authors 1 2 1

36
Title: Reading comprehension difficulties in people with aphasia: Investigating personal perception of reading ability, practice and difficulties. Authors: Janet Webster 1 , Julie Morris 1 , Jenny Malone 2 and David Howard 1 Affiliation: 1 Speech and Language Sciences, Newcastle University, UK 2 Adult Speech and Language Therapy Department, Newcastle Upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust Short Title: Reading comprehension in aphasia Corresponding Author: Janet Webster Contact Details: Speech and Language Sciences, School of Education, Communication and Language Sciences, King George VI Building, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE1 7RU. Telephone: 0191 208 5235 Fax: 0191 208 6518 Email: [email protected]

Upload: others

Post on 21-Jan-2022

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Authors 1 2 1

Title: Reading comprehension difficulties in people with aphasia: Investigating personal perception

of reading ability, practice and difficulties.

Authors: Janet Webster1, Julie Morris1, Jenny Malone2 and David Howard1

Affiliation:

1 Speech and Language Sciences, Newcastle University, UK

2 Adult Speech and Language Therapy Department, Newcastle Upon Tyne Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust

Short Title: Reading comprehension in aphasia

Corresponding Author: Janet Webster

Contact Details: Speech and Language Sciences, School of Education, Communication and Language

Sciences, King George VI Building, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE1 7RU.

Telephone: 0191 208 5235 Fax: 0191 208 6518

Email: [email protected]

Page 2: Authors 1 2 1

Abstract Background: Whilst reading difficulties are a common feature of aphasia, there has been limited

consideration of the impact of these difficulties on how people with aphasia experience reading in

their everyday lives. Previous studies, using semi-structured interviews, have highlighted the need to

consider reading from the perspective of the individual due to the complexity of factors influencing

reading and individual variation in practices and preferences. These studies have contributed to our

understanding of the individual’s perspective but have been restricted to people with less severe

aphasia and some expressive ability. We need tools to facilitate understanding the personal

perspective of people across the severity range that are feasible within a clinical setting.

Aims: The aim of this study was to investigate people with aphasia’s perception of their current

reading ability and activities and feelings about reading using a questionnaire. The validity, reliability

and clinical usefulness and acceptability of the questionnaire were considered.

Methods & Procedures: The questionnaire was developed through an iterative process, involving a

review of literature, integration of findings from an interview study, direct consultation with people

with aphasia, pilot testing and review. The questionnaire comprised of four sections: A) Current

reading B) Thoughts and feelings about reading C) Reading activities and D) Reading strategies. 81

participants completed the questionnaire during a study of reading in people with aphasia. Following

completion of data collection, tester feedback was considered and the psychometric properties of

the questionnaire in relation to aspects of validity and reliability were analysed.

Outcomes & Results: Perception of reading ability, feelings about reading and reading activity varied

considerably across participants, with not all individuals reporting difficulties. There was increased

perceived difficulty as the length of text increased. Participants highlighted that they were unhappy

with their speed of reading. The most common strategies used to support reading were re-reading,

slowing down and reducing distractions. The iterative process resulted in a 29 item questionnaire

Page 3: Authors 1 2 1

which had appropriate face validity, construct validity and test re-test reliability and was clinically

appropriate and feasible.

Conclusions: The study provides additional evidence about the individual variability in reading in

people with aphasia, emphasising the importance of considering personal perception alongside

formal assessments of reading comprehension. The questionnaire provides a practical and reliable

measure of a person’s perception of reading post-stroke and has potential as a patient centred tool,

contributing to goal setting and outcome measurement.

Key words: reading, comprehension, aphasia, personal perception, alexia

Introduction Reading difficulties are a common feature of aphasia but there has been limited consideration of the

impact of these difficulties on how people with aphasia experience reading in their everyday lives.

This study explores personal perception of reading in people with aphasia using a questionnaire. The

rationale for, development of, and psychometric properties of the questionnaire are explored

alongside the results from people with aphasia. The work described here was part of wider study of

reading; this wider study included assessment of reading comprehension and reading aloud at single

word, sentence and paragraph level.

Reading Difficulties in People with Aphasia Reading difficulties are a prominent feature post-stroke, with an estimated prevalence of 68% of

people with aphasia (Brookshire, Wilson, Nadeau, Rothi, & Kendall, 2014) when criteria are based on

oral reading. With broader criteria (including reading comprehension and reported impact on

everyday reading), difficulties may be present in all people with aphasia (Webb & Love, 1983). Post-

stroke, people with aphasia report a decline in their reading ability with a range of difficulties:-

difficulties with decoding (oral reading), difficulties with reading comprehension (affecting single

words, sentences and/or connected text) and reduced reading efficiency or speed (Knollman-Porter,

Wallace, Hux, Brown, & Long, 2015; Webster, Samouelle, & Morris, 2018). Post-stroke, there may

Page 4: Authors 1 2 1

also be co-occurring difficulties with vision (e.g. hemianopia, scanning) (Aleligay, Worrall, & Rose,

2008; Hepworth et al., 2016; Rowe et al., 2011), attention and memory (Aleligay et al., 2008; Lee &

Sohlberg, 2013; Mayer & Murray, 2002) which can impact reading. Reading comprehension is an

everyday activity, with difficulties impacting a range of activities, e.g. reading a book, a newspaper,

a recipe, letters etc. and participation in a wide range of social, leisure and work activities (Knollman-

Porter et al., 2015; Parr, 1995). There is also an increasing reliance on technology to perform

traditional literacy tasks , e.g. reading news articles on-line, and people with aphasia may struggle

with technology-based communication due to their reduced ability to understand written messages

(Dietz, Ball, & Griffith, 2011), again impacting social participation. Reading is considered by people

with aphasia as ‘necessary for life’ (Kjellén, Laakso, & Henriksson, 2017, p10). Reading difficulties

have been reported to result in feelings of loss, frustration, disappointment, annoyance (Kjellén et

al., 2017; Webster et al., 2018), with slow and effortful reading. Despite this, individuals are still

motivated to read and want to improve their skills (Kjellén et al., 2017; Webster et al., 2018).

Literacy (reading and writing) enables participation in society and self-efficacy (Kjellén et al., 2017)

and reduced participation in essential and pleasurable activities may result in reduced quality of life

(Parr, 2007).

There is extensive normal variation in reading ability, the importance of reading and reading

preferences. Everyday reading activities cannot be predicted as they vary according to the social

roles that people fulfil, for example, role within family, employment, hobbies and interests (Parr,

1992). In addition, people do not carry out all of the literacy tasks that they are capable of, with

delegation of some tasks to other people (Parr, 1992). Within qualitative interview studies, the same

variation in reading practices and preferences has been identified in people with aphasia (both pre-

and post-stroke). People with aphasia have unique motivations for reading (Kjellén et al., 2017;

Knollman-Porter et al., 2015; Webster et al., 2018), read a unique range and type of materials

(Kjellén et al., 2017; Knollman-Porter et al., 2015; Parr, 1995) and use a unique set of strategies to

aid reading (Kjellén et al., 2017; Lynch, Damico, Abendroth, & Nelson, 2013). Post-stroke, reading

Page 5: Authors 1 2 1

activities may change as a direct consequence of the reading difficulties or due to wider changes in

roles or participation (Parr, 1995, Webster et al., 2018). Individuals may also delegate literacy tasks

(e.g. reading formal letters) to or seek support from other people more frequently than prior to their

stroke (Knollman-Porter et al., 2015; Webster et al., 2018). This underlines the complexity of

assessing an individual’s reading post-stroke and the need to understand reading difficulties from

the perspective of the person with aphasia.

Exploring Perceptions of Reading Previous studies which have considered reading from the perspective of people with aphasia (e.g.

Parr, 1995; Webster et al., 2018) have primarily used semi-structured interviews exploring both pre-

and post-morbid reading. In some studies (e.g. Kjellén et al., 2017; Parr, 1995), reading has been

considered alongside writing to give a comprehensive perspective on literacy. In Lynch et al., (2013),

interviews were accompanied by recording and analysis of oral and silent reading of texts and

observation in a naturalistic setting. Knollman-Porter et al. (2015) asked participants to complete a

written questionnaire which probed materials read, frequency of reading, time spent reading and

strategies which aided reading. This was then reviewed within the context of an interview, with

additional broad questions about post-stroke reading and looking at examples of pre- and post-

aphasia reading materials. The materials were used to promote discussion about reading

behaviours, preferences and experiences and gave the researcher the opportunity to observe

participants reading. In-depth interviews have contributed significantly to our understanding of how

individuals perceive their reading difficulties and the complexity and variety of those difficulties.

However, there are many challenges to carrying out interviews with people with aphasia who often

have co-occurring difficulties with spoken comprehension and production (Luck & Rose, 2007). It is

vital to understand reading from the perspective of people with aphasia across the severity range

and interview studies have often excluded people with more severe language difficulties. Interviews

are also very time consuming and may not be feasible clinically. Within intervention planning, given

the importance of exploring the person’s perspective, we need a clinically acceptable, efficient and

Page 6: Authors 1 2 1

replicable way to consider this and to monitor changes in perception resulting from recovery or

treatment.

Within the literature, there are two specific questionnaires about reading: the questionnaire

(described above) used in the Knollman-Porter et al. (2015) study and the Reading Confidence and

Emotions Questionnaire (RCEQ) described by Cocks and colleagues (Cocks et al., 2010; Cocks,

Pritchard, Cornish, Johnson, & Cruice, 2013). The RCEQ was designed for use with people who have

reading difficulties associated with cognitive impairments resulting from traumatic brain injury

although the authors suggest the questionnaire would also be applicable post-stroke. The RCEQ asks

the person to rate their confidence in reading aloud across a range of different contexts, in different

settings and with different people (comparing pre-morbid and post-morbid reading). People are

then asked to rate emotions they associate with reading e.g. pleasure, frustration, anger, worry etc.

In each case, a ten point rating scale is used. Both questionnaires place high demands on reading

comprehension although people had the option of having assistance from a family member

(Knollman-Porter et al., 2015) or support from the interviewer (Cocks et al., 2010; Cocks et al., 2013).

The structure of the RCEQ is potentially more accessible but it has a heavy focus on reading aloud

and it does not explore specific reading activities.

There are also a number of psychosocial measures that consider the broader impact of aphasia from

the perspective of the individual; examples include the disability questionnaire within the

Comprehensive Aphasia Test (CAT, Swinburn, Porter, & Howard, 2004), the Communication

Outcome after Stroke (COAST) scale (Long, Hesketh, Paszek, Booth, & Bowen, 2008), the Assessment

of Living with Aphasia (ALA, Simmons-Mackie et al., 2014). These measures have generally been

motivated by the International Classification of Disability and Functioning (ICF) framework (World

Health Organisation, 2002) and the need for a client centred and collaborative approach to goal

setting and intervention. The measures are designed to follow aphasia friendly principles (Rose,

Worrall, Hickson, & Hoffmann, 2011), with individuals having the opportunity to respond non-

verbally. The measures also report information about validity and reliability, allowing consideration

Page 7: Authors 1 2 1

of change over time. These tools are valuable in considering the impact of aphasia generally. As with

other aspects of aphasia, it is important to think about reading within the domains of the ICF

framework (i.e. perception of impairment, impact of impairment on activity and participation,

personal factors and potential barriers or facilitators in the environment). However, within the tools

currently available there are a limited number of questions that are relevant to reading and

questions about feelings capture the overall impact of aphasia, rather than specific feelings about

reading.

Summary The nature and impact of reading difficulties is likely to be complex and variable across individual

people with aphasia, with personal preferences and needs interacting with the nature of the

impairment. Assessment of reading comprehension is crucial in characterising reading difficulties.

However, given the interaction of impairment, perception and need, it is vital that this is combined

with an understanding of reading from the perspective of the individual. Questionnaires offer a way

of obtaining information in a consistent and structured way.

Aim

The aim of this study was to investigate people with aphasia’s perception of their current reading

ability and activities and feelings about reading using a questionnaire. This allowed perception to be

considered in a large group of people with aphasia across the severity range. The validity, reliability

and clinical usefulness and acceptability of the questionnaire were considered.

Method

The questionnaire was developed through an iterative process, involving a review of literature,

integration of findings from a previous interview study led by the primary authors, direct

consultation with people with aphasia, pilot testing and review. This process of development is

Page 8: Authors 1 2 1

described in detail as it provides the rationale for the development of the questionnaire, including

the views of people with aphasia and their performance on earlier pilot versions.

Development Phase

The initial version of the questionnaire was developed in line with the following guiding principles:

Reflect current evidence about reading difficulties in people with aphasia

Be aphasia friendly and appropriate to a clinical setting

Be accessible to people with reading difficulties (minimal need for reading)

Be accessible to people with co-occurring auditory comprehension (simple instructions)

Be accessible to people with co-occurring spoken production difficulties (applicable across

the severity continuum with non-verbal responses)

Be informed by the ICF framework (World Health Organisation, 2002), considering reading

from a holistic perspective

In a previous study (described in Webster et al., 2018), ten people with mild to moderate aphasia

had been interviewed about their pre-morbid reading and current (post-morbid) reading. The

interviews, like previous studies (Parr, 1992, 1995), highlighted the complexity of factors influencing

reading and the individual variation in reading ability, the importance and frequency of reading and

reading activities. Post-morbidly, there was a perceived decline in reading ability, with multi-faceted

reading difficulties reported. The themes identified in the interviews were considered alongside the

literature during the design of the questionnaire. The interview study and the literature suggested

that the questionnaire needed to focus on the nature of the reading difficulties (Aleligay et al.,

2008), the impact of difficulties on reading activities (Parr, 1995), how people feel about their

reading (Knollman-Porter et al., 2015; Parr, 1995) and the strategies used to aid reading (Knollman-

Porter et al., 2015; Lynch et al., 2013).

Page 9: Authors 1 2 1

Initial Draft

The initial draft of the questionnaire had three sections. Section A investigated rated ability across

linguistic levels (single word, sentence, paragraph and books) and within particular tasks (e.g.

reading a text message on a phone). There was also a question about speed of reading. Section B

consisted of three sub-sections. An initial set of questions and rating scales asked how people felt

about their reading. Scales used contrastive adjectives, for example, not enjoyable - enjoyable,

effortful – easy, frustrated – not frustrated. A second set of questions asked people to agree or

disagree about particular statements about why their reading had changed. For example, ‘my

reading has changed as I don’t need to read’ (agree/disagree), ‘my reading has changed as I can’t

concentrate’ (agree/disagree). People were then asked to identify from a list of strategies what

helped them to read. Section C looked at a set of reading activities. People were asked to consider

sequentially and select ‘what are you reading well?’, ‘what are you trying to read but finding

difficult?’ and ‘what would you like to read?’ Broad activities e.g. books, instructions, newspapers

were used, with the list compiled from a number of sources (Lapointe & Horner, 1998; Parr, 1992;

Worrall, 1999). A final question asked people how often they were reading.

Consultation with People with Aphasia

This preliminary draft of the questionnaire was reviewed by the Aphasia Research User Group

(ARUG) (see https://www.ncl.ac.uk/ecls/research/speechlanguage/arug/#ourservices). ARUG is a

group whose members all have aphasia and who have attended training to facilitate their

involvement in research consultation. ARUG members reviewed the initial draft with a set of

possible rating scales with varying formats (e.g. numbering, picture support, number of questions

per page). There was interactive discussion with the researchers about i) content and format of each

section ii) format of rating scales and iii) wording and order of individual questions. The following

feedback was provided within the discussion; points to take forward were checked with group

members as the meeting progressed. People in ARUG indicated the sections covered relevant

Page 10: Authors 1 2 1

aspects about reading. They had a preference for 5 point scales, with no numbers and picture

support (faces with different expressions) at extremes. In order to aid comprehension, they

suggested there was a need for minimum change in scales across questions and for questions to

have a similar structure. The participants in ARUG did not like the scales with contrastive adjectives

as they reported they were ‘negative’ and that ‘not’ appearing on different sides of the scale was

confusing (i.e. the contrast that ‘not confident’ may be negative whereas ‘not frustrated’ may be

positive). They also did not like the ‘agree/disagree’ questions and stated these should be phrased in

a similar way to the initial rating scales about reading ability. They reported the sequential nature of

the questions in section C was difficult and that there was a need to have a category for activities

that were not applicable. They also suggested changes to the wording of individual questions and

additional activities that should be included within section C.

Pilot Version Following the consultation, a revised version was produced which took into account the feedback

provided; this was similar to the final version (see below). The revised version of the test was then

piloted with 10 people with aphasia in order to consider acceptability. The group of people with

aphasia consisted of 6 women and 4 men, mean age of 67.7 years (SD =11.88, range 45-83). There

was diversity in terms of years of education, previous occupation, type and severity of aphasia and

reading difficulties. They all had chronic aphasia, with a mean time post-onset of 88.8 months (SD =

36.12, range 27-143 months). Participants were tested in a quiet room in a clinic setting, with the

paper-based questionnaire completed prior to any other assessments. Participants were shown each

question in written form (see example in appendix 1) and the question and prompts for the rating

scales were also read aloud. Testers followed a standard script, with both quantitative ratings and

qualitative comments recorded. Participants were then given the opportunity to comment on the

format. The pilot also offered the opportunity to refine the script used by the testers and to observe

different testers completing the questionnaire to ensure the instructions and prompts could be used

consistently. The pilot participants reported that they could understand the instructions and that

Page 11: Authors 1 2 1

the length of the questionnaire (around 20-30 minutes) was acceptable. Following the pilot, there

were slight changes to the script and the wording of a small number of the follow up questions. This

version was then presented back to ARUG for discussion. ARUG agreed that the changes made were

appropriate and made some minor additional recommendations about wording and overall format

which were then taken forward.

Test Version

These development phases led to a questionnaire with four sections :- i) Section A: Current reading

ii) Section B: Thoughts and feelings about reading iii) Section C: Reading activities and iv) Section D:

Reading strategies. To ensure the questionnaire was accessible to a wide range of people with

aphasia, it was designed to be aphasia friendly, with minimal reliance on reading comprehension and

minimal changes across rating scales. Main questions could be answered non-verbally, avoiding

reliance on verbal output. There were opportunities for people to provide additional qualitative

comments to supplement and explain their responses.

Section A: Current Reading

This section focused on asking the person with aphasia about their current reading ability and

reading difficulties, with themes derived from the interview study (Webster et al., 2018). In this

section, participants were asked to rate current reading abilities using the carrier phrase ‘at the

moment how difficult do you find reading and understanding’ followed by each of the following

items ‘single words | short sentences | paragraphs | a book | a message on a phone | on the

computer‘ and then ‘at the moment, how difficult do you find … reading text out loud |

remembering what you have read | and looking at information on a page’. A five point rating scale

was presented which ranged from impossible to no problem, with visual support at the extremes

(see appendix 1). People were asked to indicate their response on the scale and could select mid

points. If the person indicated the question was not relevant e.g. reading and understanding on the

computer, this was considered not applicable. On the scoring sheet, responses were given

Page 12: Authors 1 2 1

corresponding numbers from impossible (1) to no problem (5), with higher scores consistent with

less reading difficulty. If an individual rated themselves less than three on questions probing

concentration, memory and vision, they were asked whether these difficulties impacted activities

other than reading and whether there had been changes post-stroke.

Section B: Thoughts and Feelings about Reading

This section asked the person with aphasia about how they felt about their reading. The feelings

about reading reflected those reported in the interview study (Webster et al., 2018) and are

consistent with the findings of other qualitative studies (Kjellén et al., 2017; Knollman-Porter et al.,

2015). The section included nine questions, with slight variations in format across questions. The

initial questions used the carrier phrase ‘at the moment, do you find reading’ … followed by

‘enjoyable | easy’. The second set of questions asked ‘do you feel’ …. followed by ‘confident | happy

to try | motivated’. Participants were then asked ‘is reading important to you’ and ‘are you happy

with the speed of your reading?’ A five point rating scale was presented with no and yes at the

extremes and the same visual support as in section A. People were asked to indicate their response

on the scale and could select mid points. A final question asked how frequently the person read

materials like newspapers and books. For this question, the five point scale ranged from never to

everyday. On the scoring sheet, responses were given corresponding numbers from no (never for

frequency question) (1) to yes (everyday for frequency question) (5), with higher scores consistent

with more positive feelings about reading.

Section C: Reading Activities

This section asked the person with aphasia to think about different reading materials and what they

were currently reading. The reading activities probed were derived from activities reported in the

Parr studies (Parr, 1992, 1995), the lists given within Lapointe and Horner (1998) and Worrall (1999)

and those generated by ARUG. People were asked to sort cards representing different reading

materials and place them on a scale with four points: ‘impossible/avoid’, ‘trying to read but difficult’,

Page 13: Authors 1 2 1

‘trying to read and OK’ and ‘no problem’. The cards contained black and white pictures representing

the reading material with accompanying written labels; pictures were drawn specifically for this

study (see examples in appendix 2). Individual activities included reading labels | signs |

menus/shopping lists | adverts/leaflets | instructions | timetables | text messages | reading on a

computer | personal mail | formal letters | newspaper headlines | newspaper articles | magazines

| simpler books | books. People could indicate if the reading activity was not relevant (e.g. that they

never read timetables) and this was coded as not applicable. On the scoring sheet, responses were

given corresponding numbers, impossible/avoid (1), trying but difficult (2), trying and OK (3) and no

problem (4), so higher scores were consistent with a greater range of reading activity and less overall

difficulty.

Section D: Reading Strategies

Section D asked the person to identify strategies they were using to help with their reading. The

person was asked about each strategy in turn, replying ‘yes’ or ‘no’ as to whether that helped them

to read. Strategies probed were: slowing down | focusing on the main words | reading out loud|

having pictures| reducing distractions| tracking the text with finger or ruler| reading a second time|

support from family or friends| technology; these reflected the main strategies identified in the

interview study (Webster et al., 2018) and Lynch et al. (2013). A final question asked about any

other strategies that the individual was using. It was not possible to produce pictures which clearly

represented the strategies and so gestural support was given to aid comprehension of the

written/verbal label if needed. If people indicated they used a strategy, they were asked if this was

something they did prior to the stroke. Section D was not scored. At the end of the questionnaire,

people were asked ‘Is there anything else you want to add about your reading?’

Main Study: Participants 82 people were recruited to the main study. Inclusion criteria included single symptomatic stroke

resulting in aphasia (as confirmed by a speech and language therapist), English as first language, no

reported history of pre-morbid literacy difficulties, no significant visual impairment (reported in case

Page 14: Authors 1 2 1

history or observed during testing) and no significant cognitive impairment e.g. dementia (observed

during cognitive screening). People did not have to report or present with reading difficulties and

varied in the type and severity of their aphasia. There was no upper or lower limit for time post

stroke onset. Participants needed to be able to concentrate for 45- minute assessment sessions.

Details of the 82 participants involved in the main study can be found in table 1; no participants

were excluded following the cognitive screen. Complete questionnaire data was not available for

one participant (a 68 year old man who was 2 years 2 months post-stroke); he was, therefore,

excluded from the analysis of this data.

Insert table 1 about here

Main Study: Method Participants were initially seen for 3-4 sessions in which they completed background language and

other cognitive assessments, a test of reading comprehension (single word, sentence and paragraph)

and reading aloud (single word, sentence and paragraph) and the reading questionnaire. Details

about the format of the reading assessment and the performance of the participants on the reading

comprehension sub-tests can be found in Morris, Webster, Howard, Giles, and Gani (2015). Testing

conditions were as described in the pilot phase.

A sub-group of the participants was then re-tested on the assessment of reading comprehension and

reading aloud and the questionnaire approximately 4 weeks later. 28 participants completed the

questionnaire again during re-testing. Within this group, there were 7 participants who were less

than 6 months post-stroke (2 participants at 2 months, 3 at 3 months, 1 at 5 months and 1 at 6

months post-onset); these were included to see if the questionnaire could also be used reliably with

people in the early stages of recovery.

Results

Perception of Reading Table 2 summarises the profile of the group of people with aphasia across sections A to C of the

questionnaire. Higher scores are indicative of less reading difficulty, more positive feelings about

Page 15: Authors 1 2 1

reading and increased reading activity. To allow consideration of test re-test reliability of the total

score, reading activities that were rated as not applicable were given a score of zero. Most

participants only had one or two activities that they considered not relevant so this did not influence

the overall score significantly. Reflecting the broad inclusion criteria of the study, there was a range

of scores across all three sections. Some participants reported significant difficulties and some

perceived minimal change in their reading.

Insert table 2 about here

Figures 1 to 3 show the distribution of ratings across participants for sections A to C. To allow

comparison across questions, scores at mid points for sections A and B were collapsed (i.e. 1 and 1.5

presented as 1).

Insert figure 1 about here

A range of reported reading difficulties was seen across parameters in section A (see table 2) with

some participants reporting ‘no problem’ (i.e. score of 5) across many parameters. Some individuals

reported that they did not read text messages (around half) or read on the computer (around a

third) and therefore rated these questions as not applicable. Some participants also indicated that

reading aloud was not applicable. Across participants, there was a rated hierarchy of difficulty with

single words and sentences easier than paragraphs and paragraphs easier than books. A large

number of individuals identified difficulties concentrating on reading (concentration) and

remembering what they have read (memory).

Insert figure 2 about here

In section B, the majority of participants highlighted that reading was still important and individuals

were motivated to read and happy to try. Over half of the participants were still reading newspapers

or books each day (frequency question). However, participants were not finding reading as easy as

pre-stroke and were not happy with their speed of reading, often commenting on how slow it was or

Page 16: Authors 1 2 1

how long it took to read. There were mixed feelings about the extent to which reading was

enjoyable and how confident they felt about reading.

Insert figure 3 about here

In section C, focused on reading activities, like section A, there was a range of reported difficulty (see

table 2), with some individuals reporting ‘no problem’ (i.e. score of 4) across the activities. Also like

section A, some individuals identified reading text messages and reading on the computer as not

applicable. There were also some additional reading activities identified as not relevant or applicable

by a small number of individuals:- adverts, instructions, timetables, newspaper articles, magazines,

simple books and books. Across participants, there was a hierarchy of rated difficulty which

appeared to be related to the length of text (signs/labels/newspaper headlines easier than

menus/adverts which in turn were easier than instructions/newspaper articles which were in turn

easier than books). There was, however, also a distinction between personal mail and formal letters

which is likely to reflect familiarity of content or type of language, rather than length.

Figure 4 shows the reading strategies that people identified as helping them to read. Individuals

reported using a range of strategies (average of four, range one to nine) and these had generally

been developed post-stroke. The most common strategies (used by more than half of the

participants) included re-reading, slowing down and reducing distractions. Other frequently used

strategies were support from family and friends, focusing on the main words, having pictures

alongside the text and tracing the text as they read. Less than a quarter of individuals reported using

technology or reading aloud. No participant identified other strategies they were using.

Insert figure 4 about here

Tester Evaluation Following the study, the format of the questionnaire was reviewed by considering both the data and

comments from the testers who used it. This review suggested some modification was needed.

Participants were asked about reading a text message and reading on a computer across two

Page 17: Authors 1 2 1

sections and these activities were often rated as not applicable. Participants’ responses were

consistent across sections. It was, therefore, suggested that these questions should only appear in

section C. The testers also reported issues with two questions. First, the question about the impact

of visual difficulties on reading (phrased as ‘how difficult do you find looking at information on a

page’) was confusing, with more accurate information about the nature and impact of visual

difficulties obtained via case history information. Second, the question about frequency of reading

was reported to be too broad, with the acknowledgement that most people were reading something

each day. These two questions were therefore removed alongside the questions about technology in

section A. The final questionnaire consisted of 29 questions across the three sections and the

psychometric properties of this modified version were considered.

Psychometric Properties The psychometric properties of the questionnaire were considered in relation to certain aspects of

validity and reliability. Validity was considered in terms of face and construct validity. Face validity

refers to whether the questionnaire subjectively measures the aspect of interest. The concepts and

questions within the questionnaire were driven by previous qualitative studies of reading in people

with aphasia. In addition, the people with aphasia in ARUG reported it covered relevant aspects of

reading and had the opportunity to shape questions and include or exclude feelings about reading

and reading activities. In relation to construct validity, there was consideration of whether the

questionnaire measured a single construct and the degree to which it measured perception of

reading. The questionnaire had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .932). An exploratory

factor analysis was carried out. All responses had a substantial loading (>.40) suggesting that all

items were relevant and that no question/rating should be excluded. Principal component analysis

using varimax rotation with Kaiser Normalisation resulted in a seven factor solution which accounted

for 72.4% of the variance. The initial factor accounted for 39.6% of the variance, with other factors

varying between 3.8% and 9.1%. The component matrix can be found in appendix 3. In general,

ratings contributed to more than one factor, reflecting the potential overlap and interaction

Page 18: Authors 1 2 1

between perceived reading difficulties, the impact on reading activity and feelings about reading.

There was one factor that related to technology (factor 6) and one factor (factor 4) that primarily

reflected the ratings in section B related to feelings about reading. There were significant positive

correlations between the sections of the questionnaire (Sections A and B, Spearman correlation

rs(79) = 0.543, p=<.001, Sections B and C, Spearman correlation rs(79) = 0.560, p=<.001, Sections A

and C, Spearman correlation rs(79) = 0.780, p=<.001).

Reliability was considered in relation to internal consistency and test re-test reliability; test re-test

reliability is important to ensure the results are representative of performance and if, in future, the

questionnaire is used to monitor change as a consequence of recovery or treatment. As previously

highlighted, the questionnaire had good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = .932). The items

with the lowest correlations with the other items were those related to reading and technology.

Figure 5 shows the relationship between the total scores for sections A to C during the initial

assessment and at re-test for the 28 participants. There was a significant positive correlation

(Spearman correlation, rs(26) = 0.927, p= <0.001) indicating a high degree of test re-test reliability.

Positive, significant correlations were also found for each section (section A, Spearman correlation,

rs(26) = 0.839, p<0.001, section B, Spearman correlation, rs(26)= 0.858, p<0.001 and section C,

Spearman correlation, rs(26) = 0.846, p<0.001). For the seven participants who were early post-onset

(6 months or less), the same significant positive correlation was seen between test and re-test total

scores (Spearman correlation, rs(5) = 0.857, p= 0.014) suggesting that the questionnaire was also

reliable for these participants.

Insert figure 5 about here

In relation to clinical usefulness and acceptability, the questionnaire was an appropriate way of

obtaining information about the person’s perspective on their reading and was feasible within a

clinical setting, taking around 20 minutes to complete. People with varying types and severity of

aphasia were able to complete the measure.

Page 19: Authors 1 2 1

Discussion This study aimed to add to our understanding of people with aphasia’s perspective on their reading

by developing a clinically useful, valid and reliable tool. A questionnaire was developed that could be

used to explore the person with aphasia’s perspective on their current reading ability, feelings about

reading, reading activities and strategies used to support reading; these are aspects relevant to

explore within the ICF. This section will discuss the results and will then evaluate the questionnaire

and consider how it might be used clinically.

Reading difficulties in people with aphasia Within this study, not all individuals with aphasia reported difficulties with reading. This may reflect

the broad inclusion criteria of the study but also implies that (at least from the individual’s

perspective), reading difficulties are not always present in people with aphasia. There was individual

variation in the difficulties people reported understanding single words, sentences, paragraphs and

books, but with increasing perceived difficulty as the length of text increased. As in previous studies,

many participants highlighted that they were unhappy with the speed of their reading and

emphasised the potential role of concentration and memory on reading ability (Knollman-Porter et

al., 2015 & Webster et al., 2018). As in previous studies, the exact nature of these difficulties and

what the person is rating as ‘memory’ is not entirely transparent. Within section C, some activities

were not relevant or applicable for some individuals, emphasising the individual nature of reading

activities that has been highlighted in previous studies (Kjellén et al., 2017; Knollman-Porter et al.,

2015; Parr, 1995). The activities related to the use of technology were frequently rated as not

applicable suggesting that within this group, there were many individuals not using mobile phones

or computers. This could be due to their age, with older adults and retired people using technology

less (Czaja & Lee, 2007; Dutton & Blank, 2011; Dutton, Blank, & Groselj, 2013). It could also reflect

the complexities of using technology and the compounding influence of stroke and aphasia (Menger,

Morris, & Salis, 2016, 2019). For those who were reading on the computer, additional information

would be needed as to what they were reading on the computer and the comparative difficulty of

Page 20: Authors 1 2 1

these tasks e.g. reading email, reading websites. As in other studies (e.g. Lynch et al., 2013; Webster

et al., 2018), there was also variation in the reading strategies used but again only a small number of

individuals reported using technology to support reading. Overall, the results in this large group are

consistent with the findings of the in-depth interview studies carried out with small groups of

individuals (Parr, 1995, Lynch et al., 2013, Knollman-Porter et al., 2015 & Webster et al., 2018). In

contrast to the interview studies, the questionnaire focused only on present reading rather than a

comparison of pre- and post-morbid reading. Although it is necessary to have some understanding of

pre-morbid reading, an understanding of current reading activity and preferences was considered to

be the prerequisite for goal setting and any subsequent monitoring of change.

Evaluation and potential clinical value of questionnaire The questionnaire was an appropriate way of obtaining information about the person’s perspective

of their reading, providing a useful addition to assessments of reading comprehension and reading

aloud. It was feasible to complete with a wide range of people with aphasia; people understood the

questions and were able to respond using the rating scales. The results from the ratings allowed

areas of perceived strength and areas of concern to be identified; some participants were able to

provide additional qualitative comments to elaborate a rating or in response to the follow up

questions.

The questionnaire had good face and construct validity, suggesting it was an appropriate measure of

an individual’s perception of their reading post-stroke. Concurrent validity was not investigated as

there was not another measure of self-perception of reading comprehension with which it could be

compared. The questionnaire had good internal consistency and test re-test reliability allowing it to

be used confidently as an assessment tool. Further investigation is necessary to analyse its sensitivity

to change.

The final version of the questionnaire (sections A to C) will be included in the Comprehensive

Assessment of Reading in Aphasia (CARA, Morris, Webster, Howard & Garraffa, in preparation). If

Page 21: Authors 1 2 1

reading is important for an individual, the results of the questionnaire can be considered alongside

the results of the assessments of reading ability to determine goals that are functional and relevant.

In section A, rated perceptions of comprehension difficulty (words, sentence, paragraphs and books)

can be compared with the results of any formal assessment at these levels; differences in perception

and accuracy on testing may reflect insight/awareness of difficulties and/or the relative importance

of reading. Section A also provides insight into potential contributing factors e.g. concentration,

memory which may warrant consideration when planning additional assessment and/or

intervention. Section B provides insight into how the person feels about their reading difficulty.

Section C highlights potential foci for intervention; reading activities that the person reports they are

‘trying to read but difficult’ could be an appropriate starting point, with further discussion of exact

materials e.g. what newspaper the person reads. As highlighted above, it may also be useful to ask

people to rate each activity in terms of frequency. The questionnaire may also be used to monitor

the outcome of reading intervention. With a sole focus on reading, it may be more sensitive to

changes in reading than measures which consider the overall impact of aphasia. It could also capture

changes in one aspect of reading, for example, an intervention may make minimal change in reading

ability (if concentrating on compensatory strategies) but may impact on feelings about reading

and/or reading activities.

We need to consider perception of ability and preference alongside linguistic ability. Given the

inherent variation in reading ability and preference in the general population, this is particularly vital

when we consider reading. In-depth interview studies have provided us with insights into how

people with aphasia view their reading. The questionnaire provides a practical and reliable measure

of a person’s perception of their reading post-stroke and has potential as a patient centred tool,

contributing to goal setting, and outcome measurement in clinical practice.

Page 22: Authors 1 2 1

Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank the members of ARUG who shaped the direction of the project,

Maria Garraffa and Lois McCluskey for their assistance with data collection, all of the participants for

contributing their time and the individuals and organisations who assisted with recruitment to the

study.

Declaration of Interest The authors declared no conflict of interest. This work was funded by a grant from the Stroke

Association (TSA 2011/03).

References Aleligay, A., Worrall, L. E., & Rose, T. A. (2008). Readability of written health information provided to

people with aphasia. Aphasiology, 22(4), 383-407. doi:10.1080/02687030701415872

Brookshire, C. E., Wilson, J. P., Nadeau, S. E., Rothi, L. J. Gonzalez, & Kendall, D. L. (2014). Frequency,

nature, and predictors of alexia in a convenience sample of individuals with chronic aphasia.

Aphasiology, 28(12), 1464-1480. doi:10.1080/02687038.2014.945389

Cocks, N., Matthews, N., Barnett, L., Phillips, E., Middleton, R., Gregoire-Clarke, J., & Cruice, M.

(2010). Functional reading therapy for individuals with acquired reading difficulties.

Acquiring Knowledge in Speech, Language and Hearing, 12 (1), 37-41.

Cocks, N., Pritchard, M., Cornish, H., Johnson, N., & Cruice, M. (2013). A “novel” reading therapy

programme for reading difficulties after a subarachnoid haemorrhage. Aphasiology, 27(5),

509-531. doi:10.1080/02687038.2013.780283

Czaja, S.J., & Lee, C.C. (2007). The impact of aging on access to technology. Universal Access in the

Information Society, 5(4), 341-349. doi:10.1007/s10209-006-0060-x

Dietz, A., Ball, A., & Griffith, J. (2011). Reading and writing with aphasia in the 21st century:

Technological applications of supported reading comprehension and written expression.

Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation, 18(6), 758-769. doi:10.1310/tsr1806-758

Page 23: Authors 1 2 1

Dutton, W. H., & Blank, G. (2011). Next generation users: The internet in Britain (October 16, 2011).

Oxford Internet Survey, 2011.

Dutton, W. H., Blank, G., & Groselj, D. (2013). Cultures of the Internet: The Internet in Britain. Oxford:

University of Oxford.

Hepworth, L. R., Rowe, F. J., Walker, M. F., Rockliffe, J., Noonan, C., Howard, C., & Currie, J. (2016).

Post-stroke visual impairment: a systematic literature review of types and recovery of visual

conditions. Ophthalmology Research: An International Journal, 1-43.

Kjellén, E., Laakso, K., & Henriksson, I. (2017). Aphasia and literacy—the insider's perspective.

International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52(5), 573-584.

doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12302

Knollman-Porter, K., Wallace, S. E., Hux, K., Brown, J., & Long, C. (2015). Reading experiences and use

of supports by people with chronic aphasia. Aphasiology, 29(12), 1448-1472.

doi:10.1080/02687038.2015.1041093

Lapointe, L. L., & Horner, J. (1998). Reading Comprehension Battery for Aphasia-2. Austin, Texas: Pro-

ed.

Lee, J. B., & Sohlberg, M. M. (2013). Evaluation of Attention Training and Metacognitive Facilitation

to Improve Reading Comprehension in Aphasia. American Journal of Speech and Language

Pathology, 22(2), S318-S333.

Long, A., Hesketh, A., Paszek, G., Booth, M., & Bowen, A. . (2008). Development of a reliable, self-

report outcome measure for pragmatic trials of communication therapy following stroke:

the Communication Outcome after Stroke (COAST) scale. Clinical Rehabilitation, 22, 1083-

1094.

Luck, A. M., & Rose, M. L. (2007). Interviewing people with aphasia: Insights into method

adjustments from a pilot study. Aphasiology, 21(2), 208-224.

doi:10.1080/02687030601065470

Page 24: Authors 1 2 1

Lynch, K. E., Damico, J. S., Abendroth, K. J., & Nelson, R. L. (2013). Reading performance subsequent

to aphasia: Strategies applied during authentic reading. Aphasiology, 27(6), 723-739.

Mayer, J.F., & Murray, L. L. (2002). Approaches to the treatment of alexia in chronic aphasia.

Aphasiology, 16(7), 727-743.

Menger, F., Morris, J., & Salis, C. (2016). Aphasia in an Internet age: wider perspectives on digital

inclusion. Aphasiology, 30(2-3), 112-132. doi:10.1080/02687038.2015.1109050

Menger, F., Morris, J., & Salis, C. (2019). The impact of aphasia on Internet and technology use.

Disability and Rehabilitation, 1-11. doi:10.1080/09638288.2019.1580320

Morris, J., Webster, J., Howard, D., Giles, J., & Gani, A. (2015). Reading comprehension in aphasia:

The development of a novel assessment of reading comprehension. Report for Stroke

Association.

Parr, S. (1992). Everyday reading and writing practices of normal adults: implications for aphasia

assessment. Aphasiology, 6(3), 273-283. doi:10.1080/02687038.2014.955388

Parr, S. (1995). Everyday reading and writing in aphasia: role change and the influence of pre-morbid

literacy practice. Aphasiology, 9(3), 223-238.

Parr, S. (2007). Living with severe aphasia: Tracking social exclusion. Aphasiology, 21(1), 98-123.

doi:10.1080/02687030600798337

Rose, T. A., Worrall, L. E., Hickson, L. M., & Hoffmann, T. C. (2011). Aphasia friendly written health

information: Content and design characteristics. International journal of speech-language

pathology, 13(4), 335-349.

Rowe, F. J., Wright, D., Brand, D., Jackson, C., Price, A., Walker, L., . . . Scott, C.J. (2011). Reading

difficulty after stroke: ocular and non ocular causes. International Journal of Stroke, 6(5),

404-411.

Simmons-Mackie, N., Kagan, A., Victor, J. C., Carling-Rowland, A., Mok, A., Hoch, J. S., . . . Streiner, D.

L. (2014). The assessment for living with aphasia: Reliability and construct validity.

Page 25: Authors 1 2 1

International journal of speech-language pathology, 16(1), 82-94.

doi:10.3109/17549507.2013.831484

Swinburn, K., Porter, G., & Howard, D. (2004). The Comprehensive Aphasia Test. Hove: Psychology

Press.

Webb, W. G., & Love, R. J. (1983). Reading problems in chronic aphasia. Journal of Speech and

Hearing Disorders, 48(2), 164-171.

Webster, J., Samouelle, A., & Morris, J. (2018). The brain can't cope": Insights about reading from

people with chronic aphasia. PsyArXiv. doi:10.31234/osf.io/p8xh4

World Health Organisation (2002). Towards a common language for functioning, disability and

health; ICF. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organisation.

Worrall, L. (1999). FCTP: Functional Communication Therapy Planner. Oxon, UK: Winslow Press.

Page 26: Authors 1 2 1

Appendix 1

A.1. At the moment, how difficult do you find…

reading and understanding single words

impossible no problem |______________________________|_____________________________|____________________________|______________________________|

Page 27: Authors 1 2 1

Appendix 2: Examples of Pictures Used in Section C: Reading activities

Formal letters

Newspaper articles

Page 28: Authors 1 2 1

Appendix 3: Results of Factor Analysis Rotated Component Matrixa

Component

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

A1words .458 .652

A2sentences .585 .550

A3paragraphs .392 .627 .317

A4books .631 .434 .373

A7readingaloud .829

A8concentratio

n .837

A9memory .810

B1enjoyment .574 .511

B2effort .651 .303 .373 .335

B3confidence .384 .510 .357 .328

B4anxiety .354 .333 .450

B5motivation .830

B6importance .893

B7speed .571 .431

C1labels .776

C2signs .795

C3menus .776

C4adverts .713

C5instructions .311 .596 .442

C6timetable .483 .528

C7texts .857

Page 29: Authors 1 2 1

C8computer .819

C9mail .753

C10formalletter .384 .421 .523

C11headline .752 .335

C12newspaper .568 .497

C13magazines .446 .604

C14simplebook .724

C15books .332 .664 .324

Extraction Method: Principal Component Analysis.

Rotation Method: Varimax with Kaiser Normalization.a

a. Rotation converged in 7 iterations.

Tables Table 1: Details of people with aphasia involved in the wider study of reading

Age Mean 65.9 years

SD=12.2

Range 33-87 years

Gender 48 male 34 female

Education Mean 11.8 years

SD=2.2

18 educated to degree level

Time post-onset Mean 47.8 months

SD=60.8

Range 2-252 months

Page 30: Authors 1 2 1

Table 2: Total scores for sections A to C of questionnaire

Total Score * Mean

(SD)

Range

Section A: Current reading /50 31.7

(9.0)

13-49

Section B: Thoughts and feelings

about reading /40

29.9

(7.4)

14-40

Section C: Reading activities /60 41.4

(11.7)

15-60

Total Score /150 102.9

(24.9)

45-146.5

Notes: * For the purpose of calculating the total score, activities that were rated as not applicable

were scored as zero.

Page 31: Authors 1 2 1

Legends for Figures

Figure 1: Section A: Current reading - distribution of ratings across participants

Figure 2: Section B: Thoughts and feelings about reading - distribution of ratings across participants

Figure 3: Section C: Reading activities - distribution of ratings across participants

Figure 4: Section D: Reading strategies

Figure 5: Relationship between total scores at initial assessment and re-test

Page 32: Authors 1 2 1

Figures

Figure 1

Page 33: Authors 1 2 1

Figure 2

Page 34: Authors 1 2 1

Figure 3

Page 35: Authors 1 2 1

Figure 4

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

Technology

Reading out loud

Tracking the text with finger

Having pictures

Focusing on main words

Support from friends or family

Reducing distractions

Slowing down

Reading a second time

% of Participants Reporting Use

Stra

tegy

Page 36: Authors 1 2 1

Figure 5: