reflect back on the events and people

instrumental in shaping the course of one's graduate program.

I am indebted to my major advisor, Dr. Stanley E. Curtis,

and my co-advisor, Dr. William T. Greenough. I will always be

grateful to them for allowing me to pursue the course of study

which led to this thesis. I also wish to thank Drs. Lynn A.

Barnett-Morriss, Tom R. Carr, the late Edwin M. Banks, and

Harold W. Gonyou for serving on my graduate study committee. I

extend special appreciation to Mr. Dale DeMotte; without his

assistance in developing the brain perfusion method, the work

on dendritic growth in the pig's brain would not have been

possible.

I am also indebted to Mr. Ian Taylor for assistance in

running my experiments and to Ms. Beth Palen for assistance

with neuron drawing and preparation of sections. Dr. Janice

Juraska provided invaluable assistance on developing a neuron

sorting procedure. I also gratefully acknowledge those who

unselfishly shared their statistical expertise: Mr. Robert

Hurst, Dr. Tina Widowski, and Mr. Dan Waldron. Others who

helped make this project possible include: Ms. Diane Bell, Dr.

James M. McFarlane, Mr. Michael Bilta, Ms. Rhonda Feinmehl, Mr.

Eric Fugate, Ms. Kathryn Russell, Ms. Mary Ellen Rowland, and

Mr.Kevin Walter. I also wish to thank the staffs at the Swine

Research Center and the Meats Laboratory.

m

Page 8: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

TABLE OF CONTENTS

CHAPTER Page

I. INTRODUCTION.' 1

II. LITERATURE REVIEW 2

A. Effects of Environmental Restriction and Complexity : 2

Behavioral effects of environmental restriction 2

Central nervous system effects of environmental restriction 4

Concept of optimal stimulation 7 Structural changes in the brain 8 Farm animals 10

B. Environmental Enrichment Methods 11 Choice of environmental enrichment 11 Play in farm animals 13 Activity patterns in livestock 14 Aggression and environmental enrichment. 15

C. Thesis Research Outline 16 Dendritic growth in the pig's cerebral cortex 16

Excitability and handling of the pig 16 Object play by the pig 17

III. PERFUSION METHOD FOR PREPARING PIG BRAIN CORTEX FOR GOLGI-COX IMPREGNATION 18

A. Introduction 18 B. Procedure 18 C. Results 22 D. Discussion 23

IV. DENDRITIC GROWTH IN SOMATOSENSORY REGION OF BRAIN CORTEX IN PIGS RESIDING IN A SIMPLE OR A COMPLEX ENVIRONMENT 30

A. Introduction 30 B. Experimental Procedure 32

Animals and treatments 32 Brain preparation 34 Dendritic growth determination 34 Pig behavior 37 Statistical analysis 39

C. Results 42 D. Discussion 46

iv

Page 9: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT REDUCES EXCITABILITY IN PIGS 94

A. Introduction 94 B. Experimental Procedure 95

Experiment 1 95 Experiment II 96

C. Results 98 Experiment 1 98 Experiment II 99

0. Discussion 99

ENVIRONMENTAL EFFECTS ON EASE OF HANDLING OF PIGS. 108

A. Introduction 108 B. Experimental Procedure 108

Animals and housing 108 Treatments 109 Test apparatus 110 Test procedure 111

C. Results 113 0. Discussion 114

OBJECT INTERACTION IN WEANLING PIGS 123

A. Introduction 123 B. Experimental Procedure 125

Experiment I—short-term object preference: type 125

Experiment II—cloth object preference: texture 126

Experiment III—daily object interactions 126

Experiment IV—object interaction behavior 127

Experiment V—long-term object preference 128

Experiment VI—objects and fighting 128 C. Results 129

Experiment I—short-term object preference: type 129

Experiment II—cloth object preference: texture 130

Experiment III—daily object interactions 131

Experiment IV—relationship of object interaction and other behaviors 131

Experiment V—long-term object preference 132

Experiment VI—objects and fighting 132 0. Discussion 133

v

Page 10: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

VIII. SUMMARY OF RESULTS AND CONCLUSIONS AND PROPOSED FUTURE RESEARCH 172

A. Summary of Results and Conclusions 172 B. General Conclusions 176 C. Future Research 180

LITERATURE CITED 181

VITA 195

vi

Page 11: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

LIST OF TABLES

Page

Total ring intersections (TRI) and soma and TRI x soma scores in pig somatosensory cortex (trials combined) 60

Total ring intersections (TRI) and soma and TRI x soma scores in pig somatosensory cortex (trials separated) 61

Total ring intersections (TRI) and soma and TRI x soma scores in pig visual cortex (trials combined) 62

Total ring intersections (TRI) and soma and TRI x soma scores in pig visual cortex (trials separated) 63

Gender effect on total ring intersections (TRI) and soma and TRI x soma scores in pig somatosensory cortex 64

Gender effect on total ring intersections (TRI) and soma and TRI x soma scores in pig visual cortex (trials combined) 65

Gender effect on total ring intersections (TRI) and soma and TRI x soma scores in pig somatosensory cortex (trials separated) 66

Gender effect on total ring intersections (TRI) and soma and TRI x soma scores in pig visual cortex (trials separated) 67

Excitability ratings for pigs housed in enriched and nonenriched environments (Experiment I) 103

Excitability ratings for pigs reared under different environmental enrichment regimens (Experiment II, Trials 1 and 2) 104

Excitability ratings for pigs reared under different environmental enrichment regimens (Experiment II, Trial 3) 105

Weight gain of pigs reared under different environmental enrichment regimens (Experiment II, Trials 1 and 2) 106

vii

Page 12: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

TABLE

13. Weight gains of pigs reared under different environmental enrichment procedures in Experiment II, Trial 3 107

14. Number of pigs that were electric-prodded any time during the test 117

15. Comparison of pigs that were electric-prodded any time during the test 118

16. Number of pigs which required repeated electric-prodding during the test (crowd pen and chute combined) 119

17. Number of pigs which entered the chute voluntarily 120

18. Frequencies and durations of touching and biting of objects (Experiment I) 139

19. Frequencies and durations of touching and biting of cloth objects (Experiment II) 140

20. Midpoints of periods of greatest object pulling (Experiment III) 141

viii

Page 13: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure .Page

1. Schematic drawing of system used to perfuse pig brains with fixative solution 24

2. Typical Golgi-Cox-impregnated nerve cells in pig somatosensory cortex 26

3. Typical Golgi-Cox-impregnated nerve cells in pig somatosensory cortex 28

4. Location of tissue blocks in the somatosensory and visual cortex of the pig 68

5. Location of sampled neurons in the somatosensory cortex of the pig 70

6. Location of sampled neurons in the visual cortex of the pig 72

7. Layout of apparatus for testing pig behavior in Trial 1 74

8. Layout of apparatus for testing pig behavior in Trial 2 76

9. Pig pyramidal neuron that complies with the sorting criteria 78

10. Pig pyramidal neuron that complies with the sorting criteria 80

11. Pig pyramidal neuron that has a basilar tree less than 170 micrometers wide 82

12. Pig pyramidal neuron that has an asymmetrical basilar tree 84

13. Least Squares means for unsorted pyramidal neurons in the somatosensory cortex of the pig 86

14. Least Squares means for sorted pyramidal neurons in the somatosensory cortex of the pig 88

15. Least Squares means for unsorted pyramidal neurons in the visual cortex of the pig 90

16. Least Squares means for sorted pyramidal neurons in the visual cortex of the pig 92

ix

Page 14: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure

17. Layout of apparatus for testing pig handling behavior 121

18. Activity, play, and fighting patterns on the first day after regrouping (Trial 1)., 142

19. Activity, play, and fighting patterns on the sixth day after regrouping (Trial 1) 144

20. Activity, play, and fighting pattern on the first day after regrouping (Trial 2) 146

21. Activity, play, and fighting patterns on the

sixth day after regrouping (Trial 2) 148

22. Long-term object preference of pigs 150

23. Long-term object preference (Trial 1) 152

24. Long-term object preference of pigs (Trial 2) 154

25. Regression of the differences between cloth and hose pulling 156

26. Effect of cloth objects on fighting in newly regrouped pigs 158

27. Effect of cloth objects during regrouping on the number of 5-min time segments with fighting 160

28. Effect of cloth objects prior to regrouping on the number of 5-min time segments with fighting... 162

29. Effect of previous experience with cloth objects on "play" behavior in newly regrouped pigs 164

30. Effect of cloth objects prior to regrouping on the number of 5-min time segments with object interaction 166

31. Effect of previous experience with cloth objects on "play" behavior in newly regrouped pigs (5-min segments with both fighting and object interaction subtracted) 168

32. Effect of cloth objects prior to regrouping on the number of 5-min time segments with "play" (5-min time segments with both fighting and object interaction subtracted) 170

x

Page 15: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

I. INTRODUCTION

There has been increasing societal concern about the welfare

of farm animals residing in certain modern systems which provide

less varied sensory input than natural surroundings (Harrison,

1964; Singer, 1976; Mason and Singer, 1980; Fox, 1984).

Environmental complexity affects both central nervous system

anatomical development and behavior (Bennett et al., 1964;

Diamond et al., 1964; Greenough and Chang, 1985). Barren

environments which restrict sensory input in terms of quantity,

quality, or variety tend to increase both behavioral and

central nervous system excitability. One question is: Do farm

animals residing in these systems have symptoms of sensory

restriction? Another is: Will simple, inexpensive methods of

environmental enrichment such as objects or extra contact with

people have beneficial effects on the animals' well-being? One

aim of the research reported in this thesis was to answer these

and other questions as they pertain to the pig.

A second aim was to determine if environmental enrichment

would have beneficial effects on productivity and handling

behavior. Animals which move easily during handling at the meat

packing plant will be less likely to get bruised or have stress

induced meat quality problems.

A third aim was to quantify the pig's interactions with

environmental enrichment objects and determine if environmental

enrichment would reduce fighting in newly mixed pigs.

1

Page 16: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

II. LITERATURE REVIEW

A. Effects of Environmental Restriction and Complexity

Behavioral effects of environmental restriction

This will be a review of the scientific literature on the

effect of either isolation or sensory restriction on animals

after weaning. In all cases, prior to weaning, the animals were

reared by their natural mothers. Therefore, any effect of

sensory restriction was not confounded with the effects of

maternal deprivation, which profoundly alters later behavior

(Harlow and Zimmerman, 1959; Mason, 1960).

Isolated postweaning rearing conditions will increase the

reactivity and excitability of rodents (Korn and Moyer, 1968;

Valzelli, 1973; Riittenen et al., 1986). In the Korn and Moyer

(1968) experiments on adult rats, four weeks of isolation in

standard laboratory cages was less detrimental in these respects

than fourteen weeks. Isolation heightened emotionality; isolated

rats reacted violently when they were picked up.

Hyperexcitability has been documented in dogs kept in a

sensory-restricted environment. Pairs of puppies residing in

barren kennels become unusually aroused and excited when exposed

to something new (Melzack, 1954). These puppies could hear and

smell dogs in adjacent kennels, and the kennels were illuminated

continuously so the puppies in a pair could see and interact with

each other. Almost a year after being returned to a residential

environment with a human family, the subjects were still

2

Page 17: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

hyperexcitable (Melzack, 1954) and electroencephalograph^

patterns from their brains indicated extreme arousal (Melzack and

Burns, 1965).

Animals living in a barren or restricted environment will

seek stimulation. During a four-hour experiment, rhesus monkeys

in an opaque cage worked hard to discriminate between blue and

yellow cards to operantly open a window for a brief glimpse into

the rest of the laboratory (Butler, 1953). Rats residing in a

barren environment will bar press more than those in an enriched

environment (Ehrlich, 1959). The restricted environment

consisted of five rats in a 60 x 30 x 23-cm wire cage. Enriched

environment rats were raised in groups of ten in a two-tiered

environment consisting of a 120 x 76 x 60-cm cage filled with

playthings such as corks, tunnels, ramps, and platforms. During

testing, each rat was placed individually in a Skinner box.

Reinforcement for bar pressing consisted of either clicks or

turning on a light.

Pigs residing in crates which were just wide enough to allow

turning around at a flared end, turned around 11 to 12 times per

day (McFarlane et al., 1988). Even animals which did not have to

turn around to gain access to feed or water nevertheless did turn

around about the same number times as did those which had to do

so. This is presumptive evidence of the pig's need for a certain

level of physical activity.

Pigs in intensive housing systems spend long periods

sleeping, punctuated by brief periods of intense excited

3

Page 18: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

activity, as when a door slams or a person walks into the room.

This behavior is similar to that of Melzack's puppies; in the

restricted environment, they became very excited when minor

changes were made in their cages (Melzack, 1954). Stallions kept

in stalls without exercise were harder to handle than those given

daily exercise (Dinger and Noiles, 1986).

Pigs reared in indoor pens with minimal contact with people

were more excitable and difficult to load into a trailer than

those reared outside with frequent contact with people (Warriss

et al., 1983). Stolba and Wood-Gush (1980) found that pigs in

barren pens with concrete floors reacted more strongly to and

played longer with a tire than did those in straw-bedded pens.

Wood-Gush and Beilharz (1983) found that pigs in cages spent

less time lying when their environment was enriched with a trough

filled with dirt. Maybe pigs in more barren pens sleep longer to

reduce the arousal level of an overly aroused nervous system.

Zentall and Zentall (1983) suggested that autistic children

withdraw from stimulation to prevent further arousal of an

already overly aroused nervous system.

Central nervous system effects of environmental restriction

Environmental restriction has long-term effects on an animal's

central nervous system (Melzack, 1969). Six months after being

released from a sensory-restricted environment, young dogs

still had electroencephalographic patterns which indicated an

abnormally high level of arousal (Melzack and Burns, 1965).

4

Page 19: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

The experiments reviewed in this paper support the

hypothesis put forth by Walsh and Cummins (1975) that animals

living in an enriched environment usually are less excitable than

animals in barren surroundings. Restricting sensory input makes

the nervous system of both humans and animals more sensitive and

more reactive to external stimulation. Schultz (1965) stated:

"When stimulus variation is restricted, central regulation of

threshold sensitivities will function to lower sensory

thresholds. Thus, the organism becomes increasingly sensitized

to stimulation in an attempt to restore balance." Walsh and

Cummins (1975) concluded that animals in an enriched environment

are subject to greater arousal during rearing and therefore are

less likely to exhibit overarousal in a novel or highly

stimulating environment.

Sensitization to external stimuli occurs within the central

nervous system. Placing a small cup on a person's forearm to

block tactile stimulation for one week increased tactile

sensitivity on the opposite (unshielded) forearm (Aftanas and

Zubek, 1964). This effect was quite persistent, as increased

sensitivity was still present three days after the cup had been

removed.

The sensitizing effect of restricted sensory input also can

occur across sensory modalities. The brain needs sensory input

to maintain normal reactivity levels. Zubek et al. (1964a) found

that a person who is blindfolded or wears translucent goggles for

one week has increased tactile, pain, and heat sensitivities,

5

Page 20: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

respectively. Even partial deprivation of visual input will

sensitize the skin (Zubek et al., 1964b). Trimming the whiskers

of baby rats causes the areas of the brain that receive sensory

input from the whiskers to become more excitable (Simons and

Land, 1987), and this effect persisted. Receptive fields were

still enlarged three months after the whiskers had regrown.

Other indicators of increased central nervous system arousal

are the effects of depressant and convulsive drugs, respectively,

on animals residing in different environments. Rats living

singly in small cages were compared to those in groups, and the

isolated rats had higher thresholds for anesthetics. Juraska et

al. (1983) administered both depressant and convulsive drugs to

rats isolated in small laboratory cages (isolated condition, IC)

or living in a group of 12 in an enriched environment (enriched

condition, EC) which consisted of a large enclosure with many

different toys. The IC rats injected with sodium pentobarbital

took longer to lose the righting reflex. In a second experiment,

when rats reared in IC and EC, respectively, were given the

convulsant drug, pentylenetetrazol, the IC rats went into light

flash-induced convulsions at lower doses.

Bombarding an animal with excessive stimulation can cause

detrimental signs similar to those caused by restricted

stimulation. In the IC/overstimulated condition, mice were

subjected involuntarily to intense sound, light, and vibration,

whereas those in the EC were allowed to initiate and control

their interaction with toys. Both IC and IC/overstimulated mice

6

Page 21: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

were more irritable than were EC mice, and both forced

understimulation and forced overstimulation increased

irritability.

Concept of optimal stimulation

Zentall and Zentall (1983) believe that organisms modulate

incoming sensory stimulation to maintain the nervous system at an

optimal level of arousal. In some cases, for example,

stereotyped behavior may be an attempt by a disturbed biological

system to restore homeostasis (Damasio and Maurer, 1978).

Environments offering low levels of stimulation tend to cause

behavior in animals which is stimulus-seeking. Conversely,

animals living in an overstimulating environment sometimes

attempt to reduce their central nervous system arousal by

engaging in repetitive activity (Zentall and Zentall, 1983).

Dantzer and Mormede (1983) and Dantzer (1986) suggested that

repetitive stereotypic behavior can serve a de-arousal function.

For example, food-deprived pigs will engage in repeated chain-

pulling in between food deliveries which occurred every four

minutes, and pigs which had access to the chain had lower plasma

corticosteroid levels. Stereotypies also occur in pigs living in

barren environments which provide low levels of sensory

stimulation (Ewbank, 1978; Markowitz, 1982). Therefore,

stereotypies may occur in both understimulating and

overstimulating environments. In understimulating environments,

of course, they probably serve to increase sensory input (Ewbank,

1978; Markowitz, 1982). Young children deprived of normal

7

Page 22: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

hugging will engage in excessive masturbation, which stopped when

nonsexual tactile contact with parents was increased (McGray,

1978).

Even though animals living in a barren environment may

attempt to reduce central nervous system arousal level, the

central nervous system nevertheless may remain in an abnormally

aroused state. Perhaps some of the environments that have been

imposed experimentally deprived the animals beyond their

physiological ability to cope. For example, extremely barren

environments would never be encountered in nature.

Nervous systems are endowed with a certain amount of

plasticity so the animals can deal with changing environmental

demands. The ability to adjust the relative "sensitivity" of

sensory systems to different environments would be useful to an

animal for survival in the wild. Simons and Land (1987)

concluded that sensory input affects the development of the

brain's somatosensory cortex.

Structural changes in the brain

Stimulation and opportunities for activity provided by an

enriched environment cause changes in dendritic branching in the

brain. Groups of weanling rats residing for four weeks in a

large enclosure with toys and objects to climb (enriched

condition, EC) had greater dendritic branching in the visual

cortex than did those living singly in standard laboratory cages

(isolated condition, IC) (Greenough and Juraska, 1979; Volkmar

and Greenough 1972).

8

Page 23: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

More recent research results indicate that new synapses may

be formed by neural activity induced by the enriched environment

(Greenough et al., 1985). Researchers also have compared IC and

EC with (social condition, SO), the latter consisting of two

animals living together in a 22 x 25 x 30-cm laboratory cage.

Fleeter and Greenough (1979) found no differences in dendritic

branching in the cerebellums of SO and IC monkeys (Macaca

fascicularis). The EC monkeys had the most dendritic branching.

In rats, SC animals had intermediate levels of dendritic

branching in the visual cortex (Volkmar and Greenough, 1972).

The IC rats had the least branching, the EC rats the most.

Weanlingrats in EC also had heavier brains than did those in IC

(Bennett et al., 1964; Diamond et al., 1964). Rats in EC also

had a thicker cortex and additional glial cells (Bennett et al.,

1964; Diamond et al., 1964; Diamond, et al., 1966). In these

experiments, the control rats were housed in trios in 32 x 32 x

20-cm laboratory cages, and all rats were in their respective

environments for at least 29 days.

Very short (four-day) periods of differential housing

affected cortical depth in the dorsal-medial part of the brain's

occipital cortex (Diamond et al., 1976). Cortical depth

differences in weanling rats were induced mainly by environmental

impoverishment, whereas in adults they were induced mainly by

enrichment.

Neural hypertrophy does not necessarily mean that an animal

is in an environment that is higher quality overall. Research

9

Page 24: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

indicates that neural hyptertrophy can sometimes be detrimental.

Rats exposed to continuous lighting had greater spine density in

the visual cortex (Parnavelas et al., 1973), but their retinas

were damaged (Bennett et al., 1972, O'Steen, 1970). Stimulation

of the hippocampus with an implanted electrode induced axonal

growth and reorganization of synapses, but these new connections

increased excitability and seizures developed (Sutala et al.,

1988)

Farm animals

Enriching pigs' environments may help prevent changes in the

central nervous system which lead to hyperexcitability and

stereotyped behavior. Casual observations of pigs residing in

indoor pens indicates that they often exhibit an excessive

startle reaction to door slamming or other disturbances.

Farmers have learned that playing a radio with a variety of music

and talk reduces pigs' reactivity to extraneous sounds.

Observations at slaughter plants indicate that some pigs are more

excitable and difficult to handle than others (Grandin, 1986).

Pigs living in indoor pens with minimal contact with people

were more excitable and difficult to load than those residing

outside with frequent contact with people (Warris et al., 1983).

Pigs from 1.2 x 1.2-m relatively barren pens also were slower to

approach a novel object or human strangers compared to pigs that

had had access to toys and frequent contact with people (Grandin

et al., 1983).

10

Page 25: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Pigs which are either balky or excitable are more difficult

to transport and handle at the slaughter plant. Those that are

reluctant or refuse to move are more likely to be subjected to

excessive electric-prodding. Electric prod-induced excitement is

detrimental to pork quality (Barton-Gade, 1985; Grandin, 1986).

Repeated electric-prodding also will increase bloodsplashing

(hemorrhage) in the pork carcass (Calkins et al., 1980).

Further, it is detrimental to the pig's welfare; heart rate

increases progressively with successive prods, and if it is

continued, the pig's heart rate will reach dangerously high

levels and death can result (van Putten and Elshof, 1978; Mayes

and Jesse, 1980). A pig will stop and lie down when its heart

rate is near the lethal point (Mayes and Jesse, 1980).

B. Environmental Enrichment Methods

Choice of envi ronmental enrichment

Stolba (1981) observed pigs in a semi-natural environment to

help determine suitable environmental enrichment devices. Their

studies were an important first step in determining choices pigs

make from among a variety of natural objects. It is important

that the play objects provided pigs be things that they

appreciate, and the objects must support sustained interest.

Research can be used to determine the type of objects pigs

prefer.

Choice tests have been used to determine animals'

preferences for type of flooring, social groupmates, and mate

11

Page 26: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

(Hughes and Black, 1973; Hughes, 1976; Dawkins, 1982; Farmer and

Christisdn, 1982; McGlone and Morrow, 1987); to test sheep's

preferences for type of handling facilities (Hitchcock and

Hutson, 1979; Hutson, 1981) and restraint methods (Grandin et

al., 1986; Rushen, 1986); and to determine pigs' preferred angle

and flooring surface for ramps (Phillips et al., 1988).

When choice tests are used, it is essential that they be

controlled adequately to prevent previous experiences from

influencing the choices. For example, previous experience with

different pastures can alter pasture preference in sheep (Arnold

and Mailer, 1977), and the environment in which they were reared

can affect caged hens' subsequent flooring and social preferences

(Hughes, 1976).

Another potential problem with preference tests is that

something that is preferred on a short-term basis may have

detrimental long-term effects on the animal's well-being (Hughes

and Black, 1973; Duncan, 1978; van Rooijen, 1982). For example,

animals may prefer one type of flooring during a two-hour

preference test, but the preferred flooring may injure the

animals' feet after several months.

Piglets usually prefer plastic-coated expanded-metal floor

over concrete (Farmer and Christison, 1982). In practice,

plastic-coated expanded-metal flooring is excellent for farrowing

crates and nursery pens but causes problems when used for a

finishing floor. Casual observations indicated that pigs

finished on either flattened or plastic-coated expanded metal had

12

Page 27: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

excessive hoof growth, and they balked and were difficult to

drive into a high-speed slaughter line (Grandin, 1988).

Preference tests are one useful method for determining

practical objects that producers could use to enrich a pig's

environment. Unlike flooring, feed, or housing, object choices

are unlikely to have adverse long-term effects on the pigs'

health or performance. There is a need for information on the

pig's short- and long-term preferences for means of environmental

enrichment so that recommendations can be made to producers.

Plav in Farm Animals

Farmers have offered pigs various objects in attempts to

enrich their environment and reduce vices, van Putten (1969)

recommended placing chains or rubber hoses in pens to occupy the

pigs' attention and reduce tail-biting. Weanling pigs will play

with many kinds of objects, and they vigorously chewed, tugged,

and shook suspended strips of cotton cloth (Grandin et al.,

1983). They also rooted and rolled balls, wooden boards, plastic

boxes and other objects when these items were first placed in.

their pen. When these objects became soiled with excreta,

however, the pigs lost interest in them. Suspended strips of

cloth seemed to hold the pigs' interest longer than did suspended

chains, clean objects more than did soiled.

Fagen (1984) described play as follows: "Play means

behavioral performance that emphasizes skills for interacting

with the physical and social environments and that occur under

circumstances under which the function of the exercised skills

13

Page 28: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

can not possibly be achieved." Play occurs in many farm animals,

including cattle, sheep, pigs, and horses (Brownlee, 1954, 1984;

Tyler, 1972; Schoen et al., 1976; Sachs and Harris, 1978; van

Putten, 1978; Dobao et al., 1984-85; Crowell-Davis et al., 1987),

as well as a relative of the domestic pig, the collared peccary

(Byers and Beckoff, 1981; Byers, 1984). It is interesting that

in the collared peccary both juveniles and adults play, because

ordinarily in ungulates only juveniles and subadult males play

(Byers, 1984).

Object play, including "tug-of-war", has been observed in

dogs, cats, horses, and jackals (van Lawick and Goodall, 1971;

West, 1977; Fagen, 1981; Martin, 1984). Captive bushdogs and

foxes play with sticks (Biben, 1982). A captive rhinoceros will

root a ball and other objects (Inhelder, 1978; Hediger, 1968).

Piglets also engage in object play (Gundlach, 1968; Fraedrich,

1974). Weanling pigs tugged on cloth strips in a manner similar

to that of dogs playing "tug-of-war" (Grandin et al., 1983).

Foals will pick up sticks and carry them around and toss them in

the air (Crowell-Davis et al., 1987).

Activity patterns in livestock

There are few data available on the daily activity patterns

of play in pigs. Domestic livestock do have daily patterns of

activity for grazing and other behavior (Hughes and Reid, 1951;

Squires, 1974; Arnold and Dudzindki, 1978). Pigs residing

indoors are more active during the day (Morrison et al., 1968).

In rich environments there are two peaks of activity—around 0900

14

Page 29: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

and 1700 hrs in pigs (Dantzer and Mailha, 1972; Dantzer, 1973;

Bure, 1981). When pigs had an opportunity to control the

temperature in their pen, a cooler environment was preferred at

night (Curtis and Morris, 1982).

Aggression and envi ronmental enrichment

Pigs are a relatively feisty species. When strange pigs are

mixed they will fight one another vigorously, at least in part to

establish a new social order (McBride et al., 1964). Fighting

among pigs causes injuries (Symoens and van der Brand, 1969;

Jones, 1969) and will reduce weight gains if the pigs are

overcrowded or have limited access to food (Graves et al.,

1978; Sherritt et al., 1974).

Fighting among newly weaned piglets generally has little

adverse effect on the pigs' long-term performance, but it can be

detrimental to welfare. McGlone and Curtis (1985) found that

fighting and injuries could be reduced by providing newly weaned

pigs small hides where they could place their head and shoulders.

Anecdotal reports suggest that providing pigs toys during times

of social mixing also will reduce fighting.

Straw bedding did not reduce fighting in growing pigs fed ad

libitum, but it had a tendency to reduce fighting in fasted pigs

(Kelley et al., 1980). Another factor which will influence

aggression is type of housing. Pigs in indoor pens were more

aggressive than those residing outdoors (Meese and Ewbank, 1973).

Crowding tends to increase pig aggression (Bryant and Ewbank,

1972; Kelley et al., 1980; Randolph et al., 1981), but it does

• 15

Page 30: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

not increase all kinds of aggression in a uniform manner.

Reducing floor space reduces aggression which occurs away from

the feeder, but it has no clear effect on aggression at the

feeder (Ewbank and Bryant, 1972). Restricting lying space

increased aggressive encounters when animals were standing but

had little effect on aggression in other areas (Ewbank and

Bryant, 1972). More information is needed about the effects of

environmental richness on aggression in pigs.

C. Thesis Research Outline

Dendritic growth in the pig's cerebral cortex

In the first experiment to be reported in this thesis, the

effect of environmental enrichment on dendritic growth in the

pig's brain was studied. Heretofore, many such studies have been

conducted with rodents (see review by Greenough and Chang, 1985),

and three with primates (Struble and Riesen, 1978; Fleeter and

Greenough, 1979; Stell and Riesen, 1987). The study to be

reported here was the first conducted with a domestic farm

animal. Valuable knowledge about environmental effects on brain

development may be able to be gained by studying animals that

have a larger, more complex brain than does the rat.

Excitability and handling of the fiig.

The second series of experiments were designed to determine

effects of environmental enrichment on the excitability and

handling of pigs living in intensive systems. The aim was to

determine effects of various kinds and extents of environmental

16

Page 31: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

enrichment on the excitability of pigs in space-restrictive,

relatively barren pens.

Object play by. the pig

In the third series of experiments, choice tests were

conducted to determine the types of play objects that are

preferred by pigs, both short- and long-term. Daily activity

patterns of toy play also were determined. In the last

experiment, effects of access to play objects prior to or

during social mixing of newly weaned piglets were determined to

learn whether toys would reduce agonistic behavior.

The environmental enrichment methods used in all of these

experiments were simple, practical procedures that swine

producers could use. Results of these experiments may help

answer questions as to whether relatively simple environmental

enrichment procedures will improve the productivity and welfare

of pigs residing in intensive production systems.

17

Page 32: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

III. PERFUSION METHOD FOR PREPARING PIG BRAIN CORTEX

FOR GOLGI-COX IMPREGNATION

A. Introduction

The development of Golgi-Cox staining procedures for pig

brain was required because existing methods of fixing and

preparing brain tissue that worked well with rodent or primate

tissue (Glaser and Van der Loos, 1981; Fleeter and Greenough,

1979; Green et al., 1983; Greenough 1984) gave poor results with

tissue from both visual and somatosensory cortex of pigs; fresh

tissue immersed directly in Golgi-Cox solution showed incomplete

neuron impregnation, and the same tissue fixed first in 10%

buffered formalin showed poor neuron and excessive glia staining.

Before the experiments could proceed, a new method of perfusion

and fixation of the pig brain was developed.

B. Procedure

Six or twelve 45-kg Hampshire-sired crossbred pigs (Sus

scrofa) were used on each of three occasions. At an abattoir,

the pigs were killed by exsanguination within 30 sec after having

been stunned electrically. Within 1 min after death, the pig's

entire head was removed and to prevent autolysis placed in a

bucket of ice for transport to the laboratory. (To facilitate

later location of the carotid arteries and their connection to

the perfusion apparatus, an additional 4 to 6 cm of neck was left

on the head; ordinarily the neck is transected immediately behind

18

Page 33: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

the pinnae.) Each head remained packed in ice up to 30 min

before starting perfusion with fixative.

Upon removing a head from the ice, both of its carotid

arteries were connected to an air pump perfusion system operated

at a pressure equivalent to 70 mm Hg. The system was a high-

capacity unit used to embalm large animals to be dissected by

students. Heads were perfused for 4 hr on the day of slaughter

and for an additional 4 hr the following day. The arteries were

connected to the system by insertion of a flexible plastic

catheter (2-mm-OD, 1.5-mm-ID) 1.5 cm into each artery, where it

was secured by a string ligature. Hypodermic needles (16 gauge)

were inserted into the free ends of the catheters, then the

needle hubs were inserted into short lengths of rubber tubing

(12-mm-OD, 8-mm-ID) which in turn were connected to an

appropriately sized plastic Y-tube (Nalgene #6153). The stem of

the Y-tube was then inserted into a short length of rubber tubing

(12-mm-OD, 8-mm-ID) which led to the stem of a second Y-tube

(Figure 1). One arm of the second Y from the first head was

connected directly to the pump, while the other arm was connected

to the stem of the second Y from the second head, and so on. Six

or twelve heads were perfused simultaneously in parallel by the

one system.

The perfusion fluid was a fixative solution composed (v/v)

of liquid phenol, 5%; formalin, 14%; ethylene glycol, 25%;

methanol, 28%; and water, 28%. The heads were perfused at room

temperature (~22° C).

19

Page 34: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

When the perfusion had been completed, the pig heads were

placed in a walk-in cooler (2° C) for 18 hr. Brains were removed

from the skull in one piece by carefully cutting the skull away

with a bone saw and a hammer and wood chisel. The intact brain

was placed immediately in 500 ml 10% buffered formalin for at

least 10 days prior to Golgi-Cox impregnation.

The Golgi-Cox solution was prepared from three stock

solutions, producing the solution specified by Glaser and Van der

Loos (1981)—stock solution 1: 37.5 g K2Cr207 in 750 ml

deionized water warmed to 30° C during mixing with a magnetic

stirrer; stock solution 2: 37.5 g HgCl2 in 750 ml deionized

water heated to 95° C during mixing with a magnetic stirrer; and

stock solution 3: 30 g K2Cr04 in 600-ml cold deionized water

(Greenough, 1982). Stock solutions 1 and 2 were mixed and then

added to stock solution 3 just after the latter had been diluted

with deionized water to a final volume of 1500 ml. A brown glass

bottle was used to shield the final Golgi-Cox solution from

light. The mixed solution was allowed to stand for 3 days to

allow particulate matter to settle; the supernate was decanted

and filtered through Whatman No. 42 paper (Greenough, 1984).

Blocks of tissue approximately 4 mm x 10 mm x 10 mm were cut

from each brain's visual and somatosensory cortex (Adrian, 1943;

Kruska, 1972). Each tissue block was wrapped in clean gauze and

immersed in 150-ml Golgi-Cox solution. After 24 hr, both Golgi-

Cox solution and gauze were changed. The containers with the

20

Page 35: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

tissue blocks were kept in the dark for three weeks, the minimum

time required for uniform staining.

Before embedding, a test section was developed in ammonia

water (19% NH40H) and examined under a microscope to determine

whether the impregnation was complete. If impregnation was

incomplete, the tissue block was reimmersed in the Golgi-Cox

solution for another week.

After being impregnated, the tissue was dehydrated through

1:1 ethanol:acetone for 3 hr, 100% ethanol for 20 min, and 1:1

anhydrous diethyl ether:100% ethanol for 20 min. Six and twelve

percent celloidin solutions were prepared by dissolving Parlodian

strips in 1:1 anhydrous diethyl ether:100% ethanol. Each tissue

block was placed in 6% celloidin for 6 days and then in 12%

celloidin for 6 days. The celloidin-impregnated tissue was

hardened over chloroform in a desiccator and mounted on a wooden

block.

After mounting, the blocks were stored under butanol until

sectioned at 120 urn using a sliding microtome (thick sections are

required to prevent excessive transecting of radially oriented

dendrites). The sections were processed in the following manner

(Glaser and Van der Loos, 1981): 1) 70% ethanol for 10 sec, 2)

50% ethanol for 10 sec, 3) deionized water for 30 sec, 4) ammonia

water (19% NH40H) for 20 min, 5) deionized water for two 30-sec

immersions in separate pans, 6) fixer (Kodak Rapid Fix diluted

1:7) for 10 min, 7) deionized water for two 30-sec immersions in

separate pans, 8) 50% ethanol for 10 sec, 9) 70% ethanol for 10

21

Page 36: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

sec, 10) 95% ethanol for 60 sec, and 11) 100% ethanol for two 20-

sec immersions in separate pans.

To facilitate handling, each tissue section was placed in a

compartment of a perforated plastic ice cube tray (Greenough,

1984). The 100% ethanol was changed after two ice trays had been

processed (water contamination of the final dehydration step will

ruin the sections). Trays containing the sections were blotted

on paper towels after every step. Immediately after dehydration,

the sections were placed under xylene and rapidly mounted on

glass slides using Permount. To prevent curling of the sections,

a 100-g paraffin-coated lead weight was placed over the glass

cover slip and the slides were allowed to dry for 3 weeks. The

relatively thick sections required this much drying time to

ensure stability of the covers!ip during cleaning and use.

C. Results

The procedure described in this paper produced high-quality

Golgi-Cox-impregnated neurons from both the visual and the

somatosensory cortical regions of pig brain (Figures 2 and 3).

Pig brains that had been immersed in 10% buffered formalin

solution for 5 wk after perfusion with a special fixative

solution and before impregnation yielded even higher quality

impregnated neurons that did those kept in the formalin for only

10 days.

The procedure described in this paper produced poor quality

Golgi-Cox-impregnated sections from pig cerebellar cortex. The

22

Page 37: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

cerebellum appeared to be adequately perfused, but impregnation

quality was poor.

Pig heads perfused in groups of six were fixed more

uniformly than were those perfused in a group of twelve.

0. Discussion

The perfusion fluid used for this procedure was the

traditional one used to embalm animals to be dissected by

students. The discovery that it was effective in fixing pig

brain cortex for Golgi-Cox impregnation was serendipitous.

Perfusing more than six heads with the perfusion apparatus

we used would not be recommended. It is likely that heads in

excess of six exceeded the capacity of the pump; there did not

appear to be a tendency for heads closer to the pump than the

sixth one to be perfused more uniformly than that one.

There are two advantages of perfusing isolated pig heads as

opposed to perfusing the whole body; less perfusion fluid is used

and the rest of the carcass can be salvaged.

23

Page 38: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 1. Schematic drawing of system used to perfuse pig brains

with fixative solution.

24

Page 39: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Rubber Hose Connector Plastic Catheter

Carotid Arteries Tied with String to Hold Catheter In Place

Rubber Hose

Hose to Next Head

ose to Pump System

Second Plastic Y HtJing

25

Page 40: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 2: Typical Golgi-Cox-impregnated nerve cells in pig

somatosensory cortex, x 275. Fine structural

details, such as spines, appear clearly impregnated,

whereas background staining of glial tissue varies

across laminar section.

26

Page 41: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

27

Page 42: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 3. Typical Golgi-Cox-impregnated nerve calls in pig

somatosensory cortex, x 1500.

28

Page 43: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

t t *

</

»>.

j * »

#JL

%*» &- -»

f H. F4T ^

••? s " - ^

. w l *

4.#* , » '^f-* *. ^ ^ '

^L B*ww, .

^f

29

Page 44: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

IV. DENDRITIC GROWTH IN SOMATOSENSORY REGION OF BRAIN CORTEX

IN PIGS RESIDING IN A SIMPLE OR A COMPLEX ENVIRONMENT

A. Introduction

An animal's experiences affect the anatomical development

of its brain. Volkmar and Greenough (1972) studied weanling

rats that had resided for 29 or 30 days in three different

environments:enriched condition (EC), social condition (SC),

and isolated condition (IC). In the EC, 12 animals resided

most of the time in a 45 x 60 x 70-cm cage furnished with many

different objects that were changed daily, and they also were

placed in a large object-filled box for 30 minutes of activity

every day. The objects were pieces of wood, children's toys,

ladders, and laboratory utensils. In the IC, single rats

resided in 22 x 25 x 30-cm cages with solid metal sides and a

wire mesh top and front. In the SC, the rats resided in pairs

in the same type of cage as in the IC. Rats in the EC group

had the most dendritic branching, those in the IC the least.

Differential rearing environment had the greatest effect on the

higher-order dendrites (i.e., those located farther from the

cell body).

Similar but smaller and more localized increases in

dendritic branching have been found to occur following training

in mazes and motor tasks (Black et al., 1987; Chang and

Greenough, 1982; Greenough et al., 1985).

30

Page 45: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Most studies on the effects of environmental richness on

dendritic branching and synapse growth have been conducted on

rodents (Greenough and Chang, 1985). The EC/SC/IC paradigm

described above was used in a monkey study, too (Fleeter and

Greenough, 1979). Macaca fascicularis raised in the EC (with

toys and climbing structures) had more dendritic branching in

cerebellar Purkinje cells than did paired or isolated monkeys.

The EC monkeys also had larger Purkinje somas.

In another experiment, old World monkeys (Macaca arctoides)

placed in isolation had significantly less dendritic branching

than colony-reared monkeys (Struble and Riesen, 1978). Further

studies by Stell and Riesen (1987) indicated that monkeys reared

in isolation with objects, ladders, and swings had greater

dendritic branching in the motor 1 cortex compared to monkeys

reared by their mothers in the colony or reared in isolation

without gymnastic equipment.

In cats, Spinelli et al. (1980) found that training a kitten

to avoid a shock by lifting its foreleg resulted in greater

dendritic density in the cortical area which receives sensory

input from the forearm. Beaulier and Colonnier (1987) also found

that IC cats had more FS synapses and fewer RA synapses per

neuron compared to EC cats. FS and RA synapses are the two main

morphological types of synapses in the cerebral cortex. FS

synapses have flat vesicles and RA synapses have round vesicles.

The preceding experiments have all involved conditions

specifically designed for comparison of environmental enrichment

31

Page 46: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

or training with its absence. The purpose of the present

experiment was to determine whether the quality of different

rearing environments which might be used in production

agriculture can affect the morphology of cerebral cortical

neurons in pigs. To assess this, pigs reared in a simple,

production-like condition were compared with pigs reared in a

large group with straw, a variety of different objects, and

additional contact with people.

B. Experimental Procedure

Animals and treatments

Twenty-four littermate pairs of 18- to 25-day-old Hampshire-

sired crossbred pigs were studied in two trials. Mean weight

of trial 1 pigs was 7.97 kg (SD=0.87) and trial 2 pigs was

10.78 kg (SD=1.60). In each trial, 12 pigs resided in a

complex environment (CE) (similar to the EC in the experiments

described earlier), and 12 in a simple environment (SE)

(similar to the SC described earlier). Trial 1 lasted 68 days,

and Trial 2, 65 days. Half of the animals in each trial were

used for anatomical study.

The SE consisted of rearing two pigs in each of six 1.22 x

1.22-m pens with plastic-coated expanded-metal floors in a

windowless, environmentally controlled house where air

temperature was 22 to 26 C. Pen partitions were constructed of

vertical glass fiber-reinforced plastic rods. Pigs in

different pens could see each other, but could have no physical

32

Page 47: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

contact, because there was an empty pen between every two that

were occupied. The room was illuminated continuously by

fluorescent tubes (200 lux) in Trial 1. In Trial 2, the

luminal res were timer-controlled so illumination started at the

approximate time of sunrise (0600) and stopped at the

approximate time of sunset (2030) each day; this provided the

SE pigs with approximately the same light/dark cycle as the CE

pigs. The SE pigs ate from a self-feeder and drank from a

nipple waterer. The room was entered only for feeder servicing

for 10 min every day or pen washing for 60 to 90 min every

third day.

The 12 CE pigs resided together in a house with a concrete

slab floor bedded with straw (new straw added daily) with an

adjoining outdoor pen with a concrete slab floor. In both

trials, the CE pigs were handled by the experimenter for 15 to 30

min every day. The experimenter petted the animals, scratched

their abdomens, and allowed them to chew on boots and coveralls.

In Trial 1, the CE pigs were provided a variety of objects

to manipulate. These objects were changed daily and included a

plastic milk crate, various cloth strips tied to the fence,

ropes, chains, plastic ball, dirt, corrugated cardboard,

newspapers, telephone directories, soda cans, stones, garbage

can, and pieces of wood. The CE pigs in Trial 2 were provided

only three 7.5 x 50-cm white cotton cloth strips suspended from

a rope across the pen; the strips were changed weekly. Feed

33

Page 48: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

was provided in a self-feeder with lids, and water was provided

in a float-controlled automatic waterer.

Brain preparation

At the end of each trial, six pigs from each treatment group

were sacrificed and their brains were perfused, fixed, Golgi-Cox

impregnated, sectioned, and mounted by procedures described in

Chapter III (Grandin et al., 1988), a modification of the

methods of Glaser and Van Der Loos (1981) and Greenough (1984).

Figure 4 shows the locations of the tissue blocks that were

removed from the visual and somatosensory regions of the

cerebral cortex. Somatosensory cortical tissue blocks were

from the area determined by Adrian (1943), who used evoked

responses to tactile stimuli from the brain of anesthetized

pigs. Adrian (1943) and Woolsey and Fairman (1946) are in

excellent agreement in this regard. The work of Kruska (1971,

1972) was used to determine the location of the visual cortex

in the pig brain. Results of Campbell (1905) are in accord

with Kruska's findings.

Dendritic growth determination

An Olympus BH-2 binocular microscope (Tokyo, Japan) with

an Olympus BH2-DA camera lucida attachment was used to make

pencil tracings of layer II pyramidal cells in both the

somatosensory and visual regions of the cerebral cortex. All

slides were coded so the drawings could be made with the drawer

blind to treatment. Microscope magnification was 450X.

34

Page 49: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

To determine the level of cortical lamination, 12 visual

cortex sections of each brain were counterstained with

methylene blue following the Glaser and Van Der Loos (1981)

protocol (counterstained sections were not used for data

collection). The processing procedure was the same as

described in Chapter III, but additional steps were added after

sectioning:

1. 70% ethanol for 10 sec

2. 50% ethanol for 10 sec

3. Two immersions in deionized water in separate pans

for 30 sec each

4. Ammonia water (19% NH40H) for 20 min

5. p-Phenylene di-amine (.5 g in 1000 ml water) for 5 min

6. Two immersions in deionized water in separate pans

for 2 min each

7. 1% Kodak Dektol Fixer for 2 min

8. Kodak Rapid Fixer for 7 min

9. Three immersions in deionized water in separate pans

for 5 min each

10. 1:1 tamponee:water for 5 min (formula for tamponee:

sodium acetate 13.6 g, glacial acetic acid 23.45

ml, water to make 1 L)

11. 1:1 and 2% methylene blue in deionized water

for 8.5 min.

12. Tamponee for 30 sec

13. 1:1 tamponee and deionized water for 20 min

35

Page 50: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

14. 50% ethanol in deionized water for 15 sec

15. 70% ethanol in deionized water for 15 sec

16. 95% ethanol in deionized water for 60 sec

17. Two immersions in 100% ethanol in separate pans for

20 sec each.

For each pig, a minimum of 14 drawings of basilar dendrites

of layer II pyramidal neurons in the visual and somatosensory

regions of the cerebral cortex, respectively, were made at 450X

magnification. Apical dendrites were not evaluated because many

of them had been transected by the microtome. Pig cortical

neurons are extremely large relative to those of rodents, and

therefore more difficult to align.

A stratified sampling method was developed to specify the

location of the pyramidal cells to be drawn. The outline of

each section was traced with a projection microscope (Bausch

and Lomb 42-63-59, Rochester, New York) at low power (22X).

Lines were drawn connecting landmarks on the projected drawing

(as shown in Figures 5 and 6) to determine three locations in

the somatosensory cortex and four in the visual cortex. More

than one location was used because usually there were an

insufficient number of well-impregnated or untransected cells

in any one location. In the somatosensory cortex, four

drawings were made from each location, and in the visual

cortex, three. Four sections per animal were used. The

remaining two drawings of the 14 were made from two locations

picked at random (if remaining undrawn neurons in the

36

Page 51: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

respective areas were well-impregnated) or from the undrawn

well-impregnated neurons which remained in the locations.

Dendritic complexity was quantified by concentric ring

analysis (Sholl, 1956; Greenough, 1975). A clear plastic overlay

with concentric circles at 20 m equivalent intervals was

centered visually over the neuron's soma, and the basilar

dendritic branches that crossed each ring were counted. Total

ring intersections (TRI) were then determined by summing the

counts for all rings.

Soma width was determined by measuring the width of the cell

body at its widest point, 90 degrees perpendicular to the main

apical process. Measurements were made on the camera lucida

drawings with a ruler marked at millimeter intervals.

Pig behavior

At the end of Trial 1, behavior of pigs from both

environments was tested to determine the pig's time to approach

a strange man and a novel object, respectively. Each animal

was tested individually in an octagonal arena with 1.22-m-high

white-painted plywood walls (Figure 7). The arena floor was

covered with brown plastic carpet. The vestibule floor was

broom finish concrete. The room was illuminated at 200 lux.

Tests of time to approach the strange man and time to approach

the novel object were conducted separately. The strange man

was a person unknown to the pigs. He stood still during the

test. The novel object was a new red metal feeder, standing on

its end.

37

Page 52: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Testing was alternated between CE and SE pigs. Strange-

man approach tests and strange object approach tests were

conducted successively, in random order. During testing, each

pig was placed in the vestibule for a 3 min adjustment period.

After this, the entrance gate to the arena was opened. The

timer was started when the pig's nose passed the entrance gate

and stopped when the pig walked up and touched the man or the

object. The test was terminated if the pig failed to touch the

man or the object within 3 min.

At the end of Trial 2, behavior of pigs from both

environments was tested in a narrow chute. The layout of the

apparatus is shown in Figure 8. The narrow chute was constructed

from plywood painted white. It was 1.22-m high, 27-cm wide at

the bottom and 38-cm wide at the top. The wooden chute floor was

painted gray and the vestibule floor was covered with brown

plastic carpet.

To tend to discourage the pig's movement through the

chute, the floor contained three obstacles perpendicular across

the path: a 5-cm-wide light beam, a 7.5-cm-wide perforated

metal strip, and a 2 x 3.7-cm-wide wood board. To encourage

movement through the chute, a decoy pig (not from the

experiment) was placed in a welded-wire mesh pen at the end of

the chute.

The testing procedure was similar to that in Trial 1

except there was no adjustment period in the vestibule. The

timer was started when the pig was placed in the vestibule.

38

Page 53: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

The test was terminated if the pig failed to walk all the way

through the chute to the decoy pig within 5 min. Each pig was

tested twice.

In Trial 2, behavior of pigs in both environments was

observed for 24 h during week 7 of the trial. It was

videorecorded (NV8030 time-lapse recorder, Panasonic Co.,

Secaucus, NJ). The videorecording system was operated at 0.9

frames/s. In the SE, one camera viewed three of the six pens.

In the CE, one camera viewed the inside of the straw-bedded shed,

and a second the concrete slab. Data were registered while

records were reviewed at a speed 18 times faster than the

recording speed. High-speed reviewing made subtle nosing and

rooting movements appear as readily discernible vibrations of the

snout. Nosing of other pigs and rooting or chewing objects was

quantified by one-zero sampling (Lehner, 1979) every 5 min.

The entire videorecord was viewed. A five-min interval, in

which one pig was active in the way of interest, was given a

score of 1; if two different animals were observed to be active

within the same interval, a score of 2 was given, and so on.

Statistical analysis

The individual pig was the experimental unit. The

dependent variables were total ring intersections (TRI), soma

width, and the product index, (TRI x soma width). The latter

variable was calculated to serve as an index of neuron

development.

39

Page 54: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Data were analyzed by the SAS General Linear Model (analysis

of variance) procedure (SAS, 1982). In an attempt to enhance the

power of the statistical tests, individual data (x) were

transformed as follows: x2, log x, arcsine Vx7 "Vx", and (x -

1). The ^transformed data improved the significance level, and

data resulting from this transformation will be presented and

interpreted together with the raw data.

While camera lucida drawings were being made, it became

apparent that many pyramidal cells had a distorted shape.

Apical dendrites were twisted, and some basilar trees were

grossly asymmetrical. In some cases, the asymmetry was not due

to transection by the section plane. Instead, it was the

natural shape of the cell. To determine if distortion of the

cells was affecting our ability to detect differences in

dendritic growth due to environment, a sorting procedure

suggested by Janice M. Juraska (personal communication, 1987)

was developed to arbitrarily eliminate distorted cells. In the

end, pyramidal cells had to comply with the following criteria

to be retained in the sorted category:

1. Basilar tree more than 170 m in diameter measured

perpendicularly relative to the apical process (9 mm

measured on the camera lucida drawing was equal to

20 m on the cell).

2. Soma more than 18 m wide at the widest part

perpendicular to the apical process.

40

Page 55: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

3. Symmetrical basilar tree. (If more than 75% of the

basilar dendrites were on one side of a line extending

180 degrees from the apical process, the cell was

rejected.)

4. Main apical branch more than 140 m long. (If the

main apical process was transected, but more than

4 m wide at the point of transection, the cell was

retained. This criterion was based on experience

studying pyramidal cells. Cells with apical

processes 4.5 m or thicker near the soma had

apical processes and dendrites longer than 140 m.)

These criteria were arbitrary, but they were based on

experience and intended to remove the misshapen cells and cells

that did not conform to the symmetrical pyramidal cells with

long apical processes described in several reports (Sholl,

1956; Greenough, 1975; Greenough and Juraska, 1979). Figures 9

and 10 illustrate somatosensory layer II pyramidal neurons

which satisfy the above criteria, whereas Figures 11 and 12

illustrate neurons that do not satisfy these criteria. The

basilar tree is less than 170 m wide in Figure 11, and in

Figure 12 the basilar tree is asymmetrical. Basilar trees with

a major branch transection near the cell body were also

rejected. In Trial 1, 9.91 (SD=4.50) somatosensory cortical

drawings per pig were retained in the sorted data set; in Trial

2, 7.50 (SD=1.45). In Trial 1, 5.27 (SD=1.11) visual cortical

41

Page 56: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

drawings per pig were retained; in Trial 2, 6.66 (SD=2.39).

Data for both sorted and unsorted cell sets were analyzed.

Visual cortical data for one CE barrow in Trial 1 was

unavailable for analysis due to incomplete Golgi-Cox

impregnation.

C. Results

Pigs reared in the SE had more extensive basilar dendrites

in the somatosensory cortex than did those in the CE. Total ring

intersections for both trials combined (unsorted) were 55.70 +

2.38 (mean + SE) in SE pigs and 48.62 + 2.38 in CE pigs (P<.06;

Table 1). After sorting, the significance level changed to

P<.03 (Table 1). For the raw data, the significance level was

P<.06, and on the square root-transformed data, P<.05.

Graphing the raw data revealed that the greatest differences

between the SE and CE pigs was in rings 3 and 4 (Figures 13 and

14). Rings 1 and 2 (closer to the soma) showed less difference.

Pigs from the SE also had larger somas in the somatosensory

cortex than did those from the CE: 7.72 + 0.17 versus 7.27 +

0.17 (Table 1) (P<.08, raw data; P<.08 square root-transformed

data). Sorted soma widths did not differ significantly. Both

sorted and unsorted somatosensory cortex index scores differed

significantly between environments (Table 1) (P<.05 and P<.04,

respectively); the SE pigs had higher scores.

When the data were categorized according to trial, there

were no effects of rearing environment in Trial 1.

42

Page 57: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Environmental effects on TRI and index scores approached

significance in Trial 2 (Table 2) (P<.08 and P<.09); there was

a tendency for the SE pigs to have more dendritic branching.

When data from both trials for the visual cortex were

pooled, environment had no effect on TRI, soma size, or the

product index, (TRI x soma size) (Tables 3 and 4). Graphs of

the raw data for visual cortex in Trials 1 and 2 pooled

illustrate the lack of difference between treatments (Figures

15 and 16). When Trials 1 and 2 were analyzed separately there

were no treatment differences (Table 4).

There was a strong effect of gender on somatosensory

cortical traits when data from both trials were pooled. Gilts

had larger somas (7.78 + 0.17 versus 7.22 + 0.17, P<.03) (Table

5). However, there was no gender effect for visual cortex when

data from both trials were pooled (Table 6). Analysis employing

a gender X trial interaction model revealed an effect of trial

on TRI in both the somatosensory and visual regions of the

cerebral cortex (P<.03 for somatosensory unsorted; P<.006 for

visual sorted) (Tables 7 and 8); barrows had higher TRI in

Trial 1, gilts in Trial 2. Sorting changed significance levels

of the somatosensory TRI and soma size, but had little effect

on those for the visual cortex.

In Trial 1 there was a tendency for the CE pigs to touch

both the strange man and the novel object sooner. Mean times

to approach the strange man were: CE—59.5 ± 13 sec (mean +

SE), SE—100.3 ± 18 sec (NS). Approach times toward the

43

Page 58: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

strange object were CE—49.8 + 13 sec and SE—83.5 ± 18 sec

(NS). Even though the mean times were not significantly

different, there was a significant difference between rearing

environments for the number of pigs which touched the man in

less than 60 sec. Nine out of 12 CE pigs touched the man in

less than 60 sec, whereas only 4 out of 12 SE pigs did go

(X2=4.19, P<.05). In the approach on object test, 9 out of 12

CE pigs touched within 60 sec, whereas only 5 out of 12 SE pigs

did go (X2=2.74, P<.10). When data for both the man and object

approach tests were combined, 18 out of 24 CE pigs touched

within 60 sec, whereas only 9 out of 24 SE pigs did go

(X2=6.85, P<.01).

In Trial 2, CE pigs were more willing to walk through the

chute. Mean times to walk through in the first test were:

CE—2.27 ± .53 min and SE—4.54 ± .37 min (P<.001). Mean times

for the second test were: CE—1.47 ± .53 min and SE—4.13

± .48 min (P<.001). Times were shorter for the second trial

compared to the first.

On the first chute test, 10 of 12 CE pigs walked through

within 5 min, whereas only 2 of 12 SE pigs did (X2=10.66,

P<.01). In the second test, 10 of 12 CE pigs walked through,

but only 4 of 12 SE pigs (X2=6.18, P<.02). When both tests

were combined, the results were: 20 of 24 CE pigs walked

through within 5 min, but only 6 of 24 SE pigs did go

(X2=16.540, PK.001).

44

Page 59: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Pigs in the SE engaged in significantly more nosing of each

other than pigs in the CE. The CE pigs had only 14 5-min periods

where nosing each other was observed whereas the SE pigs had 119

5-min intervals nosing each other (X2=194, P<.001). The CE pigs

also had greater overall rooting activity directed toward a

variety of objects (295 versus 215 5-min intervals, (X2 = 9.54,

P<.001).

The CE pigs directed over 90% of their rooting activity

toward objects, whereas the SE pigs spent up to 60% of their

rooting time, rooting each other.

Casual observations of the pigs during pen cleaning (SE

pigs) and petting (CE pigs) indicated that there may have been

a difference in the intensity of rooting and nosing. The CE

pigs nibbled gently on the straw, whereas the SE pigs rubbed

their noses intensely against the floor. Rubbing and massaging

another pig may be more stimulating to the somatosensory cortex

than rooting straw. The recipient of the massaging appeared to

react positively, often rolling over and presenting its belly

to the instigating pig. There also was a tendency for the SE

pigs that engaged in higher levels of nosing to have greater

dendritic growth.

The TRI scores for the three videotaped SE pigs were,

56.63, 45.05 and 50.90. The animal with the highest TRI score

also had the highest number of 5-min intervals which contained

rooting of the other pig. The number of 5-min intervals which

contained rooting were, 41, 37, and 38, respectively. The

45

Page 60: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

animal that had the lowest TRI score had the highest number of

5-min intervals which contained rooting of objects in the pen.

Number of 5-min intervals which contained rooting of objects

was 15, 20, and 16, respectively.

The SE pigs were more excitable than CE pigs. They also

appeared to be actively seeking additional stimulation. During

pen washing, these pigs became highly excited and tried to bite

the hose and play in the water stream. Every time the

experimenter moved the water stream away from the pigs, they

jumped so as to be in it again. When the SE pigs were in an

excited state during pen cleaning, they often rubbed their

noses against the floor, perhaps in redirected behavior.

Furthermore, during feeder cleaning, the SE pigs would rush

forward and bite the experimenter's hand, and when they were

pushed away, they returned instantly and bit the hand again. In

the CE, the pigs were calmer and the experimenter could easily

push the pigs aside (and they stayed away for a while) while

cleaning the feeder.

Moreover, in the SE pigs, there were signs the animals

were active while people were absent. For example, unscrewed

bolts frequently were found on the floor.

0. Discussion

The experimental environments employed—simple (SE) and

complex (CE)—corresponded approximately to the SC and EC,

respectively, used in earlier rat and monkey experiments. Pigs

46

Page 61: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

were not placed in an IC, for two reasons. First, Fleeter and

Greenough (1979) found no differences in cerebellar dendritic

branching between IC and SC monkeys. Second, isolation of

highly social animals such as monkeys and pigs may create

stress and abnormal behavior (Harlow and Zimmerman, 1959;

Melzack, 1954, 1969). In the interest of the pfgs' welfare, we

felt we did not have sufficient justification to subject the

animals to the stress of long-term isolation (IC).

Rats residing in the EC had greater dendritic branching in

the visual and temporal, but not the frontal, lateral cortex

(Greenough, 1984). Similar findings also have been found

in the neocortex of adult and middle-aged rats (Uylings et al.,

1978; Green et al., 1983; Volkmar and Greenough, 1972). Rats

residing in an enriched environment had a neocortex that was

heavier in several regions compared to those in standard 32 x

20 x 20-cm cages (Bennett et al., 1964; Diamond et al., 1964;

Katz and Davis, 1984). The enriched environment consisted of

groups of 10 to 12 rats residing in a large cage equipped with

ladders, platforms, exercise wheel, and other objects. For 30

min each day, the rats living in the EC also were placed in a

maze in which positions of barriers were changed daily (Bennett

et al., 1964). The control rats were housed in trios in small

(32 x 32 x 20-cm) cages.

Other brain studies revealed that rats from the EC also had

a thicker cortex (Bennett et al., 1964; Diamond et al., 1964).

Moreover, rats which had a thickened cortex also had additional

47

Page 62: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

glia cells in the cortex (Diamond et al., 1966). The EC also

caused increases in a wide variety of interrelated brain measures

such as synaptic density (Turner and Greenough, 1985; Bhide and

Bedi, 1984) and ratio of oligodendrocytes to neurons in the

occipital cortex (Katz and Davis, 1984).

In the present experiment, the SE pigs had greater dendritic

branching in the somatosensory cortex than did the CE pigs. The

SE pigs also had pyramidal cells with larger somas. There was no

significant effect of environment on dendritic branching or soma

size in the visual cortex. These results are contrary to our

hypothesis that the CE pigs (corresponding approximately to EC

rats) would have increased dendritic growth. Results of previous

research on rats indicated that the EC enhanced dendritic

development in both neocortical and noncortical areas of the

brain (Fleeter and Greenough, 1979; Volkmar and Greenough, 1972;

Juraska et al., 1985).

The most likely explanation for the apparent anomalous

results in this experiment is direct stimulation of the

somatosensory cortex. Previous investigators have found that

exercise or sensory stimulation will augment neuroanatomy.

Exercise will increase dendritic branching in rat cerebellum

(Pysh and Weiss, 1979). Training a kitten to avoid a shock by

lifting its foreleg resulted in greater dendritic density in

the cortical area which receives sensory input from the forearm

(Spinelli et al., 1980). Tactile, vestibular, and other

sensory stimulation applied to young dogs increased the size of

48

Page 63: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

vestibular neurons, while there were inconsistent differences

in frontal and auditory regions of the cerebral cortex (e.g.,

three of eight stimulated puppies had larger pyramidal cells)

(Fox, 1971). Shapiro and Vukovich (1970) also found that the

application of visual, auditory, and vestibular stimulation and

mild electric shocks to infant Sprague-Dawley rats increased

spine density in the visual and auditory regions of the

cerebral cortex.

Rubbing and massaging another pig may be more stimulating to

the somatosensory cortex than is rooting straw. Casual

observations indicated that SE pigs may have pressed harder with

their snouts than CE pigs. The recipient of the massaging

appeared to react positively, often rolling over and presenting

its belly to the instigating pig.

Placing an animal in a relatively barren environment will

increase excitability, irritability, and self-stimulatory

behavior. Pigs residing in a barren environment engage in more

activities involving the snout, van Putten (1980) found that

pigs housed on partially slatted floors without straw massaged

other pigs with their snouts twice as much compared to pigs with

access to straw. Stolba (1981) also reported that as the

environment was made increasingly barren, behaviors directed

toward other pigs increased. Less nibbling and massaging of

other pigs occurs in straw-bedded pens. Providing enrichment in

addition to straw further reduces activities directed toward

other pigs (Stolba, 1981).

49

Page 64: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

From a sensory standpoint, the SE pigs may have been more

deprived than SC rats. The SE pigs had no bedding to root or

nibble. They were fed a diet in the form of a finely ground

meal, which required little time to consume. SC rats had

bedding material and hard food pellets to chew. In the

relatively barren SC condition, the rats still had access to

materials which enabled them to perform natural chewing

behaviors for long periods. Even though the rats could not

burrow in the bedding, they could still manipulate it. The

pigs had no substrate to manipulate. Small amounts of straw

will reduce stereotypies in tethered sows (Fraser, 1975).

Possibly, SE pigs engaged in greater amounts of abnormal

behavior than SC rats. This could have resulted in abnormally

high amounts of input to the somatosensory cortex.

Animals that engage in stereotypies often seek abnormally

intense amounts of stimulation. They even will sometimes

injure themselves in an effort to obtain stimulation (Cross and

Harlow, 1965). Among other things, stereotyped behavior

rewards the animal by releasing brain opioids. Self-

destructive behavior in retarded children and crib-biting in

horses are both stopped by administration of opioid blocking

drugs (e.g., Naloxone, Naltrexone) (Sandman et al., 1983;

Dodman et al., 1987).

Adrian (1943) reported that a major portion of the pig's

somatosensory cortex receives input from the snout, which is

one of the most sensitive parts of the pig's anatomy.

50

Page 65: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Therefore, when the SE pigs pressed their snouts against

something, presumably intense input was relayed to the

somatosensory cortex.

The greater intensity of activity of the SE pigs at pen

washing time and the observed intense activities involving

their snouts may explain the increased dendritic branching in

their somatosensory cortex. Pigs in barren pens with concrete

floors reacted more strongly and played longer with a tire than

did pigs reared in straw-bedded pens (Stolba and Wood-Gush,

1980). Pigs reared on concrete floors grazed and rooted with

greater intensity compared to pasture-reared pigs (Friend and

Taylor, 1986). The animals were observed on a pasture which

was novel to all animals.

The behavioral tests conducted at the end of the two

trials in the present study indicated that CE pigs behaved

differently than SE pigs. The SE pigs exhibited greater

avoidance responses when they were isolated in the novel arena

or chute apparatus. Ten of 12 SE pigs refused to walk through

the chute, whereas only two CE pigs refused. The SE pigs'

behavior was similar to that of unhandled rats. Rats that are

handled during infancy explore larger areas of an open field

(Levine et al., 1967). Melzack and Thompson (1956) found that

dogs reared in a restricted environment had greater avoidance

responses when a person approached.

The SE pigs sought stimulation, but it was an approach-

avoidance situation. The first few times the pens were washed,

51

Page 66: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

the SE pigs panicked and fled to the rear of the pen. But when

they became habituated to pen washing, they approached and

excitedly bit at the water stream and hose. Within a few days,

pen washing had changed from an activity that triggered a

strong avoidance response to one that elicited strong approach

response. The SE pigs often defecated in the feeder. After

the experimenter cleaned it, they sometimes defecated in the

feeder again almost immediately. This suggests that they might

have been seeking stimulation from the presence of the

experimenter, however short the visit may have been. In a

totally novel situation, the SE pigs avoided novel stimulation;

but in their familiar pens, they actively sought stimulation by

belly nosing, biting the experimenter, and biting the water

stream and hose.

The SE pigs were more excitable than the CE pigs. Similar

observations have been made by other investigators in other

species. Animals in the IC were more excitable than were EC

animals (Korn and Moyer, 1968; Walsh and Cummins, 1975).

Isolation of mice increased reactivity, muscle tone, and

aggressive behavior (Valzelli, 1973). Pairs of puppies

confined to a barren cage with no visual contact with other

dogs responded to a new environment with increased activity

(Melzack, 1965). Puppy pairs became hyperexcitable even though

they could smell and hear other dogs. After being returned to

a normal environment a year later, the dogs still were more

excitable (Melzack, 1954). Walsh and Cummins (1975) found that

52

Page 67: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

rats became hyperactive when their toys were changed.

Interestingly, the EC in the laboratory probably is relatively

simple compared to life outside the laboratory. A garbage dump

or a rural environment has greater complexity than a laboratory

cage filled with objects and otherwise enriched.

Quality of the environment affects behavior. Riittinen et

al. (1986) compared the effects of different environments on

behavior. Weanling mice were subjected to three different

environmental treatments: standard IC, IC with

overstimulation, and EC with objects. In the IC/overstimulated

condition, the mice were subjected involuntarily to intense

sound, light, and vibration. In the EC condition, they were

allowed to initiate and control their interaction with the

objects. Both IC and IC/overstimulated mice were more

irritable than the EC mice. Thus, both forced understimulation

and forced overstimulation increased irritability, as measured

while the mouse was being "attacked" by a bottle brush moving

toward it. The most frequent response to the bottle brush by

EC mice was escape, whereas IC and IC/overstimulated mice had a

higher frequency of beating their forepaws against the side of

the cage.

Increased dendritic branching or spine density does not

necessarily mean that the animal has been in a higher quality

environment. In monkeys, colony-reared animals had less

dendritic branching in the motor I and frontal regions of the

cerebral cortex compared to animals reared in an isolated

53

Page 68: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

condition with ladders, objects, and swings (Stell and Riesen,

1987). Rats exposed to continuous lighting had greater spine

density in the visual cortex (Parnavelas et al., 1973), but

their retinas were damaged (Bennett et al., 1972; O'Steen,

1970). Stimulation of the hippocampus with an implanted

electrode induced axonal growth and reorganization of synapses,

but these new connections increased excitability and seizures

developed (Sutala et al., 1988).

In a developing nervous system, there is an overproduction

of neurons. As the brain matures, excess synapses are

eliminated. This seems to be a kind of biological sculpturing

process. Fifty to 70 percent of developing neurons are

eliminated in some parts of the developing embryonic nervous

system (Oppenheim, 1985). In Rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatto)

excess synapses are still being eliminated before full

behavioral competence (Rakic et al., 1980). Rakic et al.

(1980) suggested that complete maturation may be related to the

elimination of synapses.

Maybe increased dendritic branching in the somotosensory

cortex of the SE pigs was caused by a decrease in synapse

elimination. Intense sensory input from the pig's snout may have

provided stimulation which prevented elimination of synapses.

During the course of the experiment, the pigs' brains were

undergoing rapid development. Brain weight increases rapidly

from birth to 20 to 22 weeks of age (Dickerson and Dobbing,

1966). Cholesterol levels also increase greatly during this

54

Page 69: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

time (Dickerson and Dobbing, 1966). This is an indicator of

myelinization.

Different parts of the brain may respond differentially to

increased sensory input. In our experiment with pigs, and in

that of Stell and Riesen (1987) with monkeys, rearing

environment had no effect on the anatomy of the visual cortex,

but it affected other parts of the brain. In rats, EC-induced

changes in cortical depth were more variable in the

somatosensory cortex than in the visual cortex (Diamond et al.,

1964). Motor I cortex was augmented by increased physical

activity provided in an isolated EC condition (Stell and

Riesen, 1987). In the frontal cortex, there was a tendency for

the colony-reared monkeys to have the lowest levels of

dendritic branching compared to the IC and isolated EC monkeys.

In motor I cortex, monkeys in the two IC and the colony had

similar levels of dendritic branching (Stell and Riesen, 1987).

Increased dendritic growth in the somatosensory cortex of

SE pigs may have occurred in an abnormal manner due to extreme

sensory restriction relative to the snout. There is evidence

that direct stimulation from a sense organ is not the primary

mechanism of environmental enrichment effects on neural

hypertrophy. In cortical weight experiments, the greatest

changes occur in rat visual cortex. Cortical weight is greater

in EC animals kept in total darkness and in blind EC rats

(Krech et al., 1963; Rosenzweig and Bennett, 1969). Diamond et al.

55

Page 70: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

(1972) stated that this indicates that vision is not the

primary cause of the EC effect in the visual cortex of rodents.

In the visual cortex of the rat, effects of environment

and gender interact in complicated ways. Within the visual

cortex, different cell populations respond differentially to

environmental enrichment in male rats as opposed to females

(Juraska, 1934). Apical oblique and basilar branches of layer

III pyramidal neurons had a smaller response to the EC in

females than in males. Layer V pyramidal neurons in both

genders showed equal amounts of dendritic growth.

Even within a single neuron, different types of synapses

react in different ways to environmental enrichment. Beaulier

and Colonnier (1987) found that an enriched environment caused

FS synapses to decrease, and each residual synapse widens.

Although RA synapse numbers do not change, the total contact

area in this case is 16% greater than in FS synapses. These

experiments provide support for the idea that an entire system

of the brain may respond differently to environmental

enrichment compared to another system.

Recent research by Black et al. (1987) revealed that the

paramedian lobule of the cerebellar cortex in rats trained in a

constantly changing complex acrobatic task had a thicker

molecular layer than rats subjected to large amounts of routine

exercise. Exercise on a treadmill or exercise wheel provided far

more physical activity than the acrobatic training (Greenough and

Bailey, 1988). Maybe some parts of the brain have increased

56

Page 71: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

dendritic branching due to quantity of input whereas other areas

will respond only to increased quality of information-carrying

capacity of the input. Another possibility is that the animals

that performed the acrobatic task had to continually maintain

their balance while traversing narrow balance beams and teeter-

totters. The treadmill and exercise wheel would not force the

rats to actively maintain balance. Actively maintaining balance

on an unstable apparatus may have provided additional stimulation

to the cerebellum. Even though the balance center is located

outside the paramedian lobule, balance may affect it. In humans,

defects in both midline and lateral cerebellar zones will cause

gait difficulties (Oilman et al., 1981).

Depending upon the circumstances, there may be three

possible mechanisms which control neural responses to

environment: 1) neural hypertrophy increases in the EC due to

increased sensory input, 2) increased sensory input prevents

normal synapse elimination, or 3) more complex sensory input

due to active exploration and learning causes greater

development of dendrites and synapses.

The most likely explanation for these anomalous results is

that the SE pigs simply had greater nervous input to the

somatosensory cortex. Direct observations indicated that

rooting was more intense in SE pigs. Further research would be

necessary to verify these observations and permit conclusions

to be drawn.

57

Page 72: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

The behavior of the IC pigs resembled the animals'

excitable behavior described in sensory restriction research by

Melzack (1954). Animals raised in an enriched environment were

less excitable than those in barren surroundings (Walsh and

Cummins, 1975). The excessive belly nosing exhibited by the IC

pigs may have been a futile attempt to reduce nervous system

arousal. Repetitive stereotypic behavior can serve a de-

arousal function (Dantzer and Mormede, 1983). The environment

that the IC pigs lived in was much more barren than would ever

be would ever be encountered in nature. In conclusion, the

extent of neuronal development in the pig's somatosensory

cortex cannot be used as a measure of environmental quality.

Another unexpected result was that the barrows had greater

dendritic branching in both the visual and the somatosensory

regions of the cerebral cortex in Trial 1 (Tables 8 and 9),

whereas in Trial 2 the gilts had more. Juraska (1984) found in

rats that gender had different effects in different cortical

layers. For example, there was little gender effect on visual

cortex in layer IV, but a definite effect in layer III. Within

the same gender, cortical layer has an effect on dendritic

branching in rats: "Granule cells with somata in the

superficial third of the granule cell layer had substantially

more dendritic material than those with somata in the deep

portions of the cell layer" (Green and Juraska, 1985).

Both apical and basilar processes were drawn for all

Trial 1 pigs, but the high frequency of apical process

58

Page 73: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

transection rendered these structures useless for data

analysis. In Trial 2, no attempt was made to find cells with

relatively intact apical processes. This facilitated the

finding of more drawable cells close to the surface of the

cortex. Therefore, it is possible that in Trial 1 pigs many

cells were in deeper layers than was the case in Trial 2 pigs.

Further research would be required to verify differential

gender effects in different cortical layers of the pig.

59

Page 74: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

o

5 CO

o

o

"5.

CO

o u CO

8

c re

1 </)

•o c CO

CO

c o +-u 0)

u 01 +-c

o» c s-

CO

+-o

1 —

• o <u c

ja

u 10

ro

J-+-

X <D *> S. o o

;i

1 2 CO * *

-I 5 1 = l-SS

LLi _ J CO

I

s

d 111

2 >

O <n

i 2'

m CM

*"l CM

M N

+ CO CO

<o

s 8

Si

CM

A' O

5?

CM CM,

$1

*

s

* l

8

2I

6'

E

CO

I

CM <©

+ 1 <o' CM CM

CM CM

CM <o

+ <o o CM

+1 A' CO

(M*

CO

CM*

A'

co i n

+ 1 *>' o CM

CO o •

i n o

41 t »

CM

m

CO in

4*» co ~

m o + 1 ©>• <o CM

g

e I

&'

s

I

V

l/l e o u 3 O C

co t o

10 •t-0

+. CI w a

CM

0

JZ o 10 o E 0 i_

i n c o u 3 a> c

V

in

+• e o in O u a. e u e 9

a > c u a U l

e a

o E

o CM

o 4 -

10 3 CT O

m a * O l c

» a c • o

a ^ u 3

<0 u e> E 10 o a

• e • -c o

•& <o u 3 V) 10 a E m u e *-a E

E cs c z

m u m +•

1

g ;

+i

.^ ^

60

Page 75: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

o 10

c CD to o o «/> en a. c

CO

o U co

o CO

c ro

O CO

- a c <o

•o * X

I!

g : e .0 C « -

s s co co

A' II

(A C o

4 -

u CD CO i~ Q) +» C

o> c s-

fO +J o

1—

CM UJ

CO <

•a cu +^ (9 S_ CO

a . cu CO

CO

ro

S-4 -

X cu

+ J s_ o u

:

ii

Me

I 2 1

o co V ,

%l o i n

a CO V , + I S 1

p« o> m

in rn

S

i

m CO

s

8 i l

CO

+ 1 © '

co ©

CO

+ 1 CO1

<o <o

in o

A' i e

CM

CO o

»n o •. ±! s

CM

s s

lo d

A'

8

m

S

. 8

l i co to m

I s O O)

?! a. -o

i I

%

§ s £\ CO

• |

» CM

i l »• p .

CM

£\ zi

g

u> cS

a. * *

o L. 3 O

V

VI 4 -C a i n (0 L. a o u a 3

a >

£ U a w

c a a e

o u o t> u a • o u a •o c a *-m + 1 c

J

• o o t . 3 m a m E i n c. o t 2 B

S O c z

m c o 4 -e E

S c

• o o i> 3 m • o E

J= +• • o

» § — 1 8

61

Page 76: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

X cu t o u

§ to

>

a.

in

£ o u CO

o CO

-a c to

1 to

73 C CO

to e o

4 -o cu t o

s_ cu

4 -s "** o> c J -

ro +-> o

1 -

o UJ — 1 CO

<

•o cu c

•§ o u t o

«S

i . 4 -

*-"'

2

X

si

I!

II. I IS

M

Ji

11 Me

g

m <e o t i n , 4 | i n *

V m V

o <o •

CM

4-1 CM1

C-

r>

CO CO

p» t o

V ,

+ 1 CM1

CM r» V

CM CM

+ 1 CO1

CO

%l Si

o CM

«». 4-1 lO

CO m

V

•», 4-1 t C 1

o> m

& CM

8 * l

§'

rtl

p» m m CM,

+

o> co t o CM

±

R R

A'

m t o

+ | i n '

Ot

t o CO

o CM

1" . t o

£ Ot

*~

o CM

A' Al

V CO

Si

co o>

S'I

© CM

i i

$

e to

t

* '

CO <o

I

I > g 9 Q.

J m o

m o> Q.

CM

"g s

1

8

8 u •o a •o o 3

O

o u a. a u a 3

a >

£ u w 111

, c a a E

O

u o t ,

o • o u a T> c a 4 -l »

+ 1 a i

V m

X

• Q.

5 O

s J= 4 -

C

1 t .

• o c a

» c o u 3 o c

g

*e a c z

i n c S

t E

e c

@ u 3 m a a E

j =

4 -•o

"» a — 1 8

62

Page 77: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

X 01 4 -s-o u

CO 3 to

>

"5. c

to

o u to

o to

c (0

o to

to c o 4 -u cu to fc. cu 4 -C

o> c s.

l O 4 -

o 1 -

«* U J _ i

co <c

•o cu 4 -(O s_ CO Q. cu t o

t o

CO

s-4 - '

"—*

•o

g ;

n

?! - t? -

CM CM

I' -.' co o v i n

CM CM

t l I ' CM tO

s

CM co

co Ot

i} CM

co Ot

4-* m CM CM

CM

CM tO

X

1!

3 S 2l A1

s

s

m to

CM CM

&! co

CM CM

+ g CO

ftl

He

2

S

CM CM

41 r~ r»

CO CM

m CM

4« <o p»

Ot r»

* l t o

CM CO

•» o

4' r» CM

CO CM

co Ot

+ r» t »

*~

V o

+* 1 -r» CM

A' m

£

1

* l

S

m ±1

s

e T I

I1 v> 3

S

I

1 > g | in

e

m o>

I

2

a 3

I s t> •o

u 3

t> 3 m a

i e o o l _ a. • u o 3

a > f u a UJ

. c a a E

0 t_ O t . t . o

T> V. «J •o c CD

4 -Ul

+ 1 a o S

a

V m

X o a. § u © j = 4 -

c

§ u

•o c «3

m g t . 3 a c

o 4 -m J

°I e c z

m t . o tJ E

"5 C

T ) e 3

O e E

J=

•o

1 <D

1 J3

. <S u

63

Page 78: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

(O E O to

X

t—1 Q£ r-•o C (0

8-o

• o

«o

O f P -

C

o 4 -u cu to s . «u

c

o> c J_

(0 4 J

o 4 -c o

4 -

t§ H -<u

i . cu

• o c at

o

i n UJ

CO

2

.-x CU 4 -S_ o o

& CO c cu to o 4 -

1 O to o t

a .

c

j .

S £ a

Q

• o

o t o

"5 * •

8 e 3

a.

£ 4 -T»

»

g :

0.

J= « 4-I 2 t% "5

a.

C

o » 2

£ t L. e i n — I_ » . 10 <B —

1 1 e f — —

a.

j =

4 -• o

S

: # p g

0.

j r

gs 5 >

g St U <B

i n

M l 1 1 f* — *»

i_ o s <D O

l l

5

V co m CM,

^ l . s t »

o

CM

4*1 V

CO

s

CO

CM,

Al co m

O *.

CM CM,

+ 1

s «T

m o

J3 P-

4-1 CO'

|— p .

Ot

a CO v

+'l £ '

m

m 4 -

U

V co m CM,

6' p» m V

CM CM

4*1 i n ' p -

p -

CO

CM, 4> | P » '

p . m

CM CM,

A' i n co m

4-1 CM1

CM

p .

co V

+ 1 t o '

CM m

* o fc o CD

R

© t o

i'l CM

m o

o 4*1 O t ' CO

CM

s

Ot

•, 4 1 t o '

p .

m m

t o m

si &

m o

s «, 4-CO' p .

CM

s

CO

7*i CM'

p .

in 4 -

(9 & Eg

^

o tO

», Zl CM

2 o

+ | ff p .

CM

Ot

• •

+ | <o p>

t o m •. A'

£

s A'I *2 tO

CM

CO

I'l - CM '

P"

m s O 1—

s CD

1 3 •9 C

CO t o

4? 0 4-

m O l

o. CM

0 f

s E 0

m

O t . a « e

% 9 4-C o

1 % «. S 3

B

>

I C

5 S E

O u 2 i_ o

X ) u a 1 a 4 -IA

+ 1 e

.1

1 a e JS

c o E

O CM

2

S !? c

i CO c

8 l_ T»

ID X>

o

s ! o

4-

g "2 m !_ 3 5 1 y o

• f e

J e

c

z

m L. « |

1

• o o l_

m o g E

J= 4 -T»

B

J J

8

64

Page 79: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

to

o o to

o to

c ro

O to

• o c

c o

4 -CJ cu t o i~ cu 4 -C

o> s

s_

€0 4 -

o 4 -

C o 4 -o cu

4 -M -<U s-cu

• o c CU o

•a cu c

J O E o o t o

IO

i . 4 -

X cu 4 -s-o u _ CO 3 t o

> o>

Q .

c

JC 4 -

t o

co

>

H lis

•o

•I

II

f! «• S — b "-

s A'

* '

S

et' m

s I1

s

A' Al

2 S

Ii 5i

s

s p-to

o

+ 1 CO

CM

CO CO

p . CM

4-1 t o ' p .

6 4-1 p . ' p .

CM

P . CM

4-1 t O 1

S 9 m CM, 4-1 t o ' CO o t m

t o CM 4-Ot

CM CO m

s t o ' o*

8

S

K

&'

to <o

Ot

o +1

m e O i_ 3 O C

CO t o

10 4 -O

4 -

M O)

Q .

CM

a 3 4 -u a e

j = 4 -

c o E

o CM

O 4 -

10 3 cr •9 m a * Ol c "x K U

• o

a • o

u

m Ot

•S1 CM i n

« 4 -

o

Ot p .

CM

+ co

i n

m » o u i_ o m

g ±1 Al

— n

£ JI

a e E O L.

in e o t . 3 o c

V

m 4 -C O

s u Q. o u a> 3

10

> j r u IS

a u o

JC 4 -

C O

T> a 3 m a 1 m

a 4 -

1 E O c

z

m

2 ;

J *

65

Page 80: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

t o

2! o o to

o t o

• o sz CO

o

• o c CO

£3 • i — cu 4 - tO u cu to t o — k IO cu "i—

4 - S-C 4 -

Cf> c

c 10

4 -

o 4 -

C O

4 -

u cu

s -« f -

cu s-cu

-o c cu

«o

p ~

U J

_) CO

«=c

X

cu 4->

s-o CJ

>> i . o t o c CU t o o 4-> IO E o t o

CT> o . c

I

JC

II

t\

CM

m

A' A'

m

CO to

In

co — co — ot ©

i ii © p . in © v * m v m

m o

o a

+ 1 O t '

CM CM

© ©

Al CM CM

O ©

4 * |

CM

O ©

4 * i n

©

A ' A ' S § 8 8

A" . v m o

5 ££ .o e •»

g 8

0 .

H

in

o (9

a

m m i n , + 1 CM1

p . m

CM m

I' p> ©

© m K * , 4, m ' O t i n

Ot

* m 4 ©

i n • c

A' Al A' Ot | » . © CM CM CM

3 A' A' A '

p . Ot

S« R V

o m *

S © CM

4 * |

m ©

CM o>

M ,

+ 1 m '

s V

© CM

A' p .

a m co ©

m , m , 4-1 4 v ' m '

8 8 m i n

© © R CM

+ | + | * ' © ' o ©

p* ©

A' 8

© ©

A" I A '

2l * ) A' A« © m

Ot CM

?'| ©

© m

w

© CM

m , + | Ot

Ot m

m * o u 1 .

© CM

i n , 4

p .

CM m

m +.

© CM

m + V i n

i n

* m » o v. L.

CM CM

Al Al

S

g

g

a in 10

2

1

I

? o

i

m

I =

O CO 3 a — a — O CD O

2 CD

!1 x £ £

2 Is •o

15 •o

• I

J

66

Page 81: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

to

o o to

§ to

E ro

•O c CO

(A

g ~ • r - - o 4 - CU CJ 4 -CU ro

t % z s-c to

VI Ol — c 10

o 4 -

c o 4 -o Q>

H -M -CU

$-<u •a £Z cu o

00

UJ —1 CO <

(U 4 -

&. o o __ IO 3 to

> o>

a. c

s

J

•o

Si

If

ill

i n ©

* v , + 1

a m f >

© Ot

Ot V ,

+ 1 © '

Ot V V

* m V

I I s CM ©

m Ot

V V , + © ' V

"» «»

s © CM

+ 1 p . ' a

Ot CM

A'

© R 4*1 O t ' Ot

© CM

4*1 v ' CM

© p. p.

s A' a

§

m o

Al a CM

8

A* CM

i n o

Al ©

CM

5

5i m CM

m o

Al ©

CM

s Al © CM

m o A' © CM

p . ©

A' CM V

CM CM

51 © m

i n m

Al © rS

p . to V ,

A' o ©

Al 3 A'

o

Al

;

H

£ L.

a t p -

•3 4 -

&

*• O o n u a

3 •o

1

4 -O m

|

g

p .

R, i l a p . CM

m Ot

S«M CM

4*1 m ' m

CM

CM

A' CM

R

m CM

Al ©

p .

R, A' © CM m

CM CM

A' V

o R, Al © v

m CM

A*f

8

o J=

4-

g E

g P

§

s

5 5

§ u B JC 4-

g

II A" A*

Al Al Al ±1 p .

CM

2 2 •» •» © <n co ©

Al I ' A' Si

w O in i fc i — <o — O CD O

I

CM ©

I CD

CM

A' m

m 4 -

«

s +*! v ' ©

m B O U l_ a m

CM

Al ©

<n 4 -

o

CM

4*1 O t '

i n B 0 L. t .

CD

it-o •

I i •o v. a

• I

=S

in

•o

67

Page 82: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 4. Location of tissue blocks in the somatosensory and

visual cortex of the pig.

68

Page 83: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

LOCATION OF TISSUE BLOCKS

LOCATION OF REMOVED TISSUE

INDICATES SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX

SOMATOSENSORY CORTEX

LOCATION OF REMOVED TISSUE

INDICATES VISUAL CORTEX

VISUAL CORTEX

69

Page 84: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 5. Location of sampled neurons in the somatosensory

cortex of the pig.

Step A. A line was drawn through center of white

matter and through gyrus on each side.

Step B. Line 1 is drawn through center of white

matter and through gyrus on each side.

Step C. Line 2 is drawn 90 degrees relative to

lines 1 on both sides.

Step 0. Line 3 is drawn 90 degrees relative to

line 2.

Sampling sites A and C are located on line 3 halfway

between the center line and line 2. Sampling site B

is located on the center line.

70

Page 85: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

71

Page 86: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 6. Location of sampled neurons in the visual cortex of

the pig.

Step A. A line was drawn, centered on the sulcas.

Step B. Line 1s were drawn 90 degrees relative

to the center line. The point where

line 1 contacts the top of each gyri was

the sampling area.

72

Page 87: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

m

V

73

Page 88: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 7. Layout of apparatus for testing pig behavior in Trial 1.

74

Page 89: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

GATE

VERTICAL SLIDE GATE ENTRANCE

ARENA

LOCATION OF MAN OR NOVEL OBJECT

^K)

1.2m 2.40m

75

Page 90: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

CJ

s a Q .

cn c co

O

co

co a CO

%

CO

co

2 3 CO

76

Page 91: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

77

Page 92: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 9. Pig pyramidal neuron that complies with the sorting

criteria.

78

Page 93: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

79

Page 94: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 10. Pig pyramidal neuron that complies with the sorting

criteria.

80

Page 95: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

81

Page 96: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 11. Pig pyramidal neuron that has a basilar tree less

than 170 micrometers wide. This neuron does not

comply with the sorting criteria.

82

Page 97: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

83

Page 98: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 12. Pig pyramidal neuron that has an asymmetrical basilar

tree. This neuron does not comply with the

sorting criteria.

84

Page 99: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

85

Page 100: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

to c o L. 3 o c

CO

•o

L. >. Q.

•o CO 4J u o co c 3

u. o

4 -

<o c CO CD

a to CD u CO 3 cr CO

4 J CO CO 0) _ J

c O OT l _ co Q.

o

en a CO

j = 4J

«*-o X CD 4J l_

o CJ

>. l _ o 10 c ai CO o + J CO E O OT CO

J = +»

X co a. E o o co

CO

4 J C co E c o L.

> c CD

CO

a. E

to

co c

•t*. ay L. CO CD l _

CO o> Q .

+J c a) E C o l _

> c 01

£

86

Page 101: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

UJ

X UJ

I i CO o

T I i i

T CO

I CM

I o CO

T CNJ

ONId H0V3 dOd SNOI103SU31NI ONId "1V101 NV3W

37

Page 102: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

I 3 CD C

CO

•o

Q.

u o

2 CO CD

CO

S cr

CO

co CO CD

c o to

1_ CO a

CO

a .

co x :

3 t j 8

I to § CO

CD

X CD

U CO

T3 C CO

!

> c CD

CD

| CO CO

c

•a

§ CO CO

1 I

CO

§>

88

Page 103: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

X uj 3

It CO o t *

I I I I

4 *

— OJ

— CO

— M

CO

UJ

z o cc

CO

CM

T CM

I o

T 00

T CO

T "T CM

9NIU H0V3 yOd SNOI103SH31NI 9NIU 1V101 NV3W

89

Page 104: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

c """ 10 c o l _ 3 CO c

CO • o

E CO l_ >s o. •a CD

4 J L. o CO c 3

l _ O

«t-

co c CO CO E

to co L. CO 3 cr

CO

4 J CO CO CD

CD t_ CO

£ CO O)

a t-o c o OT l _ co a 8 o

CO

a CD

. C 4 J

«*-o X CO .

4 J u o u

CO 3 CO

> CD

j =

+J C co E e o L.

> c CD

X CO

a.

u CO

•o c co

4 J c CD E C O l _

> C CD

CD

a E

10

CO

c

£ CO

90

Page 105: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

UJ _ l Q.

CO

X UJ —I Q.

O O f l

T CM

T o

i 1 ( r CO CO "3- CM

9NIW H0V3 HOd SNOI103SW31NI 5NIW 1V101 NV3IAI

91

Page 106: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

c

CO c p l _ 3 CD C

co •a E CO L.

Q .

T3 CD

4 J L. o OT t_ o

«4-

OT C CO CD E

CO CD L. co 3 cr

CO

4-> CO co o

x» CD L. CO CD u

OT CO

Q.

M-O

C o OT L. ca a.

o

o» a. CD

- C 4 J

M-O

X co 4 J L. o o

CO 3 co

> CO

£

4-> c CD E C O L.

> C co X CD

| s co •o c CO

4 J c CD E C o u

> c CO

CD

a. E

co «o c

4J -r

CO

£ 3 co

92

Page 107: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

UJ

X UJ

* 8 CO O

f I I I I

I-en

I CM

T o

T CO

I CO

I T CM

SNia HOV3 aod SNOiiossasiNi ONia i v io i NVSIAI

93

Page 108: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

V. ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT REDUCES EXCITABILITY IN PIGS

A. Introduction

Rough handling, excitement, and excessive use of electric

prods on swine at the abattoir can have a detrimental effect on

pork quality (Calkins et al., 1980; Barton-Gade, 1985; Grandin,

1986). Some groups of pigs are more excitable and difficult to

handle than others, and these are more likely to be subjected

to excessive electric-prodding because they refuse to move

forward in an orderly manner and pile up more often.

Rearing environment can affect behaviors relevant to

handling. Pigs reared in indoor pens with minimal contact with

people were more excitable and difficult to load into a trailer

compared to pigs reared outside with frequent contact with people

(Warriss et al., 1983). Young pigs reared outdoors with access

to a variety of objects and daily contact with a human approached

both a novel object and a human stranger sooner than did those

reared in small barren pens indoors (Grandin et al., 1983).

In the research reported in this paper, we aimed in

Experiment I to determine the effect of substantial environmental

enrichment on excitability of pigs raised in small barren pens,

and in Experiment II to determine whether less environmental

enrichment would have a similar effect.

94

Page 109: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

B. Experimental Procedure

Experiment I

Animals and housing. Sixty-four Landrace-si red crossbred

pigs weighing 8.7 kg + (SD=.42) on average at the start were

used in a 4-week trial. Four pigs were placed in each of

twelve 1.2 X 1.2-m, raised, solid-sided pens with expanded-

metal floors, under continuous illumination in a windowless,

mechanically ventilated room. The pigs ate from a self-feeder

and drank from a nipple.

Treatments. Two treatments—objects and mingling—were

arranged in 2 X 2 factorial fashion. Objects were used to

enrich some of the pigs' environments. Three 7-cm-wide 41-cm-

long white polyester-cotton strips were suspended to within 16

cm of the pen floor. They were spaced 30 cm apart across the

pen beginning 30 cm from either side and changed daily. A

person mingled with the pigs in some pens for 5 min daily. The

person opened the pen gate and petted all pigs that approached.

Gates on control pens were never opened.

Behavior rating. At the end of the trial, two independent

observers who were unfamiliar with and to both the pigs and the

experimental treatments rated the pigs in terms of

excitability. A rating scale of 1 to 4 was used, where 1 =

calm when person entered pen and 4 = active avoidance of

person. During the rating process, the observer opened the pen

gate and leaned over the pigs.

95

Page 110: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Experiment II

Animals and housing. One hundred twenty-eight Landrace-

sired crossbred pigs weighing 42.5 kg + (SD=2.26) and 43.5 kg +

(SD=2.60) on average at the start were used in Trials 1 and 2,

respectively. In Trial 3, one hundred eighty Hampshire-sired

crossbred pigs weighing 32.8 kg + (SD=2.43) on average at the

start were used.

The pigs were housed in a windowless, mechanically

ventilated building with fluorescent lighting. The lights were

on a timer which turned them on for 10 h during the daytime and

off for 14 h at night.

Four pigs were housed in each of 32 (Trials 1 and 2) or 46

(Trial 3) 1.35 x 2.25-m pens with partially slatted concrete

floors. The partitions between the pens were constructed from

steel bars. Pigs in one pen could observe the activities of the

pigs in adjacent pens.

Trials 1 and 2 lasted 60 days each. Trial 3 was 105 days

long. All animals ate from a self-feeder and drank from a nipple

waterer.

Treatments. Treatments were arranged in a 2 x 2 x 2

factorial fashion, with mingle drive and objects as factors in

Trials 1 and 2. In the control groups, a human never entered the

pens, but the pigs could observe people in the aisles, filling

feeders and mingling treatments in adjacent pens.

In the mingle treatment group, a person entered each pen and

assumed a squatting position. Pigs which approached the person

96

Page 111: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

were petted gently. No attempt was made to pet pigs which failed

to approach. In Trials 1 and 2, mingling was conducted for 10

min per week during the last five weeks of the Trial. In Trial

3, mingling was conducted for 5 min per pen on weeks 8, 9, 10,

11, and 14.

For the driving treatment, a person entered each pen and

moved the animals into the aisle. The pigs were allowed to run

down one aisle and up another back to their pen for 1 min. The

approximate distance traveled was 45 m and they were handled

gently. The only force used was either light tapping with a

plastic stick or blocking with a solid panel. Driving was

conducted on the same weekly schedule as mingling.

In the object treatment group, people never entered the pens

unless objects were combined with another treatment. Three

pieces of black neoprene fiber-reinforced hose were suspended in

each pen from ropes attached to the ceiling. The hoses were 45

cm long with a 2.5-cm outside diameter. They were spaced 45

cm, apart and the ends of the hoses were 25 cm above the floor.

In Trials 1 and 2, the pigs had continuous access to hanging

rubber hoses for the duration of the Trials. In Trial 3, the

hoses were placed in the pen on week seven.

In the assertive mingle treatment (Trial 3 only) a person

entered each pen and assertively approached each pig and petted

it. The person petted every pig, even those that attempted to

move away.

97

Page 112: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Behavior rating. At the end of each trial, two

independent observers who were unfamiliar with and to the pigs

and the experimental treatments rated the pigs in terms of

excitability. The procedure was the same as in Experiment I,

except that in Experiment II the observer walked into each pen

to perform the ratings.

Statistical analysis. The experimental unit was a pen of

four pigs. Excitability ratings in Experiment I were analyzed

with a nonparametric two-factor analysis of variance (Zar,

1984). Excitability ratings in Experiment II's Trials 1 and 2

were analyzed with the a Kruskal-Wallace test.

A multiple comparison Tukey-type test using Dunn's

procedure was used in Trial 3 (Zar, 1984). This test was used

to determine the statistical differences between the

excitability ratings assigned to each treatment.

Weight gain data in all three trials were analyzed

usingthe SAS General Linear Models (analysis of variance)

procedure (SAS, 1982).

C. Results

Experiment I

Treatment had an effect on excitability (P<.03). Control

pigs were more excitable than pigs in any other treatment group

(Table 9). Pigs that had both cloth strips and mingling were

rated less excitable than those that had either cloth strips or

mingling alone.

98

Page 113: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Experiment H

Treatment had an effect on excitability (P<.003). In

Trials 1 and 2, control pigs were more excitable than pigs in

mingle, drive, objects, or combinations of these treatments

(Table 10). In Trial 3, control pigs were more excitable than

those pigs in the assertive mingle, objects and assertive

mingle, and objects and gentle mingle treatments (Table 11,

P<.05).

In Experiment II, there was no treatment effect on body

weight gain. The data are shown on Table 12 for Trials 1 and 2

and on Table 13 for Trial 3. Probability levels were P<.51 for

Trials 1 and 2 and P<.43 for Trial 3.

D. Discussion

In both Experiments I and II, environmental enrichment

procedures reduced excitability. In Experiment I, there was a

tendency for pigs which received both mingling and objects to

be even less excitable. In all trials of Experiment II, pigs

which received two or more environmental enrichment treatments

had a tendency to be less excitable than those which received

only one enrichment treatment. There also was a tendency in

both experiments for objects alone to reduce excitability.

The pigs in Trial 2 of Experiment II were a calm group of

animals, and the environmental enrichment treatments tended to

have less effect in that trial. At the end of the Trial 2, the

control pigs were calm and allowed people to touch them.

99

Page 114: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Conversely, in Trial 1 of Experiment II, the control animals

actively avoided a familiar experimenter when he entered a pen.

In Trial 3 of Experiment II, assertive mingle was more

effective than gentle mingle in reducing excitability. A

possible explanation is that all pigs were petted in the

assertive mingle groups, whereas in the gentle mingle groups

only those that approached the experimenter were petted. It

was surprising that only five minutes of mingling once a week

had such a considerable and consistent effect on excitability.

Placing animals in a restricted environment can make them

more excitable (Melzack, 1954; Walsh and Cummins, 1975; Korn

and Moyer, 1968). Isolation increased reactivity and muscle

tone in mice (Valzelli, 1973). Melzack and Burns (1965) found

that sensory-restricted dogs had highly excitable behavior and

abnormal electroencephalograms (EEG) for as long as 6 mo after

release from the restricted environment. A restricted

environment definitely increases brain excitability. Rats

housed singly in barren cages are more difficult to anesthetize

with sodium pentobarbital than those housed in group cages with

many objects with which to interact (Juraska et al., 1983).

The environmental enrichment procedures used in Experiment

II had no effect on body-weight gain. The absence of an

adverse effect on weight gain may possibly be explained by the

pig's perceptions of the person entering its pen. If an animal

perceives a person as a threat, weight gains may suffer. Pigs

intimidated by a person approaching and looming over them had

100

Page 115: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

reduced weight gain (Gonyou et al., 1986). It was expected

that the assertive mingle treatment pigs in Trial 3 of

Experiment II would lower weight gain. The experimenter in may

have appeared to be less threatening than the experimenter in

Gonyou et al. (1986). In the experiment of Gonyou et al.

(1986), the experimenter walked through the pens but ignored

the pigs. In Trial 3 here, the experimenter approached each

pig and attempted to pet it. Pigs may perceive a person who

invades their flight zone, but does not react to them, as more

threatening than one who invades the animal's flight zone and

attempts positive interaction.

Even though the experimenter petted pigs which attempted

to escape, the animals were never struck or hit. Hemsworth et

al. (1981) also reported that when an experimenter shocked a

pig when it approached, weight gain decreased. The pigs

quickly learned to avoid being shocked by moving away, but they

probably still perceived the experimenter as a threat.

Previous experiments with farm animals indicate that they

can become accustomed to handling (Reid and Mills, 1962;

Thurley and McNatty, 1973; Hughes and Black, 1976; Hails, 1978;

Gross and Siege!, 1983). Cattle readily adapt to daily

weighing with no effect on weight gain (Peischel et al., 1980).

Hens accustomed to handling had no decrease in egg production,

but hens not accustomed to handling had lowered productivity

when handled (Hughes and Black, 1976). Chicks accustomed to

101

Page 116: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

daily handling grew faster and had higher antibody titers than

unhandled chicks (Gross and Siege!, 1983).

In conclusion, simple environmental enrichment procedures

reduce excitability. Reducing excitability and providing

environmental enrichment would improve animal welfare and may

have the residual effect of reducing handling stresses during

transport and slaughter.

102

Page 117: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

TABLE 9. Excitability ratings for pigs housed in enriched and nonenriched

environments (Experiment 1)

Environment

Nonenriched Enriched

Observer control Mingle Objects Both

1 3.67 2.67

2 4.00 1.33

Both 3.83 2.00

3.33

2.33

2.83

1.33

1.00

1.17

103

Page 118: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

c cu

c cu

ro

§

> c cu

4 -

I £

S -

cu C CM

• O C CU 10

IZ in ro

•S. rh c5 -

ft 2 co

£ l2

•l— to

ro CU

u co 2 £

CQ

u u

I i5

s o

e

6

II i !

a

§ e in i n

p.

s

s c<I

e in R

lf>

CM

O o m

o in CM

m p.

CM

s CM

e m CM

S 8 S S CM — — —

o o m

e in m

in CM m

e in CM

o o m

m r>» CM

&

104

Page 119: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

ro 4-

I CU

I CD

m t -cu

• o B 3

XJ cu t . ro cu s.

to en

a .

u o «*-CO CO c

4 -«0 u

>) 4 -

.a ro + i

u X UJ

ro

X. I—

4-> c cu E

k cu a . X

UJ

to c cu E

co cu j -

4 -B cu

u

&. c CU

o +

s o 4

X

s CM

e I S

a

e co

s CM

O tf\

s CM

e o

f>

"s

a r» V

I s

UJ —I

sa

. 1 eg 2

s ? .2 5

II § c3

i

JO

V « - CM

m CM

£ CM

8 #n

R

R "S

S

m m

o <o CM

m CM

CM

O

tf\ m

o m

m CO

•n

a m CO

JO m V

m

e a o

• a M

u a J O

s

8 rl

s

&

& I

105

Page 120: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

t o

i E

£ B CU

I u

i -

s ro

4 -E CU

s B

O s-

> E cu

4 -E cu I _

fc 4 -* o

S-cu E 3

• o CU s-ro £ t o en

• r -O .

<•-o B

SL en

4 -J =

en cu

3

CM

U J — 1

co

g

CM

"CJ E ro

^~ t o

ru

&. 1—

4-i E E s. cu C L X

U J

t 1 J= u c e

UJ

•o a

JC

u e

U l

o 4 -a

+ s

o

+ X

a

a + X

o

4 -O o

JO o

s 5 1

X 5

cc

X

a JC u

~ O c u 9 4 -c e 2 8

a

£

s i'l CM m

p . CO

*.' V m

s ?' © i n

CM

4 | CO1

m m

a V

CM.

+ 1 <o' p . i n

Ot m

Si s

o> IO

II 5?

« o> * %l CO*

m

2

+'l CM1

Ot

Ot CO

Al cJ V

I' m

* p »

. * ' Ot V

CO

i l m

CM V

e t '

5

m 9>

*

t o «s

i l m

CM

CM

"5 £ e

i n o> "a. * t o

a

a

> p -

e

in o i

"a.

* t o

in 4 -C e i n e L.

£ o 3

>

I OJ

JC

I "o

\ 9

ID • o c

J? m • 1

s „*"

106

Page 121: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

TABLE 13. Weight gains of pigs reared under different, environmental

enrichment procedures in Experiment II, Trial 3

Environment

Enriched

Nonenriched Assertive Gentle

control Mingle (AM) Mingle (GM) Objects (0) AM + 0 GM + 0

72.0+0.90* 73.0+1.32 76.1+1.75 70.4+1.06 72.2+1.28 74.6+1.17

Mean _+ standard error of mean (kg). Each value represents 30 pigs.

107

Page 122: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

VI. ENVIRONMENTAL EFFECTS ON EASE OF HANDLING OF PIGS

A. Introduction

When pigs balk and refuse to move, handlers are more likely

to become frustrated. This in turn may result in excessive

electric prodding and even abuse of the animals. Rough

handling and excessive electric-prod usage at the slaughter

plant can have a detrimental effect on pork quality and cause

petechial hemorrhages (Calkins et a!., 1980; Canadian Meat

Council, 1980; Grandin, 1984, 1986; Barton-Gade, 1985).

Rearing environment can affect behavior during handling.

Warriss et al. (1983) reported that pigs reared in indoor pens

with minimal contact with people were more difficult to load

onto a trailer. The main purpose of the study reported here

was to determine if small amounts of environmental enrichment

could affect the pig's ease of handling in a chute. A

secondary purpose was to develop a method for testing a pig's

reaction to handling.

B. Experimental Procedure

Animals and housing

A total of 128 Landrace-si red crossbred pigs weighing 42.5

kg (SD=1.61) and 43.5 kg (SD=1.84) on average at the start were

used in Trials 1 and 2, respectively. They were housed in a

windowless, mechanically ventilated building with fluorescent

108

Page 123: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

lighting (175 lux). The lights were on a timer which turned

them on for 10 h daily during the daytime.

In each trial, four pigs were housed in each of 16 1.35 x

2.25-m pens with partially slotted concrete floors. To provide

additional control subjects against which to compare the

experimental treatments, an additional two pens of control

subjects were added in Trial 2. Partitions between pens were

constructed from steel bars so pigs in one pen could observe

the activities of those in adjacent pens. Pigs ate from self-

feeders and drank from nipples.

Treatments

Both trials lasted 60 d. Treatments were arranged in 2 3

factorial fashion, with mingle, drive, and objects as factors.

In the control groups, a human never entered the pens, but the

pigs could observe people in the aisles, filling feeders, and

mingling in nearby pens.

Mingling was conducted for 10 min once weekly during the

last 5 wk of the trial. In the mingle treatment groups, a

person climbed over the gate and actually entered each pen and

then assumed a squatting position near the center of the pen.

Pigs which approached the person were petted gently. No

attempt was made to pet pigs which did not approach.

Driving was conducted on the same weekly schedule as

mingling. For the drive treatment, a person opened the gate,

entered the pen, and drove the pigs into the aisle. The pigs

then were allowed to run down one aisle and up another, back to

109

Page 124: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

their pen (the distance traveled was approximately 45 m, the

elapsed time <1 min). The pigs were handled gently at all

times. The only force used was either light tapping with a

plastic stick or blocking with a solid wooden panel.

In the objects treatment groups, people never entered the

pens unless the objects were combined with the mingle or the

drive treatment. The pigs had continuous access to the objects

for the entire trial. Three pieces of black neoprene fiber-

reinforced rubber hose were suspended in each pen from woven

cotton ropes attached to the ceiling. The hoses were 45 cm

long, had a 2.5-cm outside diameter, and were spaced 45 cm

apart, and their ends were 25 cm above the floor.

Test apparatus

A 4.8-m-long single-file chute was constructed from plywood.

The layout of the apparatus is shown in Figure 17. The inside

height and width dimensions were similar to chutes used in

commercial slaughter plants (43 cm wide X 70 cm high)

(Grandin, 1982). The sides and floor of the chute were natural

finish plywood. The top consisted of five 2.5-cm-OD galvanized

pipes running the length of the chute. The floor was covered

with expanded-steel mesh.

The crowd pen had 1.2 m-high plywood walls which were

painted white, and the floor was broom-finish concrete. There

were no bars over the top of the crowd pen. Each pig was

admitted to the crowd pen through a 0.9 m-wide steel door which

110

Page 125: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

was painted brown and led directly from a main aisle of the

finishing house.

A single-animal swine scale (Marting Mfg. Co., Britt, IA)

painted bright orange was placed at the far end of the 4.8 m-

long chute. The tailgate of the scale was replaced by a

plywood vertical slide gate.

The entire apparatus was in a window!ess room illuminated

dimly to discourage the pigs from entering the chute. The

room and the entire apparatus were illuminated with two frosted

incandescent lamps. The brightest lamp (60 W) was placed over

the crowd pen, and a 15 W lamp was hung over the scale. Both

lamps were in aluminum reflectors suspended 1.5 m above the

floor. The lighting was designed to discourage the pig from

entering the chute. To further induce balking, a 30 cm-long

brass-plated chain (welded 1.27-cm links) was suspended in the

chute entrance, simulating the shocker chains that some

slaughter plants use to urge stubborn pigs to move.

Test procedure

Each pig was brought individually into the crowd pen, and

the door was closed immediately behind it. A timer was started

and the pig was allowed 2 min to voluntarily enter the chute.

If it failed to enter within 2 min, it was tapped and guided

with a semi-rigid 2.5 cm-OD plastic pipe. The handler reached

over the crowd pen wall to urge the pig. If this failed to

move the pig into the chute within 15 sec, the handler oriented

the pig's head toward the chute entrance and then gave it a

111

Page 126: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

single 2-sec-long shock with a battery-operated prodder

(Hotshot Products, Savage, MN). If one shock failed to move

the pig into the chute, the handler gave the pig additional

shocks as needed to drive it into the chute. Pigs which walked

voluntarily through the chute and onto the scale within 1 min

were not prodded or touched. Pigs which backed up in the chute

were shocked once (as described) with the electric prodder, and

if they continued to back up they were again prodded (as

needed) to prevent further backing and make them move onto the

scale. The crowd pen, chute, and scale were kept wet at all

times. If a pig defecated or urinated in the apparatus, the

excreta was washed away before the next test. The person

handling the pigs was blind to the experimental treatment a

given pig had experienced. The same person handled all the

pigs in both trials. The following force ratings were given to

the urging necessary to accomplish moving a pig through the

chute.

Level I in crowd pen = moves into chute voluntarily within 2 min.

Level 2 in crowd pen = tapped and guided with plastic pipe.

Level 3 in crowd pen = shocked once.

Level 4 in crowd pen = multiple shocks.

Level 1 in chute = moves onto scale voluntarily within 1

min and without backing up (stopping

allowed).

Level 2 in chute = backed up and prodded once.

112

Page 127: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Level 3 in chute = multiple proddings required to prevent

pig from backing out and to drive it

onto the scale.

C. Results

In Trial 1, mingling reduced the force required to drive

pigs through the chute. Control treatment versus all treatment

combinations containing the mingle factor were significantly

different (X2 = 3.84, P<.05) (Tables 14 and 15). A higher

percentage of Trial 1 pigs in the control treatment and drive

treatment groups had to be prodded (Tables 14 and 15). Thirty-

eight percent of the control pigs and 40% of the drive

treatment pigs required level 3 or 4 force in either the crowd

pen or the chute. However, there was a tendency for all

treatments, including drive, to reduce the amount of prodding

performed. Twenty-five percent of the control pigs were

electric-prodded repeatedly (level 4 force), whereas no pig in

any of the treatment groups was (Table 16).

In Trial 2, the control pigs required the least force to

move through the chute (Tables 14 and 15). Comparison of control

pigs with those in all other treatment groups approached

significance (X2 = 3.62, P<.10).

Data for pigs which entered the chute voluntarily (force

level 1) are shown in Table 17. In Trial 1, fewer control animals

entered the chute voluntarily when compared to all other

treatments (X2 = 19.92, P<.001). There were no differences

113

Page 128: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

between control pigs and those in the other treatment groups

for voluntary entry in Trial 2, although there was a tendency

for control pigs to enter more readily.

Treatment had no effect on dunging and urination in the

crowd pen. Treatment also had no significant effect on

vocalization during testing.

D. Discussion

Treatment effects varied greatly between Trials 1 and 2.

In Trial 1, mingling and objects reduced the force required to

move the pigs through the chute. Control pigs also were more

reluctant to enter voluntarily. In Trial 2, conversely, the

control pigs required the least amount of force to move through

the chute. Overall, the control pigs were the most difficult

to handle in Trial 1, but the easiest to handle in Trial 2.

Pigs studied in Trial 2 appeared to be tamer and calmer at

the start of the trial than those studied in Trial 1. This may

be explained by a change in farrowing house manager between the

two trials. The new manager was gentler, and the sows and

piglets under her care appeared to be calmer. Research by

Hemsworth et al. (1986) indicated that handled piglets are more

willing to approach a strange person later. The new manager

may have affected the pigs' behavior.

Ironically, very tame animals are often more difficult to

drive because they no longer have a flight zone (Grandin,

1987). They will no longer move away from an approaching

114

Page 129: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

person. A change in personnel caring for the pigs and applying

the treatments during Trial 2 may have resulted in additional

taming. At the end of the trials, Trial 2 control pigs allowed

people to touch them, whereas Trial 1 control pigs quickly

moved away when approached. The new personnel were very

conscientious about good animal care and may have inadvertently

tamed the Trial 2 control pigs even further.

During Trial 2, time spent washing the pens was increased

from once to twice a week. The animals were often sprayed by

the hose and allowed to play in the water. Increased pen

washing may have tamed the controls. In Trial 2, the control

pigs received stimulation and contact with people which greatly

exceeded normal commercial practice. Casual observations also

indicated that the behavior of Trial 2 control pigs was similar

to pigs in certain treatment groups in Trial 1.

It is unfortunate that the two trials were conducted so

differently. But the results showed clearly that, within

respective trials, the rearing environment did affect the way

the pigs moved through the chute.

These results have implications for the handling of

livestock in farms, stockyards, and slaughter plants.

Observations under commercial conditions indicate that

excessively flighty or wild animals are more likely to pile up,

ram fences, and become injured than calm animals. A few wild

cattle mixed in with a group of tame animals will make the

whole group more excitable. A few excitable, balky pigs mixed

115

Page 130: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

in with calm animals tend to spread excitement throughout the

group.

On the other hand, it is possible to get an animal so tame

that driving it becomes difficult. Cattle which have been

trained to lead with a halter are often difficult to drive.

These animals are accustomed to being led instead of being

driven.

There may be an optimal level of handling and contact with

people for a pig which will be fattened for slaughter. On the

other hand, sows and boars used for breeding probably would

benefit from greater amounts of handling. It is desirable to

have a calm animal that will be easy to drive, but not so tame

that driving is difficult. Excitable animals with excessively

large flight zones may be more likely to panic and become

injured or stressed, especially when confronted by a handler in

a confined area such as a slaughter plant stockyard.

Management procedures which improve the ease of handling at the

slaughter plant would greatly reduce costly bruises and stress-

induced meat quality problems.

116

Page 131: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

o 4" a 4-x

I m CM

to cu

cu

o 4-X

I m CM

en B S. 3 •a

a + o

s p-5t

ro I t>

O + X

"a.

cu • o

2 B . I

u s -4J o cu cu

2 s

to en

I m

s

CO

e s s

o e

I JS

I •g a m •A

1 o o y

s-cu "8 3

•o

If &

w —i co

117

Page 132: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

TABLE 15. Comparison of pigs that were electric-prodded any time

during the test

Environment

Trial Nonenriched control

Containing mingle

Containing objects

Containing drive '

1 3/8 prodded 2/32 prodded 3/32 prodded 7/32 prodded

38% 6% 10% 21%

2 2/15 prodded 9/32 prodded 8/31 prodded 9/31 prodded

13% 28% 26% 29%

No. of pigs prodded (force levels 3 and 4 combined)/total no. of pigs.

118

Page 133: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

co

en E

"C" -o o s. Q . 1 o s-

4 -U CU

0J

" O cu

4 -ro cu O . cu u

•a cu i-

3 cr CU i .

- S u

JC 3C to en n . **-o j .

cu

•o cu E

JD E o u CU

4 -3

J = U

•e) s ro B cu CL

•o 3 o fc. u

4 -t o cu

4 -

CU

t o

a

a 4 X

o 4 X

Q 4-Q

+

U o

I

?

o •g

ii

g

I »

§ 8

s

&

1 *

s 8 s

M CC

g

S

I 3

I 5

o

o

I

I £

£

119

Page 134: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

s-ro 4 -E 3 O > CU

4 -3

J = U

CU

- a

cu

cu -E o

to en

s-<u •§ _ 3

o JC o I .

&

i I >

O 4 a 4 X

+

a +

s

I

I

S

>

S

i n

S co

CM

in

5

CO ©

5 2 CM

e OS c

CO

>. r»

*«. m .. p -

CO

CO " V i n

*» m p . m

CO

o

H

S s : CM IO

3 CO

120

Page 135: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

CO E

c5

co Q .

CO E

4->

O

co 3 t I to

L.

o CO-

co

m

CO

iZ

121

Page 136: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

o

ill!

122

Page 137: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

VII. OBJECT INTERACTION IN WEANLING PIGS

A. Introduction

According to Fagen (1984), "play means behavioral

performance that emphasizes skills for interacting with the

physical and social environments and that occur under

circumstances under which the function of the exercised skills

can not possibly be achieved." Play in animals is difficult to

define. One way to narrow the definition is to determine which

activities are not play. Play is definitely not sleeping,

eating, elimination, mating, or serious fighting. In some

instances, play and exploration may be related. Due to the

difficulty of defining play, the word "play," will be put in

quotation marks.

"Play" occurs in many farm animals, including cattle, sheep,

pigs and horses (Brownlee, 1954, 1984; Tyler, 1972; Schoen et

al., 1976; Sachs and Harris, 1978; Van Putten, 1978; Dobao et

al., 1984-85), as well as a relative of the pig, the collared

peccary (Byers and Beckoff, 1981; Byers, 1984). It is

interesting that in these species both juveniles and adults

"play," because ordinarily in ungulates only juveniles and

subadult males "play" (Byers, 1984).

Object interaction, including "tug-of-war," has been

observed in dogs, cats, and jackals (van Lawick and Goodall,

1971; West, 1977; Fagen, 1981; Martin, 1984). Captive bushdogs

and foxes "play" with sticks (Biben, 1982). A captive

123

Page 138: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

rhinoceros will root a ball and other objects (Inhelder, 1978;

Hediger, 1968). Piglets also engage in object "play"

(Gundlach, 1968; Fraedrich, 1974). Weanling pigs tugged on

cloth strips in a manner similar to that of dogs playing "tug-

of-war" (Grandin et al., 1983).

Farmers have offered pigs various objects in attempts to

enrich their environments and reduce vices. Van Putten (1969)

recommended placing chains or rubber hoses in pens to occupy the

pigs' attention and reduce tail-biting. Weanling pigs will

interact with many kinds of objects, and they vigorously

chewed, tugged, and shook suspended strips of cotton cloth

(Grandin et al., 1983). They rooted balls, boards, and plastic

boxesm and rolled them around. But when the objects became

soiled with excreta, the pigs lost interest in them. Suspended

strips of cloth seemed to hold the pigs' interest longer than

did suspended chains, clean objects more than soiled.

If pigs have preferences, it might be advantageous to give

them access to objects they prefer, to occupy their attention,

reduce vices, and improve health and productivity. In

Experiments I and II, we determined pigs' relative preferences

for different types of objects in short-term exposures. In

Experiments III and IV, we studied the time pigs spent

"playing" with suspended cloth strips (the objects preferred at

first) and characterized daily activity patterns of pigs with

access to objects. In Experiment V, we determined pigs'

relative preferences for different types .paof objects in long-

124

Page 139: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

term exposures. In Experiment VI, the effect of suspended

cloth strips on fighting during mixing was determined.

B. Experimental Procedure

Experiment I—short term object preference: type

Eight Hampshire-sired crossbred pigs were used in each of

three 10-d trials. Two pigs were held in each of four vertical-

rod-sided 1.2 x 1.2-m pens under 24-h illumination (200 lux) in

a windowless room at 22 to 26 C. Pigs weighed 9.7 kg

(SD=0.98), 19.2 kg (SD=1.73), and 21.52 kg (SD=1.85) when

respective trials started. They had ad-libitum access to

pelleted starter diet in a self-feeder and a nipple waterer.

Two sets of three 41 cm-long objects were suspended in

each pen for 15 min some time between 1500 and 1800 h every

day. Objects in each set were: 1) a brass-plated chain

(welded 1.27-cm links), 2) a 5 cm-wide black 100% cotton cloth

strip with a 7-g nut tied to the bottom for ballast, and 3) a

black rubber hose (SAE J CRP, .64-cm ID). The objects were

suspended from a wooden bar which was 71 cm above the pen floor

and centered in the pen, parallel to the feeder. They were

fastened to 23 cm-long strings spaced 30 cm apart.

Each pig was observed for 5 min during each daily 15-min

period of access to objects. Orders of pig observation and of

objects on the bar were randomized daily. The observer stood

in the alley, in front of the feeder. Direct observations of

certain object-oriented behaviors were registered2<<2Datamyte

125

Page 140: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

800, Electro General Corp. (now Datamyte Corp.), Minnetonka,

MN->> on d 3 through 10: 1) frequency of touching each object

(no./5 min, 2) duration of touching each object (min/5 min), 3)

frequency of biting each object (no./5 min), and 4) duration of

biting each object (min/5 min).

Experiment II—cloth object preference: texture

Methods in Experiment II were as in Experiment I except

that 16 Hampshire-sired crossbred pigs, weighing 10.7 kg

(SD=1.41) at the start of Trial 1 and 15.6 kg (SD=1.51) for

Trial 2, were studied, and the experimental play objects were 5

cm-wide white cloth strips made of three kinds of material: 1)

100% cotton bed sheeting, 2) 100% polyester satin, or 3) fake

fur (.95 cm-thick 100% acrylic pile).

Experiment III—daily object interactions

Eighty Landrace-si red crossbred pigs weighing 13.8 kg

(SD=1.26) and 11.5 kg (SD=1.52) when respective trials started

were used, 40 in each of two 28-d trials. Four pigs were

placed in each of 10 solid-sided 1.2 x 1.2-m pens under 24-h

illumination (200 lux) in a windowless room. They had ad-

libitum access to meal starter diet and a nipple waterer. Pigs

in five pens served as controls, while those in each of the

five experimental pens were provided three 7 cm-wide white

cotton-polyester strips suspended to 17.5 cm above the pen

floor from a wooden bar 71 cm above. The strips were changed

daily.

126

Page 141: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

To record object-pulling activity on a 24-h basis, the

strips were tied by a knot to a 3-cm plastic chain link which in

turn.was attached to one end of a heavy string which passed

through a hole in the wooden bar. The string's other end was

attached to one leaf of a spring-loaded switch constructed from a

7.5-cm steel strap hinge. The other leaf of the hinge was

screwed to the top of the bar and an angle bracket with a light

spring attached held the stringed hinge leaf off a screw which

served as the electrical contact. When a pig pulled on a cloth

strip with sufficient force (130 g), it closed the switch by

pulling the hinge leaf against the screw. Each pull was

recorded by an event recorder3<<3Model 2755, Simpson Electric

Co., Chicago, IL.>>. Every object had its own channel. Thus,

the record reflected definite pulls but not certain other uses

of the objects, such as chewing and nosing.

Experiment IV—object interaction behavior

Forty-eight Landrace-si red pigs weighing 9.6 kg (SD=1.60),

8.6 kg (SD=2.88), and 7.7 kg (SO:1.44) when respective trials

started were used in two 7-d trials. In each trial, four pigs

were placed in each of six 1.2 x 1.2-m vertical-rod-sided pens

under 24-h illumination (200 lux) in a windowless room. Every

pen was equipped with suspended cloth objects as in

experimental pens in Experiment III.

Behavior of pigs in half the pens was

videorecorded4<<4Model NV8030, Panasonic Co., Secaucus,

NJ.>>'5<<5Model WV1150A, Panasonic Co., Secaucus, NJ.>> for one

127

Page 142: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

24-h period (d 1 and 6) in each trial, with the camera mounted

directly over the pen. Fighting and play were quantified by

one-zero sampling every 5 min for each pen of mixed pigs

(Lehner, 1979). Head in the feeder and lying, respectively,

were determined by instantaneous sampling every five minutes

(Lehner, 1979).

Experiment V—long-term object preference

Twelve Hampshire-sired crossbred pigs were used in each of

two 13-d trials. The pigs weighed 8.9 kg (SD=0.70) and 11.0 kg

(SD=0.68) at the start of respective trials. Four pigs were

placed in each pen. Objects, pens, and environment were as in

Experiment I. Object-pulling activity was recorded as in

Experiment III. The pigs had continuous access to the objects.

The order of the objects on the bar was randomized daily

(three-dimensional contingency analysis).

Experiment VI—objects and fighting

One-hundred twenty-eight Hampshire-sired crossbred or

purebred Duroc pigs weighing 9.58 kg (SD=1.49) were used in

four trials. Eight pigs from four different litters were used

in each trial. The pigs were weaned and split into two groups

of four. Four pigs from each litter were placed in a pen where

they had continuous access to suspended cloth strips, and the

other four were placed in a pen with no objects. The cloth

strips and pens used during this 7-d period were the same as in

Experiment III. The strips were changed daily.

128

Page 143: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

At the end of this 7-d period (on day 8), two pigs were

removed from each pen for regrouping (the remaining two were

regrouped the next day). On the first day of regrouping, four

pigs completely strange to one another were placed together in

each of four pens as described for Experiment IV. Half of

these pens were equipped with suspended cloth strips (as

described in Experiment III). Thus, there were four treatment

groups: objects before and after regrouping (OBJECTS/OBJECTS),

objects before but not after regrouping (OBJECTS/NONE), no

objects before but objects after regrouping (NONE/OBJECTS), and

no objects either before or after regrouping (NONE/NONE). The

remaining two pigs in each of the original pens were grouped on

day 9.

Each pen of regrouped pigs was observed for 24 h with

time-lapse video equipment (as described for Experiment III).

The video recorders were set at 1/18 normal speed. The data

collection method was the same as described for Experiment IV.

Each pen served as the experimental unit. Fighting and object

interaction data were analyzed with the SAS General Linear

Model (split plot analysis of variance) procedure (SAS, 1982;

Gill, 1986).

C. Results

Experiment I—short term object preference: type

Frequencies and durations of object touching and biting

are given in Table 18. Touching frequencies (mean ± SEM) in

129

Page 144: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Trial 1 were 6.6 ± .6 times/5 min for cloth objects vs. 4.6

± .3 for hose and 3.6 ± .3 for chain (Yates corrected chi-

square = 7.5, P<.05). In Trials 2 and 3, touching frequencies

were similar (CX2-7.29, P<.06).

Biting data indicated that cloth was the preferred object.

Pigs bit cloth objects 14.8 ± .5 times/5 min in Trial 1, 12.6

± .4 in Trial 2, and 13.7 ± .5 in Trial 3. Biting of the hose

was 3.0 + .5 in Trial 1, 9.1 ± .7 in Trial 2, and 6.1 ± .6 in

Trial 3. Chain was a distant third choice, with values of 5.4

± .3, 5.8 ± .5, and 5.6 ± .4, respectively. Chi-square values

for biting preference were 79.83 and 23.74, respectively

(P<.001). Duration of touching and of biting results for all

trials indicated that cloth objects were preferred whereas

chain was often ignored.

Experiment II—cloth object preference: texture

Frequencies and durations of touching and biting the objects

in Experiment II are given in Table 19. There were no significant

differences among objects for the pigs in general, although

individual pigs did have individual preferences. Three pigs

touched acrylic fur objects most often, two polyester, and one

cotton (Yates-corrected chi square = 5.06, P<.05), wheras 10

had no preference. Also, four pigs bit polyester strips most

often, two cotton, two acrylic, and two polyester or cotton

(Yates-corrected chi-square = 7.56, P<.01); six had no

preference. Again, group preferences were not evident, but

some individuals had distinct preferences.

130

Page 145: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Experiment III—daily object interactions

Object "play" tended to decrease over the period of 3

weeks. Mean object interaction time in wk 1 was 68.9 ± 12.0

min in Trial 1, and 60.4 ± 14.8 min in Trial 2. In wk 3, times

were 24.5 ± 4.2 min and 34.5 ± 4.7 min, respectively. There

was considerable variation among groups in time spent "playing"

with objects. During wk 1, interaction time varied from highs

of 143.4 (Trial 1) and 116.9 min (Trial 2) to lows of 35.4 and

10.1, respectively. During wk 3, interaction time varied from

highs of 40.58 (Trial 1) and 64.05 min (Trial 2) to lows of

4.56 and 19.18, respectively.

Object interactions followed a diurnal pattern, with the

two daily peaks coming around 0900 h and 1900 h, respectively.

These tended to be around an hour later in wk 3 than in wk 1

(Table 20).

Experiment IV—relationship of object interaction

and other behaviors

Frequency of object interactions had a tendency to

increase when the pigs were active. Figures 18, 19, 20, and 21

illustrate the number of pigs standing and eating of the 12

pigs in three respective pens. Both "play" and fighting tended

to occur at the same time. Figures 18 and 20 show the first

day of Trials 1 and 2, respectively, and Figures 19 and 21 show

the sixth day.

131

Page 146: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Experiment V—long-term object preference

Frequencies of object pulling are given on Figures 22, 23,

and 24. Object preference changed over the course of the 7-day

experiment. For both trials combined, the mean number of cloth

object pulls on day 1 was 4538 in 24 h. On day 7, mean cloth

pulls declined to 1072. Hose pulling for both trials combined

averaged 1601 pulls on day 1 and increased to 2450 pulls on day

7. Chain pulling averaged 1627 pulls on day 1 and declined to

only 380 pulls on day 7. Chain clearly was the least preferred

object.

During the 7-day period, the pig's preference switched

from cloth to hose. The difference between cloth and hose was

not statistically significant for any day, however the

regression of the difference was significant (P<.01). Figure

25illustrates the regression of the difference between cloth

and hose pulls. During the first 3 days, cloth pulling

decreased at a rate of approximately 717 pulls per day. On day

1 there were 2949 more cloth pulls relative to hose pulls, and

on day 7 there were 1378 more hose pulls relative to cloth

pulls (Figure 23).

Experiment VI—objects and fighting

The presence of objects in the pen during mixing of strange

pigs reduced fighting (Figures 26 and 27) (P<.02). The two

treatments, OBJECTS/OBJECTS and NONE/OBJECTS, had a mean of

23.34 (SD=8.58) and 23.25 (SD=7.96) of 5-min segments with

fighting. The two treatments, NONE/NONE and OBJECTS/NONE, had

132

Page 147: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

increased fighting: 30.62 (SD=9.99) and 33.88 (SD=15.73),

respectively. The greatest amount of fighting occurred during

the first 4 h after regrouping (Figure 27).

The provision of objects for 7 to 8 d prior to mixing had

no effect on fighting (Figure 28) (P=.47). Pigs which had

access to objects prior to mixing had reduced "play" during

mixing (P<.10) (Figures 29 and 30). The data also were

adjusted to exclude 5-min segments which contained both

fighting and object interaction. Access to objects prior to

regrouping also had a tendency to reduce this "clean play"

(P<.10) (Figures 31 and 32).

D. Discussion

Pigs preferred cotton cloth objects as against rubber-hose

or chain in Experiment I. The pigs were smaller in Trial 1 than

in Trials 2 and 3 and seemed to have more difficulty mouthing the

hose, which often would swing away from a pig attempting to mouth

it. The larger pigs in Trials 2 and 3 were better able to grab

the hose, and this might partly explain why it was a close

second choice in Trial 2. The chain was least preferred by all

pigs in all trials. One interesting observation was that often

a dominant pig in a group would push another pig away from a

hanging object even though a duplicate object was available a

few centimeters away.

When long-term preferences were studied in Experiment V,

the pigs initially preferred cloth objects. At the end of the

133

Page 148: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

two 7-day trials, preference switched from cloth to hose

objects. As the pigs' experience with the hose increased, they

became more adept at catching it in their mouths. The change

in object preference may be due to the pigs' learning how to

catch the hose in their mouths.

Pigs directed two basic behaviors toward cloth and hose

objects: 1) while holding the cloth or hose in the mouth, they

would bite it rhythmically (they did not tear the object, but

rather chewed it), and 2) they would perform a tug-shake, like

a dog playing "tug-of-war" with a towel (a pig would grasp

anobject by its teeth and then pull while shaking its head back

and forth). This tug-shake behavior was never observed being

performed with the chain. Perhaps the pigs preferred the cloth

because they could easily and comfortably perform both chewing

and tug-shake behaviors with it. The tug-shake may be the same

behavior a pig uses when it kills prey. Likewise, the badger

seems to imitate its killing shakes when it grasps an object

such as a rag and shakes it repeatedly with horizontal head

motions (Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1978). Infrequently, a pig would nip

a cloth strip apart or pull a hose off its string.

Although duration of object interactions in Experiment III

declined as the trials progressed, pigs were still maintaining

active interest in the objects and devoting considerable time to

"playing" with them at the end of every trial. There was a

similar result in Experiment VI. The presence of objects for

one week prior to regrouping reduced "play" when objects were

134

Page 149: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

introduced at time of regrouping (Figures 29, 30, 31, and 32).

Even though "play" behavior was reduced, the objects still

maintained the pigs' interest. In both Experiments III and VI,

approximately twice as much "play" occurred when the objects

were first introduced.

This suggests that they were being positively rewarded by

this activity. The initial prolonged activity may have been due

to the novelty of the objects, but in Experiment III "play" still

continued for almost 30 min per day after one week of exposure

to the objects. After seven days, the objects presumably were

no longer novel.

Another indicator of rewarding effects of "playing" with

objects was observed in Experiments I and II, in which the pigs

had access to objects for only 15 min daily. After five days

or so, the pigs anticipated the arrival of objects. When the

experimenter approached with the objects, the pigs invariably

would jump up repeatedly and try to touch them before they

could be secured to the overhead bar. The large finishing pigs

used in the excitability experiments reported in Chapter V

exhibited similar behavior; some animals that did not have toys

in their pens waited near the fence and caught a rubber hose

that swung near the fence.

Previous studies had shown that domestic livestock have

daily patterns for grazing and other activities (Hughes and Reid,

1951; Squires, 1974; Arnold and Dudzinski, 1978). Morrison et

al. (1968) found that pigs kept indoors were more active during

135

Page 150: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

the daytime. Two peaks of activity—around 0900 and 1700 h,

respectively—have been observed in pigs, especially in

relatively rich environments (Dantzer and Mailha, 1972;

Dantzer, 1973; Bure, 1981). Curtis and Morris (1982) found

that pigs with operant control of environmental temperature

prefer warm afternoons and cool nights.

The daily patterns of "play" in Experiment III are in

agreement with the daily activity patterns charted by Morrison et

al. (1968).

The placement of cloth objects in the pen during

regrouping reduced the number of 5-min segments which contained

fighting. Changes in the rearing environment can reduce

fighting. McGlone and Curtis (1985) found that providing

small hides where pigs could place their head and shoulders

reduced fighting. A series of maze-like corridors in the

finishing house also reduced fighting (Nehring 1981).

Providing pigs with straw had a tendency to reduce fighting in

fasted pigs, but had no effect on fighting in pigs fed ad-

libitum (Kelley et al., 1980).

The objects may have served to distract the pigs from

fighting. In Experiment IV, both "play" and fighting occurred

during the periods when the pigs were most active. Even though

the presence of cloth objects prior to regrouping reduced

"play" behavior during the 24-h regrouping period (Figure 29),

it had no effect on fighting. The presence or absence of cloth

136

Page 151: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

objects prior to regrouping had no effect on fighting during

regrouping of strange pigs (Figure 26).

There are several conclusions that can be drawn from this

series of experiments. When suspended objects are provided,

pigs have a definite preference for pliable objects. The hard

chain was the least preferred object. Some pigs had individual

differences in their preferences for cloth textures. "Play"

behavior occurs during the time of day when pigs are most

active and suspended objects maintained the pig's interest over

a period of 3 weeks. Suspended objects placed in the pen

during regrouping will reduce fighting in young newly mixed

pigs.

Experiments III and VI indicated that previous exposure

to objects reduced object interaction levels (Table 20; Figures

27 and 28). This may indicate that pigs in small barren pens

require additional stimulation. Perhaps pigs living in a more

stimulating environment would "play" less intensely with

objects placed in their enclosure. Weanling pigs reared in

small barren pens for 65 to 68 days (Chapter IV) appeared to be

seeking stimulation. When people were absent, they unscrewed

bolts. During feeder-cleaning, they rushed up and bit the

experimenter. They also excitedly bit at the water stream

during pen-washing. Pigs in small barren pens also engaged in

greater amounts of belly-nosing (Chapter IV). Van Putten

(1980) found that pigs on partially slatted floors without

137

Page 152: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

access to straw belly-nosed more than did pigs with access to

straw.

Stimulus seeking also has been observed in rodents and

primates. Rats residing in a barren environment will bar press

more than rats in an enriched environment (Ehrlich, 1959).

Rhesus monkeys in an opaque cage worked hard at an operant

discrimination task. Successful completion of the task was

rewarded with a brief glimpse of the laboratory through an open

window (Butler, 1953). Simple practical objects such as cloth

strips or rubber hoses may be able to provide additional

stimulation and improve welfare in agricultural production

environments.

138

Page 153: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

TABLE 18. Frequencies and durations of touching and biting of objects (Experiment 1)

Trial

1

Object

Cloth

Hose

Chain

Frequency (no./5 min)

6.6+,6*'

4.6+.3

3.6+.3

Touching

Duration (min/5 min)

.15+.02

.12+.01

.07+.01

Bitl

Frequency (no./5 min)

14.8+.5

3.O+.5

S.4+.3

ng

Duration (mln/5 min)

2.3+.09

0.2+.05

0.6+.06

2 Cloth

Hose

Chain

8.S+.6

9.0+.5

5.5+.4

.21+.01

.22+.02

.11+.01

12.6+.4

9.1+.7

5.8+.S

1.6+.08

0.7+.08

0.5+.05

3 Cloth

Hose

Chain

7.T+.6

6.0+.4

4.6+.S

.18+.02

.17+.02

.09+.01

13.7+.3

6.1+.6

5.6j ,4

2.0+.O9

.5+.07

.6+.06

Mean + standard error of mean.

139

Page 154: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

TABLE 19. Frequencies and durations of touching and biting

of cloth objects (Experiment II)

Cloth object material

Cotton

Polyester

Acryl ic fur

Pooled SE

Frequency (no./5 min)

7.0

8.1

8.5

.61

Touching

Duration (min/5 min)

.18

.19

.24

.017

Bit ing

Frequency (no./5 min)

8 .3

9.5

8 .2

.59

Duration (min/5 min)

.92

1.21

.80

.097

140

Page 155: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

TABLE 20. Midpoints3 of periods of greatest object pulling

(Experiment 111)

Touching Biting

Trial Wk I Wk 2 Wk 1 Wk 2

1 0730+36* 0954+24 1842+36 2048+42

2 0924+12 0924+18 1976+2.1 1754+24

Both 0827+24 0939+24 1909+28 1901+36

Mean + standard error of mean (time in h).

141

Page 156: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

CO J = 4J

E o co s CO 4J

to Q. O) E

4 J J = CO

«*-" O E CO

>s CO

Q.

>s + J

> 4 J o <

00

CD L. 3 co u.

« i_ p -

o> E

a 3 o u CO CO t_

u CO 4J

•*-CO

>. CO •a 4J to u

4 -

142

Page 157: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

S P - E L

II11 HI II

143

Page 158: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

&

co E l _ CO 4 J 4 J CO Q.

CO E

+* J = CO

4 -

• o E CO

» > t

to Q .

. >. +» > +» U <

CO

c p -

CO E

Q . 3 O l _ CO CD 1_

L . CD

4 J 4 -CO

>. CO

• o

J = JW X

to

CO

CD

CO

144

Page 159: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

145

Page 160: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

CO -C +* E O

CO E u CO

4 J 4J CO Q.

CO E

4 J J = CO

4 -

•o E CO

„ > t CO

a _

>s 4 J

> 4 J o <

. o CM

CO l_ 3 CO

. !«-«. CM

to t_

p -

CO E

a. 3 O l _ CO CD t_

L. CD

+ J 4 -<a >. CO

• a

4 J to i _

4 -

146

Page 161: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

H IP

CO

147

Page 162: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

CD J = 4J

E O CO E

!_ co 4J +* CO a CO E

*i J = CO

4 -

• o E CO

„ >. co a. »

>s 4->

> 4 J O < .

CM

CO l_ 3 CO

. Ctl

CO

V. p~

en E

a. 3 O L. co CO t_

L. CD 4-> 4 -CO

>. CO

• o

J = JJ-> X

CO

148

Page 163: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

o

:l

II

149

Page 164: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 22. Long-term object preference of pigs.

150

Page 165: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

8000-

7000-

6000-

O 5000, cc LU o_ co 4000 _i 3

LU 3000 O

2000-

1000-

• CLOTH

- -* CHAIN

"HOSE

r 4

DAY

*e~ — — — ^ _ — — -©

I I I

5 6 7

151

Page 166: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 23. Long-term object preference (Tr ia l 1).

152

Page 167: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

8000-

7000-

6000-1

Q 5OOO-DC LU Q_

co 4000

• •CLOTH

# # CHAIN

• "HOSE

o_ u_ 3000 O O z 2000-

1000-

f -s

T 2

n r 3 4

DAYS

153

Page 168: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 24. Long-term object preference of pigs (Tr ia l 2) .

154

Page 169: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

8000

7000-

6000-

Q 5000 EC u i CL

^ 4000

o CLOTH

@ CHAIN

"HOSE

£ 3000

2000-

1000

T r 1 2

i r 3 4

DAY

155

Page 170: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Figure 25. Regression of the differences between cloth and hose

pulling. Cloth pulling exceeds hose pulling when the

bars are above the zero line; hose pulling exceeds

cloth pulling when the bars are below the zero line.

156

Page 171: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Q 3000 — 3 E 2500 -

q 2000 -

1500-

1000 •

500 •

0 •

-500 -

-1000

-1500 •

4 5 DAY

7

157

Page 172: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

E

co

CO

E o

CJ CD

? JZ

o CO

&

I CO

2 CO CM

£ 3 co

158

Page 173: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

UJ

uj y

s i LU

I a

I m ° . a

§ s i :

I

I

1 1 1 ,,

s ffi g $ o s O CO 8 §

"T " i n

~T~ o

i

9NIJLH9U HUM SJ.N3WD3S NIW-S JO ON NV3IN iAIlVlflWOOOV

159

Page 174: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

E O

CO E

Q. 3 O V. CO to t_

CO E

L. 3

XJ

CO 4J u CO

• o J 3 O

J = +J o o

4 -o 4J u CO

4 -4 -UJ

P -OJ

CD L. 3 CO

CO E

4 J J = CO

4 -

J = 4->

* CO 4-> E CD e O) CD CO

CD E

+J

E

E 1

I O

4 -O

V. CO

J 3 E 3 E

u.

160

Page 175: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

cc 3 O I

al o CM

CO

CM

CO

• * •

'%$ a. o -J UJ

GR

O

/OB

J

UJ CO CC P-

sW SS O O £^ UJ CO CO P-UJ O CC UJ D. —' 7Z m

£ 0 O UJ UJ z

Las.

[

[

•a o

• CM

CO

CM

CO

—1

ll cc Qj

II I! CO o II o o

CD CM O CO CO ^ - CM

QNI1H9IJ HUM S1N3W93S NIIN-S NV3I I

161

Page 176: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

co j = 4 J

E O

CO E

o. 3 O t_ CO CD l_

o 4 J

L. o V. a. CO

4 J o co •>-»

J 3 O

JC 4 J O

O

4 -O

4 J U CO

.«+•

CO E

*» JZ CO

4 -

J = 4 J

* CO 4-> E CD

§> CD CO

CD E

4 J

E

f i n

4 -o L. CD

j a E 3 E

to LU

CO CM

CO

en

162

Page 177: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

o

a T CO

8 CO

d-s CO u

CD UJ

cTg CO s.

II CC UJ O - Z

3 8

a

-8 CO

CM

CO

s^ CO

a §5

CC LU

Ii

ii I — -*

CO i — i — r CM O 00 CO 'd- CM

9NI1H9U HUM S1N3W93S NIIAI'S NV3W

163

Page 178: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

E O

co 4 J o co •o J 3 O

J Z 4 J O

u JZ 4 J

3

CO u E CD

L. CO a X CO

to 3 o > CO u Q.

4 -O

4 J O m S-

co CO

a •a CO a. 3 o i_ CO CO l_

>. * CD E

E

L. O

> CO JZ CO

- O

>» CO

<M

£ CD

164

Page 179: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

o CD

m u>

o m

w T

o V

CO CO

o CO

m C\J

o CM

NOI10VU31NI103P8O HUM S1N3W93S NIW-S JO ON NV3W 3All\nf1Wn00V

165

Page 180: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

JZ 4 J

E O

CO E

a. 3

o L. CO CO L.

o 4J

(_ o 1_ Q .

co 4 J u CO •o JD O

JZ +» o u

4 -O

*> U CO

4 -4 -U l

O CO

CD L.

u CO u CD

4 J E

+J U CO

f - » J 3 O

JZ +J

* co

4 J E CD E CO CO CO

CD E

4 J

E

E 1

IO

4 -O l _ CD

J 3 E 3 E

co

166

Page 181: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

cc 3 O X

a

o CM

CO

CM

00

i-S

8 co

CM

CO CM CO CO CM

(NOIIOVHSINI sioarao ONVONIIHOU mos HUM siNaiAioss saamoNi) NOI10VH31NI103rSO HUM S1N3^93S NIN-S NV3W

167

Page 182: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

E O

(0 4-> o CD

• O •O O

JZ 4 J O

U

JZ +J

X

co u E CO

L. CD a. X CO

en 3 O

> CD l_ a

4 -O

4 J u CO

4 -4 -U i

CO

*» E co E CO CD co E

E 1

IO

CO CO

a •o CD a. 3 o u CO CO L.

>. 3t co E

E

l_ o

> 1 CD

JO

>s CO

o. t

• o CO 4 J u CO L.

4 J x> 3 CO

E O

+* u 2 to 4 J E

+J U CD

T 3 JO O

" D E co CO E

+J) JZ CO

«c JZ

« JQ

JZ Jp

3e

CO

£ 3 CO

168

Page 183: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

SI r- S3

CO p-

B m 9 m p-o UJ —3 CO O T ?

t

r, o UJ m ci UJ

z c z "

,,

- f

- CO

- CM

o CO

IO LO

o LO

m T

O f

in CO

o CO

in CM

o CM

NOI10VU31NI 103raO HUM S1N3W93S NIIN-S JO ON NV3IAI SAIIVHIWHOOV

169

Page 184: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

L. CO

J 3 E 3 E

CO JZ +J

E O

CO E

Q. 3 O «_ CO CO u o 4 J

l _

o l _ o. CO 4 J u CD

T - » JQ O

JZ *> o CJ

4 -O

4 J U CD

4 -4 -UJ

CO +» E CO E CO CO to CO E

-p

E

E 1

I O

>. CO

"a. : JZ 4J

2

co 4-> E co E CO CD to

CD E

4 J

E

E 1

IO

4 -O

•o to 4 J o £ 4 J JQ 3 CO

E O

*> o co t_ CD *-> E v-

4 J O CO

1-> JQ O

•o E co CO E

4 * JZ CO

4 -

JZ 4 J o J 3

JZ *» s

CM CO

£

170

Page 185: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

cc 3 O X a

8 CO

CM

CO

o

a

8

CO

CM

00

-4 CO CM

T o

T 00 CO CM

CJ3ACM3H 1H9I3 HUM NOI10VU31NI 103rS0 HUM S1N3W93S NIW-9 NV3W

171

Page 186: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

VIII. SUMMARY OF RESULTS AND CONCLUSIONS

AND PROPOSED FUTURE RESEARCH

A. Summary of Results and Conclusions

Dendritic growth in somatosensory region of brain cortex in pigs

residing in a simple or a complex environment.

Rearing environment had a significant effect in dendritic

growth and soma size in the somatosensory cortex of young pigs.

It had no effect on these parameters in the visual cortex.

Pigs residing in pairs in small barren indoor pens (simple

environment, SE) had greater dendritic growth and larger somas

than 12 pigs living together in a large outdoor pen with straw,

play objects, and positive daily contact with a person (complex

environment, CE).

The pigs in the SE engaged in greater amounts of belly

nosing compared to the CE pigs. The CE pigs had greater

overall rooting activity toward a variety of objects, but 95%

of that activity was directed toward objects. Even though the

SE pigs had less overall rooting activity, only some 40% of

their rooting was directed at objects, and approximately 60% of

the time was spent rooting on the other pig. Casual

observations indicated that the SE pigs were more excitable and

aggressive toward the experimenter than the CE pigs.

172

Page 187: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Environmental enrichment reduces excitability jn pigs.

Experiment I

Environmental enrichment reduced excitability in young

weanling pigs which resided in small barren pens. Control pigs

were rated more excitable than pigs which had continuous access

to hanging cloth strips or 5 min of daily petting from a

person. Pigs which had two environmental enrichment treatments

were rated less excitable than pigs which had only one.

Experiment II

Smaller amounts of environmental enrichment also reduced

excitability in older finishing pigs. Controls were rated more

excitable than animals which received continuous access to

hanging rubber hoses, 5 min of weekly petting from a person, or

a weekly drive in the aisle. A combination of environmental

enrichment treatments was more effective than a single

treatment. Environmental enrichment treatments had no effect

on weight gain.

Environmental effects on ease of handling

In Trial 1, environmental enrichment treatments reduced the

force required to move finishing pigs through a single-file

chute. The treatments were continuous access to hanging rubber

hose objects (objects), gentle petting for 10 min weekly

(mingle), weekly drives in the aisle (drive) or a combination

173

Page 188: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

of these treatments. In Trial 2, control pigs required the

least amount of force to drive them through the chute.

Trial 2 pigs were calmer and tamer at the start of the trial

compared to Trial 1 pigs. There may be an optimum level of

handling and contact which will produce a calm market animal that

is easy to drive, but not so tame that driving becomes difficult.

Trial 2 controls were calm and tame due to a change in farrowing

house manager between trials and increased pen washing in Trial

2. Pen washing is a form of handling, and in this case it

served to tame the controls. Trial 2 control pigs were further

tamed by the new farrowing house manager. Pigs in the mingle

treatment in Trial 2 became so tame that they refused to move

through the chute. Control pigs in Trial 1 were excessively

excitable, which made driving more difficult. In conclusion,

for animals that will be marketed for slaughter, it is

desirable to have a calm animal that is easy to drive, but not

so tame that driving is difficult.

Object interaction in weanling pigs

Experiment I

A short-term i5-min preference test indicated that weanling

pigs prefer suspended cloth strips. Chain was the least

preferred object. In three trials, duration of touching and

duration of biting indicated that pigs preferred cloth objects.

Duration of touching and biting of hanging rubber hoses was

174

Page 189: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

reduced because the pigs had difficulty grabbing them with their

mouths.

Experiment II

A short-term 15-min preference test similar to Experiment I

indicated that pigs had individual preferences for different

cloth textures. In general, there were no group preferences but

there were distinct individual preferences.

Experiment III

Chewing and pulling suspended cloth objects decreased over a

period of 3 weeks from an average of approximately 1 hour to 20

to 30 minutes. Even though play decreased, the pigs still

actively played with the cloth strips at the end of the trial.

Experiment IV

Object interactions had a tendency to increase when the pigs

were most active. Play and fighting both occurred more often

when the pigs were standing or eating.

Experiment V

A 7-day preference test indicated that object preferences.

changed over the time. For the first three days of the

experiment, cloth objects were preferred. At the end of the 7-

day period, however, hanging rubber hoses were preferred.

Hanging chains were the least preferred objects all along. The

change from cloth to hose preference may be due to the

development of motor coordination. Observations showed that

175

Page 190: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

small pigs often had difficulty grabbing the hose until they

had had some experience with it.

Experiment VI

The presence of hanging cloth strips during regrouping

reduced the number of 5-min periods which contained fighting.

The presence or absence of hanging cloth objects for seven days

prior to regrouping had no effect on fighting. Providing

hanging cloth strips for seven days prior to regrouping reduced

chewing and pulling of the strips during the 24-hour regrouping

period.

B. General Conclusions

Rearing environment has strong effects on both the central

nervous system and pig behavior. Small amounts of

environmental enrichment reduced excitability and fighting.

The provision of hanging cloth strips during regrouping of

young pigs reduced fighting. Suspended objects and small

amounts of contact with a person in the pen also reduced

excitability.

Environmental enrichment uniformly reduced excitability in

all the experiments, but it affected behavior during handling in

a complex manner. There may be an optimum level of contact with

people for animals which will be marketed for slaughter. Both

excessively excitable and overly tame animals are difficult to

drive and handle. In one trial, regrouping and objects reduced

the force required to drive pigs through a single-file chute.

176

Page 191: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

But, in a second trial, the controls were easiest to drive. In

Trial 2, the pigs in the mingle group were overly tame and thus

difficult to drive. The recommended amount of environmental

enrichment for finishing pigs will vary depending on genetics

and the animals'previous experiences. Management procedures

which make pigs more amenable to handling at the slaughter

plant would help prevent costly bruises. They also would help

improve meat quality because excitement in the stunning chute

increases PSE and other meat quality problems (Barton-Gade,

1985; Grandin, 1986).

There has been increasing societal concern about the welfare

of farm animals residing in certain modern systems which provide

less varied sensory input than natural surroundings (Harrison,

1964; Singer, 1976; Mason and Singer, 1980; Fox, 1984. Some pigs

kept in barren pens on modern farms may be exhibiting symptoms

of sensory restriction. The animals are excitable, as may be

judged, for example, by their jumping up suddenly when a door

slams.

Excitability and increased irritability is a symptom of

sensory restriction. Pairs of puppies residing in barren kennels

become unusually aroused and excited when exposed to something

new (Melzack, 1954). Environmental restriction may have long-

term detrimental effects on the central nervous system. The

young dogs still had abnormal electroencephalographic patterns

6 months after being removed from the barren kennel (Melzack

and Burns, 1965). Animals living in an enriched environment

177

Page 192: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

are usually less excitable than animals in barren surroundings

(Walsh and Cummins, 1975).

The experiments on dendritic branching and soma size in

young pigs indicate that rearing environment has significant

effects on nervous system development. The results were

contrary to the original hypothesis that animals in the

enriched environment (complex environment, CE) would have

greater dendritic branching. Volkmar and Greenough (1972)

found that rats residing in an enriched environment with

objects and other rats had more dendritic branching in the

visual cortex compared to pairs or isolates in plain cages.

Pigs reared in the simple environment (SE) had greater

dendritic branching in the somatosensory cortex. There were no

significant differences in dendritic growth in the visual cortex.

The behavior of the SE pigs differed significantly from

the CE pigs. The SE pigs engaged in greater amounts of belly-

nosing. Increased belly nosing may have stimulated the

somatosensory cortex. As the environment is made increasingly

barren, nibbling and massaging of other pigs will increase

(Stolba, 1981). The SE pigs also were more excitable and

aggressive toward the experimenter.

Rearing environment affects dendritic development in complex

ways. More dendritic development is not necessarily beneficial.

Monkeys reared in the colony had less dendritic branching in the

Motor 1 cortex compared to isolates in a cage with ladders and

swings (Stell and Riesen, 1987). Rats exposed to continuous

178

Page 193: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

lighting had greater spine density in the visual cortex but their

retinas were damaged (Parnavelas et al., 1973; Bennett et al.,

1972; O'Steen, 1970).

Pigs in the SE condition appeared to be actively seeking

stimulation. Walsh and Cummins (1975) state that an organism

becomes increasingly sensitized to stimulation in an attempt to

restore balance. Lack of normal sensory input causes the areas

of the brain that receive sensory input to become more excitable.

Trimming the whiskers of baby rats causes the receptive field in

the brain to become more excitable and enlarged (Simons and Land,

1987). The receptive fields were still enlarged three months

after the whiskers had regrown.

Increased belly-nosing may have been an attempt to obtain

more stimulation and reduce arousal. The SE condition was more

barren than any environment which would exist in nature.

Perhaps the SE environment deprived the animals beyond their

ability to cope. The increased dendritic branching in the

somatosensory cortex of the SE pigs may be highly abnormal.

Experimental results indicate that simple and inexpensive

environmental enrichment procedures such as suspended cloth

strips or hoses and small amounts of increased positive contact

with people will reduce both excitability and fighting in young

pigs. Reducing excitability is advantageous for animal welfare

reasons. Excessive excitability may be a sign of detrimental

effects of sensory restriction in the nervous system.

179

Page 194: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

C. Future Research

To elucidate the environmental factors which affect

dendritic growth in the somatosensory cortex, pigs should be

reared in barren pens with small amounts of environmental

enrichment such as "play" objects and extra contact with

people. The hypothesis would be that the small amounts of

environmental enrichment which reliably reduced excitability

would prevent belly-nosing and therefore prevent the abnormally

increased dendritic growth.

Further experiments also need to be conducted to accurately

quantify the differences between SE and CE pig behavior.

Observations indicated that both the quantity and the quality of

the rooting and nosing behavior was different between SE and CE

pigs. For example, SE pigs appeared to press harder during

rooting.

The effects of environmental enrichment on handling and

meat quality needs further study. Environmental factors affect

handling in a very complicated manner. Handling and care of

piglets in the farrowing house and nursery may have an effect on

driving and handling in chutes later.

180

Page 195: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

LITERATURE CITED

Adrian, E.0. 1943. Afferent areas in the brain of ungulates. Brain 66:7-103.

Aftanas, M. and J.P. Zubek. 1964. Interlimb transfer of changes in tactual acuity following occlusion of a circumscribed area of the skin. Percept. Motor Skills 18:437-442.

Arnold, G.W. and M.L. Dudzinski. 1978. Ethology of Free-Ranging Domestic Animals. Elsevier Scient. Publ. Co., Amsterdam, Netherlands.

Arnold, G.W. and R.A. Mailer. 1977. Effects of nutritional experience in early and adult life on the performance and dietary habits of sheep. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 3:5-26.

Barton-Gade, P. 1985. Developments in the pre-slaughter handling of slaughter animals. Proc. European Meeting of Meat Res. Workers. Paper 1:1, pp 1-6.

Beaulier, C. and M. Colonnier. 1987. Effect of richness of the environment on the cat visual cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 266:478-494.

Bennett, E.L., M.C. Diamond, 0. Krech, and M.R. Rosenzweig. 1964. Chemical and anatomical plasticity of the brain. Science 146:610-619.

Bennett, M.H., R.F. Dyer, and J.D. Dunn. 1972. Light induced retinal degeneration: effect upon light-dark discrimination. Exper. Neurol. 34:434-445.

Bhide, P.B. and K.S. Bedi. 1984. The effects of a lengthy period of environmental diversity on well-fed and previously undernourished rats. II. Synapse-to-neuron ratios. J. Comp. Neurol. 227:305-310.

Biben, M. 1982. Object play and social treatment of prey in bush dogs and crab-eating foxes. Behaviour 79:201-211.

Black, J.E., A.L. Jones, B.J. Anderson, K.R. Isaacs, A.A. Alcantara, and W.T. Greenough. 1987. Cerebellar plasticity: Preliminary evidence that learning rather than repetitive motor exercise, alters cerebellar cortex thickness in middle aged rats. Proc. Soc. Neurosci. 13:1596.

Brownlee, A. 1984. Animal play. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 12:307-312.

181

Page 196: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Brownlee, A. 1954. Play in domestic cattle in Britain: an analysis of its nature. Brit. Vet. J. 110:48-68.

Bryant, M.J. and R. Ewbank. 1972. Some effects of stocking rate and group size upon agonistic behaviour in groups of growing finishing pigs. Brit. Vet. J. 128:64.

Bure, R.G. 1981. Animal well-being and housing systems for piglets. In: W. Sybesma (Ed.), The Welfare of Pigs, pp. 198-206 Martinus Nijhoff Publishers, Boston, MA.

Butler, R.A. 1953. Discrimination learning by rhesus monkeys to visual-exploration motivation. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 46:95.

Byers, J.A. 1984. Play in ungulates. In: P.K. Smith (Ed.), Play in Animals and Humans, pp. 43-65. Basil Blackwell, New York, NY.

Byers, J.A. and M. Bekoff. 1981. Social spacing and cooperative analysis of the collared peccary, Tavassu Tajacu. J. Mammal. 62:767-785.

Calkins, CR., G.W. Davis, N.A. Cole, and D.A. Hutsell. 1980. Incidence of bloodsplashed hams from hogs subjected to certain antemortem handling methods. J. Anim. Sci. 50 (Suppl. 1):15 (Abstr.).

Campbell, A.W. 1905. Histological Studies on the Localisation of Cerebral Function. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Canadian Meat Council. 1980. Guide to PSE Pork. Canadian Meat Council, Islington, Ontario.

Chang, F.L.F. and W.T. Greenough. 1982. Lateralized effects of monocular training on dendritic branching in adult split-brain rats. Brain Res. 232:283-292.

Cross, H.A. and H.F. Harlow. 1965. Prolonged and progressive effects of partial isolation on the behavior of Macaque monkeys. J. Exp. Res. in Personality, Vol. 1, pp. 39-49.

Crowell-Davis, S.L., K.A. Houpt, and L. Kane. 1987. Play development in Welsh pony (Eguus cabal!us) foals. Appl. Anim. Behaviour Sci. 18:119-131.

Curtis, S.E. and G. L. Morris. 1982. Operant supplemental heat in swine nurseries. Proc. Second Int. Livestock Environment Symp. pp. 295-297. Am. Soc. Agr. Eng., St. Joseph, MI.

182

Page 197: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Damasio, A. and R. Maurer. 1978. A neurological model for chi1 hood autism. Arch. Neurol. 35:777-786.

Dantzer, R. 1986. Behavioral, physiological, and functional aspects of stereotyped behavior: A review and re-interpretation. J. Anim. Sci. 62:1776-1786.

Dantzer, R. 1973. Etude de la stabilisation des rhythmes d'activite locomotrice de porcelets introduits dans in nouvel environment. J. Physiol. (Paris) 66:495.

Dantzer, R. and G. Mailha. 1972. Mise en evidence d'un rhythme dans le comportement d'activite motrice du porcelet. C. R So. Biol. 166:456.

Dantzer, R. and P. Mormede. 1983. De-arousal properties of stereotyped behaviour: evidence from pituitary-adrenal correlates in pigs. 1983. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 10:233-244.

Dawkins, M.S. 1982. Elusive concept of preferred group size in domestic hens. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 8:365.

Diamond, M.C., C.A. Ingram, R.E. Johnson, E.L. Bennett, and M.R. Rosenzweig. 1976. Effects of environment on morphology of rat cerebral cortex and hippocampus. J. Neurobiol. 7:75-85.

Diamond, M.C., 0. Krech, and M.R. Rosenzweig. 1964. The effects of an enriched environment on the histology of the rat cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 123:111-120.

Diamond, M.C., F. Law, H. Rhodes, B. Lindner, M.R. Rosenzweig, D. Krech, and E.L. Bennett. 1966. Increases in cortical depth and glia numbers in rats subjected to an enriched environment. J. Comp. Neurol. 128:117-126

Diamond, M.C., M.R. Rosenzweig, E.L. Bennett, B. Lindner, and L. Lyon. 1972. Effects of environmental enrichment and impoverishment on rot cerebral cortex. J. Neurobio. 3:47-64.

Dickerson, J.W.T. and J. Dobbing. 1966. Prenatal and postnatal growth and development of the central nervous system of the pig. Proc. Royal Soc. (London) Bio. Sci. 166:384-406.

183

Page 198: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Dinger, J.E. and E.E. Noiles. 1986. Effect of controlled exercise on libido 2-yr-old stallions. J. Anim. Sci. 62:1220-1223.

Dobao, M.T., J. Rodriganez, and L. Silio. 1984-1985. Choice of companions in- social play in piglets. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 13:259-266.

Dodman, N.H., L. Shuster, M.H. Court, and R. Dixon. 1987. Investigation into the use of narcotic antagonists in the treatment of stereotypic behavior pattern. Crib biting in the horse. Am. J. Vet. Res. 48:311-319.

Duncan, I.J.H. 1978. The interpretation of preference tests in animal behaviour. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 4:197-200.

Ehrlich, A. 1959. The effects of past experience on the rat's response to novelty. Can. J. Psychol. 15:15-19.

Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. 1978. On the ontogeny of behavior of a male badger (Meles meles L.) with particular reference to play behavior. In: D. Mueller-Schwarze (Ed.), Evolution of Play Behavior. Benchmark Papers in Animal Behavior, Vol. 10. Dowden Hutchins Ross, Stroudsburg, PA.

Ewbank, R. 1978. Stereotypies in clinical practice. Proc. 1st World Con. Ethol. Applied to Zootechnics. pp. 499-502. Madrid, Spain.

Ewbank, R. and M.J. Bryant. 1972. Aggressive behaviour amongst groups of domesticated pigs kept at various stocking rates. Anim. Behav. 20:21-28.

Fagen R. 1981. Animal Play Behavior. Oxford University Press, New York, NY.

Fagen, R. 1984. Play and behavioural flexibility. In: P. K. Smith (Ed.), Play in Animals and Humans, pp. 159-173. Basil Blackwell, New York, NY.

Farmer, C. and G.I. Christison. 1982. Selection of perforated floors by newborn and weanling pigs. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 62:1229-1236.

Fleeter, M.K. and W.T. Greenough. 1979. Cerebellar plasticity: modification of Purkinje cell structure by differential rearing in monkeys. Science 206:227-229.

Fox, M.W. 1984. Farm Animals, Husbandry Behavior and Veterinary Practice. University Park Press, Baltimore, MD.

184

Page 199: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Fox, M.W. 1971. Integrative Development of Brain and Behavior in the Dog. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Fraedrich, H. 1974. A comparison of behaviour in the Suidae. Proc. Int. Symp. on Behavior of Ungulates and Its Relation to Management, Vol. I, pp. 133-143. Univ. of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta.

Fraser, D. 1975. The effect of straw on the behavior of sows in tether stalls. Anim. Prod. 21:59-68.

Friend, T. and L. Taylor. 1986. Open field test behavior of growing swine maintained on a concrete floor and pasture. Appl. Anim. Sci. 16:143-148.

Gill, J.L. 1986. Repeated measurement: sensitive tests for experiments with few animals. J. Anim. Sci. 63:943-954.

Oilman, S., J.R. Bloedel, and R. Lechtenberg. 1981. Disorders of the Cerebellum. F.A. Davis Co., Philadelphia, PA.

Glaser, E.M. and H. Van der Loos. 1981. Analysis of thick brain sections by obverse-reverse computer microscopy: application of a new, high clarity Golgi-Nissl stain. J. Neurosci. Methods 4:117-125.

Gonyou, H.W., P.H. Hemsworth, and J.L. Barnett. 1986. Effects of frequent interactions with humans on growing pigs. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 16:269-278.

Grandin, T. 1988. Hog psychology: an aid to handling. Agri-Practice 9(4):22-26.

Grandin, T. 1987. Animal handling in E.O. Price (Ed.), Farm Animal Behavior. Vet. Clin. N. Amer. 3:323-338.

Grandin, T. 1986. Good pig handling improves pork quality. Proc. European Meeting of Meat Res. Workers. Paper 2:12, pp. 105-108.

Grandin, T. 1984. Reducing PSE Losses. Meat Processing, March 23:53.

Grandin, T. 1982. Pig behavior studies applied to slaughter plant design. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 9:141-151.

Grandin, T., S.E. Curtis, and W.T. Greenough. 1983. Objective measurements of the effects of environmental complexity in young pigs. Swine Res. Reports, Report 1983-18.

185

Page 200: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Grandin, T., S.E. Curtis, T.M. Widowski, and J.C. Thurmon. 1986. Electro-immobilization versus mechanical restraint in an avoid-avoid choice test for ewes. J. Anim. Sci. 62:1469-1480.

Grandin, T., D.O. DeMotte, W.T. Greenough, and S.E. Curtis. 1988. Perfusion method for preparing pig brain cortex for Golgi-Cox impregnation. Stain Tech. 63:177-181.

Graves, H.B., K.L. Graves, and G. W. Sherritt. 1978. Social behavior and growth of pigs following mixing during the growing-finishing period. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 4:169-180.

Green, E,J., W.T. Greenough, and B.E. Schlumpf. 1983. Effects of complex or isolated environments on cortical dendrites of middle-aged rats. Brain Res. 264:233-240.

Green, E.J. and J.M. Juraska. 1985. The dendritic morphology of hippocampal dentate granule cells varies with their position in the granule cell layer: a quantitative Golgi study. Exp. Brain Res. 59:582.

Greenough, W.T. 1984. Anatomical substrates of behavioral plasticity. In C.W. Cotman and R.F. Thompson (Eds.) Plasticity in Neurobiology: Cell to Behavior: 1984 Society for Neuroscience Short Course Syllabus. Soc. Neurosci., Washington DC, pp. 42-53

Greenough, W.T. 1975. Experiential modification of the developing brain. Am. Sci. 63:37-46.

Greenough, W.T. 1982. Personal Communication.

Greenough W.T. and C.H. Bailey. 1988. The anatomy of a memory: convergence of results across a diversity of tests. Trends Neurosci. 11:142-147.

Greenough, W.T. and F.F. Chang. 1985. Synaptic structural correlates of information storage in mammalian nervous systems. In: Synaptic Plasticity, pp. 335-363. Guilford Press, New York, NY.

Greenough, W.T., H.N.F. Hwang, and C. Gorman. 1985. Evidence of active synapse formation or altered postsynaptic metabolism in visual cortex of rats reared in complex environments. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. 82:4549.

Greenough, W.T. and J.M. Juraska. 1979. Experience induced changes in fine brain structure: their behavioral implications. In: M. E. Hahn, C. Jensen, and B. C. Dudek (Eds.), Development and Evolution of Brain Size: Behavioral Implications, pp. 295-320. Academic Press, New York.

186

Page 201: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Gross, W.B. and P.B. Siege!. 1983. Socialization, the sequencing of environmental factors, and their effects on weight gain and disease resistance of chickens. Poult. Sci. 62:592-598.

Gundlach, V.H. 1968. Brutforsorge, Brutpflege, Verhaltensontgenese und Tagesperiodek beim Europaeischen Wildschwein (Sus scrofa L.). Z. Tierpsychol. 24:955.

Hails, M.R. 1978. Transport stress in animals: a review. Anim. Regul. Stud. 1:289-343.

Harlow, H.F. and R.R. Zimmerman. 1959. Affectional responses in the infant monkey. Science 130:421-432.

Harrison, R. 1964. Animal Machines. Stuart, London, England.

Hediger, H. 1968. The Psychology and Behavior of Animals in Zoos and Circuses. Dover Publications, New York, NY.

Hemsworth, P.M., J.L. Barnett, and C. Hansen. 1981. The influence of handling by humans on the behaviour, growth and corticosteroids in the juvenile female pig. Horm. Behav. 15:396-403.

Hemsworth, P.M., J.L. Barnett, C. Hansen, and H.W. Gonyou. 1986. The influence of early contact with humans on subsequent behavioral response of pigs to humans. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 15:55-63.

Hitchcock, O.K. and G.D. Hutson. 1979. The movement of sheep on inclines. Australian J. Exp. Agr. Anim. Husb. 19:176-182.

Hughes, B.O. 1976. Preference decisions of domestic hens for wire or litter floors. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 2:155.

Hughes, B.O. and A.J. Black. 1973. The preference of domestic hens for different types of battery cage floor. Brit. Poult. Sci. 14:615-619.

Hughes, B.O. and A.J. Black. 1976. The influence of handling on egg production, egg shell quality and avoidance behavior in hens. Brit. J. Poult. Sci. 17:135-144.

Hughes, G.P. and 0. Reid. 1951. Studies on the behavior of cattle and sheep in relation of the utilization of grass. J. Agric. Sci. 41:350.

Hutson, G.D. 1981. Sheep movement on slatted floors. Australian J. Exp. Agr. Anim. Husb. 21:474-479.

187

Page 202: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Inhelder, E. 1978. Object play in the Indian rhino (Rhinoceros unicornis). In: D. Mueller-Schwarze (Ed), Evolution of Play Behavior. Benchmark Papers in Animal Behavior, Vol. 10. Dowden Hutchinson Ross, Inc., Stroudsburg, PA.

Jones, J.E.T. 1969. The incidence and nature of diseases causing death in pigs aged 2-7 months in a commercial herd. Br. Vet. J. 125:492-505.

Juraska, J. 1987. Personal Communication.

Juraska, J.M. 1984. Sex differences in dendritic response to differential experience in the rat visual cortex. Brain Res. 295:27-34.

Juraska, J.M., J.M. Fitch, C. Henderson, and N. Rivers. 1985. Sex differences in the dendritic branching of dentate granule cells following differential experience. Brain Res. 333:73-80.

Juraska, J.M., W.T. Greenough, and J.W. Conlee. 1983. Differential rearing affects responsiveness of rats to depressant and convulsant drugs. Physiol. Behavior 31:711-715.

Katz, H.B. and C.A. Davis. 1984. Effects of differential environments on the cerebral anatomy of rats as a function of previous and subsequent housing conditions. Exper. Neurol. 83:274-287.

Kelley, K.W., J.J. McGlone, and C.T. Gaskins. 1980. Porcine aggression: measurement and effects of crowding and fasting. J. Anim. Sci. 50:336-344.

Korn, J.H. and K.E. Moyer. 1968. Behavioral effects of isolation in the rat: the role of sex and time of isolation. J. Genetic Psych. 113:263-273.

Krech, 0., M.R. Rosenzweig, and E.L. Bennett. 1963. Effects of complex environment and blindness on rat brain. Arch. Neurol. 8:403-412.

Kruska, 0. 1972. Volumenvergleich optischer hirnzentren bei wild- und hausschweinen. Anat. Entwickl. Gesch. 138:265- 282.

Kruska, 0. 1971. A volumetric comparison of some visual centers in the brains of wild boars and domestic pigs. Z. Anat. Entwickl. 138:265-282

Lehner, P.N. 1979. Handbook of Ethological Methods. Garland STPM Press, New York, NY.

188

Page 203: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Levine, S., G.C. Haltmeyer, G. Karas, and V. Denenberg. 1967. Physiological and behavioral effects of infantile stimulation. Physiol. Behav. 2:55-59.

Markowitz, H. 1982. Behavioral Enrichment in the Zoo. Van Nostrand Reinhold Co. New York, N. Y.

Martin, P. 1984. The (four) whys and wherefores of play in cats: a review of functional, evolutionary, developmental and causal issues. In: P. K. Smith (Ed.), Play in Animals and Humans. Basil Blackwell, New York, NY.

Mason, W.A. 1960. The effects of social restriction on the behavior of rhesus monkeys. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 6:582-589

Mason, J. and P. Singer. 1980. Animal Factories. Crown, New York, NY.

Mayes, H.F. and G.W. Jesse. 1980. Heartrate data in feeder pigs. Tech. Paper No. 80-4023, Amer. Soc. Agric. Engineers, St. Joseph, MI.

McBride, G., J.W. James, and N. Hodgens. 1964. Social behaviour in domestic animals: IV, Growing pigs. Anim. Prod. 6:129-139.

McFarlane, J.M., K.E. Boe, and S.E. Curtis. 1988. Turning and walking by gilts in modified gestation crates. J. Anim. Sci. 66:326-333.

McGlone, J.J. and S.E. Curtis. 1985. Behavior and performance of weanling pigs in pens equipped with hide areas. J. Anim. Sci. 60:20-24.

McGlone, J.J. and J.L. Morrow. 1987. Individual differences among mature boars in T-maze preference for estrous or non-estrous sows. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 17:77-82.

McGray, G.M. 1978. Excessive masturbation of childhood: a symptom of tactile deprivation. Pediatrics 62:277-279.

Meese, G.B. and R. Ewbank. 1973. The establishment and nature of the dominance hierarchy in the domesticated pig. Anim. Behav. 21:326-334.

Melzack, R. 1969. The role of early experience in emotional arousal. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 159:721-730.

Melzack, R. 1954. The genesis of emotional behavior: an experimental study of the dog. J. Comp. Physiol. Psych. 47:166-168.

183

Page 204: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Melzack, R. and S.K. Burns. 1965. Neurophysiological effects of early sensory restriction. Exp. Neurol. 13:163-175.

Melzack, R. and W.R. Thompson. 1956. Effects of early experience on social behavior. Canad. J. Psych. 10:82-90.

Morrison, S.R., H.F. Hintz, and R.L. Givens. 1968. A note on the effect of exercise on behavior and performance of confined swine. Anim. Prod. 10:341.

Nehring, A. 1981. One answer to the confinement pig problem. Int. J. Stud. Anim. Prob. 2(5):256-259.

Oppenheim, R.W. 1985. Naturally occurring cell death during neural development. Trends Neurosci. 8:487-493.

O'Steen. 1970. Retinal and optic nerve serotonin and retinal degeneration as influenced by photoperiod. Exp. Neurol. 27:194-205.

Parnavelas, J.G., A. Globus, and P. Kaups. 1973. Continuous illumination from birth affects spine density of neurons in the visual cortex of the rat. Exp. Neurol. 40:742-747.

Peischel, A., R.R. Schalles, and C E . Owenby. 1980 Effect of stress on calves grazing on Kansas Hills range. J. Anim. Sci. 24 (Suppl. 1):25 (Abstr.).

Phillips P.A., B.K. Thompson, and D. Fraser. 1988. Preference tests on ramp designs for young pigs. Can. J. Anim. Sci. 68:41-48.

Pysh, J.J. and G.M. Weiss. 1979. Exercise during development induces an increase in Purkinje cell dendritic tree size. Science 206:230-232.

Rakic, P., J.P. Bourgeois, M.F. Eckenhoff, N. Zecevic, and P.S. Goldman-Rakic. 1980. Concurrent overproduction of synapses in diverse regions of the primate cerebral cortex. Science 232:232-235.

Randolph, J.H., C.L. Cromwell, and T.S. Stahly. 1981. Effects of group size and space allowance on performance and behavior of swine. J. Anim. Sci. 53:922-927.

Reid, R.L. and S.C Mills. 1962. Studies of the carbohydrate metabolism of sheep. XVI. The adrenal response to physiological stress. Aust. J. Agric. Res. 13:282-426.

190

Page 205: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Riitinen, M.L., F. Lindroos, A. Kimanen, E. Pieninkeroinen, I. Pieninkeroinen, J. Sippola, J. Veilahti, M. Bergstrom, and G. Johansson. 1986. Impoverished rearing and conditions increase stress induced irritability in mice. Devel. Psychobiol. 19:105-111.

Rosenzweig, M.R. and E.L. Bennett. 1969. Effects of differential environments on brain weights and enzyme activities in gerbils, rats and mice. Devel. Psychobiol. 2:87-95.

Rushen, J. 1986. Aversion of sheep to electro-immobilization and physical restraint. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 15:315-324.

Sachs, B.D. and V.S. Harris. 1978. Sex differences and developmental changes in selected juvenile activities (play) of domestic lambs. Anim. Behav. 26:678-684.

Sandman, C.A., P.C Dotta, J. Barron, F.K. Hoehler, C Williams, and J.M. Swanson. 1983. Naloxone attenuates self-abusive behavior in developmentally disabled clients. Appl. Res. in Mental Retardation. 4:5-11.

SAS. 1982. SAS/User's Guide: Statistics. SAS Inst., Inc. Gary, NC

Schoen, A.M.S., E.M. Banks, and S.E. Curtis. 1976. Behavior of young Shetland and Welsh ponies (Equus cabal1 us). Biol. Behav. 1:192-216.

Schultz, 0. 1965. Sensory Restriction. Academic Press, New York, NY.

Shapiro, W. and K.R. Vukovich. 1970. Early experience effects upon cortical dendrites: A proposed model for development. Science 167:292.

Sherritt, G.W., H.B. Graves, and J.L. Gobble. 1974. Effects of mixing pigs during the growing finishing period. J. Anim. Sci. 39:834-837.

Sholl, 0.A. 1956. The Organization of the Cerebral Cortex, Methuen & Co. Ltd., London.

Simons, 0. and P. Land. 1987. Early experience of tactile stimulation influences organization of somatic sensory cortex. Nature 326:694-697.

Singer, P. 1976. Animal Liberation. Random House, New York, NY.

191

Page 206: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Spinelli, D.N., F.E. Jensen, and G. Viana Di Prisco. 1980. Early experience effect on dendritic branching in normally reared kittens. Exper. Neurol. 68:1-11.

Squires, V.R. 1974. Grazing distribution and activity patterns of Merino sheep on Saltbush community in southeast Australia. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 1:17.

Stell, M. and A. Riesen. 1987. Effects of early environments on monkey cortex neuroanatomical changes following somatomotor experience: Effects on layer III pyramidal cells in monkey cortex. Behav. Neurosci. 101:341-346.

Stolba, A. 1981. Alternatives to Intensive Husbandry Systems. Universities Federation for Animal Welfare, South Mimms, Potters Bar, Hertfordshire, England.

Stolba, A. and D.G.M. Wood-Gush. 1980. Arousal and exploration in growing pigs in different environments Appl. Anim. Ethol. 6:382-383.

Struble, R.G. and A.M. Riesen. 1978. Changes in cortical dendritic branching subsequent to partial social isolation in stumptail monkeys. Devel. Psychobio. 11:479-486.

Sutula, T., H. Xiao-Xian, J. Cavazos, and G. Scott. 1988. Synaptic reorganization in the hippocampus induced by abnormal functional activity. Science 239:1147-1150.

Symoens, J. and M. van der Brande. 1969. Prevention and cure of aggressiveness in pigs using the sedative Azaperone. Vet. Rec. 85:64-67.

Thurley, D.C and K.P. McNatty. 1973. Factors affecting peripheral Cortisol levels in unrestricted ewes. Acta Endocrinologica 74:331-338.

Turner, A.M. and W.T. Greenough. 1985. Differential rearing effects on rat visual cortex synapses I. Synaptic and neuronal density and synapses per neuron. Brain Res. 329:195-203.

Tyler, S.J. 1972. The behavior and social organization of the new forest ponies. Anim. Behav. Monog. 5:85-196.

Uylings, H.B.M., K. Kuypers, M.C. Diamond, and W.A.M. Veltman. 1978. Effects of different environments on plasticity of dendrites of cortical pyramidal neurons in adult rats. Exper. Neurol. 62:658-677.

Valzelli, L. 1973. The isolation syndrome in mice. Psychopharmacologia (Berl.) 31:305-320.

192

Page 207: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

van Lawick, H. and J. Goodall. 1971. Innocent Killers. Houghton Mifflin, Boston, MA.

van Putten, G. 1969. An investigation into tail biting among fattening pigs. Brit. Vet. J. 125:511

van Putten, G. 1980. Objective observations on the behaviour of fattening pigs. Anim. Reg. Stud. 3:105-118.

van Putten, G. 1978. The so-called play of piglets reared in flatdeck cages. Deutsche Tieraerztl. Wochenschi. 85:462-464.

van Putten, G. and W.J. Elshof. 1978. Observations on the effect of transport on the well being and lean quality of slaughter pigs. Anim. Regul. Stud. 1:247-271.

van Rooijen, J. 1982. The value of choice tests in assessing welfare of domestic animals. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 8:295-299.

Volkmar, F.R. and W.T. Greenough. 1972. Rearing complexity affects branching of dendrites in the visual cortex of the rat. Science 176:1445-1447.

Walsh, R.N. and R.A. Cummins. 1975. Mechanisms mediating the production of environmentally induced brain changes. Psych. Bull. 82:986-1000.

Warriss, P.O., S.C. Kestin, and J.M. Robinson. 1983. A note on the influence of rearing environment on meat quality of pigs. Meat Sci. 9:271-279.

West, M.J. 1977. Exploration and play with objects in domestic kittens. Develop. Psychobiol. 10:53.

Wood-Gush, D.G.M. and R.G. Beilharz. 1983. The enrichment of a bare environment for animals in confined conditions. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 10-209.

Woolsey, C.N. and 0. Fairman. 1946. Contralateral, ipsilateral, and bilateral representation of cutaneous receptors in somatic areas I and II of the cerebral cortex of pig, sheep, and other mammals. Surgery 19:684-701.

Zar, J.H. 1984. Biostatistical Analysis. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

Zentall, S.S. and T.R. Zentall. 1983. Optimal stimulation: a model for disordered activity and performance in normal and deviant children. Psych. Bull. 94:446-471.

193

Page 208: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Zubek, J.P., J. Flye, and M. Aftanas. 1964a. Cutaneous sensitivity after prolonged visual deprivation. Science 144:1591-1593.

Zubek, J.P., J. Flye, and D. Willows. 1964b. Changes in cutaneous sensitivity after prolonged exposure to unpatterned light. Psychon. Sci. 1:283-284.

194

Page 209: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

VITA

Temple Grandin was born August 29, 1947, at Boston,

Massachusetts. She grew up in the suburbs of Boston and took

her secondary education at Hampshire Country School, Rindge,

New Hampshire. As a girl, Temple was active in the 4-H horse

program for four years.

She attended Franklin Pierce College, also in Rindge,

where she majored in psychology and graduated as salutatorian

with a Bachelor of Arts degree in 1970. At Franklin Pierce,

she served as secretary of the Student Senate and was

recognized by her peers as the class member most likely to

succeed. She was listed in Who's Who Among Students in

American Colleges and Universities in 1970. She was active in

the FPC Psychology Club and handled publicity for the College

Social Committee.

In 1970, Dr. Grandin moved to Arizona to do graduate work

at Arizona State University, Tempe. She was awarded the Master

of Science degree in Animal Science in 1975. While a student

at ASU, she became livestock editor of Arizona Farmer-Ranchman

magazine. In this post, which she held for six years, Dr.

Grandin was an active supporter of 4-H and FFA beef, sheep, and

swine programs all over Arizona. She traveled extensively

around the state, covering livestock shows and sales and

writing feature articles for the magazine. In 1974, she also

worked for Corral Industries in Phoenix, Arizona, designing

feedlot equipment and coordinating advertising.

195

Page 210: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Dr. Grandin became an independent livestock handling

consultant in 1975. She started practice in Arizona, and it

has since grown to cover 35 states, Canada, Mexico, Australia,

Denmark, Norway, England, New Zealand, People's Republic of

China, Saudi Arabia, and Sweden. She has clients in nine

Midwestern states. Dr. Grandin specializes in designing

livestock handling facilities for farms, feedlots, packing

plants, markets, and ranches. She has designed the following

facilities: ranch corrals, complete cattle feedlots,

stockyards, cattle hospital, cattle processing areas, sheep

corrals, chutes and crowd pens for handling cattle, pigs, and

sheep at the slaughter plant, dip vats, Kosher slaughter

systems, restrainer systems for humane slaughter, livestock

auctions, buffalo handling facility, wild horse feedlot and

handling facility, sheep ship loading system, loading ramps,

handling and sorting facility for university research feedlots,

large animal veterinary clinic, hydraulic and pneumatic

activated gates, and other projects. She also consults on

humane slaughter and electrical stunning methods and has worked

with many pork packing plants to improve handling and stunning

methods to reduce PSE and bloodsplash and advises on ways to

reduce bruises and injuries during handling and transportation

of all types of livestock.

The focus of her work is on improving the handling of

livestock and in reducing stress, bruising, and meat quality

problems. She has consulted with or designed equipment for

196

Page 211: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

dozens of firms and individuals, including Red River Feedyard

(then owned by the late John Wayne) in Arizona, Wilson Foods,

Washington State University, Glenn Kluck Feedyards in Nebraska,

Kahn's and Co. in Cincinnati, Illinois veterinarian Joe Connor,

Oscar Mayer in Iowa, Manitoba Hog Marketing Board in Canada,

Moyer Packing in Pennsylvania, John Morrell in North Dakota,

Spencer Foods in Iowa, Texas Tech University in Texas, Canada

Packer's in Canada, Danish Meat Research Institute, Australian

Government, U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service, and USDA Sheep

Experiment station in Idaho. Some of her most recent projects

include a new double rail restrainer system for slaughter

plants and a state-of-the-art stockyard at Hatfield Packing

Company in Pennsylvania.

In 1974, Dr. Grandin joined the Livestock Conservation

Institute and became a member of LCI's Livestock Handling and

Safety Committee. She became chair of the committee in 1976,

and has developed several educational materials on livestock

handling and transportation for LCI. These publications have

been distributed to producers, universities, and packing plants

all over the United States and in many other countries. In

1986, she became a contributing editor to Meat and Poultry

Magazine.

In 1982, Dr. Grandin became a graduate student in the

University of Illinois Department of Animal Science; she earned

her doctoral degree in 1988. She conducted research on pig

behavior under the tutelage of Dr. Stanley Curtis. She

197

Page 212: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

maintains an active consulting practice—as Grandin Livestock

Handling Systems, Inc., Urbana, Illinois—and is in heavy

demand as a speaker at meetings of producers, packers, and

scientists all over the world.

She has 10 refereed scientific papers, 7 refereed

extension publications, 22 abstracts and papers at the American

Society of Animal Science and American Society of Agricultural

Engineers, 27 educational publications, 47 meeting proceedings

papers and reports, and 113 livestock trade publications.

Dr. Grandin participates energetically in several

professional societies, and was a director of the American

Society of Agricultural Consultants. Other memberships include

the American Society of Animal Science and its Midwestern

Section, American Registry of Certified Animal Scientists,

American Meat Science Association, Council for Agricultural

Science and Technology, and American Association for the

Advancement of Science. She is an associate member of the

American Society of Agricultural Engineers.

The Wisconsin Veal Growers Association gave Dr. Grandin

special recognition in 1982 for her work related to the welfare

of veal calves. Livestock Conservation Institute presented her

the Meritorious Service Award in 1984 and she was nominated for

Midwest American Society of Animal Science, Outstanding Young

Extension Industry Specialist Award in 1984.

Dr. Grandin has lectured on livestock handling, facility

design and human slaughter methods in six different animal

198

Page 213: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

science and veterinary courses at the University of Illinois.

She has lectured on livestock handling throughout the United

States, Canada, Australia, and Europe.

199

Page 214: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

PUBLICATIONS

Publications

Grandin, T. 1979. Designing meat packing plant handling facilities for cattle and hogs. Transactions of American Society of Agricultural Engineers 22:912-917.

Grandin, T. 1980. Observations of cattle behavior applied to the design of cattle handling facilities. Applied Animal Ethology 6:19-3.1.

Grandin, T. 1982. Pig behavior studies applied to slaughter plant design. Applied Animal Ethology 9:141-151.

Blackmore, O.K., J.C Newhook, and T. Grandin. 1983 Time of onset of insensibility in four to six week old calves during slaughter. Meat Science 9:145-149.

Grandin, T. 1984/1985. Race system for cattle slaughter plants with 1.5 m radius curves. Applied Animal Ethology 13:295.

Grandin, T., S.E. Curtis, T.M. Widowski, and J.C Thurmon. 1986. Electro-immobilization versus mechanical restraint in an avoid- avoid choice test for ewes. J. Anim. Sci. 62:1469-1480.

Grandin, T. 1986. Minimizing stress in pig handling. Lab Animal 15:15-20.

Grandin, T., O.0. DeMotte, W.T. Greenough, and S.E. Curtis. 1988. Perfusion method for preparing pig brain cortex for Golgi-Cox impregnation. Stain Technology 63:177-181.

Grandin, T. 1988. Double rail restrainer conveyor for livestock handling. Journal Agricultural Engineering Research 41 (In press).

Ray, T.C., L.J. King, and T. Grandin. 1988. The effectiveness of self-initiated vestibular stimulation in producing speech sounds in an autistic child. Journal of Occupational Therapy Research 8:186-190.

200

Page 215: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

Extension Materials

Grandin, T. 1983. Design of cattle handling corals and squeeze chutes. Fact Sheet, Range Beef Cow Management Handbook, Great Plains Extension-9 (GPE-9).

Grandin, T. 1983. Livestock psychology and handling facility design. Fact Sheet, Range Beef Cow Management Handbook, Great Plains Extension-9 (GPE-9).

Grandin, T. 1983. Handling newly arrived feeder cattle. Fact Sheet, Beef Cattle Feeding Handbook, Great Plains Extension-9 (GPE-9).

Grandin, T. 1983. Reducing transportation stress. Fact Sheet, Beef Cattle Feeding Handbook, Great Plains Extensions-9 (GPE-9).

Grandin, T., J.J. McGlone, and K. Ernst. 1989. Hog handling. National Pork Industry Handbook (In press).

Grandin, T. 1989. Sheep behavior and handling principles. Sheep Industry Development Handbook (In press).

Grandin, T. 1989. Corral systems for sheep. Sheep Industry Development Handbook (In press).

201

Page 216: INFORMATION TO USERS colored or poor quality illustrations

such as mingling and objects (hanging rubber hoses) were

more effective than a single treatment. Environmental effects

on handling behavior were complex. In one trial, mingling and

objects reduced the amount of prodding required to move pigs

through a chute. In another trial, mingled pigs were more

difficult to drive. There may be an optimal level of tameness

for pigs which will be marketed for slaughter. In barren pens,

hanging cloth strips (objects) reduced fighting in newly mixed

pigs. One week of exposure to objects prior to mixing reduced

biting and pulling of the objects during mixing. Previous

exposure to objects prior to mixing had no effect on fighting.

When objects were first introduced, activity was intense, and

it leveled off at a lower level. The pigs appear to seek

stimulation. Simple enrichment procedures may alleviate the

effects of sensory restriction.