aso 27 - 28

3

Click here to load reader

Upload: wmubats

Post on 30-Oct-2014

530 views

Category:

Technology


1 download

DESCRIPTION

 

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: ASO 27 - 28

Chapter 27 and 28

Maintenance and Transfer

ANSWERS

50. The two myths of performance

maintenance.

a. What are they?

ANSWER: The myth of perpetual-behavior

maintenance1: This myth states that if

you modify a behavior, then that

modified behavior will maintain itself

without your having to deliver anymore

behavioral consequences contingent

upon that behavior. Thus the myth states

that you can apply the intervention once,

and that is all that is necessary to make a

permanent change in behavior.

The myth of intermittent reinforcement:

An intermittent schedule of

reinforcement can be thinned (the

interval or number of responses in

between reinforcement delivery is

increased) to extremely high levels

which will in turn make the behavior

super-resistant to extinction. Thus the

myth suggests that it is possible to make

a behavior inextinguishable.

b. Provide an example of each

myth.

ANSWER:

The myth of the perpetual-behavior

mainrenance – Let’s talk about the child

with autism who engages in head hitting.

Upon performing a functional analysis,

1 For PB6e We may erroneously call this

“The myth of perpetual behavior

intervention” We are changing the

terminology from ‘intervention’ to

‘maintenance’.

we found out that the head-hitting was

being reinforced by sensory

consequences, so our intervention

consists of putting a helmet on the child

to prevent the sensory stim and leaving it

on until head hitting is well

extinguished. Therefore we can stop our

intervention and we can assume the child

will never head hit again.

More common examples of the

myth applied to behavior reduction

would be that once you’ve quit smoking

for a few weeks or quit doing crack for a

few months, you’re cool—probably you

ain’t; you may need PM contingencies

for a long time, though eventually you

may be cool. And even more common,

once weight-watchers has helped you get

down to bikini beautiful, you’re cool—

you won’t start over-eating enough to

gain much or all of the weight backi; but

that’s rarely the case.

And in terms of behavior

maintenance, after you’ve had a

semester of art or literature or music

appreciation, the intrinsic reinforcers are

so great that you’re going to spend most

of the rest of your life in art museums,

concert halls, and Barnes and Nobel.

And once you’ve experienced the

benefits of using flashcards and studying

everyday for this class that defines the

new you for all future classes (Ok,

sometimes, somewhat, but rarely is it

that easy.)

The myth of intermittent reinforcement –

Let’s say that we have reinforced

Rudolph the rat’s lever press on a

continuous reinforcement schedule.

Then we will gradually change this to a

Page 2: ASO 27 - 28

FR 5 schedule, then a FR 10, FR 20, FR

50, FR 100, FR 1000…(you get the

picture). After we have gotten the

schedule of reinforcement sufficiently

high, we will stop reinforcing the

behavior and the assumption is that the

behavior will maintain. Of course, no

one really assumes that about the

Skinner box, but there is the tendency to

make that assumption in real world

examples, e.g., uhh. Well, I can’t think

of any plausible e.g.’s; yet I don’t think

I’m exactly fighting a straw man (excuse

me, I mean “straw person.”)

c. What is the flaw with each of the

examples that you have just

provided?

ANSWER:

In the above example of the child with

autism, if the behavior occurs again, then

the child again will experience the

reinforcing consequences that he once

did for engaging in head-hitting. In this

case, the behavior will increase to its

previous levels, because the behavior is

being reinforced again. Just because we

got the frequency of the behavior down

to zero at one point in time doesn’t mean

that it stays there forever if the

intervention is no longer in place.

However, I think if we’d used a

punishment contingency, especially an

intermittent one, we might come much

closer to a perpetual intervention.

The above example where we continued

to make the reinforcement schedule for

Rudolph’s lever press increasingly

intermittent is flawed because even

though we may be able to thin the

schedule of reinforcement to a

sufficiently high level, we will never be

able to stop the delivery of

reinforcement while maintaining the

lever press. Responses won’t occur

unless they are reinforced, so any idea

that suggests stopping reinforcement for

a response will most certainly lead to

response extinction.

51. Transfer of training versus stimulus

generalization

a. What is the myth concerning

these two phenomenon?

ANSWER: The transfer of training myth: You can

explain all occurrences of transfer of

training by in terms of stimulus

generalization.

(This is a myth because NOT ALL

instances of transfer of training can be

explained through stimulus

generalization.)

b. Compare and contrast

ANSWER:

Similarities – Both transfer of

training and stimulus

generalization deal with the

performance of responses in one

setting that were acquired in a

different setting.

Crucial Difference – Transfer of

training is a term that

encompasses all instances in

which a response learned in one

setting occurs in a different

setting. Stimulus generalization

is a special case of transfer of

training. In other words it is a

sub-category that belongs under

the heading of “transfer of

training”. Stimulus

generalization really is (as you

already know) a proposed

mechanism for explaining

Page 3: ASO 27 - 28

transfer of training, namely that

performance acquired in one

setting occurs in another due to

the physical similarity between

the two settings. And while this

does serve as an adequate

explanation for those settings that

DO share some physical

similarities, stimulus

generalization does NOT explain

why transfer occurs when two

settings don’t share any physical

similarities at all.

c. Please provide an example of

transfer that exposes the error

that lies behind the transfer of

training myth.

ANSWER: Transfer of training of street

crossing skills. The training setting is a

model street made of cardboard and

plastic dolls are used to cross the model

street. Then the test setting will be a

REAL street with REAL people. If the

individuals trained to cross a street this

way were asked, there would be

discrimination between these two

settings, and yet transfer still occurs.

d. Explain why stimulus

generalization is not adequate in

explaining your example of

transfer. What does explain it?

ANSWER: In the example provided

above, there is a clear discrimination

between the training setting and the test

setting. Therefore since there are no

physical similarities between the two

settings, stimulus generalization cannot

occur. Transfer in this example really

occurs due to rules. So, in reality there

are two methods by which transfer of

training can occur; it can occur by way

of stimulus generalization OR through

the use of rules.