harris & sim - who is multiracial

15
Who Is Multiracial? Assessing the Complexity of Lived Race Author(s): David R. Harris and Jeremiah Joseph Sim Source: American Sociological Review, Vol. 67, No. 4 (Aug., 2002), pp. 614-627 Published by: American Sociological Association Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3088948 Accessed: 08/11/2009 12:41 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asa. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. American Sociological Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to American Sociological Review. http://www.jstor.org

Upload: andretothie

Post on 20-Apr-2015

27 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

Who Is Multiracial? Assessing the Complexity of Lived RaceAuthor(s): David R. Harris and Jeremiah Joseph SimSource: American Sociological Review, Vol. 67, No. 4 (Aug., 2002), pp. 614-627Published by: American Sociological AssociationStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/3088948Accessed: 08/11/2009 12:41

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available athttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unlessyou have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and youmay use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained athttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=asa.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printedpage of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

American Sociological Association is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access toAmerican Sociological Review.

http://www.jstor.org

Page 2: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

WHO IS MULTIRACIAL? ASSESSING THE COMPLEXITY OF LIVED RACE

DAVID R. HARRIS

University of Michigan JEREMIAH JOSEPH SIM University of Michigan

Patterns of racial classification in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health are examined. The survey's large sample size and multiple indicators of race

permit generalizable claims about patterns and processes of social construction in the racial categorization of adolescents. About 12 percent of youth provide inconsis- tent responses to nearly identical questions about race, context affects one's choice

of a single-race identity, and nearly all patterns and processes of racial classifica- tion depend on which racial groups are involved. The implications of the findings are discussed for users of data on race in general, and for the new census data in

particular.

he 2000 census marked a fundamental change in how we measure race in the

United States. Rather than insist that every person identify with only one racial group, as all previous U.S. censuses have done, the 2000 census instructed household infor- mants to "mark one or more races to indi- cate what this person considers himself/her- self to be" (Nobles 2000). This small change in the wording of the race question will have important consequences for civil rights en- forcement, social science research, and ra- cial politics, as policies and procedures that implicitly assume the mutual exclusivity of racial groups seek to accommodate data that impose no such constraints.

One of the first indications that our offi- cial racial classification system had changed occurred in March 2001 when the Census Bureau announced that 2.4 percent of Ameri- cans identified with two or more racial groups in the 2000 census. This announce-

Direct correspondence to David R. Harris, 426 Thompson Street, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48106-1248 (drharris@umich. edu). We wish to thank Yu Xie, Pamela Smock, Julia Adams, Deborah Malamud, and members of the MacArthur Foundation Social Interactions and Economic Inequality Research Network for help- ful comments on earlier drafts of this paper.

ment received much attention (e.g., Schmitt 2001) and was interpreted by some as mean- ing that race is now less rigid and conse- quential than it was even a few decades ago (e.g., Clegg 2001). Less attention has fo- cused on the possibility that, because race is a social construct, the 2000 census might well have yielded a different estimate of the size of the multiracial population had it se- lected an alternative, but equally plausible, measure.

We address these concerns by examining the fluidity of race for a nationally represen- tative sample of adolescents. Specifically, we (1) contrast counts of multiracial youth obtained from common schemes of racial classification, (2) describe individual-level patterns of consistency in racial reporting, and (3) examine how youth who self-iden- tify as multiracial answer questions that in- sist upon single-race responses. Throughout, we allow for subgroup differences by con- ducting separate analyses for the three larg- est multiracial groups-white/black, white/ Asian, and white/American Indian.1

1 We focus on white/nonwhite multiracial populations because they represent the largest multiracial groups (Suro 1999). Throughout, "A/ B" refers to people who identify with groups A and B.

AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2002, VOL. 67 (AUGUST:614-627) 614

Page 3: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

WHO IS MULTIRACIAL? 615

BACKGROUND

During the nineteenth and early twentieth centuries, American racial ideology was dominated by the perspective that racial groups were biologically determined and im- bued with distinctive physical, mental, and moral abilities (Banton 1983; Gould 1996; Nobles 2000; Spickard 1992). This essential- ist perspective on race later came under at- tack as growing numbers of social and physical scientists argued that there never were pure races, that on almost all traits there is greater variation within racial groups than between them, and that the boundaries of racial groups vary both over time and across social contexts (Davis 1991; Espiritu 1992; Goodman 2000; Gould 1996; Nagel 1994; Spickard 1992). In arguing that there is no objective, biological basis for defining racial groups, advocates of this social con- structionist perspective on race maintain that the function of race is to reinforce and per- petuate social differences (Blauner 1972; Omi 2001). As Spickard (1992) observes, "The process of racial labeling starts with geography, culture, and family ties and runs through economics and politics to biology, and not the other way around" (p. 16).

In addition to arguing that racial group boundaries are subjective, social construc- tionists also maintain that racial identities are fluid (Hahn, Mulinare, and Teutsch 1992; Nagel 1995; Snipp 1997). Echoing re- search on ethnic identity (Alba 1990; Lieberson and Waters 1993; Waters 1990, 1999), they argue that racial classifications can differ not only among nations and his- torical periods, but also in the day-to-day lives of individuals. From this perspective, each individual can be seen as having mul- tiple context-specific racial identities.

Racial identities can be distinguished along several dimensions. What an indi- vidual believes about his or her own race is an internal racial identity. Observers' beliefs about an individual are external racial iden- tities. Moreover, there are expressed racial identities-words and actions that convey beliefs about an individual's race. Identities can be expressed from an individual to an observer, from an observer to an individual, or between observers. Although these iden- tities need not be identical, they are not in-

dependent of one another. This point was long ago recognized by Cooley (1902), and has recently been revived by Nagel (1994), who observes that expressed ethnic identity "is the result of a dialectical process involv- ing internal and external opinions and pro- cesses" (p. 154). As race is socially con- structed, the outcome of this dialectical pro- cess can vary across contexts. In different settings, traits such as phenotype, ancestry, and culture are differently and differentially privileged as criteria for identifying one's race. Thus, as social composition, racial ide- ology, and knowledge about an individual can vary across contexts, there is the poten- tial for variation in internal, external, and expressed identities, and their relationships to one another.

Despite volumes of qualitative and anec- dotal evidence attesting to the fluidity of ra- cial identities (Davis 1991; G. Williams 1995), most empirical studies of race fail to take this point seriously. Rarely are multiple observations of an individual's race ob- tained, and the basis and perspective of ra- cial classifications are rarely made explicit. The implications of this oversimplification of measured race are likely to be especially important for multiracial populations be- cause of the relatively recent establishment of a socially recognized multiracial identity in the United States (Nobles 2000; Office of Management and Budget [OMB] 1997). As America slowly acknowledges that being a member of two or more racial groups is as legitimate as being a member of any single racial group, confusion arises about who is multiracial.

In research, the multiracial population has variously been defined according to how people identify themselves, how they are identified by others, how their ancestors were identified, and combinations of these criteria (Corrin and Cook 1999; Davis 1991; Goldstein and Morning 2000; Hirschman, Alba, and Farley 2000; Kao 1999; Tafoya 2000; Twine 1997; Xie and Goyette 1998). However, if equally plausible racial classifi- cation schemes produce disparate estimates of the size and characteristics of the multira- cial population (Goldstein and Morning 2000; Harris forthcoming; Hirschman et al. 2000) and analysts are not aware of or sen- sitive to these effects, our ability to under-

Page 4: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

6 6 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 1. Selected Measures of Race in the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health

Measure Source of Data Description

School race Interview at school "What is your race? If you are of more than one race, you may choose more than one." Options are white, black or African American, Asian or Pacific Islander, American Indian or Native American, and other."

Home race Interview at home "What is your race? You may give more than one answer." Same response categories as for school race.

Best single race Interview at home If more than one race selected for home race: "Which one category best describes your racial background?"

Parent-based race Interview with parent/ A primary caregiver is asked, "What is your race? You caregiver may give more than one answer." Next, he/she is asked

"What is the race of your current (spouse/partner)? You may give more than one answer." Again, the aforementioned five race categories were the valid responses.

stand the burgeoning multiracial population will be severely compromised. Moreover, because individuals who are defined as mul- tiracial in some schemes appear as mono- racial in others, fluidity in multiracial popu- lations can affect our understanding of single-race populations.

Unfortunately, there have been few empiri- cal assessments of the fluidity of race in the U.S. context (Goldstein and Morning 2000; Hahn et al. 1992). We know that one's racial classification can vary across contexts and observers, but we know little about the mag- nitude and patterns of racial fluidity in the United States, and even less about the cir- cumstances that facilitate these shifts. This gap between the theoretical and empirical lit- eratures is especially troubling because of the Office of Management and Budget's mandate that, by 2003, all federal data collection ef- forts must allow respondents to identify with two or more racial groups (OMB 1997). This decision will produce volumes of data that identify multiracials based on a single racial classification scheme, but there is little un- derstanding of how well race can be captured by any single measure (Harrison forthcom- ing; Root 1992). Here we examine this issue using data for a nationally representative sample of white/black, white/American In- dian, and white/Asian youth.

DATA AND METHODS

We examine data from Wave 1 of the Na- tional Longitudinal Study of Adolescent

Health (Add Health). Add Health is a school-based, longitudinal study of health behaviors for youth in grades 7 through 12. Wave 1 was collected in 1994 and 1995. Ini- tially, 83,135 in-school interviews were con- ducted with students from 80 high schools and 52 middle schools. Next, in-home inter- views were conducted with 18,924 youth from the school sample.2 Last, in-home in- terviews were conducted with a primary caregiver of each of the youth interviewed at home. In over 70 percent of cases, the in- terviewed primary caregiver was the youth's biological mother.3

For our purposes, the key features of Add Health are its large sample size and multiple measures of racial identity. The presence of multiple indicators of race distinguishes Add Health from most other surveys (Table 1). First, Add Health collects self-reported race data. However, rather than collect only one report per person, Add Health allows adoles- cents to identify with up to five racial groups in each of two interviews. If more than one racial group is selected during the home in- terview, youth are then given the opportunity to indicate which single race best describes them. Next, Add Health collects data on

2 Our analyses use weighted data, and we use the survey commands in Stata to correct for oversamples in the home sample, clustering in the school sample, and survey nonresponse (Chantala and Tabor 1999).

3 For more information about the design of Add Health see Bearman, Jones, and Udry (1997).

Page 5: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

WHO IS MULTIRACIAL? 617

coresidential biological parents that can be used to construct a limited measure of an- cestral race. Taken together, these four mea- sures-school race, home race, best single race, and parent-based race-provide rare, generalizable information about the fluidity of racial identity. The first three indicate ex- pressed internal race, while the parent-based measure is a combination of parents' ex- pressed races.

In addition to collecting multiple reports on race, Add Health is also rare in that it supports an assessment of contextual effects on expressed internal race. School and home race items are nearly identical, but whereas the school survey is self-administered, the home survey is administered by an inter- viewer and is often observed by family members. As a result, youth enjoy greater anonymity when reporting their race at school than they do at home. Consistent with Cooley (1902) and Nagel (1994), we expect that home race will therefore be more reflec- tive of the racial conceptions of older gen- erations, while school race will gravitate to- ward contemporary ideals of multiraciality.

Our analysis imposes two restrictions on the data. First, we exclude all youth who identify as Hispanic. This is necessary be- cause Add Health follows the convention of asking separate questions about race and Hispanic origin. In treating Hispanicity as distinct from race, Add Health deviates from conventional academic uses of race that tend to contrast Hispanics with non-Hispanic whites, blacks, and Asians (Farley 1996), as well as understandings of race among His- panics, many of whom treat Hispanic, white, black, Asian, and American Indian as com- parable identifiers (Hirschman et al. 2000; OMB 1997). The two-question approach lowers the threshold for identifying as His- panic (Hirschman et al. 2001) and leads to confusing responses. Comparisons cannot be made between Hispanic and non-Hispanic multiracials, because selecting two or more responses to the race question is very differ- ent from selecting a Hispanic origin in one question and a race in another. Moreover, it is not clear what people mean when they se- lect a Hispanic origin and a race. Some are indicating mixed ancestry (e.g., mestizo mother from Mexico and a white father from Ireland), while others are indicating an an-

cestry and a nationality (e.g., Japanese from Peru, German from Argentina). Given the heightened ambiguity of racial responses for Hispanics, we focus on non-Hispanic youth and leave the examination of racial fluidity among Hispanics to future analyses of data that use a combined race and Hispanic ori- gin question.

Second, to facilitate comparisons of the two self-reported measures of race, we re- strict the sample to youth who were inter- viewed both in school and at home. Twenty- four percent of non-Hispanic youth were only interviewed at home because some school administrators did not approve in- school interviews but allowed school rolls to be used for drawing the in-home sample. Overall, those who were interviewed only at home are quite similar to those interviewed in both contexts, although the former group is somewhat younger (15.3 versus 15.8), more female (50.7 percent versus 46.2 per- cent), and more likely to live in the South (42 percent versus 29 percent). However, the percentage of youth who identify as multira- cial at home is nearly identical across the two samples.

Finally, it is important to acknowledge that Add Health is a study of youth and so cannot be used to make inferences about the fluidity of race for other age groups. Never- theless, we maintain that Add Health is an important source of information about the fluidity of race for at least two reasons. First, there is no other data set that contains mul- tiple indicators of race for a large, represen- tative sample. The 2000 census reports the race of individuals as well as the race of their coresidential parents, but because there is no way of knowing who reports their own race and who has a race assigned to them by the household member who completes the census form, census data are of limited use for studying patterns of racial classification. Second, there is a large and growing litera- ture that relies on self-reported race from data sets such as Add Health, the National Educational Longitudinal Survey, and the National Longitudinal Study of Youth, to as- sess racial differences among adolescents (e.g., Jencks and Phillips 1998; Joyner and Kao 2000; Mouw and Xie 1999). This work implicitly assumes that the size and charac- teristics of racial populations are invariant to

Page 6: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

618 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

10

9

8

* School Race -1 Home Race Parents' Race

9 AO/,

.9% .6% .6% 3% .6% .4%

White/Black White/American Indian White/Asian

Figure 1. Percentage of Adolescents Classified as Multiracial, by Definition of Race: National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health

how race is classified. However, if this as- sumption does not hold, some of what we know about racial differences might be an artifact of how race is measured.

PATTERNS OF RACIAL IDENTITY

We begin by assessing levels of consistency across multiple indicators of race. If racial classification is independent of how it is measured, then all indicators should yield comparable estimates of the size of the mul- tiracial population. However, as Figure 1 clearly shows, different measures of race provide significantly different estimates of the size of the multiracial population. Re- sponses given by youth at school indicate that 6.8 percent are multiracial. By contrast, responses provided by these same youth at home reveal that only 3.6 percent are multi- racial. Yet a third estimate emerges when, following the example of numerous previous studies (e.g., Eschbach 1995; Xie and Goyette 1998), we limit the sample to youth who live with both biological parents and define multiracial adolescents as those whose parents do not identify with the same monoracial group. This common definition suggests that 4.8 percent of youth are multi- racial. A fourth estimate, not shown in Fig-

ure 1, is based on our analysis of Summary File 1 data from the 2000 census. It reveals that 2.5 percent of non-Hispanic youth be- tween the ages of 12 and 18 were identified as multiracial.

Figure 1 also shows great variation in pat- terns of expressed internal race for multira- cial populations. At one extreme are white/ black and white/Asian youth: Both at home and at school, .6 percent of youth identify as white and black. Similarly, the share of youth identifying as white and Asian does not significantly differ between the school and home interviews. At the other extreme are white/American Indian youth. Although 2.4 percent of youth identify as white and American Indian at school, a significantly smaller share claim a white/American Indian identity at home (1.5 percent). These find- ings confirm that with respect to racial self- identification there is not a single multira- cial experience.

Table 2 extends our analysis of school and home race by contrasting individual-level responses. Column 1 reports patterns of school and home race for all adolescents. Our results provide strong support for a cen- tral tenet of the social constructionist per- spective-that racial identities are fluid. In contrast to the assumptions implicit in most

7

6

5

4

3

',

0

1)

0)

1,

1) 0

3.60/

2

1

0 All Multiracial

f Ro0/

Page 7: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

WHO IS MULTIRACIAL? 619

Table 2. Percentage Distribution of Adolescents by Home- and School-Expressed Racial Identities, by Parents' Race

Parents' Race

Missing Does Not Live Parent

with Both Data Expressed Racial Identity All Mixed Not Mixed Biological Parents Incomplete

Consistent Racial Identity Same monoracial identity in 86.5 36.5 91.3 83.9 87.1

both contexts

Same multiracial identity in 1.1 11.9 .2 1.6 1.1 both contexts

Inconsistent Racial Identity Different monoracial identity 2.8 7.3 2.0 3.3 3.0

in each context Different multiracial identity .5 7.2 .1 .5 .5

in each context Multiracial at home, monoracial 2.0 19.6 1.1 2.0 2.1

at school

Monoracial at home, multiracial 5.0 16.0 3.6 6.1 4.4 at school

"Refused" in either context 2.2 1.5 1.7 2.7 2.0

Total 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0 100.0

Unweighted N 11,531 269 4,695 4,991 1,576

Weighted percentage of all 100.0 2.2 44.7 41.6 11.4 adolescents

Note: Weighted data for non-Hispanics with valid home and school race data.

empirical research, we find that only 87.6 percent of adolescents express identical ra- cial identities across contexts.4 Table 2 fur- ther illustrates that as a result of the fluidity of racial identities, youth who identify with more than one racial group at home are not simply a subset of those who identify as multiracial at school. While 8.6 percent of youth report being multiracial at home or in school, only 1.6 percent identify themselves as multiracial in both contexts, and only 1.1 percent of youth select the same combina- tion of two or more racial groups at home and in school. Consequently, 54 percent of the home multiracial population are not mul- tiracial in school data, and 75 percent of the

4 We find no support for the hypothesis that temporal gaps between school and home inter- views explain inconsistencies in expressed race. Unreported analyses show no relationship be- tween the probability of shifting expressed racial identity and the amount of time between inter- views.

school multiracial population are not multi- racial in home data. The two multiracial populations are clearly overlapping, yet most youth who report being multiracial in one context identify as monoracial in the other context. Last, column 1 of Table 2 shows that 2.8 percent of youth express a multiracial identity by switching between single-race identities, rather than by select- ing two or more racial groups in response to a particular race question. This group of multiracial youth is indistinguishable from monoracial youth in surveys that fail to col- lect multiple reports of race.

The remainder of Table 2 summarizes pat- terns of school and home race by parents' race. These results show that having parents who claim to be from different racial groups is neither a necessary nor sufficient condi- tion for expressing a multiracial identity. Column 2 summarizes racial identities for adolescents who are either the product of an interracial union, or have at least one bio-

Page 8: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

620 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 3. Percentage Distribution of Race at School for Adolescents Who Identify with Selected Multiracial Groups at Home

Race at Home

Race at School White/Black White/American Indian White/Asian

White .9 46.0 13.4

Black 20.8 .1

American Indian 20.0

Asian .7 21.8

Other 4.7 .7 2.4

White/black 59.5

White/American Indian 1.2 | 24.1 White/Asian - 45.91 Other biracial 2.5 1.0 7.3

Three or more racial groups 7.4 7.3 9.1

No answer 2.9

Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Unweighted N 108 159 82

Note: Cells are weighted column percentages. Boxed cells indicate agreement between school and home race.

logical parent who reports being multiracial. If race were simply hereditary, then we would expect these youth to self-identify as multiracial. However, the data show a marked deviation from this expectation. Al- though 54.7 percent of these adolescents do report being multiracial at least once, only 11.9 percent report the same multiracial identity in both contexts. An additional 7.3 percent of this group selects different single- race identities in the two surveys, which means that no more than two-thirds of youth with known multiracial ancestry express a multiracial identity. This finding confirms that having parents of different races is not a sufficient condition for expressing a multi- racial identity (Davis 1991).

Although researchers have long known that some youth with multiracial ancestry identify as monoracial (DuBois [1899] 1996), previous work has largely assumed that having parents from different racial groups, or at least one parent who is multira- cial, is a necessary condition for expressing a multiracial identity (Tafoya 2000). Column 3, which summarizes expressed internal race responses for adolescents whose parents re- port being from the same monoracial group, suggests otherwise: Five percent of these youth report being multiracial at least once,

and another 2 percent offer different single- race identities to the two race questions.

Column 4 in Table 2 again contrasts re- sponses to the school and home race ques- tions, though only for youth who do not live with both biological parents and thus lack appropriate data for the parent-based race measure. This is the group that is missing in studies that rely upon parents' race to iden- tify multiracial youth (e.g., Eschbach 1995; Xie and Goyette 1998). Although it has long been suspected that excluding such individu- als from analyses of multiracial children might be problematic (Root 1992), the results in Table 2 provide an empirical basis for this concern. Compared with youth who live with both biological parents, adolescents who do not live with both biological parents are sig- nificantly less likely to give the same single- race response to the school and home race questions (83.9 percent versus 88.6 percent) and are significantly more likely to ever re- port being multiracial (10.3 percent versus 7.4 percent). Our findings suggest that ado- lescents who do not live with both biological parents are more likely than other adoles- cents to express a multiracial identity, which implies that analyses of youth who live with both biological parents focus on a select sub- set of the multiracial population.

Page 9: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

WHO IS MULTIRACIAL? 621

Although the summary statistics in Table 2 provide an overall portrait of patterns of agreement between school and home re- sponses, they do not show specific transi- tions. This further information is revealed in Table 3, which reports school race for ado- lescents who identify with one of three mul- tiracial groups at home. Again we see sig- nificant diversity in the consistency of iden- tifying as white/black, white/American In- dian, or white/Asian. A fairly high level of consistency is observed for white/black youth, though even for this group 40 percent of youth who identify as white/black at home identify differently in school. Twenty- one percent identify as black, while 7.4 per- cent identify with three or more racial groups-most often white, black, and American Indian or "other"; and 4.7 percent identify as "other" alone. White/Asian youth exhibit somewhat less consistency in racial identification, with less than one-half (45.9 percent) of those selecting this identity at home also selecting it in school. Youth who identify as white/Asian at home but not at school are most likely to identify as Asian at school, with "white" being the next most likely school race. Consistent with expecta- tions (Snipp 1997), we find that the third multiracial group, white/American Indian, has the least stable racial identity (24 per- cent) and the highest probability of identify- ing as white in school (46 percent).5

FITTING MULTIRACIAL IDENTITIES INTO

MONORACIAL BOXES

Thus far we have examined expressed inter- nal racial identities in the context of "select

5 Table 3 also supports the contention that in- consistencies in reporting race are not the result of adolescents purposefully creating fictional identities for the survey. If that were the case, it is likely that not only would home and school race differ; they would differ dramatically (e.g., white/black to Asian). However, Table 3 shows that when school and home race differ, inconsis- tencies are almost always the result of a single racial group being added or dropped. Further sup- port for the argument that youth took Add Health questions seriously appears in the observation that more than 99 percent of youth reported their country of birth consistently in the two inter- views.

all that apply" race questions. Although this is consistent with how race will soon be measured throughout the federal statistical system, American society has not always been tolerant of expressions of multiracial identity. Instead, popular and administrative discourses on race have assumed that racial groups are mutually exclusive (Nobles 2000). People who identify with more than one racial group have had a single race se- lected for them by state and federal agents (Davis 1991; Hahn et al. 1992; Hirschman et al. 2000; OMB 2000) and have been que- ried about their "real" race by acquaintances (T. Williams 1996). Given the slow pace of change of everyday understandings of race (Banton 1983), it is likely that changing how we measure race on official documents will not immediately alter the pure race assump- tion that is so central to conceptions of race in the United States.

We assess how multiracial identities are converted to single-race identities by exam- ining responses to the "best single race" item. In the Add Health home interview, youth who identify with more than one ra- cial group are asked, "Which one category best describes your racial background?" Table 4 reports best single race separately for each multiracial group. White/American Indian adolescents offer the most predictable responses to the best single-race question, with nearly 86 percent selecting "white." This result is consistent with the assertion that many people regard American Indian as a costless identity that can be adopted or abandoned at will (Harris 1994; Nagel 1995; Snipp 1997).

As with white/American Indians, white/ black responses to the best single-race ques- tion are quite predictable, with almost 75 percent of white/black youth choosing "black." Given the enduring power of the one-drop rule, we suspect that many white/ black adolescents are socialized to under- stand that even if they identify as multira- cial, they are "really" black (Davis 1991). So, when a girl who has just told an inter- viewer that she is white and black is asked what single race best describes her, it is likely that she knows what the "right" an- swer is. Given this, it is noteworthy that 17.1 percent of white/black adolescents select white as the single race that best describes

Page 10: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

622 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

Table 4. Percentage Distribution of Selected Races at Home by Best Single Race

Race at Home

Best Single Race White/Black White/American Indian White/Asian

White 17.1 85.9 47.4

Black 74.5

Indian 14.1

Asian - 52.0

Other

Refused 1.8 .1

Don't know 6.7 .4

Total 100.0 100.0 100.0

Unweighted N 108 159 82

Note: Cells are weighted column percentages.

them. Further evidence of resistance to the one-drop rule is evident in the 8.5 percent of white/black adolescents who respond to the best single-race question by saying either that they do not know which single race best describes them or by simply refusing to give one. This suggests a commitment to being multiracial on the part of white/black youth that is not evident in the responses of white/ American Indian youth.

Unlike the other two multiracial groups, white/Asian youth are equally likely to iden- tify with their white and nonwhite heritages when asked to choose a single race. This finding is consistent with the results of Xie and Goyette (1998) and supports the hypoth- esis that the relatively small social distance between whites and Asians provides white/ Asian youth with the freedom to choose be- tween monoracial identities in contexts where a multiracial identity is unacceptable.

The logistic regression models in Table 5 present further detail about the monoracial identities of multiracial youth. For each mul- tiracial group we regress individual, family, and contextual factors on best single race. Our dependent variable is coded 1 if the best single race is white, and 0 otherwise. In re- sponse to previous work suggesting that gen- der and age affect racial identity (Corrin and Cook 1999; Jacobs 1992), we include indi- cators for whether the youth is female and whether he or she is at least 16 years old. We also include a measure of the educational attainment of the youth's coresidential par- ents, because prior work suggests that paren-

tal socioeconomic status is significantly re- lated to racial identity (Xie and Goyette 1998). The final set of predictors addresses the hypothesis that context affects racial identity (Snipp 1997; Spickard 1992). The specific components of context we examine are region (a proxy for ideology, racial di- versity, and the history of intergroup rela- tions), neighborhood racial composition, and whether family members were present dur- ing the home interview (i.e., private inter- view).

All three models suggest that the indi- vidual and family-level factors we examine have little effect on the choice of a best single race. Sex and parents' education are not significant predictors for any of the mul- tiracial groups, and age is only marginally significant (p < .10) for white/black youth. Among this group, youth who are at least 16 years old are about one-third as likely as younger adolescents to select white as the single race that best describes them. Instead, they are more likely to conform to the one- drop rule, maintaining that although they are white and black, they are more black than white. This finding is consistent with the ex- pectations of Cooley's (1902) looking-glass- self hypothesis and its reformulation by Nagel (1994). Both predict that older multi- racial children will be more likely to adapt their identities to meet societal expectations, as years of being "corrected" about their ra- cial identities take a toll. The finding of a racial difference in the age effect is also con- sistent with the assertion that American so-

Page 11: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

WHO IS MULTIRACIAL? 623

Table 5. Unstandardized Logistic Coefficients from the Regression of Best Single Race on Individual, Family, and Neighborhood Traits

White/Black at Home White/Indian at Home White/Asian at Home

Independent Variable Coef. Odds Ratio Coef. Odds Ratio Coef. Odds Ratio

Female .23 1.26 1.16 3.18 .22 1.24 (.65) (.77) (.78)

Age 16 or older -1.11 .33 .83 2.29 -.85 .43 (.68) (.65) (.67)

Parents at least some .52 1.69 -.18 .83 -.38 .69 college a (.77) (.67) (1.25)

South -1.62* .20 .51 1.66 -.87 .42 (.82) (.73) (1.33)

Percent non-Hispanic 1.95 7.00 .48 1.61 2.74* 15.53 white in census tract (1.08) (1.80) (1.21)

Private interview 1.56 4.78 -1.23 .29 -1.85* .16 (.83) (.68) (.82)

Constant -3.32** 1.34 --.40 (1.09) (1.60) (.93)

Model chi-square 9.87 6.16 13.87

Degrees of freedom 6 6 6 Pseudo R2 .14 .11 .16 Number of cases 106 151 81

Note: Numbers in parentheses are standard errors. Dependent variable equals 0 if best single race is not "white," 1 if best single race is "white." Data is weighted for non-Hispanics who identify as white/black, white/American Indian, or white/Asian at home and have valid responses for all independent variables.

a Measure of residential parents' education; high school or less is the omitted category. *p < .05 **p < .01 (two-tailed tests)

ciety is especially sensitive to how white/ black youth identify (Davis 1991).

Unlike age, sex, and socioeconomic sta- tus, our models suggest that context affects racial identification for all three multiracial groups. When white/black youth live in the South, they are significantly less likely to select white as their best single race. Given that the one-drop rule was born in the South and until fairly recently carried the weight of law in that region (Davis 1991), it is not surprising that so few southern white/black youth claim to be white. Contextual effects are also apparent in coefficients for neigh- borhood racial composition. For white/Asian youth, living in a neighborhood with more white neighbors significantly increases the probability that they identify white as their best single race. A similar, albeit marginally significant (p < .10), effect appears for white/black youth. These coefficients sug- gest that multiracial youth consider their cul-

ture and social networks when selecting a single-race identity, although we cannot re- ject the hypothesis that neighborhood effects are a proxy for unobserved aspects of family culture and socialization (Duncan, Connell, and Klebanov 1997).

Finally, Table 5 reveals that it is not only regional and neighborhood contexts that af- fect racial identification, but also the context of the interview. When no family members are present at the home interview, youth who initially identify as white/black are more likely to select white as their best single race, and white/American Indian and white/ Asian youth are more likely to choose American Indian and Asian, respectively. These effects are statistically significant (p < .05) for white/Asians, and approach statis- tical significance for the other two multira- cial groups (p < .10). Together, they suggest that in the presence of family members, which in Add Health usually means the

Page 12: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

624 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

youth's mother, the choice of a best single race is more likely to follow racial norms that were dominant when parents and grand- parents were adolescents (Davis 1991; Snipp 1997) than the less restrictive norms that are increasingly common among today's youth (Senna 1998).

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

Our work has examined patterns and pro- cesses of racial classification for a nationally representative sample of youth. Analysis of Add Health data shows that for a substantial minority of adolescents, race is not only so- cially constructed, it is also fluid. System- atic inconsistencies in racial classification appear between self-reports, as well as be- tween self-reports and parent-based mea- sures. We find that two social factors are es- pecially important for understanding pat- terns of racial fluidity.

First, we observe that shifting racial re- gimes exert a significant influence on racial classification patterns. Whereas parents and Add Health interviewers came of age at a time when the one-drop rule dominated thinking about race, today's youth are being raised in a society that increasingly espouses the virtues of diversity and that has made real efforts to stress the legitimacy of multi- racial identity (Nobles 2000; OMB 1997). Because expressed internal race is influ- enced by what we think about ourselves as well as by what others think about us, it is likely that changing perspectives on race and multiraciality partially explain patterns of multiracial reporting, the mismatch between parent's race and child's race, and the rela- tionship between best single race and whether a parent is present during the home interview. In each case, greater anonymity leads to racial classifications that are more consistent with contemporary understand- ings of race. Compared with the interviewer- administered home survey, the self-adminis- tered school survey provides relatively little opportunity for youth's conceptions of race to be influenced by the norms of older co- horts. By contrast, contemporary concep- tions of race figure less prominently in cen- sus race data, which in most cases reflects the beliefs of the household member who completes the census form. Similarly, we

suspect that some children of monoracial parents classify themselves as multiracial because parents and children disagree about how recently mixed one must be to report being multiracial. Also, we maintain that having family members present for the home interview affects best single race because it heightens the saliency of parental perspec- tives on race.

Second, we observe that patterns of racial classification vary because multiracial groups comprise socially distinct monoracial groups. White/American Indian youth emerge as the largest, but least committed, multiracial group. This finding is consistent with other work that suggests white/Ameri- can Indian identity is often expressed by whites who have little ancestral, phenotypi- cal or cultural connection to American Indi- ans, but who nevertheless wish to appeal to popular norms of multiculturalism by pre- senting a diverse portrait of themselves (Eschbach 1995; Harris 1994; Snipp 1997). Quite a different picture emerges for white/ black youth. Here we see evidence of the prominent, yet fading, influence of the one- drop rule on racial self-identification. Yet a third pattern emerges for youth who identify as white and Asian. In an extension of Xie and Goyette (1998), we show that racial identification is not only optional for this group when it is provided by parents on the census, but that the strong social rules gov- erning white/black and white/American In- dian classification are also absent for white/ Asian self-identification.

Although our work addresses some of the factors that affect the fluidity of race, still other factors are beyond its scope. Among these are the effects of age, period, and co- hort. Add Health provides unique insights into racial classification patterns for adoles- cents, but contains no information about how these patterns will change as the sample ages, how this cohort compares to previous cohorts of adolescents, or the extent to which racial classifications have become more fluid for all cohorts in the contempo- rary period. Given the lack of comparable data for other cohorts and the absence of panel data on race for the Add Health cohort, it is not possible to assess the extent of age, period, or cohort effects. However, it would be surprising if the growing attention to

Page 13: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

WHO IS MULTIRACIAL? 625

multiraciality in American society were not promoting greater flexibility, and therefore fluidity, in racial identity among all cohorts in the current period. It would also be sur- prising if racial fluidity were not greater for adolescents than for adults, because identi- ties are generally less stable in adolescence (Demo 1992) and also because the identities of older cohorts were formed during a time of more restrictive racial regimes. It is less clear how time will affect the racial identi- ties of today's adolescents. Research sug- gests that adulthood will bring more stabil- ity (Waters 1990), but one cannot predict how much less fluid racial identity will be- come, or whether identification with mul- tiple racial groups will subside.

As for the 2000 census, our work makes clear that it was a count of a multiracial population, not the multiracial population. Specifically, it captured the number of Americans who were identified as belonging to two or more racial groups by the person who completed their household's census form. This is probably not the same as the number who would have self-identified as multiracial, the number who self-identify as multiracial in everyday situations, the num- ber who are identified as multiracial by strangers, or the number who have ancestors from more than one racial group. This does not mean that the Census Bureau erred in its enumeration of the multiracial population. Rather, we maintain that the census, like ev- ery other data set, captures an individual's "true" race for a particular purpose, in a par- ticular context, at a particular point in time (Telles and Lim 1998). The practical impli- cation of this realization is that analysts must think critically about what they mean by race, design surveys that more precisely measure race, and be aware of the implica- tions of mismatches between available and ideal racial data. Failing to do so will ensure that as the racial diversity of the United States grows, so too will our inability to un- derstand that diversity.

David R. Harris is Assistant Professor of Sociol- ogy at the University of Michigan and Assistant Research Scientist at the Institute for Social Re- search. He continues to explore the social con- struction of race through analyses of data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent

Health and a web-based survey of University of Michigan freshmen. He is using insights drawn from this work to critique federal guidelines for collecting data on race, and to examine educa- tional and mental health outcomes for mono- racial and multiracial youth.

Jeremiah Joseph Sim received his MA in Sociol- ogy from the University of Michigan. His socio- logical interests include multiracial identity and residential racial segregation. In September he will pursue a new set of interests as he begins graduate study in the Department of Industrial and Operations Engineering.

REFERENCES

Alba, Richard D. 1990. Ethnic Identity. New Ha- ven, CT: Yale University Press.

Banton, Michael. 1983. Racial and Ethnic Com- petition. Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni- versity Press.

Bearman, Peter S., Jo Jones, and J. Richard Udry. 1997. "The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health: Research Design." Na- tional Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Retrieved February 21, 2001 (http:// www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design. html).

Blauner, Robert. 1972. Racial Oppression in America. New York: Harper and Row.

Chantala, Kim and Joyce Tabor. 1999. "Strate- gies to Perform a Design-Based Analysis Us- ing the Add Health Data." National Longitudi- nal Study of Adolescent Health. Retrieved February 21, 2001 (http://www.cpc.unc. edu/projects/addhealth/strategies.html).

Clegg, Roger. 2001. "Census Sense." National Review Online. Retrieved March 17, 2001 (http://www.nationalreview.com/contributors/ clegg030701.shtml).

Cooley, Charles Horton. 1902. Human Nature and the Social Order. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons.

Corrin, William J. and Thomas D. Cook. 1999. "Spanning Racial Boundaries: Multiracial Adolescents and Their Families, Peers, Schools, and Neighborhoods." Working Paper No. 99-20, Institute for Policy Research, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL.

Davis, F. James. 1991. Who Is Black? One Nation's Definition. University Park, PA: Pennsylvania State University Press.

Demo, David H. 1992. "The Self-Concept Over Time: Research Issues and Directions." Annual Review of Sociology 18:303-26.

DuBois, W. E. B. [1899] 1996. The Philadelphia Negro. Reprint, Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.

Duncan, Greg J., James P. Connell, and Pamela

Page 14: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

626 AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW

K. Klebanov. 1997. "Conceptual and Method- ological Issues in Estimating Causal Effects of Neighborhoods and Family Conditions on In- dividual Development." Pp. 219-50 in Neigh- borhood Poverty: Context and Consequences for Children, edited by J. Brooks-Gunn, G. J. Duncan, and J. L. Aber. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Eschbach, Karl. 1995. "The Enduring and Van- ishing American Indian: American Indian Population Growth and Intermarriage in 1990." Ethnic and Racial Studies 18:89-108.

Espiritu, Yen Le. 1992. Asian American Pan- ethnicity: Bridging Institutions and Identities. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.

Farley, Reynolds. 1996. The New American Re- ality. New York: Russell Sage.

Goldstein, Joshua R. and Ann J. Morning. 2000. "The Multiple-Race Population of the United States: Issues and Estimates." Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97:6230-35.

Goodman, Alan H. 2000. "Why Genes Don't Count (for Racial Differences in Health)." American Journal of Public Health 90:1699- 702.

Gould, Stephen Jay. 1996. The Mismeasure of Man. New York: W. W. Norton.

Hahn, Robert A., Joseph Mulinare, and Steven M. Teutsch. 1992. "Inconsistencies in Coding of Race and Ethnicity Between Birth and Death in US Infants: A New Look at Infant Mortality, 1983 through 1985." Journal of the American Medical Association 267:259-63.

Harris, David R. 1994. "The 1990 Census Count of American Indians: What Do the Numbers Really Mean?" Social Science Quarterly 75: 580-93.

. Forthcoming. "Does It Matter How We Measure? Racial Classification and the Char- acteristics of Multiracial Youth." In The New Race Question, edited by J. Perlmann and M. C. Waters. New York: Russell Sage.

Harrison, Roderick J. Forthcoming. "Inadequa- cies of Multiple Response Race Data in the Federal Statistical System." In The New Race Question, edited by J. Perlmann and M. C. Waters. New York: Russell Sage.

Hirschman, Charles, Richard Alba, and Reynolds Farley. 2000. "The Meaning and Measurement of Race in the U.S. Census: Glimpses into the Future." Demography 37:381-93.

Jacobs, James H. 1992. "Identity Development in Biracial Children." Pp. 190-206 in Racially Mixed People in America, edited by M. P. P. Root. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Jencks, Christopher and Meredith Phillips. 1998. The Black-White Test Score Gap. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.

Joyner, Kara and Grace Kao. 2000. "School Ra- cial Composition and Adolescent Racial

Homophily." Social Science Quarterly 81: 810-25.

Kao, Grace. 1999. "Racial Identity and Academic Performance: An Examination of Biracial Asian and African Youth." Journal of Asian American Studies 2:223-49.

Lieberson, Stanley and Mary C. Waters. 1993. "The Ethnic Responses of Whites: What Causes Their Instability, Simplification, and Inconsistency?" Social Forces 72:421-50.

Mouw, Theodore and Yu Xie. 1999. "Bilingual- ism and the Academic Achievement of Asian Immigrants: Accommodation with or without Assimilation?" American Sociological Review 64:232-52.

Nagel, Joane. 1994. "Constructing Ethnicity: Creating and Recreating Ethnic Identity and Culture." Social Problems 41:152-76.

.1995. "American Indian Ethnic Renewal: Politics and the Resurgence of Identity." American Sociological Review 60:947-65.

Nobles, Melissa. 2000. Shades of Citizenship: Race and the Census in Modern Politics. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Office of Management and Budget (OMB). 1997. "Revision to the Standards for the Classifica- tion of Federal Data on Race and Ethnicity." Federal Register, vol. 62, no. 210 (October 30).

.2000. "Guidance on Aggregation and Al- location of Multiple Race Responses for Use in Civil Rights Monitoring and Enforcement." Bulletin number 00-02, Office of Management and Budget, Washington, DC.

Omi, Michael A. 2001. "The Changing Meaning of Race." Pp. 243-63 in America Becoming: Racial Trends and Their Consequences, vol. 1, edited by N. J. Smelser, W. J. Wilson, and F. Mitchell. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Root, Maria P. P. 1992. "Back to the Drawing Board: Methodological Issues in Research on Multiracial People." Pp. 181-9 in Racially Mixed People in America, edited by M. P. P. Root. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Schmitt, Eric. 2001. "For 7 Million People in Census, One Race Category Isn't Enough." New York Times, March 13, pp. Al.

Senna, Danzy. 1998. "The Mulatto Millennium: Waking Up in an Age of Racial Ambiguity." Utne Reader 89:31-34.

Snipp, C. Matthew. 1997. "Some Observations about Racial Boundaries and the Experiences of American Indians." Ethnic and Racial Stud- ies 20:667-89.

Spickard, Paul R. 1992. "The Illogic of Ameri- can Racial Categories." Pp. 12-23 in Racially Mixed People in America, edited by M. P. P. Root. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Suro, Robert. 1999. "Mixed Doubles." American Demographics 21:56-62.

Page 15: Harris & Sim - Who is Multiracial

WHO IS MULTIRACIAL? 627

Tafoya, Sonya M. 2000. "Check One or More .. . Mixed Race and Ethnicity in California." Cali- fornia Counts 1:1-11.

Telles, Edward E. and Nelson Lim. 1998. "Does It Matter Who Answers the Race Question? Racial Classification and Income Inequality in Brazil." Demography 35:465-74.

Twine, France Winddance. 1997. "Brown- Skinned White Girls: Class, Culture, and the Construction of White Identity in Suburban Communities." Pp. 214-43 in Displacing Whiteness: Essays in Social and Cultural Criticism, edited by R. Frankenberg. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

Waters, Mary C. 1990. Ethnic Options: Choos- ing Identities in America. Berkeley, CA: Uni- versity of California Press.

. 1999. Black Identities: West Indian Im- migrant Dreams and American Realities. New York: Russell Sage.

Williams, Gregory Howard. 1995. Life on the Color Line: The True Story of a White Boy Who Discovered He Was Black. New York: Dutton.

Williams, Teresa Kay. 1996. "Race as Process: Reassessing the 'What Are You?' Encounters of Biracial Individuals." Pp. 191-210 in The Multiracial Experience: Racial Borders as the New Frontier, edited by M. P. P. Root. Thou- sand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Xie, Yu and Kim Goyette. 1998. "The Racial Identification of Biracial Children with One Asian Parent: Evidence from the 1990 Cen- sus." Social Forces 76:547-70.