“i know exactly where it is but i don’t know where it’s going…”

Post on 14-Apr-2018

213 Views

Category:

Documents

0 Downloads

Preview:

Click to see full reader

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/30/2019 I know exactly where it is but I dont know where its going

    1/4

    COVER STORY: THERAPY

    SPEECH & LANGUAGE THERAPY IN PRACTICE Summer 20076

    I know exactly whebut I dont know w

    READ THIS IF YOU ARE

    INTERESTED IN

    USING

    COMMUNICATION

    AIDS AS THERAPY

    TOOLS

    HOW IMPAIRMENT

    AND FUNCTIONALTHERAPY

    APPROACHES

    WORK TOGETHER

    DESIGNING

    THERAPY

    COLLABORATIVELY

    WITH CLIENTS

    P

    hoto:PaulReid.

    Trevor Harley is

    also author o

    comprehensive

    psycholinguistic

    textbook 'The

    Psychology oLanguage'. For your

    chance to win a FREE

    copy o this book, see

    the Reader Ofer on

    the inside ront cover.

  • 7/30/2019 I know exactly where it is but I dont know where its going

    2/4

    rapid shits o turn and when there is background noise.Formal assessment shows comprehension breakdown

    in sentences i the context does not assist in understand-

    ing the relationship between items.

    In terms o naming (gure 1), Helens assessment

    record shows a history o word orm errors (literal para-

    phasias), many o which are non-words. Alternatively,

    attempts are aborted ater inability to start the word or

    ater producing only the initial phonemes. Helens errors

    increase with word length and she makes more dele-

    tions or substitutions towards the end o words. Less

    requently, Helen makes semantic errors, such as substi-

    tuting ather or husband.

    Figure 1 Naming examples

    Mug muck monk mugHoover hoolaCigarette sigarantSpeakability speakaleekieCognitive neuropsychological models o speech pro-

    duction detail the component processes in speech pro-

    duction (see or example Levelt, 1989). With reerence to

    such models, Den Ouden & Bastiaanse (2005) conclude

    that the symptoms o conduction dysphasia are associ-

    ated with impaired phonological encoding. This stage -

    between word orm selection and articulatory planning

    - incorporates a slot-ller mechanism that maps the

    sounds (llers) onto their position (slots) in a word rame

    (Nickels, 1997). Den Ouden & Bastiaanse (2005) suggest

    SPEECH & LANGUAGE THERAPY IN PRACTICE Summer 2007 7

    COVER STORY: THERAPY

    e it isere its going

    When Laorag Hunter oferedHelen Gowland a high-tech aidas a communication strategy,neither suspected it wouldbecome a means o acilitating

    Helens use o multi-syllabic wordsin everyday speech. Here, withSiobhan MacAndrew and TrevorHarley, they discuss the use oimages as signposts in therapy and the kind o service weneed to provide to ensure suchopportunities are not missed.

    This article describes a specic component otherapy, a solution-ocused approach that is sim-

    ple to implement. This therapy evolved collabo-

    ratively when the introduction o a high-tech

    aid as a communication strategy opened up unplanned

    possibilities. We nd it particularly exciting because it is

    having a measurable impact on a client with conduction

    aphasia seven years ater onset.

    Our story starts seven years ago, when Helen Gowland

    settled down one evening to watch Jools Holland on TV. This

    was a moment o relaxation in a schedule involving many

    roles. Helen worked ull-time as a specialist physiotherapist,

    researcher at an international level, Chair o the school Par-

    ent Teacher Association, wie, mother to three teenagers and

    daughter to an elderly ather. She remembers wondering

    that evening why paramedics were in her house.

    Helens next memory is ten days later in neurosurgery

    nding hersel with a large surgical wound in her skull,

    unable to speak or to understand what people were say-

    ing. It took a long time to ully grasp what had happened.

    Helen had sufered a sub-arachnoid haemorrhage rom

    a large let middle cerebral artery aneurysm. An emer-

    gency craniotomy was required to clip the aneurysm.

    Two days later Helen experienced delayed ischaemia

    with seizures and severe aphasia.

    Changing needsHelens partnership with speech and language therapist

    Laorag has continued and developed over the seven

    years. Episodes o care have responded to her changing

    needs and varied in type and length. Reasons or thera-py have included assessment, rehabilitation, supporting

    and enabling (Malcolmess, 2001). Additionally, Helen

    has opted in to resources and projects including group

    work, computer classes and visual arts. Complicating

    actors afecting rehabilitation have included wound

    inections, removal o her temporal bone-ap with later

    cranioplasty to close the skull, and issues relating to epi-

    lepsy and side efects o medication.

    Helens communication prole resembles that o the

    syndrome o Conduction Aphasia (Goodglass & Kaplan,

    1983). Key eatures are uent and grammatical speech;

    di culty in the sequential order o speech sounds in

    naming and repetition; greater di culty with longer

    words; good awareness o errors; repeated attempts

    at error correction (a characteristic known as conduite

    dapproche) and relatively spared comprehension.

    Helen has excellent conversational skills and is excep-

    tionally resourceul at getting her ideas across through

    total communication methods. When she is unable

    to express the ull content o her message she will use

    drawing, ragmentary writing, pantomime, circumlocu-

    tion and even singing. Helens speech is easy to under-

    stand and is uent and grammatical.

    These strengths contrast with a history o severely im-

    paired noun production in spontaneous speech, spoken

    naming, written naming and repetition. Helens under-

    standing in everyday situations is good but vulnerable to

    errors in times, days and numbers. She notices increased

    di culties understanding in groups where there are

    Figure 2 Meta-linguistic awareness A print showing how Helen senses her aphasia. Reprinted by kind permission o Christine Kingsley.

  • 7/30/2019 I know exactly where it is but I dont know where its going

    3/4

    that, as well as a mapping decit, the error patterns o

    some people with conduction aphasia are secondary

    to a verbal memory decit. In these cases speech errors

    result rom di culties retaining the activation o the

    phonological plan.

    Meta-linguistic awarenessHelens descriptions o her experience when naming sug-gest having word orm knowledge with inability to com-

    plete the phonological orm. These descriptions show a

    high level o meta-linguistic awareness (see gure 2, p.7):

    I know the meaning o the word but I dont know what it

    looks like.

    I know exactly where it is but I dont know where its going.

    Sometimes I can start it but I dont really know how to say

    it properly.

    Helens written naming ollows a similar pattern to

    her speech in that she writes partial word orms. She re-

    quently writes more letters o a word than sounds she

    can say. When letters are omitted she can oten indicate

    the correct number o letters or syllables.

    Helens reading is slow and limited to short pieces.

    She has great di culty recognising unction words such

    as prepositions, conjunctions and determiners. This is

    a symptom o phonological dyslexia. Impaired reading

    by letter to sound conversion (grapheme to phoneme

    route) orces reading via the semantic route (Harley,

    2001). Words with poor semantic representations such

    as unction words are thereore read poorly (Friedman et

    al., 2002). Helen compensates or this di culty by using

    text-to-speech sotware to aid reading comprehension.

    Speech production stuckThrough semantic type therapy (Nickels, 2002) and practice

    o specic word sets Helen slowly made modest gains in

    naming. At the start o this therapy Helens speech produc-tion was stable; this could be re-phrased as stuck. Laorag

    ofered a communication aid as a back-up to speech to help

    transer o inormation such as address, phone number,

    numbers, and commonly used amily details. Helen liked

    the idea o using the aid to practise speech but using it in

    interactions did not appeal. She elt the aid was too big, too

    slow and di cult or people to read. She thought that peo-

    ple would be too busy to attend to it and that it negatively

    afected perception o her competence.

    Helen wanted to say letter names as they requently

    occur, or example, in addresses, clothing sizes, names o

    companies and medication (or example, DD2, RAC, CPR).

    Letter names cannot be read by letter to sound conver-

    sion and are disadvantaged in reading via a semanticroute because they are low in semantic value. Friedman et

    al. (2002) increased unction word reading in two people

    with phonological dyslexia. Using paired associate learn-

    ing Friedman improved reading o unction words (low

    semantic value) by pairing with homophones high in se-

    mantic value (not/knot, knows/nose). This is an example

    o a reorganisation o unction where intact processes are

    used to compensate or impaired processes.

    We linked the alphabet with semantically rich homo-

    phones and near homophones that Helen ound mean-

    ingul (gure 3). Near homophones begin with a sound

    that is the same as the letter name. These links were re-

    inorced by storing a picture o the link word along with

    a recording o the letter name in a Dynamo digitised

    speech communication aid.

    COVER STORY: THERAPY

    SPEECH & LANGUAGE THERAPY IN PRACTICE Summer 20078

    FunLaorag segments the target word into syllables and sug-

    gests single syllable words that are homophones or near

    homophones or each syllable. An image to represen

    each single syllable word is selected rom the PictureCommunication Symbols set (Mayer-Johnson, 1981)

    Where there is more than one possible image Helen se-

    lects the one that is most meaningul to her. Associating

    each syllable with a Picture Communication Symbol has

    drawn upon Helens strengths in word meaning, oreign

    languages, wide-ranging knowledge and creativity. It

    has also been a un part o therapy or both o us!

    We make a page or each target word on the Dynamo

    Each syllable is represented by a picture o the associated

    image rom the Picture Communication Symbols accom-

    panied by the written orm o the word. The syllable is re-

    corded so that when the picture is touched the syllable

    is heard (gure 5). Initially, Laorag made the recordings

    but ater two to three months o therapy Helen couldindependently say and record the single syllable words

    By activating the pictures in sequence Helen hears her

    recorded production o the whole word built up syllable

    Figure 3 Alphabet links

    Letter Link word Link Picture Homophone type

    A eight Near homophone

    B bee Homophone

    C sea Homophone

    D Delia Near homophone

    Helen ound the discipline to practise challenging but

    her ability to say letter names improved. Interestingly,

    the method enabled Helen to say letter names in se-

    quence (eg. MP). This suggested a potential to sequence

    single syllables to produce multi-syllabic words i eachsyllable was associated with a picture cue. Helen was

    enthusiastic and quickly identied long words she want-

    ed to say such as aphasia, Glenrothes, Victoria. Laorag

    was concerned that this was too ambitious as until now

    work on spoken output had mainly been on single syl-

    lable, picturable words.

    For our multi-syllabic words therapy, Helen makes

    lists o words that she wants to say and use but is unable

    to produce (target words). The lists typically consist o

    written ragments o each word (1-3 letters) accompa-

    nied by a drawing or picturable items. For more abstract

    words (eg. policy, resources) additional clues in the orm

    o gestures or associated words are required rom Helen

    beore Laorag can identiy the target word. Helens cho-sen vocabulary reects personal, amily and proessional

    interests, needs, seasons and world events. Most items

    are 2-5 syllables in length (gure 4).

    Figure 4 Target vocabulary (selected by Helen)

    Figure 5 Target word examples

    Aphasia

    Glenrothes

    Victoria

    Hospital

    Manchester

    Inverness

    AmbulancePharmacist

    Greece

    Management

    International

    Marks and Spencer

    St Andrews

    Hideko

    Japan

    Mackinlay

    Egypt

    Relations

    Continence

    Chlamydia

    University

    Paddington

    Psychology

    BudapestCommunication

    Australia

    Thorburn

    Ipswich

    Cambridge

    First Aid

    Africa

    Sudan

    Switzerland

    Umbrella

    Antique

    America

    Islam

    Computer

    Politics

    Czech Republic

    GraduateExam

    Leaflet

    CBIR

    Aneurysm

    Internet

    Sociology

    Pakistan

    Tsunami

    Neurology

    Stollen

    Sandwich

    Sainsbury

    Age Concern

    Address

    Stepney

    Anatomy

    HRTTheatre

    Courgette

    Broccoli

    Coleslaw

    SR flour

    Statement

    Parliament

    Bric-a-brac

    DVD

    Strathearn

    Human Resources

    Cathedral

    Elephant

    Animal

    Policy

    MSP

    NovelConcert

    Ozone

    Lebanon

    South

    Columbia

    Falkland

    Prescription

    1955

  • 7/30/2019 I know exactly where it is but I dont know where its going

    4/4

    We agree with Nickels (2002) that or the clinician

    understanding how the therapy worked is a luxury and

    would add that given the complex variables in typical

    therapy this can eel like impossibility!

    However, our method draws upon several therapy ap-proaches, unctional, re-organisation and stimulation:

    The goal is unctionalto allow Helen to say the words

    she wants to use and is driven by Helens require-

    ments and aspirations.

    We achieve production o previously errorul words

    through an alternative route (re-organisation). The pictures

    in sequence give Helen su cient inormation to allow

    phonetic encoding. This may be a direct efect o improved

    phonological assembly. Alternatively, improved production

    could be a consequence o improved monitoring. Each pic-

    ture allows Helen to know the one syllable target which she

    can check and correct at a pre-speech production stage.

    Once production o the multi-syllabic word is enabled,

    Helen can go on to practise in a stimulus-responsemanner (stimulation). This strengthens the connec-

    tions between semantic and phonological processing

    giving the possibility o the therapy being efective

    through rehabilitation o impaired processes.

    Helen has rated eatures rom most to least important

    or her. (The comments are in Helens own words.)

    The picture is most important. I dont look at the let-

    ters at all. The picture opens it (the word) up or me.

    Quick results.

    The words are always there or me to check i Im not

    sure. Thats reassuring.

    The words are the ones I really need and want to

    have.

    I can practise on my own, even in my bed. It (the Dy-

    namo) is easy to carry around in my bag.Its interesting to show others what Im doing in thera-

    py and I can explain that its about pictures.

    Im not bothered whether its my own speech or not,

    but I am glad that it s not American.

    Our success illustrates that therapy to improve a production

    disorder can be efective many years post-onset. Although

    we dont know i this would have worked earlier or Helen, it

    has prompted Laorag to earlier and more creative trialling o

    stimulability in speech production with other clients.

    Flexible serviceAspects o our organisation and management have been

    vital in enabling this work. Our service is exible to deliver

    therapy according to client need and potential or change

    1.

    2.

    3.

    1.

    2.

    3.

    4.

    5.

    6.

    7.

    rather than within set time boundaries. The method devel-

    oped through dedicated time or continuing proessional

    development activities, particularly Laorags attendance

    at British Aphasiology Society conerences. O equal im-

    portance is a management commitment to provide andmaintain communication aids or clients.

    Client need, therapist knowledge and the availability o

    technology changes over time. In this therapy these vari-

    ables came together to give a rewarding outcome. Giving

    people with aphasia the opportunity to review therapy

    options could be o benet long ater their initial period

    o rehabilitation.

    Laorag Hunter is a speech and language therapist at the Centre

    or Brain Injury Rehabilitiation, Royal Victoria Hospital, Dundee,

    DD2 1SP, e-mail laorag.hunter@nhs.net. Helen Gowland is Chair

    o Tayside Speakability and a member o the Aphasia Scotland

    project steering group. Siobhan MacAndrew is a psychologist

    in the division o psychology at Aber tay University and Trevor

    Harley is Dean o School and Chair o Cognitive Psychologyat Dundee University and the author o The Psychology o

    Language (see reader ofer on inside ront cover).

    ReerencesDen Ouden, D.B. and Bastiaanse, R. (2005) Phonologicalencoding and conduction aphasia, in Hartsuiker, R.J., Bas-tiaanse, R., Postma, A. and Wijnen, F. (eds.) Phonological

    Encoding and Monitoring in Normal and Pathological Speech .Hove: Psychology Press, pp 86-101.Friedman, R.B., Sample, D.M. & Nitzberg Lott, S. (2002) The roleo representation in the use o paired associate learning or re-habilitation o alexia, Neuropsychologia 40, pp.223-234.

    Goodglass, H. and Kaplan, E. (1983) The assessment o aphasiaand related disorders. 2nd edn. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger.Harley, T.A. (2001) The psychology o language. 2nd edn.

    Hove: Psychology Press.Levelt, W.J.M. (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation.Cambridge MA: HIT Press.

    Malcomess, K. (2001) The Reason or Care, Bulletin o the Royal Col-lege o Speech and Language Therapists595 (November), pp.12-14.Mayer-Johnson (1981) Picture Communication Index. SolanaBeach CA.Nickels, L. (1997) Spoken word production and its breakdownin aphasia. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Nickels, L. (2002) Therapy or naming disorders: Revisiting,

    revising and reviewing,Aphasiology, 16(10/11), pp. 935-979.

    ResourcesThe British Aphasiology Society, www.bas.org.ukThe Dynamo Communication Aid is available rom Dynavox

    Systems Ltd, Sunrise Medical Building, High Street, Wollas-

    ton, West Midlands, DY8 4PS (www.dynavox.co.uk)

    COVER STORY: THERAPY

    SPEECH & LANGUAGE THERAPY IN PRACTICE Summer 2007 9

    SLTP

    by syllable. Ater listening a ew times and considering

    the image associated with each syllable Helen is usually

    able within the session to record the target word as a

    whole item on the Dynamo page. At this point, her pro-

    duction oten automatically alters to accommodate thenormal stress pattern o the target word. It is as i Helens

    memory or production o the word is reactivated.

    When Helen eels condent that she has captured the

    production she practises and celebrates by playing with

    the word. Around Christmas time she delighted in exclaim-

    ing Have a piece o Stollen, Do you like Stollen?, Would

    you care or some Stollen? She also conesses to continu-

    ing this practice by talking to hersel on the bus ride home.

    Helen is comortable to delete words rom the Dynamo

    once she is satised that she can independently recall the

    graphic or the single syllables which subsequently cue

    her spoken production o the whole multi-syllabic word.

    When a previously used syllable occurs in a new target

    word Helen sometimes eels the addition o the graphicin the Dynamo is unnecessary and the written orm o the

    syllable appears alone. Occasionally Helen requests that a

    Picture Communication Symbol is changed, i she nds it

    doesnt mean anything. For example trail to represent

    the second syllable in Australia was changed rom a path

    to a child trailing a pull-along toy.

    Therapy sessions are approximately bimonthly and no

    preparation is required by the therapist secondary to ses-

    sions. Helen eels this schedule is appropriate to establish

    new vocabulary and it also accommodates Helens ac-

    tivities including volunteer work, committee work, amily

    commitments and holidays. Four to six target words are

    added each session and in 17 months Helen has mastered

    the production o over 70 target multi-syllabic words.

    Helen is now occasionally able to generate images or twosyllable words independently o therapy sessions.

    Sel-generated cuesHelens ability to say target words and use these in real com-

    municative situations shows notable increase. Additionally,

    there are signs o carryover in her use o sel-generated

    cues. Within sessions Helen has become aster at produc-

    ing target words. Qualitative changes are also apparent in

    Helens reections. Helen has a sense o achievement and

    eeling o progression as opposed to previously eeling

    stuck. The positive outcome or Helen is also clear as she

    describes imagined ears, or example, what i we hadnt

    tried this?, what i you had discharged me?, what i you

    have to take the Dynamo away?

    REFLECTIONS

    DO I RECOGNISE THAT STABLECAN FEEL STUCK?

    DO I OFFER A SERVICE BOUNDBY ARBITRARY TIME LIMITSOR FLEXIBLE TO A CLIENTSCHANGING NEED?

    DO I EVER THINK ABOUTWRITING AN ARTICLE WITH ACLIENT?

top related