work motivation: theory, practice, and future directions motivation.pdf · this chapter focuses on...

116
Work Motivation 1 Running Head: WORK MOTIVATION Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Ruth Kanfer Georgia Institute of Technology December, 2010 To be published in: Kozlowski, S. W. J. (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Co. Send all correspondence to: Ruth Kanfer School of Psychology 654 Cherry St., MC 0170 Georgia Institute of Technology Atlanta, GA 30332-0170 USA E-mail: [email protected] Telephone: 404-894-5674 Fax: 404-252-3061

Upload: vothuan

Post on 30-Aug-2018

218 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

1

Running Head: WORK MOTIVATION

Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions

Ruth Kanfer

Georgia Institute of Technology

December, 2010

To be published in:

Kozlowski, S. W. J. (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Industrial and Organizational Psychology.

Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Co.

Send all correspondence to: Ruth Kanfer School of Psychology 654 Cherry St., MC 0170 Georgia Institute of Technology Atlanta, GA 30332-0170 USA E-mail: [email protected] Telephone: 404-894-5674 Fax: 404-252-3061

Page 2: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

2

Abstract

This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired research on motivation

related to adult work. The chapter is divided into four sections. The first section reviews basic

motivation constructs and processes, and the issues that delineate the scope and content of the

field. The second section reports on research progress and the implications of new

conceptualizations for understanding and predicting work behaviors and performance. The third

section reviews findings on the major determinants of work motivation, organized into three

broad categories: Content (person variables), Context (situation variables), and Change (temporal

variables). The fourth and final section identifies current gaps in our knowledge, practical

challenges, and promising new research directions.

Keywords: Employee motivation, self-regulation, goals, implicit motives, self-efficacy, self-

determination, motivational dynamics, motivational traits, trait-performance relations, work

environment

Page 3: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

3

Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions

Introduction

Work motivation is arguably one of the most vibrant areas in work and organizational

psychology today. Since 1990 there has been astonishing progress on many topics that were

largely undeveloped two decades ago (see Kanfer, 1990). The most important change in the field

pertains to the way that dissimilar theories and research streams have coalesced around the goal

construct to form a complex, but viable "big picture" of motivation related to work. As the broad

outlines of this picture continue to become clearer, more researchers are developing and testing

formulations that integrate different portions of the motivation domain. Organizational scientists

have also sought out and incorporated advances in allied fields of psychology, economics,

communications, and sociology to help fill in remaining gaps in our understanding of

nonconscious processes and the impact of multi-level, multi-faceted social contexts on work

motivation and behaviors. New research methodologies and analytic methods have been adopted

that permit study of motivation over time and the analysis of motivation as it is embedded in

ongoing work relationships, teams, organizations, and the employee life-course. Theories

developed during the mid-20th century (that continued to dominate the scene in the 1980s) have

been transformed into formulations that provide a better fit to the scientific questions and

organizational concerns salient in the early 21st century. Prior questions about the applicability

of work motivation findings to real-world problems have been replaced by questions about work

motivation driven by real-world problems in specific contexts. New views of the self, affect, and

the context in which work motivation operates have also spurred the development of new

research programs, many of which have adopted a person-centered perspective. The result of this

Page 4: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

4

activity is a field that looks very little like the field in the 1980s. Although our understanding of

work motivation remains far from complete, the field is clearly on the move.

Overview

The purpose of this chapter is to review advances and emerging trends in work

motivation over the past few decades, and to identify promising topics for future work. Reviews

that provide greater detail on earlier developments in the field are available from a number of

sources. Historically-oriented reviews of the field through the latter part of the 20th century are

provided by Ambrose and Kulik (1999), Campbell and Pritchard (1976), Katzell and Thompson

(1990), Kanfer (1990, 1992), Kanfer, Chen, and Pritchard (2008), Latham (2007), Latham and

Pinder (2005), Mitchell and Daniels (2003), and Pinder (2008). In addition, a number of reviews

organized around specific theoretical perspectives are available, including qualitative reviews

and meta-analyses on goal setting (Austin & Klein, 1996; Locke, Shaw, Saari, & Latham, 1981),

self-regulation (Lord, Diefendorff, Schmidt, & Hall, 2010), goal orientation (Payne, Youngcourt,

& Beaubien, 2007), expectancy-value and decision theories (Klein, Austin, & Cooper, 2008;

Mitchell, 1974, 1982), organizational justice (Colquitt, Conlon, Wesson, Porter, & Ng, 2001;

Folger & Cropanzano, 1998), self-determination theory (Gagne & Deci, 2005), and work design

(Fried & Ferris, 1987; Humphrey, Nahrgang, & Morgeson, 2007; Parker & Ohly, 2008).

Reviews are also available on the role of motivation in specific situations and settings, including

for example in teams (Chen & Gogus, 2008; Chen & Kanfer, 2006; Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006;

Salas, Cooke, & Rosen, 2008), leadership (Zacarro, Hildebrand, & Nelson, 2008), job search

(Kanfer, Wanberg, & Kantrowitz, 2001), workforce aging (Kanfer, 2009; Shultz & Adams,

2007), and in learning and skill training (Beier & Kanfer, 2009; Colquitt, LePine, & Noe, 2000).

Page 5: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

5

Erez (2008) provides a review of social-cultural influences on work motivation and Gelfand,

Erez, and Aycan (2007) provide a review of work motivation across cultures.

The chapter is organized into four sections. The first section highlights foundational

issues and a work motivation definition that delineates the broad scope and content of the field.

The second section describes scientific progress on basic motivational processes, and their

relationships to outcomes of individual and organizational interest (e.g., behavior, sense of

competency, job performance). The third section describes recent findings on major determinants

of work motivation, organized into three broad categories: Content (person variables), Context

(situation variables), and Change (temporal variables). The fourth section identifies some of the

current gaps in our knowledge, and promising new research directions for the study of work

motivation over the next few decades.

Foundations for Theory and Research in Work Motivation

In the narrow sense, the study of work motivation examines the psychological processes

and mechanisms by which individuals form and commit to work-related goals, formulate plans

for goal accomplishment, allocate personal resources across a range of possible actions, and

regulate thoughts, behaviors, and affect for the purpose of goal attainment. Although there has

been a tendency to view work motivation as a cognitive phenomena, modern research makes it

clear that motivational processes are not just cognitive; they are supported by and involve

biological processes, unconscious perceptions, sensations, affect, and cognitions.

More broadly, the study of work motivation also includes theory and research on the

person and situation factors that influence motivation processes, and the pathways by which they

do so. Consistent with the Latin root of the word motivation meaning "to move," work

motivation researchers and scientists seek to understand the internal and external forces that

Page 6: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

6

facilitate or hinder behavior change. Work motivation is also a topic of great practical

importance, and includes work on the consequences of motivation for employees and the

organizations they work in. Work motivation research focuses on multiple dimensions of

behavior change, including the direction, intensity, and persistence of workplace actions and job

performance within the broader, continuing stream of experiences that characterize the person in

relation to his or her work (Kanfer, 1990).

Pinder (1998) provides an encompassing definition of work motivation as "a set of

energetic forces that originate both within as well as beyond an individual's being, to initiate

work-related behavior and to determine its form, direction, intensity, and duration" (p. 11).

Consistent with definitions of motivation found in many areas of psychology, definitions of work

motivation emphasize the following points:

1. Motivation is not directly observed and must be inferred.

Because motivation cannot be directly assessed, changes in motivation are inferred by

associated changes in behavior, learning, or task/job performance. The use of performance

measures to index motivation, however, is often problematic since performance is not univocally

determined by motivation, and is also determined by employee knowledge and skills and/or the

availability (or lack) of external resources (e.g., equipment) necessary for successful

performance. The use of performance ratings or scores to index motivation importantly depends

on the extent to which changes in motivation are directly reflected in changes in performance.

In general, performance measures to index motivation are appropriate in contexts where

task performance is effort-sensitive—that is, changes in effort produce proportional changes in

performance. However, when changes in motivation affect performance through effects on

cognitions, behavior, or affect, performance indices of motivation are less appropriate. In these

Page 7: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

7

situations, the more appropriate criteria are those changes in behavior, cognitions, and/or affect

that are the direct consequence of a change in motivation. Thus, researchers often use multiple

measures of behavior (e.g., time spent on a task) as well as performance score to index a change

in motivation. In the job search literature, for example, motivation may be assessed by time spent

on job search activities (persistence), the type of job search activities performed (direction), and

self-reports of effort expended on job search (intensity). In studies of motivation during training,

motivation is often assessed in terms of self-report measures that assess the individual's goal

commitment and self-efficacy, and/or behavioral measures of attendance, task persistence, and

self-regulatory activities.

2. Outcomes of motivation include changes in the initiation, direction, intensity,

modulation, or persistence of action.

The type of measure used to assess motivation depends upon the question of interest.

When the question is about how motives or contexts "turn on" or instigate work-related

behaviors, researchers may use initiation measures. The impact of achievement motives on

innovative performance, for example, may be evaluated by assessing the frequency an employee

asks questions or seeks information. Choice and intention measures are often used to assess the

direction of action. Intensity indices capture the proportion of an individual's personal resources

allocated to a goal or task. Intensity measures often assess subjective or subjective task effort,

energy, time spent on the task, or other personal resources directed to task performance.

Persistence measures represent assessments of the duration that an individual allocates personal

resources to a particular task or action. As such, persistence integrates direction and the temporal

dimension of intensity.

Page 8: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

8

3. Motivation emerges as a consequence of the person-in-context; motivation is not

As Pinder's (1998) definition indicates, the forces that influence motivation occur both

within as well as externally to the individual. That is, although the psychological processes

involved in work motivation occur internally, motivation is not a stable characteristic of the

person across all situations. Individuals who show high levels of motivation for action in one

situation (e.g., read a novel), may show low levels of motivation for action another situation

(e.g., read a textbook). The purpose of action and the context in which it occurs must always be

taken into account.

univocally or consistently determined by a single person attribute or feature of the environment.

Motivation is also not simply a function of the environment. Even in extremely "strong"

situations (e.g., the battlefield), individual differences in person attributes, such as tolerance for

ambiguity, contribute to motivation and performance. In the moderate or weak situations that

characterize most workplaces, motivation depends on individual propensities and preferences as

well as situational affordances and constraints. As Lewin (1938) suggested over a half century

ago, "motivation can only be properly analyzed by taking into account characteristics of the

person in the context of dynamic social, physical, and psychological environments that facilitate

and constrain person tendencies for action."

4. Motivation is always in flux.

Motivation is a state that changes constantly. Changes in motivation, with associated

changes in beliefs, behaviors, and affect occur over different time cycles (Lord et al., 2010) and

are often measured on different timescales, depending on the question of interest (Kanfer et al.,

2008). Lord et al. (2010) propose four major cycle levels for motivational phenomena that

correspond to the measurement of motivation processes and outcomes on different timescales.

Page 9: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

9

Examples of the different cycles, their associated measurement timescales and the dominant

methods used at different levels are shown in Figure 1.

-------------------------------

Insert Figure 1 about here

-------------------------------

As shown in Figure 1, intra-individual cognitive and affective processes involved in

motivation are typically fast and assessed in terms of milliseconds or minutes. Studies at this

level of analysis are typically conducted in controlled, laboratory conditions. In contrast, studies

of work motivation conducted over very long time frames, such as months, years, or decades,

capture information about the influence of the broader social and work culture and relatively

stable person traits and tendencies on motivation and the trajectories of work behaviors and

performance over the life course.

Most work motivation researchers and professionals focus on motivation changes that

occur during relatively short timescales (e.g., hours, days, weeks). Analyses at this level capture

the impact of proximal person influences (e.g., work goals) and environmental conditions (e.g.,

job autonomy) on motivation, behavior and job performance. Studies at this level are both

logistically feasible and practically useful for examining the effects of specific organizational

interventions, such as goal setting, on motivation and performance.

Because motivational processes take place on different cycles or timescales, multi-level

models are also needed to examine how factors and processes that operate at one level may

influence another level. Changes in the organizational culture over the course of a year, for

example, are likely to affect the value that is placed on different aspects of employee

performance, which in turn affect the individual's work and task goal choices. Cross-level effects

Page 10: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

10

between motivational processes that operate on different timescales may be indirect, such as

when changes in organizational culture promote the adoption of worker goals that facilitate

learning and higher levels of competence. Cross-level effects may also be moderated by short

cycle motivation processes, such as when employees who are highly anxious develop a response

of adopting performance goals (rather than learning goals) that prompt the use of less effective

learning strategies. Multi-level studies of work motivation offer an exciting opportunity for

systematizing knowledge about the dynamics of multi-scale motivation processes and their

effects on work behavior and job attitudes.

Work motivation is a unique branch of motivational science. Work motivation is not just

a subordinate section of the larger field of motivational science. In other branches of human

motivation, such as achievement motivation, research is often organized and accumulated around

a single theoretical perspective, and the context for research is often driven by the target

theoretical issue. In work motivation, however, research is also driven by the underlying

practical concern for how motivation influences organizationally-relevant work behaviors, such

as job performance. Over the past few decades, changes in the conceptualization of the criterion

space have spurred new theories of work motivation. In contrast to previous formulations that

focused on motivational influences on technical performance, such as speed and quality of

production, the changing nature of work has focused more attention on the relationship between

motivation and nontechnical or contextual dimensions of job performance, including for

example, the quality of an employee's relationships with coworkers and clients, organizational

citizenship behaviors, and adaptability to changes in job demands, organizational structure, and

the external marketplace. The shift in the dimensions of performance to be predicted has, in turn,

encouraged the development of new perspectives that address motivational effects on these

Page 11: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

11

outcomes. On the negative side, there has been a sharp increase in the number of criterion-

specific work motivation "mini-theories.” On the positive side, the enlarged criterion space has

stimulated connections with other fields, such as affective science, personality psychology, and

even mental health.

Is there more to work motivation than we can tell? A final foundational issue pertains to

the growing interest in implicit motives and nonconscious processes as they influence work

motivation and behavior. The notion of nonconscious influences on motivation is certainly not

new to psychology, but has received little research attention in work motivation until recently.

Over the past few decades, however, research on implicit motives and nonconscious processes in

other areas of psychology has begun to influence mainstream work motivation theory and

research (see, e.g., James, 1998; Johnson & Steinman, 2009; Kehr, 2004; Latham, Stajkovic, &

Locke, 2010; Lord et al., 2010). The impetus for renewed attention to nonconscious motives and

processes stems from advances that show: (a) self-report measures of individual differences in

nonability traits and action preferences do not capture important person influences on

motivation, and (b) not all motivation processes are cognitively-mediated. In accord with these

advances, recent progress in I/O psychology appears in two areas: (a) the assessment of

individual differences in implicit motives, and (b) theory building on the relationship between

implicit motives and nonconscious processes and goal selection and pursuit.

Implicit motives. In contrast to explicit motives, implicit motives are not accessible for

conscious self-report. Individual differences in explicit motives, such as extraversion, are

typically assessed using self-report measures in which persons report their behavioral tendencies

and outcome preferences. Such self-reports are cognitively-mediated in that the individual

reports what they think best describes them. In contrast, implicit motives are not accessible

Page 12: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

12

through direct self-report since these motives do not reflect cognitively-mediated action

tendencies, but rather affectively-charged motivators that are activated by intrinsic outcomes

associated with action. In contrast to explicit motives, whose impact is typically on cognitively-

mediated behaviors, implicit motives are further posited to influence spontaneous, rather than

cognitively-mediated, behaviors (Michalak, Puschel, Joormann, & Schulte, 2006). Individuals

who are high in implicit power motive, for example, would be expected to demonstrate more

spontaneous power-oriented behaviors during the performance of tasks that afford the

opportunity for acquiring influence over others than individuals low in implicit motive for

power.

Modern conceptions of implicit motives build upon arguments made by McClelland

(1987) that the low correlation often obtained between direct (self-report) and indirect

(projective; e.g. TAT) measures of individual differences in the achievement motive reflected the

difference between implicit and explicit motives for achievement, rather than measurement error.

McClelland (1987) further proposed three implicit motives: achievement, affiliation, and power,

and argued that each of these motives were distinct from explicit motives of the same or similar

names. After years of controversy, evidence to support McClelland's (1987) notions regarding

differences between implicit and explicit motives related to achievement was provided in a meta-

analytic review by Spangler (1992).

Nonetheless, measurement problems continue to thwart progress in the study of implicit

motives. Well-founded criticisms of projective and quasi-projective measures, such as the

Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), hampered research for decades. In the past 15 years or so,

however, new theoretical approaches have been employed to develop measures of nonconscious

motives through perceptual and cognitive processing (see Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz,

Page 13: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

13

1998; James & Rentsch, 2004; Schultheiss & Pang, 2007). One of the most promising new

methods for assessing implicit motive strength builds upon Conditional Reasoning Theory (CRT;

James, 1998). In CRT measures, individuals read a series of constructed scenarios and select a

response based on conditional reasoning, rather than based on affective reactions to a stimuli.

Initial evidence for the validity of these measures and the basic tenets of CRT are provided by a

series of studies by James and his colleagues (Frost, Ko, & James, 2007; James et al., 2005) that

show negligible correlations between CRT and explicit measures, and significant predictive

validity of CRT measures for achievement, aggressive, and dominant behaviors in organized

settings. Further evidence on the validity of these implicit motive measures will significantly

speed progress in implicit motivation.

Nonconscious processes. A second, complimentary stream of research focuses on the

delineation of a nonconscious motivation system and its relationship to explicit goal choice and

goal striving. Most evidence for the existence of nonconscious motivational system comes from

research findings in cognitive neuroscience and social psychology (see Ferguson, Hassin, &

Bargh, 2008). Findings in these areas show the influence of goals on preconscious attentional

processes in sensory systems, the influence of subliminal priming on nonconscious motivational

processing and action, and the impact of nonconscious neurological processes in explicit goal

choice and self-regulation (see Bargh, Gollwitzer, & Oettingen, 2010; Ruud & Aarts, 2010).

Recent work by Lord and his colleagues (Johnson, Lord, Rosen, & Chang, 2007; Johnson,

Tolentino, Rodopman, & Cho, 2010; Lord & Moon, 2006) and others (e.g., Stajkovic, Locke, &

Blair, 2006) have extended this work into the organizational domain, and have shown that fast,

nonconscious, automatic cognitive processes also affect explicit motivational processes relevant

in the work setting (see Diefendorff & Lord, 2008).

Page 14: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

14

Another line of inquiry focuses on the relationship between implicit and explicit

motivational processes (e.g., Brunstein & Maier, 2005; Schultheiss & Brunstein, 2001). Lord et

al. (2010) have proposed a dynamic model in which nonconscious motives and processes may

exert influence at multiple levels in motivational processing, including goal choice as well as

self-regulation. Kehr (2004) has proposed that implicit motives interfere with explicit motivation

when the implicit motive fails to support explicit goals. According to Kehr (2004), a basic

purpose for the instigation of goal striving, or self-regulatory processes is to prevent contrary

implicit motive tendencies from diverting critical resources away from goal accomplishment.

Rapid progress is being made in elucidating the influence of implicit motives and

nonconscious processes on explicit motivation and behavior (see Ruud & Aarts, 2010; Johnson

et al., 2010). The development of valid and reliable measures of implicit motives remains

problematic, but it is clear that new approaches based on cognitive and neuroscience advances

are overcoming problems that were for many decades insurmountable. At the same time, work

motivation researchers have begun to study how implicit motives and nonconscious processes

impact goal choice, self-regulation, and behavior. Continued progress in this area can be

expected to cause a sea-change in how work motivation is conceptualized and studied over the

next few decades.

Goal Choice and Goal Pursuit

The term motivation is often used in work and organizational psychology to encompass

all the processes by which individuals formulate and execute established goals. In motivational

science however, a distinction is often made between the processes involved in goal choice and

goal commitment, and the processes involved in goal pursuit. Among some researchers,

motivation refers to the choice portion of the system, while cognitive, affective and self-

Page 15: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

15

regulatory processes in goal pursuit are regarded as volition. Consistent with the common use of

the term motivation, I distinguish the two, related motivational subsystems in terms of goal

choice and goal striving (or pursuit) and reserve the term motivation to reference both goal

choice and goal striving processes.

Over the past few decades motivational scientists have come to a consensus on the

organization of explicit (or conscious) motivation processes. This consensus has been achieved

by using goals as the coordinating construct for disparate streams of research on goal choice and

goal striving. Although some theories remain better suited to understanding and predicting goal

choice and other theories to understanding and predicting goal striving, there has been a sharp

rise in the number of studies that simultaneously examine elements of both systems (e.g.,

Kozlowski & Bell, 2006).

To put recent developments in perspective, I first provide a brief review of late 20th

century progress on goals, goal choice, goal setting, and goal striving. Following this review, I

describe recent work on goal orientation and related perspectives.

Goals. Goals are the mental representations of outcome states that an individual seeks to

realize. In the workplace, goals may refer to learning outcomes (e.g., learn to install a pipe),

performance outcomes (e.g., design a webpage), or consequence outcomes (e.g., obtain a

registered nursing degree). Goals direct attention and help to organize and sustain the

individual's effort and actions for the purpose of goal accomplishment.

Because goals direct behavior toward the accomplishment of desired outcomes, including

job performance, work motivation researchers have studied two goal-related issues in-depth:

Goal selection (the goal that an individual adopts) and goal commitment (the extent to which the

individual binds him/herself to goal accomplishment). In these areas, research has focused on a

Page 16: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

16

number of related questions, including the role of person and situation variables on goal adoption

(e.g., Barrick, Mount, & Strauss, 1993; see Hollenbeck & Klein, 1987), how individuals allocate

attention and effort across multiple goals (DeShon, Kozlowski, Schmidt, Milner, & Wiechmann,

2004; see Mitchell, Harmon, Lee, & Lee, 2008), and how and why individuals revise their goals

(Schmidt & DeShon, 2007; Tolli & Schmidt, 2008).

Research in cognitive and personality-social psychology indicates that goals are rarely

developed in isolation. Rather, goals are situated in a web of complex, interrelated goal

hierarchies. Goals at the top of the hierarchy represent outcomes that occur as a consequence of

accomplishing goals at lower levels in the hierarchy. Earning a medical degree, for example,

requires accomplishment of lower-order goals distributed over time, such as passing different

courses required for the degree. Higher-level goals are typically distal, complex, and may be ill-

defined with respect to the lower-order goals required for higher-order goal accomplishment.

The adoption of a higher-order consequence goal, such as attaining a medical degree, sets into

motion an integrated stream of cognitive and motivational processes that direct attention and

action toward interim or lower-order goal accomplishments. Problems encountered in the

execution of lower-order goals may also redirect attention to the higher order goal, and

contribute to outcome goal revision or abandonment.

Goals are also distinguished in terms of their attributes and/or focus. Goals may be

specific or vague, easy or difficult, simple or complex, behavioral, cognitive, or affective,

proximal or distal, or adopted for different reasons (e.g., to demonstrate competence or avoid

appearing incompetent). Different theories of work motivation, such as Locke's (1976) task goal

theory and VandeWalle's (1997) goal orientation formulation, emphasize different aspects of the

Page 17: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

17

articulated goal that, in turn, has different implications for motivational processing and

performance.

Goal choice. Cognitive theories of motivation, such as Fishbein and Ajzen's (1975)

Theory of Reasoned Action, Triandis' (1980) Theory of Interpersonal Behavior, and Vroom's

(1964) Expectancy-Value formulation (VIE) are frequently used to predict behavior intentions,

goal choice, and motivational force for goal accomplishment, respectively. Although these and

related models differ in a number of ways, such as how the criterion is operationalized, the role

of affect, and the way that social influences are represented, each model has its roots in the

family of expectancy-value models developed first in economics and subsequently adopted in

psychology during the mid-1900s.

Expectancy value formulations make several strong assumptions. These models assume

that individuals are rational decision-makers who choose among perceived courses of action by

applying the hedonic principle of maximizing pleasure and minimizing pain. As rational decision

makers, an individual's goal choice or behavior intention decision is posited to reflect the

outcome of an internal, cognitive analysis regarding the relative costs and benefits associated

with different choice options. For example, according to Vroom's (1964) VIE theory, individuals

choose among different possible work goals based on their subjective perceptions about: (a)

whether the goal can be accomplished with the effort and other personal resources available to

the individual (e.g., can I accomplish the performance goal?; Expectancy), (b) whether goal

accomplishment will bring about the target outcome (e.g., the instrumentality of achieving the

performance goal for obtaining a pay raise; Instrumentality), and (c) the perceived valence of the

performance outcome (e.g., the attractiveness of a pay raise, the unattractiveness of feeling

fatigued as a result of sustained mental effort; Valence).

Page 18: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

18

The introduction of Vroom's expectancy-value formulation into the I/O literature in the

mid-1960s stimulated decades of empirical research on theoretical and methodological aspects of

expectancy-value formulations. Excellent reviews of the empirical evidence on the predictive

validity of expectancy value theories and methodological issues are provided by Mitchell (1974,

1980) and Sheppard, Hartwick, and Warshaw (1988). Overall, empirical findings on expectancy

value formulations indicate that these theories are most effective for predicting choice among

mutually incompatible courses of action, such as which of several job offers to accept.

As Mitchell (1974, 1980) indicates, VIE and related models suffer from both conceptual

and methodological problems. Some of the biggest problems with theory and research in

expectancy-value research pertain to the episodic nature of the theories, the use of between-

subject designs to test a within-subject formulation, and the inability of the theories to account

for motivation processes that occur in the gap between intention and behavior. Although tests

using within-subject designs show improved predictive validity for behavior and task

performance, expectancy-value formulations remain more useful for predicting discrete choice

than performance streams. An even broader criticism of expectancy-value theories involves the

assumption that individuals are rational decision-makers. There is substantial evidence in allied

fields that show individuals do not choose goals after performing a full rational analysis of

options or solely on the basis of maximizing positive outcomes. Image theory (Beach &

Mitchell, 1987) addresses this and other issues using a match-type model where individuals are

proposed to make decisions based on the compatibility match between images related to goals

and strategy with self-concept. Although there has been some empirical support for Image

Theory, this formulation is not often used in work motivation research.

Page 19: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

19

Later expectancy value formulations by Naylor, Pritchard, and Ilgen (1980) and Kanfer

and Ackerman (1989) incorporated advances from cognitive psychology. Extending earlier

expectancy value models, the Naylor et al. (1980) and Kanfer and Ackerman (1989) portrayed

personal resources as multidimensional, including not only attentional effort, and time, but

potentially other personal resources such as social capital. In addition, these formulations

conceptualized choice as a personal resource allocation process across a range of activities,

including self-regulatory processes and nontask activities. The conceptualization of choice as a

continuous resource allocation process that occurs as a function of person attributes, task

demands, self-regulatory and nontask activities, improved the viability of these models for

predicting ongoing behavior and skill acquisition (see Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989).

Despite the many criticisms of classic expectancy value formulations, modified

formulations remain popular. Social psychological models, such as Ajzen's (1991) theory of

planned behavior, that take into explicit account the social context in which behavioral intentions

are formed, continue to attract research attention, particularly in the prediction of discrete

behaviors, such as attendance and turnover (see Armitage & Conner, 2001; Fishbein & Ajzen,

2010). Resource allocation models also continue to be used in studies investigating multiple

goals, goal revision processes, self-regulatory activities, and performance over time in the

context of skill acquisition and teams. Using the Naylor, Pritchard, and Ilgen (1980) formulation,

Pritchard and his colleagues have also shown the efficacy of resource allocation models in the

development of programs to enhance work motivation and performance in organizational

settings (see Pritchard, Harrell, DiazGranados, & Guzman, 2008).

Goal setting. As criticisms of expectancy-value formulations in I/O psychology and

related fields mounted during the 1970s, work motivation researchers turned their attention to

Page 20: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

20

goal setting. Consistent with other cognitive formulations, Locke (1968) proposed that goals

served as the immediate regulators of action, and that difficult, specific goals led to higher levels

of performance than easy, nonspecific goals. Goal setting research findings reviewed by Locke

et al. (1981) provided strong empirical support for the beneficial influence goal setting on task

performance, particularly with respect to the setting of difficult and specific goals. Locke et al.

(1981) proposed that goals influenced performance by: (a) directing attention and action, (b)

mobilizing effort, (c) prolonging goal-directed effort over time, and (d) motivating the individual

to develop effective strategies for goal attainment.

Goal setting theory and research flourished during the late 20th century, and goal setting

rapidly eclipsed expectancy value as the dominant work motivation paradigm in the field. A

large number of empirical studies provided general support for each of the four proposed

mechanisms by which goals influenced performance, and goal setting theory quickly expanded

to incorporate self-regulation constructs and processes in the explanation of how goals

influenced task performance (Locke & Latham, 1990).

As goal setting theory and research progressed, attention began to focus on potential

boundary conditions associated with the beneficial effects of goal setting on performance. The

first issue pertained to observed differences between the differential influence of assigned vs.

self-set goals (see Locke & Schweiger, 1979). In most work settings, employees are assigned

task goals. In these instances, the issue is not predicting the direction of action, but rather what

factors predict the employee's willingness to adopt the assigned goal and the employee's

commitment or intensity of personal resource allocation to the goal. Findings by Latham, Erez,

and Locke (1988) showed the criticality of the employee's participation in the goal setting

process, even if only minimally when supervisors employed a "sell" rather than "tell" procedure,

Page 21: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

21

for goal setting to exert a positive effect on performance. The findings on the effects of

psychological participation in goal setting, in turn, spurred a new stream of theory and research

on goal commitment.

A second condition under which goal setting might be less effective pertains the

complexity of the task. Review findings by Wood, Mento, and Locke (1987) showed that goal

setting was more effective in enhancing motivation and performance when used with simple

tasks than with complex tasks. Using a complex air traffic simulation task, Kanfer and Ackerman

(1989) showed that the impact of performance goal setting on performance might be beneficial

or detrimental depending on the attentional demands of the task, the individual's cognitive

abilities, and the self-regulatory activities undertaken as a result of goal setting. In accord with

predictions, they found that among individuals with lower levels of cognitive abilities, the

provision of performance goal assignments during the early (cognitively demanding) phase of

skill learning was detrimental to performance, but that goal assignments made later in skill

acquisition (when the task was cognitively less demanding) had a beneficial effect on

performance.

The differential impact of goal setting on performance as a function of individual

differences in cognitive abilities, task demands, and self-regulatory activities focused attention

on the characteristics of goals that might divert attentional resources and hinder task

performance. Kanfer and Ackerman (1989) argued that performance goals implemented early in

training diverted critical attentional resources needed for performance toward the management of

negative emotions associated with inability to accomplish the goal. This notion was supported in

subsequent studies by Latham and his colleagues (Seijts & Latham, 2005; Latham & Brown,

2006; Winters & Latham, 1996) who showed that the provision of learning goals (that reduced

Page 22: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

22

rather than exacerbated concern about performance accomplishment) in complex task

performance exerted a greater beneficial effect on performance than performance goals. These

findings are also consistent with goal orientation theory and research that shows the purpose of

action (embedded in this paradigm as goal type), exerts a motivational influence on performance

through its effects on the direction of resource allocations during action.

Goal striving. Goal choice sets the stage for action, but when intentions cannot be

readily accomplished individuals activate self-regulatory processes to support goal-directed

action. Goal striving refers to the self-regulatory processes and actions by which individuals

support goal intentions over time and/or in the face of personal or environmental obstacles to

goal accomplishment.

Theory and research on self-regulation processes has its modern origins in the expansion

of behavioral models during the 1960s and 1970s (Bandura, 1969, 1973; Kanfer & Phillips,

1969; Mischel, 1968; Mischel & Ebbesen, 1970). Social learning, cognitive-behavioral, and

social-cognitive models of action all emphasized the self-regulatory processes by which

individuals exercise control over their behavior, affect, and cognitions for purposes of goal

attainment. Self-control is a form of self-regulation in which the individual seeks to attain a goal

that is not supported by prevailing environmental contingencies, such as when an employee

seeks to write a report while coworkers are holding a party, or when an employee's goal for

completing a project requires that he/she work late into the night despite growing fatigue. Meta-

cognition, defined by Flavell (1979) as the metacognitive knowledge and experiences used to

manage cognitions, affect, and behavior strategies in goal-directed learning, represents another

subset of the broader domain of self-regulation, often studied in the educational psychology.

Page 23: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

23

In work and organizational psychology, conscious self-regulation of action is typically

not required when goals can be readily accomplished, or involve habitual or highly routinized

behaviors, such as typing a letter. However, when goal accomplishment require planning

(studying for a test), coordination of actions (making a presentation), mid-course adjustments in

strategy (e.g., winning a race), or resisting environmental presses that run counter to goal

accomplishment (working instead of going out with friends), self-regulatory processes facilitate

sustained motivation and performance. In the modern workplace, where work often takes place

in nonwork contexts (e.g., home), involves self-management of emotions in dealing with clients

and coworkers, and is directed toward the accomplishment of complex goals over time, effective

self-regulation has become an important feature of performance.

Investigation of self-regulatory processes during the mid-20th century delineated three

key subprocesses: self-monitoring, self-evaluations, and self-reactions. Self-monitoring refers to

the individual's attention to the outcomes of one's actions related to the goal. Self-monitoring is

necessary for self-evaluation and self-reactions, though what the individual monitors may or may

not be relevant for goal attainment. Self-evaluations refer to cognitive processes that determine

goal progress. Self-evaluation processes serve both an information and motivational function,

providing information about whether the current action strategy must be adjusted for goal

attainment and whether personal resource allocations are sufficient. Self-reactions, the most

well-known of which is self-efficacy, pertain to the individual's integrated cognitive-affective

judgment of confidence regarding the likelihood of goal attainment.

Broad interest in self-regulation among organizational scientists during the late 20th

century also spurred the development of many research programs on the influence of different

self-regulation components, such as self-monitoring and self-efficacy judgments. In 1983,

Page 24: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

24

Ashford and Cummings proposed a theory of feedback seeking based on the notion that feedback

seeking from others was a primary means by which employees self-monitor their performance.

Consistent with self-regulation approaches, Ashford and Cummings (1983) showed that

individuals who did not seek feedback from others (for fear of obtaining negative feedback)

developed less accurate perceptions of performance than individuals who used active feedback

seeking strategies. From a motivational perspective, the Ashford and Cummings (1983) findings

suggest that problems in goal striving may begin early in the self-regulatory sequence if

individuals do not accurately monitor their performance.

Interest in self-regulation and goal striving among work motivation researchers also grew

in response to findings that showed goals and behavioral intentions were often only weakly

associated with performance, particularly when performance was difficult or occurred over a

protracted time period. Early studies on the role of self-regulation in work motivation and

performance were conducted by Latham and his colleagues (Frayne & Latham, 1987; Latham &

Frayne, 1989) in the context of goal setting research. Latham and Frayne (1989) examined the

effectiveness of self-regulatory training as an adjunct to goal setting in a field study designed to

increase attendance among employees with chronic histories of absenteeism. They found that

employees who participated in self-regulatory skills training showed subsequently higher levels

of job attendance than employees who did not receive self-management training. Subsequent

research by Gist, Bavetta, and Stevens (1990) compared the relative efficacy of goal setting

alone and goal setting plus self-management training on transfer of training in a salary

negotiation task. Gist et al. (1990) found that individuals in the goal setting plus self-

management training condition showed greater transfer of training and overall performance than

individuals in the goal setting-alone condition. The theoretical integration of goal setting and

Page 25: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

25

self-regulation formulations during the late 1980s (see Locke & Latham, 1990) led to numerous

studies investigating the impact of self-efficacy on goal choice, commitment, and performance.

Most modern motivation theories accord self-efficacy judgments a major causal role in

determining work motivation and performance. Self-efficacy judgments, though powerful

predictors of action in novel contexts or early skill acquisition, have also been shown to exert

weaker causal influence on motivation and performance in the context of skill acquisition (e.g.,

Heggestad & Kanfer, 2005; Mitchell, Hopper, Daniels, George-Falvy, & James, 1994; see

Kanfer, 1993). One recent controversy about the relationship between self-efficacy judgments

and performance further suggests that our understanding about the role that self-efficacy plays at

different points in the motivation system may be incomplete. In essence, the argument pertains to

the relationship between self-efficacy judgments and performance. Although many studies show

that self-efficacy judgments are positively related to motivation and performance (see Bandura &

Locke, 2003), several studies have been reported that show a negative, rather than positive

relationship between self-efficacy and performance (Vancouver, Thomson, Tischner, & Putka,

2002; Vancouver, Thomson, & Williams, 2001; Yeo & Neal, 2006). Vancouver has argued that

these findings are consistent with cybernetic models of self-regulation that posit a negative (not

positive) relationship between self-efficacy and personal resource allocations when performance

meets or exceeds the goal, in much the same way that a thermostat turns off the heater when the

desired temperature is reached. Vancouver, More, & Yoder (2008) have recently proposed a

multiple goal process explanation for reconciliation of the discrepant findings. Schmidt and

Deshon (2009, 2010) provide strong empirical evidence for explanations that focus on the

conditions in which the relationship is examined, including prior task success and task

ambiguity. lthough the controversy continues, it appears that the discrepancies obtained in the

Page 26: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

26

sign of the self-efficacy-performance relationship reflect the influence of important, understudied

questions related to the dynamics between task conditions, the purpose of action, and goal

revision over time.

Purposive Approaches to Motivation

Over the past 15 years or so, work motivation researchers have concentrated on the

motivational and performance consequences of an individual's purpose or reason for goal

accomplishment. Whereas prior goal setting research focused largely on the impact of goal

attributes such as difficulty or specificity, purposive approaches focus on the mental orientation

that accompanies the formulation and pursuit of difficult, specific goals. In this section I describe

several streams of research in this developing paradigm. First, I describe two conceptualizations

that distinguish and connect the explicit goal choice and goal striving motivation subsystems;

namely, Gollwitzer's (Gollwitzer, 1990; Gollwitzer & Kinney, 1989) mind-set theory and Meyer,

Becker, and Vandenberghe’s (2004) integrative model of employment commitment and

motivation. Next, I discuss theory and research in intrinsic motivation. Finally, I review theory

and research on the impact of goal orientation on motivation processes and performance.

Purpose and the relationship between goal choice and goal striving. According to

Gollwitzer (1990, 2003), goal choice and goal striving involve different mindsets, or

motivational orientations. Individuals engaged in goal choice are proposed to employ a

deliberative mindset that emphasizes seeking accurate, unbiased information and using that

information to perform a cognitive appraisal of goal options. Following goal choice, however,

Gollwitzer argued there is a useful change in mindset from deliberative to implemental. He

proposed that during the planning and execution phases of goal-directed action (i.e., goal

Page 27: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

27

striving) individuals adopt an implementation mindset, characterized by selective attention to

information that supports the desirability of the chosen goal.

Findings from a series of studies by Gollwitzer (Gollwitzer & Kinney, 1989; Taylor &

Gollwitzer, 1995) and others (e.g., Armor & Taylor, 2003; Webb & Sheeran, 2008) provide

empirical support for the difference between motivational orientations held during goal choice

and goal striving. These studies show that individuals who hold a deliberative mindset are more

attentive to negative goal-related information than individuals who hold an implemental mindset.

Further, individuals who hold an implemental mindset report stronger illusions of control,

increased self-efficacy and more optimistic outcome expectations about goal accomplishment

than individuals who hold a deliberative mindset.

Gollwitzer's mindset theory underscores the long-standing distinction between goal

choice as a decisional process and goal striving as an action process. Mindset findings also

increase the salience of a second long-standing question in motivation science; namely, what

connects the two mindset processes? Heckhausen (1991) suggests that the strength of the goal

developed in the choice system is an important variable in determining the initiation and

maintenance of goal striving processes. One way that goal strength has been conceptualized in

the work motivation literature is in terms of goal commitment, or the "force that binds an

individual to a course of action that is of relevance to a particular target" (Meyer & Herscovitch,

2001).

Mindset theory emphasizes the differences in mental orientation associated with goal

choice and goal pursuit. In I/O psychology, however, theory and research has focused on the

factors and processes that bridge the two motivation subsystems and bind the individual to the

pursuit of selected goals; namely, goal commitment. The Meyer and Allen (1991) multi-

Page 28: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

28

dimensional conceptualization of commitment compliments mindset theory by distinguishing

among dimensions of commitment and their influence on goal striving. Specifically, Meyer and

Allen (1991) proposed three forms of commitment; affective commitment, or the binding of the

goal to action as a function of desire, normative commitment, or the steadfastness of goal pursuit

out of a sense of obligation to others, and continuance commitment, or the resoluteness of goal

persistence as a consequence of perceived costs for not adopting the goal or goal abandonment

(also see Meyer & Allen, 1997). Although different mindsets may be involved in goal choice and

implementation processes, the Meyer and Allen (1991) conceptualization suggests the

motivation for goal commitment may also affect the manner in which decisions are implemented

in action. Consistent with this notion, Meyer et al. (2004) have recently proposed an integrative

model of commitment and motivation that links different dimensions of commitment to different

goal purposes or foci. Although the Meyer et al. (2004) model is relatively new and in need of

empirical testing, the proposed linkages between goal purpose and commitment dimension have

important implications for practice. For example, in some work settings and collectivist cultures,

the formulation of goals based sense of obligation (e.g., finish assigned work on a team project

because others depend on this performance) may yield higher levels of continuance goal

commitment and stronger self-regulatory processes than goals formulated to maximize pleasure

and sustained through affective goal commitment (e.g., finish assigned work on a team project

because it is interesting and will bring about feelings of competence).

Intrinsic and Extrinsic Motivation

The notion that goals may be adopted for intrinsic and/or extrinsic purposes and, in turn,

affect goal pursuit is well-established in psychology, and serves as the foundation for theory and

research in intrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation refers to psychological processes involved

Page 29: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

29

in task performance for the purpose of enjoyment and interest; that is, tasks performed for their

own sake. In contrast, extrinsic motivation typically refers to processes involved in task

performance for the attainment of outcomes from others or the environment (extrinsic

incentives). Theory and research on intrinsic motivation is arguably the most well-known topic

in the domain linking purpose and motivation.

Research on intrinsic motivation builds upon theories that assume all people possess

motives for autonomy, competence, and control. Theories of intrinsic motivation hold that

individuals are intrinsically motivated when they attribute the cause of their actions to be self-

determined, rather than determined by external forces (that do not permit satisfaction of

autonomy or control motives). Individuals who attribute their behavior to external causes are said

to be extrinsically motivated.

Studies by Deci, Lepper and their colleagues during the 1970s and 1980s (see Kanfer,

1990) showed that the provision of extrinsic rewards (e.g., money) undermined intrinsic

motivation, and was associated with reduced task motivation and task persistence. Early theories

(Cognitive Evaluation Theory; Deci, 1975) proposed that the detrimental influence of extrinsic

events on intrinsic motivation depended on the extent to which the individual perceived the event

as controlling (vs. informational). This explanation subsequently focused research on the

conditions under which extrinsic events, such as rewards and feedback, were interpreted as

controlling, and their downstream effects on task interest, enjoyment and behavior.

Over the past decade, Deci and his colleagues have proposed a new formulation; Self-

determination Theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan, 2000, 2002). In contrast to CET, SDT provides a

detailed description of how environments support and disrupt expression of the self-

determination motive. SDT also proposes that the motive for self-determination may be satisfied

Page 30: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

30

in some conditions that include extrinsic rewards or feedback. Specifically, SDT organizes

extrinsic and intrinsic motivation conditions along a continuum. At the lowest level on the

continuum is a motivation, a condition in which there are no intrinsic or extrinsic prompts or

attributions for action. The next level is external regulation, or the classic condition when

individuals perceive the reason for their behavior is due solely to obtain the external reward.

Beyond that however, SDT proposes another form of extrinsic motivation called introjected

regulation. In this form of extrinsic motivation there is some self-determination and autonomy,

as the individual performs in order to satisfy self-worth contingencies, such as wanting to look

competent to others. A yet more self-determining form of extrinsic motivation occurs in what

Deci refers to as identified regulation contexts, where the individual performs an activity because

he/she identifies with its value or meaning. The most self-determining form of extrinsic

motivation is posited to occur in integrated regulation, when individuals perform an activity

because the individual it has become part of the individual's sense of self. At the top level of the

continuum is intrinsic motivation, where the individual performs the activity for its own sake and

enjoyment.

In contrast to previous formulations, the SDT framework is more relevant to the

workplace, where extrinsic reward contingencies and feedback are commonplace. Further, the

mapping of attributions for action along a single regulation continuum provides a useful

framework for systematic investigation of differences in self-regulatory activities as a function

regulation level. As a consequence, research using SDT has begun to attract greater attention

among work motivation researchers (e.g., Gagne & Deci, 2005; Gagne & Forest, 2008).

Goal Orientation Formulations

Page 31: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

31

Early intrinsic motivation research focused on the effect of perceived extrinsic control on

task motivation and behavior, but did not examine how the perceived purpose for performance

influenced specific self-regulatory variables or strategies. Intrinsic motivation research also

focused on the motivational and performance consequences of the individual's (past-oriented)

attributions for the cause of action, rather than the individual's (future-oriented) goals. In the late

1970s and early 1980s, education psychology researchers began to study how the purpose of task

performance affects the goals that an individual adopts and the self-regulatory processes that are

used to attain the goal.

Nicholls (1984) and Dweck (1986; Dweck & Leggett, 1988) articulated early goal

orientation perspectives in the context of understanding why children who adopted a similar goal

often showed different patterns of goal striving, learning and performance (beyond the effects of

cognitive abilities). According to Nicholls (1984) and Dweck (1986), children who hold a

learning or task goal orientation also maintain a self-referenced conception of ability (i.e., focus

on how much had been learned), and tend to view performance improvement and greater task

mastery as positive outcomes. In contrast, individuals who hold an ego or performance goal

orientation are proposed to regard task performance as a means to an end, and to view

performance useful only in so far as it provides a demonstration of one's ability to others. These

differences in goal orientation, in turn, affect the quality of goal striving strategies used to

accomplish the goal, persistence following failure or setbacks, and learning and performance.

Similar to early research in intrinsic motivation, the applicability of goal orientation

formulations to work settings was not readily apparent and early interest among work motivation

researchers was limited (see Kanfer, 1989). However, with the development of two adult

measures of individual differences in goal orientation in the late 1990s (Button, Mathieu, &

Page 32: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

32

Zajac, 1996; DeShon & Gillespie, 2005; VandeWalle, 1997), investigations of goal orientation in

the work motivation domain grew dramatically. In the development of adult goal orientation

measures, Button et al. (1996) and VandeWalle (1997) showed that goal orientation was best

understood as a multidimensional construct, and could be fruitfully applied to understanding

work motivation in the work setting. Button et al. (1996) developed a two-dimensional adult

measure of goal orientation based upon Dweck's formulation; VandeWalle (1997) developed a

three-dimensional measure that further distinguished the performance goal dimension into two

dimensions—performance goal orientation directed to proving one's competencies and gaining

positive judgments from others (Performance-Prove), and performance goal orientation directed

toward avoiding demonstration of one's lack of ability and negative judgments from others

(Performance-Avoid).

Although goal orientation has been conceptualized as both a trait and a state, the majority

of work motivation studies to date have examined the effects of goal orientation states (induced

through instructions or context) on motivation and performance. Quantitative and qualitative

reviews of the goal orientation research literature are provided by Carr, DeShon, and Dobbins

(2001), Day, Yeo, and Radosevich (2003), Payne et al. (2007), Rawsthorne and Elliot (1999),

and Utman (1997). In general, these reviews provide support for the facilitative effect of learning

goal orientation on goal setting, self-regulatory activities, learning, and performance. In contrast,

many, but not all, studies find a significant negative relationship between performance-prove or

performance-avoid goal orientation, goal striving, and performance. Findings with respect to the

impact of performance-prove goal orientation are inconsistent. Building on these findings,

current research has broadened to examine the impact of goal orientation in leadership (e.g.,

Whitford & Moss, 2009), in training (e.g., Chiaburu & Marinova, 2005; Colquitt & Simmering,

Page 33: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

33

1998; Cox & Beier, 2009), and job search (e.g., Creed, King, Hood, & McKenzie, 2009; Van

Hooft & Noordzij, 2009). Recently, some researchers have proposed integrative formulations

that place goal orientation processes within broader self-regulation frameworks (DeShon &

Gillespie, 2005; Yeo, Loft, Xiao, & Kiewitz, 2009).

Motivational Orientation: Trait Conceptualizations

In contrast to goal orientation at the state level, stable individual differences in goal and

motivational orientation at the trait level operate over a longer timescale. Individuals who are

high in trait learning goal orientation, for example, are not only more likely to adopt a learning

goal orientation to specific tasks, but also to show higher levels of self-regulatory skill in

overcoming obstacles to goal attainment. That is, goal and motivation orientation traits can be

expected to exert cross-level effects at multiple points in motivational processing. Elliot and

McGregor (2001) and Elliot and Thrash (2001) provide summary reviews of findings with

respect to the effects of achievement and avoidance motivational orientations on goals and self-

regulatory processes .

During the past two decades, interest in trait level goal and motivational orientation has

burgeoned. Different programs of research in neurobiology, personality and social psychology,

and motivation provide convergent evidence for two trait orientations and their differential

impacts on motivational processing, learning, and performance. Formulations by Elliot and

Harackiewicz (1996), Higgins (1998), and Kanfer and Heggestad (1997), posit that individuals

who score high on what is variously described as approach motivation (Elliott & Harackiewicz,

1996), promotion regulatory focus (Higgins, 1998), or performance mastery orientation (Kanfer

& Heggestad, 1997) engage in more effective learning, meta-cognitive, and self-regulatory

activities during goal adoption and goal striving than persons who score high in avoidance

Page 34: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

34

motivation (Elliott & Harackiewcz, 1996; Kanfer & Heggestad, 1997), prevention regulatory

focus (Higgins, 1998) or performance-avoid goal orientation (VandeWalle, 1997). These

motivational orientations are further consistent with evidence from neurobiology theory and

research on the existence of two general neurobehavioral systems; a behavioral activation system

(BAS) sensitive to rewards and characterized by positive emotion and approach, and a behavioral

inhibition system (BIS) sensitive to punishment and characterized by negative emotion and

inhibition.

Although the antecedents, mechanisms, and consequences studied in different approaches

vary, findings across these research paradigms provide convergent empirical evidence for the

positive influence of approach-related goal and motivational orientation on learning and

performance. However, findings on the deleterious impact of avoidance-related goal and

motivational orientations on learning and performance are less uniform and appear to depend on

the nature of the task as well as the motivation and performance outcomes studied. In the

organizational domain, Diefendorff & Mehta (2007) found that workplace deviance measures

were negatively related to approach motivation traits but positively related to avoidance

motivation traits. Similarly, Heimerdinger & Hinsz (2008) found that avoidance motivation was

negatively related to performance in an idea-generation task.

Summary

Work motivation researchers have taken advantage of recent advances in other areas of

psychology to develop new theories and conduct research directed toward understanding the

impact of implicit motives and nonconscious processes on explicit goal setting and self-

regulation processes. Two issues currently hinder further advances in these areas. First, the

development of reliable and valid measures of individual differences in implicit motives is still in

Page 35: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

35

the early stages. Second, most research to date has looked at the influence of nonconscious

processes on behaviors and motivational mechanisms as they occur in controlled settings using

carefully designed tasks. Research is needed to determine whether these findings scale up to

affect goal choice and behavior in work settings. Questions about the generalizability of findings

to the workplace include, but are not limited to understanding when nonconscious processes are

most likely to affect behavior, the role of nonconscious affect-driven processes in goal

commitment and goal shielding, and the extent to which individuals are able to effectively

modulate nonconscious tendencies that disrupt progress toward goal attainment (e.g., to control

feelings of fatigue in order to complete a project). During the past decade, work motivation

researchers have worked on both these issues. Early findings suggest that investigation of

implicit motives and nonconscious processes offers great promise in further clarifying the role of

personality and affect in work motivation and performance outcomes.

Goals remain the focal point for most work motivation theorizing and research. But the

focus of goal-related research has changed. Investigations of how specific attributes of the goal

affect self-regulation and performance has declined as work motivation researchers have adopted

a more holistic and person-centered view of goals. New person-centered perspectives emphasize

the impact of purpose on how information is processed (mindsets associated with selecting a goal

and accomplishing a goal) and the meta-cognitive strategies used in self-regulatory activities in

goal pursuit. With the exception of self-efficacy, theory and research on self-regulatory processes

that occur during goal pursuit has also shifted. In contrast to earlier research focused on the

impact of goal attributes on specific self-regulatory components (such as performance

monitoring), recent studies have focused on the relationship between goal orientation and self-

regulatory strategies and patterns during goal pursuit.

Page 36: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

36

Work motivation research has also begun to focus on abiding and difficult questions

about the impact of goal dynamics on motivational processes and performance. In many

instances, the impetus for research on these questions comes from the real-world work setting. In

the workplace, individuals are often assigned more than one task, each with unique task

demands, goals, and timelines for completion. Understanding how individuals allocate resources

and regulate their activities across tasks is likely to yield knowledge that can be used in work

design. Another important area in goal dynamics pertains to the person and situation factors that

contribute to goal revision, the resolution of goal conflicts, and goal abandonment. From a

practical perspective, research is needed to understand different ways that individuals cope with

goal conflicts and the distinct motivational orientation states and self-regulatory patterns

associated with goal revision compared to goal abandonment.

Determinants of Motivation: Content, Context, and Change

Organizations and the employees who work in them often turn to work motivation

researchers for evidence-based knowledge about the person and situation characteristics that

most influence work motivation and the ways in which this influence occurs. Not surprisingly, a

large portion of theory and research in work motivation pertains to the influence of person

attributes and environmental conditions on motivation, learning and performance. Over the years

a multitude of person characteristics and situation factors have been studied, alone and in

different combinations. Although the development of the Five Factor Model has helped to

organize research on the influence of personality traits on work motivation, there have been few

attempts to systematize research on the effects of situation and context on work motivation (see

Johns, 2006; Meyer, Dalal, & Hermida, 2009). And neither the Five Factor Model nor situational

frameworks address motivation issues related to time.

Page 37: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

37

Recently, Kanfer et al. (2008) proposed a broad "Three C's" meta-heuristic scheme for

organizing the relevant determinants of work motivation. A non-exhaustive list of determinants

in the Content, Context, and Change categories is provided in Table 1.

-------------------------------

Insert Table 1 about here

-------------------------------

Content determinants include variables related to interindividual differences, such as

knowledge, skills, abilities, personality traits, motives, affective tendencies, interests, and values.

Context determinants refer to exogenous (external to the individual) features of the action

setting. Context variables may be further organized into features of the immediate work setting

(e.g., supervision, work demands), features associated with the broader socio-technical work

context (e.g., organizational policies, organizational climate), variables related to the individual's

nonwork demands and activities (e.g., care-giving demands), and variables related to the broader

socio-cultural and economic environment (e.g., cultural norms, values, unemployment rate). The

third C refers to change; that is, factors associated with the temporal dimension. Temporal

influences on work motivation may come into play several ways. In terms of motivational

processes per se, time-sensitive factors, such as fatigue, may influence goal pursuit and the

willingness to revise one’s goal. Alternatively, time-sensitive factors, such as the development of

knowledge and skills as a result of job performance, may affect work motivation by altering

perceived person-task or person-job fit. Time also plays a role in distinguishing among

employees. Changes in cognitive abilities, motives, and interests over the life course can affect

work motivation through its impact on self-efficacy, the utility of performance for attaining

valued outcomes, and age-related changes in the utility of high levels of cognitive and physical

Page 38: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

38

effort (Kanfer, 1987). Figure 2 displays a schematic of the “three C’s” and their relationship to

motivation processes. In the remainder of this section I describe notable advances in each

determinant class.

-------------------------------

Insert Figure 2 about here

-------------------------------

Content Influences: Person Attributes and Traits

In 1990, with the exception of research in achievement motivation, there was relatively

little systematic research on the influence of personality traits on work motivation and behavior

(see Kanfer, 1990). A major reason for this state of affairs was the absence of a conceptually

sound structure for organizing the multitude of personality traits that had been studied (see Guion

& Gottier, 1965). The introduction of the Five Factor Model (FFM) of personality structure into

I/O psychology in the early 1990s provided a much-needed solution to this problem, and

launched a prolific period of research on the impact of personality traits on motivation and

performance (see Judge & Ilies, 2002, and Kanfer & Kantrowitz, 2002, for reviews).

Over the past two decades, research on person determinants of work motivation has also

broadened beyond the study of the broad five personality traits. Recent studies have focused on

traits not well-specified in the FFM, including affective traits (e.g., negative and positive

affectivity), motivational traits (e.g., action orientation), and self-related traits (e.g., core self-

evaluations, personal initiative). To further organize the broad array of person attributes currently

under investigation in work motivation, I use a modified version of the framework originally

proposed by Thorndike in 1947 for the purpose of categorizing different types of person

Page 39: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

39

influences on test performance. The adapted meta-organizing framework for person attributes is

shown in Figure 3.

-------------------------------

Insert Figure 3 about here

-------------------------------

Consistent with the Thorndike scheme, person attributes are classified on the basis of

their permanence (i.e., lasting or temporary) and scope (i.e., general or specific). For present

purposes, I classify individual differences in cognitive abilities, knowledge, and skills, and

nonability traits, such as the FFM personality traits, as lasting influences on motivation and

action. These person characteristics are not posited to vary appreciably across contexts (although

their expression may vary across contexts) or time, and are generally viewed as exerting an

indirect or distal influence on motivation through their effects on goals. Permanent person

attributes that are also general include most personality traits, such as conscientiousness. Lasting

but specific person attributes, such as job knowledge, exert their influence on motivation in a

narrower range of contexts. In contrast to lasting and general person attributes, lasting but

specific person characteristics, such as interests, are likely to affect motivation through their

impact on select variables, such as goal commitment and self-efficacy.

Transient person attributes refer to person influences that occur as a consequence of the

person-situation interaction. These person characteristics contribute to motivation by creating

what is often referred to as a motivational state. Biologically-based person attributes, such as

fatigue and stress, are examples of general (pervasive) but temporary person attributes that can

affect motivation through their impact on resource availability. In contrast, transient and context-

Page 40: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

40

specific person attributes, such as regulatory focus or anger are likely to influence motivation

through their effect on self-regulation processes.

The organization of person influences on motivation using Thorndike's (1947)

classification scheme is generally consistent with findings across disparate research literatures

that show an indirect influence of general person attributes and a direct influence of transient

person attributes on motivational processes. To date, most work motivation research has focused

on general and lasting attributes (such as personality traits) and transient and specific person

characteristics (such as anger).

Personality traits. In contrast to earlier reviews of the relationship between personality

traits and performance that did not benefit from an empirically-derived organization of the

personality trait domain (e.g., Guion & Gottier, 1965), the Barrick and Mount (1991) meta-

analysis of personality trait-performance relations using the FFM trait scheme revealed

significant relationships between individual differences in conscientiousness and neuroticism and

diverse measures of performance. Specifically, conscientiousness showed a positive relationship

to performance while neuroticism showed a negative relationship to performance. Subsequent

studies on personality-performance relations (see Hurtz & Donovan, 2000) provide further

support for the predictive validities of these broad personality traits on performance.

In an attempt to explain these observed relations, Kanfer (1992) suggested that

personality traits influence performance through their effects on motivational processes.

Empirical support for this notion was obtained in studies by Barrick et al. (1993) and Barrick,

Stewart, and Piotrowski (2002), in which motivational variables were found to mediate the trait-

performance relationship, and in a study by Chen, Gully, Whiteman, & Kilcullen (2000) that

found that the impact of trait variables on performance were mediated by goals and self-efficacy.

Page 41: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

41

Results of a later meta-analysis by Judge and Ilies (2002), investigating the relationship between

personality traits and motivational variables (e.g., goal level) further indicated the robustness of

the relationship between two personality traits (Conscientiousness and Neuroticism) and

motivation using criterion variables from three different work motivation theories. Although

research investigating the influence of the FFM trait dimensions on motivation and performance

remains popular, there have also been more studies investigating select traits that are not well-

represented in the Five Factor structure of personality, such as motivational traits (Kanfer &

Heggestad, 1997), core-self-evaluations (Judge, Locke, & Durham, 1997) and personal initiative

(Frese & Fay, 2001; Frese, Kring, Soose, & Zempel, 1996).

The Thorndike meta-organizational scheme implies that general and lasting person

attributes (personality and other nonability traits such as interests) operate in a similar manner to

affect work motivation. However, to date, most work motivation research on personality traits

has been studied in isolation from studies that examine other important general and lasting

person attributes, such as individual differences in cognitive abilities, vocational interests, or

values. Ackerman (1997) and Lubinski (2000) have suggested that an integration of historically

disparate streams of research in cognitive abilities, personality, vocational interests, knowledge,

and values can improve our understanding and prediction of motivation and performance. These

researchers argue that communalities among general and lasting person attributes arise as a

consequence of the common biological influences and environmental affordances that promote

the tandem development of individual differences in cognitive and noncognitive traits over the

life course. In developed countries, for example, individual differences in cognitive abilities are

typically assessed prior to entry into elementary school and used to place students in different

learning environments that are considered optimal for the child’s aptitudes. These environments

Page 42: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

42

directly impact the level and type of intellectual and social challenges the child experiences in

the school setting. It is not unreasonable to expect that the interaction of the individual's

characteristics and his/her school environment, in turn, influences later work interests, work

values, as well as the work opportunities available to the individual during the transition to adult

employment.

From a structural perspective, the association of different general and lasting person

characteristics into broad constellations or complexes may be conceptualized hierarchically, with

basic dimensions of cognition, temperament and motives often showing strong associations with

the development of select personality traits, vocational interests, and even work values. Research

that use theory-driven integrations of related person attributes to study their effects on motivation

and performance encourages the development of more parsimonious and practically-useful

approaches to understanding person influences on motivation and performance.

Organizational justice motives. One complex of general and lasting person attributes that

continues to command attention in the work motivation literature pertains to universal motives

for justice. Similar to traits, interests, and values, universal motives for justice reflect outcome

preferences that remain relatively stable across situations and the life course, even if the

particular conceptualization of justice may be culture-specific. In contrast to personality traits,

however, individuals do not appreciably differ in strength for satisfaction of justice motives.

Rather, justice motives activate motivational processes as a consequence of the person-situation

transaction. In this sense, although justice motives are lasting and general, the influence of the

motive on motivation and action occurs only when the individual perceives a threat to motive

satisfaction.

Page 43: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

43

Early theories of organizational justice grew out of equity and exchange formulations

(e.g., Adams, 1965). According to equity theories, situations an individual perceives to be unfair

or unjust creates heightened psychological tension. Motivation arises from the desire to reduce

the psychological tension, though the way that individuals accomplish this reduction in tension

may be through changes in cognition rather than changes in behavior. Theory and research on

organizational justice through the 1980s focused largely on delineating the motivational

pathways by which perceptions of procedural injustice or unfairness influenced work behavior

(see Greenberg & Cropanzano, 1999).

Over the past two decades, theories of organizational justice have matured and research

has expanded to examine the determinants and consequences of other forms of justice, including

interactional justice, relational justice, and informational justice. As with research on procedural

justice, research on relational and informational forms of justice has focused primarily on the

contextual features that elicit perceptions of injustice and subsequent behavior change. In a

comprehensive meta-analysis of justice research findings between 1975 and 1999, Colquitt et al.

(2001) found that both distributive and procedural forms of justice were positively related to

motivational outcomes, such as job commitment and task performance.

In the past few years, research on the impact motive violations on motivation has

declined as attention has shifted to examining impact of person characteristics on justice

perceptions. Studies by Truxillo, Bauer, Campion, and Paronto (2006) and Shi, Lin, Wang, and

Wang (2009), for example, examined the relationship between the FFM personality traits and

justice perceptions in the context of personnel selection and among incumbent employees,

respectively. Both studies found a positive relationship between Agreeableness and

organizational justice perceptions, and a negative relationship between Neuroticism and

Page 44: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

44

organizational justice perceptions. From a motivational perspective, these results suggest that

violations of justice motives in the workplace may be more salient to individuals high in

neuroticism than individuals low in neuroticism. To date, however, there has been little attention

to the impact of fairness perceptions on motivational orientation and self-regulatory activities

used for goal accomplishment. Studies are also needed to illuminate the relationship between

different forms of perceived injustice and associated patterns of motivation and behavior over

time.

Summary

Research on the influence of person characteristics on work motivation has increased

dramatically over the past two decades. This increase is due largely to the introduction of the

FFM of personality into the organizational domain. Using this scheme, work motivation

researchers have been able to accumulate research findings and obtain consistent evidence for

the relationship between Conscientiousness and Neuroticism traits and motivational variables

and outcomes. Emerging programs of research on specific variables, such as personal initiative,

reflect a further maturation of this portion of the field, as researchers attempt to identify and

study the impact of key behavioral tendencies for work motivation. Consistent with

recommendations by Ackerman (1996, 1997) and Lubinski (2000), programs of research by

Judge and his colleagues on core self-evaluations (e.g., Judge & Bono, 2001; Judge & Hurst,

2007) and Kanfer and Ackerman and their colleagues (e.g., Kanfer, Ackerman, & Heggestad,

1996; Kanfer, Wolf, Kantrowitz, & Ackerman, 2009) have also focused on delineating the

relationship between trait complexes, motivation processes and performance.

Nonetheless, there are still a number of important gaps in our knowledge. Theory and

research on the impact of person attributes in the general but temporary category, including

Page 45: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

45

health, stress, and fatigue variables, has received substantial research attention in the

occupational health literature, but has yet to be incorporated into mainstream work on

motivation. Similarly, there is also surprising little research directed toward the influence of

individual differences in affect on motivation and performance. Seo, Feldman-Barrett, and

Bartunek (2004) proposed an integrative model for the role of affective experience in work

motivation, using the concept of "core affective experience" to capture the impact of affective

experiences on goal choice and goal commitment. In addition, there has been little attention to

identifying the distinct motivational signatures of discrete emotions (Kanfer & Stubblebine,

2008). Research to identify the goal characteristics and self-regulatory strategies that uniquely

associated with different emotions is likely to be helpful to supervisors in the services sector. It

may also be for example, that individual differences in affective tendencies influence not only

the experience of an emotion, but also the goals that are formed in response to the emotion and

the effectiveness of self-regulatory activities to modulate the emotion. Research to investigate

the impact of specific affective person attributes, such as hostility, on emotion and motivation is

another area that has potentially important theoretical and practical implications.

Contextual Influences on Motivation

Context refers to the milieu in which work motivation takes place, and may be described

in a multitude of ways. At the simplest level, context may be distinguished in terms of function,

such as skill acquisition, job search, or teamwork. Although such an organizing scheme is useful

for noting the importance of motivation across the range of organizational activities, it does not

capture the complex relationships among situational variables or the communalities and

distinctions among contextual variables across settings.

Page 46: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

46

Most work motivation theory and research over the past 50 years has focused on the

impact of variables active in the immediate performance setting, such as rewards, decision

latitude, social interactions, and task variety (see Hackman & Oldham, 1976; Karasek, 1979,

1989). Although research findings in these perspectives often show significant relationships

between job demands and work conditions with work motivation, several factors have

contributed to a general decline in scientific study of context using these models. First,

researchers have identified several conceptual and methodological problems with formulations

that focus primarily on job task variables. In addition, the changing nature of work has shifted

attention to the question of whether and how other layers of context, such as societal culture,

influence work motivation (e.g., see Gelfand et al., 2007).

In a related vein, the introduction of social, information-processing (SIP) approaches by

Salancik and Pfeffer (1978) and Lord and Foti (1986) encouraged new ways of thinking about

context and its effects on work motivation. Specifically, these researchers argued that individuals

actively construct perceptions of the work context based on social cues in the environment.

These perceptions are then organized and stored in schemas that, once developed, tend to

promote the neglect of information that is inconsistent with the existing schema (Lord & Foti,

1986). Although SIP models have not attracted a great deal of attention among work motivation

researchers, the SIP perspective differs from job-based formulations in several important ways

for work motivation. First, SIP approaches formally recognize the role of preconscious processes

that direct attention to particular features of the work environment. Contemporary research in

nonconscious motivation processes expand upon this notion. Second, building on advances in

cognitive, information-processing psychology through the 1980s, Lord and his colleagues

showed how features of work were mentally represented in cognitive schemas, and the impact of

Page 47: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

47

schemas for decision-making. In modern work motivation research, nonconscious schemas

contribute to the development of automaticity, as well to problems in behavior change. Third,

SIP approaches refocus attention to the role that interpersonal relations play in perceptions of the

workplace and job context. In the modern work motivation theory, the impact of social cues on

work motivation has moved to investigation of the influence that an individual’s coworker

relations, social network, and social capital plays in motivational outcomes, such as

organizational citizenship behavior (e.g., Bowler & Brass, 2006) and turnover (Randel & Ranft,

2007).

As the physical and psychological context for job performance broadens (e.g., coffee

shop meetings; teamwork), organizational researchers have begun to study context influences

from a variety of levels, ranging from the impact of features associated with the immediate work

environment to the impact of organizational and national cultures on expectations and norms for

work behavior. One potentially useful heuristic for thinking about different levels of context is to

use the analogy of an onion. Like an onion, people perform work in a context that is multi-

layered, with each layer influencing other layers. With the individual at the center, variables that

capture features of the immediate work setting, such as task demands and social relations, can be

represented as the layer that is most proximal to the individual, yet fully embedded in broader

organization, represented as the next layer of the onion. Salient socio-technical features of the

organization context that may exert direct or indirect effects on the individual include, for

example, the unique culture, climate, and norms of the organization. In turn, organizations are

embedded in societies and cultures that may also be distinguished in terms of characteristics such

as norms, values, and orientations with respect to social relations and power.

Page 48: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

48

Similar to an onion, changes at any layer may exert direct and lagged impact on other

layers and the individual. The introduction of new production technology, such as the desktop

computer in the 1980s for example, can rapidly change the individual’s context of work within a

work unit and the schedule of intrinsic rewards associated with performance of reconfigured

jobs. Perhaps the most important point in using the onion analogy is that changes at different

levels of context rarely occur in isolation. Features of the workplace, compensation, the

organization, leadership, and societal norms for how and when nonwork family and leisure

activities should occur often covary in ways that create distinct, complex situation constellations

that distinguish occupational families. For example, knowledge work, whether it be directed

toward product design, software development, or teaching, is often performed in settings

characterized by high levels of decision latitude, task interdependence, noncontingent pay

structures, nonhazardous work conditions, and the use of reward structures that reinforce

outcomes (e.g., new product development, teamwork) rather than single behaviors (e.g.,

attendance). Further, these features of work are often associated with the use of a management

style and an organizational culture and climate that differ significantly from the style and culture

that exists in production work. Even more broadly, organizations that produce knowledge, rather

than products, tend to grow best in stable economic mediums and in developed or rapidly

developing countries.

Managing the context in which work is performed in order to promote work motivation

and positive work attitudes is arguably the principal task of most supervisors and unit leaders.

Unfortunately, however, there is currently no theory of situations to guide managers or work

motivation researchers in systemizing and aggregating findings about the effects of context on

motivation (Johns, 2006). Meyer and his colleagues (Meyer & Dalal, 2009; Meyer, Dalal, &

Page 49: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

49

Bonaccio, 2009; Meyer, Dalal, & Hermida, 2010) have recently proposed a formulation that

organizes features of the work setting using the psychological concept of “situational strength.”

Based in part on Mischel’s (1968) notion of situational strength, Meyer et al. (2010) define

situational strength as “implicit or explicit cues provided by external entities regarding the

desirability of potential behaviors.” According to Meyer et al. (2010), the overall strength of a

work situation is comprised of strength in four dimensions: clarity, consistency, consequences,

and constraints. Clarity refers to the extent to which attributes of the workplace and social cues

create a clear understanding of work role and job responsibilities, Consistency refers to the

extent that workplace attributes and social cues create perceptions of the job as comprised of

compatible tasks and roles. Constraints refer to the extent to which physical, technical, and social

attributes of the job place limits on the influence of an individual’s decisions and behaviors. The

consequences dimension refers to the extent to which contextual variables affect the significance

of action for self, coworkers, the unit, and the organization.

Meyer et al. (2009) provide initial evidence for the feasibility of this framework in an

investigation of the moderating role that occupation-level constraints and consequences have on

the relationship between conscientiousness and job performance. Concordant with their theory,

Meyer et al. (2009) found that the conscientiousness-performance relation was stronger in

occupations characterized by low levels of constraint on the consequences and constraint

dimensions than higher levels on these dimensions. The utility of this approach for organizing

the impact of various contextual variables appears promising, but requires further empirical

testing. In particular, research to identify the differential impact of situational strength

dimensions on self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation is likely to be quite useful for building a

conceptual bridge between context and motivation.

Page 50: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

50

Both older and newer approaches to context influences on work motivation hold that the

impact of exogenous variables on work motivation obtains as a consequence of how features of

the environment are perceived and interpreted by the individual. An important next step in this

area will be to illuminate the effects of person attributes, time and variable constellations on the

perception and interpretation of work milieu, and subsequent influences on goal choice and goal

pursuit. It may well be, for example, that the impact of situational variables on work motivation

changes over time as a function of the individual’s goals. Among novice emergency medical

technicians (EMTs), for example, strong situations that provide detailed guides for what to do

and pay attention to while trying to stabilize a patient for hospital transfer is likely to enhance

effective resource allocations and performance. Among experienced EMTs, however, strong

situations may diminish work motivation (although not job performance) for precisely the same

reason; namely, perceived inflexibility in how performance is enacted and reduced opportunity

for self-determination. How different layers of context influence situational strength, and the

conditions in which strength facilitates or diminishes work motivation are important questions

for future research in this area.

Context-grounded motivation research. Motivation is always studied in context, but over

the past few decades changes in the nature of work and the workforce have focused attention on

the role of motivation in two specific contexts; namely, training and teams. The widespread

integration of self-regulation perspectives in training theory and research and development of

more sophisticated models of learning has revitalized motivation research in this context (Bell &

Kozlowski, 2002, 2008; Ford, Smith, Weissbein, Gully, & Salas, 1998; Sitzmann, Bell, Kraiger,

& Kanar, 2009). In contrast to older models of learning that assume stability in motivation across

the course of training, contemporary models propose more dynamic conceptions of motivation

Page 51: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

51

during training. Similarly, the extension of self-regulation to the study of team processes has

spurred theory and research on the influence of teams and team dynamics on individual

motivation and performance in the team context. Recent advances in these situations are

highlighted below.

Motivation in training and development. There is a long history of theory and research on

the role of motivation in job skill training and development (Goldstein, 1993). Prior to the latter

part of the 20th century, however, training and development research focused largely on the

impact of training design characteristics (e.g., feedback, modeling) on performance of new job

entrants engaged in job-specific skill training, such as typing or operating a printing press. Little

attention was paid to individual differences in motivation to learn, perform, and transfer training

outcomes or to motivational processes as they unfolded during training (see Noe, 1986).

Over the past four decades, however, there has been a steady increase in the role that

motivation plays in training and development. In the current work world, training is no longer

constrained to the front-end of careers and jobs, but rather an activity that can be expected to

occur with regularity across the career course. In some occupational sectors, such as IT and

healthcare, new technologies are driving major changes in the skill sets required for maintaining

a high level of job performance over time. For individuals who are increasingly likely to spend

five or more decades in the labor workforce, life-long learning is becoming a prerequisite for

career success and employability. In the research arena, investigations of goal setting and self-

regulation in learning and training environments have repeatedly shown the importance of

motivational processing in complex skill acquisition (e.g., Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989). Given

these trends, it is not at all surprising that theory and research on the role of motivation in

contemporary training contexts has burgeoned (Kozlowski & Salas, 2010).

Page 52: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

52

One area of great interest to organizations concerned with continuous learning pertains to

understanding of what motivates individuals to enroll in development activities. The decision to

participate in skill training represents the first and perhaps most critical step in training, since

failure to participate makes the question of motivation during learning and motivation for

transfer of training moot. Meta-analytic findings by Colquitt et al. (2000) indicate that individual

differences in select traits, such as locus of control, play an important role in motivation for

training. Findings by Baldwin, Magjuka, and Loher (1991), Guerrero and Sire (2001), and others

(see Mathieu & Martineau, 1997) also indicate that employees who are allowed to decide

whether they participate in organizationally-sponsored or supported training show higher levels

of motivation for training (as reflected in training commitment, allocation of time and effort

toward class attendance, time spent in on-task learning) than employees who are not allowed to

participate in the enrollment decision. These findings are consistent with motivation theories that

emphasize self-determination and participatory decision-making. In terms of voluntary training

participation, Hurtz and Williams (2009) found that perceived availability of development

activities and high learning goal orientation exerted significant positive effects on training

participation.

However, factors that motivate the decision to enroll in training may be quite different

from factors that motivate sustained learning over the duration of training (Beier & Kanfer,

2009). A large number of studies in educational and social/personality psychology as well as in

industrial and organizational psychology have examined self-regulatory processes during

learning and skill acquisition (Elliot, 2008; Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989; Zimmerman & Schunk,

2001). Findings from these complementary streams of research indicate the importance of self-

efficacy and active self-regulation of emotion and behavior during learning for positive learning

Page 53: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

53

outcomes. Many studies also show that performance goal orientation exerts a negative effect on

learning outcomes, though the mechanisms by which this negative effect occurs is still not well-

understood (DeShon & Gillespie, 2005; Payne et al., 2007).

Two current streams of research in training, by Frese and his colleagues and Bell and

Kozlowski and their colleagues, highlight the role of motivation in modern training

environments. In the early 1990s, Frese and Zapf (1994) suggested that giving trainees the

opportunity to make errors and encouraging trainees to learn from their errors during training

may improve learning outcomes. In contrast to training approaches that focus on error avoidance,

error management training (EMT) assumes that errors are unavoidable in active learning and that

total elimination of errors may be difficult to achieve in complex tasks. From a motivational

perspective, the EMT-active learning approach is posited to improve learning in part by

enhancing task engagement. As Keith and Frese (2005) noted, however, the positive effects of

EMT on performance are moderated by the extent to which the task and training environment

provide clear, unambiguous feedback. Keith and Frese (2005) examined the motivational

influence of EMT compared with error avoidant training approaches using a sample of college

students learning to use the PowerPoint 2000 computer program. After an introduction to the

program, participants were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: (a) error avoidant

training, (b) error management training, or (c) error management plus meta-cognitive instruction.

The results provided support for the proposed mediating role of emotion regulation and

metacognitive activities in the effects of training condition on performance. Consistent with

resource allocation theories (e.g., Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989), Keith and Frese (2005) found that

the positive effects of the two error management training conditions over the error-avoidant

condition on performance were fully attributable to their influence on enhancing emotion control

Page 54: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

54

and meta-cognitive activities during training. Specifically, trainees in the error management

conditions reported higher levels of emotion regulation following errors than trainees in the error

avoidant conditions.

Results of a recent meta-analysis of EMT effects on training and transfer task

performance by Keith and Frese (2008) provide support for basic tenets of the framework. From

a motivational perspective, the positive impact of EMT on adaptive transfer performance appears

to occur through two pathways: (a) reduction of disruptive emotional reactions that divert

attentional resources away from learning, and (b) more frequent activation of meta-cognitive

processes following error detection. However, the findings in EMT research suggest several

important boundary conditions on the efficacy of EMT for motivation and enhanced transfer

performance, including the provision of training environments that provide clear performance

feedback and the impact of training on adaptive transfer performance (versus analogous or near

transfer performance). In addition, investigations on the effectiveness of EMT approaches for

adaptive transfer performance have been limited to college-educated adult samples for use in

software skills and other computer-related skills. Additional research is needed to assess the

generalizability of these findings to other workers in different domains.

A second distinct, but related stream of motivation research in the training context by

Kozlowski, Bell and their colleagues has examined the impact of different active learning

interventions on training performance, as well as skill transfer and adaptation. Bell and

Kozlowski (2008) examined three self-regulatory pathways (cognitive, motivational, and

emotional) by which different active learning interventions influenced performance. Individuals

who adopted a mastery goal orientation to training showed increased levels of self-efficacy,

intrinsic motivation, and metacognitive ability. Bell and Kozlowski (2008) also showed that error

Page 55: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

55

framing interacted with trainees’ trait goal orientations to affect state goal orientation during

training. However, the effect of error framing was largely to increase state mastery goal

orientation among trainees low in trait mastery goal orientation; error framing had no effect on

individuals already high on mastery goal orientation. The Bell and Kozlowski (2008) findings

shed light on the importance of evaluating the interactive effects of trainee traits and training

designs on learning outcomes.

Motivation in teams. Historically, the bulk of theory and research on individual level

motivation in the team context has focused on the impact of group characteristics (e.g., size) and

the downward impact of team-level phenomena, such as team cohesion, on individual team

member motivation and behavior. For example, recent findings by Pearsall, Christian, and Ellis

(2010) show that reward system characteristics play an important role in motivating team

member effort and mitigating team-level social loafing phenomena. Over the past few decades,

however, there has been a trend toward the development of theories and research on motivation

in the team context that focus on the relationship between individual and team-level processes

Recent theoretical work by Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro (2001), and Chen and Kanfer (2006) on

motivational processes in team contexts build upon evidence of homology between goal choice

and self-regulatory motivational processes at the individual and team level, and advances in

multilevel modeling (Chen, Bliese, & Mathieu, 2005; Chen et al., 2002; DeShon et al., 2004).

Meta-analytic findings by DeChurch and Mesmer-Magnus (2010) provide empirical support for

the influence of proposed motivational states on team performance. Micro-analytic studies by

Chen, Kirkman, Kanfer, Allen, and Rosen (2007) and Chen, Kanfer, DeShon, Mathieu, and

Kozlowski (2009) provide additional support for the cross-level influence of team efficacy on

individual level motivational processes over time.

Page 56: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

56

Change (Temporal) Influences on Motivation

Most work and organizational theories focus on the effects of motivation on behavior or

performance at a specific point in time or on average over an aggregated period of time (e.g., a

week or month). Nonetheless, motivational scientists have long recognized that motivation

occurs over time, and that time and time-sensitive factors can also exert important influences on

the direction and intensity of resource allocations. Over the past decade, interest in temporal and

time-related influences on work motivation has blossomed, with researchers pursuing questions

about the influence of time from many different perspectives (e.g., Carstensen, 1993; Fried,

Grant, Levi, Hadant, & Slowik, 2007; Kanfer & Ackerman, 2004; Marks et al., 2001; Schmidt,

Dolis, & Tolli, 2009; Simons, Vansteenkiste, Lens, & Lacante, 2004; Steel & Konig, 2006;

Vancouver et al., 2001).

The first problem that confronts researchers interested in investigating the effects of time

on motivation is how to conceptualize and assess the effects of time on motivation separately

from the array of changes in other determinants that take place over time. For example,

endogenous, biological factors contribute to increasing levels of fatigue as a function of time-on-

task. Motivation, however, may mediate the impact of fatigue on performance, and support a

pattern of sustained performance over time (Ackerman, in press). Investigations to examine the

impact of time on goal choice or goal pursuit thus requires repeated assessment of proximal

internal and external determinants and consequences of motivation, rather than just distal traits

and performance trends.

The study of temporal influences on motivation highlights an abiding problem in

motivation psychology; namely, the interplay of biological factors, self-related factors, and

environment demands (e.g., task demands) on the pattern of resource allocation observed. Most

Page 57: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

57

studies of work motivation focus on the impact of a person or situational variable (e.g.,

performance incentives) on the allocation of resources to a specific target task independent of

other tasks and factors. However, as Kanfer and Ackerman (1989) note, the impact of person and

situation factors on motivation for a performance on a single task must also take into account

task demands and time-linked changes in task demands as a consequence of practice. The

provision of difficult, specific goals, for example, may initially increase motivation and facilitate

learning. For tasks that can be proceduralized (e.g., driving), however, the demands on attention

lessen with practice and task performance may be sustained by changes in knowledge and skill,

rather than continued high levels of resource allocation. That is, motivation may remain constant

over time, but the impact of motivation on performance changes as a function of how practice

influences task demands. In these instances, high levels of performance after practice may be

maintained by knowledge and skill development rather than by high levels of motivation.

In contrast, in jobs and tasks where performance cannot be improved through the

development of automaticity through practice, sustained levels of motivation are required to

maintain performance. In these situations, performance provides a relatively accurate assessment

of motivation change over time, but motivation may be more strongly determined by within-

subject factors than by an intervention such as goal setting. In summary, the use of performance

measures to assess motivation over time requires consideration of dynamic task demands, self-

processes, and competing goal demands.

The influence of time on work motivation processes may be captured at different levels

of analysis, and assessed directly or indirectly. Repeated measurement of expectancies, goals,

goal orientation, self-efficacy, self-evaluative behaviors, the direction of attentional effort, the

use of different self-regulatory strategies, and time spent on the task permits direct assessment of

Page 58: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

58

changes in motivation over time. Direct methods may be used to assess changes in motivational

processes as a function of time on task or to assess changes in motivation across longer

timescales, such as during a new employee’s probationary period. The influence of time-related

variables may also be studied indirectly using between-subject designs to evaluate how variables

such as an employee’s future time perspective and chronological age, affects goal choice, goal

striving and changes in resource allocation policies over time within and across tasks.

Research on the effects of time on motivational strength for performance of a specific

task has waxed and waned over the past 40 years. Atkinson and Birch (1970) proposed a theory

of motivational dynamics and task switching in which target task performance was hypothesized

to decline over time as a function of the declining value of further performance and the

increasing attractiveness of performing an alternative task. Raynor and Entin (1982) proposed an

extension of the expectancy-value formulation, taking into account the influence of the length of

the subordinate pathway for final outcome goal accomplishment and the relationship among

elements in the pathway to the final goal. Neither the Atkinson and Birch (1970) or the Raynor

and Entin (1982) formulations generated much interest in work motivation, although there has

been some recent work on the impact of future time perspective. For example, Simons, Dewitte,

and Lens (2004) suggest that in the context of student achievement, individual differences in

future time perspective influence intrinsic motivation and the self-regulatory strategies used to

accomplish task performance.

One currently prominent line of inquiry on the effects of time on motivation involves the

temporal depletion and replenishment of personal resources needed for motivational processes.

Baumeister’s Ego Depletion Theory (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998;

Muraven, Tice, & Baumeister, 1998) posits that the personal resources required for self-

Page 59: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

59

regulation and self-control diminish with use. According to Ego Depletion theory, sustained self-

regulation (as is often required to perform a complex task over long periods of time) drains the

personal resource pool and diminishes the capacity for and effectiveness of self-regulatory

activities. Several studies provide support for the notion that the effectiveness of self-regulation,

particularly in the service of emotion regulation, diminishes over time in situations that involve

continuous use. Resource depletion has also been shown to occur when individuals must engage

as a function of engaging in goal choice activities over time, such as might occur among EMTs

when responding to a disaster. Baumeister argues that individuals are unlikely to fully deplete

their limited resources for goal choice and self-regulation, particularly in situations where they

anticipate that self-regulation will be required for future performance. That is, unlike a flashlight

battery that will completely discharge if the flashlight is left on, Baumeister et al. (1998) and

Hobfoll (1989) argue that individuals attempt to conserve or slow the rate of resource depletion

when they perceive future demands for self-regulation prior to any opportunity for resource

replenishment (i.e., pacing oneself with respect to resource consumption). Whereas Baumeister’s

theory emphasizes resource depletion associated with length of use, Hobfill’s Conservation of

Resources theory (1989) emphasizes the impact of job stressors on rate of resource loss and

depletion.

Baumeister also argues that resources needed for motivation and self-regulation may be

replenished over time through rest and the pursuit of activities that do not require self-regulation.

Sonnentag and her colleagues (Sonnentag & Frese, 2003; Sonnentag & Fritz, 2007; Sonnentag &

Kruel, 2006) have examined this idea in a series of studies designed to identify the conditions

that promote resource recovery following work-related resource depletion. Using experience

sampling methodology, Sonnentag investigated work and nonwork factors that might influence

Page 60: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

60

resource recovery following depletion associated with job performance (Binnewies, Sonnentag,

& Mojza, 2009; Fritz & Sonnentag, 2005, 2006).

A central question for future research in this line of inquiry is to determine what features

of work most strongly influence resource depletion and recovery. For example, Hobfoll’s (1989)

theory suggests that work conditions, such as supervisory support, may buffer the negative

impact of resource loss associated with sustained self-regulation, and a study by Tice,

Baumeister, Shmueli, and Muraven (2007) suggests that positive affect may also buffer resource

depletion. Further research is needed to understand the role of tasks, person characteristics, and

social context on resource depletion and recovery (Bakker, Hakanen, Demerouti, &

Xanthopoulou, 2007). For example, in the context of skill acquisition, Kanfer and Ackerman

(1989) found that individuals with higher levels of attentional resources (i.e., cognitive abilities)

reported fewer resource-consumptive off-task cognitions during skill acquisition than individuals

with lower levels of attentional resources. Extending this finding to the workplace suggests that

certain forms of supervisory or coworker support or technologies that reduce work load may

attenuate resource depletion in demanding tasks. In emergency situations and high-risk

teamwork, for example, individuals often demonstrate high levels of sustained motivational

intensity over long periods of time. Investigation of how team member interactions operate to

slow the pace of resource depletion, and so facilitate sustained task effort for long periods of

time is a promising avenue for future research.

Individuals may also implement their own strategies to reduce or prevent resource

depletion associated with job demands. Research in the lifespan literature suggests two broad

strategies, accommodation and transformation, by which individuals may reduce demands on

personal resources (e.g., Heckhausen & Schultz, 1995). Accommodation refers to actions an

Page 61: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

61

individual may take to alter the environment in ways that better accommodate personal goals,

needs, and motives. During the course of performance, for example, anxious individuals who

experience resource depletion associated with emotion regulation, may revise work goals

downward to conserve resources and shield secondary goals related to sense of competence.

Similarly, individuals with more available resources and less demand for emotion regulation

during job performance may revise work goals upward to sustain task motivation and satisfy

secondary goals related to mastery. To date, however, there has been little research on what

motivates the use of accommodation strategies in taxing jobs and tasks.

Individuals may also use transformation strategies to reduce resource depletion.

Transformation strategies refer to behaviors taken to improve the person-job fit by making self-

changes that promote resource conservation. As Heckhausen and Schultz (1995) note,

transformation strategies require additional resource allocations and are typically only used when

accommodation strategies are not possible or ineffective. Improving job skills and knowledge is

one obvious transformation strategy for resource conservation in the workplace. However, as

Heckhausen and Schultz (1995) suggest, the additional resource demands associated with

learning and behavior change in order to better meet job demands may make transformation

strategies less attractive than accommodation strategies that shape the environment to the person.

Although research using Baumeister and Hobfill’s resource frameworks have only recently

gained the attention of work motivation researchers, these conceptualizations appear quite

promising for understanding the costs of sustained emotion regulation in the workplace, and the

most effective interventions for slowing the rate of depletion and improving resource recovery in

the workplace. In particular, it would be quite useful to know the conditions that trigger the use

Page 62: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

62

of self-initiated strategies for resource conservation, and the factors that determine the choice to

use transformation versus accommodation strategies for resource conservation.

Another interesting approach for understanding motivation over time focuses on a time-

linked outcome of considerable importance in the workplace, namely, procrastination. Although

there have been several studies investigating the influence of traits (e.g., action control, see Kuhl,

1986) on procrastination, Steel and Konig (2006) have recently proposed a general motivation

formulation that specifically addresses resource allocation policies across activities as a function

of time and their effects on procrastination. According to Steel and Konig (2006), time

influences individual resource allocations across tasks through its effect on the value that

individuals attach to events and outcomes that occur at in the future. Using temporal discounting

theory, Steel and Konig (2006) propose individuals will discount the value of events and

outcomes that occur in the future and allocate fewer resources to accomplishment of distal

outcomes than to accomplishment of proximal goals that have not been discounted. Steel and

Konig (2006) describe a number of interesting implications of TMT for the development of

motivational interventions to mitigate the temporal discounting effect on goal choice and

allocations of time across tasks, and for the mitigation of procrastination behavior.

As Dalal and Hulin (2008) note, task goals provide a naturally occurring and useful

demarcation for studying motivation processes over time. Analysis of time effects across single

goal performance cycles also corresponds well to single-cycle theories of work motivation.

However, other units of analysis may shed further light on how time affects resource allocation

policies across concurrent task goals. Use of temporal cycles organized around person attributes,

such as Type A tendencies, may also shed light on whether individual differences exert a

Page 63: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

63

separate effect on the speed and quality of explicit goal choice processes and the strength of self-

regulatory activities over time.

Time influences on work motivation associated with employee age. Research in

developmental and lifespan psychology documents changes within the person that occur across

the lifespan, including declines in select cognitive processing abilities that contribute to fluid

intelligence, increases in declarative knowledge that contributes to crystallized intelligence,

changes in motive primacy, and changes in intensity of select personality traits (see Kanfer &

Ackerman, 2004, for a review). Intra-individual differences in these abilities and traits occur very

slowly, and the impact of age-related differences in motivation is typically studied using cross-

sectional samples that compare individuals at different chronological age periods (e.g., young,

midlife, old). Findings obtained to date indicate a general downward shift with age in the valence

of common extrinsic rewards, such as a promotion or increased pay, and a general upward shift

with age in the attractiveness of intrinsic outcomes, such as autonomy (in the form of flexible

work scheduling) and competency (in the form of opportunity to utilize skills; Warr, 2008).

Age has also been shown to be negatively related to training speed and learning self-

efficacy, most likely as a consequence of the age-related decline in fluid intellectual abilities that

influence both these variables. Taken together with the age-related decline in the attractiveness

of extrinsic rewards, these findings suggest that older workers (with lower levels of learning self-

efficacy) are likely to be less motivated to participate in new skill learning than younger workers.

However, findings by Simpson, Greller, & Stroh (2002) indicate that older workers are no less

motivated than younger workers for learning opportunities, and were more likely to participate in

such opportunities when the criterion for participation extended to nonorganizationally

sponsored learning opportunities. Studies by Sitzmann and her colleagues (Sitzmann et al., 2009;

Page 64: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

64

Sitzmann & Ely, 2010) suggest that the use of prompts to employ self-regulatory strategies

during training enhanced learning and reduced attrition over the course of training. These results

suggest that motivational declines associated with perceptions of slow progress and poor

performance among older workers may be mitigated by the use of instructional designs that

assist and promote effective self-regulation strategies.

Transactional Perspectives

The allocation of personal resources to work-related activities is not a single decision, but

rather an ongoing process that is influenced by changes in the person, the environment, and the

transaction between the person and the environment. In this section I briefly review recent work

on the motivational processes by which person-environment transactions influence performance

and attitudes.

Match theories of job performance and work attitudes. A central thesis in the person-

environment research literature, and a widely-held belief among organizational practitioners is

that alignment of employee and organization goals, values, interests, and competencies facilitates

work motivation, positive job attitudes and job intentions (e.g., job satisfaction, organizational

commitment), and performance. These models do not posit that values, objectives, and

competencies needed by the organization and possessed by the individual be identical, only that

the individual perceive that his/her values, objectives and competencies are congruent with the

demands of the job. As George and Brief (1996) suggested, a high level of perceived congruence

between an employee's goals and that of his/her team members may exert a positive impact on

performance through its effect on adjusting resource allocation policies, or as Kristof (1996)

suggests, by helping persons know "the right thing" to do. Similarly, perceptions of poor

alignment or lack of congruence between one's goals and those of other team members is

Page 65: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

65

expected to exert a negative impact on motivational processes and, in turn, work behavior and

attitudes.

Perceptions of congruence are contextually sensitive, and research has shown the impact

of a wide range of person and situational variables on different fit perceptions (Kristof, 1996, for

a review). Organization change programs and the way in which they are implemented, for

example, may substantially change employee perceptions of fit and congruence at one or more

levels, including organizational, work group, and job levels. Employee perceptions of fit may

also occur as a function of job tenure, age-related and/or nonwork related change associated with

the person, rather than the environment. At hire, employees may perceive job tasks as

challenging and report a high level of perceived congruence between the cognitive demands of

the job and one's knowledge, skills, and abilities. Over time, however, the perception of abilities-

demand fit may weaken as the demands of the job no longer require substantial effort or

attention. For these employees, a once challenging job has become boring.

A large research literature exists on the consequences of different forms of fit between

an individual and his/her work environment (Kristof, 1996). Surprisingly, however, only a

relatively small portion of this research focuses directly on motivational processes. During the

1980s, Dawis and Lofquist (1984) proposed a Theory of Work Adjustment that emphasized the

importance of person-environment fit, and described fit in terms of its effects on employee

motivation and need satisfaction. Although Dawis and Lofquist (1984) delineated the

motivational processes by which perceptions of fit direct and energize work performance, there

has been relatively little research to examine the validity of the their proposed pathways between

fit and work outcomes. From a different but complementary perspective, French, Caplan, & Van

Harrison (1982) highlighted the negative motivational consequences associated with perceptions

Page 66: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

66

of poor person-environment (P-E) in terms of higher feelings of job stress, higher levels of

negative affect, and more physical health problems (see Edwards, 1996; Shaw & Gupta, 2004).

Greguras and Diefendorff (2009) describe a promising new direction for theory and

research on the motivational processes and mechanisms by which perceived congruence may

affect performance and attitudes. Noting the communality between need satisfaction models and

universal motive theories, Greguras and Diefendorff (2009) proposed that intrinsic motives for

autonomy, relatedness, and competence mediate perceived fit-outcome relations. Findings

obtained using a sample of managers provide support for the mediating role of individual

differences in perceived satisfaction of these motives in different perceived fit - performance and

attitude relations. Consistent with SDT, as well as the Theory of Work Adjustment (Dawis &

Lofquist, 1984), these findings indicate that perceptions of congruence in different dimensions

influences work performance and attitudes through its influence on intrinsic motive satisfaction.

The Greguras and Diefendorff (2009) findings represent part of a growing trend in fit research to

investigate the positive, rather than negative, consequences of perceptions of different types of fit

on work behavior and performance (Vancouver & Schmitt, 1991).

Consequences of Motivation: The Criterion Problem

Most work motivation research evaluates motivation in terms of the behaviors and

performance observed on a single target task. In the modern workplace, however, employees are

often assigned multiple tasks, each with a different deadline for completion. In these instances,

assessments of motivation require measurement of the individual’s resource allocation across

tasks as well as resource allocations within each task. In multiple goal or task situations, the

assessment of motivation across tasks typically involves measures of time allocated to each task.

To fully evaluate motivation in multiple goal or task environments, however, researchers must

Page 67: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

67

also assess the impact of time to total resource allocation across all tasks, and how changes in

goals, self-efficacy, and goal pursuit in each component task affect time allocations to other

tasks. An excellent example of the complexities and advantages involved in assessing motivation

in a multiple goal regulation setting is provided by Schmidt et al. (2009).

Changes in the nature of work also raise the question of what are the appropriate indices

of work motivation. Extant models of work motivation are designed to provide prediction of

effort and persistence on a single, well-defined task (e.g., painting a room). However, many

modern jobs require motivation to accomplish multiple, inter-dependent tasks (e.g., prepare and

launch a new store opening) or ill-defined tasks (e.g., produce a new company slogan).

Motivation in these job tasks is likely to involve complex and inter-related resource allocation

policies governing the use of time, effort intensity, and the use of personal capital (e.g., social

and technical knowledge) for successful performance. Research is needed to determine the

factors that influence effective and ineffective strategies for goal pursuit in multi-form and

creative task accomplishments. Findings from this line of work are likely to be of substantial

practical importance for the early identification of motivational problems, and for the

development of effective interventions in these task settings.

Work Motivation Strategies and Practices

Advances in work motivation are closely connected to changes in the nature of work.

During most of the 20th century, motivation theory and practice focused on identifying the

fundamental determinants of work motivation, the psychological processes involved in goal

choice and goal pursuit, and effective organizational strategies to increase employee effort on the

job and to reduce turnover. Expectancy-value theories, goal setting, participative decision-

Page 68: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

68

making approaches, job characteristics theories, and equity theory generated research that added

new knowledge to predicting motivation for a specific work task.

At the start of the 21st century, however, most employees perform multiple work tasks,

often concurrently. Over the course of a workweek, a registered nurse may treat a dozen patients,

communicate information about each patient to physicians, radiologists, surgeons, and physical

therapists, update electronic patient files, counsel patient families, and negotiate schedule and

workload with other nurses on the unit. Accomplishment of each task requires allocation of

personal resources that vary as a function of task demands, task deadlines, importance, and

intrinsic interest in the task. It is no longer feasible or desirable for organizations to monitor and

reward performance of each job task as it occurs. In the 21st century job, employees are often

tasked with motivating themselves to organize job tasks, to allocate personal resources to

different task goals as conditions dictate, and to modify allocation policies when performance

progress is not sufficient. In short, job performance in many workplaces today demands self-

management. From a resource allocation perspective, the most effective motivational strategy is

one in which performance accomplishments are associated with attainment of valued rewards. In

short, the most effective strategy is intrinsic motivation, in which tasks are performed for their

own sake and performance accomplishments satisfy intrinsic motives for competence,

achievement, control, self-determination, and autonomy. Consistent with this notion, purposive

theories, such as Dweck’s goal orientation theory Deci’s SDT, have rapidly gained popularity.

Purposive theories of motivation emphasize goal pursuit and the use of intrinsic

incentives to power self-regulatory activities over time for goal accomplishment. As Steel and

Konig (2006) TMT suggests, extrinsic incentives such as yearly bonuses, are frequently too far

in the future to motivate allocations of time and effort to the accomplishment of immediate sub-

Page 69: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

69

goals prerequisite for achieving the complex goal outcome at some future date. In training, for

example, extrinsic incentives (e.g., promotion or pay raise contingent on final course

performance) are not powerful incentives for allocating more time to studying at the beginning of

the course. And the provision of extrinsic incentives for attainment of each subgoal is not a

realistic option. SDT and purposive theories of motivation address this practical problem by

delineating the conditions that promote sustained self-regulation of effort over time using

intrinsic incentives, such as a sense of self-determination, communion with others, competence,

control, and autonomy.

Accordingly, the problem in modern motivation theory and practice is not whether

extrinsic rewards undermine intrinsic motivation, but rather how to use extrinsic rewards to

promote and sustain intrinsic task motivation. Although more empirical research is needed on

this question, the findings to date suggest that the negative impact of extrinsic rewards on

intrinsic motivation may be limited to specific person-situation conditions. In routine jobs, such

as cashiering, initial interest in work tasks and performance motivation may be low without the

provision of performance-contingent extrinsic bonus payments to stimulate the adoption of

difficult goals and goal striving. Over the course of goal pursuit, however, individuals are likely

to become more proficient at the job, and goal attainment may yield intrinsic as well as extrinsic

rewards. As intrinsic incentives become more salient, extrinsic rewards for performance

accomplishment may be phased out, as individuals pursue higher levels of performance for

purposes of intrinsic motive satisfaction. Depending on the task, however, intrinsic motivation

may wane over time as further allocations of time and effort yield smaller improvements in

performance and correspondingly weaker intrinsic rewards. Experienced cashiers, for example,

are likely to perform at a high level with less effort due to task proceduralization, making further

Page 70: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

70

performance improvement and intrinsic rewards for performance less likely unless the individual

allocates substantial increases in time and effort. However, the provision of performance-

contingent extrinsic rewards again at this point, in the form of social recognition or

employee/team competitions among similarly experienced individuals, may stimulate the higher

allocations of time and effort necessary for improved performance and heightened feelings of

competence and self-determination. In summary, this analysis suggests that the primary purpose

of extrinsic rewards in the modern workplace (beyond rewards associated with the employment

contract) is to promote and sustain intrinsic work motivation, rather than to directly sustain

motivation and performance indefinitely.

Findings in goal setting and goal orientation research provide indirect evidence to support

this conceptualization. Numerous studies show that extrinsic rewards exert a positive influence

goal choice, but can also exert a detrimental impact on goal pursuit. Findings in goal orientation

further show that individuals who adopt goals for purposes related to the satisfaction of intrinsic

motives related to learning, mastery and accomplishment employ more effective self-regulatory

strategies and show higher levels of performance than persons who adopt goals for purposes

related to satisfaction of other-oriented, extrinsic motives (to prove one’s ability to others, to

avoid looking incompetent to others). In some instances, however, when task demands are low or

individuals are experienced at the task, the adoption of a performance prove goal orientation may

promote the use of effective self-regulation strategies and higher levels of performance.

To date, researchers have focused on the goal attributes that facilitate intrinsically

rewarded forms of goal pursuit and higher levels of performance. However, as reviews by Parker

and Ohly (2008) and Fried and Ferris (1987) suggest, intrinsic motivation may also be promoted

or diminished by the context of action. For example, using Meyer’s notion of situational

Page 71: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

71

specificity, work contexts that are high in clarity and consistency are likely to produce

environments that are conducive to the development of intrinsic motivation, while environments

that are high in constraints and consequences are likely to diminish intrinsic motivation.

Similarly, the effectiveness of transformation leaders in enhancing subordinate motivation may

be best understand in terms of what these leaders do to create conditions that induce intrinsic

motivation among subordinates. Organizing diverse motivational strategies in terms of their

impact on facilitating intrinsic forms of motivation is likely to greatly facilitate the translation of

research findings into effective managerial practices.

Practical Challenges and Emerging Research Directions

The previous sections highlight recent progress in work motivation theory and practice.

Although theory and research continues in the quest for basic understanding (e.g., Stokes, 1997),

practical concerns have spurred research in a number of new directions. In this section I describe

a few current practical challenges, and their implications for research and practice.

Aging and work motivation. Over the next three decades, the proportion of the workforce

over the age of 65 is predicted to grow to nearly 30%. Organizations are already struggling with

how to effectively recruit, manage, motivate, retrain, and retain older workers. Early theory and

research on work motivation as a function of adult development suggests that age-related

changes in cognitive abilities and nonability traits, motives, and interests exert an important

influence on work motivation and performance. In particular, age-related changes in abilities and

skills among individuals that perform jobs which place strong demands on these abilities and

skills (e.g., firefighter) is likely to have a direct influence on perceptions of person-job fit, work

attitudes, and retirement intentions. Age and job tenure may also exert a negative influence on

work motivation among individuals who perform a routine job that does not make demands on

Page 72: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

72

age-sensitive abilities and skills, but rather affords few opportunities for skill utilization or

intrinsic motive satisfaction. Employee age may also play a negative role in work motivation as a

consequence of age-sensitive attitudes and behaviors of coworkers, supervisors, or clients. In

these instances, motivation for work may decline sharply as opportunities for satisfaction of

affiliation motives and a positive self-concept shrink.

To date, organizations have responded to age-related problems in work motivation using

a variety of strategies, including changing the work role, providing age-appropriate incentives for

staying on the job, and educating managers about the influence of age on work competencies.

However, few applications are evidence-based and studies of age-related changes in motives,

interests, and self-regulation strategies remain sparse (for exceptions see Kooij, De Lange,

Jansen, Dikkers, & Kanfer, in press; see Shultz & Adams, 2007, for a review). Future research in

this area will require a reconsideration of the achievement construct and longitudinal research

designs that permit investigation of how person and occupation characteristics influence

motivation to work and motivation at work.

Motivation in teams. Teams are ubiquitous in organizations, and new technologies have

spurred the increasing use of inter-professional teams and teams of teams, or multi-team systems

in the workplace (Marks, DeChurch, Mathieu, Panzer, & Alonzo, 2005). Corresponding to this

trend, theory and research on team-level processes and motivation surged during the late 20th

century. Nonetheless, there are still a number of practically-important questions to be addressed

regarding the determinants and consequences of motivational processes in the team context.

Marks et al. (2001) suggest that motivational processes play a crucial role in team-level activities

to manage interpersonal relationships that support team performance. Despite these advances in

team motivation, our understanding of cross-level influences between team-level and team

Page 73: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

73

member motivation processes remains incomplete. To date there has been relatively little

research examining the relationship between different motivational states and interpersonal

management strategies (see Rico, Sanchez-Manzanares, Gil, & Gibson, 2008). We also do not

fully understand the key factors that influence the spread of affective states, such as

disappointment, across members of the team, or the cross-level effects of affective states on

individual and team-level decision making as they may play out in real-world military,

healthcare, and other work team settings. In a related vein, research to understand the influence

of motivational orientations on action processes and interpersonal goal conflicts is another

promising direction for future research. Using the adaptive team leadership model proposed by

Kozlowski, Watola, Jensen, Kim, and Botero (2009) as an organizing framework for such

research may be quite fruitful.

Relational dynamics and work motivation. As jobs move from production to services,

relationships with coworkers, supervisors, and clients take on increased importance in ratings of

job performance. Findings across a variety of topics suggest that these social relationships are

also potentially important determinants of work motivation and behavior. Numerous studies in

leadership, for example, have focused on the impact of a leader’s relationship with a subordinate

on employee work motivation, job attitudes, and performance. Studies of emotion regulation in

service jobs, and research on events that trigger strong affective reactions also suggest that

interpersonal conflicts demand additional resource allocations for emotion regulation and

restoration of attentional focus to work goals. Interpersonal dynamics are also frequently

implicated in the direction of resource allocations, such as toward helping others or engaging in

counterproductive behaviors.

Page 74: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

74

Research is needed to understand how coworker dynamics facilitate and dampen work

motivation and influence the direction of action. Coworker relations influence the interpretation

of work events and can also elicit strong, other-oriented affective feeling states, such as pride,

shame, guilt, and jealousy. Social relations and the individual’s social ties at work can also

contribute to an individual’s work identity. Social power is also likely to influence the success of

job crafting efforts. In the modern workplace, coworker relations are typically embedded in a

social network that can affect motivation for coming to work and for behavior at work (e.g.,

Bowler & Brass, 2006). Research is needed to examine how attributes of an individual’s social

network, and change in relationships with key members of the network influence an individual’s

goals and methods for task accomplishment.

Motivation and leadership. Motivation is an integral part of leadership theory and

practice. Advances during the 20th century focused on the behaviors and processes by which

supervisors and leaders win the “mind” of their subordinates and followers. Investigations of

how leaders motivate workers in the team context represents a fertile area for investigating the

impact of implicit leader behaviors on subordinate motivation, as well as the role of affectively-

driven leader behaviors on winning the “hearts” of followers through goal orientation and

commitment processes.

Work motivation over time. As noted previously, we still know relatively little about the

determinants of work motivation cycles or their natural timescales. Studies of within-subject

variability have focused largely on affect and mood states. Within-subject studies to identify the

impact of personality traits and occupational demands on variability in the direction of resource

allocations, motivational state, and goal revision cycles will help to inform the relative

Page 75: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

75

contribution of situational strength and implicit motivation processes on work behavior. Findings

from this line of research also have practical implications for work design.

Motivation in training and development. The development of dynamic models of learning

and investigations of motivation within these models reflects the renaissance that is taking place

in this domain. Although there has been substantial progress made in specifying the motivational

processes involved in self-regulated learning, organizations and social policy makers continue to

raise important research questions in this area. For example, new instructional technologies and

e-learning opportunities raise many questions about how these structures may affect situational

strength and the self-regulatory strategies used during learning and for transfer. With tens of

millions of adults currently engaged in on-line skill learning, a better understanding the

motivational implications of these environments on learning, behavior, and performance is

crucial.

Research on motivation in training would also benefit from closer attention to the

trainee’s learning environment. Increasingly, individuals complete training in groups and classes

characterized by diversity in age, gender, and ethnicity. Training is often conducted on-line

during nonwork time. Anecdotal stories by trainees suggest that trainee class diversity, limited

social interactions with other students and the teacher, evaluation apprehension, and the cost of

training on nonwork activities may exert deleterious effects on training motivation. Research to

identify best practices for sustaining motivation during training, particularly among mid- and

late-life trainees is urgently needed.

Putting back the “I” in work motivation. Theory and research in work motivation

continues to focus on the role that self-related variables, such as self-efficacy and core self-

Page 76: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

76

evaluations play in goal choice, goal pursuit, and job performance. A logical next step in this

direction is to consider the role of broader self constructs, such as work identity on motivation.

Work identity refers to self-concept that is grounded in the tasks that individuals perform,

the work roles that they adopt, the organizations they work in, the occupations with which they

identify, and the ways that they go about performing their work (Walsch & Gordon, 2008). As

such, work identity is a developmental process that likely creates a work schema which may

show greater resistance to change with age.

A small but growing number of researchers have begun to study work motivation from an

identity perspective. Research by Wrzesniewski (2003; Wrzesniewski, McCauley, Rozin, &

Schwartz, 1997), for example, distinguishes between individuals who view their work as a job

(focused on attainment of extrinsic rewards, such as pay), a career (focused on advancement), or

a calling (intrinsically motivated engagement, for the purpose of performing socially useful

work). In a related vein, Wrzesniewski and Dutton (2001) introduced the term “job crafting” to

refer to the ways in which individuals attempt to shape the job in a way to provide greater

opportunities for satisfaction of intrinsic and social motives (similar to accommodation strategies

discussed previously). Although the main focus of work motivation theory and research will

likely remain on proximal influences on work behavior and job performance, identity theories

offer a higher-level conceptualization of motivational orientation that may be particularly helpful

in identifying patterns of goal choice, goal pursuit, job crafting, affective reactions, and work

outcomes associated with individuals with different perceptions of the purpose for their work.

Work motivation and well-being. Self-concordance theory (Sheldon, 2002; Sheldon &

Elliot, 1999) posits that individuals who pursue goals and activities they enjoy and believe in

experience higher levels of subjective well being. As applied to work, this theory suggests that

Page 77: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

77

work motivation plays an important role in worker well-being. Although work motivation has

been rarely studied in terms of worker well being beyond that of job satisfaction (for an

exception, Judge, Bono, Erez, & Locke, 2005), mounting societal and organizational concern

about worker well-being suggests that more attention be given to the factors that promote self-

concordant motivation and the consequences of self-concordant motivation for worker well-

being. Findings by Bono and Judge (2003) indicate the role of transformational leadership in

promoting self-concordant motivation among subordinates. Further research is needed to extend

these findings and more precisely delineate the motivational processes that enhance worker well-

being.

Summary and Discussion

Although work motivation remains an area of scientific and practical importance, this

review suggests that there has been substantial change in the key constructs, processes, and

issues that currently command the attention of motivational scientists and practitioners. In

contrast to prior theories that emphasized rational models of goal choice, explicit person motives,

and self-management processes, emerging formulations focus on the determinants and

consequences of the individual’s goal orientation, and the impact of nonconscious processes and

implicit motives on goal choice, goal pursuit, affective states, and performance. Although self-

efficacy, goals, and personality traits continue to be studied in work motivation, the focus of

attention has subtly shifted toward understanding the dynamics by which these inputs change

over time. And affect, long accorded a subordinate role in motivational processing, continues to

garner research attention as an independent influence on behavior and a major determinant of

self-regulatory processing. Scientific advances, the changing nature of work, and the growing

emphasis on worker adjustment has, in turn, shifted research attention to different aspects of

Page 78: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

78

content, context, and change influences on work motivation. As discussed previously in this

chapter, Table 2 highlights some of the current gaps in our knowledge and potentially fruitful

areas for future research.

-------------------------------

Insert Table 2 about here

-------------------------------

At a more general level, two themes permeate recent theory and research in work

motivation. The first theme pertains to the increasing use of person-centric rather than

performance-centric approaches. Performance-centric approaches minimize within-person

dynamics and emphasize how changes in work design, managerial practices, and organization-

level variables (e.g., climate) affect motivation and performance of different individuals at a

particular point in time. Accordingly, performance-centric approaches tend to bias investigations

about the determinants of motivation and performance toward the study of stable person traits

and changes in the environment. Such approaches have been quite useful, particularly in the

study of motivation in production environments where job structure is often high, an individual’s

goal choice can be accomplished through an immediate increase in allocations of effort (rather

than prolonged goal pursuit), and behavior is often closely coupled to job performance.

In contrast to performance-centric approaches, person-centric approaches emphasize the

role of adult development and ongoing person-situation interactions in work motivation. Person

characteristics, goals, and experiences are posited to influence what features of the environment

are salient to the individual, the interpretation of these perceptions, and their translation into

motivational variables, such as self-efficacy. Person-centric approaches also assume that changes

in motivation and work behavior bring about changes in both the individual and the environment.

Page 79: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

79

The adoption of a person-centric approach to work motivation offers several distinct

theoretical and practical advantages. Person-centric approaches encourage multi-level,

longitudinal research designs that can illuminate potential differences in the impact of context

both across persons and within persons over time, as well as the potential impact of person traits

on context and within-person motivation over time. From a practical perspective, person-centric

approaches provide organizations with information about age-normed influences on work

motivation and how different interventions affect different aspects of work behavior over time.

This is particularly important in the modern workplace, where organizations may not be able to

precisely predict which behaviors and work outcomes are going to have the greatest importance

for job performance in the future. The many advantages of person-centric approaches for

studying person and situation factors that influence goal formulation and goal pursuit in the

workplace are likely to increase the dominance of this perspective for some time.

A second theme that runs through much of the recent work in the field pertains to the

growing interest in the role that affect and social relations play in work motivation. For most of

the 20th century, theories of work motivation viewed affect as a static influence on performance,

either in terms of the valence that individuals attach to different work outcomes, or as a work

attitude (Weiss & Cropanzano, 1996). However, research on implicit motives, nonconscious

motivational processes, and events that trigger nonconsciously mediated behaviors (such as

anger) maintain a very different view of affect. These approaches generally view affect as

dynamic and as distinct, biologically-driven, nonconscious processes associated with behavior

tendencies that activate during action in much the same way that explicit traits influence

cognitively-mediated motivation processes. This conceptualization puts motivation and affect on

a more equal footing, and suggests that motives and nonconscious affective tendencies operate in

Page 80: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

80

unison to influence goal choice and goal pursuit (i.e., work motivation). Indeed, newer theories

suggest that explicit self-regulatory activities largely operate in the service of managing

nonconscious affective tendencies that conflict with explicit goal accomplishment (e.g., Kehr,

2004), and the existence of a nonconscious goal and regulatory system for these affectively-

driven action tendencies (Bargh, 2008).

Work motivation researchers are also devoting more attention to how social interactions

and interpersonal dynamics influence affective states and nonconscious affective action

tendencies. The rich social medium in which modern work is performed increases the likelihood

that different emotions will be activated, either as a direct consequence of social interactions or

in the anticipation of social interactions (e.g., anxiety). Research currently in progress to

investigate the relationship between social interactions and the stream of emotions that occur

over the workday can be expected to importantly contribute to further progress in understanding

the impact of affect on work motivation.

The adoption of person-centric approaches and the emphasis on social relations and

affective processes increases the salience of several thorny questions in motivational science,

such as how individuals prioritize resource allocations in multiple goal/task settings, why and

how individuals adjust resource allocations to multiple goals/tasks over time, and how

individuals resolve goal conflicts, when individuals abandon goals, and factors that affect

strategy choice for regulation of implicit motive tendencies and negative emotions. As this

review attests, there is good reason to be optimistic that the field will continue to make progress

answering these and other central questions about work motivation. Theory and research

advances over the past two decades also show that work motivation researchers are equally

inspired by pressing practical problems in the workplace. Examples of emerging research

Page 81: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

81

streams inspired by real-world issues include studies on motivation in and of teams, the impact

of culture and work unit diversity on motivation and performance, the antecedents and

consequences of different motivational strategies during job search, the effects of motivation on

successful aging in the workplace, the influence of motivational conflicts on self-regulated job

performance, an motivational influences on creativity and innovation. The nature of recent

advances, driven equally by the goal of translating basic research for practical use, and the goal

of translating organizational and worker motivation concerns into workable research questions,

bodes well for the field.

Page 82: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

82

References

Ackerman, P. L. (1996). A theory of adult intellectual development: Process, personality,

interests, and knowledge. Intelligence, 22, 229-259.

Ackerman, P. L. (1997). Personality, self-concept, interests, and intelligence: Which construct

doesn’t fit? Journal of Personality, 65, 171-204.

Ackerman, P. L. (in press). 100 years without resting. In P. L. Ackerman (Ed.), Cognitive

fatigue. Multidisciplinary perspectives on current research and future applications.

Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. Advanced Experimental Social Psychoogy, 62,

335-343.

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 50, 179-211.

Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, C. T. (1999). Old friends, new faces: Motivation research in the

1990s. Journal of Management, 25, 231-292.

Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-

analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471-499.

Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2003). The effects of mindset on behavior: Self-regulation in

deliberative and implemental frames of mind. Personality and Social Psychology

Bulletin, 20, 86-95.

Ashford, S. J., & Cummings, L. L. (1983). Feedback as an individual resource: Personal

strategies for creating information. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance,

32, 370-389.

Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action. New York: Wiley.

Page 83: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

83

Austin, J. T., & Klein, H. J. (1996). Work motivation and goal striving. In K. R. Murphy (Ed.),

Individual differences and behavior in organizations (pp. 209-257). San Francisco:

Jossey-Bass.

Bakker, A. B., Hakanen, J. J., Demerouti, E., & Xanthopoulou, D. (2007). Job resources boost

work engagement, particularly when job demands are high. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 99, 274-284.

Baldwin, T. T., Magjuka, R. J., & Loher, B. T. (1991). The perils of participation: Effects of

choice of training on trainee motivation and learning. Personnel Psychology, 44, 51-65.

Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self-efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 88, 87-99.

Bargh, J. A. (2008). Free will is un-natural. In J. Baer, J. Kaufman, & R. Baumeister (Eds.), Are

we free? The psychology of free will (pp. 128-154). New York: Oxford University Press.

Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2010). Motivation. In S. Fiske, D. Gilbert, & G.

Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 268-316). New York: Wiley.

Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job

performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1-26.

Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Strauss, J. P. (1993). Conscientiousness and performance of

sales representatives: Tests of the mediating effects of goal setting. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 78, 715-722.

Page 84: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

84

Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., & Piotrowski, M. (2002). Personality and job performance: Test

of the mediating effects of motivation among sales representatives. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 87, 43-51.

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego-depletion: Is the

active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252-

1265.

Beach, L. R., & Mitchell, T. R. (1987). Image theory: Principles, goals, and plans in decision-

making. Acta Psychologica, 66, 201-220.

Beier, M. E., & Kanfer, R. (2009). Motivation in training and development: A phase perspective.

In S. W. J. Kozlowski & E. Salas (Eds.), Learning, training, and development in

organizations (pp. 65-97). New York: Psychology Press.

Bell, B. S., & Kozlowski, S. W. J. (2002). Adaptive guidance: Enhancing self-regulation,

knowledge, and performance in technology-based training. Personnel Psychology, 55,

267-306.

Bell, B. S., & Kozlowski, S. W. J. (2008). Active learning: Effects of core training design

elements on self-regulatory processes, learning, and adaptability. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 93, 296-316.

Binnewies, C., Sonnentag, S., & Mojza, E. J. (2009). Daily performance at work: Feeling

recovered in the morning as a predictor of day-level job performance. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 30, 67-93.

Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Self-concordance at work: Toward understanding the

motivational effects of transformational leaders. Academy of Management Journal, 46,

554-571.

Page 85: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

85

Bowler, W. M., & Brass, D. J. (2006). Relational correlates of interpersonal citizenship behavior:

A social network perspective. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 70-82.

Brunstein, J. C., & Maier, G. W. (2005). Implicit and self-attributed motives to achieve: Two

separate but interacting needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 205-

222.

Button, S. B., Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1996). Goal orientation in organizational research:

A conceptual and empirical foundation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 67, 26-48.

Campbell, J. P., & Pritchard, R. D. (1976). Motivation theory in industrial and organizational

psychology. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational

Psychology (pp. 63-130). Chicago: Rand McNally.

Carr, J. Z., DeShon, R. P., & Dobbins, H. W. (2001). A process model of goal orientation. In R.

P. DeShon (Chair), New directions in goal orientation: Exploring the construct and its

measurement. Presented at 16th Annual Meeting of the Society for Industrial and

Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA.

Carstensen, L. L. (1993). Motivation for social contact across the life span: A theory of

socioemotional selectivity. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 40.

Chen, G., Bliese, P. D., & Mathieu, J. E. (2005). Conceptual framework and statistical

procedures for delineating and testing multilevel theories of homology. Organizational

Research Methods, 8, 375-409.

Chen, G., & Gogus, C. (2008). Motivation in and of work teams: A multilevel perspective. In R.

Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Motivation: Past, present, and future (pp.

285-318). New York: Routledge.

Page 86: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

86

Chen, G., Gully, S. M., Whiteman, J. A., & Kilcullen, R. N. (2000). Examination of relationships

among trait-like individual differences, state-like individual differences, and learning

performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 835-847.

Chen, G., & Kanfer, R. (2006). Toward a systems theory of motivated behavior in work teams.

Research in Organizational Behavior, 27, 223-267.

Chen, G., Kanfer, R., DeShon, R. P., Mathieu, J. E., & Kozlowski, S. W. J. (2009). The

motivating potential of teams: Test and extension of Chen and Kanfer’s (2006) cross-

level model of motivation in teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 110, 45-55.

Chen, G., Kirkman, B. L., Kanfer, R., Allen, D., & Rosen, B. (2007). A multilevel study of

leadership, empowerment, and performance in teams. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92,

331-346.

Chen, G., Webber, S. S., Bliese, P. D., Mathieu, J. E., Payne, S. C., Born, D. H., & Zaccaro, S. J.

(2002). Simultaneous examination of the antecedents and consequences of efficacy

beliefs at multiple levels of analysis. Human Performance, 15, 381-409.

Chiaburu, D. S., & Marinova, S. V. (2005). What predicts skill transfer? Exploratory study of

goal orientation, training, self-efficacy, and organizational supports. International

Journal of Training and Development, 9, 110-123.

Colquitt, J. A., Conlon, D. E., Wesson, M. J., Porter, C., & Ng, K. Y. (2001). Justice at the

millenium: A meta-analytic review of 25 years of organizational justice research. Journal

of Applied Psychology, 86, 425-445.

Page 87: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

87

Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training

motivation: A meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 85, 678-707.

Colquitt, J. A., & Simmering, M. J. (1998). Conscientiousness, goal orientation, and motivation

to learn during the learning processes: A longitudinal study. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 83, 654-665.

Cox, C. B., & Beier, M. E. (2009). The moderating effect of individual differences on the

relationship between the framing of training and interest in training. International

Journal of Training and Development, 13, 247-261.

Creed, P. A., King, V., Hood, M., & McKenzie, R. (2009). Goal orientation, self-regulation

strategies, and job-seeking intensity in unemployed adults. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 94, 806-813.

Dalal, R. S., & Hulin, C. L. (2008). Motivation for what: A multivariate, dynamic perspective of

the criterion. In R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Motivation: Past, present,

and future (pp. 63-100). New York: Routledge.

Dawis, R. V., & Lofquist, L. H. (1984). A psychological theory of work adjustment: An

individual-differences model and its applications. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota

Press.

Day, E. A., Yeo, S., & Radosevich, D. J. (2003). Comparing two- and three-factor models of

goal orientation: A meta-analysis. Paper Presented at the 18th Annual Meeting of the

Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Orlando, FL.

DeChurch, L. A., & Mesmer-Magnus, J. R. (2010) Measuring shared team mental models: A

meta-analysis. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 14, 1-14.

Page 88: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

88

Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Publishing Co.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goals pursuits: Human needs and

the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227-268.

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002). Self-determination research: Reflections and future

directions. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research

(pp. 431-441). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

DeShon, R. P., & Gillespie, J. Z. (2005). A motivated action theory account of goal orientation.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 1096-1127.

DeShon, R. P., Kozlowski, S. W. J., Schmidt, A. M., Milner, K. R., & Wiechmann, D. (2004). A

multiple-goal, multilevel model of feedback effects on the regulation of individual and

team performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1035-1056.

Diefendorff, J. M., & Lord, R. G. (2008). Goal striving and self-regulation processes. In R.

Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Motivation: Past, present, and future (pp.

151-196). New York: Routledge.

Diefendorff, J. M., & Mehta, K. (2007). The relations of motivational traits with workplace

deviance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 967-977.

Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motvational processes affecting learning. American Psychologist, 41,

1040-1048.

Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and

personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256-273.

Edwards, J. R. (1996). An examination of competing versions of the person-environment fit

approach to stress. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 292-339.

Page 89: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

89

Elliot, A. J. (2008). Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.

Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoidance achievement goals and

intrinsic motivation: A meditational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 70, 461-475.

Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 X 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501-519.

Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2001). Approach-avoidance motivation in personality: Approach

and avoidance temperaments and goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

82, 804-818.

Erez, M. (2008). Social-cultural influences on work motivation. In R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D.

Pritchard (Eds.), Motivation: Past, present, and future (pp. 501-538). New York:

Routledge.

Ferguson, M. J., Hassin, R., & Bargh, J. A. (2008). Implicit motivation. In J. Y. Shah & W. L.

Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science (pp. 150-166). New York: Guilford

Press.

Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to

theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and changing behavior: The reasoned action

approach. New York: Psychology Press.

Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-

developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906-911.

Page 90: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

90

Folger, R., & Cropanzano, R. (1998). Organizational justice and human resource management.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Ford, J. K., Smith, E. M., Weissbein, D. A., Gully, S. M., & Salas, E. (1998). Relationships of

goal orientation, metacognitive activity, and practice strategies with learning outcomes

and transfer. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(2), 218-233.

Frayne, C. A., & Latham, G. P. (1987). The application of social learning theory to employee

self management of attendance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 387-392.

French, J. R. P., Caplan, R. D., & Van Harrison, R. (1982). The mechanisms of job stress and

strain. New York: Wiley.

Frese, M., & Fay, D. (2001). Personal initiative (PI): An active performance concept for work in

the 21st century. Research in Organizational Behavior, 23, 133-187.

Frese, M., Kring, W., Soose, A., & Zempel, J. (1996). Personal initiative at work: Differences

between East and West Germany. Academy of Management Journal, 39, 37-63.

Frese, M., & Zapf, D. (1994). Action as the core of work psychology: A German approach. In H.

C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and

organizational psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 271-340). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting

Psychologists Press.

Fried, Y., & Ferris, G. R. (1987). The validity of the job characteristics model: A review and

meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 40, 287-322.

Fried, Y., Grant, A. M., Levi, A. S., Hadant, M., & Slowik, L. H. (2007). Job design in temporal

context: A career dynamics perspective. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 28, 911-

927.

Page 91: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

91

Fritz, C., & Sonnentag, S. (2005). Recovery, well-being and job performance: Effects of

weekend experiences. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 10, 187-199.

Fritz, C., & Sonnentag, S. (2006). Recovery, well-being, and performance-related outcomes: The

role of workload and vacation experiences. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 936-945.

Frost, B. C., Ko, C. E., & James, L. R. (2007). Implicit and explicit personality: A test of a

channeling hypothesis for aggressive behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1299-

1319.

Gagne, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and work motivation. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 26, 331-362.

Gagne, M., & Forest, J. (2008). The study of compensation systems through the lens of self-

determination theory: Reconciling 35 years of debate. Canadian Psychology, 49, 225-

232.

Gelfand, M. J., Erez, M., & Aycan, Z. (2007). Cross-cultural organizational behavior. Annual

Review of Psychology, 58, 479-514.

George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1996). Motivational agendas in the workplace: The effects of

feelings on focus of attention and motivation. Research in Organizational Behavior, 18,

75-109.

Gist, M. E., Bavetta, A. G., & Stevens, C. K. (1990). The effectiveness of self-management

versus goal setting training in facilitating training transfer. Academy of Management Best

Papers Proceedings, 117-121.

Goldstein, I. L. (1993). Training in organizations (3rd ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Page 92: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

92

Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino

(Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 53-92). New York: Guilford

Press.

Gollwitzer, P. M. (2003). Why we thought that action mind-sets affect illusions of control.

Psychological Inquiry, 14, 259-267.

Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kinney, R. F. (1989). Effects of deliberative and implemental mind-sets in

the control of action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1119-1127.

Greenberg, J., & Cropanzano, R. (1999). Advances in organizational justice. Stanford, CA:

Stanford Press.

Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). Measuring individual

differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 74, 1464-1480.

Greguras, G. J., & Diefendorff, J. M., (2009). Different fits satisfy different needs: Linking

person-environment fit to employee attitudes and performance using self-determination

theory. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 465-477.

Guion, R. M., & Gottier, R. F. (1965). Validity of personality measures in personnel selection.

Personnel Psychology, 18, 135-164.

Guerrero, S., & Sire, B. (2001). Motivation to train from the workers’ perspective: Example of

French companies. International Journal of Human Resource Management, 12, 988-

1004.

Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through the design of work: Test of a

theory. Organizational Behavior & Human Performance, 16, 250-279.

Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action. Berlin: Springer Verlag.

Page 93: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

93

Heckhausen, J., & Schultz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review, 102,

284-304.

Heggestad, E. D., & Kanfer, R. (2005). The predictive validity of self-efficacy in training

performance: Little more than past performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Applied, 11, 84-97.

Heimerdinger, S. R., & Hinsz, V. B. (2008). Failure avoidance motivation in a goal-setting

situation. Human Performance, 21, 383-395.

Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory focus as a motivational principle.

In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 1-46).

New York: Academic Press.

Hobfoll, S. E. (1989). Conservation of resources: A new attempt at conceptualizing stress.

American Psychologist, 44, 513-524.

Hollenbeck, J. R., & Klein, H. J. (1987). Goal commitment and the goal-setting process:

Problems, prospects, and proposals for future research. Journal of Applied Psychology,

72, 212-220.

Humphrey, S. E., Nahrgang, J. D., & Morgeson, F. P. (2007). Integrating motivational, social,

and contextual work design features: A meta-analytic summary and theoretical extension

of the work design literature. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1332-1356.

Hurtz, G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job performance: The big five revisited.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 869-879.

Hurtz, G. M., & Williams, K. J. (2009). Attitudinal and motivational antecedents of participation

in voluntary employee development activities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 635-

653.

Page 94: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

94

James, L. R. (1998). Measurement of personality via conditional reasoning. Organizational

Research Methods, 1, 131-163.

James, L. R., McIntyre, M. D., Glisson, C. A., Green, P. D., Patton, T. W., LeBreton, J. M.,

Frost, B. C., Russell, S. M., Sablynski, C. J., Mitchell, T. R., & Williams, L. J. (2005). A

conditional reasoning measure for aggression. Organizational Research Methods, 8, 69-

99.

James, L. R., & Rentsch, J. R. (2004). JUSTIFY to explain the reasons why: A conditional

reasoning approach to understanding motivated behavior. In B. Schneider & D. B. Smith

(Eds.), Personality and organizations (pp. 230-250). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Johns, G. (2006). The essential impact of context on organizational behavior. Academy of

Management Review, 31, 386-408.

Johnson, R. E., Lord, R. G., Rosen, C. C., & Chang, C. H. (2007). The implicit effects of

(un)fairness on motivation: What we aren’t aware of might be important! Unpublished

manuscript.

Johnson R. E., & Steinman L. (2009). The use of implicit measures for organizational research:

An empirical example. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 41, 202-212.

Johnson, R. E., Tolentino, A. L., Rodopman, O. B., & Cho, E. (2010). We (sometimes) know not

how we feel: Predicting job performance with an implicit measure of trait affectivity.

Personnel Psychology, 63, 197-219.

Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001) Relationship of core self-evaluations traits—self-esteem,

generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability—with job satisfaction

and job performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 86, 80-92.

Page 95: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

95

Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Erez, A., & Locke, E. A. (2005). Core self-evaluations and job and life

satisfaction: The role of self-concordance and goal attainment. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 90, 257-268.

Judge, T. A., & Hurst, C. (2007). Capitalizing on one’s advantages: Role of core self-evaluations.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1212-1227.

Judge, T. A., & Ilies, R. (2002). Relationship of personality to performance motivation: A meta-

analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 530-541.

Judge, T. A., Locke, E. A., & Durham, C. C. (1997). The dispositional causes of job satisfaction:

A core evaluations approach. Research in Organizational Behavior, 19, 151-188.

Kanfer, R. (1987). Task specific motivation: An integrative approach to issues of measurement,

mechanisms, processes, and determinants. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 5,

237-264.

Kanfer, R. (1989). Non-cognitive processes, dispositions, and performance: Connecting the dots

within and across paradigms. In R. Kanfer, P. L. Ackerman, & R. Cudeck (Eds.),

Abilities, motivation, and methodology: The Minnesota symposium on learning and

individual differences (pp. 375-388). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Kanfer, R. (1990). Motivation theory and industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D.

Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 1, 2nd ed.,

pp. 75-130). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Kanfer, R. (1992). Work motivation: New directions in theory and research. In C. L. Cooper & I.

T. Robertson (Eds.), International Review of Industrial and Organizational Psychology

(Vol. 7, pp.1-53). London: John Wiley.

Page 96: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

96

Kanfer, R. (1993). Education from a workplace perspective: Issues of self-management.

Resources in Education ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. 348-717.

Kanfer, R. (2009). Work and older adults: Motivation and performance. In C. J. Czaja & J. Sharit

(Eds.), The future of work for an aging population. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins

University Press.

Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An integrative/

aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. [Monograph]. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 74, 657-690.

Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (2004). Aging, adult development, and work motivation.

Academy of Management Review, 29, 440-458.

Kanfer, R., Ackerman, P. L., & Heggestad, E. (1996). Motivational skills and self-regulation for

learning: A trait perspective. Learning and Individual Differences, 8, 185-209.

Kanfer, R., Chen, G., & Pritchard, R. D. (2008). The three C’s of work motivation: Content,

context, and change. In R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Motivation: Past,

present, and future (pp. 1-16). New York: Routledge.

Kanfer, R., & Heggestad, E. (1997). Motivational traits and skills: A person-centered approach

to work motivation. In L. L. Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organizational

behavior (Vol. 19, pp. 1-57). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Kanfer, R., & Kantrowitz, T. M. (2002). Ability and non-ability predictors of performance. In S.

Sonnentag (Ed.), Psychological management of individual performance: A handbook in

the psychology of management in organizations (pp. 27-50). England: John Wiley.

Kanfer, R., & Phillips, J. S. (1969). Learning foundations of behavior therapy. Oxford, England:

John Wiley.

Page 97: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

97

Kanfer, R., & Stubblebine, P. (2008). Affect and work motivation. In N. M. Ashkansay & C. L.

Cooper (Eds.), Research companion to emotions in organizations (pp. 170-182).

Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar Publishing Ltd.

Kanfer, R., Wanberg, C., & Kantrowitz, T. M. (2001). Job search and employment: A

personality-motivational analysis and meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 86, 837- 855.

Kanfer, R., Wolf, M. B., Kantrowitz, T. M., & Ackerman, P. L. (2009). Ability and trait complex

predictors of job performance: a person-situation approach. Applied Psychology: An

International Review, 59, 40-69.

Karasek, R. (1979). Job decision latitude, job demands and mental strain: Implications for job

redesign. Administrative Science Quarterly, 24, 285-308.

Karasek, R. (1989). The political implications of psychosocial work redesign. Internet Journal of

Health Services, 19, 481-508.

Katzell, R. A., & Thompson, D. E. (1990). Work motivation: Theory and practice. American

Psychologist, 45, 144-153.

Kehr, H. (2004). Integrating implicit motives, explicit motives and perceived abilities: The

compensatory model of work motivation and volition. Academy of Management Review,

29, 479-499.

Keith, N., & Frese, M. (2005). Self-regulation in error management training: Emotion control

and metacognition as mediators of performance effects. Journal of Applied Psychology,

90(4), 677-691.

Keith, N., & Frese, M. (2008). Effectiveness of error management training: A meta-analysis.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 59-69.

Page 98: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

98

Klein, H. J., Austin, J. T., & Cooper, J. T. (2008). Goal choice and decision processes. In R.

Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Motivation: Past, present, and future (pp.

101-150). New York: Routledge.

Kooij, D. T. A. M., De Lange, A. H., Jansen, P. G. W., Dikkers, J. S. E., & Kanfer, R. (in press).

Age and work-related motives: Results of a meta-analysis. Journal of Organizational

Behavior.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Bell, B. S. (2006). Disentangling achievement orientation and goal

setting: Effects on self-regulatory processes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 900-916.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Ilgen, D. R. (2006). Enhancing the effectiveness of work groups and

teams. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7, 77-124.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Salas, E. (Eds). (2010). Learning, training, and development in

organizations. New York: Routledge/Taylor-Francis Group.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., Watola, D. J., Jensen, J. M., Kim, B. H., & Botero, I. C. (2009). Developing

adaptive teams: A theory of dynamic team leadership (pp. 113-155). In E. Salas, G. F.

Goodwin, & C. S. Burke (Eds.), Team effectiveness in complex organizations: Cross-

disciplinary perspectives and approaches. New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis

Group.

Kristof, A. L. (1996). Person-organization fit: An integrative review of its conceptualizations,

measurement, and implications. Personnel Psychology, 49, 1-49.

Kuhl, J. (1986). Human motivation: From decision making to action control. In B. Rehmer, B.

Jungermann, P. Laures, & G. Sevon (Eds.), New directions in research on decision

making (pp. 5-28). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

Page 99: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

99

Latham, G. P. (2007). Work motivation: History, theory, research and practice. Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage

Latham, G. P., & Brown, T. (2006). The effect of learning vs. outcome goals on self-efficacy,

satisfaction and performance in an MBA program. Applied Psychology: An International

Review, 55, 606-623.

Latham, G. P., & Frayne, C. A. (1989). Self management training for increasing job attendance:

A follow-up and a replication. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 411-416.

Latham, G. P., Erez, M., & Locke, E. A. (1988). Resolving scientific disputes by the joint design

of crucial experiments by the antagonists: Application to the Erez-Latham dispute re

participation in goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, 753-772.

Latham, G. P., & Pinder, C. C. (2005). Work motivation theory and research at the dawn of the

21st century. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 485-516.

Latham, G. P., Stajkovic, A. D., & Locke, E. A. (2010). The relevance and viability of

subconscious goals in the workplace. Journal of Management, 36, 234-255.

Lewin, K. (1938). Conceptual representation and the measurement of psychological forces.

Durham: Duke University Press.

Locke, E. A. (1968). Toward a theory of task motivation and incentives. Organizational

Behavior & Human Performance, 3, 157-189.

Locke, E. A. (1976). The nature and causes of job satisfaction. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M.

Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1319-1328).

Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task performance.

Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Page 100: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

100

Locke, E. A., Shaw, K. N., Saari, L. M., & Latham, G. P. (1981). Goal setting and task

performance: 1969-1980. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 125-152.

Locke, E. A., & Schweiger, D. M. (1979). Participation in decision-making: One more look.

Research in Organizational Behavior, 1, 265-340.

Lord, R. G., Diefendorff, J. M., Schmidt, A. M., & Hall, R. J. (2010). Self-regulation at work.

Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 543-568.

Lord, R. G., & Foti, R. J. (1986). Schema theories, information processing and organizational

behavior. In H. P. Sims, Jr. & D. A. Gioia (Eds.), The thinking organization: Dynamics of

organizational social cognitions (pp. 20-48). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Lord, R. G., & Moon, S. M. (2006). Individual differences in automatic and controlled regulation

of emotion and task performance. Human Performance, 19, 327-356.

Lubinski, D. (2000). Scientific and social significance of assessing individual differences.

“Sinking shafts at a few critical points.” Annual Review of Psychology, 51, 405-444.

Marks, M. A., DeChurch, L. A., Mathieu, J. E., Panzer, F. J., & Alonso, A. (2005). Teamwork in

multiteam systems. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 964-971.

Marks, M. A., Mathieu, J. E., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2001). A temporally based framework and

taxonomy of team processes. Academy of Management Review, 26, 35-376.

Mathieu, J. E., & Martineau, J. W. (1997). Individual and situational influences on training

motivation. In J. K. Ford & Associates (Eds.), Improving training effectiveness in work

organizations (pp. 193-222). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1991). A three-component conceptualization of organizational

commitment. Human Resources Management Review, 1, 61-89.

Page 101: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

101

Meyer, J. P., & Allen, N. J. (1997). Commitment in the workplace: Theory, research, and

application. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Meyer, J. P., Becker, T. E., & Vandenberghe, C. (2004). Employee commitment and motivation:

A conceptual analysis and integrative model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 992-

1007.

Meyer, J. P., & Herscovitch, L. (2001). Commitment in the workplace: Toward a general model.

Human Resource Management Review, 11, 299-326.

Meyer, R. D., & Dalal, R. S. (2009). Situational strength as a means of conceptualizing context.

Industrial and Organizational Psychology: Perspectives on Science and Practice, 2, 99-

102.

Meyer, R. D., Dalal, R. S., & Bonaccio, S. (2009). A meta-analytic investigation into the

moderating effects of situational strength on the conscientiousness-performance

relationship. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30, 1077-1102.

Meyer, R. D., Dalal, R. S., & Hermida, R. (2009). A review and synthesis of situational strength

in the organizational sciences. Journal of Management, 36, 121-140.

Michalak, J., Puschel, O., Joormann, J., & Schulte, D. (2006). Implicit motives and explicit

goals: Two distinctive modes of motivational functioning and their relations to

psychopathology. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13, 81-96.

Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley.

Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of gratification. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 16, 329-337.

Page 102: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

102

Mitchell, T. R. (1974). Expectancy models of job satisfaction, occupational preference, and

effort: A theoretical, methodological, and empirical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 81,

1053-1077.

Mitchell, T. R. (1980). Expectancy-value models in organizational psychology. In N. Feather

(Ed.), Expectancy, incentive and action (pp. ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Mitchell, T. R. (1982). Motivation: New directions for theory, research, and practice. Academy of

Management Review, 7, 80-88.

Mitchell, T. R., & Daniels, D. (2003). Motivation. In W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen, & R. J.

Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology, Vol. 12: Industrial psychology (pp. 225-254).

New York: Wiley.

Mitchell, T. R., Harmon, W. S., Lee, T. W., & Lee, D-Y. (2008). Self-regulation and multiple

deadline goals. In R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Motivation: Past,

present, and future (pp. 197-232). New York: Routledge.

Mitchell, T. R., Hopper, H., Daniels, D., George-Falvy, J., & James, L. R. (1994). Predicting

self-efficacy and performance during skill acquisition. Journal of Applied Psychology,

79, 506-517.

Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-control as a limited resource:

Regulatory depletion patterns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774-

789.

Naylor, J. C., Pritchard, R. D., & Ilgen, D. R. (1980). A theory of behavior in organizations. New

York: Academic Press.

Page 103: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

103

Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Conceptions of ability and achievement motivation. In R. Ames & C.

Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education (Vol. 1., pp. 39-73). San Diego, CA:

Academic Press.

Noe, R. A. (1986). Trainees’ attributes and attitudes: Neglected influences on training

effectiveness. Academy of Management Review, 11, 736-749.

Parker, S. K., & Ohly, S. (2008). Designing motivating jobs: An expanded framework for linking

work characteristics and motivation. In R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.),

Motivation: Past, present, and future (pp. 233-284). New York: Routledge.

Payne, S. C., Youngcourt, S. S., & Beaubien, J. M. (2007). A meta-analytic examination of the

goal orientation nomological net. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 128-150.

Pearsall, M. J., Christian, M. S., & Ellis, A. P. J. (2010). Motivating interdependent teams:

Individual rewards, shared rewards, or something in between? Journal of Applied

Psychology, 95, 183-191.

Pinder, C. C. (1998). Work motivation in organizational behavior. Saddle River, NJ: Prentice

Hall.

Pinder, C. C. (2008). Work motivation in organizational behavior (2nd ed.). New York:

Psychology Press.

Pritchard, R. D., Harrell, M. M., DiazGranados, D., & Guzman, M. J. (2008). The productivity

measurement and enhancement system: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology,

93, 540-567.

Randel, A. E., & Ranft, A. L. (2007). Motivations to maintain social ties with coworkers: The

moderating role of turnover intentions on information exchange. Group and Organization

Management, 32, 208-232.

Page 104: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

104

Rawsthorne, L. J., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: A meta-

analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3, 326-344.

Raynor, J. O., & Entin, E. E. (1982). Theory and research on future orientation and achievement

motivation. In J. O. Raynor, & E. E. Entin (Eds.), Motivation, career striving, and aging

(pp. 13-82). Washington, DC: Hemisphere.

Revelle, W. R. (1989). Personality, motivation, and cognitive performance. In R. Kanfer, P. L.

Ackerman, & R. Cudeck (Eds.), Abilities, motivation, and methodology (pp. 297-341).

Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Rico, R., Sanchez-Manzanares, M., Gil, F., & Gibson, C. (2008). Team implicit coordination

processes: A team knowledge-based approach. Academy of Management Review, 33,

163-184.

Ruud, C., & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the pursuit of goals operates outside of

conscious awareness. Science, 329, 47-50.

Salancik, G. R., & Pfeffer, J. (1978). A social information processing approach to job attitudes

and task design. Administrative Science Quarterly, 23(2), 224-253

Salas, E., Cooke, N. J., & Rosen, M. A. (2008). On teams, teamwork, and team performance:

Discoveries and developments. Human Factors, 50, 540-547.

Schmidt, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2007). What to do? The effects of discrepancies, incentives,

and time on dynamic goal prioritization. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 928-941.

Schmidt, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2009). Prior performance and goal progress as moderators of

the relationship between self-efficacy and performance. Human Performance, 22, 191-

203.

Page 105: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

105

Schmidt, A. M., & DeShon, R. P. (2010). The moderating effects of performance ambiguity on

the relationship between self-efficacy and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,

95, 572-581.

Schmidt, A. M., Dolis, C. M., & Tolli, A. P. (2009). A matter of time: Individual differences,

contextual dynamics, and goal progress effects on multiple goal self-regulation. Journal

of Applied Psychology, 94, 692-709.

Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). Assessment of implicit motives with a research

version of the TAT: Picture profiles, gender differences, and relations to other personality

measures. Journal of Personality Assessment, 77, 71-86.

Schultheiss, O. C., & Pang, J. H. (2007). Measuring implicit motives. In R. W. Robins, R. C.

Fraley, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology

(pp. 322-344). New York: Guilford Press.

Seo, M., Feldman-Barrett, L. F., & Bartunek, J. M. (2004). The role of affective experience in

work motivation. Academy of Management, 29, 423-439.

Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2005). Learning versus performance goals: When should each be

used? Academy of Management Executive, 19, 124-131.

Shaw, J. D., & Gupta, N. (2004). Job complexity, performance, and well-being: When does

supplies-values fit matter? Personnel Psychology, 57, 847-879.

Sheldon, K. M. (2002). The self-concordance model of healthy goal-striving: When personal

goals correctly represent the person. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of

self-determination theory (pp. 65-86). Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Page 106: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

106

Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need-satisfaction, and longitudinal well-

being: The self-concordance model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,

482-497.

Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P. R. (1988). The theory of reasoned action: A meta-

analysis of past research with recommendations for modifications and future research.

Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 325-343.

Shi, J., Lin, H., Wang, L., & Wang, M. (2009). Linking the big five constructs to organizational

justice. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 37(2), 209-222.

Shultz, K., & Adams, G. A. (2007). Aging and work in the 21st century. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.

Simons, J., Dewitte, S., & Lens, W. (2004). The role of different types of instrumentality in

motivation, study strategies, and performance: Know why you learn, so you’ll know what

you learn! British Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 343-360.

Simons, J., Vansteenkiste, M., Lens, W., & Lacante, M. (2004). Placing motivation and future

time perspective theory in a temporal perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 16,

121-139.

Simpson, P. A., Greller, M. M., & Stroh, L. K. (2002). Variations in human capital investment

activity by age. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 61, 109-138.

Sitzmann, T., Bell, B. S., Kraiger, K., & Kanar, A. M. (2009). A multilevel analysis of the effect

of prompting self-regulation in technology-delivered instruction. Personnel Psychology,

62(4), 697-734.

Sitzmann, T., & Ely, K. (2010). Sometimes you need a reminder: The effects of prompting self-

regualtory processes, learning and attrition. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 132-144.

Page 107: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

107

Sonnentag, S., & Frese, M. (2003). Stress in organizations. In W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen, & R.

J. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (Vol. 12, pp. 453-491). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Sonnentag, S., & Fritz, C. (2007). The Recovery Experience Questionnaire: Development and

validation of a measure for assessing recuperation and unwinding from work. Journal of

Occupational Health Psychology, 12, 204-221.

Sonnentag, S., & Kruel, U. (2006). Psychological detachment from work during off-job time:

The role of job stressors, job involvement, and recovery-related self-efficacy. European

Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology, 15, 197-217.

Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT measures of need for achievement:

Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 140-154.

Stajkovic, A. D., Locke, E. A., & Blair, E. S. (2006). A first examination of the relationships

between primed subconscious goals, assigned conscious goals, and task performance.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 1172-1180.

Steel, P., & Konig, C. J. (2006). Integrating theories of motivation. Academy of Management

Review, 31, 889-913.

Stokes, D. E. (1997). Pasteur’s quadrant. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution Press.

Taylor, S. E., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1995). Effects of mindset on positive illusions. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 213-226.

Thorndike, R. L. (Ed.). (1947). Research problems and techniques (Rep. No. 3). Washington,

DC: U.S. Printing Office.

Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2007). Restoring the self: Positive

affect helps improve self-regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental

Social Psychology, 43(2), 379-384.

Page 108: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

108

Tolli, A. P., & Schmidt, A. M. (2008). The role of feedback, causal attributions, and self-efficacy

in goal revision. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 692-701.

Triandis, H. C. (1980). Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. Nebraska Symposium on

Motivation 1979: Beliefs, Attitudes and Values (pp. 195-259). Lincoln: University of

Nebraska Press.

Truxillo, D. M., Bauer, T. N., Campion, M. A., & Paronto, M. E. (2006). A field study of the

role of big five personality in applicant perceptions of selection fairness, self, and the

hiring organization. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 14, 269-277.

Utman, C. H. (1997). Performance effects of motivational state: A meta-analysis. Personality

and Social Psychology Review, 1, 170-182.

Van Hooft, E. A., & Noordzij, G. (2009). The effects of goal orientation on job search and

reemployment: A field experiment among unemployed job seekers. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 94, 1581-1590.

Vancouver, J. B., More, K. M., & Yoder, R. J. (2008). Self-efficacy and resource allocation:

Support for a nonmonotonic, discontinuous model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93,

35-47.

Vancouver, J. B., & Schmitt, N. (1991). An exploratory examination of person-organization fit:

Organizational goal congruence. Personnel Psychology, 44, 333-352.

Vancouver, J. B., Thompson, C. M., Tischner, E. C., & Putka, D. J. (2002). Two studies

examining the negative effect of self-efficacy on performance. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 87, 506-516.

Page 109: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

109

Vancouver, J. B., Thompson, C. M., & Williams, A. A. (2001). The changing signs in the

relationships between self-efficacy, personal goals, and performance. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 86, 605-620.

VandeWalle, D. (1997). Development and validation of a work domain goal orientation

instrument. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 8, 995-1015.

Vroom, V. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.

Walsch, K., & Gordon, J. R. ( 2008). Creating an individual work identity. Human Resource

Management Review, 18, 46-61.

Warr, P. B. (2008). Work values: Some demographic and cultural correlates. Journal of

Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 81, 751-775.

Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Mechanisms of implementation intention effects: The role of

goal intentions, self-efficacy, and accessibility of plan components. British Journal of

Social Psychology, 47, 373-395.

Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affective events theory: A theoretical discussion of the

structure, causes, and consequences of affective experiences at work. Research in

Organizational Behavior, 18, 1-74.

Whitford, T., & Moss, S. A. (2009). Transformational leadership in distributed work groups: The

moderating role of follower regulatory focus and goal orientation. Communication

Research, 35, 1026-1046.

Winters, D., & Latham, G. P. (1996). The effect of learning versus outcome goals on a simple

versus a complex task. Group and Organization Management, 21, 235-250.

Wood, R. E., Mento, A. J., & Locke, E. A. (1987). Task complexity as a moderator of goal

effects: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72, 416-425.

Page 110: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

110

Wrzesniewski, A. (2003). Finding positive meaning in work. In K. S. Cameron, J. E. Dutton, &

R. E. Quinn (Eds.), Positive organizational scholarship (pp. ). San Francisco: Berrett-

Koehler.

Wrzesniewski, A., & Dutton, J. E. (2001). Crafting a job: Revisioning employees as active

crafters of their work. Academy of Management Review, 26, 179-201.

Wrzesniewski, A., McCauley, C., Rozin, P., & Schwartz, B. (1997). Jobs, careers, and callings:

People’s relations to their work. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 21-33.

Yeo, G., Loft, S., Xiao, T., & Kiewitz, C. (2009). Goal orientations and performance:

Differential relationships across levels of analysis and as a function of task demands.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 710-726.

Yeo, G., & Neal, A. (2006). An examination of the dynamic relationship between self-efficacy

and performance across levels of analysis and levels of specificity. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 91, 1088-1101.

Zaccaro, S. J., Hildebrand, K., & Nelson, J. (2008). Leadership processes and work motivation.

In R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. D. Pritchard (Eds.), Motivation: Past, present, and future

(pp. 319-360).New York: Routledge.

Zimmerman, B. J., & Schunk, D. H. (2001). Self-regulated learning and academic achievement:

Theoretical perspectives (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Page 111: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

111

Table 1. Work motivation determinants organized by Content, Context, and Change domain.

Cognitive Abilities Personality and Motivational Traits

Content (Inter-Individual) Influences on Work Motivation

Knowledge and Skills Affective/Implicit and Explicit Motives

Interests Values

Beliefs, Attitudes Self-Concepts

Context Influences on Work Motivation

Culture (Societal) Off-job Demands and Constraints

Organizational and Team Culture/Climate Organizational Practices and policies

Leadership/Social Relations Work role and job demands/design

Change-Related Influences on Work Motivation

Organizational Change Adult Development (within-person change)

Team Processes Self-Regulatory Activities

Work role/Job Redesign Job Experience/Learning

Page 112: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

112

Table 2. Examples of current knowledge gaps in work motivation research.

Content (Person) Influences

Person Well-Being (e.g., health)

:

Non-Conscious/Affective Motives (e.g., Hostility)

Motivational Signatures of Discrete Emotions

Trait Complexes

Context (Environment) Influences:

Situational Strength

Intra-team processes (e.g., conflict)

Multi-team processes (e.g., cultural differences, goal coordination demands)

E-Learning

Change-Related Influences:

Intrinsic motivation processes

Resource Depletion, Recovery, and Fatigue

Adult Development (Aging)

Page 113: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

113

Figure Captions

Figure 1. A static representation of the “Three C’s” model of work motivation.

Figure 2. Timescales/orders of magnitude, motivation targets, and methods in work motivation

research (adapted from Revelle, 1989).

Figure 3. An organizing framework for person attributes that influence work motivation (adapted

from Thorndike) and example person attributes.

Page 114: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Work Motivation

114

Page 115: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations
Page 116: Work Motivation: Theory, Practice, and Future Directions Motivation.pdf · This chapter focuses on recent scientific advances and use-inspired ... Employee motivation, self ... Foundations

Temporary

Lasting

Fatigue Cognitive Abilities

Stress Personality Traits General

Attribute Scope

Job Knowledge & Skills Specific

Vocational Interests Regulatory Focus

Attribute Permanence