when bigger is better (and when it is not): implicit bias...

78
1 When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias in Numeric Judgments ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* Forthcoming in the Journal of Consumer Research

Upload: others

Post on 07-Oct-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

1

When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias in Numeric Judgments

ELLIE J. KYUNG

MANOJ THOMAS

ARADHNA KRISHNA*

Forthcoming in the Journal of Consumer Research

Page 2: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

2

* Ellie J. Kyung ([email protected]) is Associate Professor of Business Administration at the Tuck School of Business, Dartmouth College, 100 Tuck Hall, Hanover, NH 03755. Manoj Thomas ([email protected]) is Associate Professor of Marketing at the Johnson Graduate School of Management, Cornell University, 353 Sage Hall, Ithaca, NY 14853. Aradhna Krishna ([email protected]) is the Dwight F. Benton Professor of Marketing at the Ross School of Business, University of Michigan, 701 Tappan Street, Ann Arbor, MI 48109. The authors would like to thank Linda Hagen for her help with German translation, Geoff Gunning, Kimberlee Hayward, and Matthew Paronto for their feedback and editorial assistance, Melissa Li for her research support, and Anne Fries for bringing the Stiftung Warentest rating system to their attention. They would also like to thank Mrinal Thomas, Maxine Park, and Roxane Park for their assistance in determining the title of this paper through a unanimous vote. Finally, the authors would also like to thank the reviewers, associate editor, and editor for their invaluable comments that greatly enhanced the paper. Supplemental materials, including experiment stimuli, additional graphs, and supplementary analyses are available in the Web Appendix online.

Page 3: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

3

Abstract Numeric ratings for products can be presented using a bigger-is-better format (1=bad, 5=good) or a smaller-is-better format with reversed rating poles (1=good, 5=bad). Seven experiments document how implicit memory for the bigger-is-better format—where larger numbers typically connote something is better—can systematically bias consumers’ judgments without their awareness. This rating polarity effect is the result of proactive interference from culturally determined numerical associations in implicit memory and results in consumer judgments that are less sensitive to differences in numeric ratings. This is an implicit bias that manifests even when people are mindful and focused on the task and across a range of judgment types (auction bids, visual perception, purchase intent, willingness-to-pay). Implicating the role of reliance on implicit memory in this interference effect, the rating polarity effect is moderated by (i) cultural norms that define the implicit numerical association, (ii) construal mindsets that encourage reliance on implicit memory, and (iii) individual propensity to rely on implicit memory. This research identifies a new form of proactive interference for numerical associations, demonstrates how reliance on implicit memory can interfere with explicit memory, and how to attenuate such interference. Keywords: implicit memory, interference, numerical cognition, rating format, mindset, cross-cultural marketing

Page 4: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

4

Cause bigger is better And big is just the best If you take my advice

You’ll outshine the rest - Princess Amber’s advice on party planning to Sofia in Disney’s Sofia the First

Whenever people are asked to evaluate things they encounter in life—products, job

candidates, employee performance, research proposals—numeric ratings are inevitably involved.

These numeric ratings can use a format where larger numbers indicate that something is better

(1=bad, 5=good) or have reversed rating poles where smaller numbers indicate that something is

better (1=good, 5=bad). The format that people are accustomed to encountering can be

culturally determined. For example in the United States, product ratings and student grade point

averages are typically presented with bigger-is-better polarity, and there is a strong association

between larger numbers and more positive evaluative judgments, not to mention a strong

association between “bigger” and “better” that permeates adverting for product size, popular

culture, and even children’s songs. However, in Germany, it is the opposite—lower numbers

indicate higher product quality and better student grade point averages, and people have a

culturally determined association between “smaller” and “better.” How then, can varying the

polarity of ratings used to evaluate products, students, or job applications influence judgments?

People might find themselves making evaluations based on a rating polarity format they

are less used to in cross-cultural contexts or simply when an evaluation system uses an opposite

rating polarity. For example, not only does the German equivalent of Consumer Reports, Stiftung

Warentest, rate products using a smaller-is-better system where 0.5 = very good and 5.5 =

unsatisfactory, but also grant applications for the National Institutes of Health in the U.S. are

scored by reviewers on a system ranging from 1 = exceptional to 9 = poor. These ratings are

used in peer review meetings to make funding decisions. We posit that when people attempt to

make evaluations using a rating polarity format opposite to the one they have grown up with, the

Page 5: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

5

numerical association they are accustomed to using can bias their judgments because they

experience a form of memory interference.

In the memory literature, interference where information from the past inhibits people’s

ability to use information learned in the future is referred to as proactive interference (Jonides

and Nee 2006; Keppel and Underwood 1962; Wickens, Born, and Allen 1963). For example, you

might have difficulty remembering a new phone number after a move because the old phone

number that you have had for years interferes with your ability to remember the new one.

Similarly, after encountering an advertisement for a brand that includes information such as

price, attribute, or tagline information, consumers’ ability to learn the same information for new

brands encounter later can be impaired due to interference (Blackenship and Whiteley 1941;

Burke and Srull 1988; Keller 1987; Keller, Heckler, and Houston 1998; Unnava and

Sirdeshmukh 1994).

In our research, we introduce a new form of proactive interference for numerical

associations that can systematically bias consumer evaluations. The culturally determined

numerical association that people learn over time becomes part of their implicit memory—the

type of memory that influences judgment without conscious awareness (Graff and Schacter

1987; Roediger 1990; Schacter 1987). This numerical association in implicit memory can then

interfere with people’s ability to make evaluations using a newly learned format with opposite

rating polarity, resulting in judgments that are less sensitive to numeric differences in quality

level. We refer to this new form of proactive interference for numerical associations as the

“rating polarity effect.”

Over a series of seven experiments, we demonstrate that consumers’ decisions are

repeatedly and persistently affected by the rating polarity effect, even when they are well aware

Page 6: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

6

that they are using a system with opposite rating polarity. The numeric association in implicit

memory surreptitiously influences consumers’ evaluations without their awareness. When asked

in an online forum whether the polarity of rating format should influence product evaluations,

79% of participants indicated they thought it should have no effect on their judgments (92

participants, 47% female, Mage: 38.7 years). Why, then, is it that people fall prey to this rating

polarity effect, yet believe they are immune to it? We hypothesize that this effect stems from

interference from implicit memory. We designed experiments to delineate the role of implicit

memory in this effect. In doing so, we show that the interference effects between implicit and

explicit memory can be attenuated by reducing reliance on implicit memory. Thus, together the

culturally determined numerical association in implicit memory and the rating polarity of the

evaluation system determine when “bigger-is-better,” and when it is not.

THEORETICAL BACKGROUND

Implicit Numerical Associations

Decades of research on memory and judgments suggest that two different types of

memory processes influence our everyday judgments: 1) information stored in explicit memory,

and 2) associations stored in implicit memory (Graf and Schacter 1987; Schacter 1987). Explicit

memory is characterized by intentional, conscious recollection of episodic information. In

contrast, implicit memory influences a task or judgment without conscious awareness or intent—

it encompasses the influence of past exposures and experiences, and can spontaneously, even

surreptitiously, influence judgments (Graff and Schacter 1987; Roediger 1990; Schacter 1987).

Page 7: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

7

Previous research suggests that implicit associations can play an important role in

numerical evaluations (Adaval and Monroe 2002; Bagchi and Davis 2012; King and Janiszewski

2011; Mishra, Mishra, and Nayakankuppam 2006; Monroe and Lee 1999; Monga and Bagchi

2012; Raghubir and Srivastava 2009; Thomas and Morwitz 2009). These implicit associations

can also form based on cultural context. For example, in some cultures people learn to associate

numbers with bad luck—13 in the U.S., 4 in China, Korea, and Japan, and 7 in Ghana, Kenya,

and Singapore (Jahoda 1969; Yates 2007). In others, people with left-to-right reading habits

associate larger numbers with a right-orientation and smaller numbers with a left-orientation, and

this spatial-numerical association is weaker for people in cultures, such as Iran, where people

read from right to left (Dehaene, Bossini, and Giraux 1993).

In this research, we characterize a new type of implicit numerical association: that

between numeric ratings of a particular magnitude and an evaluate judgment. For example,

people in the U.S. tend to have an implicit association in memory that bigger-is-better, naturally

associating higher numbers with higher quality, while those in countries such as Germany tend to

have an implicit association that smaller-is-better, naturally associating lower numbers with

higher quality. This implicit numerical association influences judgments even in situations where

they should not, leading to proactive interference.

Proactive Interference for Numerical Associations

Classic work in proactive interference typically examines how previously learned content

in explicit memory interferes with memory for new learned information. For example, in the

classic Keppel and Underwood (1962) paradigm, participants were shown a series of nonsense

Page 8: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

8

consonant syllables one at a time (KQF, MHZ, CXJ) and asked to repeat them back to the

experimenter after retrieval times of various lengths. Their research showed that the successive

recall accuracy for each syllable decreased as a result of proactive interference. Similarly, from

work in consumer behavior, when people were exposed to an initial advertisement with

information about price or other attributes, and then shown a second advertisement with similar

information (versus not), their recall of subsequent information was less accurate (Blackenship

and Whiteley 1941; Burke and Srull 1988; Keller 1991).

We examine a new type of proactive interference that can occur between a numerical

association in implicit memory and a numerical association explicitly provided in a rating

format. Specifically, when using a rating format with polarity opposite to that of the numerical

association stored in implicit memory, people’s evaluations will be unconsciously shifted in the

direction of their implicit numerical association. Thus, when an American consumer who is used

to bigger-is-better rating polarity comes across a rating format with smaller-is-better rating

polarity, her final evaluation will be unconsciously anchored in the direction of a spontaneously,

self-generated implicit numerical association in memory. This anchoring will bias the final

evaluation in the opposite direction towards the bigger-is-better rating polarity.

H1: In cultures where people hold the bigger-is-better numerical association in implicit

memory, product evaluations will be less responsive to differences in numeric

ratings when products are rated using smaller-is-better (vs. bigger-is-better) rating

polarity.

When products are rated at multiple quality levels, we expect that the slope of the relationship

between quality rating and subjective evaluations will be less steep when the products are rated

using smaller- versus bigger-is-better rating polarity.

Page 9: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

9

Note that unlike the conscious anchoring-and-adjustment process studied by researchers

such Tversky and Kahneman (1974) and other scholars, the anchoring effect in the rating

polarity effect is unconscious. Because of this, we posit that the underlying process is more

consistent with the literature on subliminal numeric priming (Adaval and Monroe 2002;

Mussweiler and Englich 2005) and unconscious stereotyping (Gilbert and Hixon 1991).

Specifically, because the anchoring process is unconscious and stems from an implicit

association in memory in the rating polarity effect, people do not adjust from an anchor they are

not aware they are using (see Mussweiler and Englich 2005 for a more detailed exposition of

anchoring effects that are not caused by insufficient adjustments).

The Role of Implicit Memory

Although proactive interference is a complex phenomenon that can be studied from

several theoretical perspectives such as memory, learning, automaticity, cognitive control, and

metacognitive monitoring, we restrict our focus to unearthing the role of implicit memory

because proactive interference for numerical associations is based on an underlying implicit

association in memory. If interference from the implicit numerical association, which occurs

automatically without awareness, underlies the rating polarity effect, then proactive interference

for numerical associations should be moderated by factors that activate or inhibit reliance on

implicit memory. We identify three such factors: cultural norms, construal mindset, and

individual propensity to rely on implicit memory.

Cultural Norms. Our theorizing suggests that cultural norms are an important moderator

of proactive interference in numerical cognition. Thus, in a country such as Germany where

Page 10: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

10

people hold an opposite numerical association in implicit memory, we hypothesize that the effect

of using a rating with bigger-is-better versus smaller-is-better rating polarity will reverse:

H2: In cultures where people hold the smaller-is-better numerical association in implicit

memory, their product evaluations will be less responsive to differences in numeric

ratings when products are rated using bigger-is-better (vs. smaller-is-better) rating

polarity.

Construal Mindset. Recent research suggests that construal mindset can influence

reliance on implicit memory. People make judgments and decisions along a continuum from

‘abstract’ to ‘concrete’ (Freitas, Gollwitzer, and Trope 2004; Trope and Liberman 2003;

Vallacher and Wegner 1989). In a more abstract mindset, people focus more on higher-level, gist

representations in memory while in a more concrete mindset, people focus more on lower-level,

verbatim representations. As it relates to memory, reliance on gist memory increases the use of

implicit associations in everyday judgments, whereas reliance on verbatim memory reduces it

(Fukakara, Ferguson, and Fujita 2013; Smith and Trope 2006; Rim, Uleman, and Trope 2009).

Based on this premise, Fukakara, Ferguson, and Fujita (2013) show that when comparing multi-

attribute stimuli, people in an abstract mindset are more likely to rely on gist memory, while

those in a concrete mindset rely more on verbatim memory. Using a false recognition paradigm,

Smith and Trope (2006; see experiment 4) demonstrate that an abstract mindset increases

reliance on implicit associations in gist memory, which in turn increases false recognition. Their

results also show that the effect of abstraction on false recognition judgments can be independent

of changes in effort or motivation. Similarly, research has shown that people in an abstract

mindset are more likely to make spontaneous trait inferences (Rim, Uleman, and Trope 2009)

and rely on stereotypes when making judgments (McCrea, Weiber, and Myers 2012), both of

Page 11: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

11

which rely on implicit associations. Thus, prior research suggests that an abstract mindset

increases reliance on implicit memory. Therefore, people should be more susceptible to

spontaneous proactive interference from an implicit numerical association under an abstract

mindset. A concrete mindset, which reduces reliance on implicit associations in memory, should

attenuate this interference.

H3: The rating polarity effect is more likely under conditions of an abstract construal

mindset than under conditions of a concrete construal mindset.

Direct Measure of Implicit (vs. Explicit) Memory. People differ in their propensity to rely

on implicit associations versus explicit rules in everyday judgments as can be seen in their

performance on dual task-tests that require people to allocate their attention between tasks that

require both implicit and explicit memory (de Neys 2006). If the rating polarity effect stems from

spontaneous interference from implicit memory when using a particular rating format, it should

be more pronounced for people who tend to rely more on their implicit memory, but not

necessarily for those who rely more on explicit memory. Based on this premise, we predict:

H4: The rating polarity effect will be stronger for people who chronically rely to a

greater extent on implicit memory.

OVERVIEW OF EXPERIMENTAL PARADIGM

Examining the effects of proactive interference requires a specific experimental

paradigm. As described by Jacoby (1991), measuring the extent of interference between implicit

associations and explicit rules requires comparing outcomes where the implicit association and

explicit rule are consistent versus when they are inconsistent. In our experiments we compare

two conditions—one condition where the numerical association of the rating format and the

Page 12: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

12

numerical association in implicit memory are consistent (consistent rating polarity), and another

where the numerical association of the rating format and the numerical association in implicit

memory are inconsistent (inconsistent rating polarity). If there is no interference between the

implicit numerical association in memory and the numerical association used in the rating

format, then judgments in the consistent and inconsistent rating polarity conditions should be

identical. However, if the judgments vary across the two conditions, this is evidence for

interference. The difference in evaluations between participants in the consistent versus

inconsistent rating polarity conditions reflects the extent of interference (Jacoby 1991).

Note that, a key objective of this research is to demonstrate that the rating polarity effect

is caused by implicit memory interference and that it manifests without the participants’

intention or awareness. To this end, for each experiment we conduct pre-evaluation and post-

evaluation comprehension tests. The pre-evaluation comprehension test after exposure to the

rating format, but before exposure to the stimuli, ensures that participants have read and

understood the information on rating polarity. Participants are allowed to proceed to the

experiment only after they pass this test. The post-exposure comprehension test is done after the

experiment and enables us to rule out inattention, miscomprehension, or forgetting as possible

alternative explanations for our results.

PRETEST: WILLINGNESS TO PAY

As a preliminary test of this experimental paradigm, we conducted an experiment

involving a sealed bid auction. Seventy two participants at a U.S. university were given the

opportunity to bid on a stainless steel mug that was described either as having a quality rating of

Page 13: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

13

6.1 on a scale where 1 = unsatisfactory and 7 = very good (bigger-is-better) or an equivalent

rating of 1.9 on scale where 1 = very good and 7 = unsatisfactory (smaller-is-better). Those that

read a description using bigger-is-better rating polarity bid significantly more (M = $4.42) than

those that read one using smaller-is-better rating polarity (M = $2.70, F (1, 70) = 4.80, p = .03;

see Web Appendix A for full description). This difference between the two conditions offers

preliminary support for the proposed interference effect.

EXPERIMENT 1: VISUAL PERCEPTION

Experiment 1 examines whether the rating polarity effect in the domain of visual

perception. Can the rating polarity effect distort visual perception and fool our eyes? All

participants were shown the same set of before and after photographs, and we examined whether

the rating polarity effect influenced their visual perception across products of both high and low

quality. Comprehension, mood, and need-for-cognition were also measured to rule out potential

alternative explanations.

Method

Participants. One hundred and seventy-two undergraduates from a U.S. university (63%

female; average age: 21.1 years) participated in this computer study in exchange for course

credit. They were randomly assigned to one of four conditions in a 2 (rating polarity: consistent

vs. inconsistent) x 2 (quality level: low vs. high) between-subjects design.

Procedure. Participants were told they would be presented information about a new

Page 14: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

14

peroxide-free, home teeth-whitening product currently available in Europe that the producer is

considering launching in the U.S. Their task was to evaluate how much change they saw in the

before versus after photographs that accompanied the product information. Participants were told

that a quality rating would be provided by a reputable consumer welfare agency in Europe

known for its evaluation of consumer products. Those in the consistent rating polarity condition

were told that 1 = unsatisfactory and 7 = very good while those in the inconsistent rating polarity

condition were told that 1 = very good and 7 = unsatisfactory. Half the participants were given a

high quality rating (consistent condition: 6.1; inconsistent: 1.9) and half were given a low quality

rating (consistent condition: 1.9; inconsistent: 6.1). Participants were asked to confirm that they

understood this rating format [Yes/No] before proceeding.

Pre-evaluation Comprehension Test. Participants were then asked the meaning of “1” and

“7” rating poles for the quality ratings [very good, unsatisfactory]. If they responded incorrectly,

a message asked them to correct their response, and they could proceed only after answering

these questions correctly. This was done to ensure that the results did not stem from inattention

or miscomprehension of the rating poles.

Visual Perception. All participants were then shown the same photographs of teeth before

and after treatment (see figure 1) and descriptive information, which included the quality level

for the product. The “before” and “after” photographs were pretested as showing moderate

improvement. The quality level rating that participants were shown was either 6.1 or 1.9—each

quality rating corresponded with a low versus high quality product depending on the rating

polarity. Participants were then asked: “How much whiter do the teeth look in the 'after' versus

the 'before' photograph?” [not at all whiter / much whiter], “How much cleaner do the teeth look

in the 'after' versus the 'before' photograph?” [not at all cleaner / much cleaner], “How much

Page 15: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

15

improvement do you see in how the teeth look after using the whitener?”[no improvement /

significant improvement]. These questions were asked using an unnumbered slider scale, but

coded as 0 to 100 by the program so as to avoid any numerical association (see Web Appendix

B). The three measures were used as an index of visual perception.

______________________________

Insert figure 1 about here ______________________________

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. After entering their bid, participants were asked

the meaning of the rating poles 1 and 7 [very good or unsatisfactory]. This was done to confirm

that the participants did not become confused or forget about the meaning of the ratings.

Rating Polarity Typicality. Participants were also asked to indicate which of the two

numerical associations is more typical: “Higher Numbers Indicate Better Quality” or “Lower

Numbers Indicate Better Quality.” They could also choose a third option labeled “not sure.”

Additional Measures. To rule out possible alternative explanations, participants were also

asked questions about their current mood [slider scales anchored at bad / good, unpleasant /

pleasant, negative / positive], given the 18 items from the short form Need For Cognition (NFC)

scale (Cacciopo, Petty, and Kao 1984), and asked questions about their demographics.

Results and Discussion

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. After the main task, all participants answered the

two-rating-pole comprehension test questions. Two percent of participants in the inconsistent and

0% of participants in the consistent rating polarity conditions did not answer both of these

questions correctly. Participants who answered either of these questions incorrectly were

Page 16: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

16

excluded from the analysis for this and all subsequent studies, although including them did not

change any of the main results. We followed this procedure in each of our experiments to ensure

that the reported results cannot be ascribed to inattention or miscomprehension.

Rating Polarity Typicality Check. A majority of participants indicated that the bigger-is-

better numerical association is more typical (83.7%), 14.0% indicated a smaller-is-better

association is more typical, and 2.3% indicated they were not sure, confirming the bigger-is-

better numerical association is more dominant in implicit memory for U.S. consumers.

Visual Perception. A two-way ANOVA with rating polarity and quality level as

independent measures and the visual perception index as the dependent measure (α = .82)

revealed a marginally significant effect of quality level. Participants saw the higher quality

product as more effective (M = 75.5) than the lower quality product (M = 70.9, F(1, 165) = 3.71,

p = .06). More importantly, we obtained a significant interaction (F(1, 165) = 4.17, p = .04).

Planned comparison tests revealed, as predicted, that in the consistent rating polarity conditions

(bigger-is-better), participants saw the high quality product as demonstrating significantly greater

improvement in the before versus after photographs (Mhigh = 78.3) than the low quality product

(Mlow = 68.8, F(1, 165) = 7.48, p < .01). However, in the inconsistent rating polarity conditions

(smaller-is-better), the effect of quality rating is attenuated (Mhigh = 72.8 versus Mlow = 73.1, F(1,

165) = .007, p = .93, see figure 2). When the rating polarity used a numerical association

inconsistent with the numerical association in implicit memory, participants’ evaluations of

visual improvement were less responsive to differences in quality level, supporting H1. Web

Appendix C summarizes the means and 95% confidence intervals by conditions across all

experiments, and for further data visualization, also plots the means and 95% confidence

intervals for the consistent versus inconsistent rating polarity conditions.

Page 17: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

17

______________________________

Insert figure 2 about here ______________________________

Ruling Out Alternative Explanations. Mood: The three measures of mood were averaged

into an index (α = .93), and a two-way ANOVA with rating polarity and quality level as

independent measures revealed no significant main effects (ps > .80) or interaction (p = .13) on

mood. Thus negative mood from a format with inconsistent rating polarity cannot account for the

results. NFC: We conducted a linear regression analysis where the independent measures were

rating polarity (dummy coded: consistent = 0, inconsistent = 1), the mean centered NFC score,

and the interaction of the two with visual perception as the dependent measure. We obtain no

significant main or interaction effects for NFC (ps > .57) indicating that the rating polarity effect

is independent of NFC, suggesting that even participants with high need for cognition are

susceptible to the rating polarity effect and may not be aware of its effects.

Experiment 1 provides supporting evidence for the rating polarity effect: people

perceived the visual change in before-and-after photos as less impressive when the quality rating

was reported using smaller-is-better polarity even though they all viewed the exact same set of

photos. Their judgments were less responsive to differences in product quality when using a

rating system with polarity inconsistent with the numerical association they hold in implicit

memory. Confusion, forgetting, differences in need-for-cognition, and mood cannot account for

the results.

Although this experiment and the pretest provide evidence of the rating polarity effect,

there are several important questions that remain. First, does the rating polarity effect manifest

for only single judgments or will it continue to manifest across repeated judgments? If the rating

polarity effect stems from spontaneous interference from implicit memory without people’s

Page 18: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

18

awareness, it should continue to manifest over multiple judgments, even as participants have

more practice using a format with inconsistent rating polarity. Second, does the rating polarity

effect persist for multiple levels of product quality? And third, does the rating polarity effect

occur only with particular types of response alternatives? If it stems from spontaneous

interference from implicit memory, it should be robust to multiple types of response alternatives.

The following two experiments address these questions.

EXPERIMENTS 2A & 2B: REPEATED EVALUATIONS

Experiments 2a and 2b test for the rating polarity effect in a more conservative context

where each participant evaluates 15 products across 5 levels of product quality. If the rating

polarity effect is caused by confusion or inattention, then it should not manifest in a repeated

measures design where each participant has the opportunity to evaluate many products and learn

from experience. However, if it stems from spontaneous proactive interference from implicit

associations, the effect should manifest even with a repeated measures format, further supporting

H1. Furthermore, to further rule out the possibility that the rating polarity effect manifests due to

orientation of the response format, we ran the experiments using two different response formats:

purchase intent as a binary “yes / no” measure (experiment 2a) and willingness-to-pay as a

vertical drop down menu of dollar amounts in 50-cent increments (experiment 2b). Since the two

experiments were identical in procedure and differed only in the response format, we report the

experiments together. Because differences in mood and need-for-cognition did not affect the

results of the first experiment and or any subsequent experiments, these analyses for possible

alternative accounts are detailed for all subsequent experiments in Web Appendix D.

Page 19: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

19

Method for Experiments 2a and 2b

Participants. U.S.-based participants on mTurk (verified by IP address) participated in

these two experiments in return for $1.50: 213 participants in experiment 2a (49% female;

average age: 37.1 years) and 221 participants in experiment 2b (50% female; average age: 36.3

years). They were randomly assigned to the consistent or inconsistent rating polarity condition

within each experiment.

Procedure. Participants were told that the study was being conducted by a large retail

store to understand American consumers’ evaluations of European brands that might be

introduced in the U.S. They were shown brands of five quality levels (1, 2, 3, 4, and 5) in each of

the three different product categories (water, margarine, and toothpaste). Presentation order was

randomized for each participant across the 15 brands (see Web Appendix B for example stimuli).

For each brand, participants saw the brand name, the quality level (ostensibly taken from

Consumer Reports), a photo, and a short tag line communicating the brand positioning.

Participants assigned to the consistent rating polarity condition (bigger-is-better) were

informed that 1 = inadequate, 2 = adequate, 3 = fair, 4 = good, 5 = very good. Those in the

inconsistent rating polarity condition (smaller-is-better) were informed that 1= very good, 2 =

good, 3 = fair, 4 =adequate, 5 = inadequate.

Pre-evaluation Comprehension Test. Before proceeding to the main task, participants

were asked five pre-evaluation comprehension test questions: “If a product had a quality rating

of X, what does it mean?” where X was 5, 4, 3, 2, or 1 respectively. They were asked to select

one meaning for each rating value from the options very good, good, fair, inadequate, and

Page 20: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

20

adequate. If a response to any question was incorrect, a message appeared informing the

participant which responses were incorrect and asked them to correct these answers. Participants

could proceed to the main task only after correctly answering all five questions.

Key Dependent Measure. Purchase Intent (Experiment 2a): Participants were asked,

“Would you purchase this product” [Yes or No]. Willingness-to-Pay (Experiment 2b):

Participants were asked to indicate their willingness to pay for each brand in U.S. dollars using a

drop-down list with price options increasing in 50-cent increments from $0 to $6.00.

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. After evaluating all 15 brands, participants were

again asked to indicate the meaning of the rating poles 1 and 5 using the same response options

as in the pre-evaluation comprehension task.

Rating Polarity Typicality. Participants were asked the same typicality question as in

experiment 1.

The experiment ended with demographic questions—age, gender, marital status,

measures of mood (7-point scales anchored at bad / good, unpleasant / pleasant, negative /

positive) and involvement (5-point scales anchored at not at all / very).

Results and Discussion

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. Participants who responded to at least one of the

rating pole comprehension test questions incorrectly were excluded from the analysis. In

experiment 2a, this included 0.5% of the participants in the consistent and 4.7% of those in the

inconsistent rating polarity condition. In experiment 2b, this included 0.9% of the participants in

the consistent and 0.5% of those in the inconsistent rating polarity condition.

Page 21: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

21

Rating Polarity Typicality Check. A vast majority of participants indicated that the

bigger-is-better numerical association is more typical (2a: 97%; 2b: 96%) than the smaller-is-

better one (2a: 1.5%; 2b: 3%), while a small percentage were not sure (2a: 1.5%; 2b: 1%),

confirming that the bigger-is-better numerical association is more dominant in implicit memory.

Purchase Intent (Experiment 2a). Our objective was to test whether rating polarity

moderated the effect of quality level on purchase intent (yes = 1, no = 0). To do so, we dummy-

coded rating polarity (consistent = 0, inconsistent = 1), and coded quality level as a continuous

variable (inadequate = 1 to very good = 5). Quality levels in the inconsistent rating polarity

condition were reverse coded so that higher numeric scores also indicated higher quality level.

Purchase intent was submitted to a repeated measures logistic regression analysis using

PROC GENMOD in SAS with three independent variables: quality level, rating polarity, and

their interaction. This method treats quality level as a repeated, within-subjects continuous

variable and rating polarity as a between-subjects categorical variable. Because category effects

do not affect the main results across our studies, we included category (toothpaste, margarine,

and water) as an additional within-subjects factor but report results aggregated across the three

product categories across all experiments.

The simple effect of rating polarity was marginally significant (β = .76, p < .06) while the

simple effect of quality level was significant (β = 1.47, p < .01). Most importantly, the two-way

rating polarity x quality level interaction was significant (β = –.34, p < .01). The signs of the

coefficients suggest that a greater proportion of participants are willing to purchase brands with

higher quality levels, but the effect of quality level is weaker for participants evaluating brands

rated with a format using inconsistent versus consistent rating polarity, supporting H1. To

corroborate that these results are not an artifact of the regression assumptions, we plotted the

Page 22: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

22

percentage of participants who were willing to purchase the products for the two rating polarity

conditions for each level of quality in figure 3. The pattern of means is consistent with our

interpretation of the results.

______________________________

Insert figure 3 about here ______________________________

Note that the coefficient for the rating polarity x quality level interaction term represents the

extent of interference that stems from the rating polarity effect. Statistically, it is the difference in

evaluations when using a rating format that employs rating polarity that is consistent (bigger-is-

better) versus inconsistent (smaller-is-better) with the numerical association in implicit memory.

Willingness-to-Pay (Experiment 2b). We used the same coding for independent variables

and analysis procedures as experiment 2a, but submitted the willingness-to-pay measure to a

repeated measures regression analysis using PROC MIXED in SAS.

The simple effect of rating polarity was significant (β = .35, p < .01), as was the simple

effect of quality level (β = .54, p < .01). The two simple effects were qualified by a significant

two-way rating polarity x quality level interaction (β = –.10, p < .01). Supporting H1, the signs

of the coefficients suggest that willingness-to-pay increases with higher quality levels, but the

effect of quality level is weaker for participants evaluating brands rated with a format using

inconsistent versus consistent rating polarity. When participants used a format with rating

polarity consistent with their implicit numerical association, on average, a one-level change in

quality level changed willingness-to-pay by $0.54. However, when the rating polarity was

inconsistent with their implicit numerical association, participants’ willingness-to-pay changed

by $0.44 points for a one-level change in quality.

Experiments 2a and 2b provide further evidence of the robustness of the rating polarity

Page 23: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

23

effect across 15 repeated measures and 5 levels of product quality. Product evaluations,

measured through binary intent to purchase and vertically-oriented willingness-to-pay, are less

responsive to differences in quality level when products are rated using a format with a rating

polarity that is inconsistent (vs. consistent) with the implicit numerical association in memory.

Mood and involvement cannot account for the results (Web Appendix D).

THE ROLE OF IMPLICIT MEMORY IN THE RATING POLARITY EFFECT

The first three experiments provide evidence for the rating polarity effect and its

robustness, even across repeated judgments and a variety of dependent measures with both

horizontal and vertical orientation. But what causes the rating polarity effect? One question that

might come to mind is whether the rating polarity effect is caused by misattribution of the

subjective experience of fluency. Conceivably, judgments based on formats with consistent

rating polarity are more fluent or easier to make than those with inconsistent rating polarity.

Preference fluency is usually associated with more favorable evaluations (Schwarz 2004; see

also Alter and Oppenheimer 2009 and Lee and Labroo 2004). The observed pattern of results

does not support the fluency account because we do not observe only an effect of rating polarity

where using inconsistent rating polarity results in more unfavorable evaluations overall. Instead

we observe an interaction effect such that using inconsistent rating polarity results in less

sensitivity to differences in product quality overall.

Furthermore, task disfluency or judgment difficulty is typically associated with increased

response latency. To test whether the observed effects can be ascribed to difficulty, we

conducted supplemental analyses using reaction time as a covariate (see Web Appendix E). Even

Page 24: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

24

when we include reaction time as a covariate, the effect of rating polarity on judgments remains

significant. Thus the observed effects cannot be ascribed to judgment difficulty.

We propose that the rating polarity effect is caused by spontaneous interference from

implicit memory. To support our proposition that the interference from implicit memory is

spontaneous and occurs without the awareness of the influence of this interference on judgments,

note that we include only those participants that correctly identify the rating polarity used for

their judgments at the end of the judgment task. Yet even participants with high need-for-

cognition are unable to overcome the effect of rating polarity (see Web Appendix D). Similarly,

participants are unable to overcome the rating polarity effect over 15 repeated judgments. We

conducted additional analyses including order as an independent variable for experiments 2a, 2b,

and all of the experiments that follow (see Web Appendix F). In all of these experiments, the

two-way interaction representing the rating polarity effect remains significant while the three-

way interactions between rating polarity, quality level, and order is not significant in any of the

experiments, indicating that the rating polarity effect does not dissipate over fifteen judgments.

If the rating polarity effect stems from the automatic reliance on implicit memory when

making judgments, then it should be moderated by factors that influence the implicit numerical

association itself or the tendency to rely on implicit memory when making judgments such as: 1)

cultural norms that influence numerical associations held in implicit memory (experiment 3), 2)

mindsets that increase reliance on implicit versus explicit memory (experiments 4a and 4b), and

3) measuring direct reliance on implicit memory (experiment 5).

EXPERIMENT 3: MODERATION BY CULTURAL NORMS

Page 25: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

25

In a country where a smaller-is-better numerical association is more likely to be held in

implicit memory, such as Germany, using a rating format with bigger-is-better rating polarity

should result in product evaluations that are less responsive to differences in quality level,

supporting H2. Experiment 3 tests this proposition with German participants.

Method

Participants. One hundred members of an online panel in Germany (51% female;

average age: 43.0 years) participated in this experiment in exchange for approximately €4.

Procedure. The stimuli were nearly identical to experiments 2a & 2b, with the following

differences. The questionnaire was translated into German, and the cover story was slightly

modified for a German audience. Participants were informed that a retail store is considering

introducing several European brands in the U.S. market and that the quality ratings were

provided by a “reputable consumer welfare protection agency widely respected in the U.S. for its

unbiased evaluation of consumer products.” To test the robustness of the previous results, we

used a 7-point, non-numeric semantic differential scale of purchase intentions (anchored at

unlikely to buy and likely to buy, with the center labeled as neutral) as the main dependent

variable in this study and the studies that follow.

Results and Discussion

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. Twelve percent of participants in the inconsistent

and 10% in the consistent condition did not correctly answer both rating-pole comprehension test

Page 26: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

26

questions. As in the previous experiments, these participants were excluded from the analysis, so

the results could not be ascribed to confusion or forgetting (but, all effects persist when we

include these participants).

Rating Polarity Typicality. Forty-nine percent of the participants indicated that the

smaller-is-better numerical association was more typical, 31% indicated that they found the

bigger-is-better numerical association more typical, and 20% indicated they were not sure. These

results suggest that although the smaller-is-better format is more typical in Germany, it is not as

ubiquitous as the bigger-is-better format is in the United States.

Purchase Intentions. We used a regression model identical to experiment 2b, except that

in this case, the smaller-is-better rating polarity was the consistent rating polarity condition and

the bigger-is-better rating polarity was the inconsistent rating polarity condition. Purchase

intention was submitted to a repeated measures regression analysis with quality level, rating

polarity, and their interaction as predictors. The simple effect of rating polarity was significant (β

= .86, p < .01) as was the simple effect of quality level (β = .59, p < .01), but most importantly,

the two-way rating polarity x quality level interaction representing interference from the rating

polarity effect was significant (β = -.28, p < .01).

______________________________

Insert figure 4 about here ______________________________

Mirroring the results of experiments 2a and 2b, purchase intention increases with higher

quality levels, and consumer evaluations are less responsive to changes in quality level when the

brands were rated using a format with rating polarity that is inconsistent versus consistent with

the numerical association in implicit memory. However, in this case, it is the bigger-is-better

rating polarity that is inconsistent with the implicit numerical association in memory. When

Page 27: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

27

participants evaluate brands using rating polarity consistent with their implicit numerical

association (smaller-is-better), on average, a one-level difference in quality level changes

purchase intention by 0.59 points; however, it only changes purchase intention by 0.31 points

when participants use rating polarity inconsistent with their implicit numerical association

(bigger-is-better), supporting H2. The means of purchase intention are plotted in figure 4.

Moderation by Implicit Numerical Association. Among German participants,

approximately half of the participants held the smaller-is-better numerical association as more

typical (49%). However, the remaining participants (51%) did not consider smaller-is-better

more typical. If the rating polarity effect stems from spontaneous proactive interference from the

implicit numerical association in memory, then it should be moderated by the strength or

flexibility of this implicit numerical association. More specifically, those participants who have

the implicit numerical association that smaller numbers indicate higher quality should continue

to exhibit the rating polarity effect. But those participants who hold a more flexible implicit

numerical association (e.g. are not sure about the association or find a bigger-is-better

association more typical in a country that typically uses smaller-is-better rating polarity) are less

likely to experience as strong of an interference effect from a competing smaller-is-better

implicit numerical association when using a rating system with opposite rating polarity. Thus,

they are less likely to exhibit the rating polarity effect.

To test this prediction, purchase intentions was submitted to a repeated measures

regression analysis used PROC MIXED in SAS with implicit numerical association (bigger-is-

better or not sure = 0, smaller is better = 1) as an independent variable in addition to rating

polarity and quality level and their two and three-way interaction terms as predicting variables

(see table 1). The three-way interaction between rating polarity, quality level, and implicit

Page 28: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

28

numerical association was significant (β = .56, p < .01). Follow-up contrasts reveal that for those

participants with a smaller-is-better implicit numerical association, the rating polarity x quality

level interaction is significant (β = -.54, p < .01), but for those participants with either a bigger-

is-better implicit numerical association or unsure of their numerical association, the rating

polarity effect does not manifest (β = .01, p = .87, see figure 5 for pattern of means).

Experiment 3 provides initial support implicating the role of implicit memory in the

rating polarity effect. Culture moderates the rating polarity effect: In Germany, where the

smaller-is-better numerical association is more common, the rating format with bigger-is-better

polarity results in more muted responses to differences in quality level, supporting H2.

Furthermore, the additional regression analysis with implicit numerical association as a

moderator revealed that the rating polarity effect is eliminated for those participants with a more

flexible implicit numerical association in memory. This suggests that people who have a less

strongly held implicit numerical association in memory do not experience the same spontaneous

interference from a numerical association. Mood and involvement cannot account for these

effects (Web Appendix D).

______________________________

Insert table 1 and figure 5 about here ______________________________

While experiment 3 provides evidence that having different implicit numerical

associations can reverse the effect of using ratings of different polarity and attenuate the rating

polarity effect, it does not specifically implicate the reliance on implicit versus explicit memory.

The remaining experiments directly examine the role of reliance on implicit memory.

EXPERIMENT 4A: MODERATION BY PRIMED CONSTRUAL MINDSET

Page 29: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

29

As discussed earlier, previous research has shown that people in an abstract (versus

concrete) mindset rely more on implicit associations in gist memory. Experiment 4a tests H3 that

the rating polarity effect is more likely to manifest for people in an abstract versus concrete

mindset. Participants are induced with a concrete versus abstract mindset using the Frietas,

Gollwitzer, and Trope (2004) construal mindset manipulation before completing the same

product evaluation task as in experiment 3.

Method

Participants. One hundred and seventy-two U.S.-based mTurk participants (33% female;

average age: 31.2 years) participated in this experiment in return for $1.50. They were randomly

assigned to one of four conditions: rating polarity (consistent v. inconsistent) x construal mindset

(abstract v. concrete).

Procedure. The basic procedure was similar to the previous experiment, with the addition

of a construal mindset manipulation after the pre-evaluation comprehension task and before the

key dependent measures (purchase intention, post-evaluation comprehension task, rating polarity

typicality, mood, involvement, demographics). Participants completed a mindset-priming

manipulation similar to that developed by Freitas, Gollwitzer, and Trope (2004) and were told it

was a thought-listing task that would help design future experiments. Those in the abstract

mindset condition were asked to answer a series of increasingly high level questions about

“Why” they would “Improve and maintain one’s general knowledge,” while those in the concrete

mindset condition were asked to answer a series of increasingly low level questions about “How”

Page 30: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

30

they would engage in this same behavior.

Results

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. Five percent of participants in the inconsistent and

1% in the consistent rating polarity condition responded to at least one of the two rating-pole

comprehension test questions incorrectly. These participants were excluded from the analysis.

Rating Polarity Typicality. The majority of the participants indicated that the bigger-is-

better numerical association is more typical (94%) than the smaller-is-better one (3%), while 3%

were not sure, confirming that the bigger-is-better association is dominant in implicit memory.

Purchase Intentions. We analyzed the data using a regression model, dummy coding

rating polarity (consistent = 0, inconsistent = 1) and construal mindset (concrete = 0, abstract =

1) and coding quality level as a continuous variable. Purchase intentions were submitted to a

repeated measures regression analysis using PROC MIXED in SAS with the three independent

variables (rating polarity, mindset, and quality level) and their two- and three-way interaction

terms as predicting variables (see table 2). The simple effect of quality level was significant (β =

.91, p < .01), as was the two-way rating polarity x mindset interaction (β = .76, p = .03); the two-

way rating polarity x quality level interaction was marginally significant (β = -.10, p = .09). Most

importantly, the three-way rating polarity, mindset, and quality level interaction was significant

(β = -.20, p = .01). Other effects were not significant (ps > .34, see table 2).

______________________________

Insert table 2 about here ______________________________

To examine the significant three-way interaction, we probed the two-way quality level x

Page 31: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

31

rating polarity interaction for abstract versus concrete mindsets. We find that the coefficient for

the quality level x rating polarity interaction is statistically significant for those in an abstract

mindset who rely more on implicit memory (β = -.31, p < .01). However, the magnitude of the

coefficient was smaller and marginally significant for participants in a concrete mindset, who

were less likely to rely on implicit memory (β = -.10, p = .09, see table 2). These results suggest

that the rating polarity effect is indeed less likely to manifest for those participants in a more

concrete (vs. more abstract) mindset who rely less on implicit memory, supporting H3.

EXPERIMENT 4B: MODERATION BY CHRONIC CONSTRUAL MINDSET

It is possible that the mindset manipulation, which was administered before the main

experiment, might have introduced an inadvertent confound in the previous study. To rule out

any such possibility and provide further evidence that reliance on implicit versus explicit

memory moderates the rating polarity effect, in this experiment construal mindset was measured

using an established scale (Vallacher and Wegner 1989), rather than manipulated.

Method

Participants. Two hundred and nine undergraduates at a college in the northeast U.S.

participated in an online study in exchange for $5 (43% female; average age: 20.1 years).

Procedure. The procedure for this experiment was identical to the previous experiment,

except that the mindset was measured after the product evaluation task using the Vallacher and

Wegner (1989) Behavioral Identification Form (BIF) and before the post-evaluation

Page 32: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

32

comprehension task and measures of mood, attention, and demographics. Participants were

presented with a series of 24 different behaviors (making a list), each with a more abstract

response (getting organized) and a more concrete response (writing things down), and asked to

indicate which of the two descriptions of the behavior they preferred.

Results

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. Three percent of participants in the inconsistent

and 1% in the consistent rating polarity condition responded to at least one rating-pole

comprehension test question incorrectly. These participants were excluded from the analysis.

Rating Polarity Typicality. The vast majority of participants indicated that the bigger-is-

better numerical association is more typical (96%) than the smaller-is-better association (1%),

and 3% of participants indicated they were not sure. To confirm that chronic mindset did not

influence which rating polarity was seen as more typical, we conducted a logistic regression to

confirm that there was no significant effect of chronic mindset, rating polarity, or the interaction

between the two on perceived typicality (ps > .30).

Effect of Rating Polarity on Mindset. Participants’ responses to the BIF study were coded

as concrete = 0 and abstract = 1, and summed across the 24 responses, resulting in a continuous

BIF score ranging from 0 to 24 (M = 12.12, STDV = 4.57). This variable was mean-centered for

the key analyses described below. A one-way ANOVA with rating polarity as the independent

measure and construal mindset as the dependent measure confirmed that rating polarity did not

have a significant effect on mindset (p > .23).

Purchase Intention. We again analyzed the data using a regression model, dummy coding

Page 33: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

33

rating polarity (consistent as 0, inconsistent 1), entering chronic mindset as a continuous variable

(mean-centered BIF score), and coding quality level as a continuous variable. Purchase

intentions were submitted to a repeated measures regression analysis with the three independent

variables (rating polarity, quality level, chronic mindset) and their two- and three-way interaction

terms as predicting variables using PROC MIXED in SAS (see table 2).

The simple effects of quality level (β = .84, p < .01), rating polarity (β = 1.01, p < .01),

and mindset (β = -.06, p = .05) were significant. The rating polarity x quality level (β = -.28, p <

.01) and rating polarity x mindset (β = .14, p < .01) two-way interactions were significant. The

two-way quality level x mindset interaction was not significant (p > .15). Most importantly, the

three-way interaction was significant (β = -.024, p = .01). Follow-up spotlight tests (Aiken and

West 1991) at one standard deviation above and below the mean of chronic mindset revealed that

for those with a more chronically abstract mindset (more likely to rely on implicit memory), the

two-way interaction between rating polarity and quality level was significant (β = -.39, p < .001,

see table 2). For those with a more chronically concrete mindset (less likely to rely on implicit

memory), the two-way rating polarity x quality level interaction was also significant (β = -.17, p

< .01), but the magnitude of the coefficient was lower, indicating an attenuation of the rating

polarity effect, consistent with H3. Follow-up floodlight analysis are included in Web Appendix

G, which provide further evidence that the coefficient representing the rating polarity effect and

its significance decrease for participants with an increasingly chronic concrete mindset.

EXPERIMENT 5: MODERATION BY INDIVIDUAL RELIANCE ON

IMPLICIT MEMORY

Page 34: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

34

The previous two experiments indirectly manipulate or measure reliance on implicit

versus explicit memory. In experiment 5, we directly examine the role of reliance on implicit

versus explicit memory in driving the rating polarity effect. Specifically, we test that the rating

polarity effect will be stronger for people who rely to a greater extent on implicit memory (H4).

To test this, we use the well-established dual-task paradigm for measuring individual reliance on

different types of memory (de Neys 2006, Jacoby 1991). In this paradigm, participants are

concurrently given two tasks where one task requires implicit memory and another task requires

explicit memory—performance on each of the two simultaneous tasks determines individuals’

reliance on implicit and explicit memory (de Neys 2006, Jacoby 1991). We created a

computerized dual-task modeled after the prevailing dual-tasks that can be administered online in

which the computer coded the responses (see Web Appendix B). In this dual-task, participants

had to simultaneously complete a true-false task (which measured reliance on implicit memory),

and a recall task (which measured reliance on explicit memory).

More specifically, in the dual-task, participants were shown a series of simple arithmetic

facts and had to submit a true or false judgment (e.g., “7-5 = 1”). The judgment had to be made

in 3 seconds, and while concurrently engaging in a recall task. For the recall task, participants

had to memorize the series of digits on the right hand side of the equations (e.g., 1 in the

equation above) and report them at the end of a series of true or false questions. Thus, when

making the true-false judgments in this dual-task paradigm, participants had neither time nor

working memory resources, and had to rely on pattern recognition stored in their implicit

memory. Prior research has shown that when you look at “7-5 = 1”, you can intuitively sense,

using implicit memory, whether this statement is true or false, without actually subtracting

(Schunn et al. 1997). Success in the true-false task, measured as the number of correct responses

Page 35: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

35

to the true-false questions in this dual-task paradigm, is a direct measure of participants’ reliance

on implicit memory. Success in the recall task, measured as the number of correct responses to

the recall questions in this dual-task paradigm, is a measure of participants’ reliance on explicit

memory. We predicted that the rating polarity effect would be stronger for those participants

who receive a higher score in the true-false task (indicating that they rely more on implicit

memory). Because we posit that the rating polarity effect stems from spontaneous interference

from implicit memory, we predict that subjects’ performance on the implicit memory task, but

not the explicit memory task, will moderate the rating polarity effect.

Method

Participants. Two hundred and two U.S.-based mTurk participants (38% female; average

age: 33.4 years) participated in this experiment in return for $1.50.

Procedure. This experiment was identical to experiment 4b, except that the task

measuring implicit and explicit memory replaced the BIF measurement. After the main

experiment, participants responded to an ostensibly unrelated dual-task test where they were told

that they would complete two tasks simultaneously—a “True-False Task” and a “Recall Task.”

They were shown a series of simple arithmetic facts. For the True-False task, they had to make a

determination of whether the arithmetic fact was true or false within three seconds. For example,

in a block with four facts, participants saw the following four arithmetic facts, one at a time, and

they had to indicate whether each of them is true or false: “7-5 = 1”; “2+3 = 5”; “2+5=6”;

“4+5=8” (see Web Appendix B for stimuli examples). In this block the correct responses to the

four equations are false, true, false, and false respectively. While making these judgments, they

Page 36: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

36

had to simultaneously complete a memorization task. For the memorization task, they needed to

memorize the number on the right hand side of equation in the order they saw the number and

recall these numbers in order. For example, while completing the true-false task for the first

block, they had to memorize the right-side digits of the equations listed above and recall the

string of digits 1568 at the end of the block. Participants completed eight such blocks of

equations with increasing span of the recall task; there were four, five, six, six, seven, seven,

eight, and eight equations in each succeeding block (see Web Appendix B). Thus, over eight

blocks, participants made a total of 51 true-false judgments and simultaneously responded to 51

explicit recall questions. Before completing the main dual task of these eight blocks, participants

received three practice blocks with feedback, to understand the task.

Results

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. Two percent of participants in the inconsistent and

2% in the consistent rating polarity condition responded to at least one rating-pole

comprehension test question incorrectly. As in the previous studies, these participants were

excluded from the analysis. Additionally, one participant who got all 51 true-false judgments

incorrect was excluded from the analyses.

Rating Polarity Typicality. The vast majority of the participants indicated that the bigger-

is-better numerical association is more typical (84%) than the smaller-is-better one (16%),

confirming that the bigger-is-better association is more dominant in implicit memory.

Effect of Rating Polarity on Implicit Memory Task Performance. Participants’ propensity

to rely on implicit memory was measured by the number of correct responses to the true-false

Page 37: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

37

task. Larger numbers indicate a greater propensity to rely on implicit memory. These scores

ranged from 18 to 51 with a mean of 42.65 and standard deviation of 6.80. A one-way ANOVA

with rating polarity as the independent variable and performance on the true-false task as the

dependent variable confirmed that rating polarity did not have a significant effect on

performance on the true-false task (F < 1).

Purchase Intentions. We analyzed purchase intentions using a regression model, dummy

coding rating polarity (consistent = 0, inconsistent = 1), entering performance on the implicit

memory task as a continuous variable (mean-centered), and coding quality level as a continuous

variable (inadequate = 1, adequate = 2, fair = 3, good = 4, very good = 5). Purchase intentions

were submitted to a repeated measures regression analysis with the three independent variables

(rating polarity, quality level, implicit memory task performance) and their two- and three-way

interaction terms as predicting variables, using PROC MIXED in SAS (see table 3).

The simple effects of rating polarity (β = .60, p < .01), quality level (β = 1.06, p < .01), and

propensity to rely on implicit memory (β = -.05, p < .01) were significant. The two-way

interactions of rating polarity x quality level (β = -.14, p < .01) and quality level x implicit

memory reliance (β = .010, p < . 01) were significant. The two-way interaction between rating

polarity x implicit memory reliance was not significant. Most importantly, the three-way

interaction was significant (β = -. 01, p = .05).

______________________________

Insert table 3 about here ______________________________

Follow-up spotlight tests (Aiken and West 1991) at one standard deviation above and below

average performance scores on the implicit memory task revealed that for those who performed

above average on the implicit memory task, the two-way rating polarity x quality level

Page 38: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

38

interaction representing the rating polarity effect was significant (β = -.22, p < .001, see table 3).

However, for those participants who performed below average on the implicit memory task, the

two-way rating polarity x quality level interaction was not significant (β = -. 07, p = .18),

indicating an attenuation of the rating polarity effect. These results support H4. We performed a

similar regression analysis using performance on the explicit memory performance task rather

than the implicit memory task as the dependent measure. The three-way interaction between

rating polarity, quality level, and explicit memory reliance was not statistically significant (β =

.002, p = .59), indicating that although a direct measure of reliance on implicit memory

moderates the rating polarity effect, the direct measure of reliance on explicit memory does not.

DISCUSSION OF EXPERIMENTS 4A, 4B, AND 5.

Together, experiments 4a, 4b, and 5 provide evidence that the rating polarity effect is

more likely to occur when people rely more on their implicit memory. Experiments 4a and 4b

show that participants under an abstract (versus concrete) construal mindset, which increases

reliance on implicit memory, are more likely to exhibit the rating polarity effect. The size of the

coefficient for the rating polarity x quality level interaction, which represents the extent of

interference from the implicit numerical association when using a rating format with inconsistent

rating polarity, is smaller for those participants who are either induced with a concrete mindset

(experiment 4a) or chronically adopt a more concrete mindset (experiment 4b).

In experiment 5, we directly measured the use of implicit versus explicit memory using

the dual-task paradigm and found that the rating polarity effect was moderated by propensity to

use implicit memory: the rating polarity effect was stronger for those participants who relied on

Page 39: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

39

implicit memory to a greater extent during the dual-task. Furthermore, reliance on explicit

memory does not moderate the rating polarity effect, providing further evidence that it is an

effect driven by spontaneous interference from the numerical association in implicit memory.

The strongest moderating effect comes from directly measuring individuals’ use of implicit

memory (experiment 5). Mood and involvement cannot account for these effects (Web Appendix

D). These experiments support H3 and H4, demonstrating that people are more likely to

experience spontaneous proactive interference from an implicit numerical association in memory

when in a mindset that encourages reliance on implicit memory or if their individual chronic

propensity is to rely more on implicit memory.

GENERAL DISCUSSION

Across seven experiments, we provide evidence of a new form of proactive interference

between numerical associations: the rating polarity effect. The first three experiments

demonstrated the effect, supporting H1: evaluating a product using a rating format with a

numerical association opposite the one that people hold in implicit memory results in evaluations

that are less responsive to differences in quality level. This effect is robust to a consumer

evaluations ranging across bidding behavior (paradigm pretest), visual perception (experiment

1), purchase intent (experiments 2a, 3, 4a, 4b, and 5), and willingness-to-pay (experiment 2b). It

manifests across a variety of types of response alternatives (open ended, horizontal and vertical

orientation, with and without numerical anchors, binary and scale). Confusion, forgetting, mood,

and involvement are ruled out as potential alternative explanations. The effect occurs for single

and repeated evaluations and is not moderated by need-for-cognition or order, demonstrating the

Page 40: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

40

spontaneous and unintended effect of the implicit numerical association in memory.

To understand the mechanism behind the rating polarity effect and why this proactive

interference effect persists even when participants are well aware of the rating polarity

employed, we examine the role of implicit memory. If the rating polarity effect is caused by the

spontaneous use of the implicit numerical association in memory, then the rating polarity effect

should be moderated by factors that moderate reliance on implicit memory. Supporting H2,

experiment 3 demonstrates that for participants from a country with an opposite numerical

association as a cultural norm, the effect of bigger-is-better versus smaller-is-better rating

polarity reverses. In addition, for those participants with a more flexible set of implicit numerical

associations in memory, the rating polarity effect does not manifest as they do not experience

proactive interference from a strong implicit numerical association. Further illustrating the role

of implicit numerical associations behind the rating polarity effect, this proactive interference of

numerical associations diminishes when people rely less on implicit memory whether it is based

on a mindset that leads people to rely less on implicit memory (experiment 4a, 4b, supporting

H3) or on individual differences in relying on implicit memory as directly measured through a

dual-task paradigm (experiment 5, supporting H4).

Theoretical and Managerial Contributions

The current research presents new insights into numerical cognition and makes three

theoretical contributions. First, we characterize a new type of implicit numerical association that

differs depending on cultural context—the association between the magnitude of a number and

an evaluative judgment. Culture can influence whether consumers have a bigger-is-better versus

Page 41: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

41

smaller-is-better numerical association in implicit memory, and this implicit association can

spontaneously influence their judgments when using a rating polarity with an opposite numerical

association. Second, our research demonstrates the persistence of interference effects that stem

from an implicit association in memory that has been culturally cultivated over time. The rating

polarity effect manifests over different types of evaluations, over multiple consumer evaluations,

and even when taking into account measures of difficulty and need-for-cognition (NFC).

Third, our research also contributes to the memory literature by identifying novel

moderators of proactive interference, based on unintentional reliance on implicit memory. We

show that the strength of the implicit numerical association in memory moderates the effect of

proactive interference. Furthermore, (both manipulated and measured) construal mindset can

moderate proactive interference, and add to the burgeoning literature on how cognitive mindsets

change memory processes. In particular, in contrast to work that demonstrates that abstract

construal mindsets can attenuate interference when explicit memory interferes with implicit

memory (Kyung and Thomas, 2016), this research demonstrates that concrete construal mindsets

can attenuate interference when implicit memory interferes with explicit memory. Thus, whether

a concrete versus abstract construal mindset can attenuate memory interferences depends on

which form of memory is causing the interference effect. In addition, we show that individual

propensity to rely on implicit memory (as measured by performance on the implicit memory

portion of the dual-task exercise) can moderate proactive interference. If one thinks of the

implicit numerical association (bigger-is-better) as a culturally-induced metaphor (see Casasanto

2014), then our research introduces the notion of interference that stems from metaphorical

incongruity. Future research could examine other metaphors for interference.

From a managerial perspective, our research suggests that rating polarity is an important

Page 42: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

42

element of market research design in cross-cultural contexts and for the presentation of results.

Presenting information using rating polarity inconsistent with the numerical association in

implicit memory can make consumers less responsive to differences in numeric quality ratings of

products. As such, retailers should translate numeric ratings into a format that is consistent with

the numerical association that their target customers have in memory. Similarly, any decision

maker that encounters numeric information utilizing rating polarity opposite the numerical

association they hold in implicit memory should be aware of the potential impact on their

judgments: admissions officers or human resource managers who use grade point average

information (GPA) across cultures; policymakers making global decisions based upon cross-

cultural research, such as life satisfaction surveys where some countries use smaller-is-better

rating polarity and some do not (Diener, Kahneman, and Helliwell 2010); reviewers evaluating

the $30 billion in research grants for the National Institutes of Health (NIH) where proposals are

evaluated using a format where 1 = exceptional and 9 = poor.

Finally our work underscores the importance of understanding cultural implicit

associations that might bias people’s judgments and how difficult it can be to overcome these

implicit associations, such as the implicit association between “bigger” and “better” in the United

States. Thus, we encourage managers and researchers to understand what these implicit

associations might be and how they can influence judgments1.

                                                                                                                         1 And with that, we conclude this paper as we have found that the association between “bigger” and better” does not apply to the length of academic papers.

Page 43: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

43

Data Collection Paragraph

All study designs and analyses were discussed and agreed upon by all three authors. The first

author supervised the data collection for experiments 1a and 3b through Dartmouth College

students, experiment 1b through Cornell University students, study 2 through a Qualtrics panel

of German participants, and studies 2a, 2b, 3a, and 4 through the mTurk online panel. The first

and second authors separately and jointly analyzed the data from these studies, confirming all

results.

Page 44: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

44

REFERENCES

Adaval, Rashmi, and Kent B. Monroe (2002), “Automatic Construction and Use of Contextual

Information for Product and Price Evaluations,” Journal of Consumer Research, 28(4), 572-

88.

Aiken, Leona S. and Stephen G. West (1991), Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting

Interactions, Sage Publications.

Ajzen, Icek, and Martin Fishbein (2005), “The Influence of Attitudes on Behavior,” The

Handbook of Attitudes, eds. Dolores Albarracin, Blair T. Johnson, and Mark P. Zanna,

Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 173-221.

Alter, Adam L., and Daniel M. Oppenheimer (2009), “Uniting the Tribes of Fluency to Form a

Metacognitive Nation,” Personality and Social Psychology Review, 13(3), 219-35.

Bagchi, Rajesh and Derick F. Davis (2012), “$29 for 70 items or 70 items for $29? How

Presentation Order Affects Package Perceptions,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39(1), 62-

73.

Blankenship, Albert B., and Paul L. Whitely, (1941), “Proactive Inhibition in the Recall of

Advertising Material,” The Journal of Social Psychology, 13(2), 311-22.

Burke, Raymond R., and Thomas K. Srull (1988), “Competitive Interference and Consumer

Memory for Advertising,” Journal of Consumer Research, 15(6), 55-68.

Casasanto, Daniel (2014), “Experiential origins of mental metaphors: Language, Culture, and the

body”, in The Power of Metaphor: Examining Its Influence on Social Life, M.]. Landau, M.

D. Robinson, and B. P. Meier (Editors).

Conner, Mark T., Marco Perugini, Rick O’Gorman, Karen Ayres, and Andrew Prestwich (2007),

“Relations between Implicit and Explicit Measures of Attitudes and Measures of Behavior:

Evidence of Moderation by Individual Difference Variables,” Personality & Social

Page 45: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

45

Psychology Bulletin, 33(12): 1727–40.

Dehaene, Stanislas, Serge Bossini, and Pascal Giraux (1993), “The Mental Representation of

Parity and Number Magnitude,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 12 (3), 371-

96.

De Neys, Wim (2006), “Dual Processing in Reasoning Two Systems but One Reasoner,”

Psychological Science, 17(5), 428-33.

Diener, Ed, Daniel Kahneman, and John Helliwell (2010), International Differences in Well-

being, Oxford University Press.

Florack, Arnd, Martin Scarabis, and Herbert Bless (2001), “When Do Associations Matter? The

Use of Automatic Associations toward Ethnic Groups in Person Judgments,” Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 37(6), 518–24.

Freitas, Antonio L., Peter Gollwitzer, and Yaacov Trope (2004), “The Influence of Abstract and

Concrete Mindsets on Anticipating and Guiding Others' Self-regulatory Efforts,” Journal of

Experimental Social Psychology, 40(6), 739-52.

Fukukura, Jun, Melissa J. Ferguson, and Kentaro Fujita (2013), “Psychological Distance Can

Improve Decision Making Under Information Overload via Gist Memory,” Journal of

Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 658-65.

Gilbert, Daniel T., and J. Gregory Hixon (1991), “The Trouble of Thinking: Activation and

Application of Stereotypic Beliefs,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(4),

509-17.

Graf, Peter, and Daniel L. Schacter (1987), “Selective Effects of Interference on Implicit and

Explicit Memory for New Associations,” Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,

Memory, and Cognition, 13(1), 45-53.

Page 46: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

46

Jacoby, Larry L. (1991), “A Process Dissociation Framework: Separating Automatic From

Intentional Uses of Memory,” Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 513-41.

Jahoda, Gustav (1969), The Psychology of Superstition, Oxford, England: Penguin Books.

Jonides, John, and Derek E. Nee (2006), “Brain Mechanisms of Proactive Interference in

Working Memory,” Neuroscience, 139 (1), 181-93.

Keller, Kevin Lane, Susan E. Heckler, and Michael J. Houston (1998), “The Effects of Brand

Name Suggestiveness on Advertising Recall,” Journal of Marketing 62(1), 48-57.

Keller, Kevin Lane (1987), “Memory Factors in Advertising: The Effect of Advertising Retrieval

Cues on Brand Evaluations,” Journal of Consumer Research, 14(3), 316-33.

Kevin Lane Keller (1991), “Memory and Evaluations in Competitive Advertising

Environments,” Journal of Consumer Research, 17 (3), 463-76.

Keppel, Geoffrey, and Benton J. Underwood (1962), “Proactive Inhibition in Short-term

Retention of Single Items,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1(3), 153-61.

King, Dan, and Chris Janiszewski (2011), “The Sources and Consequences of the Fluent

Processing of Numbers,” Journal of Marketing Research, 48(2), 327-41.

Kyung and Thomas (2016), “When Remembering Disrupting Knowing: Blocking Implicit Price

Memory,” Journal of Marketing Research, 53(6), 937-53.

Labroo, Aparna A. and Vanessa M. Patrick (2009), “Psychological Distancing: Why Happiness

Helps You See the Big Picture,” Journal of Consumer Research, 35(5), 800-9.

Lee, Angela Y., and Aparna A. Labroo (2004), “The Effect of Conceptual and Perceptual

Fluency on Brand Evaluation,” Journal of Marketing Research, 41(2), 151-65.

McCrea, Sean M., Frank Wieber, and Andrea L. Myers (2012), “Construal Level Mind-sets

Moderate Self-and Social Stereotyping,” Journal of Personality and Social

Page 47: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

47

Psychology, 102(1), 51-68.

Mishra, Himanshu, Arul Mishra, and Dhananjay Nayakankuppam (2006), “Money: A Bias for

the Whole,” Journal of Consumer Research, 32(4), 541-49.

Monga, Ashwani and Rajesh Bagchi (2012), “Years, Months, and Days vs. 1, 12, and 365: The

Influence of Units Versus Numbers,” Journal of Consumer Research, 39(1), 185-98.

Monroe, Kent B., and Angela Y. Lee (1999), “Remembering versus Knowing: Issues in Buyers’

Processing of Price Information,” Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 27(2), 207-

25.

Mussweiler, Thomas, and Birte Englich (2005), “Subliminal Anchoring: Judgmental

Consequences and Underlying Mechanisms,” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 98(2), 133-43.

John T. Cacioppo, Petty, Richard E., and Chuan Feng Kao (1984), “The Efficient Assessment of

Need for Cognition,” Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 306-7.

Raghubir, Priya, and Joydeep Srivastava (2009), “The Denomination Effect,” Journal of

Consumer Research, 36(4), 701-13.

Rim, SoYon, James S. Uleman, and Yaacov Trope (2009), “Spontaneous Trait Inference and

Construal Level Theory: Psychological Distance Increases Nonconscious Trait Thinking,”

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1088-97.

Roediger, Henry L. (1990), “Implicit Memory: Retention without Remembering,” American

Psychologist, 45 (9), 1043-56.

Schacter, Daniel L. (1987), “Implicit Memory: History and Current Status,” Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(3), 501-18.

Schunn, Christian D., Lynne M. Reder, Adisack Nhouyvanisvong, Daniel Richards, and Philip J.

Page 48: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

48

Stroffolino (1997), “To Calculate or Not to Calculate: A Source Activation Confusion Model

of Problem Familiarity's Role in Strategy Selection,” Journal of Experimental Psychology:

Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23 (1), 3-29.

Schwarz, Norbert (2004), “Meta-cognitive Experiences in Consumer Judgment and Decision

Making,” Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14(4), 332-48.

Smith, Pamela K., and Yaacov Trope (2006), “You Focus on the Forest When You're in Charge

of the Trees: Power Priming and Abstract Information Processing,” Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 90(4), 578-96.

Spiller, Stephen A., Gavan J. Fitzsimons, John G. Lynch, and Gary H. McClelland (2013),

“Spotlights, Floodlights, and the Magic Number Zero: Simple Effects Tests in Moderated

Regression,” Journal of Marketing Research, 50(2), 277-88.

Thomas, Manoj and Vicki G. Morwitz (2009), “Heuristics in Numerical Cognition: Implications

for Pricing,” in Handbook of Research in Pricing.

Trope, Yaacov, and Nira Liberman (2003), “Temporal Construal,” Psychological Review,

110(3), 403-21.

Tversky, Amos and Daniel Kahneman (1974), “Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and

Biases,” Science, 185 (4157), 1124–31.

Unnava, H. Rao, and Deepak Sirdeshmukh (1994), “Reducing Competitive Ad

Interference,” Journal of Marketing Research, 31(3), 403-11.

Vallacher, Robin R., and Daniel M. Wegner (1989), “Levels of Personal Agency: Individual

Variation in Action Identification,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4),

660-71.

Wickens, Delos D., David G. Born, and Charles K. Allen (1963), “Proactive Inhibition and Item

Page 49: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

49

Similarity in Short-term Memory,” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 2(5),

440-45.

Yates, J. K. (2007), Global Engineering and Construction, Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

Page 50: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

50

Table 1

Experiment 3 Regression Results: Moderation by Implicit Numerical Association

β SE DF t p

Intercept 1.15 0.27 85 4.19 <0.01 Rating Polarity 1.60 0.38 85 4.26 <0.01 Implicit Numerical Association 0.93 0.39 85 2.40 0.02 Rating Polarity x Implicit Numerical Association

-1.56 0.55 85 -2.85 0.01

Quality Level 0.74 0.06 1242 13.23 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.54 0.08 1242 -7.11 <0.01 Implicit Numerical Association x Quality Level

-0.29 0.08 1242 -3.70 <0.01

3-way Interaction 0.56 0.11 1242 5.00 <0.01

PLANNED CONTRASTS

β SE DF t p

Planned Contrast: Implicit Numerical Association is Smaller-is-Better Rating Polarity X Quality Level -0.54 0.08 1242 -7.11 <0.01 Planned Contrast: Implicit Numerical Association is Bigger-is-Better or Not Sure Rating Polarity X Quality Level 0.01 0.08 1242 0.16 0.87

Page 51: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

51

Table 2

Experiments 4A & 4B Regression Results: Moderation by Construal Mindset

Experiment 4a Experiment 4b

β SE t p β SE t p Intercept 0.76 0.16 4.67 <.01 0.95 0.12 7.93 <0.01 Simple Effects Rating Polarity 0.12 0.25 0.50 0.62 1.01 0.17 5.97 <0.01 Quality Level 0.91 0.04 23.11 <.01 0.84 0.03 27.93 <0.01 Mindset* -0.10 0.24 -0.44 0.66 -0.06 0.03 -1.97 0.05 2-way Interaction Effects Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.10 0.06 -1.71 0.09 -0.28 0.04 -6.49 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Mindset 0.76 0.34 2.22 0.03 0.14 0.04 3.68 <0.01 Quality x Mindset 0.06 0.06 0.96 0.34 0.01 0.01 1.41 0.16 3-way Interaction Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Mindset -0.20 0.08 -2.45 0.01 -0.02 0.01 -2.56 0.01

PLANNED CONTRASTS  

Experiment 4a Experiment 4b β SE t p β SE t p Greater Reliance on Implicit Memory** Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.31 0.06 -5.27 <.01 -0.39 0.06 -6.42 <.01

Lower Reliance on Implicit Memory*** Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.10 0.06 -1.71 0.09 -0.17 0.06 -2.73 0.01

* More abstract mindset (positive coefficient) indicates greater reliance on implicit memory ** More abstract mindset. *** More concrete mindset.

Page 52: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

52

Table 3

Experiment 5 Regression Results: Moderation by Reliance on Implicit Memory

  β SE t p Intercept 0.139 0.115 1.21 0.23 Simple Effects Rating Polarity 0.596 0.161 3.7 <.01 Quality Level 1.057 0.026 40.05 <.01 Implicit Memory Reliance -0.047 0.017 -2.85 <.01 2-way Interaction Effects Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.144 0.037 -3.89 <.01

Rating Polarity x Implicit Memory Reliance 0.024 0.024 1.01 0.31

Quality x Implicit Memory Reliance 0.01 0.004 2.73 0.01 3-way Interaction Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Implicit Memory Reliance -0.011 0.005 -2.00 0.05

PLANNED CONTRASTS

  β SE t p Greater Propensity to Use Implicit Memory* Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.218 0.052 -4.17 <.01

Lower Propensity to Use Implicit Memory** Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.07 0.052 -1.33 0.18

** One STDV above the mean for each memory type. *** One STDV below the mean for each memory type.

Page 53: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

53

FIGURE 1

EXPERIMENT 1 STIMULI: BEFORE AND AFTER PHOTOGRAPHS

Page 54: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

54

FIGURE 2

EXPERIMENT 1: EFFECT OF QUALITY LEVEL ON VISUAL PERCEPTION BY RATING POLARITY (U.S.)

Note: All errors bars represent standard errors

68.8 73.1

78.3

72.8

60

65

70

75

80

85

90

Consistent Rating Polarity

(Bigger-is-better)

Inconsistent Rating Polarity

(Smaller-is-better)

Vis

ual I

mpr

ovem

ent

Low Quality High Quality

Page 55: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

55

FIGURE 3

EXPERIMENT 2A: EFFECT OF QUALITY LEVEL ON PURCHASE BY RATING POLARITY (U.S.)

0%

25%

50%

75%

100%

Poor Adequate Fair Good Very Good

Prop

ortio

n th

at w

ould

pur

chas

e

Quality Level

Consistent rating polarity (Bigger-is-better) Inconsistent rating polarity (Smaller-is-better)

Page 56: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

56

FIGURE 4

EXPERIMENT 3: EFFECT OF QUALITY LEVEL ON PURCHASE INTENTION BY RATING POLARITY (GERMANY)

1

2

3

4

5

Poor Adequate Fair Good Very Good

Purc

hase

Inte

ntio

n

Quality Level

Consistent rating polarity (Smaller-is-better) Inconsistent rating polarity (Bigger-is-better)

Page 57: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

57

FIGURE 5

EXPERIMENT 3: MODERATION BY IMPLICIT NUMERICAL ASSOCIATION (GERMANY)

1

2

3

4

5

Poor Adequate Fair Good Very Good

Purc

hase

Inte

ntio

n

Quality Level

A. Strong Smaller-is-better Implicit Association

Consistent (Smaller-is-better) Inconsistent (Bigger-is-better)

1

2

3

4

5

Poor Adequate Fair Good Very Good

Purc

hase

Inte

ntio

n

Quality Level

B. Weak Smaller-is-better Implicit Association

Consistent (Smaller-is-better) Inconsistent (Bigger-is-better)

Page 58: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

1

When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias in Numeric Judgments

ELLIE J. KYUNG

MANOJ THOMAS

ARADHNA KRISHNA*

Web Appendix Contents

• Web Appendix A: Pretest—Bidding Behavior in an Auction • Web Appendix B: Experiment Stimuli • Web Appendix C: Experiment Results (means, standard deviation, and confidence

intervals; graphs of means and confidence intervals) • Web Appendix D: Ruling out possible Alternative Explanation (means and standard

deviation; analyses details) • Web Appendix E: Analyses with Reaction Time as Covariate • Web Appendix F: Order Effect Analyses • Web Appendix G: Experiment 4B Floodlight Analysis

Page 59: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

2

WEB APPENDIX A:

PRETEST—BIDDING BEHAVIOR IN AN AUCTION

In this pretest experiment, we test the rating polarity effect (testing H1) in a sealed-bid auction that entailed financial consequences. We examined whether rating polarity influenced participants’ bids. Comprehension, involvement, mood, and need-for-cognition were also measured to rule out potential alternative explanations. Method

Participants. Seventy-two undergraduates from a U.S. university (60% female; average age: 20.5 years) participated in the computer study in exchange for $4. Participants were randomly assigned to either the consistent (bigger-is-better) or inconsistent (smaller-is-better) rating polarity condition.

Procedure. Participants were told that they would be offered the opportunity to bid for a Japanese insulated travel mug that was not available for purchase locally, and that the participant who placed the winning bid would receive the travel mug. They could use all or some portion of their $4 payment to bid on the mug. If the winning bid was less than $4, the winner would receive the difference between the payment and their bid price. They were also told they could bid more than $4, but that they would personally have to pay the difference between their bid price and the $4 payment to receive the mug. Participants were asked to confirm [Yes / No] that they understood the instructions for the bidding process before proceeding.

Before they were shown the product information, participants were told that a quality rating for the mug would be provided by a reputable consumer welfare agency outside of the U.S.—an agency known for its evaluation of consumer products. They were asked to carefully read the meaning of the ratings provided. Those in the consistent rating polarity condition were told that 1 = unsatisfactory and 7 = very good, while those in the inconsistent rating polarity condition were told that the rating had an opposite polarity of 1 = very good and 7 = unsatisfactory. They were asked to confirm that they understood this rating format [Yes / No].

PRETEST EXPERIMENT STIMULI

Page 60: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

3

Pre-evaluation Comprehension Test. Participants were then asked the meaning of “1” and “7” rating poles for the quality ratings [very good, unsatisfactory] before proceeding. If they responded incorrectly, a message came up on the screen asking them to correct their response. Participants could proceed only after they had answered these questions correctly. This was done to ensure that the results did not stem from inattention or miscomprehension of the rating poles. Bid. Participants were then shown a photograph and descriptive information about the insulated mug, which included a quality rating (see Web Appendix B). The quality rating was 6.1 in the consistent rating polarity condition (bigger-is-better) and 1.9 in the inconsistent rating polarity condition (smaller-is-better). Note that the two ratings are normatively identical. Participants were then asked, “How much would you like to bid on this mug?” and were required to make a bid, but could make any bid they wished, including $0.

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. After entering their bid, participants were asked what the exterior of the mug was made of [stainless steel or plastic], the country of origin of the product [Germany or Japan], and the meaning of the rating poles 1 and 7 [very good or unsatisfactory]. This was done to confirm that the participants did not become confused or forget about the meaning of the ratings during the bidding process.

Rating Polarity Typicality. Participants were also asked to indicate which of the two numerical associations is more typical: “Higher Numbers Indicate Better Quality” or “Lower Numbers Indicate Better Quality.” They could also choose a third option labeled “not sure.”

Additional Measures. To rule out other possible alternative explanations, participants were also asked questions about their current mood [slider scales anchored at bad / good, unpleasant / pleasant, negative / positive], given the 18 items from the short form Need For Cognition (NFC) scale (Cacciopo, Petty, and Kao 1984), asked how involved / thoughtful / attentive they were during the study [slider scales anchored at not at all / very], and asked questions about their demographics. Results

Post-Evaluation Comprehension Test. All participants answered both of the questions on the meaning of the rating poles correctly. Thus, they were neither confused about, nor did they forget, the meaning of the poles.

Monetary Bid. A one-way ANOVA revealed that, as predicted, participants in the consistent rating polarity condition bid more on the insulated travel mug (M = $4.42) than those in the inconsistent rating polarity condition (M = $2.70, F (1, 70) = 4.80, p = .03, see figure 1).

FIGURE 1: BID VALUES BY RATING POLARITY

$4.42  

$2.70  

$0  

$2  

$4  

$6  

Consistent  ra2ng  polarity  (Bigger-­‐is-­‐be;er)  

Inconsistent  ra2ng  polarity  (Smaller-­‐is-­‐be;er)  

Bid  on

 Mug  ($

)  

Page 61: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

4

Web Appendix C summarizes the means and 95% confidence intervals by conditions across all experiments, and for further data visualization, also plots the means and 95% confidence intervals for the consistent versus inconsistent rating polarity conditions.

Rating Polarity Typicality. The majority of participants indicated that the bigger-is-better numerical association is more typical (90.3%) than the smaller-is-better one (9.7%), and thus the dominant numerical association in implicit memory.

Ruling Out Alternative Explanations. Mood: To rule out the possibility that an inconsistent rating polarity (smaller-is-better) might have put participants in a more negative mood and influenced their information processing, we averaged three mood measures into an index (α = .92) and conducted a one-way ANOVA, which revealed no difference in mood by condition (p = .42). Involvement: Similarly, to rule out the possibility that participants might have been more involved when attempting to use a rating format with inconsistent rating polarity, we averaged the three measures of involvement into an index (α = .87), and a one-way ANOVA revealed no difference in involvement by condition (p = .60). Need-for-cognition: We propose that the rating polarity effect manifests because participants are not aware of, and therefore cannot control for, its effects. Previous research has shown that individuals who are higher NFC are more systematic in their judgments (Florack and Scarabis 2001; Conner et al. 2007) because they are more thoughtful and reflective (Ajezen and Fishbein 2005)—for example, they are less likely to make judgments in line with negative stereotypes. However, if the rating polarity effect operates outside of people’s awareness, then high NFC individuals should not be able to overcome the effect of interference from implicit numerical associations in memory.

We conducted a linear regression where the independent measures were rating polarity (dummy coded: consistent = 0, inconsistent = 1), the mean centered NFC score, and the interaction of the two with the bid as the dependent measure. The results revealed a significant effect of rating polarity (β = -1.84, p = .03), but the effects of NFC (p = .29) and the interaction between NFC and rating polarity (p = .66) were not significant. This suggests that the rating polarity effect manifests for people with both low and high NFC and provides some evidence that the spontaneous interference of implicit memory is non-conscious because high NFC participants cannot adjust for it.

The means and standard deviations for these additional variables (mood, involvement, NFC) across all experiments are in Web Appendix D. These measures also show that participant involvement and attention are relatively high through all of the experiments, across experimental conditions, and low involvement or attention cannot account for the results.

Page 62: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

5

WEB APPENDIX B

EXPERIMENT 1 STIMULI

Page 63: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

6

EXPERIMENT 2B STIMULI

Page 64: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

7

EXPERIMENT 4A STIMULI

‘WHY’ MINDSET MANIPULATION

Page 65: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

8

‘HOW’ MINDSET MANIPULATION

Page 66: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

9

EXPERIMENT 5 STIMULI

TRUE / FALSE TASK

RECALL TASK

Page 67: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

10

WEB APPENDIX C: EXPERIMENT RESULTS

MEANS, STANDARD DEVIATIONS, AND + / - 95% CONFIDENCE INTERVAL

BIB SIB

Quality Level Mean SD

Confidence Interval Quality

Level Mean SD

Confidence Interval

Lower-Bound

Upper-Bound

Lower-Bound

Upper-Bound

PRETEST EXPERIMENT

6.10 4.42 4.12 3.04 5.79 1.90 2.70 2.18 1.95 3.45

EXPERIMENT 1

Low 68.77 16.88 63.86 73.68 1 73.08 13.92 68.55 77.61

High 78.28 13.97 73.48 83.07 2 72.80 17.27 68.12 77.48

EXPERIMENT 2A

1 5.0% 1.2% 2.6% 7.4% 1 8.6% 1.6% 5.4% 11.8%

2 7.2% 1.5% 4.4% 10.1% 2 14.2% 2.0% 10.2% 18.1%

3 39.0% 2.7% 33.6% 44.4% 3 20.5% 2.3% 15.9% 25.0%

4 83.0% 2.1% 78.9% 87.2% 4 72.9% 2.6% 67.9% 78.0%

5 86.8% 1.9% 83.1% 90.5% 5 80.9% 2.3% 76.4% 85.3%

EXPERIMENT 2B

1 0.73 0.82 0.59 0.88 1 1.00 1.33 0.86 1.14

2 0.97 0.79 0.83 1.12 2 1.24 1.24 1.09 1.38

3 1.72 0.94 1.57 1.86 3 1.57 0.85 1.43 1.72

4 2.33 1.08 2.19 2.47 4 2.33 0.92 2.18 2.47

5 2.75 1.34 2.61 2.90 5 2.67 1.05 2.53 2.81

EXPERIMENT 3

1 2.43 1.71 0.15 2.14 1 3.05 1.83 0.16 2.74

2 2.70 1.60 0.14 2.43 2 2.89 1.68 0.14 2.60

3 2.90 1.31 0.11 2.68 3 3.10 1.58 0.14 2.83

4 4.36 1.83 0.16 4.05 4 3.88 1.53 0.13 3.62

5 4.57 1.97 0.17 4.23 5 4.12 1.89 0.16 3.80  

   

Page 68: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

11

MEANS, STANDARD DEVIATIONS, AND + / - 95% CONFIDENCE INTERVAL CONTINUED

 

BIB SIB

Quality Level Mean SD

Confidence Interval Quality

Level Mean SD

Confidence Interval

Lower-Bound

Upper-Bound

Lower-Bound

Upper-Bound

EXPERIMENT 4A

CONCRETE

1 1.83 1.35 1.60 2.05 1 1.89 1.61 1.58 2.20

2 2.36 1.38 2.12 2.59 2 2.29 1.39 2.02 2.55

3 3.43 1.34 3.21 3.66 3 3.23 1.36 2.97 3.49

4 4.59 1.65 4.32 4.87 4 4.16 1.79 3.82 4.50

5 5.26 1.75 4.97 5.56 5 5.00 1.93 4.63 5.37

ABSTRACT

1 1.82 1.29 1.59 2.05 1 2.35 1.91 2.02 2.68

2 2.25 1.20 2.03 2.46 2 2.74 1.75 2.43 3.04

3 3.52 1.32 3.29 3.76 3 3.43 1.42 3.18 3.67

4 4.84 1.61 4.56 5.13 4 4.18 1.74 3.87 4.48

5 5.35 1.75 5.04 5.66 5 4.93 2.07 4.57 5.29

EXPERIMENT 4B

1 2.06 1.57 1.88 2.24 1 2.60 2.02 2.37 2.83

2 2.24 1.43 2.08 2.40 2 3.02 1.86 2.81 3.23

3 3.33 1.52 3.16 3.51 3 3.51 1.62 3.33 3.69

4 4.60 1.66 4.42 4.79 4 4.27 2.02 4.04 4.50

5 5.11 1.81 4.90 5.31 5 4.83 2.13 4.59 5.08

EXPERIMENT 5

1 1.45 1.12 1.32 1.58 1 1.82 1.50 1.65 1.99

2 1.87 1.30 1.72 2.02 2 2.33 1.38 2.17 2.48

3 3.22 1.36 3.07 3.38 3 3.34 1.23 3.20 3.48

4 4.73 1.75 4.53 4.93 4 4.60 1.90 4.38 4.81

5 5.29 1.99 5.06 5.52 5 5.24 2.03 5.01 5.47

Page 69: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

12

MEAN AND CONFIDENCE INTERVAL GRAPHS

Each graph plots the mean evaluation + / - the 95% confidence interval for the consistent and inconsistent rating polarity conditions. (BIB = bigger-is-better; SIB = smaller-is-better)

0  

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

7  

Mean  +  /  -­‐    95%  Con

fiden

ce  In

terval  

Pretest  

Consistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (BIB)  Inconsistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (SIB)  

60  

65  

70  

75  

80  

85  

Mean  +  /  -­‐  9

5%  Con

fiden

ce  In

terval  

Quality  Level  

Exp  1  

Low  Quality   High  Quality  

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

0   2   4  

Mean  +  /  -­‐  9

5%  Con

fiden

ce  In

terval  

Quality  Level  

Exp  3  

Inconsistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (BIB)  Consistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (SIB)  

-­‐25%  

0%  

25%  

50%  

75%  

100%  

0   2   4  Mean  +  /  -­‐  9

5%  Con

fiden

ce  In

terval  

Quality  Level  

Exp  2a  

Consistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (BIB)  Inconsistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (SIB)  

Page 70: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

13

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

0   1   2   3   4   5  Mean  +  /  -­‐  9

5%  Con

fiden

ce  In

terval  

Quality  Level  

Exp  4a:  Concrete  

Consistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (BIB)  Inconsistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (SIB)  

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

0   1   2   3   4   5  

Mean  +  /  -­‐  9

5%  Con

fiden

ce  In

terval  

Quality  Level  

Exp  4a:  Abstract  

Consistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (BIB)  Inconsistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (SIB)  

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

0   1   2   3   4   5  

Mean  +  /  -­‐  9

5%  Con

fiden

ce  In

terval  

Quality  Level  

Exp  4b  Consistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (BIB)  Inconsistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (SIB)  

1  

2  

3  

4  

5  

6  

0   1   2   3   4   5  

Mean  +  /  -­‐  9

5%  Con

fiden

ce  In

terval  

Quality  Level  

Exp  5  Consistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (BIB)  Inconsistent  Ra2ng  Polarity  (SIB)  

Page 71: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

14

WEB APPENDIX D: RULING OUT ALTERNATIVE EXPLANATIONS

MEANS AND STDV OF POSSIBLE ALTERNATIVE EXPLANATIONS

BIB SIB

Measure Mean SD Mean SD PRETEST

Mood1 63.86 14.62 60.72 17.80 Involvement1 66.32 14.06 64.33 17.40 NFC 3.70 0.46 3.64 0.57

EXPERIMENT 1 Mood1 70.40 17.09 70.95 16.51 NFC 3.55 0.54 3.58 0.51

Good Product Mood1 72.14 15.52 68.70 15.29 NFC 3.58 0.51 3.52 0.44

Bad Product Mood1 68.57 18.63 73.01 17.46 NFC 3.51 0.57 3.63 0.57

EXPERIMENT 2A Mood2 5.81 1.12 5.43 1.27 Involvement 4.61 0.50 4.55 0.66

EXPERIMENT 2B Mood2 5.74 1.40 5.40 1.38 Involvement 4.60 0.51 4.67 0.48

EXPERIMENT 3 Mood 3.99 0.98 4.13 0.75 Involvement 4.14 0.74 4.32 0.63

EXPERIMENT 4A Mood2 4.01 0.85 4.06 0.74 Involvement 4.54 0.62 4.53 0.49

Concrete Mindset Mood2 4.01 0.74 4.03 0.72 Involvement 4.45 0.74 4.52 0.51

Abstract Mindset Mood2 4.01 0.98 4.09 0.76 Involvement 4.63 0.44 4.54 0.49

EXPERIMENT 4B Mood2 4.62 0.94 4.75 0.98 Involvement 3.93 0.60 3.94 0.69

EXPERIMENT 5 Mood2 4.33 1.68 4.54 1.81 Involvement 4.82 0.38 4.79 0.39

1100-point scale, 27-point scale, all others 5-point scale

Page 72: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

15

ANALYSIS DETAILS RULING OUT POSSIBLE ALTERNATIVE EXPLANATIONS

Experiments 2a & 2b

Mood: We averaged mood measures into an index (2a: α = .97; 2b: α = .97) and

conducted an ANOVA with rating polarity as the dependent measure. This analysis for

experiment 2a revealed a marginally significant effect of rating polarity where those in the

consistent rating polarity reported slightly more positive mood (M = 5.75) than those in the

inconsistent rating polarity condition (M = 5.40, F(1, 205) = 3.26, p = .07). In order to rule out

the possibility that mood could account for the results, we conducted the analysis using mood as

an additional independent variable. The effect of mood was not significant (β = .11, p = .52) and

the rating polarity x quality level interaction remained significant (β = -.46, p = .01), ruling out

the possibility that mood might account for the results. This analysis for experiment 2b revealed

a significant effect of rating polarity where those in the consistent rating polarity reported

slightly more positive mood (M = 5.82) than those in the inconsistent rating polarity condition

(M = 5.43, F(1, 216) = 5.59, p = .02). In order to rule out the possibility that mood could account

for the results, we conducted the willingness-to-pay analysis using mood as an additional

independent variable. The effect of mood was not significant (β = .07, p = .92) and the rating

polarity x quality level interaction remained significant (β = -.24, p < .01), ruling out the

possibility that mood might account for the results. Involvement: Similarly, to rule out the

possibility that participants might have been more involved when attempting to use a rating

format with inconsistent rating polarity, we averaged the measures of involvement into an index

(2a: α = .92; 2b: α = .95), and an ANOVA revealed no difference in involvement by condition

(2a: p = .30; 2b: p = .44).

Page 73: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

16

Experiment 3

Mood: We averaged three mood measures into an index (α = .93) and a PROC GLM

analysis that revealed no difference in mood by condition (p = .46). Involvement: Similarly, we

averaged the measures of involvement into an index (α = .90), and the same PROC GLM

analysis revealed no difference in involvement by condition (p = .22).

Experiment 4a

Mood: The three mood measures were averaged together (α = .90). A regression with

rating polarity, mindset, and their interaction as independent measures and the averaged mood

measure as the dependent measure confirmed that there was no significant effect of rating

polarity, mindset, or their interaction on mood (all ps >.68). Involvement: Similarly, the four

measures of participant involvement were averaged into an index (α = .93), and a regression with

the same independent measures and the involvement index as a dependent measure confirmed

there was no significant effect of rating polarity, mindset, or their interaction on involvement and

attention to the evaluation task (all ps > .23).

Experiment 4b

Mood: The mood measures were averaged together (α = .82). A regression with rating

polarity, mindset, and their interaction as independent measures and the averaged mood measure

as the dependent measure confirmed that there was no significant effect of rating polarity,

mindset, or their interaction on mood (all ps >.29). Involvement: Similarly, the four measures of

participant involvement were averaged into an index (α = .90), and a regression with the same

independent measures and the involvement index as the dependent measure confirmed there was

no significant effect of rating polarity, mindset, or their interaction on involvement and attention

Page 74: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

17

to the evaluation task (all ps > .79).

Experiment 5

Mood: The three mood measures were averaged together (α = .98). A regression with

rating polarity, memory type, and their interaction as independent measures and the averaged

mood measure as the dependent measure confirmed that there was no significant effect of rating

polarity, memory, or their interaction on mood (all ps > .40). Involvement: Similarly, the

measures of participant involvement were averaged into an index (α = .78), and a regression with

the same independent measures and the involvement index as a dependent measure confirmed

there was no significant effect of rating polarity, memory type, or their interaction on

involvement with the evaluation task (all ps > .35).

Page 75: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

18

WEB APPENDIX E: ANALYSIS WITH REACTION TIME AS COVARIATE

Analysis for each experiment was conducted as a regression (paradigm pretest, experiments 1) or repeated measures regression (remaining experiments) with the evaluation as the dependent measure and log reaction time as a covariate. Rating polarity was dummy coded (0 = consistent rating polarity, 1 = inconsistent rating polarity).

Effect β SE DF t / Z p

Pretest Rating Polarity -1.77 0.77 69 -2.30 0.02 Reaction Time 0.86 0.47 69 1.84 0.07

Experiment 1

Rating Polarity 4.56 3.42 164 1.33 0.19 Quality Level 9.70 3.51 164 2.77 0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -10.00 4.82 164 -2.07 0.04 Reaction Time -1.44 2.79 164 -0.52 0.61

Experiment 2a

Rating Polarity 0.70 0.41 202 1.69 0.09 Quality Level 1.47 0.08 3095 17.67 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.33 0.12 3095 -2.70 <0.01 Reaction Time 0.04 0.08 3095 -.052 0.61

Experiment 2b

Rating Polarity 0.35 0.11 215 3.27 <0.01 Quality Level 0.54 0.02 3011 34.24 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.10 0.02 3011 -4.53 <0.01 Reaction Time 0.05 0.03 3011 1.69 0.09

Experiment 3

Rating Polarity 0.87 0.27 87 3.22 <0.01 Quality Level 0.59 0.04 1243 14.78 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.28 0.06 1243 -4.95 <0.01 Reaction Time 0.19 0.08 1243 2.53 0.01

Experiment 4a

Rating Polarity 0.14 0.25 162 0.55 0.58 Quality Level 0.91 0.04 2319 23.06 <.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.11 0.06 2319 -1.76 0.08 Construal Mindset -0.11 0.24 162 -0.46 0.64 Rating Polarity x Construal Mindset 0.75 0.35 162 2.17 0.03 Quality Level x Construal Mindset 0.06 0.06 2319 0.97 0.33 3-way interaction -0.20 0.08 2319 -2.40 0.02 Reaction Time -0.04 0.05 2319 -0.85 0.40

Experiment 4b

Rating Polarity 1.02 0.17 197 5.97 <.01 Quality Level 0.84 0.03 2800 27.88 <.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.28 0.04 2800 -6.49 <.01 Construal Mindset -0.06 0.03 197 -1.99 0.05 Rating Polarity x Construal Mindset 0.14 0.04 197 3.71 0.00 Quality Level x Construal Mindset 0.01 0.01 2800 1.53 0.13 3-way interaction -0.03 0.01 2800 -2.66 0.01 Reaction Time -0.05 0.04 2800 -1.18 0.24

Experiment 5

Rating Polarity 0.61 0.16 192 3.76 0.00 Quality Level 1.06 0.03 2719 40.24 <.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.15 0.04 2719 -3.94 <.01 Implicit Memory -0.03 0.02 192 -1.90 0.06 Rating Polarity x Implicit Memory 0.01 0.03 192 0.59 0.55 Quality Level x Implicit Memory 0.01 0.00 2719 1.53 0.13 3-way interaction -0.01 0.01 2719 -1.59 0.11 Reaction Time 0.02 0.04 2719 0.63 0.53

Page 76: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

19

WEB APPENDIX F: ORDER EFFECTS ANALYSES

Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Order Effects are highlighted in gray. Order was coded as 1 to 15 for each of the 15 products in experiments 2a, 2b, 3, 4a, 4b, and 5 and mean centered for the analysis.

β SE DF t p

Exp 2a

Rating Polarity 0.74 0.43 204 1.73 0.08 Quality Level 1.49 0.09 3095 16.82 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.34 0.13 3095 -2.70 <0.01 Order -0.18 0.06 3095 -2.95 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Order -0.01 0.09 3095 -0.17 0.87 Quality Level x Order 0.06 0.02 3095 3.29 <0.01 3-way Interaction -0.01 0.03 3095 -0.31 0.76

Exp 2b

Rating Polarity 0.36 0.11 217 3.40 <0.01 Quality Level 0.54 0.02 3060 34.28 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.10 0.02 3060 -4.48 <0.01 Order 0.0004 0.01 3060 0.03 0.97 Rating Polarity x Order -0.02 0.02 3060 -0.94 0.35 Quality Level x Order 0.001 0.00 3060 0.15 0.88 3-way Interaction 0.003 0.01 3060 0.66 0.51

Exp 3

Rating Polarity 0.84 0.27 87 3.06 <0.01 Quality Level 0.59 0.04 1240 14.59 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.28 0.06 1240 -4.88 <0.01 Order -0.05 0.03 1240 -1.54 0.12 Rating Polarity x Order 0.02 0.05 1240 0.47 0.64 Quality Level x Order 0.02 0.01 1240 1.70 0.09 3-way Interaction -0.01 0.01 1240 -0.76 0.45

Exp 4a

Rating Polarity 0.88 0.24 163 3.64 <0.01 Quality Level 0.96 0.04 2326 23.07 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.30 0.06 2326 -5.14 <0.01 Mindset 0.08 0.24 163 0.35 0.72 Rating Polarity x Mindset -0.68 0.35 163 -1.96 0.05 Quality Level x Mindset -0.05 0.06 2326 -0.87 0.39 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Mindset 0.17 0.08 2326 2.06 0.04 Order -0.03 0.03 2326 -0.86 0.39 Rating Polarity x Order 0.01 0.04 2326 0.25 0.80 Quality Level x Order 0.01 0.01 2326 1.39 0.16 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Order -0.01 0.01 2326 -0.77 0.44 Mindset x Order 0.06 0.04 2326 1.38 0.17 Rating Polarity x Mindset x Order -0.16 0.07 2326 -2.52 0.01 Quality Level x Mindset x Order -0.02 0.01 2326 -1.28 0.20 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Mindset x Order 0.06 0.02 2326 2.88 <0.01

 

Page 77: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

20

WEB APPENDIX F: ORDER EFFECTS ANALYSES

 

β SE DF t p

Exp 4b

Rating Polarity 1.00 0.17 197 5.89 <0.01 Quality Level 0.83 0.03 2802 27.42 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.27 0.04 2802 -6.37 <0.01 Mindset -0.05 0.03 197 -1.92 0.06 Rating Polarity x Mindset 0.14 0.04 197 3.70 <0.01 Quality Level x Mindset 0.01 0.01 2802 1.37 0.17 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Mindset -0.03 0.01 2802 -2.65 <0.01 Order -0.04 0.02 2802 -1.78 0.08 Rating Polarity x Order 0.03 0.03 2802 0.92 0.36 Quality Level x Order 0.03 0.01 2802 3.97 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Order -0.01 0.01 2802 -1.29 0.20 Mindset x Order 0.002 0.01 2802 0.38 0.71 Rating Polarity x Mindset x Order -0.01 0.01 2802 -0.69 0.49 Quality Level x Mindset x Order -0.002 0.002 2802 -1.12 0.26 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Mindset x Order 0.004 0.002 2802 1.62 0.11

Exp 5

Rating Polarity 0.53 0.17 193 3.14 <0.01 Quality Level 1.04 0.03 2549 36.48 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.12 0.04 2549 -2.95 <0.01 Implicit Memory -0.05 0.02 193 -2.78 <0.01 Rating Polarity x Implicit Memory 0.02 0.02 193 0.95 0.35 Quality Level x Implicit Memory 0.01 0.00 2549 2.40 0.02 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Implicit Memory -0.01 0.01 2549 -1.71 0.09

Order -0.01 0.02 2549 -0.27 0.78 Rating Polarity x Order -0.03 0.03 2549 -1.01 0.31 Quality Level x Order 0.01 0.01 2549 1.75 0.08 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Order -0.001 0.01 2549 -0.14 0.89 Implicit Memory x Order 0.004 0.003 2549 1.19 0.23 Rating Polarity x Implicit Memory x Order 0.001 0.005 2549 0.24 0.81 Quality Level x Implicit Memory x Order -0.001 0.001 2549 -0.80 0.42 Rating Polarity x Quality Level x Judge x Order -0.0005 0.001 2549 -0.32 0.75

CONTRASTS

High Reliance on Implicit Memory: Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.19 0.06 2549 -3.29 <0.01

Low Reliance on Implicit Memory: Rating Polarity x Quality Level -0.05 0.06 2549 -0.87 0.39

Page 78: When Bigger is Better (and When it is Not): Implicit Bias ...tuck-fac-cen.dartmouth.edu/images/uploads/faculty/... · ELLIE J. KYUNG MANOJ THOMAS ARADHNA KRISHNA* ... Cornell University,

   

21

WEB APPENDIX G: EXPERIMENT 4B FLOODLIGHT ANALYSIS

DIFFERENCE IN THE RATING POLARITY x QUALITY LEVEL INTERACTION ACROSS BIF VALUES

BIF Value (0=most concrete, 24=most abstract)

Interference from Rating Polarity Effect

(Rating Polarity x Quality Level Interaction Coefficient)

Lower 95% Confidence

Interval

Upper 95% Confidence

Interval t(2810) p

0 0.02 -0.22 0.26 0.13 0.89 2 -0.03 -0.24 0.17 -0.31 0.76 4 -0.08 -0.25 0.09 -0.91 0.36 6 -0.13 -0.27 0.01 -1.79 0.07 8 -0.18 -0.29 -0.06 -3.05 0.002 10 -0.23 -0.32 -0.13 -4.76 <.0001 12 -0.27 -0.36 -0.19 -6.41 <.0001 14 -0.32 -0.41 -0.23 -7.00 <.0001 16 -0.37 -0.48 -0.26 -6.62 <.0001 18 -0.42 -0.56 -0.28 -6.01 <.0001 20 -0.47 -0.64 -0.30 -5.47 <.0001 22 -0.52 -0.72 -0.32 -5.05 <.0001 24 -0.56 -0.80 -0.33 -4.72 <.0001

Floodlight analysis follows procedures outlined in Spiller et al. (2013).