constraints on noticing the gap

Upload: soroush-sabbaghan

Post on 09-Apr-2018

223 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    1/28

    SSLA, 25, 99126. Printed in the United States of America.DOI: 10.1017.S0272263103000044

    CONSTRAINTS ONNOTICING THE GAP

    Nonnative Speakers Noticingof Recasts in NS-NNS Interaction

    Jenefer PhilpUniversity of Tasmania

    Interaction has been argued to promote noticing of L2 form in a con-

    text crucial to learningwhen there is a mismatch between the input

    and the learners interlanguage (IL) grammar (Gass & Varonis, 1994;

    Long, 1996; Pica, 1994). This paper investigates the extent to which

    learners may notice native speakers reformulations of their IL gram-

    mar in the context of dyadic interaction. Thirty-three adult ESL learn-

    ers worked on oral communication tasks in NS-NNS pairs. During

    each of the five sessions of dyadic task-based interaction, learners

    received recasts of their nontargetlike question forms. Accurate im-mediate recall of recasts was taken as evidence of noticing of re-

    casts by learners. Results indicate that learners noticed over 6070%

    of recasts. However, accurate recall was constrained by the level of

    the learner and by the length and number of changes in the recast.

    The effect of these variables on noticing is discussed in terms of

    processing biases. It is suggested that attentional resources and

    processing biases of the learner may modulate the extent to which

    learners notice the gap between their nontargetlike utterances and

    recasts.

    The findings reported in this paper are based on part of my doctoral dissertation completed at the

    University of Tasmania. Earlier versions were presented at the annual meeting of the American Asso-ciation of Applied Linguistics in 1999 (Stanford, CA) and at the Second Language Research Forum in1997 (Michigan State University). I am very grateful to Alison Mackey for her encouragement andastute criticisms on many earlier drafts, to Susan Gass and Noriko Iwashita for their insightful input,and to the anonymous SSLA reviewers for their detailed comments. Remaining errors are my own.

    Address correspondence to: Jenefer Philp, P.O. Box 100, Scottsdale, TAS 7260, Australia; e-mail:[email protected].

    2003 Cambridge University Press 0272-2631/03 $12.00 99

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    2/28

    100 Jenefer Philp

    For language learners, conversations with more competent speakers can be a

    rich source of exposure to the target language (TL). Some researchers haveargued that learners benefit from such input only if they attend to the lan-

    guage forms they hear (Corder, 1967; Gass, 1997; Long, 1996; Schmidt, 1993).

    In conversation with native speakers, struggles for mutual comprehension typ-

    ically result in modifications to both the language and the structure of the dis-

    course itself (Hatch, 1978, 1983; Long, 1983, 1985; Long & Sato, 1983; Swain,

    1985; Wagner-Gough & Hatch, 1975). These interactional modifications provide

    learners with implicit feedback on their own IL production (Gass; Long, 1996;

    Pica, 1994; Swain, 1995). This feedback comes at a time crucial to learning

    when there is a mismatch between the input and the learners IL grammar

    (Gass & Varonis, 1994; Long & Robinson, 1998).

    In (1) a native speaker (NS) and a nonnative speaker (NNS) carry out a

    story task together.1 The NNS apparently has difficulty in framing her question

    (first line), and her interlocutors initial response seems to be incongruentwith what she had intended to say (second and third lines). This pushes the

    learner to try again; this time she is able to produce the key lexical item

    guide, and the NS provides her with a targetlike version, or recast, of herquestion (fourth line). The recast functions as a confirmation check of her

    meaning.

    (1) NNS: The girl is it what the gui for the for for the boy for the boy. [laughs]NS: Uh huh she likes him, too.NNS: She likes him no ah I mean is ( . . . ) the girl is this um ya be be a guide? guide?NS: Oh is she a guide?NNS: Uh huh.NS: Um I dont know what she does maybe shes a student.

    Researchers have argued that it is the perception and resolution of the mis-

    match between the learners IL utterance and the TL response that may lead

    to destabilization and IL restructuring (Ellis, 1991; Faerch & Kasper, 1986;

    Gass, 1991, 1997; Gass & Varonis, 1994; Long, 1996). Recasts, in which the

    learners nontargetlike utterance (e.g., The girl is this um ya be be a guide?)is subsequently reformulated by the interlocutor as a TL version (e.g., Is shea guide?), exemplify this process: Recasts are congruent with the learnersown production and juxtapose the incorrect with the correct. If the learner is

    focused on both form and message, such feedback may present a catalyst for

    change (Long; Long & Robinson, 1998; Mackey & Philp, 1998; Oliver, 1995; Sax-

    ton, 1997). Alternatively, the learner may simply consider a recast to be confir-

    mation of meaning (Chaudron, 1977; Lyster, 1998; Lyster & Ranta, 1997) rather

    than linguistic correction. In (1) it is unclear whether the learner notices theNSs reformulation of her own question or simply responds to the reiteration

    of her meaning (line 5). This paper explores the extent to which learners no-

    tice the form of recasts of their nontargetlike utterances.

    Following L1 acquisition research, recasts have generally been described

    as utterances that rephrase a childs utterance by changing one or more sen-

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    3/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 101

    tence components (subject, verb, or object) while still referring to its central

    meanings (Long, 1996, p. 434; see also Baker & Nelson, 1984; Farrar, 1990,1992). Although there is variation, particularly concerning the degree to which

    the recast might expand the initial utterance (Bohannon, Padgett, Nelson, &

    Mark, 1996) or add emphasis to the changed elements (Chaudron, 1977; see

    Nicholas, Lightbown, & Spada, 2001, for an overview of recasts in L1 and L2

    acquisition research), this definition has been widely adopted in SLA research

    (e.g., Lyster, 1998; Mackey & Philp, 1998; Oliver, 1995). Recasts in the present

    study included all NS utterances immediately following a NNSs nontargetlike

    utterance that reformulated part or all of the utterance while maintaining the

    central meaning (Long, Inagaki, & Ortega, 1998). No explicit emphasis was

    given to the changed elements in the recast; rather, the recasts were provided

    as confirmation checks, as seen in (1).

    The central research question addressed in this study, in the context of

    dyadic interaction, is: Do learners notice recasts? Arguably, noticing is funda-mental to the potential that feedback can have for the learner. According to

    Schmidts Noticing Hypothesis (Schmidt, 1990, 1993, 1998, 2001), it is only

    what the learner notices about the input that holds potential for learning be-

    cause intakethat is, the detection, processing, and storage of input (Gass,

    1997; VanPatten, 1990)is conditional upon noticing. Long (1996), in a reform-

    ulation of the Interaction Hypothesis, also points to the importance of noticing

    (i.e., selective attention): Negotiation work that triggers interactional adjust-ments by the NS or more competent interlocutor facilitates acquisition be-cause it connects input, internal learner capacities, particularly selective

    attention, and output in productive ways (pp. 451452, emphasis in original).

    Evidence that learners notice interactional modifications, such as recasts,

    would provide support for the connection between input, the learners atten-

    tional resources, and intake (Carroll, 1999; Corder, 1967), and it may further

    our understanding of the processes of SLA.2

    Currently, there is a paucity of work that directly investigates learners no-

    ticing of forms as a result of linguistic and conversational modifications in oral

    interaction. 3 In part, this lack of research is due to the difficulty of operational-

    izing noticing for the context of interaction, which engages both the aural and

    oral skills of the learner. How can the internal processes of noticing be tapped

    at the time in which the learner is engaged in interaction? Therefore, one of

    the challenges for this study was to develop a means of measuring noticing in

    oral interaction.

    NOTICING AND THE MEASUREMENT OF NOTICING

    Noticing has been described as the part of the attentional system that involves

    the detection and consequent registration of stimuli in memory (Posner

    & Peterson, 1990; Robinson, 1995; Tomlin & Villa, 1994). Noticing of stimuli

    makes it potentially available for inclusion in long-term memory and for fur-

    ther processing, hence Schmidts (1995) claim that noticing is requisite for

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    4/28

    102 Jenefer Philp

    learning. Robinson distinguished between noticing and detection on the basis

    of awareness.4

    More recently, Simard and Wong (2001, p. 119) suggested thatwe view alertness, orientation, detection, and awareness as . . . coexisting and

    interacting in graded levels. Noticing may then be conceived as detection ac-

    companied by lesser and greater degrees or levels of awareness (see Leow,

    2000; Philp, 1998). Further, as Williams (1999) and Robinson suggested, de-

    grees of noticing (and, indeed, of awareness) may relate to the amount and

    nature of rehearsal in memory that occurs with detection.5

    Following Robinson (1995), noticing is viewed in the present study as one

    step beyond detection, being what is both detected and then further acti-

    vated following the allocation of attentional resources (p. 297). Schmidt

    (1995) argued that L2 learning must entail awareness and particularly that

    the noticing hypothesis claims that learning requires awareness at the time oflearning (p. 26, emphasis in original). Typically, in SLA research, noticing has

    been addressed through measuring awareness. Awareness has been variouslydescribed using both online procedures, such as think-alouds (Alanen, 1995;

    Leow, 1997, 2000; Rosa & ONeill, 1999) or private speech (Ohta, 2000), and

    offline procedures, such as a written questionnaire following treatment (Rob-

    inson, 1995, 1996a, 1997). The section Constraints on Noticing discusses var-

    ious methods used to tap noticing of form in oral contexts.

    To date, noticing of form by learners engaged in oral interaction has been

    assessed through retrospective methods rather than online methods (Mackey,

    Gass, & McDonough, 2000; Schmidt & Frota, 1986). Schmidt and Frotas often

    quoted diary study, in which Schmidts noticing of input at various stages in

    his development is traced through diary entries, relates to instruction and nat-

    uralistic input rather than specifically to negotiated interaction. Their study

    traced the acquisition of 21 verbal constructions by Schmidt as a beginning

    learner of Portuguese. The forms he produced were also those that had ap-

    peared in diary notes. On this basis, Schmidt and Frota stressed the role of

    conscious learning, suggesting that input only has an impact on IL production

    if it is noticed and becomes intake. Diary studies such as Schmidt and Frotas

    provide a useful, albeit partial view of L2 learning processes. However, diary

    entries involve more than noticing and fall short of arriving at actual pro-

    cesses (Tomlin & Villa, 1994).

    In a recent study, Mackey et al. (2000) used a variation on think-alouds to

    tap perception of feedback by L2 learners of English and Italian during oral

    interaction (see also Gass & Mackey, 2000). They used stimulated recalls in

    which learners were asked to respond to video replays of their own task-

    based interaction with NSs. As learners watched the video, it was stopped at

    particular points in the interaction (i.e., during sequences of interactionalmodification), and they were asked to recall what they were thinking at the

    time. Learners perceptions (rather than noticing) were operationalized as the

    learners articulations of what they considered to be the focus of the feed-

    back. The researchers noted a much higher proportion of accurate reports

    about lexical and phonological feedback than of reports about morphological

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    5/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 103

    or syntactic feedback. The Mackey et al. study, although not online, provides

    an effective means of examining attention to modifications in the context oforal interaction. Their study, which investigated the extent to which learners

    perceive feedback as such, is one step beyond noticing. Stimulated recall in-

    volves verbal articulation of noticed feedback entailing detection and further

    processing of input. Clearly, as the authors noted, failure to notice cannot be

    inferred by a failure to verbalize something (see also Robinson, 1995).

    An alternative means of accessing noticing may be immediate recall, a tech-

    nique that, along with shadowing, has been commonly used within cognitive

    psychology to access detection and rehearsal in short-term auditory memory.

    Shadowing describes a task in which the subject simultaneously listens to and

    repeats the input given; in an immediate recall task, the subject responds to a

    cue to replay what was heard at a particular point in the input (e.g., Darwin,

    Turvey, & Crowder, 1972; Glucksberg & Cowan, 1970; Moray, Bates, & Barnett,

    1965, as cited in Baddeley, 1990). Results from studies on shadowing (Cherry,1953; Glucksberg & Cowan; Norman, 1969, as cited in Coren, Ward, & Enns,

    1994), in which two messages are given (a different message in each ear) but

    only one repeated by the participant, have implications for the use of recall.

    Tests revealed that participants could not recall at the end of listening any of

    the unshadowed message. However, if interrupted during shadowing they

    could recall at least five to seven units of the message (words, numbers, etc.).

    This suggests that at some level people do detect filtered, seemingly unat-

    tended input. This input is available for processing for a short time, but, if

    not attended to, it is not stored in short-term memory. Schmidts (1995) de-

    scription of noticing essentially captured this in his claim that input that was

    not attended to was not held in short-term memory and was not available for

    further processing.

    The connection between recall and noticing may best be understood

    through a brief description of so-called working memory, as formulated by

    Baddeley and Hitch (1974) and further elaborated by Baddeley and others

    (Baddeley & Logie, 1999; for review, see Baddeley, 1986). According to their

    model, verbal input is handled by the articulatory loop, a subcomponent of

    working memory, and is held in phonological store. Information recently at-

    tended to, while held in working memory, is available for conscious recall.

    Thus, immediate recall can be a measure of what has been noticedthat is,

    detected and then further activated following the allocation of attentional re-

    sources (Robinson, 1995, p. 297). Additionally, as an oral medium, immediate

    recall could be appropriate for measuring noticing in the context of interac-

    tion.

    Constraints on Noticing

    Having discussed means of measuring noticing in oral interaction, I return to

    the central question posed by this study of whether learners notice recasts in

    NS-NNS interaction. Although noticing depends initially on available atten-

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    6/28

    104 Jenefer Philp

    tional resources, a myriad of other factors have been suggested to mediate

    noticing (see Simard & Wong, 2001). These include: the readiness of thelearner (Pienemann, 1989, 1999); frequency and saliency in the input (Bardovi-

    Harlig, 1987; Ellis, 1994; Gass, 1997); L1 influence (Carroll, 1999; Zobl, 1979);

    prior knowledge (Carroll; Ellis; Gass); familiarity, novelty of the input, or both

    (Ellis); linguistic content of the input (Mackey et al., 2000); the degree to

    which the discourse is understood (VanPatten, 1990, 1996); the degree of auto-

    maticity involved and the distinctiveness and complexity of the tasks (Robin-

    son, 1995, 1996b; Rosa & ONeill, 1999); individual differences in working

    memory capacity and abilities (Mackey, Philp, Fujii, Egi, & Tatsumi, 2002; Rob-

    inson, 2001); and the relevance and contiguity the discourse has for the

    learner (van Lier, 1994). The present study focuses on three independent vari-

    ables that may affect noticing of recasts: the level of the learner, recast length,

    and the degree of difference between the recast and the learners initial utter-

    ance.

    Developmental Level of the Learner. In an exploration of the connections

    between the input and the learners attentional resources (Long, 1996), the

    level of the learner may be a key factor modulating noticing for two reasons.

    First, research suggests that more experienced learners may benefit from the

    increasing automaticity that comes with repeated practice, which allows at-

    tentional resources to be focused on higher order aspects of speech process-

    ing (Ellis, 1994; McLaughlin, 1987; VanPatten, 1996). Second, there is the

    related issue of readiness. Speaking of the effectiveness of recasts on L1 acqui-

    sition of morphemes, Farrar (1990) claimed that so-called linguistic readiness

    is a determining factor. Farrar found recasts to be particularly effective at a

    certain stage in the childs development of morphemes but not prior to the

    time the children are cognitively ready to extract a morpheme or once they

    have successfully extracted it (p. 621). Similarly, there may be a prime time

    in which recasts are effective in facilitating particular L2 forms, such as ques-

    tion forms in English (Mackey, 1999; Mackey & Philp, 1998). It may be that

    learners tend not to notice input that is beyond their level of acquisition.

    Others have examined this issue of readiness in terms of the learners prior

    knowledge. Saxton (1997) suggested that a childs use of recasts depends on

    his or her prior knowledge that two variant forms fulfill identical grammatical

    functions (e.g., bought, buyed). It is on the basis of this knowledge that thechild can then identify a recast as a contrast between two forms, one of which

    the adult prefers. Carroll (1999), speaking of input in general, argued that in-

    take is . . . determined by our grammars . . . [and] . . . we . . . perceive what our

    linguistic systems enable us to perceivebased on the categories and struc-tures of the grammar (p. 343). Other SLA researchers have also identified

    prior knowledge in terms of familiarity with lexical or grammatical items, sug-

    gesting that it may affect noticing of particular forms (Gass, 1997; Schmidt &

    Frota, 1986). Schmidt and Frota, for example, reported that Schmidt, as a

    learner of Portuguese, did not notice (at the level of articulation of form in a

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    7/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 105

    diary entry) forms that were abundant in the input until they were highlighted

    through instruction. Perhaps the forms were not noticed at this high level ofawareness until the learner was ready (in terms of psycholinguistic process-

    ing) or simply because the forms were not sufficiently encoded in long-term

    memory and were therefore not recognizable to the learner.6 In summary,

    readiness, in terms of processing mechanisms, prior knowledge, or both, may

    modulate noticing.

    Length of Recast. The length of the recast is another factor that may criti-

    cally affect noticing, as attentional capacity is limited. Units of information

    may be held in working memory for about 1520 seconds, although by re-

    hearsal (i.e., how long it takes to repeat the words) they may be held longer

    (Cowan, 1988). What remains in working memory is a factor of the rate of re-

    hearsal as well as the rate of decay in the phonological store (Baddeley, 1986;

    Cowan, 1992, 1993, 1995; Cowan et al., 1992). Thus, longer recasts, which mayexceed the limits of temporary phonological store, might be less accurately

    recalled than shorter recasts.

    Degree of Difference. A third factor that may limit noticing is the degree

    of difference between the recast and the learners trigger utterance. A number

    of studies researching the effectiveness of recasts in L1 acquisition have con-

    sidered whether recasts were more likely to be provided after single or multi-

    ple errors (Bohannon & Stanowicz, 1988; Farrar, 1992). SLA research has

    suggested differences in incorporation of these recasts by learners (Oliver,

    1995). If recasts are too different from the learners original utterance, they

    may be unlikely to be imitated, as they are too far removed from this first

    attempt (Long, 1996). The present study considers whether L2 learners notice

    recasts involving multiple changes.

    RESEARCH QUESTIONS AND HYPOTHESES

    Do learners notice recasts? Noticing of recasts, the dependent variable, was

    operationalized as the learners cued, correct recall of a recast immediately

    following production of the recast. On the basis of the research previously

    outlined, the following questions and hypotheses were formulated:

    1. Is the ability to recall a recast constrained by the level of the learner?

    Hypothesis 1: Accuracy of recall is correlated with the level of the learner such that

    the higher the level of the learner, the greater the accuracy of recall.7

    2. Is the ability to recall a recast constrained by the length of the recast?

    Hypothesis 2: Accuracy of recall will be higher for shorter recasts than longer re-

    casts.

    3. Is the ability to recall a recast constrained by the number of changes made by the

    recast?

    Hypothesis 3: Accuracy of recall will be higher the fewer the changes made in the

    recast utterance.

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    8/28

    106 Jenefer Philp

    METHOD

    Design and Participants

    Following a preliminary placement session, each learner was involved in five

    NS-NNS dyadic interaction sessions over 2 weeks (100 minutes).8

    Participants

    were 33 adult ESL students, enrolled in 68-week intensive English courses at

    an Australian university. There were 18 female and 15 male students who were

    of different ESL proficiencies, had various L1 backgrounds (Japanese, Korean,

    Thai, Cantonese, Russian, and Indonesian), and were between 17 and 30 years

    of age. Participation in the project was voluntary. Three NSs, including the

    researcher, acted as partners in NS-NNS dyads; there were two females and

    one male between 25 and 35 years of age. Participants were randomly as-

    signed to NSs and rotated so that all participants were paired with all NSs

    over the five sessions.

    Targeted Form

    The order of acquisition for ESL question formation had been identified

    through previous research (Cancino, Rosansky, & Schumann, 1978; Johnston,

    1985; Lightbown, 1980; Meisel, Clahsen, & Pienemann, 1981; Pienemann, 1984,

    1989; Pienemann & Johnston, 1987; Pienemann, Johnston, & Brindley, 1988;

    Pienemann & Mackey, 1993; Spada & Lightbown, 1993, 1999). Thus, learners

    production of questions was considered to be a reliable indicator of their lev-

    els of development in terms of question forms and was chosen as the targeted

    structure of recasts. In SLA, the work of Pienemann and others has demon-

    strated that question forms in English are acquired in a fixed order regardless

    of instruction. I verified that, during the period of data collection, questionforms were not a focus of instruction in the learners ESL classes.

    9

    Group Assignment

    For coding purposes, following data transcription participants were assigned

    to one of three groupsLow, Intermediate, or Highaccording to their per-

    formance in a preliminary placement session and in the first treatment ses-

    sion. This group assignment was based on learners production of particular

    question forms that were the structures targeted in the study. 10 The size of

    each group was constrained by the number of students studying at the lan-

    guage center. Learners fell within four of the six stages of development pre-

    sented by Pienemann and colleagues (Pienemann & Johnston, 1987; Pienemannet al., 1988), as shown in Table 1.11

    The distinction between groups was made

    according to learners readiness to acquire stage 4 and stage 5 question

    forms. Previous use of the treatment materials had established that these

    forms were most elicited by the tasks and therefore most likely to elicit re-

    casts. The Low group was composed of seven learners (four at stage 3 and

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    9/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 107

    Table 1. Summary of stages reached by participants in pretest performance

    Group Stage Description Example

    Low 2 Canonical word order with rising into- You have girl?nation

    Low 3 Yes-no questions in which an auxiliary Do you have red alien?(e.g., do or wh-word) fronts canonical Is he is son?word order

    Intermediate 4 Yes-no questions in which subject and Is he angry?main verb or auxiliary are inverted or Where is the girl?the wh-word fronts the verb

    High 5 Auxiliary (e.g., do or have ) in second What does he do?position after wh-word and preceding What is your alien holding?the main verb

    Note. Adapted from Mackey (1999). See also Mackey (1995), Pienemann and Johnston (1987), and Spada and Light-bown (1993, 1999).

    three at stage 2) who did not show any evidence of being able to produce

    stage 4 or stage 5 forms. The Intermediate group comprised 11 learners, all of

    whom could produce stage 4 question forms. Many in this group also showed

    emergence of IL stage 5 question forms, although not in sufficient numbers to

    consider that they had acquired stage 5 forms. Typically, any production of

    stage 5 forms was very limited and generally could be described as productive

    use of a formula pattern (Ellis, 1984; Weinart, 1995; Wong-Fillmore, 1976), such

    as What is he doing? What is he looking? In terms of readiness, it appears thatthis group was at an optimum stage for acquiring stage 5 forms. Learners in

    the High group were clearly able to produce both stage 4 and stage 5 forms.

    Typically, these learners produced both IL and TL question forms at stages 4and 5.

    Learners were assessed as being at a certain level if they made at least two

    productive uses of question types of a particular stage in at least two different

    contexts (Mackey, 1999; Mackey & Philp, 1998; see also Lightbown & Spada,

    1997; Pienemann & Johnston, 1987; Pienemann et al., 1988; Silver, 2000; Spada

    & Lightbown, 1993, 1999). For example, a learner would be assessed as being

    at stage 4 if he or she were able to generate in different contexts various forms

    of the same stage 4 question type, such as Is it book? Are they yellow?, and didnot simply repeat a formula, such as Is it book? Is it yellow? Is it big house onleft?Assessment was based on consistent performance over two sessions: thepretest and the first treatment session.

    Materials

    Sets of pictures were used to elicit questions from the participants in both

    treatment and test sessions.12

    Two tasks were used in treatment sessions. The

    picture-drawing task required the NNS to ask questions to discover the con-

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    10/28

    108 Jenefer Philp

    tents of a hidden picture held by her NS partner. The NNS had to ask ques-

    tions about the picture and draw a picture to resemble as closely as possiblethe objects and their position in the hidden picture. In the story-completion

    task, the NNS was presented with a pictorial story. Six pictures were shown in

    sequence, one by one. As each picture was shown, the NNS was instructed to

    ask any questions to discover the story behind the pictures. Two tasks were

    used in the test sessions: a story-completion task in the treatment sessions,

    and a spot-the-difference task in which the NNS asked questions to find the

    differences between one picture and an almost identical one held by the NS

    partner.

    Procedure

    The protocol for treatment sessions was developed through piloting of the

    test instruments and use of recall 2 months prior to data collection. NS inter-

    locutors were trained to a high level of consistency in task protocols by read-

    ing instructions and sample transcripts and by participating in and observing

    role plays. In each of the five 20-minute sessions, NNSs were paired with a NS

    and together performed three tasks: a warm-up task, a story-completion task,

    and a picture-drawing task. The warm-up training task involved the serial re-

    call of numbers. Participants listened as their NS partner read a string of 12

    random numbers. The reading was interrupted at places by the sound of two

    knocks, after which participants were required to attempt to recall the last

    two numbers they had just heard. No feedback was given as to whether recall

    was correct or incorrect. This warm-up task was used to train participants to

    recall the NSs previous utterance in response to a knocking sound that was

    made by the NS who knocked twice on the table. All participants were able tocomplete this simple task without difficulty.

    While working on picture tasks, the NNS asked the NS questions. The NS

    provided recasts in response to any nontargetlike utterance, particularly ques-

    tion forms.13 Following the recast, the NNSs response was interrupted by the

    sound of two knocks on the table.14 The sound was a cue to the NNS to repeat

    the last thing he or she heard prior to that soundnamely, the recast, as illus-

    trated in (2).

    (2) NNS: Why he is very unhappy?NS: Why is he very unhappy? [2 knocks]NNS: Yeah why is very unhappy?

    The five sessions were audio-recorded and took place over a 2-week periodduring and following classes, depending on the schedule of the participants.

    One week prior to the treatment sessions, a similar placement session was

    administered for the purpose of group assignment. This session consisted of

    20 minutes of NS-NNS, task-based interaction (without recasts) using a story-

    completion task and a spot-the-difference task. Data from this session and the

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    11/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 109

    first treatment session were used to assess the developmental level of the

    learner.

    The Use of Recall as an Instrument for Measuring Noticing. In the pres-

    ent study, it was important to preserve the flow of normal conversation, yet

    in some way access learners short-term auditory storage of recasts at the

    time the learner heard them.15 Cued immediate recall was used as a measure

    of a particular level of noticing (Leow, 1997, 2000; Rosa & ONeill, 1999). Un-

    doubtedly, learners may notice input yet be unable to recall it. For example,

    they may notice that something in the recast was different from their own ut-

    terance but be unable to perceive what it was that was different, or they may

    at least be able to report what they perceive. Mackey et al. (2000) demon-

    strated that certain input (e.g., morphological information) is less susceptible

    to verbal report of noticing than other types (e.g., phonological information).

    Alternatively, learners may notice the difference yet be unable to reproduce itbecause of the limitations of working memory. Thus, accurate recall is a very

    conservative measure, unlikely to capture all that is noticed. However, if re-

    casts are recalled, it is evident that noticing at some level has taken place:

    Input has been detected and further processed in working memory to the ex-

    tent that it is available for recall. Obviously, the task conditions, consistently

    providing recasts of question forms, and alerting learners to their interlocu-

    tors speech through the use of the recall signal made learners more alert to

    the details of that speech than they may have been ordinarily. Nevertheless,

    only what was already detected and entered in working memory was available

    for immediate recall.16

    Coding Scheme

    Data were transcribed and coded according to the degree of accuracy with

    which learners recalled the recasts as well as the length of the recast and the

    number of corrections made by the recast.17 Accuracy of recall was catego-

    rized as correct, modified, or no recall. Correct recall was applied to rep-

    etition of the recast utterance. Modified recall described those instances in

    which the learners recall of the recast was inaccurate but represented a mod-

    ification of the original trigger utterance; see (2) for an example. Both the rep-

    etition of the trigger utterance without change and failure to respond to the

    sound cue were categorized as no recall.

    The degree of difference between the learners initial utterance and the re-

    cast was coded for the number of changes to the initial utterance: one, two,

    or more changes. The changes examined were only those errors directly re-lated to question forms, the focus of the study.18

    Thus, in (2), inversion counts

    as one change. Recasting What are you doing? as What is he doing? involvestwo changesone to the subject and one to the auxiliary. A complete re-

    phrasal such as What is he carrying? for Does he the hand what? involvesmore than two changes.

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    12/28

    110 Jenefer Philp

    Table 2. Recasts provided to each group

    Percentage of questionRecasts forms in recasts

    Group n N M Stage 3 Stage 4 Stage 5

    High 15 659 43.93 7 (44) 65 (415) 28 (179)Intermediate 11 531 48.93 8 (42) 62 (316) 30 (155)Low 7 379 54.14 6 (15) 63 (237) 33 (122)

    Note. N= total number of recasts provided to the group. M= mean number of recastsfor each individual in the group. The percentage of type of question form presentedin recasts was calculated as the number of recasts of that stage question form di-vided by the total number of recasts for that group. Numbers in parentheses are totalraw scores.

    Table 3. Length of recasts and changes to learners utterancesin recasts: Percentages by group

    Length of recast Number of changes

    Group n Short Long 1 change 2 changes 3 changes

    High 15 38 62 39 30 31Intermediate 11 52 48 37 31 32Low 7 67 33 30 30 40

    Recasts were coded as long or short on the basis of morpheme count.19

    Longer and shorter utterances were compared; longer utterances were those

    of more than five morphemes. This distinction was based on the pilot testing

    of the cued immediate recalls.20

    RESULTS

    One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with a priori paired comparisons (con-

    trasts) and t-tests were carried out to test the hypotheses. ANOVA was usedto determine differences between groups, whereas t-tests for paired sampleswere used to compare accuracy of recall with each variable for each individ-

    ual group. An alpha level of .05 was used for all statistical tests.

    The provision of recasts depended entirely on the production of nontar-

    getlike forms by each learner. Generally, as illustrated in Table 2, each learner

    received 4455 recasts of question forms over five sessions, with those in theLow group generally receiving higher numbers of recasts.21

    Of these recasts,

    all groups received over 60% of recasts of stage 4 questions.

    As shown in Table 3, the High group was presented more frequently with

    long recasts (62%), whereas the Low group received more short recasts

    (67%). Similar numbers of recasts with one, two, or three or more changes to

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    13/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 111

    Figure 1. Comparison by group of proportion ofnumber of changes in recasts.

    Table 4. One-way ANOVA for three groups on correct recall

    Source df SS MS F ratio Fprob.

    Between groups 2 951.2820 475.6410 4.1695* .0252Within groups 30 3422.2649 114.0755Total 32 4373.5469

    *p < .05.

    the learners trigger utterance were received by all groups, although the Low

    group received slightly more of the latter. A comparison between groups isshown in Figure 1.

    Noticing and the Level of the Learner

    To test hypothesis 1, which predicted that recall of recasts would be more

    accurate the higher the level of the learner, the High, Intermediate, and Low

    groups were compared. The results of a one-way ANOVA, provided in Table 4,

    show a significant effect for learner level on recall of recasts. With an alpha

    level of .05, the effect of learner level on recall of recasts was statistically sig-

    nificant, F(2, 30) = 4.1695, p < .05. A priori contrasts, tested by the t statistic,were computed to establish the source of difference between groups. A signifi-

    cant difference was found between the High and Intermediate groups on theone hand and the Low group on the other (p < .05). The High and Intermediategroups were not significantly different in performance on recall (p = .814). TheANOVA results were confirmed by t-test comparisons between High and Lowlearners, t(14.18) = 2.76, p < .05, and 2 = .35, and between Intermediate andLow learners, t(11.81) = 2.71, p < .05, and

    2= .38. The strength of association

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    14/28

    112 Jenefer Philp

    Table 5. Correct recall by groups

    Correct recall

    Group n M% SD SE of M

    High 15 74.179 12.059 3.114Intermediate 11 73.203 8.896 2.682Low 7 60.675 9.957 3.764

    Figure 2. Accuracy of recall for each group.

    between the level of the learner and accuracy of recall was moderately high,

    at 3538%, which suggests that level is one important factor but other vari-

    ables also play a part in noticing. As shown in Table 5, High and Intermediate

    learners recalled over 70% of recasts accurately, whereas the Low learners ac-curately recalled 60% of recasts. A comparison by group according to recall is

    shown in Figure 2. The Low group modified more recasts in recall than the

    higher level groups (27% compared to an average of 19%). Fewer than 11% of

    recasts were not recalled at all. Notably, immediate recall involved some

    change to the trigger utterance in 88% of cases for the Low group and over

    9293% for the higher level groups.

    In summary, the High and Intermediate groups were significantly more ac-

    curate on recall of recasts than the Low group. This is an indication that the

    higher level learners noticed recasts more consistently than the Low group.

    Noticing and the Length of the Recast

    The length of the recast utterance was investigated as a constraint on accu-

    rate recall, irrespective of the level of the learner, and hypothesis 2 was sup-

    ported. The results of a t-test for paired samples performed on data from allgroups found that recasts of five or fewer morphemes were recalled with

    greater accuracy than recasts of six or more morphemes, t(32) = 5.47, p < .05,

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    15/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 113

    Figure 3. Percentage of correct recall ac-cording to the length of the recast utter-

    ance.

    and 2= .48. The eta squared result reveals that almost 50% of the variability

    in this sample can be accounted for by the length of the recast. Differences

    are reflected in Figure 3. For the High and Intermediate groups, the percentage

    of correct recall was as much as 15% higher for short recasts than for long

    recasts, with over 80% accuracy on recall of short recasts. Similarly, the Low

    group performed far better on shorter recasts than on longer recasts, recall-

    ing the latter with only 50% accuracy on average. Thus, length of recast had a

    significant effect on accuracy of recall, which suggests that short recasts were

    more consistently noticed by learners. This result held irrespective of the

    learners level.

    Noticing and the Number of Changes in the Recast

    Finally, the difference between the learners nontargetlike utterance and the

    recast offered in response was another potential constraint on noticing. The

    results of a paired sample t-test indicated that all groups performed better thefewer the changes to the trigger utterance, t(32) = 5.74, p < .05, and 2 = .51.The eta squared result reveals a strong association between noticing and the

    number of changes to the trigger utterance. For the High and Intermediate

    groups, there was a significant difference in accuracy of recall for recasts with

    only one change compared to recasts with two changes. However, the

    strength of association between these variables is low, t(25) = 2.07, p < .05, and

    2= .15. This was also true of recasts with two changes compared to recasts

    with three changes, t(25) = 3.71, p < .05, and 2 = .36. Figure 4 shows the rela-

    tive performance of the three groups according to the number of changes inthe recast. The High and Intermediate groups showed at least 20% greater ac-

    curacy on recasts containing only one change compared to recasts with three

    or more changes. The Low groups accuracy was 14% greater for recasts with

    one or two changes compared to recasts with three or more changes.

    In summary, all groups recalled recasts with three or more changes with

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    16/28

    114 Jenefer Philp

    Figure 4. Accuracy of recall according to number ofchanges in recast.

    significantly less accuracy than they recalled recasts with fewer changes. This

    result held irrespective of level, and it is an indication that the more different

    the recast is from the original utterance, the less likely the learner is to notice

    all features of the recast with accuracy.

    DISCUSSION

    In general, the results support the claim that learners notice a considerable

    amount of implicit feedback provided through interaction in a primed context.

    However, recast length, the number of changes made in the recast, and the

    readiness of the learner are all factors that appear to constrain the noticing of

    feedback provided through recasts in the context of interaction.

    The Level of the Learner

    Bias. Learner biases may account for the relationship between the IL level

    of the learner and noticing. Both L1 and L2 acquisition researchers have ar-

    gued that learners are biased to some degree to the input they hear by their

    current IL knowledge (Carroll, 1999; Gass, 1997; Harley, 1994; Newport, Gleit-

    man, & Gleitman, 1977; VanPatten, 1996; White, 1987). This bias modulates the

    learners apperception of the recast and, ultimately, what becomes intake for

    the learner. As White (p. 97) noted, the learners current grammar . . . acts

    as a filter on the input. . . . That is, the learner rejects input which cannot be

    interpreted in terms of his or her current knowledge, or modifies it so that itcan be dealt with [emphasis added].This is illustrated in (3), in which the Low learner reinterprets two consec-

    utive recasts according to her own IL grammar. In each case, consistent with

    her own IL production, she uses a stage 3 rather than a stage 5 question form

    in recall.

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    17/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 115

    (3) a. NNS: Does does he uh the hand uh on the hand ah what what.

    NS: What is he carrying?NNS: Yeah what he is carry?b. NNS: What are you doing what are you doing the pic uh the boys in the picture?

    NS: What is he doing?NNS: Yeah what hes doing?

    The learners struggle to express her question is resolved by the NSs re-

    cast (second line). The learner modifies this recast in her recall, maintaining

    the lexical item carry but modifying the syntax and morphology (third line).She uses a stage 3 form in which the question word is placed before canonical

    word order. This pattern is repeated in (3b). This time the NNS initially uses

    a formulaic question, What are you doing? (first line).22 In response to therecast she reformulates the stage 5 question as a stage 3 form, again using a

    question word to front canonical word order, ignoring the use of inversion in

    the NSs recast (last line). This inversion, required in stage 5 question forms,

    is not consistent with the learners current IL grammar. Arguably, the learner

    may have noticed that her IL form was different from the recast, but her ap-

    perception of the recast was partial; she received an incomplete picture of the

    input given. Thus, it may be that the learner is biased by her own IL grammar

    and potential immediate developments beyond it.23 An alternative argument is

    that apperception was accurate, but retrieval was problematic. It appears that

    where the recast is sufficiently long for the learner to have to reconstruct it

    to some degreethat is, where the whole recast utterance cannot accurately

    be represented in working memoryrecall may be affected by reliance on

    long-term memory and the learners own IL system.

    Clearly, the level of the learner is not the sole determining factor in terms

    of what is noticed. Even learners in the Low group were able to retain veryshort recasts in working memory and recall them accurately, such as the

    stage 4 form Where is she? Perhaps such short chunks, although beyond thelevel of the learner, form the basis for future development. The possibility that

    long-term rather than short-term effects are one outcome of interactional feed-

    back has been suggested by other researchers (Brock, Crookes, Day, & Long,

    1986; Lightbown, 1998; Mackey, 1999; Mackey & Philp, 1998). This is in keeping

    with the claim that pattern extraction is a key mechanism in language acquisi-

    tion and that learners slowly accumulate frequent sequences in the input (El-

    lis, 1996, 2002).

    Familiarity. Familiarity with form in the input may also account for the dis-

    parity between groups in accuracy of recall, particularly with longer recasts

    when attentional resources are taxed. Compare, for example, the responses ofthese two learners to recasts of their nontargetlike utterances:

    (4) Intermediate learnerNS: Hes selling the house.NNS: Why he is sell the house?

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    18/28

    116 Jenefer Philp

    NS: Why is he selling the house? [knock knock]

    NNS: Why he : is : selling the house?(5) High learner

    NNS: Why they want to sell to the house?NS: Why do they want to sell the house?[knock knock]NNS: Why : do they want to sell the house?

    In (4), the Intermediate learner constructed a question using the language pro-

    vided by the NS in the preceding utterance and omitted the morpheme pre-

    viously provided with the verb (second line). In the recast, a stage 5 form was

    provided; the auxiliary and subject were inverted, and the morpheme was re-

    peated in the verb (third line). Following the recast, it was the morphological

    change that the NNS apparently noticed; perhaps now hearing it for the sec-

    ond time, she did not pick up the syntactic changes made (fourth line). Her

    recall was marked by pauses and hesitation, she appeared to have difficultyrepeating the recast from working memory, and she did not manage to recall

    it accurately. Stage 5 forms were not part of this learners IL grammar, and her

    unfamiliarity with the form may have affected recall. In (5), the High learner

    recalled with accuracy a rather lengthy recast in which there were two

    changes: the insertion of the auxiliary do and the deletion of the particle toafter sell (third line). This was perhaps a performance error on her part.Both directly before and after this recast episode this learner produced the

    auxiliary in stage 5 question forms in a targetlike way, as shown in (6). Argu-

    ably, familiarity with the form allowed her to focus on other corrections in the

    utterance of which she may have been unsure initially. She recalled the previ-

    ous recast accurately.

    (6) High learnerNNS: Why ah does he want to wash the window before he goes to work?

    (later turn)NNS: Why he oh why does he want to call the painter?

    Phonotactic familiarity was found to affect performance in a study by Ser-

    vice (1992). In a comparison of Finnish childrens repetition of Finnish- and

    English-sounding pseudowords, Service found that the children could repeat

    Finnish pseudowords with almost 100% accuracy but had difficulty with En-

    glish-sounding words. Service suggested that the familiar sounding pseudo-

    words created better-quality or longer-lasting traces in the phonological input

    store and were therefore easier to repeat (p. 44). The same may be true of

    these adult L2 learners.

    In addition to phonotactic difficulties, lack of familiarity with the lexicalitems encountered in recasts may account for poor recall among the Low

    group. This is supported by the findings of L1 research. Cowan (1993), for ex-

    ample, reported on a study in which participants were more successful in re-

    call of English words than nonsense words (Hulme, Maughan, & Brown, 1991)

    and concluded that ones long-term lexical familiarity with the material to be

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    19/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 117

    activated makes a big difference in working memory tasks (p. 166). The same

    mechanisms concerning phonotactic and lexical familiarity may hold for mor-phosyntactic forms (see Ellis, 1996).

    24

    In summary, both the learners processing biases and familiarity with the

    target structure may modulate noticing of recasts. In terms of acquisition of

    question forms, recasts may be of more or less potential benefit to the learner

    according to how well the recast matches the learners readiness to acquire

    the form (see Mackey & Philp, 1998; Spada, 1997). Recognition of units within

    a recast may have contributed to increased accuracy according to the level of

    the learner: The higher level learner may have had the advantage of prior fa-

    miliarity with the input. As working memory is limited in capacity, learners

    who have a larger store of L2 data and greater automaticity in comprehension

    and production may have an advantage.

    The Length of the Recast

    The fact that all learners, irrespective of level, found shorter recasts easier to

    recall suggests that recast length is a factor related to the limitations of work-

    ing memory rather than one linked to developmental level. Working memory

    appears to offer a brief window of time in which data may be retained through

    rehearsal (Baddeley, 1986; Cowan, 1988, 1993, 1995; Ellis, 1996), and the re-

    sults concerning length of recast reflect that phenomenon.

    The finding that the length of the recast affects accuracy of recall is rele-

    vant to the claim that learners make comparisons between L2 input and the

    IL (Gass, 1991, 1997; Gass & Varonis, 1994). To do this, learners should first

    notice the differences and process them sufficiently for long-term memory

    storage. Previous research on the learning of new words (Baddeley, Pa-

    pagno, & Vallar, 1988; Cowan, 1995) and on morphosyntactic development (El-

    lis, 1996; see Ellis & Sinclair, 1996, for review) has suggested that short-term

    phonological store, through processes of rehearsal, allows input to remain in

    working memory potentially long enough for subsequent comparison and con-

    solidation. Cowan (1988, p. 66) suggested that because auditory memory can

    retain input for a limited time this input is available for comparison of utter-

    ances just as it is for problem-solving tasks. Further, Ellis claimed that mor-

    phosyntactic and other aspects of language are acquired through implicit

    analysis of memorized sequences from L2 input. He asserted that the repeti-

    tion of sequences in working memory results in consolidation of long-term

    representations of this sequence information (p. 113). This reflects the im-

    portance of auditory memory in language development.These findings suggest that shorter recasts may be of more benefit to learn-

    ers because they can be accurately retained in working memory and thus

    made available for comparison and further processing: Lengthy recasts (over

    five morphemes) may overload time limitations of phonological store and are

    difficult to retain in working memory in precisely the form given. Having said

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    20/28

    118 Jenefer Philp

    this, length of the recast is just one of many factors affecting retention in

    working memory.

    The Number of Changes Made by the Recast

    In a similar way, recasts that are closer to the trigger utterance and that

    change the utterance in few ways linguistically may be of more benefit to

    learners, as they were recalled with greater accuracy in this study. These re-

    sults support the claims of researchers such as Gass (1991) and Boulouffe

    (1986), who noted that if the mismatch between the TL utterance and the

    learners utterance is too great, it will not be perceptible to the learner; cogni-

    tive comparison (Ellis, 1994) by the learner will be unlikely.

    Previous research on the incidence of recasts in L1 and L2 acquisition has

    suggested that recasts are less likely to be provided when there are more er-

    rors (Bohannon & Stanowicz, 1988; Doughty, 1994a; Farrar, 1992; Oliver, 1995).The findings of the present study suggest that such recasts are also less likely

    to be noticed by learners. Again, factors related to working memory, such as

    length of recast, are likely to interact with the number of changes in the re-

    cast.

    Limitations and Future Research

    Generalizability of Findings. It is emphasized that this has been a report

    of an essentially exploratory study. The majority of learners were educated to

    at least postsecondary level, most were socioeconomically advantaged, and,

    in general, they were motivated to study the L2. They were young adults, en-

    rolled in intensive, short ESL courses in the context of an English-speaking

    country. For many, their course was preparatory to university studies either

    in home countries or in Australia. The learners were eager to practice their

    English with NSs. Different populations involving larger samples from both for-

    eign and second language settings and with less educated learners would

    allow for factors such as L1 and instructional context to be controlled and for

    findings to be generalized.

    This study involved one-on-one NS-NNS interaction in a controlled setting,

    which is clearly different from many other contexts, including the L2 class-

    room. Differences in context, description of feedback, and analysis reveal dif-

    ferent results with regard to recasts (Nicholas et al., 2001). Lyster (1998)

    argued (in the context of the French immersion primary-grade classroom) that

    recasts provided by the teacher, although frequent, are unlikely to be noticed

    by the learner as corrective feedback. However, examining a different type offeedback (using more explicit, narrowly focused corrective recasts in a con-

    tent-based ESL science class), Doughty and Varela (1998) found contrary re-

    sults. Ohta (2000), evaluating responses to recasts through noting the private

    speech of L2 Japanese university students, similarly suggested that learners

    did notice recasts. These differences point to the need for more classroom-

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    21/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 119

    based research that focuses on various types of feedback and that includes

    noticing.25

    Additionally, given that NNS-NNS groups are more common in thelanguage classroom, a replication of the study with NNS-NNS dyads or groups

    would be of interest, particularly in light of earlier work (Gass & Varonis, 1989;

    Porter, 1986) on the (non)incorporation of NNS corrections.

    As debate over implicit versus explicit feedback continues (Norris & Or-

    tega, 2000; Spada, 1997), an extension of the current research would also be

    to compare learners noticing of recasts with noticing of NS input or other

    types of NS feedback. Finally, the analysis was confined to learners noticing

    of morphosyntactic changes to question forms. Obviously, this represents a

    small part of all the linguistic features presented to learners in recasts. Future

    research might explore learners noticing of phonological, lexical, and prag-

    matic elements as well as other morphological and syntactic forms.

    Learner Production and Noticing. Another avenue for research lies in theconnection between output and noticing. Within the context of the classroom

    a number of researchers have suggested that recasts are less effective as a

    form of corrective feedback to the learner because they do not lead to self-

    repair and thus do not push learners in their output (Allwright & Bailey, 1991;

    Calve, 1992; Chaudron, 1988; Lyster, 1998; Pica, 1988; Pica et al., 1989; van Lier,

    1988). However, it remains unclear whether the internal processes of noticing,

    comparison, and integration occur following recasts, regardless of whether

    pushed output is an outcome (Mackey & Philp, 1998). Other researchers have

    debated the relationship between feedback and immediate output and its ef-

    fect on IL change (Gass, 1989; Gass & Varonis, 1994; Schachter, 1983). An im-

    portant theoretical question is: Does production per se make learners more

    attentive to the feedback they receive? (See also, on the issue of learners ac-

    tive participation in negotiated interaction, Ellis, Tanaka, & Yamazaki, 1994;Mackey, 1999; Pica, 1992; Swain, 1995; Swain & Lapkin, 1995.)

    CONCLUSION

    The results demonstrate that these adult ESL learners for the most part no-

    ticed the changes made to their nontargetlike utterances through recasts.

    Learners whose level of acquisition of question forms matched those provided

    in recasts accurately recalled at least 70% of recasts and modified the trigger

    utterance in over 90% of cases. This finding suggests that recasts of question

    forms may be effectively used by learners when developmental level and feed-

    back correspond.

    However, learners did not always notice recasts, and they did not alwaysnotice every detail. In part, difficulties in recall may reflect the limitations of

    working memory. Unfamiliar input, multiple corrections, complex changes,

    and long utterances all pose high demands on learners attentional resources.

    Recasts may be less accessible to low-level learners in particular, who, while

    struggling with the unfamiliarity of the input, may find the disparity between

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    22/28

    120 Jenefer Philp

    the recast and their own attempts too great to deal with. Recasts may often

    present forms far beyond learners IL grammar. The learners own processingbiases may limit noticingthat is, biases to comprehend the message over

    analyzing form and biases to perceive form in terms of the IL grammar. In

    terms of understanding processes of SLA, these findings support the claim for

    an interface between interaction, noticing, and SLA (Long, 1996). However, the

    relationship between interactional modifications, noticing, and intake is highly

    complex, balancing the learners IL knowledge and attentional resources

    against linguistic forms in the input. Future research on the role of noticing in

    L2 development through interaction needs to take this balance into account.

    (Received 7 May 2002)

    NOTES

    1. All data is from Philp (1998).2. This study focuses on recasts as one type of implicit corrective feedback to learners occur-

    ring in interaction. Recasts may function, though not exclusively, as negotiation for meaning (Long &Robinson, 1998). Although reducing the possibility of pushed output (Calve, 1992; Pica, Holliday,Lewis, & Morgenthaler, 1989; Swain, 1995), recasts have been argued to be an important source offeedback to L2 learners (Long & Robinson, 1998; Mackey & Philp, 1998; Ohta, 2000) and appear tobe more prevalent in the classroom than other types of feedback (Doughty, 1994b; Lyster & Ranta,1997).

    3. Many of the recent studies in SLA that have investigated learners noticing of form tended touse reading and written tasks or focus on instruction (input enhancement); for example: Alanen(1995), Doughty and Varela (1998), Fotos (1993), Jourdenais, Ota, Stauffer, Boyson, and Doughty(1995), Leow (1997, 1998), Robinson (1996a), Rosa and ONeill (1999), Slimani (1989), Swain and Lap-kin (1995), VanPatten (1990), and VanPatten and Cadierno (1993). Spada (1997), in a review of re-search on form-focused instruction, described it as drawing the learners attention to form eitherexplicitly or implicitly (p. 73). Typically, research in this area assumes rather than tests noticingand looks at outcomes rather than internal processes (Truscott, 1998). Observation of learner up-take, for example, assumes noticing of interactional modifications and focuses on use, rather than

    seeking to directly measure noticing per se as a first step (e.g., Ellis, Basturkmen, & Loewen, 2001;Lyster & Ranta, 1997).

    4. Both Robinson (1995) and Schmidt (1994) clearly wished to distinguish between detectionleading to registration in short-term memory and detection as evidenced by transient, subliminalexposure effects, such as those measured in priming experiments (Tomlin & Villa, 1994). This distinc-tion is also made here as the issue of unconscious learning (e.g., Robinson, 1997) is beyond the goalsof this study.

    5. I am thankful to an anonymous SSLA reviewer for this observation.6. The idea that prior knowledge primes the learner to apperceive specific elements in the input

    (Gass, 1997) is upheld in general learning theory (Ausubel, Novak, & Hanesian, 1978; Bruner, 1961,as cited in Driscoll, 1994). Ausubel (1968), for example, claimed that we perceive and interpret verbalmessages in light of existing knowledge and that the most important single factor influencing learn-ing is what the learner already knows (p. vi). Bruner (1961, 1973, as cited in Driscoll, 1994) sug-gested that feedback be provided in a mode that is both meaningful (i.e., related to what the learneralready knows) and within the information processing capacity of the learner.

    7. Level here refers to development, not proficiency, as determined by learners production ofquestion forms (see Mackey, 1999).

    8. This study represents part of a larger study that examined learners noticing and use of re-casts (Philp, 1998). The complete study, including three posttest sessions, took place over 6 weeksand was based on a design developed by Mackey (1999); see also Mackey and Philp (1998).

    9. Records of weekly lesson plans were made available to the researcher.10. This study focused on IL production of question forms. Therefore, group assignment was not

    made according to proficiency level. However, it is noted that groupings roughly corresponded toclass groupings in the school, which were based on in-house proficiency measures: Six of the seven

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    23/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 121

    subjects in the Low group came from beginner-level classes, 7 of the 11 in the Intermediate groupcame from intermediate-level classes, and 10 of the 15 in the High group came from two advanced-level classes.

    11. The hierarchy of stages is derived from hypothesized processing constraints that underlieeach stage. This hierarchy is implicational in that each stage is a prerequisite to the formation ofstructures in the following stage.

    12. The picture tasks used in the sessions were based on those developed at the Language Acqui-sition Research Centre (LARC), University of Sydney, with funding from Language Australia. Thetasks were chosen for demonstrated elicitation of question forms (see Mackey, 1999; Mackey & Philp,1998; Pienemann & Mackey, 1993).

    13. Although NS partners were instructed to recast any nontargetlike utterance (so as to mini-mize the likelihood of learners recognizing question forms as the focus of the research), in practice,recasts of other forms were disparate and few. NS partners provided 912 recasts on average fornontargetlike question forms compared to 02 recasts of other forms per session.

    14. For the importance of using a sound cue rather than a verbal cue, see Baddeley (1990).15. Clearly, any intrusion interrupts the natural flow of conversation to some extent. The tran-

    scripts, however, indicate that the use of recall posed minimal interference insofar as learners re-mained on task.

    16. Note that with regard to NS feedback this study focuses only on noticing of recasts.

    17. All sessions were transcribed by the researcher. To ensure accuracy, approximately 10 hoursof data were also transcribed by two research assistants. The researcher coded all transcripts andrecoded approximately 15% of all data at least 6 months after the initial coding. The intrarater reli-ability on coding of accuracy of recall, based on percentage agreement of 135 recasts from 10 ran-dom transcripts from learners of all levels, was 99.92%. One-third of the treatment transcripts weredouble-coded by six assistants. The interrater reliability between three coders on coding of accuracyof recall in treatment sessions was based on 50 recasts from four random transcripts from learnersof all levels, and it was 91%. Intrarater reliability on coding of changes to the trigger utterance, basedon percentage agreement of 100 recasts from eight random transcripts, was 89%.

    18. Thus, unrelated errors not pertinent to the question form, such as phonological change orthe addition of an article in a recast, were not considered in the analysis.

    19. Given the work of Baddeley (1986), Cowan (1995), and others, in which the word-length effecton recall appears to be a function of rate of rehearsal, the length of time taken to articulate therecast may have been a more accurate measure rather than a morpheme or syllable count. However,the relationship between speech rate and memory span is still a contentious and complex issue (fordiscussion, see Cowan, 1993) involving not only duration of words but also pauses between words.Although important, this issue is beyond the means or goals of this paper.

    20. During piloting of cued immediate recall by six learners of different levels, learners demon-strated less accuracy in reproducing recasts of questions longer than five morphemes. Stage 5 ques-tion forms, such as What is he doing?, were accurately recalled more consistently by learners thanlonger versions, such as What is she going to do?or What is that angry man saying?

    21. Differences between groups on the provision of recasts were not considered problematic be-cause, for each research question, group performance was treated separately in the analyses. Wherecomparisons were made, averages were given.

    22. Although it is possible that the question used by the learner is not formulaic, this is veryunlikely here, as this learners production is consistent with stage 3 utterances and no other exam-ples of stage 5 question forms are found in the data. Additionally, What are you doing? is a com-monly found formulaic question form in these learners production (see Lightbown, 1998, fordiscussion of analysis of unanalyzed chunks). Assessment of the learners IL production is a resultof data comprising a minimum of 100 minutes over 5 days.

    23. Whereas High and Intermediate learners had acquired stage 4 prior to treatment sessions,present in over 60% of recasts, Low learners had yet to acquire these forms. This explanation for thedifference in performance between the higher level groups and the Low group provides support forthe notion of readiness and the sequential acquisition of question forms in ESL (Pienemann, 1984,

    1989, 1999). In this framework, acquisition is constrained by the prior acquisition of certain forms.Here it is suggested that readiness to acquire a structure affects the learners noticing of that struc-ture in the input, a step argued by most to be requisite to acquisition (Gass, 1991, 1997; Schmidt,1990).

    24. Ellis (1996) argued that much of languagelexical, phonological, and morphosyntacticinboth L1 and L2 is acquired through implicit analysis of memorized sequences of language. Arguably,the Low learners had not yet built up this resource of sets of sequences in long-term memory.

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    24/28

    122 Jenefer Philp

    25. In this study, although recall provided a limited measure of noticing, it nevertheless was auseful means of accessing noticing in oral interaction at the time that feedback was provided tolearners. The use of recall provided information not only about noticing but also about what mor-phosyntactic elements in particular were noticed by learners and how this related to their own rep-resentations of the data.

    REFERENCES

    Alanen, R. (1995). Input enhancement and rule presentation in second language acquisition. In R.Schmidt (Ed.), Attention and awareness in foreign language learning (Tech. Rep. No. 9, pp. 259302). Honolulu: University of Hawaii, Second Language Teaching and Curriculum Center.

    Allwright, D., & Bailey, K. (1991). Focus on the language classroom: An introduction to classroom re-search for language teachers. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Ausubel, D. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Ausubel, D., Novak, J., & Hanesian, H. (1978). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. New York:

    Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Baddeley, A. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learningand motivation: Vol. 8. Advances in research and theory (pp. 4790). San Diego, CA: AcademicPress.

    Baddeley, A., & Logie, R. (1999). Working memory. The multiple-component model. In P. Shah & A.Miyake (Eds.), Models of working memory(pp. 2861). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Baddeley, A., Papagno, C., & Vallar, G. (1988). When long-term learning depends on short-term stor-age. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586595.

    Baker, N. D., & Nelson, K. E. (1984). Recasting and related conversational techniques for triggeringsyntactic advances by young children. First Language, 5, 322.

    Bardovi-Harlig, K. (1987). Markedness and salience in second language acquisition. Language Learn-ing, 37, 385407.

    Bohannon, J., Padgett, R., Nelson, K., & Mark, M. (1996). Useful evidence on negative evidence. Devel-opmental Psychology, 32, 551555.

    Bohannon, J., & Stanowicz, L. (1988). The issue of negative evidence: Adult responses to childrenslanguage errors. Developmental Psychology, 24, 684689.

    Boulouffe, J. (1986). Intake as a locus of equilibration in language learning. Language Learning, 36,245274.

    Brock, C., Crookes, G., Day, R., & Long, M. (1986). The differential effects of corrective feedback innative speaker/non-native speaker conversation. In R. R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversationin second language acquisition (pp. 327351). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Calve, P. (1992). Corriger ou pas corriger, la nest pas la question [To correct or not to correct, thatis not the question]. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 48, 458471.

    Cancino, H., Rosansky, E., & Schumann, J. (1978). The acquisition of English negatives and interroga-tives by Spanish speakers. In E. Hatch (Ed.), Second language acquisition: A book of readings (pp.207230). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Carroll, S. (1999). Putting input in its proper place. Second Language Research, 15, 337388.Chaudron, C. (1977). A descriptive model of discourse in the corrective treatment of learners errors.

    Language Learning, 27, 2946.Chaudron, C. (1988). Second language classrooms. New York: Cambridge University Press.Cherry, E. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two years.

    Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975979.Corder, S. P. (1967). The significance of learners errors. International Review of Applied Linguistics in

    Language Teaching, 5, 161170.Coren, S., Ward, L., & Enns, J. (1994). Sensation and perception. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace.

    Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual con-straints within the human information processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104, 163191.

    Cowan, N. (1992). Verbal memory span and the timing of spoken recall. Journal of Memory and Lan-guage, 31, 668684.

    Cowan, N. (1993). Activation, attention, and short-term memory. Memory and Cognition, 21, 162167.Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Cowan, N., Day, L., Saults, J., Keller, T., Johnson, T., & Flores, L. (1992). The role of verbal output

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    25/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 123

    time in the effects of word length on immediate memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 31,117.

    Darwin, C., Turvey, M., & Crowder, R. (1972). An auditory analogue of the Sperling partial reportprocedure: Evidence for brief auditory storage. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 255267.

    Doughty, C. (1994a). Fine-tuning of feedback by competent speakers to language learners. In J. Alatis(Ed.), Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics 1993 (pp. 96108). Wash-ington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

    Doughty, C. (1994b, October). Finely-tuned feedback as focus on form. Paper presented at the SecondLanguage Research Forum, Montreal.

    Doughty, C., & Varela, E. (1998). Communicative focus on form. In C. Doughty & J. Williams (Eds.),Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 114138). New York: Cambridge Uni-versity Press.

    Driscoll, M. (1994). Psychology of learning for instruction. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.Ellis, N. C. (1996). Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order. Studies

    in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 91126.Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories

    of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24, 143188.

    Ellis, N. C., & Sinclair, S. (1996). Working memory in the acquisition of vocabulary and syntax: Putting

    language in good order. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A(1), 234250.Ellis, R. (1984). Classroom second language development. Oxford: Pergamon.Ellis, R. (1991). The interaction hypothesis: A critical evaluation. In E. Sadtono (Ed.), Language acqui-

    sition in the second/foreign language classroom (pp. 179211). Singapore: SEAMEO Regional Lan-guage Centre.

    Ellis, R. (1994). A theory of instructed second language acquisition. In N. Ellis (Ed.), Implicit and ex-plicit learning of languages (pp. 79114). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Ellis, R., Basturkmen, H., & Loewen, S. (2001). Learner uptake in communicative ESL lessons. Lan-guage Learning, 51, 281318.

    Ellis, R., Tanaka, Y., & Yamazaki, A. (1994). Classroom interaction, comprehension, and the acquisi-tion of L2 word meanings. Language Learning, 44, 449491.

    Faerch, C., & Kasper, G. (1986). The role of comprehension in second language learning. AppliedLinguistics, 7, 257274.

    Farrar, M. (1990). Discourse and the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. Journal of Child Lan-guage, 17, 607624.

    Farrar, M. (1992). Negative evidence and grammatical morpheme acquisition. Developmental Psychol-ogy, 28, 9098.

    Fotos, S. (1993). Consciousness-raising and noticing through focus on form: Grammar task perfor-mance versus formal instruction. Applied Linguistics, 14, 385407.

    Gass, S. (1989). How do learners resolve linguistic conflicts? In S. Gass & J. Schachter (Eds.), Linguis-tic perspectives on second language acquisition (pp. 183200). New York: Cambridge UniversityPress.

    Gass, S. (1991). Grammar instruction, selective attention, and learning processes. In R. Phillipson, E.Kellerman, L. Selinker, M. Sharwood Smith, & M. Swain (Eds.), Foreign/second language pedagogyresearch (pp. 134141). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Gass, S. (1997). Input, interaction, and the second language learner. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Gass, S., & Mackey, A. (2000). Stimulated recall methodology in second language research . Mahwah, NJ:

    Erlbaum.Gass, S., & Varonis, E. (1989). Incorporated repairs in nonnative discourse. In M. Eisenstein (Ed.),

    The dynamic interlanguage (pp. 7186). New York: Plenum Press.Gass, S., & Varonis, E. (1994). Input, interaction, and second language production. Studies in Second

    Language Acquisition, 16, 283302.Glucksberg, S., & Cowan, G. J. (1970). Memory for nonattended auditory material. Cognitive Psychol-

    ogy, 1, 149156.

    Harley, B. (1994). Appealing to consciousness in the L2 classroom. AILA Review, 11, 5768.Hatch, E. (1978). Discourse analysis and second language acquisition. In E. Hatch (Ed.), Second lan-

    guage acquisition: A book of readings (pp. 402435). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Hatch, E. (1983). Psycholinguistics: A second language perspective. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.Hulme, C., Maughan, S., & Brown, G. (1991). Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for

    a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language,30, 685701.

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    26/28

    124 Jenefer Philp

    Johnston, M. (1985). Syntactic and morphological progressions in learner English. Canberra, Australia:Department of Immigration and Ethnic Affairs.

    Jourdenais, R., Ota, M., Stauffer, S., Boyson, B., & Doughty, C. (1995). Does textual enhancement pro-mote noticing? A think aloud protocol analysis. In R. Schmidt (Ed.), Attention and awareness inforeign language learning (Tech. Rep. No. 9, pp. 183216). Honolulu: University of Hawaii, Sec-ond Language Teaching and Curriculum Center.

    Leow, R. (1997). Attention, awareness, and foreign language behavior. Language Learning, 47, 467505.

    Leow, R. (1998). Toward operationalizing the process of attention in SLA: Evidence for Tomlin andVillas (1994) fine-grained analysis of attention. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 133159.

    Leow, R. (2000). A study of the role of awareness in foreign language behavior: Aware versus un-aware learners. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 557584.

    Lightbown, P. M. (1980). The acquisition and use of questions by French L2 learners. In S. Felix (Ed.),Second language development: Trends and issues (pp. 151175). Tubingen: Gunter Narr.

    Lightbown, P. M. (1998). The importance of timing in focus on form. In C. Doughty, & J. Williams(Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 114138). New York: Cam-bridge University Press.

    Lightbown, P. M., & Spada, P. (1997). Learning English as a second language in a special school inQuebec. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 53, 315355.

    Long, M. H. (1983). Linguistic and conversational adjustments to nonnative speakers. Studies in Sec-ond Language Acquisition, 5, 177193.

    Long, M. H. (1985). Input and second language acquisition theory. In S. Gass & C. Madden (Eds.),Input in second language acquisition (pp. 377393). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W. C.Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 413468). San Diego,CA: Academic Press.

    Long, M., Inagaki, S., & Ortega, L. (1998). The role of implicit negative feedback in SLA: Models andrecasts in Japanese and Spanish. The Modern Language Journal, 82, 357371.

    Long, M., & Robinson, P. (1998). Focus on form: Theory, research, and practice. In C. Doughty & J.Williams (Eds.), Focus on form in classroom second language acquisition (pp. 1541). New York:Cambridge University Press.

    Long, M., & Sato, C. (1983). Classroom foreigner talk discourse: Forms and functions of teachersquestions. In H. Seliger & M. Long (Eds.), Classroom-oriented research in second language acquisi-tion (pp. 268285). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Lyster, R. (1998). Recasts, repetition, and ambiguity in L2 classroom discourse. Studies in SecondLanguage Acquisition, 20, 5181.

    Lyster, R., & Ranta, L. (1997). Corrective feedback and learner uptake: Negotiation of form in commu-nicative classrooms. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 3766.

    Mackey, A. (1995). Stepping up the pace: Input, interaction, and interlanguage development: An empiri-cal study of questions in ESL. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Sydney, Australia.

    Mackey, A. (1999). Input, interaction, and second language development: An empirical study of ques-tion formation in ESL. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 21, 557587.

    Mackey, A., Gass, S., & McDonough, K. (2000). How do learners perceive interactional feedback? Stud-ies in Second Language Acquisition, 22, 471498.

    Mackey, A., & Philp, J. (1998). Conversational interaction and second language development: Recasts,responses, and red herrings? The Modern Language Journal, 82, 338356.

    Mackey, A., Philp, J., Fujii, A., Egi, T., & Tatsumi, T. (2002). Individual differences in working memory,noticing of interactional feedback, and L2 development. In P. Robinson & P. Skehan (Eds.), Indi-vidual differences in L2 learning(pp. 181208). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    McLaughlin, B. (1987). Theories of second language learning. London: Edward Arnold.Meisel, J., Clahsen, H., & Pienemann, M. (1981). On determining developmental stages in natural sec-

    ond language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 3, 109135.Newport, E., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. (1977). Mother, Id rather do it myself: Some effects and

    non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. Snow & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Lan-guage input and acquisition (pp. 109149). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Nicholas, H., Lightbown, P., & Spada, N. (2001). Recasts as feedback to language learners. LanguageLearning, 51, 719758.

    Norris, J., & Ortega, L. (2000). Effectiveness of L2 instruction: A research synthesis and meta-analysis.Language Learning, 50, 417528.

    Ohta, A. (2000). Rethinking recasts: A learner-centered examination of corrective feedback in the Jap-

  • 8/8/2019 Constraints on Noticing the Gap

    27/28

    Constraints on Noticing the Gap 125

    anese classroom. In J. K. Hall & L. Verplaeste (Eds.), The construction of second and foreign lan-guage learning through classroom interaction (pp. 4771). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Oliver, R. (1995). Negative feedback in child NS-NNS conversation. Studies in Second Language Acquisi-tion, 17, 459481.

    Philp, J. (1998). Interaction, noticing, and second language acquisition: An examination of learners notic-ing of recasts in task-based interaction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Tasma-nia, Australia.

    Pica, T. (1988). Interlanguage adjustments as an outcome of NS-NNS negotiated interaction. LanguageLearning, 38, 4573.

    Pica, T. (1992). The textual outcomes of native speakernon-native speaker negotiation: What dothey reveal about second language learning? In C. Kramsch & S. McConnell-Ginet (Eds.), Textand context: Cross-disciplinary perspectives on language study(pp. 198237). Lexington, MA: D.C.Heath & Co.

    Pica, T. (1994). Research on negotiation: What does it reveal about second-language learning condi-tions, processes, and outcomes? Language Learning, 44, 493527.

    Pica, T., Holliday, L., Lewis, N., & Morgenthaler, L. (1989). Comprehensible output as an outcome oflinguistic demands on the learner. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 11, 6390.

    Pienemann, M. (1984). Psycholinguistic constraints on the teachability of languages. Studies in SecondLanguage Acquisition, 6, 186214.

    Pienemann, M. (1989). Is language teachable? Psycholinguistic experiments and hypotheses. AppliedLinguistics, 10, 5279.

    Pienemann, M. (1999). Language processing and second language development: Processability theory.Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Pienemann, M., & Johnston, M. (1987). Factors influencing the development of language proficiency.In D. Nunan (Ed.), Applying second language acquisition research (pp. 45141). Adelaide, Austra-lia: National Curriculum Resource Centre, Adult Migrant Education Program.

    Pienemann, M., Johnston, M., & Brindley, G. (1988). Constructing an acquisition-based procedure forsecond language assessment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 10, 217243.

    Pienemann, M., & Mackey, A. (1993). An empirical study of childrens ESL development. In P. McKay(Ed.), ESL developmentLanguage and literacy in schools: Vol. 2. Documents on bandscale devel-opment and language acquisition (pp. 115259). Canberra: National Languages and Literacy Insti-tute of Australia and Commonwealth of Australia.

    Porter, P. (1986). How learners talk to each other: Input and interaction in task-centered discussions.In R. Day (Ed.), Talking to learn: Conversation in second language acquisition (pp. 200224). Row-ley, MA: Newbury House.

    Posner, M., & Peterson, S. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuro-

    science, 13, 2542.Robinson, P. (1995). Attention