final copy tra

100
An application of the Theory of Reasoned Action on Tax Compliance: The Case of Small Business Enterprises in Uganda Masters’ thesis Msc International Business and Management specializing in International Financial Management (University of Groningen) and Msc in Business and Economics (University of Uppsala) Kiconco Rebecca Isabella [email protected] Acknowledgement: 1

Upload: rebecca-kiconco

Post on 18-Apr-2015

123 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

Research into the behaviors of tax payers in Uganda. A psychology perspective

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Final Copy TRA

An application of the Theory of

Reasoned Action on Tax Compliance:

The Case of Small Business Enterprises

in Uganda

Masters’ thesis

Msc International Business and Management specializing in

International Financial Management (University of Groningen)

and Msc in Business and Economics (University of Uppsala)

Kiconco Rebecca [email protected]

Acknowledgement:

I would like to thank my supervisor Dr Bartjan Pennink for his encouragement and guidance during the writing of this thesis, I have benefited from his experience immensely. Further, I thank Dr Mark De Vires for taking the time to read through my work and give practical comments. I would like to extend my gratitude to Arthur Sserwanga, for his assistance in developing a workable idea and rendering his help in my research. Last but not least, I would like to thank my family.

1

Page 2: Final Copy TRA

Table of contents

Contents Page

No.

ABSTRACT.................................................................................................................41.2 Problem statement.................................................................................71.3 Research question:.................................................................................8

1.1.3 Sub-questions.......................................................................................81.4 Significance...............................................................................................8

Chapter 2.................................................................................................................10Literature review..................................................................................................10

2.0 Introduction............................................................................................102.1 Presumptive income tax.....................................................................102.3 Tax compliance......................................................................................112.2 Existing theories on tax compliance.............................................122.5 Psychology in tax compliance;.........................................................142.6 The theory of reasoned action.........................................................15

2.6.1 Attitudes...............................................................................................162.6.2 Social norms.......................................................................................172.6.2.1 Subjective norms:.........................................................................182.6.3 Behavioral intention........................................................................19

2.7 Conceptual framework.......................................................................19Chapter 3.................................................................................................................21Methodology...........................................................................................................21

3.0 Introduction............................................................................................213.1 Research Design....................................................................................213.2 Survey population.................................................................................213.3 Sampling and Sample size................................................................213.4 Methods of Data Collection..............................................................223.5 Measurement of variables:...............................................................23

3.5.1 Measurement of Behavior.............................................................233.5.2 Measurement of Intention:...........................................................233.5.3 Measurement of attitude towards behavior:.........................243.5.4 Measurement of subjective norms:...........................................243.5.5 Measurement of tax compliance behavior..............................25

3.6 Demographic and practice related control variables:...........253.7 Data Processing.....................................................................................26

3.7.1 Normality:............................................................................................263.7.2 Reliability test....................................................................................26

2

Page 3: Final Copy TRA

Chapter Four..........................................................................................................28Results.......................................................................................................................28

4.0 Introduction..............................................................................................284.0 Descriptive statistics........................................................................28

4.1.1 Sample characteristics...................................................................284.1.2 Descriptive statistics for theory variables..............................304.1.3 Correlations:.......................................................................................314.1.4 Objective 1: Predictors of Intention.........................................314.1.5 Objective 2: Predictor of tax compliance................................324.1.6 Supplement analyses.......................................................................33

Chapter five.............................................................................................................35Discussion of findings.........................................................................................35

5.0 Introduction: An Overview of Work Done...................................355.1 Discussion of Results...............................................................................35

5.1.1 Sample characteristics...................................................................355.2 Hypothesis...................................................................................................36

5.2.1 Hypothesis I........................................................................................365.2.2 Hypothesis II.......................................................................................385.2.3 Hypothesis III....................................................................................39

5.3 Supplementary analysis:....................................................................40Chapter Six..............................................................................................................41Conclusions and recommendations...............................................................41

6.0 Introduction.................................................................................................416.1 Conclusion...............................................................................................416.2 Implications and Recommendations.............................................41

6.2.1 Theoretical implications.................................................................426.2.2 Policy implications:..........................................................................42

6.3 Areas for further research................................................................426.4 limitations to the study.......................................................................43

REFERENCES:.......................................................................................................45

3

Page 4: Final Copy TRA

4

Page 5: Final Copy TRA

ABSTRACT

There are currently few studies on tax compliance invoking the

theory of reasoned action in explaining tax compliance in a

developing country such as Uganda. Uganda still has a research

gap in the area tax compliance. Many economic and administrative

studies have been done to determine factors that influence tax

compliance in Uganda. Psychological factors are yet to be

discussed in the Ugandan context. This study went out to

investigate if the theory of reasoned action (psychology theory

made up of subjective norms and attitudes influencing intentions to

comply which in turn influence behavior in this case tax compliance

behavior) has an influence on the tax compliance behavior on

individual small business enterprises in Uganda and further to see

if the explanatory power of the theory is better than its individual

components’ explanatory power. In order to achieve this data was

collected and analyzed using SPSS 17.0 where hierarchical

multiple regression and simple regression models were used to

receive results. The findings show that the theory of reasoned

action does indeed influence tax compliance behavior and

Ugandans generally do not intend to comply with tax regulations,

they have negative attitudes towards intending to comply with their

tax obligations. However, SBEs are influenced positively by

subjective norms in their intentions to comply with their tax

obligations.

5

Page 6: Final Copy TRA

Chapter 1If the Lord loveth a cheerful giver, how he must hate the taxpayer!

  ~John Andrew Holmes

1.0 Introduction:

The prevalence of poverty in developing countries demands that

these countries should improvise internal revenue generating

projects to supplement, and ultimately reduce dependence on

foreign donor funding. One such internal revenue-generating

mechanism and perhaps the most commonly used, is taxation.

Developing countries face formidable challenges in generating tax

revenue because the majority of citizens in these countries either

engage in subsistence agriculture or in small or informal sectors.

Consequently modern means of raising revenue such as income

taxes and consumption taxes play a diminishing role, given the

nature of Uganda’s economy. Low revenue collections are also

attributed to low compliance levels, difficulties in enforcement,

political interference, poor revenue management and

administration. Tax administration faces a problem of revenue

fraud in the form of smuggling, undervaluation, under declaration

of income and taxable goods, and misclassification of goods.

The tax evasion rates are high in most economies around the world

especially developing countries with Uganda being no exception.

Maital (1982) contends that tax evasion falls solidly in the free-

rider box. Further he says, the more people seek free rides and

6

Page 7: Final Copy TRA

evade taxes, the more those who do pay have to fork over and the

greater the incentive to evade. Tax non-compliance reduces

government revenues; thus addressing non-compliance problems is

central to the development of many developing economies around

the world today. The economics of tax compliance has been

approached from many perspectives; it has been viewed as a

problem of public finance, law enforcement, organizational design,

labor supply, or ethics or a combination of these (Andreoni, Erard

and Feinstein, 1998). However, in Uganda tax compliance has

been mainly investigated and apprehended from a tax

administration or tax collector perspective. There are few empirical

findings on tax compliance using a taxpayer approach.

In Uganda, a number of Small Business Enterprises (hence forth

referred to as SBEs) remain outside the tax system through tax

non-compliance. It is easier for SBEs to remain outside the tax net

because they can remain inconspicuous to the tax administration.

Furthermore SBEs find it easier to slip out of the tax collectors’

net, because the enforcement costs would exceed the potential tax

revenue collected from the SBEs (Gauthier and Reinikka, 2001).

SBEs find it beneficial to take advantage of loopholes in the tax

system in order to minimize their tax payments (Wallace, 2002).

As a complex phenomenon, tax non-compliance can be addressed

from a variety of perspectives. Taxpayers’ stance is influenced by

many factors, including perceived fairness of the taxes, prevailing

social norms and morals, complexity of tax laws and regulations,

audit rates and penalties. Without questioning the relevance of

ethical and sociological motivations, the analysis of non-compliance

has focused on how non-compliance or evasion is deterred through

detection and sanctions (Franzoni, 1999), which approach cannot

wholly explain non-compliance among taxpayers. Many scholars

perceive tax audits as a deterrent to non-compliance.

7

Page 8: Final Copy TRA

The foregoing offers an analytical framework for examining some

salient aspects of tax compliance in Uganda. To date much of the

studies have focused on penalties, audits and fairness as the crucial

determinants for non-compliance, but little is understood about the

personalities of tax payers. Efforts have to be directed to addresses

the psychological and sociological aspects of the tax payers in a bid

to improve compliance rather than depending on the economic,

legal and tax administrative aspect. Non-compliance is still a

complex phenomenon that cannot be eradicated by partial

diagnosis of its determinants and ignoring the role of psychology.

Psychology plays an important role in compliance, though often

ignored by public and tax policy makers.

In order to introduce psychology in tax compliance, the theory was

reasoned action was employed by two distinct studies; (Bobek and

Hatfield, 2003; Meints and Roberts, 2009). These studies however,

were both conducted in the USA.. No known study has employed

the theory of reasoned action to explain tax compliance in Uganda.

Tax compliance has taken a tax administrator/collector approach

(with little success), but TRA takes an individual tax payer based

approach. Personality has been an overlooked factor in nearly all

studies of evasion behavior (Meints and Roberts, 2009).

Consequently, the primary purpose of this study is to identify which

personality traits influence tax compliance behavior by making use

of the theory of reasoned action (hence forth referred to as TRA)

(Ajzen and Fishbein, 1975). TRA postulates that intentions to

engage in a specific behavior are determined by two social

cognitive constructs; attitudes and perceived subjective norms,

(Ajzen 1991). Intentions then predict actual behavior. In this case,

attitude is one’s personal opinion of a behavior and perceived

subjective norms refers to how one thinks significant others judge

the behavior. TRA posits that attitude and perceived subjective

8

Page 9: Final Copy TRA

norms predict intended behavior. Previous studies have shown that

taxpayers who hold favorable attitudes toward evading taxes as

well as taxpayers who perceive evading taxes to be a normative

behavior are more likely to evade taxes (see Gilligan and

Richardson 2005; Davis et al. 2003; Wenzel 2004).

1.2 Problem statement Academic scholars have been on a quest to explain the

phenomenon of tax compliance for many years. The interesting face

of the tax non-compliance paradox is that though many theories

have been applied by scholars to explain the yet prevalent non-

compliance in developing countries; specifically an academic

literature gap exists on Uganda in terms of tax compliance. There

are currently few studies on tax compliance invoking the theory of

reasoned action in explaining tax compliance in a developing

country such as Uganda.

The theory of reasoned action (TRA) models decision processes

where people have a high degree of volitional control and make

reasoned choices among alternatives. (Parasuraman, Zeithaml and

Berry, 1985 Prior tax compliance research in developing countries

has mainly focused on the tax administration perspective using;

audit rates (Gauthier and Reinikka, 2001), tax penalties (Jackson

and Jones, 1985; Beck, Davis and Jung, 1991; Alm, Jackson and

McKee, 1992; Alm, McClelland and Schulze, 1992), tax fairness

(Torgler, 1999; Smith, 1992; Tyler and Smith, 1998; Franzoni 1999)

and tax compliance costs (Mueller, 1971). Further still, limited

research has been done in respect to the small business enterprises

perspective in Uganda, (Nabawesi, 2009, Fjeldstad, 2001). In

addition there is no available empirical research investigating tax

compliance behavior using the theory of reasoned action in

Uganda. Taking a psychological and sociological approach of the

tax payer, this study attempts to add to the tax compliance debate,

9

Page 10: Final Copy TRA

the theory of reasoned action as a predictor of tax compliance in a

developing country context, principally focusing on Small Business

enterprises. With true audit rates averaging at one percent (or less

in some periods) in the past few decades, the real puzzle of

compliance, as Alm (1991) notes, is why people pay taxes at all.

1.3 Research question:The main research question this study seeks is to explore the

extent to which the theory of reasoned action explains variations in

tax compliance behavior among small business enterprises in

Uganda. The research question is classified under the composition

category of Thomas (2008). There are quite many parameters that

could explain the tax compliance phenomenon; however, the

literature is still incomplete. This study will explore yet another

factor categorized broadly as the theory of reasoned action and

broken down into attitudes, subjective norms and behavioral

intentions.

1.1.3 Sub-questions1. What is the influence of attitudes and subjective norms of on

behavioral intentions of taxpayers (SBEs) in Uganda?

2. What is the influence of behavioral intentions of SBEs on tax

compliance in Uganda?

3. What is the influence of subjective norms of SBEs on tax

compliance in Uganda?

4. What is the influence of attitudes of SBEs on tax compliance in

Uganda?

Sub- questions 3 and 4 will serve to compare the theory of reasoned

action explanatory variance to the individual predictors within the theory.

1.4 SignificanceThe study will extend the application and relevance of the theory of

reasoned action to a new arena of tax compliance in Uganda. Prior

10

Page 11: Final Copy TRA

to this study, the theory of reason action has had few linkages to

tax compliance behaviours, especially in developing countries like

Uganda. This study goes out to match two areas (psychology and

management) divergent and yet commonly bound fields of

academic research. Not as much of research has maneuvered these

two areas simultaneously so it is a relatively new application.

The theory of reasoned action has been used to explain an

individual behavior with focus on developed countries. This study

will extend the application to developing countries and in

particular, Uganda. Further still, the application of the TRA will

provide new insights into variables that influence compliance in

Uganda. Furthermore this may lead to more tracks for other

developing countries to assess the impact SBEs’ have on tax

revenue collections which are further reflected in their ability of

economic stability as well as financial independence.

This study will go further to address tax academic scholars and

enrich tax literature related to developing countries; in addition it

will address tax practitioners especially the Uganda Revenue

Authority which is the tax body in Uganda giving them further

insight on how the theory of reasoned action can enrich tax

administration.

11

Page 12: Final Copy TRA

12

Page 13: Final Copy TRA

Chapter 2

Literature review

2.0 Introduction This chapter profiles the tax system of small business enterprises in

Uganda today as a starting point for investigating the tax

compliance of SBEs. It briefly reviews the classical approaches to

tax compliance and the literature available on this field. This

chapter further gives insight on vast literature of the theory of

reasoned action and compiles a conceptual framework that will

guide the research. It also extracts research hypotheses for this

study.

2.1 Presumptive income taxPresumptive taxation was introduced in Uganda in 1997 to offer

the possibility of reducing tax evasion at low cost and broadening

the revenue base under the informal sector. (Sserwanga, 2002)

This mainly focused on taxpayers that do not keep records and are

in informal employment and whose turnover in a year is less than

50 million Uganda shillings (a presumptive tax does not apply to

persons carrying on professional services). Presumptive tax makes

very insignificant contribution to tax revenue in Uganda (about 2

percent of income tax and less than 0.5 percent of total tax) and it

has been a very difficult tax to collect. The informal sector, world

over, is difficult to tax because: Governments lack a comprehensive

list of these potential taxpayers; the administrative cost of dealing

with a sea of returns from these small taxpayers is extremely high;

and the informal sector businesses themselves keep poor accounts.

Further these small businesses are characterized by high tax

evasion levels, mainly under informal sector, high mobility rates,

high collapse rates, mainly sole proprietors, partnerships and

NGOs, generally have a negative attitude to taxation, and high

levels of illiteracy and innumeracy.

13

Page 14: Final Copy TRA

Christian, (ND) states that a presumptive income tax (P-tax for

short) is a tax based on some measure of economic activity in lieu

of taxable income, rather than on taxable income itself. For

instance, it may be assessed on the basis of a firm's inventory of

output, of some input of the production process, or of gross sales

over a period of time. In the case of Uganda, presumptive tax is

assessed as a percentage of annual gross turnovers. In any case,

the aim of the tax authority is to estimate the taxable income of the

whole economic activity at hand. Bulutoglu (1995) classifies

presumptive taxes according to the type of measure of income used

and their objective. Common measures are production inputs,

assets, revenue, and external indicators. Bulutoglu (1995) finds

four common targets: hard-to-tax groups, whose income is not

easily observable; multinationals with transfer-pricing possibilities;

small businesses with high compliance costs; and as pointers of

likely tax evaders. As already stated, SBE’s are usually outside the

tax net as they are inconspicuous due to being small taxpayers.

According to the Ugandan tax structure a small business enterprise

is defined as one with gross turnover of 50 million Uganda shillings

or less and are assessed using a presumptive method. (Sserwanga,

2006). Mueller, (ND) asserted that few decisions made by the small

businessman have such serious or varied consequences as those

related to taxation. Inherent in any system of taxation are the

problems of compliance. Further, he states that the heart of the

problem for the small businessman it seems is the general lack of

knowledge about taxes and the inability to cope with them

effectively. Tax compliance was complicated by the general

practice of employing public accountants, public bookkeepers, and

tax specialists to prepare the accounting records and tax returns. It

was common among small businessmen to view the total costs of

these services to be the cost of tax compliance. Most small firms

14

Page 15: Final Copy TRA

lack qualified personnel to perform bookkeeping and tax duties.

These duties are generally performed by the overworked owner, a

member of the household, or a part-time bookkeeper with little or

no formal training. (Nalukenge, 2007)

2.3 Tax complianceTax compliance is a concern of governments around the world (see

Alm and Sanchez, 1995; Feige, 1989; Frey and Weck-Hanneman,

1984). As a public finance topic, tax compliance spans the notions

of equity, efficiency, and incidence. (Andreoni, Erard and Feinstein,

1998). Wenzel (2005) contends that research on tax compliance has

been dominated by an economic analysis which frames the

taxpayer’s decision to pay or shun tax as an individual’s rational

attempt to maximize profits (see, Allingham & Sandmo, 1972).

However, traditional economic models of tax compliance, which

primarily emphasize enforcement and detection variables (see

Andreoni et al., 1998), are unable to comprehensively explain

current levels of compliance. Recent research, however, supports

the claim that non-economic social factors also influence taxpaying

behavior; for example ethics, perceived fairness, social norms and

psychological reactance (e.g., Alm, Sanchez, & DeJuan, 1995;

Cowell, 1992; Kirchler, 1999; Wenzel, 2002, 2004).

Economics researchers studying tax compliance in the United

States (U.S.) (see Andreoni et al. 1998) have called for more

attention to social (as opposed to economic) influences on tax

compliance (Bobek, Roberts and Sweeney, 2007). Economists in

particular have focused on the concept of ‘‘social norms’’ (e.g., Alm

et al., 1999; Pommerehne et al., 1994; Scholz and Pinney, 1995;

Wenzel, 2004). However, most of these economic studies Wenzel,

(2004) do not specify precisely what these social norms are instead

they model a variable that affects compliance in a manner

consistent with a strong effect from some outside social influence.

15

Page 16: Final Copy TRA

With tax return audit rates continually declining (Schnepper,

2004), a better understanding of this important influence on tax

compliance is essential.

Baldry (1986) argues that the decision whether or not to evade is

influenced by “moral compunctions. Baldry (1987) asserts that

experimental findings show the prediction that a taxpayer will not

attempt tax evasion, as long as the expected gain is positive, cannot

be supported. However, taxpayers may be driven by moral rules

and sentiments. A taxpayer might bear moral costs if she/he does

not pay the taxes and act as a free-rider. Elffers (2000), attempts to

shows that it is a long way before a person becomes a tax evader.

He defines three steps in the staircase to tax evasion. First,

taxpayers have to be seized by a will not to comply. In a second

step, Elffers (2000) argues that not everyone with “an inclination to

dodge his taxes is able to translate his intention into action”. Many

individuals have not the opportunity or the knowledge and

resources to evade. In a third step, you can find individuals that

feel inclined not to comply and check for the opportunity to evade

taxes. Elffers (2000) further argues that this is the phase where

standard economic theory comes into play, where individuals

evaluate the expected value of evasion.

2.2 Existing theories on tax complianceDeterrence theory has been the primary theoretical framework

applied to tax compliance. This theory assumes that taxpayers

consciously weigh the pros and cons of noncompliance in light of

the probability of detection and the severity of punishment. This

model has been labeled the most pervasive tax compliance model,

(describing taxpayer expected utility as a function of tax rates,

potential penalties, and probability of audit (Allingham and Sandmo

1972). Essentially, this approach builds on Becker’s (1968) rational

economic agent approach to crime as a lottery. While this model

16

Page 17: Final Copy TRA

has been used and modified over the decades to address tax

compliance, researchers have long struggled with its major

shortcoming; it fails to accurately predict at a macroscopic level

the very phenomena it seeks to model. More specifically, this model

predicts much lower levels of compliance than actually observed in

most industrialized nations.

In addition, two psychological theories that have been broadly

discussed in the context of tax compliance are guilt and shame.

After reviewing several well-known psychological theories, Erard

and Feinstein (1994) adapted guilt and shame to the context of tax

compliance. They argue that a taxpayer who is filling out his return

is likely to anticipate guilt while contemplating underreporting and

escaping detection, but is likely to anticipate shame when

contemplating underreporting and subsequently being caught.

However there are several draw backs to their approach; first, just

how guilt and shame enter the utility function is arbitrary and

cannot be derived from economic or psychological theory, second –

since guilt and shame are not directly observable, identification is

based totally on functional form assumptions.

Furthermore, the game theory approach was also used to explain

tax compliance, Game theory helped to create and evaluate an

analytic explanation of a particular situation. The strength of game

theory is that it makes explicit strategic aspects of social

interactions. The logic of game theory helps to simplify the

complexity of tax compliance. It outlines the range of choices

available to a player. Levi (1997) argues that to understand why

one path becomes an equilibrium path, it is important to

understand why individuals did not follow other possibilities. She

argues that game theory allows specifying the behavior that failed

to happen because it is off an equilibrium path (Greenberg, 1984;

Graetz et al., 1986; Cowell, 1990; Frey and Holler, 1998; Van Vugt

17

Page 18: Final Copy TRA

et al., 2000) if a taxpayer does not pay the taxes, public goods will

not immediately disappear. Game theory has paid attention to the

aspects of cooperation. It helps to think about the interaction

between taxpayers themselves and the government or the tax

administration in a simple and compelling manner.

Bobek and Hatfield (2003) investigated the theory of planned

behavior and the role of moral obligation in tax compliance, where

they determined the beliefs that underlie taxpayers’ attitudes. They

found a significant explanation of tax non compliance in three

different scenarios after a series of experiments. To add to this

debate, a more recent and somewhat different approach to the

compliance issue, Feld and Frey (2007) argue that tax compliance

is the result of a “psychological tax contract” where emotional ties

and loyalties bond the taxpayer and state together to create a tax

morale that reinforces compliance.

2.5 Psychology in tax compliance;Trivedi, Shehata and Mestelman, (2005) classified the two classes

of theories in their paper as; economics-based theories which

emphasize incentives and psychology based theories which

emphasize attitudes. Economic theories of compliance suggest that

taxpayers make calculations of the economic consequences of

different compliance alternatives (such as whether or not to evade

tax), the probabilities of detection and the consequences thereof,

and then choose the alternative which maximizes their expected

after-tax return (possibly after adjustment for the desired level of

risk). In contrast, the second class of theories assumes that

psychological factors -- including moral and ethical concerns -- are

also important to taxpayers, and so taxpayers may comply even

where the risk of audit is low. Of course, some taxpayers’ behavior

may follow the economic theories while others may follow the

psychological theories, and mixtures are also possible.

18

Page 19: Final Copy TRA

Andreoni, Erard & Feinstein, (1998) further called for more work

that was needed to be done in exploring diverse psychological,

moral and social influences on compliance behavior and integrating

these factors into economic models of compliance. In general,

incorporating non-economic motivations, such as moral preference

for honest reporting, does reduce predicted non-compliance.

Researchers have widened their lens in looking at the causes of

noncompliance. Specifically, the literature has focused on social

drivers of compliance such as social norms, individual ethics, and

how taxpayers reference themselves to others in their social

network. (Korobow and Johnson, 2007) Wenzel (2005) notes that

social factors such as “ethics and norms” might not only create

deterrents to compliance, but might also mitigate the impact of

potential penalties associated with noncompliance.

More current research has sought to modify the A&S model to

incorporate social factors. For example, Traxler (2006) modifies the

classic approach to include “tax morale” and interprets this

modification as an “internalized social norm” for tax compliance.

His results have yield higher levels of compliance given low audit

rates. Fiscal psychology has considered the effects of noneconomic

variables on tax compliance behavior over a long period of time. In

fact, Jackson & Milliron (1986) have identified tax fairness as a key

noneconomic variable in this regard. (Richardson, 2006). Recently

a new theory has been applied in tax compliance; the theory of

reasoned action.

2.6 The theory of reasoned actionIn basic terms, the theory of reasoned action (TRA) indicates that a

person’s behavior is determined by their attitude towards the

outcome of that behavior and by the opinions of the person’s social

environment. The TRA is a multi-attribute attitude model that

19

Page 20: Final Copy TRA

predicts and explains the behavioral intentions and consequent

behavior of individuals (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980). On the whole,

this theory explains an individual’s decision to perform or not to

perform a specific behavior such as a criminal act, enrolling in

graduate school among others. In this study, the application of TRA

is being extended to behavioral intentions of small business

enterprises’ owners towards tax compliance. The study will focus

on the owners or entrepreneurs of the SBEs as they are the

decision makers. The theory of reasoned action attempts to model

decision processes where people have a high degree of volitional

control and make reasoned choices among alternatives. (Omar &

Owusu-Frimpong, 2007). Correspondingly, tax compliance of SBEs

involves a high degree of volitional control and making reasoned

choices among compliance alternatives.

The theory of reasoned action (TRA) is a multi-attribute attitude

model that predicts and explains the behavioral intentions and

consequent behavior of consumers (Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980).

The TRA employs four constructs; attitudes, subjective norms,

intentions and behavior. The behavioral focus of this particular

study is tax compliance. The theory hypothesizes that behavior is

influenced by one’s intention to perform the behavior. Intention in

turn is influenced by one’s attitude (positive or negative evaluation

about performing the behavior) and subjective norm (perceived

social influence whether to perform the behavior). Attitude itself is

the product of beliefs and evaluations of the consequences of

performing the behavior. It can be defined as the individual’s

personal judgment about whether a specific behavior is desirable

or not based on his/her pre-existing beliefs about the desirability of

different kinds of behavior. Scholars such as Ajzen and Fishbein

(ND) put forth that attitude is best considered to be a person’s

degree of favorableness or un-favorableness with respect to a

psychological object. Similarly, subjective norm is determined by

20

Page 21: Final Copy TRA

the beliefs of whether someone important to us expects or does not

expect us to perform the behavior, and whether we want to comply

(Lin, Chan and Wei, 2006).

Lin et al, (2006) spelt out that TRA is not without its limitations.

They stated these as three boundary conditions that can affect the

relationship between intention and behavior: (a) the degree to

which the measure of intention and the behavior criterion

corresponds with respect to the levels of specificity of action,

target, context, and time frame; (b) the stability of intentions

between time of measurement and performance of the behavior;

and (c) the degree to which carrying out the intention is under

one’s volitional control (Fishbein & Ajzen 1975). Many researchers

in attitudinal research study behavioral intention not under the

volitional control (Davis &Warshaw, 1991; Madden, Ellen, & Ajzen,

1992; Sheppard et al., 1988; Warshaw & Droge, 1986). Ajzen

(1985) addresses the issue by proposing an extension of the TRA by

incorporating perceived behavioral control as an antecedent to

behavior intention as well as behavior in a theory called the theory

of planned behavior. Below the components of the theory of

reasoned action; attitudes, subjective norms and intentions are

developed.

2.6.1 AttitudesAttitudes are defined as latent disposition or tendency to respond

with some degree of favorableness or un favorableness to a

psychological object. (Fishbein and Ajzen, 2010) Trivedi et al,

(2005) used five indicators to measure attitudes of taxpayers

namely; the importance to the tax compliance decisions of fulfilling

one’s moral and ethical obligations, funding the government,

fulfilling duties of citizen, recognition of good ethics by community,

and the presence of records (computer records or receipts).

Further they added four indicators load on this component – the

21

Page 22: Final Copy TRA

importance to their tax compliance decisions of their perceptions

regarding having taken advantage of the system or of factors

necessary for doing so, specifically being able to brag about their

actions, knowledge of the income tax system, feeling of beating the

system, and effort required to prepare their tax return. Also

monetary considerations were added by two indicators; the

prospect of receiving a refund, and of having extra cash

consequent to their actions

Chan et al. (2000) found that taxpayer attitude had a positive

relationship with tax compliance in both Hong Kong and the U.S.

Other studies have found no association between taxpayers’

attitudes and tax compliance behavior (see, Vogel, 1974; Porcano,

1988; Antonides & Robben, 1995). However, as was suggested by

Jackson & Milliron (1986) and Richardson & Sawyer (2001), a

credible reason for this inconsistency is the multidimensional

nature of attitudes as a tax compliance variable (Richardson, 2006).

Thus the following hypothesis;

H1 Attitudes influence tax compliance behavioral intention of

taxpayers.

2.6.2 Social normsTorgler & Friedrich (2007) asked an interesting question; How,

then, do norms of compliance originate? Sociology stresses that

norms are learned through social interaction with others (Williams,

1968; Blau, 1964). The proposition that social norms are influential

in tax compliance decisions is consistent with the moral psychology

literature (Kohlberg, 1969; Rest, 1986; Bobek, et al, 2007). Social

norms are defined as ‘‘rules and standards that are understood by

members of a group, and that guide and/or constrain social

behavior without the force of law’’ (Cialdini and Trost, 1998). The

four categories of social norms identified by Cialdini and Trost are:

descriptive norms, injunctive norms, subjective norms, and

22

Page 23: Final Copy TRA

personal norms. For this study subjective norms are the focus

within social norms. According to Bobek et al, (2007), subjective

norms relate specifically to the expectations (injunctive norms) of

referent others (namely; family, friends, and co-workers). Further

they state that the social goal of subjective norms is to represent

the injunctive norms of those closest to an individual. Thus they aid

in building and maintaining social relationships with people whose

opinions matter the most. On the question of when will the norms

be influential they asserted that; subjective norms will influence

behavior when individuals are motivated to comply with the norms

of referent others.

Thus, individuals will comply and pay taxes as long as they believe

that compliance is a social norm (see Alm et al., 1999. Polinsky and

Shavell (2000) argue that social norms can be seen as a general

alternative to law enforcement in channeling individuals’

behaviours. The violation of social norms has consequences like

internal sanctions (guilt, remorse) or external legal and social

sanctions as gossip and ostracism. There is evidence that many

countries with similar fiscal systems have different compliance

experiences (Alm et al., 1995; for the United States see Yankelovich

et al., 1984; Vogel, 1974, for Sweden; Smith, 1986; for the United

Kingdom and De Juan et al., 1993 for Spain). The main conclusions

are that (i) individuals who comply tend to view tax evasion as

immoral; (ii) compliance is higher if moral appeals are made to the

taxpayer, (iii) individuals with tax evaders among their friends are

more likely to be evaders themselves, and (iv) compliance is

greater in societies with a stronger sense of social cohesion. Bobek

and Hatfield, (2003)

2.6.2.1 Subjective norms:Ajzen (1991) describes subjective norms as the influence of

referent others. Subjective norms refer to a person’s beliefs about

23

Page 24: Final Copy TRA

whether specific individuals or groups approve or disapprove of the

individual performing a specific behavior and to what extent the

individual is motivated to conform with other individuals or groups.

Hanno and Violette (1996) measured taxpayers’ beliefs about

specific referent groups namely; family members, employers,

friends and spouse. In addition to the above indicators, Trivedi et

al, (2005) added the tax preparer as well as peers of the individual.

In a review of factors affecting compliance, Jackson and Milliron

(1986) report on numerous studies that find that the compliance

behavior of one’s peers is significantly related to an individual’s

own level of compliance.

Several lines of evidence indicate that subjective norms provide

poorer prediction of intentions, compared to attitude and Perceived

behavioral control / PBC (Armitage & Conner, 2001a; Sheeran,

Trafimow, Finlay, & Norman, 2002; Trafimow & Finlay, 1996;

Langdridge, Sheeran and Connolly, 2007). However, concerning

social norms, many studies on tax evasion have found a relationship

between one’s own tax noncompliance and the perceived

noncompliance of others. (Wenzel, 2005) People who believe that

tax cheating is wide-spread among their peers or colleagues tend to

have more favorable attitudes towards tax evasion and are more

likely to cheat on taxes (De Juan, Lasheras, & Mayo, 1994; Kaplan

& Reckers, 1985; Wallschutzky, 1984; Webley, Robben, & Morris,

1988). Bobek et al (2007) in their study found subjective norms and

injunctive norms were significantly related to compliance

intentions. A striking observation was the magnitude of the effect

of subjective norms. The influence of subjective norms was much

greater than the influence of traditional economic variables on

cheating intentions.

For purposes of this study subjective norms have been broken

down in to injunctive and descriptive norms.

24

Page 25: Final Copy TRA

H2 subjective norms influence tax compliance behavioral intentions

of taxpayers.

2.6.3 Behavioral intentionLangdridge et al, (2007) Intention summarizes the person’s

motivation to perform a behavior and indicates the amount of time

and effort that he or she is prepared to devote in order to ensure

that an action is undertaken (Ajzen, 1991). Intention is determined

by three constructs: attitude and subjective norm and perceived

behavioral control (PBC). Attitude is the person’s overall evaluation

of what it would be like to perform a behavior (e.g., “Having a child

at some time in the future would be good/ bad) PBC is a concept of

self-efficacy and refers to people’s appraisals of the ease or

difficulty of performing the behavior (e.g., “Having a child at some

time in the future would be easy/difficult”). PBC can also influence

intention because people are unlikely to intend to perform

behaviours over which they have little control. Trivedi et al (2005)

employed two variables in respect to behavioral intention;

monetary Intent to Comply being significant.

The problematic nature of this variable is that subjects are hesitant

to admit to unethical and illegal behavior (Wenzel, 2004),

potentially contributing to the relatively low significance levels of

most compliance models. Bobek et al, (2007) In the present study,

consideration will be made to taxpayers’ compliance intentions,

rather than requesting subjects to reveal their actual tax

compliance behavior just as in the study of Bobek et al, (2007)

Behavioral intentions have been shown to be highly correlated with

actual behavior (Ajzen, 1991).

H3 taxpayers’ behavioral intentions influence their tax

compliance behavior.

25

Page 26: Final Copy TRA

Attitudes

Subjective Norms

Behavioral intention

Tax Compliance

Behavior

PenaltiesAuditsFairnessSocial normsGovernment responsiveness

2.7 Conceptual frameworkFrom the literature reviewed above, we develop the conceptual

framework below to guide the tax compliance investigation.

Source: Martin Fishbein and Icek Ajzen, 1975; 1980; Lin et al, 2006; Omar & Owusu-

Frimpong, 2007; Bobek and Hatfield, 2003; Meints and Roberts, 2009 and extant

literature.

There are many factors that could possibly explain tax compliance

as indicated (penalties, audits, fairness, social norms and

government responsiveness) that have been derived from existing

literature above. However for purposes of this study we are

confined to attitudes, subjective norms and behavioral intention of

SBEs.

26

Page 27: Final Copy TRA

Chapter 3

Methodology

3.0 IntroductionThis chapter elaborates how the study was designed and

accomplished. It provides an account of the research design,

measurement of variables, study population, sampling design, unit

of analyses, data collection methods, processing and analysis of the

data.

3.1 Research DesignA cross sectional survey design was adopted for this study. In a

cross-sectional study, a particular phenomenon is studied at a

particular period of time. Cross sectional designs are suited for

studies aimed at finding out the prevalence of a phenomenon,

situation, problem or attitude, by taking a cross-section of the

population at a given time. Cross-sectional studies are relatively

inexpensive, quick and easy to do, useful for generating and

clarifying hypothesis and can lay the ground work for decisions

about future follow up studies (Kraemer, 1994).

3.2 Survey populationThe population consisted of SBEs operating in Kampala District.

Responses were obtained from the following tax districts, i.e.

Rubaga, Makindye, Bwaise, and Nakawa. According to Uganda

Revenue Authority records, there are about 208,500 SBEs, in the

27

Page 28: Final Copy TRA

above tax districts. For purposes of this study, SBEs had an annual

turnover of less than fifty million Uganda shillings. (Hereby,

defined as the qualifying factor of SBEs according to Uganda’s tax

structure). This included those, which use the presumptive tax

method, those small business enterprises which opted to use the

conventional tax method and those small business enterprises

barred from using the presumptive method. (such as small law

firms, private health clinics, and audit firms). SBEs which

voluntarily use the conventional tax method are those who are

charged 30% corporate tax and keep proper books of accounts,

despite being small business enterprises.

3.3 Sampling and Sample sizeThe sample size was based on Roscoe’s (1975) rule of the thumb

for estimating the sample size. Roscoe contends that a sample size

between 30 and 500 is appropriate for most studies. Furthermore,

Saunders, Lewis and Thornhill (1997) provide a guide for minimum

sample sizes required from different sizes of the population at 95%

confidence level. They suggest a sample size of 384 respondents for

populations between 100,000 and 1,000,000. Therefore given a

population of 208,500 SBEs, in the above regions, a sample size of

384 was recommended for this study. Using the average response

rate (90%), for previous empirical studies carried out in Uganda on

the small-scale enterprises (Sserwanga 2002) the sample size was

raised to 426 elements to carter for possible non-responses. This

was calculated as follows:

90X /100 =384

90X = 38,400

X =38400/90

X = 426

Where X represents the sample size

Given a high mortality rate of SBEs, plus the fact that SBEs that

are not registered for tax purposes are not known with certainty,

28

Page 29: Final Copy TRA

purposive sampling was used to select a representative sample. All

respondents were given a small symbolic gift for their

participation.

3.4 Methods of Data CollectionA cross-sectional survey targeting different categories of SBEs was

carried out using interviewer-administered, structured

questionnaires. The questionnaire consisted of mainly closed-ended

questions using Likert scales. Few open-ended questions were

included in the questionnaire to ensure response clarity and

consistence. The questionnaires were prepared in English but later

translated into local languages for non-English speaking

respondents.

Given the uneven distribution of communication facilities like

telephones, internet and postal facilities etc in Uganda, telephone

interviews and mail surveys were not practical. The researcher

visited the SBE respondent, introduced herself, and requested the

respondents to complete the questionnaire. Time and effort was

dedicated to the construction and testing of the questionnaire. Pre-

tests were carried out to fine tune the instrument and to ascertain

the most appropriate way to ask the questions. Adjustments were

made to the instruments especially in respect to wording, structure

of the questions and interpretation of the words, given that most of

the scales adapted were developed outside Uganda.

3.5 Measurement of variables:In combination, attitude toward the behavior and subjective norms

lead to the formation of a behavioural intention. As a general rule,

the more favourable the attitude and subjective norm, the stronger

should be the person’s intention to perform the behavior in

question.

29

Page 30: Final Copy TRA

3.5.1 Measurement of BehaviorAccording to Ajzen (2006), behavior of interest is defined in terms

of its Target, Action, Context, and Time (TACT) elements. In this

case - SBEs paying their taxes in full and promptly to Uganda

Revenue Authority (URA) at the end of the financial year. Defining

the TACT elements is somewhat arbitrary. Paying is clearly part of

the action element. The URA considered the target and in full the

context, the time element referred when the behavior was

performed, and in this example it is defined as at the end of the

financial year. In Uganda’s case this is the end of June of any given

year. The financial year runs from 1st July to 30th June.

Compatibility;

No matter how the TACT elements of the behavior are defined, it is

important to observe the principle of compatibility which requires

that all other constructs (attitude, subjective norm, and intention)

be defined in terms of exactly the same elements. Thus, the attitude

compatible with this behavior was the attitude toward paying taxes

in full and promptly to Uganda Revenue Authority (URA) at the end

of the financial year, the subjective norm was the perceived social

pressure to do so. Attitude, subjective norm, and intention were

assessed directly by means of standard scaling procedures. When

developing the scales, the measures were directly compatible with

the behavior in terms of action, target, context, and time elements.

3.5.2 Measurement of Intention:A seven point likert scale ranging from extremely likely – extremely

unlikely was used to assess behavioural intention. The statement

read as follows;

I intend to pay my taxes in full and promptly to Uganda Revenue

Authority (URA) at the end of the financial year

30

Page 31: Final Copy TRA

Extremely unlikely: _____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:

extremely likely

3.5.3 Measurement of attitude towards behavior:The criterion for item selection has to do with the qualitative

aspects of evaluation represented by the adjective scales. Attitude

toward a behavior was defined as a person’s overall evaluation of

performing the behavior in question. However, empirical research

has shown that overall evaluation often contains two separable

components. One component is instrumental in nature, represented

by such adjective pairs as valuable — worthless, and harmful —

beneficial. The second component has a more experiential quality

and is reflected in such scales as pleasant — unpleasant and

enjoyable — un-enjoyable. The study used the former scale as it is

more applicable given the nature of the study. The measures took

the following form as the owner of the SBE responding;

For me, paying taxes in full and promptly to Uganda Revenue

Authority (URA) at the end of the financial year is

Good: _____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____: bad

3.5.4 Measurement of subjective norms:Several questions were formulated depending on the social

environment of the SBEs and thus questions were formed given the

answers provided as to whose opinions they value. These included;

spouses, family, friends, and fellow SBE owners among others.

They took the following format.

The people in my life whose opinions I value would

Approve: _____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____: disapprove

Of my paying taxes in full and promptly to Uganda Revenue

Authority (URA) at the end of the financial year

However, responses to such items were often found to have low

variability because important others were generally perceived to

31

Page 32: Final Copy TRA

approve of desirable behaviours and disapprove of undesirable

behaviours. To alleviate this problem, it was recommended that the

initial set of items also include questions designed to capture

descriptive norms, i.e., whether important others themselves

performed the behavior in question. This study captured this aspect

by posing the following types of questions.

Most people who are important to me pay their taxes in full

and promptly to Uganda Revenue Authority (URA) at the end of the

financial year

Completely true: _____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:

completely false

Many people like me pay their taxes in full and promptly to Uganda

Revenue Authority (URA) at the end of the financial year

Extremely unlikely: _____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:_____:

extremely likely

3.5.5 Measurement of tax compliance behavior Tax compliance was measured basing on the four items from

Wenzel, (2001) which include; non-lodgments, tax arrears, pay

income and promptness. To establish non-lodgment; respondents

were asked if they had made attempts to avoid taxes by not

declaring at all. Tax arrears was measured by inquiring from the

respondent if they had any outstanding taxes un paid, while pay

income; as measured by establishing whether the respondent had

attempted to under declare taxes. Lastly promptness was measure

by inquiring from the respondents if they had ever delayed to pay

taxes beyond the stipulated time.

3.6 Demographic and practice related control variables: In addition, we control for demographic factors such as age,

gender, education, marital status. As regards age, we assume that

older people who have acquired more social capital (Tittle, 1980;

32

Page 33: Final Copy TRA

Hanno & Violette, 1996) may feel a stronger attachment to the

community, which might in turn induce additional restrictions that

lead to a positive correlation between age and tax compliance

behavior. (Pommerehne and Weck- Hannemann, 1996). Tittle

(1980) observed that younger taxpayers were more risk-seeking,

were less sensitive to penalties, and also reflected the social and

psychological differences related to the period in which they were

raised. (Richardson, 2006) Prior studies have found a positive

association between age and tax compliance behavior (e.g., Vogel,

1974; Tittle, 1980). Gender is another important variable

Moreover, not only has social psychological research suggested

that women are more compliant and less self-reliant than men (e.g.,

Tittle, 1980).

Prior studies have shown a positive association between gender

and tax compliance (Vogel, 1974; Spicer and Becker, 1980; Tittle,

1980; Spicer and Hero, 1985; Baldry, 1987;, Mason & Calvin, 1978;

Hanno & Violette, 1996). Education represents an additional key

tax compliance variable. Jackson & Milliron (1986) found that

education has two important elements: the general degree of fiscal

knowledge, and the specific degree of knowledge regarding tax

evasion opportunities. They argue that enhancing the level of

general fiscal knowledge improves tax compliance by means of

more positive perceptions of taxation, while increased knowledge

of tax evasion opportunities has a negative influence on tax

compliance as it assists noncompliance.

3.7 Data Processing

3.7.1 Normality:A total of 400 questionnaires were collected of data to be analyzed.

The data collected was coded, and analyzed using of the SPSS 17.0

statistical package. Time and effort was dedicated to data cleaning

in order to minimize errors in the data. After controlling for

33

Page 34: Final Copy TRA

outliers, data based on 394 respondents was used. Despite the fact

that almost all the scales used in this research are adapted from

earlier works, with high reliability and validate rankings, these

scales were tested for reliability, because they were applied a new

environment, where they have never been used. First the data was

tested for normality using histograms and the Q-Q Plots (See

appendix II). Kolmogorov-Smirnov and the Shapiro-Wilk tests were

used to check whether the distribution as a whole deviates from a

comparable normal distribution. The non-significance level being

P>0.05 implying the distribution of the sample is not significantly

different from a normal distribution and therefore probably normal.

Results are below. The results show a probable normal distribution.

Table: 1

Kolmogorov-Smirnov and the Shapiro-Wilk testTests of Normality

Kolmogorov-Smirnova Shapiro-Wilk

Variable df Sig. Variable df Sig.Attitudes 400 .000 Attitudes 400 .000

Subjective norms

400 .021 Subjective norms 400 .044

Intentions 400 .000 Intentions 400 .000

Tax compliance

400 .000 Tax compliance 400 .000

a. Lilliefors Significance Correction

3.7.2 Reliability testThe questionnaire scales were tested for their reliability. One of the

key aspects of reliability is the scale’s internal consistency.

Reliability tests ensure that measurements consistently reflect the

construct that it is measuring. One of the most commonly used

indicators of internal consistency is Cronbach’s alpha co-efficient.

Kline (1999) notes that although the generally accepted value is 0.8

for cognitive tests, ability tests have a cut-off point of 0.7. He goes

on to say that when dealing with psychological constructs; values

34

Page 35: Final Copy TRA

below even 0.7 can realistically be expected because of the

diversity of the constructs being measured. However Cortina

(1993) notes that the value of alpha depends on the number of

items on the scale as the top half of the alpha equation includes the

number of items squared. With the above in mind, the researcher

followed Pallant, (2005) assertion that short scales with around ten

items, can have low Cronbach’s alpha values from 0.5.

The negatively worded questions (ATT; 3, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11 and

INT; 3, 5, 6, 8, 11, 12- see appendix III)’s scales were reversed

before checking the reliability, (scales 1-7 were changed to 7-1 in

the reverse order) in order to reduce response bias. Further the

Cronbach’s alpha when item deleted was employed and deleted the

following items from the scales. On the scale of attitudes, items 3,

11 and 13 were deleted. On the intentions scale, items 5, 6, 10 and

12 were deleted. Whereas on the subjective and descriptive scales

none of the items was deleted, on the tax compliance scale, 1, 6, 8

and 11 had to be deleted. (See appendix III) The reasons for

eliminating these items could include reasons such as; they are

factual, they are evaluatively ambiguous or they were irrelevant as

indicated by failure to correlate with the total scale score. The

Cronbach’s alpha is thus reflected in table 2.

Table 2:

Reliability test statistics

Variable Cronbach’s alpha

Cronbach’s alpha based on standardized items

No of items

Attitudes 0.625 0.639 10

Subjective norms

0.769 0.760 5

Intentions 0.614 0.664 8

Descriptive norms

0.620 0.628 5

Tax 0.602 0.612 6

35

Page 36: Final Copy TRA

compliance

36

Page 37: Final Copy TRA

Chapter Four

Results

4.0 IntroductionThis chapter presents the study findings. First, the researcher

presents a description statistics of the small business taxpayers in

the sample. The second part comprises of explanatory component

of the study. In this section we present the results of the

correlation and regression analysis. In this analysis we test the

hypotheses as stated in Chapter 2. The Pearson coefficient was

used to determine the correlation between the TRA variables. For

objective 1 of the study, a model employing hierarchical multiple

linear regression analysis was used to assess the influence of

attitudes and subjective norms on behavioural intention of SBEs

taxpayers in Uganda. In this model, measures of attitude and

subjective norm served as the independent variables, with intention

being the dependent variable.

The second objective was also analysed using a simple linear

regression to assess the influence of behavioural intentions of SBEs

on tax compliance in Uganda. In this model, behavioural intentions

are the independent variable and tax compliance behaviour is the

dependent. To further analyze the data, the researcher constructed

two more simple linear regressions, one having attitudes as the

independent variable and tax compliance behaviour as the

dependent and the second having subjective norms as the

independent and the former as the dependent. This was to assess

the extent of the relevance of the theory to explain tax compliance

behaviour in comparison to the two individual measurements of

attitudes and subjective norms.

37

Page 38: Final Copy TRA

4.0 Descriptive statistics

4.1.1 Sample characteristicsAs illustrated below, about 46% of the respondents in the sample

were female, while males contribute the majority at 53% of the

sample. Owing to social, cultural, economic and psychological

factors, males are generally more represented in the business

sector than females. The average age of the entrepreneurs in the

sample was about 31 years. The youngest respondent was 16 years,

while the oldest was 64 years. The majority of the respondents are

in the 26-35 yeas age brackets and about 86% of the entrepreneurs

were 45 years or younger. About 47% of the respondents were

married and another sizeable category 31% of the respondents

were single as exhibited in table 3.

By and large, the majority (about 94%) of the respondents in this

sample had received some formal education, while about 6% of the

respondents had never received any form of education; this implies

that on the whole, the majority of the entrepreneurs in this sample

were educated.

Table 3Demographic related characteristics of respondentsCharacteristic Small Business Enterprises

Gender Female (%) 186(46.5) Male (%) 214(53.5) Missing (%) 0(0)

Age group 16-25(%) 84(21.0) 26-35(%) 157(39.3) 36-45(%) 104(26.0) 46-55(%) 37(9.3) 55-65(%) 18(4.5)

Marital status Single (%) 125(31.3) Married (%) 187(46.8) Living together (%) 64(16.0) Divorced/ Separated (%) 14(3.5)

38

Page 39: Final Copy TRA

Widow/Widower (%) 10(2.5)

Education levels Uneducated (%) 24(6.0) Primary level (%) 43(10.8) O’level (%) 70(17.5) A’ level (%) 61(15.3) Vocational certificate (%) 40(10.0) Diploma (%) 77(19.3) Bachelor’s degree (%) 74(18.5) Master’s degree (%) 11(2.8)

Despite the fact that our population consisted of small business

enterprises, we went further to profile the respondents according

to size using their annual turnover. We used the annual turnover,

because it’s the basis for levying taxes from small business

enterprises. The very Small business enterprises with an annual

turnover between 500,000 and - 5,000,000 Uganda shillings (1

Euro approximately = 2700 Uganda Shillings) dominated the

sample (62%) and about 4.3% of the respondents could not tell

their annual turnover because they did not have any business

records.

In Uganda, tax payers are required by law to file a self-assessment

tax return annually. Respondents were asked to indicate the

number years in which they ha filed a self-assessment tax return.

The majority of the respondents (45.25%) had never filed a self-

assessment tax return with the Uganda Revenue Authority, the tax

collection body. For the respondents who file self assessment tax

returns with Uganda Revenue Authority, we also interested to

know who prepares their tax returns. As exhibited below, the

majority of the respondents (89.2%) prepared their own tax

returns, while about 10.8% of the respondents hire tax experts to

prepare the tax returns.

Table 4Practice related characteristics of respondents

Characteristics Small Business enterprises

39

Page 40: Final Copy TRA

Size of the enterprise (UGX) 500,000-5,000,000 (%) 247(61.8) 5,000,001-25,000,000 (%) 109(27.3) 25,000,000-50,000,001 (%) 27(6.8) Do not know (%) 17(4.3)

Longevity of filing tax returns Never filed (%) 181(45.3) 1-5 yrs (%) 141(35.3) 6-10yrs (%) 47(11.8) 11-15yrs (%) 17(4.3) 16-20yrs (%) 10(2.5) More than 20yrs (%) 4(1.0)

Tax return preparation Tax preparer (%) 175(43.8) Tax expert (%) 51(12.8) Missing (%) 174(43.5)

4.1.2 Descriptive statistics for theory variablesTable 5 summarizes the means and standards deviation of the TRA

variables. The mean behavioural intention score was 5.36

(SD±0.804), scores here ranged from 3.55 – 6.49. The mean score

of the measure of attitudes was 3.96 (SD±0.924) and interestingly

both the highest and lowest mean scores 1.95 and 5.57 had the

lowest standard deviation. For subjective norms the mean score

was 3.75 (SD±0.637) ranging from 2.36 to 5.02. The mean

response to the tax compliance behavior was 2.61 (SD±0.777),

reflected by the mean range of 1.74-3.75. (See Appendix 1)

Table 5:Descriptive statistics for the theory of reasoned action variables

Variable Mean

Std. Deviation

Attitudes 3.98 0.924Intentions 5.36 0.805Tax compliance behavior 2.61 0.777Subjective norms 3.75 0.637

40

Page 41: Final Copy TRA

4.1.3 Correlations:All correlations between the theory variables were significant

(P<0.01). Table 6 describes the correlation between the theory

variables. Attitudes were negatively correlated with intentions,

subjective norms (r=-0.319, r=-0.111, all at P<0.01) respectively,

positively correlated with tax compliance (r=0.143, at P<0.01).

There was a significant negative correlation between the intention

and tax compliance (r=-0.373, at P<0.01 respectively) and a

significant positive correlation with subjective norms (r=0.199 at

P<0.01).

Table 6Correlation among the theory of reasoned action variablesVariable 1 2 3 4

1. Attitudes - -0.319**

0.143** -0.111*

2. Intentions - -0.373** 0.199**

3.Taxcompliance - -0.179**

4. Subjective norms

-

**. Correlations are significant at the 0.01 level (2-tailed).

However one cannot test hypotheses using bivariate correlations,

since these correlations may be influenced by other variables, a

phenomenon also known as the third variable problem (Field,

2005). To control for such third variable problems, one needs to

perform regression analysis.

4.1.4 Objective 1: Predictors of Intention

A multiple regression model I was constructed to assess the ability

of attitude and subjective norms to predict intention. Table 7

describes the results of the regression analyses. In this model, the

utility of the TRA variables in explaining SBEs intention to pay or

not pay their taxes was determined using the above constructs. As

can be seen, a significant model emerged (β= -0.216, p < .001) for

measure of attitudes and (β = 0.248, p < .001) for the measure of

subjective norms. Adjusted R square is 0.107 or 10.7%, therefore

41

Page 42: Final Copy TRA

attitudes and subjective norms account for 10.7% change in tax

payers intentions to comply with tax laws and regulations, note

however that both are predictors are significant. The question here

is how much unique explanatory variance is attributable to each of

the predictors? The shared prediction is accounted for in the

difference between the adjusted r square and the r square (.112 –

107). The multiple regressions show that subjective norms provide

a larger independent contribution to the prediction of intentions

than attitudes. This is reflected in the betas.

Table 7Regression resultsPredictors’ Unstandardized

Coefficients (β) Standard errorSig (p)

Model I(R square = .112)(Adjusted r sq =.107)Attitudes -0.216* 0.040 .000Subjective norms 0.248* 0.056

.000

*P < .001Dependent variable; intentionsIndependent variable; attitudes, subjective norms

4.1.5 Objective 2: Predictor of tax complianceIn this second step of the analysis, the aim is to explain tax

compliance behavior among small business enterprises. In line with

the theory of reasoned action, at this level, the researcher used

intentions to predict tax compliance and therefore this model II

employs a simple linear regression. The dependent variable in this

case is tax compliance behavior and the independent being

intentions this time. This model was also statistically significant (β

= -0.412, p < .001) for the measure of intentions. This model

explained 15.9% (adjusted r square) of the variance in tax

compliance behavior. Results are displayed in table 8.

42

Page 43: Final Copy TRA

Table 8

Predictor Unstandardized Coefficients (β) Standard error

Sig (p)Model II

(R square = .161)(Adjusted r sq =.159) Intentions -0.412* 0.048

.000

*P < .001Dependent variable; tax complianceIndependent variable; intentions

4.1.6 Supplement analysesObjective 3 & 4:

Subjective norms and Attitudes predicting tax compliance

independently.

The models III and IV were used to predict tax compliance in

relation to subjective norms and attitudes respectively. One was a

significant predictor that is subjective norms (β = -0.234, p <

0.001) with adjusted r square of 0.035. Thus subjective norms

alone can explain 3.5% of the change in tax compliance. Whereas

attitudes was not significant (β = 0.119, p > 001) with adjusted r

square being 0.017 explaining only 1.7% of the variance in tax

compliance behavior. It is interesting to note the difference in the

adjusted r square between the model 1 and II (the theory of TRA)

and using models III and IV in explaining tax compliance behavior

(Supplement analysis). The results for models III and IV are

reflected in table 9.

Table 9 Predictor Unstandardized

Coefficients (β) Standard errorSig (p)

43

Page 44: Final Copy TRA

Model III(R square = .038)(Adjusted r sq =.035)Subjective norms -0.234* .060 .000

Model IV(R square = .019)(Adjusted r sq =.017)Attitudes 0.119 .043 .006

*P < .001Dependent variable; tax complianceIndependent variables; subjective norms (model III); attitudes (model IV)

The table below shows the contribution of the control variables in

explaining tax compliance. As can been seen only one of the

predictors is significant. It is also quite interesting. This is marital

status. This means marital status can determine whether SBE

owners take the risk to evade taxes

Table 10Regression results for control variablesPredictor Unstandardized

Coefficients (β) standard error sig (p)

Age -.011 .004 .013Marital status .161* .044

.000Gender .101 .072

.164Highest level of education -.029 .019 .136Gross turnover Income -.017 .063

.788

*P < .001Dependent variable; tax complianceIndependent variable; Age, marital status, gender, highest level of education, gross turnover

44

Page 45: Final Copy TRA

Chapter five

Discussion of findings

5.0 Introduction: An Overview of Work Done

The major objective of this study was to investigate the extent to

which the theory of reasoned action explains variations in tax

compliance behavior among small business enterprises in Uganda.

In this study, a distinction was made between the components of

the theory of reasoned action and general factors within the theory

that have been used in literature to explain tax compliance before.

TRA variables included; attitudes, subjective norms, behavioral

intentions and tax compliance behavior. Employing hierarchical

multiple regression as well as simple linear regression, the study

predicted the extent to which the theory (TRA) explains variance in

tax compliance behavior. Further supplementary analysis was

made to compare the explanatory variance difference of tax

compliance between the TRA and individual predictors of attitudes

and subjective norms.

5.1 Discussion of Results

The findings discussed in this chapter are presented in Chapter

four, starting with the descriptive statistics, correlations, multiple

regression analysis and finally the simple linear regressions. The

main objective of this chapter is to attach meaning to the study

findings within the study context and tax compliance behavioral

literature.

5.1.1 Sample characteristics

45

Page 46: Final Copy TRA

The gender-business statistics are close to the findings of Walter et

al. (2003; 2004) and Sserwanga (2010) in Uganda and Krauss

(2003) in Zimbabwe and South Africa, where female participation

in entrepreneurship was found to be lower than male participation.

The age findings imply that in Uganda, the young people are the

most active in business related sectors. The findings are consistent

with the findings of Sserwanga (2010) who reported an average

age of 30 years and 84% of the respondents were 45 years or

younger in his entrepreneurial quality study. The size of the

business was measured using turnover. The findings are consistent

with the findings of Sserwanga (2010), whose sample of

entrepreneurs in Uganda consisted of 64% micro enterprises he

makes a similar conclusion with about of the enterprises in his

sample labeled. It’s important to note that Sserwanga (2010) used

the number of persons employed, while this study based annual

turnover to measure firm size, coming up with consistent results. In

addition Snyder (2000) used number of employees to classify

Ugandan firms into respective sizes and came to a similar

conclusion about small and medium enterprises, labeling them

vibrant, fragile and underdeveloped.

5.2 Hypothesis The tax compliance prediction models disclose both varied and

interesting results about the hypotheses developed in Chapter two.

The statistical analysis in the paper mostly confirmed the theory.

The theory was particularly effective in explaining actual

compliance behavior. The theory of reasoned action explains about

10.7% of the variation in tax compliance in Uganda SBEs. It is

therefore important to note however that there are other factors

that explain 89.3% (i.e. the difference between 100% and what TRA

explains) tax compliance. These factors are likely to include; audit

rates, penalties, tax compliance costs, tax payer ethics, tax

46

Page 47: Final Copy TRA

fairness, and social norms among others (Alm, Sanchez, and

DeJuan, 1995; Cowell, 1992; Kirchler, 1999; Wenzel, 2002, 2004).

5.2.1 H1 - tax payers’ attitudes influence their tax compliance behavioral intentionIt has been shown in the preceding chapter using multiple

regressions that attitudes influence behavioral intentions . Note

however that the two have a significant negative correlation which

is later reflected in the beta of the regression. The results confirm

our hypothesis Further the relationship established by the negative

β means that a positive change in attitudes will result in a negative

change in behavioral intentions and vice versa. Implying a

percentage increase in attitudes will lead to 21.6% decrease in tax

compliance. However since attitudes are not a dichotomous

variable then we take this explanation in a different sense. This

then implies that Ugandan SBE owners generally have a negative

attitude towards intentions to comply with tax regulations and

laws.

The interesting question here would be why do SBEs have a

negative attitude towards paying tax? During the interviews

conducted, the SBE owners by and large said that the government

is corrupt and therefore they see no need why they should pay

more taxes and yet they don’t benefit from them. This shows that

taxpayers are normally sensitive to the way government uses tax

revenues. Taxes are supposed to be used to construct

infrastructure and provide social services to the public however,

the SBEs do not see the results of their efforts to pay tax. Taxes

therefore paid by individuals can be interpreted as the price paid

for governments’ positive action. literature and empirical findings

attest to this when Cummings, Martinez-Vazquez, McKee and

Torgler, (2004) found in South Africa and Botswana that

perceptions of the government as being corrupt will reduce the

47

Page 48: Final Copy TRA

willingness to comply with taxes. A higher trust of the population

might tend to increase taxpayers positive attitudes and

commitment to the tax payment which finally has a positive effect

on tax compliance. (Smith, 1992) Another reason could be the wide

spread tax evasion evident in the country. Literature explains this

as; people who believe that tax cheating is wide-spread among

their peers or colleagues tend to have more favourable attitudes

towards tax evasion and are more likely to cheat on taxes (See De

Juan, Lasheras, & Mayo, 1994; Kaplan & Reckers, 1985;

Wallschutzky, 1984; Webley, Robben, & Morris, 1988).

From the analysis, we further find that subjective norms have a

greater influence on intention than attitudes. This is not in

agreement with previous TRA literature which reports that attitude

has a stronger influence on intention than subjective norms. The

role of attitude and social norms in explaining intentions however

is contingent upon the behavior under investigation. (Khanna,

Kavookjian, Scott, Kamal, Miller and Neal, 2009). From this we

deduce that in tax compliance studies attitudes generally explain

intentions to a greater extent than subjective norms. Literature

relates that the contribution of attitudes, subjective norms to the

prediction of intentions is expected to vary not only depending on

behavior under study but also from one group of individuals to

another. The intentions of some populations may be affected mostly

by attitudes and less so by subjective norms. (Ajzen and Fishbein,

2004) It is possible that in the Ugandan population, subjective

norms are a more important consideration to intentions to comply

decisions in relation to attitudes. The negative β though makes it an

interesting finding, not only are the SBEs in Uganda having a

negative attitude towards intending to pay their tax but subjective

norms are also the bigger psychological consideration than

personal beliefs or attitudes.

48

Page 49: Final Copy TRA

Existing literature further indicates that to stimulate behavioural

intention, a favourable attitude needs to be formed. The more

favourable is the attitude, the stronger is the behavioural intention,

and the more likely is the performance of the behavior. (Ramayah,

Rouibah, Gopi and Rangel, 2009) This could explain further why

the regression results reflect a negative relationship between

intention and tax compliance. It could have originated from the

influence of the negative relationship with attitudes. We should

also bear in mind that all the theory of reasoned action variables

are not dichotomous; generally we deduce from the findings that

intentions too have a negative relationship with tax compliance.

This will be explained further in the discussion.

5.2.2 H2 - taxpayers’ subjective norms influence their tax compliance behavioral intentionsThe model I multiple regression in the preceding chapter

established that subjective norms do influence tax compliance

behavioral intentions. To add to this, a significant positive

correlation of .199 resulted between subjective norms with

behavioral intentions. This means that a positive change in

subjective norms will result in a positive change in tax compliance

behavioral intentions. This was further reflected in a positive β of

0.248, meaning that a percentage increase in subjective norms will

lead to a 24.8% increase in tax compliance behavioral intentions.

Note however, the dichotomous nature of the subjective norm

variable. This means that generally, Ugandan SBEs owners’

families, friends, peers and other parameters in their social

environment influence them positively to intend to pay their taxes.

This finding is in line with Fishbein and Ajzen (2010) who found

that other things being equal, the stronger the perceived social

pressure, the more likely it is that an intention to perform the

behavior will be formed. The direct positive relationship of

49

Page 50: Final Copy TRA

subjective norm to intention is in line with prior research

conducted by Fishbein and Ajzen (1975), Taylor and Todd (1995),

Mathieson (1991), Bhattacherjee (2000), Ajzen (1991), Ajzen

(2002), Ramayah et al. (2002), Ma’ruf et al. (2003), Eri (2004), May

(2005), Athiyaman (2002), and Gopi and Ramayah (2007). Yet

again, the question to ask is why SBE owners are influenced

positively by their peers, friends and families to conform to tax

regulations and laws? This could be attributed to the cultural set up

of Uganda. Uganda is generally has a communal background,

generally collective in nature, where people live in societies from

birth onwards are integrated into strong, cohesive in-groups, often

extended families which continue protecting them in exchange for

unquestioning loyalty. So positive influence from the society

members are reflected by individuals. Thus in Uganda, society

influences SBEs positively in terms of intentions to comply with the

tax laws. This study will add to the not so vast literature on

subjective norms and tax compliance as it broke them down to

injunctive and descriptive.

5.2.3 H3 taxpayers’ behavioral intentions influence their tax compliance behaviorEagly and Chaiken (1993), said that the fact that I am capable of

performing a behavior does not necessarily, imply that I will do it.

To put it in another way; the fact that one intends to do something

doesn’t imply that they will do it. This will be reflected in the

discussion below about tax compliance of SBEs in the Ugandan

context.

Hypothesis 3 was confirmed by our results. Behavioral intentions

influence tax compliance behavior of SBEs. A negative correlation

was found between the two variables, it was significant and further

reflected in the regression by the β of 0.401. This implies that a

positive change in intentions leads to negative change in tax

compliance behavior. Recollect the dichotomous nature of our

50

Page 51: Final Copy TRA

variables, a percentage increase in behavioral intentions will lead

to a 40.1% reduction in tax compliance. This is an interesting

finding for Uganda Revenue Authority and for research purposes.

The above finding implies that on the whole, Ugandan SBEs owners

don’t intend to comply with tax regulations and laws. Applicable

reasons here could be ones mentioned before that have to do with

SBE owners’ attitudes in general. As was already mentioned, the

more favourable the attitude is, the stronger the behavioural

intention, and the more likely the performance of the behavior.

(Ramayah, Rouibah, Gopi and Rangel, 2009), and the reverse could

apply in Uganda’s situation.

Another interesting finding was that intentions explain 15.9% of the

variance in tax compliance. This is quite substantial; however we

must acknowledge that there are other factors that influence tax

compliance too. This could make up the extra 84.1% (100-15.9)

percentage. It is further a significant predictor and fitted in our

model well. Other factors that literature and empirical findings

have come up with include culture (Cummings, Martinez-Vazquez,

McKee and Torgler, 2004), audit rates, penalties, fairness, tax and

admistration, among others (Alm, Sanchez, & DeJuan, 1995;

Cowell, 1992; Kirchler, 1999; Wenzel, 2002, 2004). These would

provide a more interesting study if combined.

5.3 Supplementary analysis:This part of discussion will focus on objectives 3 and 4 of the

preceding chapter. The researcher included this part of the study

to establish whether attitudes or subjective norms could

individually explain tax compliance better than the theory of

reasoned action. As can be inferred from the table 9, attitudes

explain 1.7% of variance in tax compliance behavior. Another

interesting and yet unexpected finding given the above discussion

was that attitudes scored a positive β of 0.119. This implies that a

percentage increase in attitudes will lead to a 11.9% increase in tax

51

Page 52: Final Copy TRA

Attitudes

Subjective norms

Intentions

Tax complian

ceBehavior

-0.216*

0.248*

-0.412*

-0.234*

0.119

compliance; therefore generally Ugandan SBEs have positive

attitudes towards tax compliance. Thus independently, without the

theory of reasoned action, attitudes of SBEs towards tax are

positive.

The subjective norms could explain only 3.5% of the variance in tax

compliance. Interestingly and also unexpectedly, subjective norms

have a negative influence on tax compliance of SBEs in Uganda.

This is reflected by the β coefficient of 0.234, therefore a

percentage increase in subjective norms will lead to a 23.4% drop

in tax compliance of SBEs. This implies that Ugandan SBEs are

influenced negatively by subjective norms when complying with tax

regulations and laws. Note however that the theory doesn’t

examine attitudes or subjective norms towards tax compliance

directly but uses the intervening variable of intentions.

So the framework comes out as follows after the analysis.

52

Page 53: Final Copy TRA

Chapter Six

Conclusions and recommendations

6.0 Introduction

The major objective of this study was to investigate the extent to

which the theory of reasoned action can explain variance in tax

compliance behavior among SBEs in Uganda. This objective was

further broken down into four sub questions; the influence of

attitudes and subjective norms of on behavioral intentions of

taxpayers (SBEs) in Uganda; the influence of behavioral intentions

of SBEs on tax compliance in Uganda; the influence of subjective

norms of SBEs on tax compliance in Uganda and finally; the

influence of attitudes of SBEs on tax compliance in Uganda. The

study was grounded in the theory of reasoned action and

supplementary analysis was done on exterior the theory.

6.1 ConclusionThe study established that the theory of reasoned action indeed

provides insight on the tax compliance behavior of SBEs. Not only

did the study ascertain the extent to which TRA influences tax

compliance but also ascertained the kind of relationship/ influence

it has on tax compliance behavior of SBEs in Uganda. The findings

provide evidence for the negative attitudes SBEs have on intentions

to comply with tax regulations and the extent to which these

attitudes can affect SBEs behavior. Further the findings also

illustrated that subjective norms influence SBEs tax compliance

intentions in a positive manner. However the over all appearance of

these intentions lead to a negative effect on tax compliance

behavior of these SBEs. Thus on the whole, SBEs appear not to

intend to pay their taxes. Possible reasons were also explained in

chapter five as to why these results would appear so. Subsequent

analysis exterior of TRA however provided surprising results by

53

Page 54: Final Copy TRA

SBEs having positive attitudes towards paying their taxes as well

as being influenced negatively by subjective norms.

6.2 Implications and RecommendationsThe results of this study are endowed with theoretical, and policy

implications. These implications are discussed below and pertinent

recommendations proposed. We nevertheless note that many of the

recommendations are cut across the categories, which calls for an

integrated approach when dealing with tax compliance issues.

6.2.1 Theoretical implicationsThe theory of reasoned action has been used to explain behavior in

numerous situations in psychology. This study has used this theory

not only in a new setting but also in a new geographical, economic

and administrative setting; Uganda. Therefore this implies that not

only is this theory relevant in explaining psychological social issues

but it is also relevant in explaining economic behavior of

individuals namely; tax compliance. In addition this theory has

provided an explanatory difference in its individual predictors

(attitudes and subjective norms) and the structural predictors of

the theory (attitudes and subjective norms leading to intentions and

intentions leading to behavior). This difference thus creates a

lacuna in research that is worth exploring. So the question that

would arise is where does this difference come from and why? A

recommendation here would be a broader study into this area to

answer the above questions.

6.2.2 Policy implications:The research was carried out with certain audiences in mind;

academicians, the Uganda Revenue Authority, and general

audiences interested in tax compliance issues, prior to this study,

none of the above predictors were used in assessing the tax

54

Page 55: Final Copy TRA

environment in Uganda. It has been established that generally

SBEs don’t intend to pay their taxes. This could have been an

influence of the negative attitudes they have. The researcher

recommends that attitudes of SBEs should be taken as a serious

consideration in tax administration. Tax administration should

come up with policy measure that will improve the SBES intentions

to apply tax SBEs make up the biggest population of businesses in

Uganda and though they don’t contribute the biggest percentage of

taxes, if they indeed refrained from paying their taxes, the effect

would be felt by the economy at large. This would fall to the URA to

establish why attitudes of SBEs are negative.

6.3 Areas for further researchThe above recommendations bring us to areas for further research.

The theory of reasoned action was will be later discussed in

relation to the revised to the theory of planned behavior (hence

forth referred to as TPB). This study didn’t focus on TPB, as the

revised theory has additional predictors. The researcher wanted to

keep the study to a manageable level given the time and logistic

constraints. It would be interesting to investigate if the revised

theory can explain tax compliance behavior better compared to the

TRA. This would be a comprehensive study and since the data for

the second part of that study is already available in this report, it

would be simpler to add to the debate a comparison between the

two theories.

The findings from this study are interconnected with each other but

give different views on attitudes and subjective norms. A more

comprehensive study about attitudes and subjective norms in

relation to tax compliance in Uganda would be appropriate in this

instance and gain insight on what drives attitudes and subjective

norms. The core essence of the two predictors would be the

starting point to renew/ revise the tax system for better

55

Page 56: Final Copy TRA

enforcement. This research would facilitate tax administration in

Uganda in a large way.

While carrying out the study, the researcher got into contact with

colleagues who were investigating an interesting problem that

could be a challenge especially in developing countries like

Uganda. This is the “free rider problem”. This is a problem in

Africa on issues such as resources and issues that have to do with

an individual being a recipient. However this particular “tax free

rider problem” has to do with individuals requesting the authorities

to levy taxes on them so that they eradicate the free riders from the

society. It would be fascinating to find out if this kind of problem

can arise in Ugandan tax compliance issues.

A further additional study that could be intriguing would be to

compare the effect of attitudes and subjective norms on tax

compliance between developed and developing countries. The

developed countries have vast literature on tax compliance in

existence as compared to matters/issues that arise from developing

countries. A boost to the research gap especially in Africa can be

filled by such a variety of studies.

6.4 limitations to the studyThe theory of reasoned action since its postulation in 1975 has

been revised to include other predictors. In the first instance,

perceived behavioral control was included to predict intention but

this was not included in this study. However the revised theory is

referred to as the “theory of planned behavior. In this research the

old theory was to explain tax compliance behavior. The application

of the theory of planned behavior to explain tax compliance is

suggested for future research.

As no research is perfect some limitations may arise; the theory at

large has been used in association with consumer behavior and

56

Page 57: Final Copy TRA

thus this new application to tax compliance is unique so it raises

questions of generalizing the findings. Out of the 426

questionnaires the researcher was supposed to collect, 400 were

collected, and 394 were good for usage in analysis. However this

still cant be a reason not to generalize the findings to Uganda, but

this was a capital city population and therefore the findings cant be

generalized to Uganda as a whole and therefore on a larger scale

not even to developing countries.

Ajzen revised methodological considerations for this kind of study

recommended a pilot study before collecting actual data. Due to

time constraints it was not possible to do a pilot study to establish

the drivers of attitudes and therefore a former study questionnaire

was used and adopted for the study. A further constraint in this

was it was used in Canada and therefore may have not been the

best in a Ugandan case and may have affected the quality of data

collected, though given the reliability tests we were able to get

usable data.

Issues of confidentiality arose with some respondents as tax

payment is a sensitive issue in Africa. Thus cooperation from

respondents was with difficulty as SBEs may did not wish to

disclose their tax information or compliance rate or ability for that

matter. The results could have been therefore biased to some

extent.

57

Page 58: Final Copy TRA

REFERENCES:Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of planned behavior. In J. Kuhi & J. Beckman (Eds.), Action-control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 11–39). Heidelberg: Springer.

Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioural control, self-efficacy, locus of control and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 32, 1–20.

Ajzen, I., and M. Fishbein (1980) Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Allingham, Michael G., and A. Sandmo (1972) "Income Tax Evasion: A Theoretical Analysis," 1 Journal of Public Economics 323.

Alm, J. “A Perspective on the Experimental Analysis of Taxpayer Reporting.” Accounting Review 66 No. 3 (July, 1991): 577–93.

Alm, J.; Jackson, B.; McKee, M. (1992). Estimating the determinants of taxpayer compliance with experimental data. National Tax Journal, XLV(1), 107–114.

Alm, J, Gary H. McClelland and William D. Schulze, “Why do people pay taxes?” (1992), vol. 48, no. 1 Journal of Public Economics 21-38.

Alm, J., McClelland, G. H., and Schulze, W.D., (1999) “Changing the social norm of tax compliance by voting”, vol. 52, no. 2 Kyklos 141-171.

Alm, J., Sanchez, I., & DeJuan, A. (1995). Economic and noneconomic factors in tax compliance. Kyklos, 48, 3–18.

Andreoni, J, Erard, B and Feinstein, J. 1998. ‘‘Tax Compliance.’’ Journal of Economic Literature 36:818–60.

Antonides, G., and Robben, H.S.J. (1995) True positives and false alarms in the detection of tax evasion. Journal of Economic Psychology, 17, 617-640.

Armitage, C.J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471–499.

Athiyaman, A. (2002). Internet users’ intention to purchase air travel online: An empirical investigation. Marketing Intelligence & Planning, 20(4), 234–242.

58

Page 59: Final Copy TRA

Baldry, J.C., (1986). Tax evasion is not a gamble: a report on two experiments. Economics Letters 22, 333–335.

Baldry, J.C., (1987). Income tax evasion and the tax schedule: some experimental results. Public Finance 42, 357– 383.

Beck, P. J., Davis, J. S., and Jung, W. (1991) “Experimental Evidence on Taxpayer reporting under uncertainty” Acc. Rev. July, 66 (3) pp. 535-58

Becker, G. S. (1968) "Crime and Punishment: An Economic Approach," 76 Journal of Political Economy 169

Bhattacherjee, A. (2000). Acceptance of Internet applications services: The case of electronic brokerages. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics – Part A: Systems and Humans, 30, 411–420.

Blau, P. M., (1964). Exchange and Power in Social Life. New York: J. Wiley.

Bobek, D. and R. Hatfield: (2003), An Investigation of the Theory of Planned Behavior and the Role of Moral Obligation in Tax Compliance_, Behavioural Research in Accounting 15, 13–38.

Bobek, D. D., Roberts, R. W., and Sweeney, J. T., (2007). The Social Norms of Tax Compliance: Evidence from Australia, Singapore, and the United States. Journal of Business Ethics (2007) 74:49–64 _ Springer

Bulutoglu, K, (1995) Presumptive Taxation. In Tax Policy Handbook. Washington DC: International Monetary Fund, pp. 258-262.

Chan, C.W., Troutman, C.S. and O’Bryan, D. (2000). An expanded model of taxpayer compliance: Empirical evidence from the United States and Hong Kong. Journal of International Accounting, Auditing and Taxation, 9, 83-103.

Christian R. J., (ND) On the use of presumptive income taxes to alleviate the burden of tax compliance, University of MichiganCowell, F. A. (1992). Tax evasion and inequity. Journal of Economic Psychology, 13, 521–543.

Cialdini, R. and M. Trost: (1998). Social Influence: Social Norms, Conformity, and Compliance_, in D. Gilbert, S. Fiske and G. Lindzey (eds.), The Handbook of Social Psychology4th edition, (Oxford University Press, New York).

Cowell, F. A. (1990) Cheating the Government: The Economics of Evasion. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Cummings, R. G., J. Martinez-Vazquez, and M. McKee (2006). Experimental evidence on mixing modes in income tax evasion. Public Finance Review 34(6): 663-686

59

Page 60: Final Copy TRA

Davis, J. S., G. Hecht, and J. D. Perkins (2003). Social behaviours, enforcement, and tax compliance dynamics. The Accounting Review 78 (1): 39-69

Davis, F.D., & Warshaw, P.R. (1991). Choice sets and choice intentions. The Journal of Social Psychology, 131, 823–830.

De Juan, A., Lasheras, M.A., Mayo, R., (1993) Voluntary Compliance and Behavior of Spanish Taxpayers. Instituto de Estudios Fiscales, Madrid, Spain.

De Juan, A., Lasheras, M. A., & Mayo, R. (1994). Voluntary tax compliant behavior of Spanish income tax payers. Public Finance, 49, 90–105.

Eagly, A. H., and Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes. Forth Worth, TX: Harcourt Brace.

Elffers, H., (2000) But taxpayers do cooperate! In: VanVugt, M., Snyder, M., Tyler, T.R., Biel, A. (Eds.), Cooperation in Modern Society. Promoting the Welfare of Communities, States and Organizations. Routledge, London, pp. 184–194.

Erard, B, and Feinstein, J. “Honesty and evasion in the tax compliance game.” Rand Journal of Economics 25 No. 1 (1994): 1–19.

Eri, Y. (2004). Retailing on Internet the Buying Intention, MBA Thesis, School of Management, University Sains Malaysia, Penang.

Feige, E. L., (1989) The Underground Economies. Tax Evasion and Information Distortion. Cambridge U. Press, Cambridge

Feld, L. P., and Frey, A. B. “Tax Compliance as the Result of a PsychologicalTax Contract: The Role of Incentives and Responsive Regulation.” Law & Policy 29 No. 1 (January, 2007): 102–20.

Fishbein, M. and Ajzen, I. (1975) Belief, attitude, intention and behavior: An introduction to theory and research (Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley, 1975).

Fishbein, M., and I. Ajzen., (2010) Predicting and Changing behavior: The reasoned action approach. Psychology Press; Taylor and Francis group

Frey, B.S., Holler, M.J., 1998. Tax compliance policy reconsidered. Homo economicus 15, 27–44

Frey, B. and J. Weck-Hannemann: (1984), _The Hidden Economy as an ‘Unobserved’ Variable, European Economic Review 26, 33–53.

60

Page 61: Final Copy TRA

Gilligan, G., and G. Richardson, (2005) Perceptions of tax fairness and tax compliance in Australia, Hong Kong – A preliminary study . Journal of Financial Crime 12 (4): 331-343

Gopi, M., & Ramayah, T. (2007). Applicability of theory of planned behavior in predicting intention to trade online: Some evidence from a developing country. International Journal of Emerging Markets, 2(4), 348–360.

Graetz, M.J., Reinganum, J.F., Wilde, L.L., (1986). The tax compliance game: toward an interactive theory of law enforcement. Journal of Law, Economics, and Organization 2, 1–32.

Greenberg, J., (1984). Avoiding tax avoidance: a (repeated) game-theoretic approach. Journal of Economic Theory 32, 1–13.

Hanno, D., and G. Violette, (1996). An analysis of moral and social influences on taxpayer behavior. Behavioural Research in Accounting 8: 57-75

Jackson, B. R., and V. C. Milliron. (1986). Tax compliance research: Findings, problems, and prospects. Journal of Accounting Literature 5: 125–157.

Kaplan, S. E., and P. M. J. Reckers. (1985). A study of tax evasion judgments. National Tax Journal 38 (March): 97–102.

Khanna R. M. S, Kavookjian, J., Scott, V. G., Kamal, K. M., Miller, L. N., and Neal, W. A., (2009) Using the theory of reasoned action to determine Physicians’ intention to measure body mass index in children and adolescents. Research in Social and Administrative Pharmacy 5 (170.181)

Kirchler, E. (1999). Reactance to taxation: Employers_ attitudes towards taxes. Journal of Socio- Economics, 28, 131–138.

Kohlberg, L. (1969). “Stages and Sequences: The cognitive development approach tosocialization”, in D. Goslin (ed.), Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research,(Chicago, IL: Rand McNally,).

Korobow, A., and Johnson, C., (2007). An Agent–Based Model of Tax Compliance with Social Networks. National Tax Journal Vol. LX, No. 3 September 2007

Kraemer, H. C. (1994). Special methodological problems of childhood development follow-up studies: Focus on planning. In S. L. Friedman & H. C. Haywood (eds.), Developmental follow-up: Concepts, domains, and methods (pp. 259-76). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

61

Page 62: Final Copy TRA

Krauss, S. (2003). Psychological success factors of small and micro business owners in Southern Africa : A longitudinal approach. (Dissertation, University of Giessen).

Levi, M., 1997. Consent, Dissent, and Patriotism. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Langdridge, D., Paschal, S., and Connolly, K. J., (2007) Analyzing Additional Variables in the Theory of Reasoned Action Journal of Applied Social Psychology, (37), 8, pp. 1884–1913.

Lin, J., Chuan Chan, H., and Wei, K. K., (2006) Understanding Competing Application Usage With the Theory of Planned Behavior; Journal of the American society for information science and technology, 57 (10): 1338-1349

Ma’ruf, J. J., Mohamad, O., & Ramayah, T. (2003). Intention to purchase via the Internet: A comparison of two theoretical models. The Proceedings of the 5th Asian Academy of Management Conference, Challenges of Globalized Business: The Asian Perspective, Hyatt Hotel, Kuantan, Pahang, Malaysia.

Madden, T.J., Ellen, P.S., & Ajzen, I. (1992) A comparison of the theory of planned behavior and the theory of reasoned action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 3–9.

Maital, S., (1982) Minds, Markets, and Money. Basic Books, New York. Margolis, H., 1997. Religion as paradigm. Journal of Institutional and Theoretical Economics 153, 242–252.

Mason, R., and L. D. Calvin., (1984) "Public Confidence and Admit-ted Tax Evasion," 37 National Tax Journal 489.

Mathieson, K. (1991). Predicting user intention: Comparing the technology acceptance model with the theory of planned behavior. Information Systems Research, 2(3), 173–191.

May, O. S. (2005). User Acceptance of Internet Banking in Penang: A Model Comparison Approach, MBA Thesis, School of Management, University Sains Malaysia, Penang.

Meints, J. M and Roberts, B. W. (2009) The Psychology of Individual Tax Evasion: An Exploration of Personality and Social Cognition.

Mueller, J. Fred (ND) Tax compliance and Small business

Ogenyi E. O., and N. Owusu-Frimpong. (2007) Life Insurance in Nigeria: An Application of the Theory of Reasoned Action to Consumers’ Attitudes and Purchase Intention The Service Industries Journal, Vol.27, No.7, October , pp.963–976

Parasuraman, A., Zeithaml, V.A. and Berry, L.L. (1985) A conceptual model of service quality and its implications for future research, Journal of Marketing, 49(autumn), pp.41–50.

62

Page 63: Final Copy TRA

Polinsky, M.A., Shavell, S., (2000). The economic theory of public enforcement of law. Journal of Economic Literature 38, 45–76.

Pommerehne, W.W., Hart, A., Frey, B.S., (1994). Tax morale, tax evasion and the choice of policy instruments in different political systems. Public Finance 49 (Suppl.), 52–69.

Pommerehne, W. W., and Weck-Hannemann, H., (1996). ‘‘Tax Rates, Tax Administration and Income Tax Evasion in Switzerland.’’ Public Choice 88:161–70. Putnam, Robert

Porcano, T. M. (1988). Correlates of tax evasion. Journal of Economic Psychology, 9(1), 47–67.

Ramayah, T., Kamel, R., Gopi, M., and Rangel, G. R., (2009) A decomposed theory of reasoned action to explain intention to use Internet stock trading among Malaysian investors; Computers in Human Behavior (25) 1222–1230

Rest, J. R. (1986) Moral Development: Advances in Research and Theory (Prager Press, New York, NY).

Rest, J. R., (1987) Guide for the Defining Issues Test (Minneapolis, MN: Centre for the Study of Ethical Development).

Richardson, G., (2006). The Impact of Tax Fairness Dimensions on Tax Compliance Behavior in an Asian Jurisdiction: The Case of Hong Kong; International tax journal winter

Richardson, M., and Sawyer, A.J. (2001). A taxonomy of the tax compliance literature: Further findings, problems and prospects. Australian Tax Forum, 16, 137-320.

Schnepper, J. (2004). ‘Deduct it: Odds against an Audit are good’: http://www.moneycentral.msn.com/content/ taxes/Avoidanaudit/P33948.asp.

Scholz, J. T., and N. Pinney. (1995). ‘‘Duty, Fear, and Tax Compliance: The Heuristic Basis of Citizenship Behavior.’’ American Journal of Political Science 39:490–512.

Sheeran, P., Trafimow, D., Finlay, K. A., and Norman, P. (2002). Evidence that the type of person affects the strength of the perceived behavioural control intention relationship. British journal of social psychology, 41, 253-270

Sheppard, B.H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P.R. (1988). The theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with recommendations for modifications and future research. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 325–343.

Sheppard, B.H., Hartwick, J. and Warshaw, P.R. (1988) The theory of reasoned action: a meta-analysis of past research with recommendations

63

Page 64: Final Copy TRA

for modifications and future research, Journal of Consumer Research, 15(3), pp.325–43.

Smith, S., (1986). Britain’s Shadow Economy. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Smith, K. W. 1992. ‘‘Reciprocity and Fairness: Positive Incentives for Tax Compliance.’’Pp. 223–58 in J. Slemrod, ed., Why People Pay Taxes. Tax Compliance and Enforcement. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

Spicer, Michael W. (1974) "A Behavioral Model of Income Tax Evasion." Ph.D.Dissertation, Division of Public Administration, Ohio State University, Columbus.

Spicer, M. W., and L. A. Becker (1980) "Fiscal Inequity and Tax Evasion: An Experimental Approach," 33 National Tax Journal 171.

Spicer, M. W., and S. B. Lundstedt (1976) "Understanding Tax Evasion," 31 Public Finance 295.

Spicer, M. W. and Hero. R., (1985). ‘‘Tax Evasion and Heuristics. A Research Note.’’ Journal of Public Economics 26:263–67.

Taylor, S., & Todd, P. (1995). Understanding the information technology usage: A test of competing models. Information Systems Research, 6(2), 144–176.

Thurman, Q. C., C. John, and L. Riggs (1984) "Neutralization and Tax Evasion: How Effective Would a Moral Appeal Be in Improving Compliance to Tax Laws?" 6 Law and Policy 309

Tittle, C. (1980). Sanctions and Social Deviance: The Question of Deterrence. New York, NY: Praeger

Torgler, B., and Schneider, F., (2007) What Shapes Attitudes Toward Paying Taxes? Evidence from Multicultural European Countries. Social science quarterly, volume 88, number 2, June

Trafimow, D., & Finlay, K. A. (1996). The importance of subjective norms for a minority of people: Between-subjects and within-subjects analyses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 820-828.

Trivedi, V. U. Mohamed Shehata, M. and Mestelman, S. (2005) Attitudes, incentives, and tax compliance.

Van Vught, M. Snyder, T. R. Tyler, and A. Biel, EdS., 2000. ‘‘Why Do People Cooperate in Groups?’’ Pp. 65–82 Cooperation in Modern Society. Promoting the Welfare of Communities, States and Organizations. London: Rout ledge.

Vogel, J (1974) "Taxation and Public Opinion in Sweden: An Interpretation of Recent Survey Data," 27 National Tax Journal 499.

64

Page 65: Final Copy TRA

Wallace, S. L. (2002) Osteoporosis prevention in college women: An application of the expanded health belief model. American journal of health behavior, 26, 163-172

Walter, T., Balunywa, W., Rosa, P., Sserwanga, A., Barabas, S. & Namatovu, R. (2004). Global Entrepreneurship Monitor, Executive Report 2003. Makerere University Business School.

Warshaw, P.R., & Droge, C. (1986). Economic utility versus the attitudinal perspective of consumer choice. Journal of Economic Psychology, 7, 37–60.Wallschutzky, I. G. (1984). Possible causes of tax evasion. Journal of Economic Psychology, 5, 371–384.

Webley, P., Robben, H., & Morris, I. (1988). Social comparison, attitudes and tax evasion in a shop simulation. Social Behaviour, 3, 219–228.

Wenzel, M. (2002). The impact of outcome orientation and justice concerns on tax compliance: The role of taxpayers_ identity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 629–645.

Wenzel, M. (2004). An analysis of norm processes in tax compliance. Journal of Economic Psychology, 25, 213–228.

Wenzel, M., (2005) Misperceptions of social norms about tax compliance: From theory to intervention; Journal of Economic Psychology 26 (2005) 862–883

Williams, R. M., (1968). ‘‘The Concept of Norms.’’ In The International Encyclopaedia of Social Sciences, Vol. 11. New York: Free Press.

Yankelovich, Skelly, and White, Inc. (1984). Taxpayer Attitudes Study: Final Report. Prepared for the Public Affairs Division of Internal Revenue Service. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing officeAppendix I:

Table 3

Descriptive statistics for theory of reasoned action variables

Mean Standard Variable deviation Attitudes 3.98 0.9241.Recognition of my good ethics by the community as a consequence of my Complying with the tax laws is:

2.30 1.844

2.Fulfillment of my personal, moral, and ethical obligations as a consequence of my Complying with the tax laws is:

2.20 1.660

3.Fulfillment of my duty as a citizen as a consequence of my Complying with the tax laws is:

1.95 1.547

4.Feeling guilty as a consequence of Not Complying with the tax laws is:

5.32 1.909

65

Page 66: Final Copy TRA

5.The ability to brag to others of non-compliance consequent to Not Complying with tax laws is:

5.32 2.258

6.Not complying with the tax laws consequent to having knowledge of the income tax system is:

4.77 2.211

7.Feeling of having beaten the “system” as a consequence of Not Complying with tax laws is:

5.22 2.093

8.Having more cash available as a consequence of my Not Complying with the is:

5.12 2.084

9.Paying penalties and fines to URA as a consequence of my Not Complying with the tax laws is:

5.57 1.919

10.Providing money to the government for necessary services as a consequence of my Complying with tax laws is:

Intention:

1.98 1.657

1.Complying with the tax laws will result into the recognition of my good ethics by the community:

5.81 1.713

2.Complying with the tax laws will result in the fulfillment of my duty as a citizen:

6.22 1.332

3.Not Complying with the tax laws will enable me to brag about such noncompliance to others:

6.49 .901

4.My Complying with the tax laws is consequent to the presence of accounting records of the business (e.g. computer records or receipts):

3.55 1.752

5.Complying with the tax laws will result in providing money to the government for necessary services:

6.05 1.606

6.Not Complying with the tax laws will result in the payment of penalties and fines to URA:

4.81 2.235

7.Complying with the tax laws will result in the fulfillment of my personal, moral, and ethical obligations:

5.95 1.417

8.My Not Complying with the tax laws is consequent to my knowledge of the income tax system:

5.50 1.378

9.I intend to correctly report my income and deductions on my current year's tax return even if an opportunity to underreport income or not pay on time and in the in the right place arises: Subjective and descriptive norms:

3.85 2.379

1.If I complied with the tax laws, my family (other than my significant other/spouse) would:

4.82 2.016

2.If I complied with the tax laws, my friends would: 4.44 1.9183.If I complied with the tax laws, my significant other would:

4.50 1.889

4.If I complied with the tax laws, my Tax-return Preparer would:

5.02 1.361

5.If I complied with the tax laws, my Business Contacts/Peers would:

5.01 1.745

66

Page 67: Final Copy TRA

6.My family (other than my significant other) comply with the tax laws

2.36 1.718

7.My friends comply with the tax laws 2.84 1.5898.My significant other (spouse) complies with the tax laws

3.01 1.938

9.My tax-preparer complies with the tax laws 3.03 1.46410.My business contacts/ Peers comply with the tax laws

2.44 1.492

Tax compliance behavior:1.Sometimes we attempt to avoid taxes

2.62 1.479

2.URA has threatened us about outstanding taxes to clear

2.09 1.304

3.We have outstanding taxes with URA 2.07 1.1994.URA made us pay a penalty when we delayed to pay tax

1.74 1.103

5.We have under declared income for tax 3.01 1.4096.We delay paying taxes 2.99 1.3527.The money retained through not paying taxes fully helps in business expansion

3.75 1.619

67

Page 68: Final Copy TRA

Appendix II:

Q-Q plots and histograms showing normal distribution

68

Page 69: Final Copy TRA

69

Page 70: Final Copy TRA

Appendix III:

Questionnaire

Interviewer’s NAME:

......................................

......................DATE:

................................

..................

BACKGROUND INFORMATION

1. Age Age --------------------2. Status Marital status

1 ( ) single (not yet married)2 ( ) married3 ( ) living together4 ( ) divorced or separated 5 ( ) widower/widow

3. Gender Gender 1 ( ) Female 2 ( ) Male

Please tick those that apply to you.4. Educ What is your highest level of education?1 ( ) Did not attend School2 ( ) Primary Level 3 ( ) O’ Level4 ( ) A’ Level5 ( ) Vocational Certificate6 ( ) Diploma 7 ( ) Bachelor’s degree8 ( ) Master’s degree 9 ( ) Others, _________________

70

Data Entry ID nr:

Page 71: Final Copy TRA

5. Inc What is your gross turnover income? a) Under 500,000 to 5,000,000 UGXb) 5,000,001 to 25,000,000 UGXc) 25,000,001 to 50,000,000 UGX (Please circle one)

6. Taxr For how many years have you filed tax returns? ________________

7. Ptaxr Who prepares your tax returns? a) Myself/Ourselvesb) Tax Preparer? (Circle)

ATTITUDE

Please evaluate the following possible consequences of

complying or not complying with the presumptive tax laws.

1

Extremely good

2 3 4 5 6

7

Extre

mely

bad

att1 Recognition of my good ethics by

the community as a consequence of

my Complying with the tax laws is:

att2 Fulfilment of my personal, moral,

and ethical obligations as a

consequence of my Complying with

the tax laws is:

att3 Saving on the effort required to

prepare my tax return correctly by

Not Complying with the tax laws

is:

att4 Fulfilment of my duty as a citizen as

a consequence of my Complying

with the tax laws is:

att5 Feeling guilty as a consequence of

71

Page 72: Final Copy TRA

Not Complying with the tax laws

is:

att6 The ability to brag to others of non-

compliance consequent to Not

Complying with tax laws is:

att7 Not complying with the tax laws

consequent to having knowledge of

the income tax system is:

att8 Feeling of having beaten the

“system” as a consequence of Not

Complying with tax laws is:

att9 Having more cash available as a

consequence of my Not Complying

with the is:

att1

0

Paying penalties and fines to URA

as a consequence of my Not

Complying with the tax laws is:

att1

1

Making my friends feel awkward as

a consequence of their discovery of

my Noncompliance with the tax

law is:

att1

2

Providing money to the government

for necessary services as a

consequence of my Complying with

tax laws is:

att1

3

Avoiding an audit by Revenue

Canada as a consequence of my

Complying with tax laws is:

INTENTION

72

Page 73: Final Copy TRA

Please indicate below your belief of how likely or unlikely

each one of the following outcomes is of occurring

consequent to your complying or not complying with tax

laws.

1Extremely unlikely

2 3 4 5 6 7

extre

mely

likely

int

1

Complying with the tax laws will

result into the recognition of my

good ethics by the community:

int

2

Complying with the tax laws will

result in the fulfilment of my duty as

a citizen:

int

3

Not Complying with the tax laws

will enable me to brag about such

noncompliance to others:

int

4

My Complying with the tax laws is

consequent to the presence of

accounting records of the business

(e.g. computer records or receipts):

int

5

Not Complying with the tax laws

will result in my feeling guilty:

int

6

Not Complying with the tax laws

will result in making my peers feel

awkward when they discover that

fact:

int

7

Complying with the tax laws will

result in providing money to the

government for necessary services:

int

8

Not Complying with the tax laws

will result in the payment of

73

Page 74: Final Copy TRA

penalties and fines to URA:

int

9

Complying with the tax laws will

result in the fulfilment of my

personal, moral, and ethical

obligations:

Int

10

Complying with the tax laws will

result in the payment of a large

percentage of extra earnings for

taxes:

Int

11

My Not Complying with the tax

laws is consequent to my knowledge

of the income tax system:

int

12

Not Complying with the tax laws

will result in more cash available for

use by me:

INTENT

Please respond to the following question on a seven-point

scale with end-points labelled unlikely and likely provided

after it:

1

Strongly disagree

2 3 4 5 6 7

Stro

ngly

agre

e

intt

1

I intend to correctly report my

income and deductions on my

current year's tax return even if an

opportunity to underreport income

or not pay on time and in the in the

right place arises:

74

Page 75: Final Copy TRA

Subjective norms

Please indicate below your belief of whether the following

groups of people related to you would approve or disapprove

of your complying with the tax laws:

1Disapprove

2 3 4 5 6 7

app

rov

e

Sub

1

If I complied with the tax laws, my

family (other than my significant

other/spouse) would:

Sub

2

If I complied with the tax laws, my

friends would:

Sub

3

If I complied with the tax laws, my

significant other would:

Sub

4

If I complied with the tax laws, my

Tax-return Preparer would:

Sub

5

If I complied with the tax laws, my

Business Contacts/Peers would:

DESCRIPTIVE NORMS

Please indicate below if the following groups of people

related to you comply with their own tax obligations.

1Completely true

2 3 4 5 6 7

com

plet

ely

false

75

Page 76: Final Copy TRA

des

1

My family (other than my significant

other) comply with the tax laws

des

2

My friends comply with the tax laws

des

3

My significant other (spouse)

complies with the tax laws

des

4

My tax-preparer complies with the

tax laws

des

5

My business contacts/ Peers comply

with the tax laws

MOTIVATION

Please indicate below the extent to which you care about the

following groups of people, related to you, approving or

disapproving of your complying with the tax laws:

1Not at all

2 3 4 5 6 7

very

muc

h

mot

v1

How much do you care whether

your family (other than your

significant other) approve or

Disapprove of your correct

reporting of your income and paying

on time and in the right place?

mot

v2

How much do you care whether

your friends approve or disapprove

of your correct reporting of your

income and paying on time and in

the right place?

76

Page 77: Final Copy TRA

mot

v3

How much do you care whether

your significant other approves or

disapproves of your correct

reporting of your income and paying

on time and in the right place?

mot

v4

How much do you care whether

your Tax return Preparer approves

or disapproves of your correct

reporting of your income and paying

on time and in the right place?

mot

v5

How much do you care whether

your Business Contacts/Peers

approve or disapprove of your

correct reporting of your income

and paying on time and in the right

place?

77