does increasing schooling improve later health habits

Upload: universityofmelbourne

Post on 04-Jun-2018

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    1/29

    Melbourne Institute Working Paper Series

    Working Paper No. 1/14

    Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits?

    Evidence from the School Reforms in Australia

    Jinhu Li and Nattavudh Powdthavee

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    2/29

    Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits?

    Evidence from the School Reforms in Australia*

    Jinhu Liand Nattavudh PowdthaveeMelbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research,

    The University of Melbourne

    Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research,

    The University of Melbourne; and Centre for Economic Performance,

    London School of Economics and Political Science

    Melbourne Institute Working Paper No. 1/14

    ISSN 1328-4991 (Print)

    ISSN 1447-5863 (Online)

    ISBN 978-0-7340-4340-5

    January 2014

    * This paper uses data from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in

    Australia (HILDA) Survey. The HILDA Project was initiated and is funded by the

    Australian Government Department of Social Services (DSS) and is managed by the

    Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research (Melbourne Institute).

    The findings and views in this paper are those of the authors and should not be

    attributed to either DSS or the Melbourne Institute.

    Melbourne Institute of Applied Economic and Social Research

    The University of Melbourne

    Victoria 3010 Australia

    Telephone(03) 8344 2100

    Fax (03) 8344 2111

    [email protected]

    WWW Address http://www.melbourneinstitute.com

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    3/29

    2

    Abstract

    This study estimates the causal effect of schooling on health behaviors. Using changes

    in the minimum school leaving age laws that varied by birth year and states in

    Australia from age 14 to 15 as a source of exogenous variation in schooling, the

    instrumental variables (IV) regression estimates imply that there is a positive,

    sizeable, and statistically significant effect of staying an extra year in school on later

    healthy lifestyle choices, including diet, exercise, and the decision to engage in risky

    health behaviors. We also demonstrate that the magnitudes of the schooling effect

    were effectively moderated by a selection of pre-determined characteristics of the

    individuals. Finally, this paper provides some evidence on the potential underlying

    mechanisms linking education and health behaviors by showing that increasingschooling also raised individuals conscientiousness levels and the perceived sense of

    control over ones life.

    JEL classification:I12, I21, C26

    Keywords: Health behaviors, schooling, instrumental variables, education, HILDA,

    Australia

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    4/29

    3

    1. IntroductionA vast literature in health sciences shows how differences in unhealthy behaviors

    account for a large variation in peoples health outcomes. For example, a study by

    McGinness and Foege (1993) has estimated that approximately half of the deaths in

    the United States occurred in 1990 are caused by unhealthy behaviors, such as

    smoking, poor diet and not enough exercise, excessive drinking, and illicit use of

    drugs. A more recent study from Mokdad et al. (2004) shows that while smoking

    remains the leading cause of mortality in 2000, poor diet and physical inactivity may

    soon overtake tobacco as the leading cause of death. Unhealthy behaviors have also

    often been cited as the main predictors of premature and preventable morbidity such

    as cancer and heart disease (Doll & Hill, 1956; Wilson, 1994; Boffetta et al., 2006).

    This raises an important, policy-related question: Why do some people invest more in

    a healthy lifestyle than others?

    Economists have provided a theoretical framework in which education plays

    an important role in the health production process. Based on the demand for health

    model (Grossman, 1972; Grossman, 2000), more schooling may lead to more efficient

    use of a given set of health inputs by improving decision-making abilities (productive

    efficiency), as well as improve the allocative efficiency among different health

    inputs by increasing ones ability to acquire and process health information. Ceteris

    paribus,this higher productive efficiency generated by education raises future returns

    (in terms of both future health and lifetime earnings) to health investments, therefore

    better-educated individuals are more likely than others to choose healthier lifestyles.

    Empirical evidence estimating the association between education and health

    has shown that health-related behavior is one of the key mediators of the education

    effect on health outcomes. Leigh (1983) estimated that much of the positive effect of

    schooling on health can be explained by exercise, smoking, and occupational choices.

    Cutler and Lleras-Muney (2010) also showed that education is associated with a

    significant variety of positive health behaviors, and that every year of education

    lowers the mortality risk by 0.3 percentage points, or 24 percent, through reduction in

    risky behaviors such as drinking, smoking and excessive weight. In a more recent

    study by Brunello et al. (2011), the mediating effect of health behaviors measured

    by smoking, drinking, and body mass index has been estimated to account for

    around 23% to 45% of the entire effect of education on self-reported health status,

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    5/29

    4

    depending on gender.

    Given the important role of health behaviors in explaining the education-

    health gradient, we aim to contribute to the literature by investigating the causal link

    between education and health behaviors. While cross-sectional evidence suggests that

    people of higher educational background do indeed tend to smoke less, consume less

    alcohol, exercise more, eat healthier food, and have more frequent health checks than

    the average population (Kenkel, 1991; Droomers et al., 1999; Cutler & Glaeser, 2005;

    Cutler & Lleras-Muney, 2010), it is not sufficient to believe that education causes

    people to adopt healthier lifestyles. Estimates using OLS may be biased in both

    directions. For example, an important bias is the measurement error bias associated

    with the way education is typically measured in surveys, which can bias the estimated

    schooling effect toward zero (Blackburn and Neumark, 1995). More importantly,

    causality may run in reverse from health behaviors to more schooling, i.e. students

    with healthier lifestyles may also be more efficient producers of additional human

    capital, thus implying that estimates of the schooling effect on health behaviors will

    be biased upward. There may also be important omitted variables from the estimation

    model, such as rates of time preferences (Fuch, 1982) and heritable endowments

    (Behrman & Rosenzweig, 2002), which can simultaneously influence both schooling

    and health-related behaviors. One could imagine, for example, that people who are

    more future oriented (i.e. those who desire more leisure at older ages) will stay in

    school for longer, work more at younger ages, as well as invest more in positive

    health-related behaviors during most stages of the life cycle. Thus, the effect of

    schooling will be biased upward if one fails to control for time-preference. Because

    there are both positive and negative biases involved, the direction of the overall bias is

    unclear on a priori ground.

    Recent studies that attempted to estimate the causal effect of schooling on

    health behaviors have mainly dealt with the endogeneity issue by relying on natural

    experiments such as changes in the compulsory schooling laws or an idiosyncrasy of

    geography or birthdate to generate an exogenous variation in peoples schooling level

    (Eide & Showalter, 2011). The empirical evidence is, however, mixed. Instrumental

    variables (IV) estimates range from schooling having a positive and statistically

    significant effect (Arendt, 2005; Park & Kang, 2008; Jrges et al., 2011) to its having

    an ambiguous or a nonsignificant effect on a variety of health behaviors, including

    smoking, drinking, and dieting (Kenkel et al., 2006; Braakmann, 2011; Kemptner et

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    6/29

    5

    al., 2011; Clark & Royer, 2013).

    What explains why different studies find differing results when they all seem

    to have credible identification strategies? One reason for this is that context matters in

    determining whether schooling and health behaviors are causally related. For

    example, some researchers have found the effect of schooling on health to vary

    significantly across gender (e.g. Powdthavee, 2010; Jrges et al, 2013), with men

    typically benefitting more from staying an extra year in school than women. Such

    differences in the schooling effect by gender partly explain why studies that include

    both men and women in the sample have found ambiguous or nonsignificant

    schooling effects.

    Results can also differ along other dimensions, including individuals country

    of residence and family background measured at the time when the minimum school

    leaving age law first applied to the child. For example, the size of the schooling effect

    on later health behaviors may depend significantly on parental circumstances and

    home environment when the child was 14. Because of a lower level of initial

    endowment in human capital, one could imagine that the marginal utility from an

    extra year of schooling would be larger for children from a less fortunate background

    than their rich counterpart. Currently, the empirical evidence on the causal effect of

    schooling on health behaviors outside the US and Europe is small and the extent of

    heterogeneity in the schooling effect on health behaviors across different

    characteristics of the individual continues to be imperfectly understood.

    The aim of the current study is to provide new evidence on the causal effect of

    schooling on a range of health behaviors for Australian adults. We adopt the same

    identification strategy as in Leigh and Ryan (2008) and rely on exogenous changes in

    the amount of schooling received by individuals generated by changes in the

    minimum school leaving age laws across different years and Australian States. Our

    paper thus adds to the existing literature on the causal links between schooling and

    non-market outcomes such as health and health behaviors by studying a different

    institutional environment that has not been previously examined in the literature;

    examples include studies that use changes in the compulsory schooling laws in the

    United States (Adams, 2002; Lleras-Muney, 2005), United Kingdom (Oreopoulos,

    2007; Siles, 2009; Powdthavee, 2010; Clark & Royer, 2013; Jrges et al., 2013),

    Germany (Jrges et al., 2011), Denmark (Arendt, 2005), Sweden (Spasojevic, 2003),

    France (Albouy & Lequien, 2009), and South Korea (Park & Kang, 2008).

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    7/29

    6

    The current study also advances the existing literature by analyzing whether

    the schooling effect on health behaviors is significantly moderated by different

    individual characteristics, such as gender and family background at the time of the

    introduction of the minimum schooling requirements. While previous studies have

    attempted to establish the causal links between schooling and health behaviors by

    gender (e.g., Braakmann, 2011; Jrges et al., 2011), virtually no attempts have been

    made to study the extent of heterogeneity in the schooling effect by early home

    environment of the child.

    Finally, we provide some evidence on the potential underlying mechanisms

    linking education and health behaviors. Cutler and Lleras-Muney (2010) argue that

    one possible reason for the education- health behavior gradient is that individual with

    different education levels may differ in their psychological capacity to make behavior

    changes. This also relates to psychological theories that individual need to be ready

    to change and feel able to do so. In light of this we specifically test the hypotheses

    that increasing schooling raises individuals conscientiousness levels and the

    perceived sense of control over ones life, which in turn contribute to the adoption of

    healthier lifestyles.

    2. Data and empirical strategy2.1.DataThe data comes from the Household, Income and Labour Dynamics in Australia

    (HILDA) Survey, a longitudinal survey that has been tracking members of a

    nationally representative sample of Australian households since 2001. A total of 7,682

    households participated in wave 1, providing an initial sample of 19,914 persons

    (Wooden et al., 2002). The members of these participating households form the basis

    of the panel pursued in subsequent annual survey waves. Interviews are conducted

    with all adults (defined as persons aged 15 years or older) who are members of the

    original sample, as well as any other adults who, in later waves, are residing with an

    original sample member. Annual re-interview rates (the proportion of respondents

    from one wave who are successfully interviewed the next) are reasonably high, rising

    from 87% in wave 2 to over 96% by wave 9 (Watson & Wooden, 2012).

    Alongside socio-economic questions typically asked in standard household

    surveys, the HILDA Survey regularly collects data on a number of health behavior

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    8/29

    7

    variables. We follow Cobb-Clark et al. (2013) and use adaptations of the Healthy

    Eating Index and Alameda-7, which appeared in Waves 7 (2007) and 9 (2009), as the

    key dependent variables in our study.

    Originally developed by the US Department of Agriculture to gauge peoples

    overall diet quality, the Healthy Eating Index (HEI) consists of five components taken

    from the USDA Food Guide Pyramids (number of servings for grains, vegetables,

    fruit, milk and meat) and five components from the 1995 Dietary Guidelines for

    Americans (total fat in the diet, percentage of calories from saturated fat, cholesterol

    intake, sodium intake, and variety of diet). The HILDA data set allows an

    approximation of HEI to be constructed based on the following indicators: (1) eating

    fruits seven days a week; (2) eating vegetables seven days a week; (3) avoiding (i.e.

    eating less than once per month) fatty, high cholesterol foods such as potatoes, French

    fries, hot chips or wedges; and (4) avoiding milk fat by drinking skim or low fat milk.

    These four variables are then added together to form an index that ranges from 0

    unhealthiest diet to 4 healthiest diet.

    The Alameda-7 (ALMDA) index is originally designed to capture healthy

    habits (e.g. Schoenborn, 1986), which is more of an all-round measure of healthy

    lifestyle choices compared to the healthy eating index. It comprises of seven healthy

    habit variables: (1) having never smoked; (2) drinking less than five drinks at one

    sitting; (3) sleeping 7-8 hours a night; (4) exercising; (5) maintaining desirable weight

    for height; (6) avoiding snacks; and (7) eating breakfast regularly. However, the

    HILDA data set only allows us to construct an amended version of ALMDA based on

    the following behaviors: (1) eating breakfast seven times a week; (2) drinking

    moderately (avoid binge-drinking), i.e. less than 7 for men and 5 for women on any

    given sitting; (3) exercising at least three times per week at moderate or intensive

    physical exertion; and (4) not smoking. Like HEI, the ALMDA variable also ranges

    from 0 worst combination of health habits to 4 best combination of health habits.

    Following Leigh and Ryan (2008), estimates of years of education are derived

    from respondents highest educational attainment. Thus a respondent reporting having

    completed secondary school (Year 12) is assumed to have completed 12 years of

    education, a person completing an ordinary university degree is assumed to have

    completed 15 years of education, and so on. As is conventional, we are not measuring

    actual years spent in education (which would vary with the time with which

    qualifications are completed, the number of qualifications obtained, and time spent

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    9/29

    8

    studying that did not lead to a qualification) but instead the time typically taken to

    obtain the highest qualification reported.

    The main sample used in the analysis of the schooling effect on health

    behaviors consists of all adults aged 22 to 65 who participated in Waves 7 and 9 and

    who responded to the health behaviors questionnaires, as well as completed secondary

    school in Australia. A potential drawback of the current HILDA Survey dataset is that

    we do not have information about the state in which the respondent attended school,

    and so we proxy this by the current state of residence. See Appendix A for a table of

    descriptive statistics for each of the variables used in our analysis.

    2.2.Raising of the school leaving age (ROSLA) in AustraliaPrevious research has used a wide range of variables to instrument for schooling.

    Included here are distance from home to college (Card, 1995), the introduction of

    restrictive compulsory schooling laws (Harmon & Walker, 1995), and regional

    spending on education in regions where the individual was a student (Berger & Leigh,

    1989), all of which are variables that have the propensity to affect directly

    individuals decision to stay in school but are otherwise uncorrelated with their future

    behaviours outside school. In this paper, we exploit the changes in the school leavinglaws in Australia (which were introduced in different states at different times) to

    obtain the exogenous variation of schooling across birth cohorts and states.

    We adopt the identification strategy outlined in Leigh and Ryan (2008) by

    using changes in the minimum school leaving age laws as a source of exogenous

    variations in peoples education levels. We also adjust our coding of the instrumental

    variables to account for the way in which the leaving age rule binds across different

    states. Hence a state where students can leave school on their 15 thbirthday is coded as

    15, while a state where students can leave school at the end of the year in which they

    turn 15 is coded as 15.5 (for a summary of the compulsory school laws by birth year

    and Australian state, see Appendix B). Here the exclusion restriction assumption is

    that these education reforms would have had a direct impact on the education level of

    those who would otherwise have left school at the old minimum school leaving age,

    but they should not have a direct effect on health-related behaviors measured at the

    later stages of the life cycle beyond their impact on education. This is because the

    changes in the laws have made some youths irrespective of their abilities and socio-

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    10/29

    9

    economic backgrounds stay in school who would have left earlier in absence of the

    more restrictive laws (Harmon & Walker, 1995; Card, 2001; Oreopoulos & Salvanes,

    2011).

    Finally, given that the minimum school leaving age laws were implemented in

    different states at different times, we follow Leigh and Ryan (2008) and allow for the

    possibility that our IV operates differently for people from different states and birth

    cohorts. For example, given the evidence of a continuing reduction in the fraction of

    students dropping out of school at the earliest opportunity over time, one could

    imagine that the minimum school leaving age law introduced in the 1940s will have a

    smaller effect on peoples education compared to the one that was implemented later

    in the 1960s. We do this by interacting the compulsory schooling law variable with

    the individuals birth year and use these interacted dummy variables as instruments

    for education. As in Leigh and Ryans paper, this approach leads to a significant

    improvement in the estimates precision in the first-stage regression; i.e., the F-test on

    the excluded instruments produces larger F-statistics using this approach.

    2.3.Empirical strategyThe current study focuses on one particular prediction made on the demand for healthmodel (Grossman, 1972): education has a positive effect one that is both causal and

    sizeable on peoples behaviors towards health and health behaviors. To formally test

    this hypothesis, we estimate the following set of regression equations on the pooled

    sample:

    (1)

    (2)

    where denotes years of schooling for individual i; is a standardized measure of

    health behavior (HEI and ALAMEDA-7) with mean zero and a standard deviation of

    one; denotes a vector of indicator variables representing different compulsory

    schooling laws; represents individual is birth year; is a vector of control

    variables, including gender, age, age-squared, state of residence, and birth year

    dummies; andare the error terms. is the predicted level of schooling obtained

    from (1). Assuming that the compulsory schooling laws affect directly but are

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    11/29

    10

    otherwise unrelated to - i.e. , 0, the effect of schooling on health

    behavior, , can be estimated consistently and free from bias.

    One relating issue concerning the estimation of (1) and (2) is that the

    interaction terms have to be reasonably strong predictors of in order to

    avoid the weak instrument problem (Bound et al., 1995; Staiger and Stock 1997).

    The rule of thumb for a strong set of IVs is that the F-statistic obtained from

    conducting an F-test on the excluded instruments in (1) has a value that equals or is

    greater than 10. In addition to this, the IVs must also satisfy the exclusion restriction

    assumption in that they are completely exogenous to the outcome variable in (2). This

    can be tested using the Sargan (or Hansen) test of over-identifying restrictions, which

    is based on the assumption that the residuals in (2) must be uncorrelated with the set

    of exogenous variables for the IVs to be truly exogenous. A rejection of the null

    hypothesis here would imply that the IVs are correlated with the error term in (2),

    which would make the IVs invalid to use.

    We estimate equations (1) and (2) using full adult sample, as well as sub-

    samples by gender, by fathers occupational status when the child was 14 (variable:

    fmfo61), and by fathers highest educational level when the child was 14 (fmfhlq,

    fmfpsq, andfmfsch). All of our regression equations are estimated using either pooled

    Ordinary Least Squares (OLS) or Instrumental Variables (IV) estimator, both with

    robust standard errors clustering at the individual level, i.e. by personal identifier.

    3. Results3.1. Schooling and health behaviors in adulthood

    Is there a causal link between schooling and healthy lifestyle choices among

    Australian adults? To make a first pass at this, Table 1 estimates simple linear health

    behaviors regression equations (OLS and IV) on the full sample and gender sub-

    samples in which years of education is the independent variable of interest.

    We can see from Panels A and B of Table 1 (the first three columns), in which

    schooling is assumed to be exogenous in both HEI and ALMDA equations, that years

    of schooling correlate positively and statistically significantly with individuals

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    12/29

    11

    healthy lifestyle choices. The size of the estimated coefficient on schooling remains

    relatively consistent across different sub-samples used in the estimation; staying an

    extra year in school is associated with approximately 0.1 standard deviation increase

    in the healthy eating index and in the healthy habit index for both genders. In addition

    to this, women tend to have a healthier lifestyle than men; the estimated coefficients

    on Female are 0.460 [S.E.=0.020] in the HEI equation and 0.285 [S.E.=0.023] in

    the ALMDA equation. There is little evidence that age matters to health behaviors,

    although this is partly due to the inclusion of birth year dummies as control variables.

    We begin instrumenting for schooling with as IVs in the second set

    of panels of Table 1 (the last three columns). While many of the individual

    interactions between Compulsory School Law and Birth Year are not statistically

    significant, a test of the joint significance of the excluded variables produces

    statistically significant F-statistics with magnitudes that are greater than 10 in all

    specifications, which is a rule of thumb for a strong set of IVs (Bound et al. 1995;

    Staiger and Stock 1997). It can also be seen from the table that our set of IVs

    produces Hansen J-statistics that are not large enough to reject the null hypothesis that

    the error term is uncorrelated with the instruments, thus providing us with further

    statistical validation that the minimum school leaving age laws are truly exogenous in

    these health behavior equations.

    Instrumenting for years of education leads to an increase in the size of the

    estimated schooling effect on both measures of health behaviors across most of the

    samples used in the analysis, thus suggesting that the overall bias on the relationship

    between education and health behaviors may have been negative rather than positive.

    Using the full sample, our IV estimates suggest that an extra year of education for

    individuals who would have left at the earliest opportunity has raised their later HEI

    and ALMDA by approximately 0.17 and 0.18 standard deviations, respectively.

    Given the distributions of both health behavior indices, these are large effects; for

    example, the estimated effect of schooling on ALMDA is larger than half of the effect

    switching gender has on the same outcome.

    We can also see that the positive effect of staying an extra year in school on

    both measures of health behaviors is larger and statistically more robust for women

    than for men. More specifically, a one-year increase in education respectively raises

    individuals HEI and ALMDA by approximately 0.24 and 0.15 standard deviations

    for women, compared to 0.15 and 0.04 standard deviations for men. Note, however,

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    13/29

    12

    that these results are inconsistent with previous studies that have found the health

    benefits from staying an extra year in school to be larger for men than for women

    (e.g. Powdthavee, 2010; Jrges et al., 2011).

    Does the effect of schooling on health behaviors also differ across different

    family backgrounds recorded at the time when the laws first applied to them, i.e.

    when the respondents were 14 years old? One hypothesis is that children from a lower

    socio-economic background may have benefitted more on average from staying an

    extra year in school as a result from changes in the minimum school leaving age law.

    To test this, Tables 2 and 3 separate the data by fathers occupational classes and

    highest education levels when the child was 14 years old.

    Take Table 2s estimates first, in which the data has been broken down into (i)

    managers/professionals/technicians and trade workers/community and personal

    service workers/clerical and administrative workers/sales workers (High-skilled

    fathers); and (ii) machinery operators and drivers/laborers (Low-skilled fathers).

    Looking at the OLS estimates, we can see that, across different paternal occupational

    categories (high- versus low-skilled fathers), staying an extra year in school is

    associated with an increase in both HEI and ALMDA of around 0.1-standard

    deviation. However, in both sets of health behavior equations, there is a noticeable

    gradient in the size of the estimated IV coefficients on years of schooling by fathers

    occupational categories. For example, a one-year increase in education raises later

    HEI by approximately 0.21-standard deviation for individuals whose father was

    classified as a low-skilled worker when the respondent was about 14 years old,

    compared to around 0.05-standard deviation for those whose father was classified as a

    high-skilled worker when the respondent was about 14 years old.

    A similar pattern is also obtained in Table 3 when we separate the data by

    fathers education levels into (i) less than 12 years of formal education; and (ii) 12

    years or more of formal education. As in the previous table, staying an extra year in

    school is associated with an increase in both HEI and ALMDA of around 0.1-standard

    deviation. However, the IV estimates suggest that the marginal effect of schooling is

    roughly twiceas large for individuals whose father had less than 12 years of formal

    education, compared to those whose father completed 12 years or more of formal

    education when the respondent was about 14 years old. However, care must be taken

    in interpreting some of these coefficients, given that some of the F-tests on the

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    14/29

    13

    excluded instruments though statistically significant have a value that is less than

    the recommended rule of thumb, i.e. 10.

    These results appear to offer statistical credence to the hypothesis that

    increasing schooling improves later health habits for the individuals, on average. In

    contrast to previous studies, women in Australia tend to benefit more than men from

    being forced to stay an extra year in school. We also provide new evidence that the

    protective effect of education on health behaviors is larger for individuals from a

    relatively poorer background recorded in childhood. Thus, what our results imply is

    that there may be considerable heterogeneity in the causal effect of schooling on

    healthy habits by individuals fixed and/or early childhood characteristics which, if

    left unaccounted for, may confound the estimated effect of education on healthy

    lifestyle choices.

    3.2.Conscientiousness and locus of control

    Up to this point, this paper has concentrated on the estimation of the causal effect of

    schooling on measures of health behaviors. Such an approach seems to be of some

    worth in its own right. However, the next natural question is why does more

    schooling matter to health behaviors in the first place? One hypothesis is that more

    schooling improves the allocative efficiency of health inputs, thereby allowing more

    educated individuals to invest more than the less educated in healthy behaviors in the

    presence of time constraints common to both groups. An alternative hypothesis one

    that is testable in our data set is that more schooling raises individuals

    psychological capacity including locus of control and conscientiousness levels, both

    of which have been considered by researchers in the field as important predictors of

    healthy behaviors over the life-cycle (Friendman et al., 1995; Bogg and Roberts,

    2004; Cobb-Clark et al., 2013).

    As an explorative exercise, Table 4 estimates for the full-sample, as well as

    the gender sub-samples, OLS and IV regression equations with the standardized

    internal locus of control (LOC) and conscientiousness (CONSC) as the new

    dependent variables. To construct the LOC variable, we added up the seven items

    taken from the Psychological Coping Resources component of the Mastery Module

    developed by Pearlin and Schooler (1978) recorded in Waves 3, 4, 7 and 11 of the

    HILDA (as LOC was not collected in Wave 9). Each respondent was asked to rank

    the extent of which they would agree or disagree with seven statements, such as I

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    15/29

    14

    have little control over the things that happen to me or there is little I can do to

    change many of the important things in my life. The respondents can choose from

    strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (7). Two of the seven items can be

    interpreted as internal control whereas the other five items can be interpreted as

    external control. We recoded the scales so that the higher values represent higher

    levels of internal control tendencies. We then used simple average value of the seven

    scales as our measure of the LOC index. The LOC index is then standardized to have

    a zero mean and a standard deviation of 1. Using the same construct, Cobb-Clark et

    al. (2013) have been able to show that people with higher internal sense of control

    tend to eat better and exercise more regularly, on average. All four waves (Waves 3,

    4, 7, and 11) are used in the estimation of the LOC equations.

    The CONSC variable was constructed from responses to the standard Big Five

    questionnaires in Waves 5 and 9. In a self-completed questionnaire, survey

    participants were presented with 36 descriptive words (e.g., talkative, jealous,

    sympathetic, intellectual, orderly) and asked, How well the following words describe

    you? (Goldberg, 1993). For each word, participants were asked how well the word

    described them on a 1 to 7 scale with 1 meaning does not describe me at all to 7

    meaning describes me very well. We used the responses to the questions related to

    the conscientious characteristics e.g. respondent tends to do a thorough job and

    respondent does thing efficiently to construct our CONSC variable. We coded the

    scales so that the higher values represent higher levels of conscientiousness. The

    CONSC index is then standardized to have a zero mean and a standard deviation of 1.

    Bogg and Roberts (2004) have shown conscientiousness-related traits to be negatively

    related to risky health behaviors (e.g., tobacco use and alcohol consumption) and

    positively related to positive health behaviors (e.g., diet and regular exercise). Both

    waves 5 and 9 are then used in the estimation of the CONSC equations.

    The OLS estimates in Table 4 suggest that individuals years of schooling

    correlate positively and statistically significantly with both LOC and CONSC; people

    who remained in school for longer tend to be conscientious individuals or individuals

    who have a higher perceived sense of control over their life. The same general finding

    also applies for both men and women.

    Instrumenting for years of education by generally leads to an

    increase in the size of the estimated schooling coefficients and their standard errors

    (see the last three columns of Table 4). With respect to LOC, the estimated IV

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    16/29

    15

    coefficient on years of schooling is positive and statistically significant at

    conventional levels across the full sample, male sub-sample, and female sub-sample

    (although it is statistically significant at the 1% level only for females). This indicates

    that LOC can be changed by the course of education thus is not-fixed over the life

    cycle. It is inconsistent with the conclusion from a previous study on the stability of

    LOC, which suggests changes in LOC are modest on average and are unlikely to be

    economically meaningful (Cobb-Clark and Schurer 2013). However, since their

    conclusion is based on analyzing the extent to which the change in individuals LOC

    is linked to various negative or positive life events, our results are not surprising given

    we provide evidence on the effect of education rather than labor market and health

    events. Similarly, staying an extra year in school caused by a change in the

    compulsory schooling law appears to have a significant effect at raising CONSC for

    women but not for men, thus implying that women may also benefit more than men in

    terms of personality outcomes from staying an extra year in school. The magnitude of

    this effect is relatively small though even for women. Nonetheless, care must be taken

    when interpreting some of the IV coefficients, given that some of the F-tests on the

    excluded instruments provide F-statistics that have a value that is less than 10.

    Hence, what Table 4s results seem to suggest is that there may be more than

    one causal route in which schooling can improve healthy lifestyle choices, two of

    which are the beneficial effects of schooling on individuals internal locus of control

    and conscientiousness levels. In addition to this, the extent to which schooling can

    influence the magnitudes of these routes may also vary significantly according to the

    individuals pre-determined characteristics (such as gender, for example).

    4. ConclusionsThe objective of this paper is to provide new empirical evidence on the causal link

    between education and health habits in Australia. Using changes in the compulsory

    schooling laws that varied by states and birth cohorts to provide an exogenous source

    of variation in peoples education, we were able to show that there is a positive,

    sizeable, and statistically significant effect of staying an extra year in school on later

    healthy lifestyle choices for people who would otherwise have left school at the old

    minimum school leaving age, including diet, exercise, and the decision to engage in

    risky health behaviors. This is different from the evidence generated by studies in the

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    17/29

    16

    US, UK and Germany (Kenkel et al., 2006; Clark & Royer, 2013; Braakmann, 2011;

    Kemptner et al., 2011), but is in line with the few studies in other countries such as

    Denmark and South Korea (Arendt, 2005; Park & Kang, 2008). This might be a

    reflection of the differences in the education and health care systems, or an interaction

    between these two systems, across different countries. Further cross-country

    comparisons are warranted to provide us with a better understanding on the

    environment in which education may exert a sizable effect on health-related behaviors

    and health outcomes.

    Not only the context of the residing country but also the context in terms of

    early-life family circumstances might be important reason for the observed

    heterogeneity in the causal effect of education on health behaviors. We were also able

    to demonstrate that the magnitudes of the schooling effect were effectively moderated

    by a selection of pre-determined characteristics of the individuals, e.g., the effect is

    larger for females than for males, and for people from a relatively poorer background

    when they were about 14 years old.

    Finally, this paper provided some statistical evidence on the potential

    underlying mechanisms linking education and health behaviors by showing that

    increasing schooling also raised individuals conscientiousness levels and the

    perceived sense of control over ones life. This adds to the literature in searching for

    the potential reasons why better-educated people invest more in healthier behaviors.

    Our results might explain why in Cutler and Lleras-Muney (2010) the mediating

    effect of cognition function and knowledge can only explain some, but not all, of the

    education-behavior gradient.

    There are several positive and normative implications to our study. The first is

    that it provides yet another evidence in favor of Michael Grossmans (1975) remarks

    that an increase in expenditure on education rather than on health itself is perhaps the

    most cost-effective way to improve the nations health. Second, while we have found

    a significant average effect of schooling on later health behaviors for people who

    were directly affected by changes in the compulsory schooling laws in Australia, we

    have also demonstrated that there is a considerable heterogeneity in the schooling

    effect across different groups of individuals. Future research especially qualitative

    research should come back to investigate how different pre-determined

    characteristics and early home environments can moderate the causal effect of

    schooling on health behaviors. For example, why do Australian women benefit more

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    18/29

    17

    than men in terms of health behaviors and health outcomes from an extra year of

    education when studies in other countries have found just the opposite? Or how do

    parental characteristics interact with a positive shock in childs education that leads to

    some benefiting more than others later? And finally, we have been able to

    demonstrate that an extra year of schooling brought about by an increase in the

    minimum school leaving age from 14 to 15 years old also caused a significant change

    in peoples especially womens psychological traits that are also known to govern

    health-related behaviors. This is consistent with the idea that personalities are

    changeable over time (Boyce et al., 2013), and that there may be scope for later policy

    interventions to try and improve the personality traits of adults that are related to

    healthy habits.

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    19/29

    18

    References

    Adams, S.J. (2002). Educational attainment and health: Evidence from a sample of older

    adults.Education Economics, 10(1), 97109.

    Albouy, V., and Lequien, L. (2009). Does compulsory education lower mortality?Journal of Health Economics, 28(1), 155168.

    Arendt, J.N. (2005). Does education cause better health? A panel data analysis using

    school reforms for identification.Economics of Education Review, 24(2), 149160.

    Barcan, A. (1980). A history of Australian Education: Oxford University Press,

    Melbourne.

    Behrman, J. R., and Rosenzweig, M. R. (2004). Returns to birthweight. Review of

    Economics and Statistics, 86(2), 586601.Berger M.C. and Leigh, J.P. (1989) Schooling, Self-Selection, and Health. Journal of

    Human Resources, 24(3), 433455.

    Blackburn, M.L., and Neumark, D. (1995). Are OLS estimates of the return to schooling

    biased downward? Another look. Review of Economics and Statistics, 77(2), 217

    229.

    Boffetta, P., Hashibe, M., La Vecchia, C., Zatonski, W., and Rehm, J. (2006). The

    burden of cancer attributable to alcohol drinking. International Journal of Cancer,119(4), 884887.

    Bogg, T., and Roberts, B.W. (2004). Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: a

    meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality. Psychological

    Bulletin, 130(6), 887919.

    Bound, J., Jaeger, D.A., and Baker, R. (1995). Problems with instrumental variables

    estimation when the correlation between the instruments and the endogenous

    explanatory variable is weak. Journal of the American Statistical Association,90(430), 443450.

    Boyce, C.J., Wood, A.M., and Powdthavee, N. (2013). Is personality fixed? Personality

    changes as much as variable economic factors and more strongly predicts changes

    to life satisfaction. Social Indicators Research, 111(1), 287305.

    Braakmann, N. (2011). The causal relationship between education, health and health-

    related behavior: evidence from a natural experiment in England. Journal of Health

    Economics, 30(4), 753763.Brunello, G., Fort, M., Schneeweis, N., and Winter-Ebmer, R. (2011). The causal effect

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    20/29

    19

    of education on health: what is the role of health behaviors? IZA Discussion Paper

    No. 5944, University of Bonn.

    Card, D. (1995). Using geographic variation in college proximity to estimate the return

    to schooling. In L. N. Christodes, E. K. Grant, & R. Swidinsky (Eds.),Aspects oflabour market behaviour: Essays in honour of John Vanderkamp(pp. 201222).

    Toronto, Buffalo and London: University of Toronto Press.

    Card, D. (2001). Estimating the Return to Schooling: Progress on Some Persistent

    Econometric Problems,Econometrica, 69(5), 112760.

    Clark, D., Royer, H. (2013). The effect of education on adult mortality and health:

    evidence from Britain.American Economic Review, 103(6), 20872120.

    Cobb-Clark, D., Kassenboehmer, S.C., and Schurer, S. (2013). Healthy habits: theconnection between diet, exercise, and locus of control.Journal of Economic

    Behavior and Organization, forthcoming.

    Cobb-Clark, D. A. and Schurer, S. (2013). Two Economists' Musings on the Stability of

    Locus of Control. The Economic Journal, 123(570), F358F400.

    Cutler, D.M., and Glaeser, E. (2005). What explains differences in smoking, drinking,

    and other health related behaviors.American Economic Review, 95(2), 238242.

    Cutler, D.M., and Lleras-Muney, A. (2010). Understanding differences in healthbehaviors by education.Journal of Health Economics, 29(1), 128.

    Doll, R., and Hill, A.B. (1956). Lung cancer and other causes of death in relation to

    smoking.British Medical Journal, 2(5001), 10711081.

    Droomers, M., Schrijvers, C.T.M., Stronks, K., van de Mheen, D., and Mackenbach, J.P.

    (1999). Educational differences in excessive alcohol consumption: the role of

    psychosocial and material stressors.Preventive Medicine, 29(1), 110.

    Eide, E.R., and Showalter, M.H. (2011). Estimating the relation between health andeducation: what do we know and what do we need to know.Economics of Education

    Review, 30(5), 778791.

    Friedman, H.S., Tucker, J.S., Schwartz, J.E., Joseph, E., Martin, L.R., Tomlinson-

    Keasey, C., Wingard, D.L., and Criqui, M.H. (1995). Childhood conscientiousness

    and longevity: health behaviors and cause of death. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 68(4), 696703.

    Fuch, V.R. (1982). Time preference and health: An exploratory study. In Victor R. Fuch(ed), Economic Aspects of Health, University of Chicago Press: Chicago, pp. 93120.

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    21/29

    20

    Goldberg, L.R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality-traits. American

    Psychologist, 48(1), 2634.

    Grossman, M. (1972). The demand for health: A theoretical and empirical investigation.

    Columbia University Press for the National Bureau of Economic Research: NewYork.

    Grossman, M. (1975). The correlation between health and schooling. In Nestor E.

    Terleckyj (ed.),Household Production and Consumption, Columbia University Press

    for the National Bureau of Economic Research: New York, pp. 147211.

    Grossman, M., (2000). The Human Capital Model. In: Culyer, A. J., Newhouse, J. P.

    (Eds.), Handbook of Health Economics, vol. 1. Elsevier BV, Amsterdam, pp. 347

    408, Part 1, Chapter7.

    Harmon, C. and Walker, I. (1995). Estimates of the return to schooling for the United

    Kingdom.American Economic Review, 85(5), 12781286.

    Jrges, H., Reinhold, S., and Salm, M. (2011). Does schooling affect health behavior?

    Evidence from the educational expansion in Western Germany. Economics of

    Education Review, 30(5), 862872.

    Jrges, H., Kruk, Eberhard, and Reinhold, S. (2013). The effect of compulsory schooling

    on health evidence from biomarkers.Journal of Population Economics, 26(2), 645672.

    Kemptner, D., Jrges, H., and Reinhold, S. (2011). Changes in compulsory schooling

    and the causal effect of education on health: evidence from Germany. Journal of

    Health Economics, 30(2), 340354.

    Kenkel, D.S. (1991). Health behavior, health knowledge, and schooling. Journal of

    Political Economy, 99(2), 287305.

    Kenkel, D.S., Lillard, D., and Mathios, A. (2006). The roles of high school completionand GED receipt in smoking and obesity. Journal of Labor Economics, 24(3), 635

    660.

    Leigh, J. P., (1983). Direct and Indirect Effects of Education on Health. Social Science

    and Medicine, 17(4), 227234.

    Leigh, A. and Ryan, C. (2008). Estimating returns to education using different natural

    experiment techniques.Economics of Education Review, vol. 27(2), 14960.

    Lleras-Muney, A. (2005). The relationship between education and adult mortality in theUnited States.Review of Economics Studies, 72(1), 189221.

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    22/29

    21

    McGinness, M.J., and Foege, W.H. (1993). Actual causes of death in the United States.

    Journal of the American Medical Association, 270(18), 22072012.

    Mokdad, A.H., Marks, J.S., Stroup, D.F., Gerberding, J.L. (2004). Actual causes of death

    in the United States 2000.Journal of American Medical Association, 291(10), 12381245.

    Oreopoulos, P. (2007). Do dropouts drop out too soon? Wealth, health and happiness

    from compulsory schooling.Journal of Public Economics, 91(1112), 22132229.

    Oreopoulos, P. and Salvanes, K. G. (2011). Priceless: The Nonpecuniary Benefits of

    Schooling,Journal of Economic Perspectives, 25(1), 15984.

    Park, C., and Kang, C. (2008). Does education induce healthy lifestyle?Journal of

    Health Economics, 27(6), 15161531.Pearlin, L.I., and Schooler, C. (1978). The Structure of Coping. Journal of Health and

    Social Behavior, 19(1), 221.

    Powdthavee, N. (2010). Does education reduce the risk of hypertension? Estimating the

    biomarker effect of compulsory schooling in England. Journal of Human Capital,

    4(2), 173202.

    Schoenborn, C.A. (1986). Health habits of US adults, 1985: the Alameda 7 revisited.

    Public Health Reports, 101, 571580.Silles, M. A. (2009). The Causal Effect of Education on Health: Evidence from the

    United Kingdom.Economics of Education Review, 28(1),122128.

    Spasojevic, J. (2003). Effects of education on adult health in Sweden: Results from a

    natural experiment. PhD Dissertation, City University of New York Graduate Center,

    New York.

    Staiger, D. and Stock, J.H. (1997). Instrumental variables regression with weak

    instruments.Econometrica, 65(3), 557586.

    Watson, N. and Wooden, M. (2012). The HILDA Survey: a case study in the design and

    development of a successful household panel study. Longitudinal and Life Course

    Studies, 3(3), 36981.

    Wilson, P.S. (1994). Establish risk factors and coronary artery disease: the Framingham

    study.American Journal of Hypertension, 7(pt.2), 75125.

    Wooden, M., Freidin, S. and Watson, N. (2002). The Household, Income and Labour

    Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) Survey: wave 1. The Australian Economic Review,35(3), 33948.

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    23/29

    22

    Table1:HealthBehaviors

    RegressionsWithandWithoutCompulsorySchoolLawsasInstrumentsforYearsofEducation,

    HILDA

    2007and2009

    OLS

    IV

    A

    )Dependentvariable:HEI(standardized)

    All

    Males

    Females

    All

    Males

    Females

    Y

    earsofschooling

    0.103***

    0.093***

    0.112***

    0.165***

    0.147***

    0.242***

    [0.00

    4]

    [0.006]

    [0.006]

    [0.043]

    [0.029]

    [0.035]

    A

    ge

    0.059*

    0.065

    0.057

    0.061*

    0.069

    0.065

    [0.03

    5]

    [0.049]

    [0.051]

    [0.035]

    [0.048]

    [0.053]

    A

    ge-squared/100

    -0.023

    -0.048

    -0.005

    -0.025

    -0.043

    -0.006

    [0.03

    2]

    [0.046]

    [0.045]

    [0.032]

    [0.045]

    [0.047]

    F

    emale

    0.460***

    0.457***

    [0.02

    0]

    [0.021]

    F

    -testontheexcludedinstruments:

    F-statistic

    14.67

    13.64

    13.58

    H

    ansenJstatistics(over-identificat

    iontest):p-value

    [0.715]

    [0.848]

    [0.381]

    O

    bservations

    11,651

    5,454

    6,197

    11,651

    5,454

    6,197

    R

    -squared

    0.18

    6

    0.133

    0.160

    0.165

    0.116

    0.067

    B

    )Dependentvariable:ALMDA

    (standardized)

    All

    Males

    Females

    All

    Males

    Females

    Y

    earsofschooling

    0.096***

    0.096***

    0.096***

    0.184***

    0.039

    0.152***

    [0.00

    5]

    [0.007]

    [0.006]

    [0.041]

    [0.035]

    [0.044]

    A

    ge

    -0.0

    2

    -0.003

    -0.032

    -0.017

    -0.025

    -0.028

    [0.04

    0]

    [0.057]

    [0.056]

    [0.040]

    [0.057]

    [0.055]

  • 8/13/2019 Does Increasing Schooling Improve Later Health Habits

    24/29

    23

    A

    ge-squared/100

    0.03

    9

    0.037

    0.032

    0.045

    0.051

    0.040

    [0.03

    5]

    [0.051]

    [0.049]

    [0.035]

    [0.050]

    [0.049]

    F

    emale

    0.285***

    0.284***

    [0.02

    3]

    [0.023]

    F

    -testontheexcludedinstruments:

    F-statistic

    12.83

    12.83

    16.22

    H

    ansenJstatistics(over-identificat

    iontest):p-value

    [0.691]

    [0.820]

    [0.537]

    O

    bservations

    10075

    4,848

    5227

    10075

    4848

    5227

    R

    -squared

    0.13

    1

    0.124

    0.111

    0.090

    0.107

    0.094

    Note:

    *