basic visual science core · 2018. 10. 3. · basic visual science core absolute and increment...

63
BASIC VISUAL SCIENCE CORE Absolute and Increment Thresholds Ronald S. Harwerth Fall, 2018

Upload: others

Post on 30-Jan-2021

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • BASIC VISUAL SCIENCE CORE

    Absolute and Increment Thresholds

    Ronald S. Harwerth

    Fall, 2018

  • 1. Psychophysics of Vision

    2. Light and Dark Adaptation

    Michael Kalloniatis and Charles Luu

  • The Neuron Doctrine for Perception

    • The neural basis for

    behavior - there should be a

    correlation between

    psychophysically

    determined functions and

    neurophysiology.

    • An early example - Hartline

    proposed that lateral

    inhibition, similar to that

    measured in a Limulus eye,

    could account for Mach

    bands.

    Mach Bands

  • Mach Bands

  • Limulus Eyes

    • The lateral eyes of Limulus

    (Horseshoe crab) are faceted.

    • The visual receptors (ommatidia) are

    connected by lateral neurons.

    • The lateral interactions between

    ommatidia are inhibitory.

  • A: Limulus lateral eye. Dark spots in

    photograph are individual ommatidia.

    Width of eye ∼1 cm.

    B: Schematic drawing of an ommatidium

    in the lateral eye. l, lens; a, aperture; b,

    rhabdomere; r, retinular cell; p,

    pigment cell; e, eccentric cell; i,

    synaptic sites of self and lateral

    inhibition; x, site of spike generation.

    Diameter of ommatidium ∼250 μm.

    C: Functional diagram of optical and

    neural mechanisms operating in the

    lateral eye that transform visual

    scenes into patterns of optic nerve

    activity, or neural images.

    Limulus Eyes

    Passaglia, et al., J Neuro Physiol 1998;80:1800-1815.

    http://www.google.com/url?sa=i&source=images&cd=&cad=rja&docid=hctN_L7CoGnXnM&tbnid=bVgymsiRdi3gfM:&ved=0CAgQjRwwAA&url=http://jn.physiology.org/content/80/4/1800/F1.expansion&ei=DRJwUvbhIaWF2gXsn4FA&psig=AFQjCNH8xggObb5HIBrC67r5OsZRLjbJpw&ust=1383162765585690http://www.google.com/url?sa=i&source=images&cd=&cad=rja&docid=hctN_L7CoGnXnM&tbnid=bVgymsiRdi3gfM:&ved=0CAgQjRwwAA&url=http://jn.physiology.org/content/80/4/1800/F1.expansion&ei=DRJwUvbhIaWF2gXsn4FA&psig=AFQjCNH8xggObb5HIBrC67r5OsZRLjbJpw&ust=1383162765585690

  • Lateral Inhibition in Limulus Eyes

    Log intensity falling

    on inhibiting spot

    Distance between inhibiting

    and inhibited facets

    De

    cre

    as

    e in

    fre

    qu

    en

    cy

    (im

    pu

    lse

    s p

    er

    se

    c)

    • Lateral inhibition in the Limulus eye is directly proportional

    to the intensity of the inhibiting stimulus and inversely

    proportional to the distance of the inhibiting ommatidium.

  • Lateral Inhibition and Mach Bands

    • Based on the

    neurophysiology of the

    Limulus eye, the lateral

    inhibition from receptors to

    the left of the green spot will

    be greater than the inhibition

    from the receptors to the

    right.

    • The lower figure shows

    recordings from an

    ommatidium as an edge

    moves across the eye with

    either, all other ommatida

    occluded (triangles) or

    illuminated (circles).

  • • The activity of a single nerve cell and influences

    of other cells is a complete enough description

    for understanding the nervous system.

    • The sensory system is organized to achieve as

    complete representation as possible with the

    minimum number of active neurons.

    • The frequency of neural impulses codes

    certainty: a high impulse frequency corresponds

    to a high degree of certainty that the cause of

    the percept is present.

    The Neuron Doctrine for Perception:

    Barlow’s Dogma

    Barlow, HB. Perception 1972;1:371 – 394.

  • Definitions:

    Absolute and Increment Thresholds

    Response continuum

    Stimulus continuum

    R0

    S0

    • Absolute threshold - the lowest energy level (S0 ) that can be

    perceived (R0).

    • Increment threshold (differential threshold) - the lowest

    amount of energy which must be added to an existing

    stimulus intensity in order to see a change in the stimulus.

    • Increment thresholds often follow the predictions of Weber’s

    law (I / I = K) or Fechner’s law (R = K * log(S)).

    R1

    S1

    R2

    S2

    I I

  • Absolute and Increment Thresholds

    Weber’s Law

    • I / I = K

    • Log transformation

    log(I) = log(I) + log(K)

    • Weber’s law predicts a

    linear relationship between

    the log threshold intensity

    and log background

    intensity with a slope = 1

    and y-intercept = K.

    Log background intensity

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    in

    ten

    sit

    y

    Background

    Test

    field

  • Absolute and Increment Thresholds

    Fechner’s law• R = K * log(S)

    • Fechner’s law predicts a

    linear relationship between

    the response magnitude and

    log background intensity

    with a slope = K and y-

    intercept = 0.

    Log background intensity

    Resp

    on

    se m

    ag

    nit

    ud

    e

    Background

    Test

    field

  • Absolute and Increment Thresholds

    Log background intensity

    I0

    The form of empirical data

    follows the form:

    I = (IB2 + ID

    2) 0.5

    where

    I = increment threshold

    IB = background intensity

    ID = intrinsic noise

    IB

    ID

  • The Classic Experiments of

    Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    • Experiments designed to determine the absolute

    minimum intensity of light needed for vision.

    • The results provided important data to answer two

    basic questions.

    1. Is there an absolute threshold for seeing?

    2. What is the minimum number of neural

    impulses required for seeing?

    Hecht, Shlaer, & Pirenne, J Gen Physiol. 1942;25:819–840.

  • The Classic Experiments of

    Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    1. Is there an absolute threshold for seeing?

    • The psychometric or

    “frequency-of-seeing”

    function is an ogive or “S-

    shaped” curve (black line),

    not a step function (green

    line) that would be predicted

    for a system with and

    absolute threshold.

    • The shape of the function

    indicates that there is noise

    in the measurement.

  • The Classic Experiments of

    Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    2. What is the minimum number of neural impulses

    required for seeing?

    • Light has a quantum nature

    (E = h).

    where:

    E = energy of a quantum

    h = Plank’s constant

    = the frequency of light

    • The number of quanta at

    threshold = the number of

    rhodopsin isomerizations =

    the number of neural

    impluses generated.

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • There are three peaks in the

    absorption spectrum of

    rhodopsin.

    • The action of light on

    rhodopsin causes a shift in

    the absorption peak in visible

    light (. peak).

    • The absorption spectrum also

    represents the probability of

    absorption of a quantum of

    light.

    1. The wavelength of the test stimulus (510 nm).

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • After correction for pre-retinal

    light losses, the spectral

    sensitivity of the dark adapted

    eye matches the absorption

    spectrum of rhodopsin.

    • The optimal wavelength - the

    highest probability for

    absorption of quanta - will be

    close to the peak absorption

    of rhodopsin at 505 nm.

    1. The wavelength of the test stimulus (510 nm).

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • The density of cones

    and rods varies

    across the retina.

    • The optimal stimulus

    location will be the

    eccentricity with the

    highest density of

    rod photoreceptors -

    about 20 deg, on

    temporal retinal to

    avoid the optic nerve

    head.

    2. The retinal area for the test stimulus (20 deg, temporal retina).

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • Diagram of fixation and test

    field.

    2. The retinal area for the test stimulus (20 deg, temporal retina).

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • The visual threshold

    decreases with time in the

    dark (dark adaptation).

    • The absolute threshold cannot

    be measured until the time of

    the final rod threshold.

    3. The state of adaptation (30 - 40 min of dark adaptation).

    Cone phase

    Rod phase

    Rod - cone break

    Final rod

    threshold

    Time in the dark (min)

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • The visual threshold

    decreases with time in the

    dark (dark adaptation).

    • The time of the rod-cone

    break and the final rod

    threshold depend on the

    amount of light adaptation

    prior to dark adaptation.

    • The final rod threshold will

    occur after 30 - 40 min of

    dark adaptation, even after

    intense rhodopsin bleaching.

    3. The state of adaptation (30 - 40 min of dark adaptation).

    Time in the dark (min)

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • Bloch’s law (I * t) = k for t < tc,

    I = k for t > tc, where

    I = threshold intensity

    t = time

    k = constant

    • For stimulus duration of less

    than 80 - 100 mesc, threshold

    is determined by total energy

    (I * t).

    • For stimulus duration greater

    than a critical duration (tc),

    threshold is determined by

    stimulus intensity (I = k).

    4. The duration of the stimulus (1 msec).

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    in

    ten

    sit

    y

    Log stimulus duration (msec)

    -1 0 1 2

    tc

    (I * t) = k

    I = k

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • Bloch’s law (I * t) = k for t < tc, I

    = k for t > tc, where

    I = threshold intensity

    t = time

    k = constant

    • The plotted data show the

    product I * t as a function of t

    for central and peripheral

    stimuli, to illustrate the

    constant integration across

    the retina.

    4. The duration of the stimulus (Bloch’s law).

    Log time

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • Ricco’s law (I * a) = k for a < ac,

    I = k for a > ac, where

    I = threshold intensity

    a = area

    k = constant

    • The ac depends on the retinal

    eccentricity - at an eccentricity

    of 20 deg the reciprocity for

    area and intensity holds for

    stimulus diameters up to

    about 1 deg.

    • For foveal vision, ac is about 5

    min diameter.

    5. The size of the stimulus (10 min diameter).

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    in

    ten

    sit

    y

    Log stimulus diameter (deg)

    -2 -1 0

    ac

    (I * a) = k

    I = k

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    • Ricco’s law (I * a) = k for a < ac,

    I = k for a > ac, where

    I = threshold intensity

    a = area

    k = constant

    • The psychophysical

    thresholds as a function of the

    diameter of the stimuli for

    foveal and peripheral stimuli

    illustrate the differences in

    integration of central and

    peripheral retina.

    5. The size of the stimulus (Ricco’s law).

    Log radius

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The critical parameters for empirical measurements of

    absolute thresholds:

    6. The psychophysical method for determining

    thresholds.

    1. The method-of-adjustment - The subject adjusts the

    intensity of the stimulus to a “just-seen” (or “not

    seen”) level.

    2. The method-of-limits - Stimuli intensities are decreased

    (or increased) in discreet steps to determine the

    threshold.

    3. The method-of-constant stimuli - Stimuli of fixed

    intensities are presented in random order to determine

    the frequency-of-seeing for each intensity level.

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The results: Method-of-limits measurements

    • For the three observers, 54 -148 quanta, at the cornea, were

    necessary for seeing. How many quanta were absorbed by

    rhodopsin?

    • Approximate values for the major losses of light

    1. The cornea reflects 4% of the light.

    2. The crystalline lens absorbs 50% of light at 505 nm.

    3. 80% of the light passes between the rod receptors.

    • The threshold intensities represent 5 - 14 quanta absorbed

    by the photopigments.

    • Conclusion - the threshold for seeing is determined by a

    small number of rods, each absorbing a single quantum,

    within a certain area and time.

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The results: The absolute threshold

    1. A single quantum is sufficient to activate a rod.

    • At the 20 deg eccentricity, the 10 min diameter test stimulus

    covered approximately 500 rods, therefore, the probability

    of “double hits” for a single receptor is extremely small.

    2. A single quantum is insufficient for seeing.

    • A threshold of a single quantum is incompatible with

    Ricco’s law or probability summation.

    • At the retinal area of stimulation, Ricco’s law predicts

    pooling of signals from over 17,000 rods.

    3. If there is an absolute threshold for seeing, why is there

    a finite slope for psychometric functions?

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The results: Method-of-constant stimuli

    1. Further evidence for an absolute threshold was derived

    by modeling their psychometric functions.

    Log average number of quanta per flash-1 0 1

    • The Poission distribution

    pn = an / ea n!

    • The Poission probability

    distribution is a binomial

    distribution that is valid for

    a situation where there is a

    large number of possible

    events, but a very few

    actually occur.

    n = 1

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The results: Method-of-constant stimuli

    1. The family of Poission probability distributions represent templates to describe the psychometric function for each

    value of n.

    2. The shape of the function

    will be constant,

    regardless of its location

    on the abscissa because of

    the logarithmic coordinate.

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The results: Method-of-constant stimuli

    1. The templates for 5 - 7 quanta matched the psychometric

    functions for the three investigators.

    2. The model is compatible with an absolute threshold.

    For example, Subject S.H. reported “seen” any time 6,

    or more, quanta were absorbed and never said “seen” if

    less than 6 quanta were absorbed.

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    The results: Method-of-constant stimuli

    3. If the quantum efficiency is 1.0, then S.H. saw the light

    every time that 6 neural impulses were generated.

    4. If there is an absolute threshold, then the noise in the

    threshold measure must arise from non-biological sources,

    probably the variability of the stimulus.

  • Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne

    Further considerations: Is there an absolute threshold?

    1. The experiments did not consider criterion effects on

    threshold measurements. For the data shown, the subjects

    all used a very conservative criterion for saying “seen”

    because the false alarm rate for each psychometric

    function is zero.

  • Is there an absolute threshold?

    1. In Sakitt’s experiments

    (“Counting Every

    Quantum”), the subjects

    were trained to scale the

    apparent brightness of test

    stimuli seen at near

    threshold intensities.

    2. She found that some

    subjects could reliably

    detect a single quantum

    and discriminate between

    intensity increments of

    one quantum - there was

    no absolute threshold.

    Sakitt, B. J Physiol. 1972;233:131-150

  • Is there an absolute threshold?

    1. In more recent

    experiments, Teich, et al.,

    investigated the effect of

    criterion on the absolute

    threshold.

    2. They confirmed the

    results of HS&P if they

    instructed their subjects

    to use a strict criterion for

    reporting “seen” (< 5%

    false alarm rate).

    Teich, MC. et al., J Opt Soc Am. 1982;72:419-431

  • Is there an absolute threshold?

    3. With a more lax response

    criterion, the subject’s

    false alarm rates were

    elevated, but the

    psychometric functions

    still demonstrated a

    systematic relationship to

    the intensity of the

    stimulus.

    4. An absolute threshold

    (60% correct) of 1 quantum

    is possible, if a false alarm

    rate of 55% is acceptable.

    Teich, MC. et al., J Opt Soc Am. 1982;72:419-431

  • Frequency of seeing curves for subject using a lenient criterion (diamonds), to progressively more strict criteria (squares),

    (triangles), and (circles).

    After adjusting for the non-zero false positive rate 1the curves for the two most lenient criteria, collapse onto each other, indicating

    no net benefit to detection.

    Solid curves are nonlinear least-squares Weibull fits to the frequency of seeing data.

    The Absolute Threshold for Cone Vision

    Koeing, D & Hofer, H. Jvis. 2011, 11

    https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3671617/#FD1

  • Is there an absolute threshold for

    photopic vision?

    1. The photopic system is most

    often studied under light

    adapted conditions because it

    is a high resolution, color

    system - not ordinarily

    operating in a quantum limited

    environment.

    2. An absolute threshold with dark

    adaptation has been measured.

    Under optimal conditions

    (small, short, foveal test target),

    the absolute threshold is about

    1000 quanta - statistically

    compatible with 5 - 7 quanta

    absorptions per cone, with

    pooling over about 40 cones.

  • Is there an absolute threshold for

    photopic vision?

    1. The more recent analyses of

    the slopes of psychometric

    functions and spatial

    summation functions under

    dark and light adaptation

    have provided evidence

    against a multiple hit

    hypothesis.

    2. The higher threshold for

    photopic vision may be

    caused by higher intrinsic

    noise in cones than rods.

    cones rods

    Donner, K. Vision Res. 1992;32:853-866

  • Brightness Discrimination

    (increment-threshold spectral sensitivity)

    Adaptation field

    Test field

    Methods:

    1. White or chromatic adaptation

    field (background) to obtain static

    photopic state.

    2. Threshold - the smallest

    perceptible light increment (I of

    the monochromatic test field

    added to the adapting

    background (I).

    3. Sensitivity = log (1 / threshold

    intensity).

    Distance

    Inte

    nsit

    y

    I

    I

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function.

    1. The increment threshold as a

    function of background

    intensity is commonly known

    as a tvi (threshold-versus-

    intensity) function.

    2. The three independent

    variables of tvi functions are

    • the absolute threshold (a)

    • the intrinsic noise (Id)

    • the slope of the adaptation

    portion of the function (b)

    (It)

    (Ib)

    The tvi relationship is

    It = a (Ib + Id) b

    b

    Ida

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function.

    1. A common paradigm for

    increment thresholds is to

    determine the least perceptible

    amount of light that must be

    added to a stationary

    background as a function of the

    intensity of the background.

    Background field

    500 nm

    Test field

    580 nm

    Log background intensity

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    in

    ten

    sit

    y

    10 deg parafoveal

    function -

    500 nm background

    580 nm test

    Stiles, WS. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA., 1959;45:100-114.

  • Subjective methods of defining

    the cone fundamentals

    • Stiles’ two-color, increment-threshold

    spectral sensitivity.

    • Assumptions:

    1. In any area of the retina there are

    several different photopigments, but

    the one with highest sensitivity will

    determine the visual threshold.

    2. Photopigments that differ in their

    spectral locations will adapt at

    different rates for a given wavelength

    of the background.

    3. Cone mechanisms can be identified

    by “threshold-versus-intensity” (TVI)

    functions.

    Background Intensity

  • Stiles’ TVI functions

    • With the correct choice of test

    field and background

    wavelengths, the color vision

    mechanisms can be isolated

    and their spectral sensitivities

    investigated.

    • The example shows two

    independent color vision

    mechanisms, one more

    sensitive to the 480 nm test

    field at absolute threshold and

    which adapts more rapidly to

    the 540 nm background field.

  • Stiles’ TVI functions

    • Measurements of TVI

    functions across

    wavelengths provides

    data to derive the

    spectra response

    function for each

    mechanism.

    • Initially, Stiles called

    the functions cone

    photopigments, but

    later called them the “pi

    mechanisms.”

  • Stiles’ pi mechanisms

    • Stiles presented data for 5

    independent photopic mechanisms

    and 1 scotopic mechanism.

    0 - rod photopigment

    1 - SW mechanism

    2 - SW & MW mechanism

    3 - SW mechanism

    4 - MW mechanism

    5 - LW mechanism

  • Human Cone Spectral Sensitivities

    • Cone spectral sensitivities based on measurements in

    normal trichromats, dichromats, and monochromats.

  • Cone Sensitivities by Objective Methods

    Wavelength (nm)

    • The spectral sensitivities of

    the three cone types in the

    primate retina.

    • The sensitivities were

    determined over a 6 log unit

    range.

    • The MW and LW cone

    sensitivities overlap across

    the entire visible spectrum,

    but the SW cone sensitivity is

    more separated (max’s at

    435, 535, & 570 nm).

    Wavenumber (cm-1)

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - rod saturation.

    Background field

    580 nm

    Test field

    500 nm

    Log background intensity

    (scotopic trolands)

    9 deg parafoveal

    function -

    580 nm background

    500 nm test

    1. Aguilar & Stiles - Why do the

    rods stop contributing to

    vision at photopic adaptation

    levels?

    2. The rod response saturates at

    about 100 scotopic trolands;

    cone responses do not

    saturate.

    Aguilar, M, & Stiles, WS. Acta Optica 1954;1:59-65

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    1. Westheimer functions - also called spatial interaction functions

    or perceptive fields - are psychophysical measures of the center

    (++) and surround (--) dimensions of retinal receptive fields.

    2. The measurement

    involves the increment

    threshold for a small test

    field (~ 5 arcmin) on a

    background of variable size,

    but with a constant

    intensity.

    + +

    - - -

    - - -

    Westheimer, G. J Physiol. 1967;190:139–154

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    1. The first measurement, without a background,

    establishes the threshold of the receptive field without

    interacting stimuli.

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    2. Subsequent measurements involve increment

    thresholds with a varying size background. When the

    background is smaller than the receptive field center,

    the increased noise causes an elevation of the

    increment threshold.

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

    •2

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    3. As measurements are made with larger backgrounds,

    as long as the background is smaller than the receptive

    field center, the elevation of the increment threshold is

    proportional to the size of the background field.

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

    2

    3

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    4. The highest increment threshold will occur when the

    size of the background matches the size of the receptive

    field center. The portion of the Westheimer function

    showing increasing threshold as a function of increasing

    field size is called the desensitization limb.

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

    •2

    3

    4 Desensitization

    limb

    Size of

    receptive

    field center

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    5. When the size of the background is larger than the

    receptive field center, the opponent activity of the

    surround reduces the noise induced by the center and,

    thus, results in a relatively lower threshold .

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

    •2

    3

    4

    5

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    6. The reduction in the increment threshold intensity

    continues with increasing field size, because the

    background stimulation of the surround is more effective

    in canceling the activity of the receptive field center .

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

    •2

    3

    4

    5

    6

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    7. When the size of the background matches the size of the

    surround of the receptive field, the field is maximally

    effective in canceling the activity of the center by the

    receptive field surround mechanism.

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

    •2

    3

    4

    5

    6 7

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    8. Background sizes larger than the total size of the

    receptive field do not cause changes in the increment

    threshold intensity. The portion of the Westheimer

    function showing decreasing threshold as a function of

    increasing field size is called the sensitization limb.

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

    •2

    3

    4

    5

    6 7 8

    Sensitization

    limb

    Total size of

    receptive field

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    The final result is a psychophysical measurement of the

    center and surround sizes of the receptive field sizes at the

    retinal location of the measurement.

    Diameter of background (deg)

    Lo

    g t

    hre

    sh

    old

    of

    test

    field

    • ••

    •• • •

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5

    6 7 8++

    - - -

    - - -

    center center + surround

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    • Examples of Westheimer functions, under photopic

    conditions, as a function of retinal eccentricity.

    • The sizes of the center and surround increase as a function of

    eccentricity, but a clear center-surround organization is

    apparent at all eccentricities.

  • Increment threshold studies of visual

    function - Westheimer functions.

    • Examples of Westheimer functions, under scotopic

    conditions, as a function of retinal eccentricity.

    • The size of the receptive field center increases as a function

    of eccentricity, but under scotopic viewing the receptive

    fields do not exhibit clearly the center-surround properties

    of photopic functions.

  • The Neuron Doctrine for Perception

    There should be a correlation between psychophysically determined functions and neurophysiology.

    There are systematic relationships between stimulus parameters (size, duration, wavelength) that are based on

    physiological properties.

    The work of Hecht, Shlaer and Pirenne demonstrates the power of combining empirical data and modelling.

    Studies of Westheimer functions have demonstrated relationships between the structure of receptive fields and

    measures of vision function by increment thresholds.