cognitive biases 3. hw2 problem #1 here are two sequences of coin-flips. x is one side of the coin,...

67
Cognitive Biases 3

Upload: moshe-varney

Post on 13-Dec-2015

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Cognitive Biases 3

Page 2: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #1

Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin landed on side X, then side X again, then side O. 1. XXOXXXOXOXOOOXXOOXOO 2. XOXXOXOXOOXXOXOOXOXO Which of the two was generated randomly by me flipping a coin, and which is a non-random sequence that I made up? Make sure to tell me how you know the answer.

Page 3: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #1

Remember that if the sequence is truly random (how the coin lands the second time is independent of how it lands the first time), then the number of times where it lands the same as it did before should be about equal to the number of times where it lands different to what it landed before.

Page 4: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #1

Let’s color-code “landing the same as the previous toss as blue, and landing different as red. Then sequence #1 is:

XXOXXXOXOXOOOXXOOXOO

Here we have 8 same tosses and 11 different ones.

Page 5: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #1

For the second sequence we have:

XOXXOXOXOOXXOXOOXOXO

This is very different. Here there are only four same tosses and 15 different ones.

Page 6: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #1

I made up the second sequence. Because I wanted it to look random, I tended to put X’s after O’s and O’s after X’s. This made most of the tosses turn out different– but that’s not what we should expect to happen.

Page 7: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #2

If I flip a fair coin and ten times in a row it lands heads, which of the following should I most likely expect for the next ten flips? Circle one: A. It will “regress to the mean” and land tails more than heads to balance things out. B. Since it’s a fair coin, it is most likely to land heads half the times and tails the other half.

Page 8: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #2

The correct answer is B:

B. Since it’s a fair coin, it is most likely to land heads half the times and tails the other half.

Future tosses of a coin are independent of past ones. Fair coins land, on average, 50% heads and 50% tails.

Page 9: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #2

Answer A is a trick answer, for several reasons:

A. It will “regress to the mean” and land tails more than heads to balance things out.

Page 10: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #2

Regression to the mean happens when we have two imperfectly correlated variables X and Y, and X takes on a very extreme value. Then we expect Y to take on a less extreme value.

But coin flips are not imperfectly correlated. They are not correlated at all. Past coin flips do not influence future coin flips.

Page 11: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #2

Answer A also contains a misunderstanding about regression.

It suggests that if the variable “past coin flips” takes on one extreme value (“lots of heads”) we should expect “future coin flips” to take on an extreme value in the opposite direction (“lots of tails”).

Page 12: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #2

But this is not how regression works. Even if past and future coin flips are correlated, what we expect is that extreme values of one variable are paired with more average, normal, or “mean” values of the other variable.

It’s regression to the mean, not regression to the opposite extreme!

Page 13: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #3

Suppose that the government conducts a survey of traffic intersections to find out which had the most accidents in the past month. At every intersection where there was a large number of accidents, the government installs cameras. Next month they do a survey again and notice than on average there are fewer accidents at the locations where cameras are installed.

Page 14: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #3

The government claims: Claim: Installing the cameras reduced the number of accidents. Suppose that this claim is false. Which cognitive bias/ fallacy (that we learned about in class) might be responsible for the government falsely believing the claim, in light of the evidence they have about the reduced number of accidents?

Page 15: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #3

The answer I am looking for is the regression fallacy.

The variables “traffic accidents this month” and “traffic accidents next month” are imperfectly correlated. Intersections with lots of accidents this month will likely have lots next month; intersections with few this month will likely have few next month.

Page 16: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #3

If you have two imperfectly correlated variables, and one of them (“traffic accidents this month”) takes on an extreme value, the other will have a more moderate value. So the intersections that were the worst this month will be, on average, bad-but-less-bad next month. The will, on average, improve– simply through regression to the mean.

Page 17: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #3

The regression fallacy is attributing a cause (the cameras the government installed) to an effect (the decrease in accidents at the intersections that had the most accidents last month) that is really just regression to the mean.

Page 18: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #3

None of this means the cameras didn’t work!

To truly test whether cameras work, you must install them at a random sampling of intersections– good and bad ones. If accidents then go down on average, you can be confident the cameras worked. We’ll talk more about random samples later in the course.

Page 19: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #4

Suppose that I present you with four cards. On each card there is a number on one side and a letter on the other. I claim: Claim: If a card has a vowel on one side then it has an even number on the other side. Which of the four cards do you need to turn over to tell whether this claim is true?

Page 20: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #4

• Card #1: 6

• Card #2: 7

• Card #3: E

• Card #4: F

Page 21: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #4

Card #1, 6, doesn’t matter. If you turn it over and it has a vowel, then the claim could be true. But it could also be false– maybe some other card has a vowel and an odd number on it.If you turn it over and it does not have a vowel, then the claim again can be either true or false. Card #1 gives you no information.

Page 22: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #4

Card #4, F, also gives you no information.

The claim is about what cards with vowels on one side have on the other side. F is not a vowel, so the claim is not about cards with F’s on one side.

Page 23: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #4

Card #2, 7, is important.

7 is an odd number. If there is a vowel on the other side of card #2, then the claim is false, because #2 is a card with a vowel on one side but no even number on the other side.

You must turn over #2 and make sure there is not a vowel on the other side.

Page 24: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #4

Card #3, E, is also important.

E is a vowel. If the claim is true, this card must have an even number on the other side. If it has an odd number on the other side, then the claim is false.

You must turn over #3 and make sure there is not an odd number on the other side.

Page 25: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #5

Consider the following contingency table. I claim that I can predict which students will get an A in my course (Prediction = Yes = student will get an A) and which will not get an A (Prediction = No = student will not get an A).

Page 26: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #5Observation = Yes Observation = No

Prediction = Yes 37 12

Prediction = No 52 24

Page 27: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #5

So I got 37 out of 49 (37 + 12) of my “yes” predictions correct, for a rate of 75.5%.

Additionally, I got 24 out of 76 (24 + 52) of my “no” predictions correct, for a rate of 31.6%

Altogether, I got 61 out of 125 guesses correct, for a total of 48.8%.

Page 28: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #5

Am I good predictor of which students will get an A or not?

This is a hard question. Notice that I don’t do all that much better on guessing who gets an A than if I predicted that everyone would get an A. Then the chart would look like this:

Page 29: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

89/125 = 71.2%Observation = Yes Observation = No

Prediction = Yes 89 36

Prediction = No 0 0

Page 30: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

HW2 Problem #5

I’ll accept any answer for this question, if you gave me some reasons for your answer, and those reasons aren’t crazy.

Page 31: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

SEEING WHAT WE EXPECT TO SEE

Page 32: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Bias

Our expectations often influence how we evaluate claims and evidence.

We easily accept as true those things that we expect to be true, but are much more skeptical about things that are unexpected.

Page 33: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Bias

Bias can be a good thing. If someone tells you they saw a construction worker, it makes sense to believe them– construction workers are numerous, and we expect there to be numerous sightings of them.

But if someone tells you they saw an extraterrestrial, things are different. You’ll be right to be skeptical: that is very unexpected.

Page 34: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Bias

Bias can also be a bad thing. If you’re biased against black people (“blacks tend toward criminal behavior,” you think), then you might be more likely to accept a negative statement about someone who is black, and more skeptical of believing positive things about them– even if they’re totally innocent, wonderful human beings.

Page 35: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Loftus & Palmer 1974

How things are described to us can affect how we see them.

In one study, subjects were shown pictures of a car accident involving multiple cars. They were asked:

“About how fast were the cars going when they (hit/smashed/collided/bumped/contacted ) each other?”

Page 36: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Loftus & Palmer 1974

Page 37: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Loftus & Palmer 1974

Additionally, the subjects were asked one week later whether they remembered seeing broken glass (from the cars) in the photographs.

There was no glass, but subjects who had been asked “smash” or “collided” questions were more likely to remember some than subjects asked “contacted” or “hit” questions.

Page 38: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Context Affects Expectation

This study shows that context (how a picture is described to us) can affect how we see a thing (the picture itself), and what we remember about it.

Page 39: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Studies have shown that people in many cultures have negative associations with the color black. They are biased against black-colored things.

Page 40: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Frank & Gilovich 1988

One study asked professional referees (for American football) to watch a video clip of a play and decide whether the players deserved a penalty.

In one version of the clip, the players wore white; in another, their uniforms were changed to black.

Page 41: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Frank & Gilovich 1988

The referees were more likely to say that the players deserved a penalty if they were wearing black.

Frank & Gilovich also found that teams with black uniforms actually did get penalized more often than teams with other colored uniforms!

Page 42: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Disconfirmation Bias

Our biases can lead us to accept evidence that agrees with our views and reject evidence against our views, even when the “for” and “against” evidence is of the same quality.

Page 43: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Lord, Ross & Lepper 1979

One study looked at how people who were in favor of the death penalty evaluated arguments for and against it, and how people who were against the death penalty evaluated those same arguments.

There were four types of arguments:

Page 44: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Lord, Ross & Lepper 1979

AGAINST-SAME. A study that showed that murder rates in a state increased after that state instituted the death penalty.

AGAINST-DIFF. A study that showed that murder rates were higher in states that had the death penalty than in states that didn’t.

Page 45: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Lord, Ross & Lepper 1979

FOR-SAME. A study that showed that murder rates in a state decreased after that state instituted the death penalty.

FOR-DIFF. A study that showed that murder rates were lower in states that had the death penalty than in states that didn’t.

Page 46: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Lord, Ross & Lepper 1979

All the subjects got one study AGAINST the death penalty and one study FOR it. If the study they got AGAINST it was the SAME condition, then they got FOR-DIFF; if the study they got AGAINST it was DIFF, then they got FOR-SAME.

Page 47: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Lord, Ross & Lepper 1979

People who liked the death penalty and received AGAINST-SAME and FOR-DIFF argued that SAME studies were bad, and DIFF ones were good. They liked the study that supported them.

If they got AGAINST-DIFF and FOR-SAME, they argued the opposite: that DIFF studies were bad, and that SAME studies were good.

Page 48: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Lord, Ross & Lepper 1979

The same was true for people who opposed the death penalty: they liked SAME studies when they got AGAINST-SAME, but not when they got FOR-SAME; they liked DIFF studies when they got AGAINST-DIFF, but not when they got FOR-DIFF.

Everyone liked the studies that agreed with them!

Page 49: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Lord, Ross & Lepper 1979

What’s interesting is that the arguments given by the subjects about why DIFF studies are bad (or why SAME studies are bad) were good arguments.

No one was arguing in bad faith. But their biases made them see the flaws in studies that disagreed with them, and made them ignore the flaws in the studies that agreed with them.

Page 50: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Disconfirmation Bias

This is sometimes called disconfirmation bias.

It is the tendency to subject evidence against your views to a greater degree of scrutiny than evidence in favor of your views.

It is a double-standard for evidence evaluation.

Page 51: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Gamblers

A similar study was seen in a study of gamblers (in particular, ones who bet on sports matches).

Why do gamblers keep gambling when they lose money so often? Don’t they ever learn from their mistakes? Are they just forgetting about all those times they lost?

Page 52: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Gamblers

The study showed that gamblers didn’t forget their losses– they were more likely to remember them.

What they did was explain away the times they lost. Losses were counted as accidents, bad luck, “near wins”, etc. They weren’t counted as bad gambles or mistakes.

Page 53: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Gamblers

Wins however were never attributed to accident or luck. They were the result of skill.

So wins and losses were treated differently. A win was taken as evidence that the gambler was good at choosing bets. But losses were not taken as evidence that the gambler was bad at choosing bets, just that he was “unlucky”.

Page 54: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Multiple Endpoints

One way that our expectations can influence our beliefs is called “the problem of multiple endpoints.”

Page 55: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Multiple Endpoints

For example, suppose someone claims that spending time on Facebook is bad for your social life.Well what does that mean? There are lots of effects of Facebook that might be counted as bad for your social life (spending less time face-to-face with friends) and lots of ways that might be counted as good (talking more often with a larger range of friends).

Page 56: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Multiple Endpoints

Here, if I’m biased because I dislike Facebook, I can interpret “bad for your social life” in a way that Facebook is bad; if I like Facebook, I can interpret it in a way that is good.

There are multiple different events (“endpoints”) that I can focus on when evaluating the claim.

Page 57: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Multiple Endpoints

People often say to parents, “your child looks so much like you!” even if (unknown to them) the child is adopted.There are lots of ways that any two people can look alike (hair, eyes, mouth, nose…). Most people will be similar in some ways. If you expect that two people will look similar (you think they’re biologically related), you will focus on those “endpoints”.

Page 58: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Two-Sided Events

Some events are “one-sided” and some events are “two-sided”.

A “two-sided” event is an event that can turn out one of two ways. For example, if I bet on a football match, I can either win or I can lose. Winning is one “side” of the event and losing is the other “side”.

Page 59: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

One-Sided Events

“One-sided” events are different. They can either (i) turn out in exactly one way or (ii) not exist.

Page 60: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

One-Sided Events

For example, suppose I believe that the phone always rings when I’m in the shower.

If I’m in the shower and the phone rings, that seems like an event– something happened: I was in the shower, and the phone rang.

Page 61: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

One-Sided Events

For example, suppose I believe that the phone always rings when I’m in the shower.

But if I’m in the shower and the phone doesn’t ring, it seems like nothing happened. I don’t say “look what happened when I was in the shower– the phone didn’t ring!”

Page 62: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Two-Sided Events & Memory

For two-sided events, we notice both outcomes equally. So when evaluating claims about them, like “I always win when I bet on a match,” we will be confronted with the negative evidence (all those times I bet on a match and lost).

Page 63: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

For one-sided events, we only notice when they happen, not when they don’t happen. So it may seem to me that my phone rings frequently when I’m in the shower, because I never notice the many, many times it does not ring when I’m in the shower.

Page 64: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Confirmations and Non-confirmations

Sometimes events are “one-sided” because we are never inclined to notice the other side.

Only when the event confirms a certain belief do we notice its relevance to the belief; if it doesn’t confirm the belief, then we’re uninclined to notice its relevance.

Page 65: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Confirmations and Non-confirmations

Suppose a fortune teller predicts that I will have twins. If many years later, I have twins, I may remember the prediction. I have evidence that fortune tellers are accurate. If instead I have only one child, I’m less likely to remember the prophecy. One child doesn’t remind me of a prophecy about two children.

Page 66: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

Open-Ended Claims

Sometimes events are “one-sided” because the claims they are relevant to are open-ended.

For example, suppose I predict that you will someday break a bone. If you break a bone tomorrow, next week, next month, next year, 30 years from now, etc. then the relevant event happened. I was right.

Page 67: Cognitive Biases 3. HW2 Problem #1 Here are two sequences of coin-flips. X is one side of the coin, O is the other. XXO, for example, means that the coin

But if you don’t break a bone tomorrow, nothing happened. If you don’t break it next week, nothing happened. If you don’t break it next month, nothing happened.

None of this shows that I’m wrong. Only if you died without breaking a bone would I be wrong. And you wouldn’t be around to notice that.