visual and auditory sensitivity in autism spectrum...

57
VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS by TIA NICOLE HOLTZCLAW LAURA KLINGER, COMMITTEE CHAIR MARK KLINGER EDWARD MERRILL ANGELA BARBER A THESIS Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in the Department of Psychology in the Graduate School of The University of Alabama TUSCALOOSA, ALABAMA 2011

Upload: others

Post on 11-Feb-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY

IN AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

by

TIA NICOLE HOLTZCLAW

LAURA KLINGER, COMMITTEE CHAIR MARK KLINGER

EDWARD MERRILL ANGELA BARBER

A THESIS

Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts

in the Department of Psychology in the Graduate School of

The University of Alabama

TUSCALOOSA, ALABAMA

2011

Page 2: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

Copyright Tia Nicole Holtzclaw 2011 ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

Page 3: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

ii

ABSTRACT

Individuals with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often cannot tolerate certain sights

and sounds, such as fluorescent lights, vacuum cleaners, and babies crying, which affects their

ability to engage in activities at home and in the community. One theory that may account for

these sensory impairments is the Enhanced Perceptual Functioning theory, which posits that

individuals with ASD are more sensitive to auditory and visual stimuli. Previous studies in

support of this theory found that individuals with ASD demonstrated an enhanced ability to

detect differences in pitch, discriminate changes in visual stimuli, and detect novel targets in

visual arrays. The response type and task demands differed greatly among these studies, making

it difficult to draw firm conclusions. Furthermore, no studies have compared visual and auditory

perception directly.

The current study sought to fill these gaps by examining visual and auditory perception in

a sample of 13 children with ASD and 13 children with typical development aged 8-12 years. It

was predicted that individuals with ASD would show increased sensitivity in both auditory and

visual domains. To assess perceptual abilities, participants completed an auditory discrimination

task examining pitch and volume and a visual discrimination task examining hue and luminance.

Using signal detection theory comparing hits and false alarm rates (d-prime) to analyze

their performance, children with ASD showed enhanced perception for pitch only compared to

children with typical development. In the ASD group, high overall sensitivity were related to an

overall measure of autism severity, supporting the notion that enhanced perception, particularly

Page 4: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

iii

pitch sensitivity, may be a phenotypic marker for ASD. This is the first study to demonstrate a

relationship among perceptual sensitivity and ASD symptoms. These results have clinical

significance for understanding children with ASD. For example, caregivers and teachers taking

children with ASD to a noisy environment such as a gymnasium or the mall may want to provide

earplugs or headphones to dampen the noise. It is possible that auditory hyper-sensitivity may

underlie the development of difficulties with social-communication and may lead to repetitive

behaviors in an effort to manage the environment.

Page 5: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

iv

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS AND SYMBOLS

d' D prime: Measure of sensitivity based on the separation between the means of the hit rate

and false-alarm rate in units of standard deviation

d Cohen’s d: Measure of effect size for use with t-tests or ANOVA

F Fisher’s F ratio: A ratio of two variances

df Degrees of freedom: Number of values free to vary after certain restrictions have been placed on the data

SD Standard Deviation: Measure of variation from the mean

2η Partial eta squared: Measure of effect size for use in ANOVA

p Probability associated with the occurrence under the null hypothesis of a value as

extreme as or more extreme than the observed value

r Pearson product-moment correlation

t Computed value of t test

< Less than

= Equal to

Page 6: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

v

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I wish to thank the many colleagues, friends, and faculty members who have helped me

with this research project. In particular, I would like to thank Dr. Laura Klinger, the chairperson

of this thesis, for sharing her research expertise and wisdom regarding children with ASD and

Dr. Mark Klinger for his encouragement and expertise in data analysis. I would also like to thank

their fellow committee members, Dr. Ed Merrill and Dr. Angela Barber, for their invaluable

input, inspiring questions, and support of both the thesis and my academic progress. I am most

indebted to Nicole Broka and Jessica Emmons for their encouragement and assistance with

recruitment, data collection, and data entry. I would also like to thank my fellow graduate

students and labmates, as well as Dr. Michelle DeRamus in the ASD Clinic, who generously

donated their time during the development and completion of this project. A big thank you goes

to the many sites who allowed me to recruit potential participants. I am especially grateful to the

children and families who participated in my research – I literally could not have done it without

you! Finally, this research would not have been possible without the support of my friends and

family, particularly my husband, who has been a constant source of encouragement and support.

Page 7: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

vi

CONTENTS

ABSTRACT ............................................................................................ ii

LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS AND SYMBOLS...................................... iv

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ........................................................................ v

LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................ vii

1. INTRODUCTION ................................................................................ 1

2. METHOD ........................................................................................... 16

a. Participants ......................................................................................... 16

b. Apparatus ........................................................................................... 17

c. Stimuli ................................................................................................ 17

d. Procedure ........................................................................................... 19

e. Measures............................................................................................. 21

3. RESULTS .......................................................................................... 25

4. DISCUSSION .................................................................................... 37

REFERENCES ....................................................................................... 44

Page 8: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

vii

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1: Characteristics of Included Participants ..................................... 25

Table 2: Mean d’ scores for the Auditory Perception Task ..................... 27

Table 3: Mean d’ scores for the Visual Perception Task .......................... 27

Table 4: Results of the Omnibus ANCOVA ........................................... 29

Table 5: Results of the Auditory ANCOVA ............................................ 31

Table 6: Factor Scores ........................................................................... 33

Table 7: Relations among Perception, Intellectual Ability, and Autism Measures in the ASD Group Controlling for FSIQ .................... 34 Table 8: Relations among Perception, Intellectual Ability, and Autism Measures in the Control Group Controlling for FSIQ ................ 36

Page 9: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

1

Introduction

A fifteen-year-old girl with Asperger’s Syndrome describes her experience when she

goes out in public:

“…everything is upside down and spinning all around you. You hear all these noises coming from everywhere - you hear like everybody's conversation. It feels like lights are flashing in your head - in your eyes I mean, and your brain just goes nuts. So, what I do to try to control that is I jump up and down and flap my hands - as if that doesn't look weird. That's my coping skill for now” (Anonymous, 2009).

Her compelling description captures several diagnostic features of Autism Spectrum Disorders

(ASD), including the repetitive behaviors and the social-communication difficulties that define

the disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) . Interestingly, she links these diagnostic

symptoms to sensory differences that lead to a feeling of being disoriented and overwhelmed by

one’s environment. These sensory differences are often reported by individuals with ASD but are

not part of the diagnostic criteria in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,

4th edition (DSM-IV-TR). The purpose of this study was to more closely examine the sensory

differences experienced by individuals with ASD.

Autism Symptomatology

According to the DSM-IV-TR, Autism Spectrum Disorders include the diagnoses of

Autistic Disorder, Asperger’s Syndrome, and Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise

Specified (PDD-NOS) (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). However, the proposed

version of the DSM-V indicates that the diagnostic criteria for ASD will soon undergo changes

Page 10: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

2

in the upcoming DSM-V. It has been proposed that the diagnosis of Asperger’s Syndrome will

not be included in the DSM-V because research has not supported the distinction between

Asperger’s and High Functioning Autism (First, 2008). In the current DSM-IV-TR, the diagnosis

of Autism Spectrum Disorders is based on impairment in the three domains of social,

communication, and repetitive behaviors. However, research presented at the diagnosis-related

planning conference held by the American Psychiatric Association showed that the current three

domains are not supported by factor, cluster, or latent class analyses, which suggests a unitary

underlying factor structure (First, 2008). Therefore, it has been proposed that the social and

communication domains will be combined into one domain, and the repetitive behavior domain

will be simplified in the DSM-V.

Due to the heterogeneity of ASD symptoms, there is a wide variety in the presentation of

social-communication symptoms. These may include impairments in using nonverbal behaviors

such as facial expressions, gestures, and body postures to regulate social interactions. Social

impairment may also be expressed as a lack of social or emotional reciprocity, a failure to

develop appropriate peer relationships, or a lack of spontaneous seeking to share enjoyment,

interests, or achievements with others. In addition, social imitative play is often impaired in

young children with ASD. Severely impaired individuals with ASD may not develop spoken

language at all, or language development may be limited or delayed. In individuals who develop

adequate speech, they may demonstrate a marked impairment in their ability to initiate or sustain

a conversation, or use repetitive language (American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

The diagnostic domain that consists of restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped patterns of

behavior, interests and activities can be divided into two subtypes: lower-order and higher-order

behaviors (Turner, 1999). Lower-order behaviors consist of two categories: (a) stereotyped

Page 11: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

3

movements, which are seemingly purposeless movements or actions that are repeated in a similar

manner; and (b) self-injurious behaviors, which are any movements or actions that have the

potential to cause redness, bruising, or other injury to the body and that are repeated in a similar

manner. Higher-order behaviors consist of four categories: (a) compulsions (behavior that is

repeated and performed according to a rule, or involves things being done “just so”); (b) need for

routines or rituals (performing activities of daily living in a similar manner); (c) insistence on

sameness (resistance to change or insisting that things stay the same); and (d) restricted interests

(limited range of focus, interest, or activity). Definitions for each category above are from the

Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000).

Many repetitive behaviors have a sensory aspect to them, such as finger-flicking near the

eyes, listening to the same piece of music repeatedly, or rubbing a certain texture constantly.

Hutt and Hutt (1968) proposed that repetitive behaviors reduce physiological arousal by

providing endogenous stimulation that allows the individual to block sensory input. There has

been no evidence to support the notion that the primary function of repetitive behaviors is to

modulate arousal levels, although extreme arousal levels may increase rates of repetitive

behavior in individuals with autism as they do in individuals with typical development and

animals of other species (Turner, 1997). Dawson (1991) explored the arousal regulation theory in

the context of difficulty processing unpredictable information. She suggested that if individuals

have difficulties with arousal, this would negatively affect social interactions, and these

difficulties may be a consequence of disruptions in early patterns of social interaction.

Although sensory symptoms are not part of the diagnostic criteria for ASD in the DSM-

IV, one of the proposed revisions for the DSM-V includes adding hyper- or hypo-reactivity to

sensory input or unusual interest in sensory aspects of the environment under the restricted,

Page 12: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

4

repetitive behavior category. Sensory symptoms occur very frequently in ASD. Baranek and

colleagues (2006) reported that 69% of preschoolers with ASD showed sensory symptoms. In a

parent-report study using the Short Sensory Profile, Rogers and colleagues (2003) found that

preschoolers with ASD showed significantly higher scores than children with typical

development on auditory filtering and sensitivity to taste, smell, and tactile stimuli, indicating

greater sensory impairment in these areas. Studies of sensory processes in ASD have shown that

individuals with ASD have both hyper- and hyposensitivity to various stimuli (Baranek, David,

Poe, Stone, & Watson, 2006; Rogers, Hepburn, & Wehner, 2003). Hyper-responsiveness is an

exaggerated behavioral response to sensory stimuli (e.g., aversive reaction to lights, covering

ears for certain sounds, avoidance of touch). Hypo-responsiveness refers to a lack of response, or

insufficient intensity of response to sensory stimuli (e.g., diminished response to pain, lack of

orienting to novel sounds).

In the field of autism, reports of superior performance in auditory perception, such as

cases of perfect pitch (Heaton, Davis, & Happé, 2008), and in visual perception, such as on block

design tasks and embedded-figures tasks (Frith, 2003; Shah & Frith, 1983) led to the

development of the theory on Enhanced Perceptual Functioning (Mottron & Burack, 2001). The

Enhanced Perceptual Functioning theory is an information-processing theory that posits that

individuals with ASD have enhanced auditory and visual perceptual abilities due to the over-

functioning of the brain regions typically involved in primary perceptual functions (Mottron &

Burack, 2006). Perception is the process of attaining awareness and understanding of sensory

information, including visual, auditory, olfactory, gustatory, vestibular, and proprioceptive

stimuli. The individual selectively attends to the complex stream of sensory input from the

environment, then perceptual processes translate this sensory input into meaningful information,

Page 13: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

5

which can then be used to guide the individual’s thoughts and behaviors. Thus, perception is the

intersection between selective attention and cognition. The areas of attention, cognition, and

perception have been extensively studied in ASD, and some relevant research from each area

will be presented here in order to give a framework for the study of perceptual abilities in

children with ASD. A brief review of research on selective attention will be presented first,

followed by a description of two cognitive theories that affect how information is processed

meaningfully. Finally, support for the Enhanced Perceptual Functioning theory will be discussed.

Selective Attention

The role of attention in perception primarily occurs at the level of orienting, i.e., what

sensory information the individual attends to. In a recent review of studies on attention in autism,

Sanders et al. (2008) indicated that there have been mixed findings in the attention literature

about orienting performance in individuals with ASD. The majority of the evidence suggests that

attention orienting is impaired (Akshoomoff & Courchesne, 1992, 1994; Casey, Gordon,

Mannheim, & Rumsey, 1993; Courchesne et al., 1994; Dawson, Meltzoff, Osterling, Rinaldi, &

Brown, 1998; Landry & Bryson, 2004; Renner, Klinger, & Klinger, 2006; Rinehart, Bradshaw,

Moss, Brereton, & Tonge, 2001; Senju, Tojo, Dairoku, & Hasegawa, 2004; Townsend, Harris, et

al., 1996; Wainwright & Bryson, 1996; Wainwright-Sharp & Bryson, 1993). However, other

evidence suggests that orienting is intact or even enhanced (Gomot, Giard, Adrien, Barthelemy,

& Bruneau, 2002; larocci & Burack, 2004; Leekam, López, & Moore, 2000). These

discrepancies could be due to differences in automatic orienting processes based on what

captures the attention of individuals with ASD. When attending to sensory input, attentional

mechanisms can be automatic (i.e., bottom-up processing) or conscious (i.e., top-down

processing) (Wegener, Ehn, Aurich, Galashan, & Kreiter, 2008). According to Wegener and

Page 14: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

6

colleagues, automatic mechanisms of attention are mainly driven by the perceptual saliency of a

stimulus, and this is referred to as attentional capture. Conscious mechanisms of attention are

mainly driven by endogenous factors based on the individual’s previous experience and are

modulated by cognitive processes.

Many researchers have suggested that individuals with ASD tend to rely more on

automatic rather than conscious mechanisms of attention (Frith, 2003; Iarocci et al., 2006;

Mottron & Burack, 2001). A metaphor that is frequently used to capture this phenomenon states

that the attention of individuals with ASD is similar to a spotlight with a narrow focus, while the

attention of individuals with typical development is more like a searchlight that takes in a broad

range of sensory information (Bruckner & Yoder, 2007). If individuals with ASD do not

automatically attend to the same sensory stimuli in the environment as individuals with typical

development do, this may cause differences in their sensory perception, which could lead to

changes in their cognition, and ultimately, their behavior.

Cognitive Theories of Information Processing

This bias towards an “attentional spotlight” in individuals with ASD fits well with the

explanations of ASD symptomatology offered by two current cognitive theories in ASD. Weak

Central Coherence (Frith, 2003) and Implicit Learning (Klinger, Klinger, & Pohlig, 2007)

theories propose that individuals with ASD have difficulty integrating information into a

meaningful context. The theory of Weak Central Coherence states that individuals with ASD

have a cognitive style that is characterized by preferential processing of local features instead of

global features of the environment, which can lead to a failure to integrate visual and perceptual

details into a coherent whole (Frith & Happé, 1994). According to Frith and Happé’s theory,

lower-level processing is not integrated into higher, more central processes. For example,

Page 15: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

7

children with ASD who are hyperlexic can read at a higher level than they understand,

suggesting that the lower level phonological processing of words is independent of higher level

semantic interpretations (Aram & Healy, 1988).

This theory predicts that individuals with ASD would show difficulties with tasks that

require integration, but it also predicts that they would show strengths on tasks that demand

attention to detail. The classic test of central coherence is the embedded-figures task, where

participants are asked to detect a geometric shape among a complex background. Individuals

with ASD show superior performance on the embedded-figures task, showing a strength for

featural discrimination (Shah & Frith, 1983). Similarly, on the Wechsler Block Design subtest,

individuals with ASD are not aided by pre-segmentation of patterns, and their errors tend to be

ones of global configuration rather than local detail (Shah & Frith, 1993). Belmonte and

colleagues (2004) suggest that Weak Central Coherence may form the cognitive underpinning of

behavioral disturbances in autism by impairing the use of contextual information. They propose

that perceptual filtering in ASD occurs in an all-or-none manner with little regard for the

relevance, specificity, or sensory modality of the stimulus. This may lead to enhanced perception

for some details but inefficient processing of the complex stream of sensory input.

The Implicit Learning theory suggests that the ability to automatically integrate

information during learning is impaired in ASD (Klinger et al., 2007). According to Implicit

Learning theory, individuals with ASD rely more on explicit learning strategies as a

“bootstrapping” mechanism to compensate for difficulties with implicit learning. This

impairment may lead to a rigid, narrow focus of attention that prevents individuals from

perceiving the gestalt of a situation. In support of this theory, Klinger and colleagues have found

impaired performance on a variety of implicit learning tasks including prototype learning and

Page 16: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

8

artificial grammar learning (Klinger and Dawson, 2001; Klinger et al., 2007). Further, Klinger

and colleagues theorize that repetitive behaviors are a coping strategy to make the environment

more explicit in order to handle the difficulties with implicit social interactions. They predicted

that implicit learning would be strongly negatively correlated with social communication

processes and modestly negatively correlated with repetitive behaviors. Using two implicit

learning tasks, they found the predicted pattern in a sample of 50 children with high-functioning

ASD and 50 children with typical development aged 5-17. These relationships among autism

symptoms and learning style provide support for the idea that the behaviors seen in ASD are due

to underlying difficulties with information processing.

Central Coherence and Implicit Learning theories are not independent of attention.

Indeed, according to the Implicit Learning theory, individuals with ASD may focus on certain

aspects of information in the environment to the exclusion of other important information. In an

eye-tracking study examining the prototype effect using cartoon animals, children with ASD had

fewer fixations within the areas of interest than children with typical development, which

indicated that they were not attending to the salient information in the task and thus showed

impairments in prototype learning (Klein, 2006).

This review of research on attention orienting and cognitive processing provides a

necessary framework for a consideration of auditory and visual perception. When taking these

areas of research into account, the emerging picture suggests that perception may be enhanced in

individuals with autism due to fundamental differences in brain processes that determine what

information is important to attend to and how that information is interpreted in context.

Page 17: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

9

Enhanced Perception

There are two closely-related theories that address the cause of sensory differences in

autism, one put forth by Plaisted, O’Riordan, and Baron-Cohen (1998), and the Enhanced

Perceptual Functioning theory put forth by Mottron and Burack (2001). Plaisted’s theory is

limited to visual processing and will thus be subsumed under the Enhanced Perceptual

Functioning theory, which encompasses both visual and auditory processing, for the purposes of

this review. According to this theory, individuals with ASD show enhanced discrimination of

perceptual features, and processing of these low-level features interferes with higher order

processing. One example given by Mottron and Burack is that of a child who stares at a fan for

hours; in this case, the lower-order perception of movement disrupts the child’s ability to explore

the environment.

Plaisted and colleagues (1998) propose that individuals with ASD have enhanced

perception for certain perceptual features. They suggest that the features that are unique to a

given stimulus are processed well by individuals with ASD, while the features held in common

between stimuli are processed poorly. This enhanced perception for novel features, but not

shared features, may underlie the reduced ability to generalize learned information that is seen in

autism. The sequence of enhanced perception occurs when there is a deficit in a cognitive

function that is followed by compensatory mechanisms (i.e., neural plasticity) and overtraining

of those mechanisms due to increased demand (Mottron & Burack, 2001). While compensatory

mechanisms are usually adaptive (for example, in the case of a person with visual impairments

who displays increased sensitivity to sounds and odors), these mechanisms may become

maladaptive if they develop beyond the optimal level. For example, increased sensitivity to

Page 18: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

10

sound in an individual with ASD may lead to an inability to tolerate loud noises or particular

noises such that the individual is not able to function effectively in his or her environment.

Enhanced auditory perception. Across several studies, both children and adults with ASD

have been reported to show enhanced auditory perception as measured by the ability to detect

differences in pitch and the direction of pitch changes (Heaton, Williams, Cummins, & Happé,

2008; Heaton, 2005). Bonnel and colleagues (2003) tested pitch sensitivity using two tasks in a

sample of adolescents with high-functioning autism (n=12) compared to children with typical

development (n=12) matched on chronological age and IQ. In the pitch sensitivity tasks,

participants heard two tones, some of which were identical and some of which varied by 1%

(hard), 2% (medium), or 3% (easy) of the first tone. In the first task, participants were asked to

make same/different discriminations, and in the second task, they were asked to make high/low

discriminations. Adolescents with high-functioning autism showed higher sensitivity than the

comparison sample for pitch discrimination in all three conditions of the categorization task and

in the easy and medium conditions of the discrimination task.

Heaton (2005) examined the ability to identify pitch direction in a musical scale (i.e.,

whether a short sequence of notes was ascending or descending) and the ability to identify

changes in a melody in 7-14 year old children with high-functioning autism (n=15) compared to

two groups of children with typical development, one group matched on verbal IQ and age and

one group matched on nonverbal IQ and age (two children in the comparison sample attended

specialist schools due to moderate learning difficulties and the remaining children attended

mainstream schools). In the pitch direction task, the musical scales were manipulated to have

small, medium, and large pitch intervals, where small pitch intervals contain notes with minute

changes in pitch (1-4 semitones) and are thus more difficult to discriminate than large pitch

Page 19: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

11

intervals, where the notes were further apart (9-12 semitones). On the pitch direction task, there

was a significant interaction between diagnosis and interval type for small pitch intervals but not

medium and large pitch intervals, indicating that children with ASD were more sensitive at

detecting minute changes in pitch. In the melody processing task, participants were presented

with pairs of short melodies with three conditions: same, different with a maintaining change

(i.e., the new tone was consistent with the melody), or different with a violating change (i.e., the

new tone was discordant with the melody). There were no significant differences between groups

on this task, suggesting that superior pitch discrimination does not necessarily lead to superior

musical ability.

Heaton and colleagues (2008) sought to replicate their findings in a sample of children

with low- and high-functioning autism. They examined the ability to identify pitch direction and

absolute pitch in 11-19 year old children with ASD (n=32; nonverbal IQs ranging from 55 to

108) compared to a sample of children with typical development (n=35) matched on

chronological age and IQ (approximately half of the children in the comparison sample attended

specialist schools due to moderate learning difficulties and the remaining children attended

mainstream schools). Participants completed two auditory tasks that involved a visual display of

a person moving up and down a set of stairs that corresponded with the presentation of the tones.

For the pitch direction task, participants first completed practice trials where they were given

reference points for the tones, then they heard ascending or descending pitch intervals and were

asked to touch the step that corresponded to the tones they heard. Pitch memory was tested one

week later to assess for absolute pitch (i.e., the ability to identify pitches without reference to a

musical standard). Participants were not reminded of the reference points before being presented

with six sets of tones, and they were then asked to touch the corresponding steps. Across both

Page 20: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

12

tasks, only a subset of children with ASD showed enhanced perception; 3 out of 33 children with

ASD performed 4 to 5 standard deviations above the mean on both tasks. Enhanced perception

was not related to IQ; low-functioning children with ASD performed as well as those without

intellectual impairment. The authors suggest that this non-replication of findings may be due to

increased task demands because of the combination of visual and auditory stimuli.

Enhanced visual perception. Several studies have demonstrated that individuals with

ASD have enhanced visual perception as measured by the ability to discriminate changes in

visual stimuli and to detect novel targets in visual arrays (Bertone, Mottron, Jelenic, & Faubert,

2005; Leader, Loughnane, McMoreland, & Reed, 2008; O'Riordan & Plaisted, 2001; Plaisted,

O'Riordan, & Baron-Cohen, 1998). Bertone et al. (2005) examined perception of luminance in a

sample of children and adults (aged 11-31 years) with high-functioning autism (n=13) or typical

development (n=13), with groups matched on age and IQ. Luminance (which requires simple

visual information-processing) and texture (which requires more complex processing) were

manipulated by decreasing the contrast of a line pattern against a grey background. Participants

were asked to judge the orientation of the lines (i.e., horizontal or vertical), which were more

difficult to perceive in low-contrast conditions. Participants with ASD demonstrated enhanced

perception for luminance-defined stimuli, but inferior perception for texture-defined stimuli,

which require more complex neural networks in the visual pathway. Bertone and colleagues

suggest that these findings indicate that sub-cortical perceptual processing is intact in ASD but

low-level perceptual information-processing is altered due to atypical neural connectivity.

Leader and colleagues (2008) reported two experiments where they examined color

discrimination in a sample of low-functioning children and adults with ASD. In the first

experiment, 16 children with low-functioning autism (with a mean chronological age of 12:2

Page 21: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

13

years and a mean mental age of 8:2 years) were matched on mental age with 16 children with

typical development (with a mean chronological age of 7:11). The saturation of a color was

manipulated to produce stimuli with lower levels of color intensity, and participants were

exposed to either an equal salience condition (i.e., both stimuli had the same level of intensity) or

an unequal salience condition (i.e., some stimuli had 25% lower color intensity). Participants

were reinforced for choosing the “correct” stimuli (i.e., the one that matched the training card),

and their most and least selected responses were analyzed to determine if their performance

differed across the equal and unequal saliency conditions. The group with ASD, but not the

group with typical development, showed an effect in the unequal saliency condition, indicating

that the group with ASD was more sensitive to changes in color saturation than the comparison

group. In the second experiment, these findings were replicated in a sample of 15 adults with

low-functioning autism (with a mean chronological age of 18:6 and a mean mental age of 4:2

years), who were matched on mental age with 15 children with typical development (with a

mean chronological age of 5:0 years).

Plaisted and colleagues (1998) examined featural discrimination of novel and familiar

patterns of stimuli in high-functioning adults with ASD (n=8) compared to a sample of age- and

IQ-matched adults with typical development (n=10). The data from the control group indicated a

perceptual learning effect in that they were more accurate at discriminating familiar stimuli (i.e.,

stimuli that had been displayed in the pre-exposed condition) than novel stimuli. Although the

ASD group did not show the perceptual learning effect as their performance was similar across

both familiar and novel conditions, adults with ASD were significantly more accurate than the

comparison group at detecting novel stimuli. In a later study, O’Riordan and Plaisted (2001)

examined featural discrimination using a target detection task with 6-11 year old children with

Page 22: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

14

ASD (n=13) compared to children with typical development (n=13) matched on age and

nonverbal ability. In this study, they manipulated the similarity of the distractor stimuli to the

target stimuli by changing the color or form of the distractor stimuli across four conditions (e.g.,

in the most difficult condition, participants searched for a red “F” target hidden among pink “F”

and red “E” distractors). There was a significant interaction between group and condition, such

that the superiority of the performance of the ASD group increased as target-distractor similarity

increased. Thus, children and adults with ASD showed an enhanced ability to discriminate

among visual features.

These studies provide some support for the Enhanced Perceptual Functioning theory. The

tasks used to study visual perception involved manipulating the contrast of luminance vs. texture,

examining color saturation in equal vs. unequal saliency conditions, and examining response

patterns for novel vs. familiar stimuli and similar vs. dissimilar targets. The response type and

task demands differed greatly among these studies, making it difficult to draw firm conclusions

about the perceptual abilities of individuals with ASD. Most of the auditory perception tasks

looked at discrimination of auditory stimuli along with a visual display, which could be a

confound given hypothesized difficulties with multimodal processing (for a review, see Iarocci

& McDonald, 2006). For example, a recent study of audiovisual integration in 7-16 year old

children with high-functioning autism using the McGurk effect (where incongruent speech

sounds and visual speech stimuli are presented simultaneously) found that younger children with

ASD showed delayed visual accuracy and audiovisual integration compared to children with

typical development (Taylor, Isaac, & Milne, 2010). Therefore, a study that examines the

auditory and visual modalities in isolation from other sensory modalities and also uses simple

task demands is necessary to test the Enhanced Perceptual Functioning theory. Furthermore,

Page 23: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

15

there have not been any studies that examined the relationship between visual and auditory

domains on comparable tasks in the same group of individuals. There have also not been any

studies that looked at the relationship between enhanced perception and autism symptomatology

other than IQ.

The current study sought to fill these gaps in the literature by examining the performance

of children with ASD on a visual and an auditory same/different discrimination task and by

correlating their perceptual abilities to autism severity. There are two hypotheses for this study:

(a) individuals with ASD will show increased sensitivity on both visual and auditory perception

tasks indicated by a higher d-prime (d’) based on the findings of similar research, and (b)

increased perceptual sensitivity will be positively correlated with autism symptomatology. This

study will provide more information about perceptual processing and sensory patterns that

potentially contribute to the development of autism symptoms.

Page 24: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

16

Method

Participants

A total of 13 children with a previous diagnosis of ASD and 13 children with typical

development participated in the study. Participants were required to meet the following inclusion

criteria: 1) age 8-12 years inclusive; 2) brief Full Scale IQ greater than 70; 3) less than one year

of formal musical training; 4) absence of acute medical or genetic condition (according to parent

report); 5) normal hearing levels (at least 20 decibels (dB)) on a screening test, and 6) normal

visual acuity (corrected to 20/20) and normal color vision on a screening test (see below for more

details). In addition, participants with ASD were required to meet criteria on the Autism

Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) and/or the Autism Diagnostic Interview (ADI-R).

Participants with typical development were excluded based on the following criteria: 1) a score

above the ASD cutoff on the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS); 2) a history of psychiatric or

developmental disorder; 3) currently taking any psychotropic medication; and 4) an immediate

family member with an ASD diagnosis. In the ASD group, nine additional children were

recruited and excluded. Four were excluded because they did not meet criteria for a current

diagnosis on the ADOS, two were excluded because their IQ was below the cutoff, and three

were excluded because they did not complete one or both perception tasks (one failed the hearing

screening, one failed the vision screening, and two did not meet criteria on the visual task

practice trials). In the control group, six additional children were recruited and excluded; two

exceeded the cutoff score on the SRS, one child had a family member with ASD, and three did

Page 25: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

17

not complete one or both perception tasks (two failed the hearing screening, one failed the vision

screening, and two did not meet criteria on the visual task practice trials). Thus, the final sample

was composed of 13 children with ASD and 13 children with typical development.

Participants with typical development were recruited through local elementary schools.

Participants with ASD were recruited from local autism service agencies, including after school

and summer programs, and the University of Alabama (UA) ASD Research Registry. The

Registry consists of individuals seen in the UA ASD Clinic who indicated an interest in research

and received a DSM-IV diagnosis of ASD by a licensed clinician experienced in the assessment

and diagnosis of autism.

Apparatus

The tasks were administered using an Elo 12.1” touchscreen and a Dell Inspiron 6400

laptop with Windows Vista. The laptop was set to high resolution (1024 x 768 pixels) and Vista

Basic 32 bit color scheme. Participants were seated in a stationary chair at a viewing distance of

approximately 20 inches from the touchscreen. Sony noise-cancelling headphones (MDR-NC7)

were used for the auditory task, and the volume was set to level 10 according to the laptop’s

sound control setting. The tasks were programmed using Inquisit version 2.0. The visual stimuli

were created using Adobe Photoshop Elements version 7.0, and the auditory stimuli were created

using Praat version 5.1.09 (created by Paul Boersma and David Weenink at the University of

Amsterdam). Prior to beginning the study, these tasks were piloted with individuals with typical

development until satisfactory task manipulations were achieved.

Stimuli

The visual stimuli consisted of geometric shapes that differed on the dimensions of

luminance and hue. Three geometric shapes of a primary color were created using numerical

Page 26: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

18

red/green/blue (RGB) color values: a green circle (0.255.0), a red hexagon (255.0.0), and a blue

square (0.0.255). For each dimension, the stimuli were created to produce easy, medium and

hard conditions by manipulating the RGB value. The dimension of luminance was systematically

manipulated by changing the RGB value for that color to differ from the first stimulus by 40

units in the easy condition, 30 units in the medium condition, and 10 units in the hard condition

(either lower or higher). For example, a pair of red hexagons in the luminance-hard condition

might have the RGB values of 215.0.0 and 205.0.0. To create incremental changes in hue, the

RGB values for a different color were manipulated by 50 (easy), 40 (medium), and 30 (hard)

units. For example, a pair of blue squares in the hue-easy condition might have the RGB values

of 0.0.255 and 0.50.255 (shifting towards a green hue). Each stimulus was presented individually

for 170 milliseconds (ms) with an interstimulus interval of 750 ms. The intertrial interval varied

because the experimenter controlled the trial presentation (to ensure the participant was attending

to the touchscreen prior to each trial). Prior to each trial, a crosshair was presented in the center

of the screen. There were 24 trials for each condition (i.e., luminance-same, luminance-easy,

luminance-medium, luminance-hard, hue-same, hue-easy, hue-medium, and hue-hard), with 8

trials of each shape, yielding 192 total trials. There were four experimental blocks in which

luminance and color trials were intermixed, and the order of trials within each block was

randomized.

The auditory stimuli consisted of pure tones that differed on the dimensions of pitch and

volume. The stimuli that were manipulated consisted of 500 hertz (Hz), 750 Hz, 1000 Hz, 1250

Hz, and 1500 Hz tones. Pitch (Hz) was manipulated in increments of 3% (easy), 2% (medium),

and 1% (hard) of the first stimulus presented. Volume (dB) was manipulated in increments of

40% (easy), 25% (medium), and 10% (hard) of the first stimulus. Each stimulus was presented

Page 27: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

19

individually for 100 ms with an interstimulus interval of 1000 ms. There were 24 trials for each

condition (i.e., pitch-same, pitch-easy, pitch-medium, pitch-hard, volume-same, volume-easy,

volume-medium, and volume-hard), yielding 192 total trials.

Procedure

Potential participants completed a brief screening call based on the inclusion/ exclusion

criteria. Participants and their parents were told that the purpose of the study was to evaluate the

perceptual abilities of children with ASD compared to children with typical development and to

see how this might relate to ASD symptoms. Participants were tested in a quiet room free from

distractions. The consent and assent forms were reviewed with the participant and their parent(s),

and they were given an opportunity to ask questions. The parent was asked to complete autism

rating scales while the participant completed the assessments. Upon completion of the testing

session, participants received $15, and parents were mailed a brief report. This study was

approved by the University of Alabama Institutional Review Board.

Participants were asked to complete visual and auditory discrimination tasks to assess

their perceptual sensitivity. The order of these two tasks was counterbalanced between

participants. For both tasks, participants were presented with two successive stimuli and asked to

indicate if they were the same or different by pressing the word on the touchscreen. Participants

received the following instructions for the visual discrimination task: “You will see some shapes,

and your job is to decide if the shapes are the same color or if they’re different shades of a color.

Some of the shapes might look the same, but they may be just a little bit different. If you think

they are the same, press the ‘same’ button, and if you think they are different, press the

‘different’ button.” The instructions for the auditory discrimination task were very similar: “You

will hear some sounds, and your job is to decide if the two sounds are the same or different. The

Page 28: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

20

second sound may be a little higher or lower, or a little louder or softer. Some of the sounds

might sound like they’re the same, but they may be just a little bit different. If you think they are

the same, press the ‘same’ button, and if you think they are different, press the ‘different’

button.”

Participants completed two sets of practice blocks and teaching items before beginning

the experimental blocks. The first practice block consisted of five trials of the same condition

and five trials of the luminance-easy and hue-easy conditions. Participants repeated this practice

block until they reached 70% accuracy; the task was discontinued if they did not reach this

criterion after four attempts. All included participants met these criteria. Following this practice

block, there were two teaching trials in which the visual stimuli were presented simultaneously

for the hue-medium and luminance-hard conditions, and the participants were told that the

stimuli were different. The second practice block consisted of four trials of the same condition

and five trials randomly selected from the easy, medium, and hard conditions for both

dimensions; there was no criterion for this practice block. The purpose of this practice block was

to teach the children that some of the comparisons would be difficult and to give feedback prior

to starting experimental trials. Participants did not receive feedback during the experimental

trials. None of the stimuli used for the practice blocks were identical to the stimuli in the

experimental blocks, although they were manipulated in the same way (e.g., the hue was shifted

by the same amount but towards a different color). For the trials in which the stimuli were

different, they differed only on the dimension that was being manipulated.

Similar to the visual discrimination task, participants completed two practice blocks and a

set of teaching items before beginning the experimental blocks. For the auditory task, the first

practice block consisted of five trials of the same condition and five trials of the pitch-easy and

Page 29: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

21

volume-easy conditions. Following this practice block, there were four teaching trials in which

the auditory stimuli were presented without an interstimulus interval for the medium and hard

conditions for both pitch and volume, and the participants were told that the stimuli were

different. The second practice block consisted of four trials of the same condition and four trials

randomly selected from the easy, medium, and hard conditions for both dimensions. In all other

ways, the auditory task was similar to the visual task (i.e., same criterion for the first practice

block, four experimental blocks with randomized pitch and volume trials and no feedback during

experimental blocks). The order of these two tasks was counterbalanced between participants.

Measures

Hearing and vision screening. Hearing and vision tests were administered to screen for

impairments. Participants were required to hear at the threshold of 20 dB at the frequencies of

500, 750, 1000, and 1250 Hz using a portable audiometer (testing was conducted in a quiet but

not sound-proof room). For visual testing, participants were required to read at a 20/20 level of

acuity using a standard eye wall chart and to pass the Ishihara Test for Color Blindness (i.e.,

correctly identify the numbers in six color plates).

Cognitive testing. A brief full scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ) score was obtained using the

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999). The WASI has been

normed for ages 4-89. The reliability coefficients for 6-16 year olds for the Verbal IQ,

Performance IQ, and Full scale IQ were .93, .94, and .96, respectively. The WASI demonstrates

good convergent validity with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) and the

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS), with r’s ranging from .76-.92.

Autism symptomatology. Participants with ASD were required to meet criteria on the Autism

Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000) and/or the Autism Diagnostic

Page 30: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

22

Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994). Participants who were recruited

from local service agencies were administered the ADOS by a trained clinician as part of the

current study. If participants had been diagnosed at the UA ASD Clinic and their parents signed

a release of information form, then their clinical records were used to confirm their diagnosis.

The ADOS and ADI-R are considered “gold standard” autism diagnostic assessment instruments.

Internal consistency for these measures was high; item-total correlations ranged from .52-.91.

Furthermore, comparisons of means indicated strong discriminant validity of ASD from non-

spectrum individuals.

The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; Constantino et al., 2004), a caregiver

questionnaire, was used to measure current severity of autistic symptomatology including social,

communication, and repetitive behavior symptoms. This measure has good convergent and

divergent validity; the SRS correlated highly with the ADI-R (r= .70) and was not correlated

with IQ (Constantino et al., 2003). The SRS was normed with a sample of more than 1,600

children aged 4-18 from the general population and separated by identity of rater (parent or

teacher) and gender of child (Constantino et al., 2004). Inter-rater reliability ranged from .75-.91,

and internal consistency was high for parent, teacher, and clinical ratings, ranging from .93-.97

for all groups. Test-retest reliability ranged from .77 to .85 for parent ratings. Regarding

interpretation, the SRS yields standardized scores with a cutoff score for ASD spectrum

disorders. For the SRS total, scores of 59 or below are considered in the normal range, scores of

60 to 75 are considered to be in the mild to moderate range of an ASD, and scores of 76 or

higher indicate an ASD in the severe range.

Two informant-based rating scales were used to measure repetitive behaviors. The

Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R; Bodfish et al., 2000) assesses 43 discrete types of

Page 31: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

23

lower-order and higher-order repetitive behavior within five categories (motor stereotypy,

repetitive self-injury, compulsions, routines/sameness, and restricted interests). The RBS-R has

not been normed, but it was validated with a sample of 307 individuals with pervasive

developmental disorders ranging in age from 3-48 years (Lam & Aman, 2007). Internal

consistency was high for all of the subscales (Cronbach’s alphas ranged from .78 to .91), and

interrater reliability for the subscales ranged from .57 to .73. Construct validity was evaluated

using an exploratory factor analysis that produced a five-factor solution with mean factor

loadings ranging from .51 to .60, and the five factors (subscales) accounted for approximately

47.5% of the variance. Mean item-total correlations for each subscale ranged from .54 to .65.

Higher scores on the RBS-R indicate higher levels of repetitive behaviors.

The Childhood Routines Inventory (CRI; Evans et al., 1997) is a 19-item questionnaire

that was developed to measure repetitive behaviors in preschoolers with typical development.

There are two subscales that measure “just right” behaviors and repetitive behaviors (both

higher-order behaviors), and these subscales were validated using a principal components

analysis. The CRI showed strong internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha of .89) in a sample of

1492 parents with children with typical development aged 8-72 months. Barber and colleagues

(2011) recently replicated this factor structure in older children (aged 6-17) with typical

development and with ASD, indicating this measure has potential for comparing these two

groups. The CRI does not have standardized scores; higher scores indicate higher levels of

repetitive behaviors.

The Sensory Experiences Questionnaire version 2.1 (SEQ; Baranek et al., 2006) was used

to measure sensory processing patterns. This parent questionnaire provides information on

patterns of sensory hyper- and hypo-responsiveness across social and non-social contexts. The

Page 32: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

24

SEQ was normed with a sample of 258 0.5-6.6 year olds and demonstrated good reliability

(internal consistency of .80) and construct validity in differentiating children with autism from

children with developmental delay and typical development. For the SEQ Total, scores in the

range of 33 to 74 are considered typical, score in the range of 75 to 86 are considered at risk for

sensory processing problems, and scores in the range of 87 to 165 indicate deficiencies in

sensory processing.

In addition to these rating scales, parents were also asked to complete a form with

questions gathering demographic information, including the child’s age, sex, ethnicity, current

interventions used (e.g., medications and/or behavioral therapy), and parental education.

Page 33: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

25

Results Participant Characteristics

Table 1

Characteristics of Included Participants

ASD group (n=13) Control group (n=13) Test for group differences

Age in months Mean (SD) 126.69 (18.63) 118.38 (18.29) p=.26 Range 102-153 101-151 Brief FSIQ range 75-142 89-129 Mean (SD) 104.23 (19.41) 116.38 (11.27) p=.06 Verbal IQ range 72-146 102-136 Mean (SD) 103.62 (23.88) 117.00 (9.95) p=.08 Performance IQ range 79-129 79-131 Mean (SD) 103.31 (14.15) 112.23 (14.67) p=.13 Gender 10 males, 3 females

6 males, 7 females Χ2= .11

Race 10 Caucasian, 2 African American, 1

Hispanic

9 Caucasian, 2 African American, 2 more than

one race

Χ2= .59

Paternal Education 5 post-secondary, 7 high school or

below, 1 unknown

4 post-secondary, 8 high school or below, 1

unknown

Χ2= .42

Maternal Education 10 post-secondary, 2 high school or

below, 1 unknown

12 post-secondary, 1 unknown

Χ2= .35

SRS Total 79.33 (10.66) 42.33 (3.55) p<.001** RBS-R Total 22.00 (18.73) 0.33 (0.65) p=.002** CRI Total 42.50 (15.29) 26.83 (6.13) p=.005** SEQ Total 71.33 (12.07) 43.75 (6.15) p<.001**

Note: *p< .05 ** p<.01

Page 34: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

26

Participants who met inclusion criteria were matched on chronological age. The t-tests

revealed no significant differences between groups on age, t=1.15, p=.26, but there was a

marginally significant difference on FSIQ, t=-1.95, p=.06, as well as Verbal and Performance

IQs, with the control group having a higher mean FSIQ (see Table 1). Chi-square tests did not

detect significant group differences on gender, race, or parental education. However, there was

an unequal distribution of genders across groups, with the ASD group having more males than

females compared to the control group, which had slightly more females than males. With the

small sample size, the Chi-square test was not robust enough to reach significance. Given

possible concerns about differences in perception across gender, gender was entered as a

covariate. Thus, FSIQ and gender were entered as covariates in analyses of perceptual

discrimination.

On the parent questionnaires, the ASD group showed the expected group differences with

higher reported levels of symptoms than the control group. There were significant group

differences on all subscales of the SRS in addition to the total score, indicating that the groups

differed on autism-specific measures of social communication and autistic mannerisms (all

p<.001). On the RBS-R and CRI, there were significant group differences on the total and all

subscales (all p<.05). Regarding sensory behaviors, there were significant group differences on

the SEQ total and all subscales (all p<.01).

Statistical Analysis using Signal Detection

The sensitivity index d prime (d’) statistic from signal detection theory has been applied

to data analysis where a stimuli is either present or absent as a means to analyze sensitivity to

differences in perceptual stimuli. To analyze data using d’, the trials were first sorted into hits

(correct identification) and false alarms (false positive), then d’ was calculated as Z(hit rate) –

Page 35: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

27

Z(false alarm rate). D’ is reported in standard deviation units, and provides the separation

between the means of the signal and the noise distribution. Thus, a higher d’ indicates greater

sensitivity to differences in stimuli. Partial eta squared (η2) was used as a measure of effect size,

which can be interpreted as follows: .01 is a small effect, .06 is a medium effect, and .14 is a

large effect (Pierce, Block, & Aguinis, 2004).

Group Differences on Perceptual Discrimination

Table 2

Mean d’ scores for the Auditory Perception Task

Volume Easy

Volume Medium

Volume Hard

Pitch Easy Pitch Medium

Pitch Hard

ASD group Mean (SD)

1.40 (1.04)

1.27 (0.95)

1.26 (0.93)

2.05 (1.47)

1.40 (0.96)

0.66 (0.71)

Control group Mean (SD)

1.38 (1.07)

1.22 (0.86)

0.99 (1.04)

1.17 (1.23)

0.60 (0.95)

0.23 (0.82)

Table 3

Mean d’ scores for the Visual Perception Task

Luminance Easy

Luminance Medium

Luminance Hard

Hue Easy Hue Medium

Hue Hard

ASD group Mean (SD)

2.04 (0.79)

1.47 (0.67)

0.32 (0.42)

1.13 (0.64)

0.98 (0.53)

0.74 (0.55)

Control group Mean (SD )

1.89 (0.62)

1.41 (0.73)

0.57 (0.35)

1.32 (0.38)

1.19 (0.49)

0.85 (0.41)

Perceptual discrimination. In order to compare performance on auditory and visual

discrimination tasks, a 2 (Diagnosis: ASD, typical development) x 2 (Modality: Auditory,

Visual) x 3 (Difficulty: Easy, Medium, Hard) x 4 (Dimension: Pitch, Volume, Luminance, Hue)

repeated measures ANCOVA using d’ was conducted with gender and FSIQ entered as

covariates. See Tables 2 and 3 for d’ values for each group by modality and dimension. Gender

Page 36: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

28

was not a significant covariate, F(1,22)=1.16, p=.29, η2=.05, and FSIQ was a marginally

significant covariate, F(1,22)=3.49, p=.08, η2=.14. There was a significant four-way interaction

for Modality by Dimension by Difficulty by Diagnosis, F(1,22)=11.65, p=.002, η2=.35, which

suggests that diagnosis is different across all of these comparisons, however there was not a

significant effect of Diagnosis, F(1,22)=2.55, p=.12, η2=.10. As a result of this four-way

interaction, it is difficult to interpret significant two-way and three-way interactions. All

significant and marginally significant effects of this omnibus ANCOVA with F values greater

than or equal to 2.29 and p values less than or equal to .14 are reported in Table 4. All other

effects resulted in smaller F values and greater p values and are not reported. Thus, the analysis

for comparing auditory and visual perception yielded a significant four-way interaction with a

small to medium effect size, which suggests that there was differential performance for each

diagnostic group across the auditory and visual tasks. To tease apart these significant findings,

separate ANCOVAs were run for the auditory and visual discrimination data.

Page 37: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

29

Table 4

Results of the Omnibus ANCOVA

F η2 p

Diagnosis 2.55 .10 .12

Modality 2.37 .10 .14

Modality by Diagnosis 5.31* .19 .03

Difficulty by FSIQ 4.70* .18 .04

Difficulty by Diagnosis 3.38 .13 .08

Modality by Difficulty 4.12 .16 .06

Modality by Dimension by Diagnosis 5.93* .21 .02

Modality by Difficulty by FSIQ 3.15 .13 .09

Dimension by Difficulty by Gender 3.67 .14 .07

Modality by Dimension by Difficulty by FSIQ

2.29 .09 .14

Modality by Dimension by Difficulty by Diagnosis

11.65** .35 .002

Note: *p< .05 ** p<.01, df (1,22) for all F values

Auditory perception. In order to examine differences on auditory perception, a 2 (Diagnosis:

ASD, typical development) x 2 (Dimension: Pitch, Volume) x 3 (Difficulty: Easy, Medium,

Hard) repeated measures ANCOVA using d’ was conducted with gender and FSIQ entered as

covariates. Gender was not a significant covariate, F(1,22)=1.49, p=.24, η2=.06, and FSIQ was a

marginally significant covariate, F(1,22)=3.38, p=.08, η2=.13. There was a significant main

effect for Diagnosis, F(1,22)=4.28, p=.05, η2=.16. Across all six conditions in the Auditory task,

participants with ASD had a higher d’ than participants with typical development. This main

Page 38: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

30

effect was qualified by two significant interactions. There was a significant two-way interaction

for Difficulty by FSIQ, F(1,22)=5.93, p=.02, η2=.21, with participants across both groups with

higher IQs showing higher sensitivity in easy > medium > hard conditions. There was also a

significant three-way interaction for Dimension by Difficulty by Diagnosis, F(1,22)=8.55,

p=.008, η2=.28, which suggests that participants with ASD were significantly better than

participants with typical development at identifying differences in pitch compared to volume,

particularly for easier conditions. All significant and marginally significant effects of this

ANCOVA with F values greater than or equal to 2.33 and p values less than or equal to .14 are

reported in Table 5. Separate ANCOVAs for pitch and volume were run, and these follow-up

analyses confirmed that there were significant effects for pitch, but not for volume. When

examining sensitivity to pitch, FSIQ was a marginally significant covariate, F(1,22)=3.98, p=.06,

η2=.15. There was a significant main effect for Diagnosis, F(1,22)=6.73, p=.02, η2=.23, as well

as significant two-way interactions for Difficulty by Diagnosis, F(1,22)=5.89, p=.02, η2=.21, and

for Difficulty by FSIQ, F(1,22)=4.83, p=.04, η2=.18, when analyzing pitch. For volume, there

was a trend towards significance for the two-way interaction, Difficulty by FSIQ, F(1,22)=2.61,

p=.12, η2=.11. All other effects resulted in smaller F values and greater p values and are not

reported.

Overall, analysis of the auditory discrimination task showed patterns among the data

wherein individuals with ASD showed superior perception for pitch, particularly for easier

conditions (i.e., in the pitch easy condition, the d’ of the ASD group was nearly double the d’ of

the control group, 2.05 vs. 1.17, respectively). There was a small to medium effect size for this

interaction. This finding is consistent with the hypothesis for pitch but not volume, indicating

that individuals with ASD do not have superior perception across all domains. In order to

Page 39: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

31

determine whether there were subgroups of participants who showed a pattern of hyposensitivity,

Levene’s test of the equality of variances was run for the four dimensions of the auditory and

visual discriminations tasks. Levene’s test was not significant for any of the dimensions:

Volume, F(1,12)=.04, p=.84, Pitch, F(1,12)=.02, p=.89, Color, F(1,12)=.1.95, p=.18, or

Luminance, F(1,12)=.99, p=.33. This indicates that there were not subgroups of participants

who showed hyposensitivity on the perceptual tasks.

Table 5

Results of the Auditory ANCOVA

F η2 p

Diagnosis 4.28* .16 .05

Difficulty 2.33 .10 .14

Difficulty by FSIQ 5.93* .21 .02

Difficulty by Diagnosis 2.49 .10 .13

Dimension by Diagnosis 2.70 .11 .11

Dimension by Difficulty by Gender 2.33 .10 .14

Dimension by Difficulty by Diagnosis

8.55** .28 .008

Note: *p< .05 ** p<.01, df (1,22) for all F values

Visual perception. A 2 (Diagnosis: ASD, typical development) x 2 (Dimension: Luminance,

Hue) x 3 (Difficulty: Easy, Medium, Hard) repeated measures ANCOVA was conducted to

examine differences on visual perception using d’ with gender and FSIQ entered as covariates.

Neither gender nor FSIQ were significant covariates (Gender: F(1,22)=0.19, p=.67, η2=.009;

FSIQ: F(1,22)=1.63, p=.22, η2=.07). There was not a significant effect for Diagnosis,

Page 40: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

32

F(1,22)=0.02, p=.89, η2=.001. Contrary to the hypothesis, analysis of the visual discrimination

task yielded only a three-way interaction for Dimension by Difficulty by Diagnosis that trended

toward significance, F(1,22)=3.05, p=.10, η2=.12. Separate ANCOVAs for luminance and hue

were run, and these follow-up analyses yielded no significant main effects or interactions. All

other effects resulted in p values of greater than .14 and are not reported.

Factor Analysis

An exploratory factor analysis was conducted to examine underlying patterns of

performance across perceptual tasks. The 12 measures of d’ were entered into the factor analysis

(i.e., luminance-easy, luminance-medium, luminance-hard, hue-easy, hue-medium, hue-hard,

pitch-easy, pitch-medium, pitch-hard, volume-easy, volume-medium, and volume-hard). This

yielded a three-factor model; see Table 6 for the loadings from this factor analysis. As can be

seen, the first factor contains high positive loadings for all variables, so this factor seems to

represent overall perceptual sensitivity for auditory and visual stimuli. The second factor

contains high positive loadings for visual sensitivity and negative loadings for auditory

sensitivity. This second factor seems to represent modality-specific sensitivity (i.e., those who

are more sensitive to discriminating either auditory or visual stimuli). The third factor contains

high positive loadings for pitch and hue and negative loadings for volume and luminance. Pitch

and hue are manipulated by changing the frequency of a stimulus, while volume and luminance

are manipulated by changing the intensity of a stimulus. Thus, this third factor seems to represent

stimulus-specific sensitivity (i.e., those who are more sensitive to discriminating either the

frequency or intensity of stimuli, regardless of whether they are auditory or visual stimuli).

Page 41: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

33

Table 6

Factor Scores

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3

Volume Easy 0.86 -0.12 -0.42

Volume Medium 0.87 -0.10 -0.43

Volume Hard 0.76 -0.14 -0.58

Pitch Easy 0.82 -0.44 0.06

Pitch Medium 0.78 -0.43 0.19

Pitch Hard 0.68 -0.52 0.29

Hue Easy 0.84 0.26 0.22

Hue Medium 0.80 0.05 0.38

Hue Hard 0.69 0.27 0.54

Luminance Easy 0.58 0.68 0.06

Luminance Medium 0.62 0.62 -0.16

Luminance Hard 0.16 0.80 -0.08

Based on this factor analysis, three scores were computed to create d’ scores that

paralleled each factor: 1) high overall sensitivity factor score (created by calculating the average

of all 12 measures of d’), 2) high auditory sensitivity factor score (created by subtracting the

average of the hue and luminance d’ scores from the average of the volume and pitch d’ scores),

and 3) high frequency sensitivity factor score (created by subtracting the average of the volume

and luminance d’ scores from the average of the pitch and hue d’ scores). Thus, a high overall

sensitivity score indicates high sensitivity across auditory and visual domains, a high auditory

Page 42: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

34

sensitivity score indicates higher sensitivity to auditory than visual stimuli, and a high frequency

sensitivity score indicates higher sensitivity to the frequency than the intensity of a stimulus.

These sensitivity factors were then correlated with the measures of autism symptomatology.

Relations between Perceptual Discrimination and ASD Symptoms

Although this study had a small sample size, exploratory correlations were run to

examine the relations among perception and parent-report measures of autism symptomatology.

Two-tailed Pearson partial correlations were run separately for the ASD group and the control

group with FSIQ as a covariate. According to Cohen (1988), a correlation is described as small if

r=.1-.3, medium if r=.3-.5, and large if r=.5-1.0. See Table 7 for correlations in the ASD group

and Table 8 for correlations in the Control group.

Table 7 Relations among Perception, Intellectual Ability, and Autism Measures in the ASD Group Controlling for FSIQ

Measure Overall Sensitivity

High Auditory

High Frequency

SEQ Total

RBS-R Total

SRS Total

High Auditory

.56 p=.07

-- -- -- -- --

High Frequency

.06 p=.85

.26 p=.44

-- -- -- --

SEQ Total .72* p=.01

.02 p=.96

-.14 p=.67

-- -- --

RBS-R Total .50 p=.12

.01 p=.98

-.33 p=.32

.81** p=.003

-- --

SRS Total .58 p=.06

.57 p=.07

.29 p=.39

.51 p=.11

.38 p=.25

--

SRS Motivation

.64* p=.03

.47 p=.15

.14 p=.68

.71* p=.02

.67* p=.03

.90** p<.001

Note: *p< .05 ** p<.01

Page 43: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

35

In the ASD group, the SEQ total was significantly correlated with high overall

sensitivity, as would be expected. This finding supports the notion that parent ratings of overt

sensory behaviors are related to the child’s perceptual abilities and lends validity to the SEQ for

children with ASD. Moreover, the SEQ total was significantly correlated with the RBS-R total,

supporting the overlap between sensory symptoms and repetitive behaviors. In general, high

levels of overall perceptual sensitivity were related to higher levels of autism symptomatology

on the measure of repetitive behavior (RBS-R) and the measure of overall symptomatology

(SRS). Because the SRS contains measures of both repetitive behaviors and social skills,

correlations were run with the subscales of the SRS to see whether both repetitive behaviors and

social skills were related to perceptual sensitivity. All scales yielded a positive correlation

between symptoms and perceptual sensitivity (all r’s > .33) with the only significant correlation

between overall sensitivity and the Motivation subscale.

Differential sensitivity to auditory rather than visual stimuli was less clearly related to

autism symptomatology. Differential sensitivity to auditory stimuli was not related to sensory

behavior on the SEQ or repetitive behavior on the RBS-R. However, the SRS Total and

Motivation subscale were positively related to differential auditory sensitivity with greater levels

of auditory sensitivity related to increased autism symptoms. No pattern of relations between

high frequency sensitivity and autism symptoms was found.

Page 44: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

36

Table 8 Relations among Perception, Intellectual Ability, and Autism Measures in the Control Group Controlling for FSIQ Measure Overall

Sensitivity High

Auditory High

Frequency SEQ Total

RBS-R Total

SRS Total

High Auditory

.81** p=.002

-- -- -- -- --

High Frequency

.06 p=.87

.25 p=.46

-- -- -- --

SEQ Total -.04 p=.92

-.19 p=.58

-.32 p=.34

-- -- --

RBS-R Total -.50 p=.12

-.19 p=.57

-.22 p=.52

.37 p=.27

-- --

SRS Total .14 p=.68

-.05 p=.88

-.84** p=.001

.25 p=.46

.15 p=.65

--

SRS Motivation

.44 p=.18

.11 p=.74

-.53 p=.10

.28 p=.40

-.21 p=.53

.76** p=.007

Note: *p< .05 ** p<.01

In the group with typical development, autism symptoms across all measures were

negatively correlated with the high frequency sensitivity score, although the only significant

correlation was between the SRS total score and high frequency sensitivity (the negative

correlation indicates sensitivity to intensity rather than frequency). This pattern indicates that

among children with typical development, those who show higher levels of autism-like

symptoms show higher sensitivity to the intensity of sensory stimuli in their environment (i.e.,

volume and luminance).

Page 45: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

37

Discussion

Enhanced Perceptual Functioning Theory

Regarding the hypothesis that children with ASD would show enhanced perception in

auditory and visual domains, the ASD group did show higher sensitivity than the control group

for pitch but not for volume, hue, or luminance. Although this was a relatively small sample,

there were small to medium effect sizes for the analyses, indicating that the sample size was

sufficient to detect group differences. Groups were matched on chronological age, but due to

slight differences between the groups on FSIQ and gender, these variables were controlled for in

the analyses to make the groups more similar. The ASD group had significantly higher scores

than the control group on all of the parent-report measures of autism symptomatology, as

expected.

This study provides some support for the Enhanced Perceptual Functioning Theory. In

addition to replicating the findings of Bonnel et al. (2003), who found enhanced discrimination

for pitch in a sample of adolescents with ASD, this study demonstrated that superior pitch

discrimination occurs in younger children than had previously been studied. These findings

support the notion that enhanced perception for pitch may be a phenotype of ASD rather than a

splinter skill demonstrated by a small subset of individuals with ASD. Although Heaton et al.

(2008) only found enhanced pitch perception in a subset of their sample, there may be two

reasons for these discrepant findings: 1) to assess perception of pitch direction, there was a visual

display which may have involved cross-modal sensory processing, and 2) they tested for absolute

Page 46: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

38

pitch, which is an extremely rare ability that is estimated to occur in only about 0.01% of the

population (Takeuchi & Hulse, 1993).

Discrimination for changes in volume had not been previously studied in ASD, and the

lack of group differences on this dimension suggests that the individuals with ASD do not have

broad enhanced auditory perception but that the ability to detect changes in pitch is a unique

ability in individuals with ASD. This could be due to differences in the way pitch is processed in

the brain due to atypical pathways in the left frontal cortex, as suggested by Gomot et al. (2002).

The attention literature in ASD shows that there are differences in automatic orienting, and

enhanced pitch perception may be due to hyper-sensitivity towards novel sounds. Further

research on the neurobiology of pitch perception is needed to elucidate how the brain of

individuals with ASD processes auditory input.

The lack of significant group differences in the visual domain suggest that the enhanced

visual discrimination that has been reported previously may be due to other visual aspects than

luminance or color saturation (i.e., featural discrimination of novel patterns and target detection).

This non-replication of findings from previous studies may also be due to differences in

methodology. Leader et al. (2008) found enhanced perception for color saturation; however, their

study examined perception in the context of a learning task in a sample of low-functioning

children and adults. Color saturation was consistently manipulated by 25%, which may have

been easier than making same/different discriminations across three levels of difficulty. The

methodology of the present study also differed from that of Bertone et al. (2005), who found

enhanced perception for luminance. Their task involved asking individuals to identify the

orientation of lines on a circle, and individuals may recruit different neural pathways when

looking for a pattern on a stimulus as opposed to judging whether two stimuli are the same or

Page 47: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

39

different. Previous research on enhanced visual perception in individuals with ASD suggests

they may use a different strategy for target detection based on recruitment of different brain

regions.

Relationships among Perceptual Sensitivity and Autism Symptomatology

According to the second hypothesis, perceptual sensitivity would be related to autism

symptomatology. An exploratory factor analysis yielded a three-factor model. The first factor

seemed to represent high overall sensitivity, the second factor seemed to represent modality-

specific sensitivity, and the third factor seemed to represent stimulus-specific sensitivity. Based

on these factors, d’ sensitivity scores were computed and correlated with measures of autism

symptomatology. This is the first study to demonstrate a relationship among perceptual

sensitivity and ASD symptoms. Performance on the perceptual tasks was not correlated with all

measures of autism symptomatology, and some interesting patterns emerged from these

correlations.

As would be expected, there was a relationship between higher perceptual sensitivity and

sensory behaviors as measured by the SEQ in the ASD group. Although not surprising, this

finding indicates that children’s perceptual sensitivity is expressed through overt behaviors that

can be observed and reported by parents, which lends ecological validity to these findings about

perceptual sensitivities in ASD. This finding also supports the validity of the SEQ as it relates to

auditory and visual perception. In the ASD group, there was evidence of a relationship between

overall and auditory sensitivity and autism severity as measured by the SRS, particularly

difficulties with social motivation. Although the direction of the relationship cannot be inferred

from these correlations, one possible explanation for this relationship may be that children with

ASD who are more sensitive to visual and auditory stimuli are reluctant to approach social

Page 48: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

40

situations where there may be overwhelming sights and sounds. Interestingly, a similar pattern

was detected in the group with typical development. There was a relationship between higher

levels of autism-like symptoms and higher sensitivity to the intensity of sensory stimuli in their

environment (i.e., volume and luminance). There was a marginally significant relationship

between this measure of perceptual sensitivity and difficulties with social motivation in the

control group.

A longitudinal study would be necessary to tease apart the directionality of these

relationships. The developmental pathway of enhanced sensitivity is not clearly delineated from

this study. It may be that children with ASD maintain a higher level of sensitivity from very

early childhood, or they may become more sensitive over time. Alternatively, both groups of

children may have a high level of sensitivity and children with typical development may

experience decreased sensitivity over their childhood. For example, research on first language

acquisition (Kuhl, 2009; Werker & Fennell, 2009) shows that infants are able to distinguish

among a wide variety of phonetic sounds and that there is a critical period in which they lose the

ability to distinguish certain phonemes if they are not exposed to them (e.g., Japanese infants

lose the ability to make the distinction between r and l). A similar developmental process may

occur for pitch sensitivity wherein individuals with typical development become less able to

distinguish between different pitch levels because this aspect of auditory information is not

usually relevant to social interactions. In contrast, individuals with ASD may attend less to

social information and maintain a high level of sensitivity to pitch differences.

There was a strong relationship between the SEQ and the RBS-R totals in the ASD

group. This finding is consistent with other studies that have found relationships between sensory

and repetitive behaviors (Boyd, McBee, Holtzclaw, Baranek, & Bodfish, 2009; Gabriels et al.,

Page 49: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

41

2008). Gabriels and colleagues reported correlations between the RBS-R Total and sensory

behaviors as measured by the Sensory Profile, and Boyd and colleagues found correlations

between sensory behaviors as measured by the Sensory Questionnaire and specific repetitive

behaviors as measured by the RBS-R, including Compulsions, Stereotypies, and the Total score.

Further research into the relationship between sensory and repetitive behaviors will be needed to

determine the direction of this relationship and the pattern of correlations among particular

sensory behaviors and subtypes of repetitive behaviors in a larger sample. Future studies should

examine the effect of processing unpredictable or novel information as a potential mediator of

this relationship.

Overall, the findings from this study provide further support for the inclusion of sensory

symptoms as a diagnostic criterion for ASD under the restricted, repetitive behavior category in

the DSM-V. Even with a relatively small sample size, there was strong evidence that children

with ASD demonstrated a superior ability to detect differences in pitch compared to children

with typical development matched on age and controlling for FSIQ and gender, which replicated

and extended previous research. The three-factor model yielded three dimensions of perception:

overall sensitivity, modality-specific sensitivity, and stimulus-specific sensitivity. The pattern of

correlations suggested that overall sensitivity was most related to ASD symptoms, which

supports the Enhanced Perceptual Functioning theory.

Clinical Significance

These results have clinical significance for understanding children with ASD. Enhanced

pitch perception may be adaptive, such as for a person with musical interests (although, as

Heaton (2005) concluded, superior pitch discrimination does not necessarily lead to superior

musical ability). If individuals with ASD perceive sounds in a different way, some sounds may

Page 50: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

42

be experienced as pleasant. However, some sounds may be experienced as painful, such as the

sound of vacuum cleaners, alarms, babies crying, and other sounds that are frequently reported in

clinical accounts as being problematic for individuals with ASD. Hypersensitivity to sounds may

lead to becoming easily overwhelmed in a noisy environment or having difficulty following a

conversation. Given that individuals with ASD do not show overall elevations of sensory

sensitivities, clinicians may need to identify specific sensitivities in a person with ASD in order

to address the individual’s needs. Perceptual sensitivities need to be understood better in order to

adequately accommodate for them in the community as well. For example, caregivers and

teachers taking children with ASD to loud public venues such as malls, movie theaters, and

school gymnasiums, should be aware of the potential negative perceptions of these loud

environments and may want to provide a way to dampen the sound (e.g., headphones) or

intersperse loud environments with quiet rooms for “sensory breaks.”

Limitations and Future Research

Two limitations for this study included the small sample size, particularly for the

correlations, and the cross-sectional nature of the research. Further research studies with larger

sample sizes using a longitudinal design are needed to replicate these findings and to examine

the directionality of the relationship between perception and autism measures. Another limitation

was that the study only included children with high-functioning autism. Enhanced perception has

been tested in children with low-functioning ASD as well, so these findings should be replicated

in individuals with a wider range of cognitive abilities. A developmental study that examined

enhanced perception across a range of ages, including younger children, would also contribute to

the literature by investigating whether perceptual abilities change over time. Studying perception

Page 51: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

43

in infants who are at risk for ASD would allow researchers to determine if perceptual sensitivity

is a “red flag” for ASD.

Future studies could examine other potential correlates of perception, including cognitive

measures such as contextual cueing, category formation, and generalization, in order to

determine if higher-level processing is affected by lower-level perceptual differences in ASD.

Children who show higher perceptual sensitivity may be more aware of details in the

environment, which could affect their style of cognitive processing (e.g., they may use a more

explicit learning strategy for cognitive tasks like category formation because they are more likely

to see stimuli as more distinct than individuals with lower perceptual sensitivity). Further

research on the perceptual features that influence attention orienting is needed to elucidate the

types of stimuli that capture the attention of individuals with ASD. This information may provide

insight into the underlying neural processes that contribute to the ASD phenotype. Research on

cross-modal processing is warranted to determine the nature of the interaction between auditory

and visual perception. Examining perception in family members of individuals with ASD could

potentially contribute to genetic research in ASD.

This study contributed to the literature on perceptual processing and sensory patterns that

may lead to the development of cognitive and behavioral symptoms in ASD. The present study

yielded novel findings about the relationship between perceptual sensitivities and ASD

symptoms, suggesting that repetitive behaviors and social skill difficulties are related to

increased perceptual sensitivity. Enhanced perception may be due to fundamental differences in

brain processes that determine what information is important to attend to and how that

information is interpreted in context. Certainly, it is likely that the ability to perceive particular

stimuli differently may lead to altered patterns of behaviors and social interactions.

Page 52: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

44

References

Akshoomoff, N. A., & Courchesne, E. (1992). A new role for the cerebellum in cognitive operations. Behavioral Neuroscience, 106(5), 731-738. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.106.5.731

Akshoomoff, N. A., & Courchesne, E. (1994). ERP evidence for a shifting attention deficit in patients with damage to the cerebellum. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(4), 388-399. doi:10.1162/jocn.1994.6.4.388

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (Revised 4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Anonymous (2009). My life with Asperger's Syndrome. [Video].

Aram, D. M., & Healy, J. M. (1988). Hyperlexia: A review of extraordinary word recognition. In D. Fein (Ed.), The exceptional brain: Neuropsychology of talent and special abilities. (pp. 70-102). New York, NY US: Guilford Press.

Baranek, G. T., David, F. J., Poe, M. D., Stone, W. L., & Watson, L. R. (2006). Sensory experiences questionnaire: Discriminating sensory features in young children with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 591-601. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01546.x

Belmonte, M. K., Cook, E. H. J., Anderson, G. M., Rubenstein, J. L. R., Greenough, W. T., Beckel-Mitchener, A., et al. (2004). Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: Directions for research and targets for therapy. Molecular Psychiatry, 9(7), 646-663.

Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain : A Journal of Neurology, 128(Pt 10), 2430-2441. doi:10.1093/brain/awh561

Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & Lewis, M. H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: Comparisons to mental retardation. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 237-243.

Bonnel, A., Mottron, L., Peretz, I., Trudel, M., Gallun, E., & Bonnel, A. (2003). Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: A signal detection analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(2), 226-235. doi:10.1162/089892903321208169

Page 53: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

45

Boyd, B. A., McBee, M., Holtzclaw, T., Baranek, G. T., & Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Relationships among repetitive behaviors, sensory features, and executive functions in high functioning autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(4), 959-966. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2009.05.003

Bruckner, C. T., & Yoder, P. (2007). Restricted object use in young children with autism: Definition and construct validity. Autism : The International Journal of Research and Practice, 11(2), 161-171.

Casey, B. J., Gordon, C. T., Mannheim, G. B., & Rumsey, J. M. (1993). Dysfunctional attention in autistic savants. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 15(6), 933-946. doi:10.1080/01688639308402609

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (second ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., et al. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 427-433. doi:10.1023/A:1025014929212

Constantino, J. N., Gruber, C. P., Davis, S., Hayes, S., Passanante, N., & Przybeck, T. (2004). The factor structure of autistic traits. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 719-726. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00266.x

Courchesne, E., Townsend, J., Akshoomoff, N. A., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Lincoln, A. J., et al. (1994). Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108(5), 848-865. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.108.5.848

Dawson, G. (1991). A psychobiological perspective on the early socio-emotional development of children with autism. In D. Cicchetti, S. L. Toth, D. Cicchetti, S. L. Toth (Eds.) , Rochester Symposium on Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 3: Models and integrations (pp. 207-234). Rochester, NY US: University of Rochester Press.

Dawson, G., Meltzoff, A. N., Osterling, J., Rinaldi, J., & Brown, E. (1998). Children with autism fail to orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(6), 479-485. doi:10.1023/A:1026043926488

Evans, D. W., Leckman, J. F., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S., Henshaw, D., King, R. A., et al. (1997). Ritual, habit, and perfectionism: The prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Development, 68(1), 58-68.

First, M. B. (2008, February). The future of psychiatric diagnosis: Refining the research agenda. Paper presented at the Autism and Other Pervasive Developmental Disorders Conference, Sacramento, CA.

Page 54: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

46

Frith, U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Boston: Blackwell Publishing.

Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50(1-3), 115-132.

Gabriels, R. L., Agnewa, J. A., Miller, L. J., Gralla, J., Pan, Z., Goldson, E., et al. (2008). Is there a relationship between restricted, repetitive, stereotyped behaviors and interests and abnormal sensory response in children with autism spectrum disorders? Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(4), 660-670. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2008.02.002

Gomot, M., Giard, M. H., Adrien, J. L., Barthelemy, C., & Bruneau, N. (2002). Hypersensitivity to acoustic change in children with autism: Electrophysiological evidence of left frontal cortex dysfunctioning. Psychophysiology, 39(5), 577-584. doi:10.1017.S0048577202394058

Heaton, P. (2005). Interval and contour processing in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 787-793. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0024-7

Heaton, P., Davis, R. E., & Happé, F. G. (2008). Research note: Exceptional absolute pitch perception for spoken words in an able adult with autism. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 2095-2098. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.02.006

Heaton, P., Williams, K., Cummins, O., & Happé, F. (2008). Autism and pitch processing splinter skills: A group and subgroup analysis. Autism : The International Journal of Research and Practice, 12(2), 203-219.

Hutt, S. J., & Hutt, C. e. (1968). Stereotypy, arousal and autism. Human Development, 11(4), 277-286.

larocci, G., & Burack, J. A. (2004). Intact covert orienting to peripheral cues among children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 257-264. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000029548.84041.69

larocci, G., & McDonald, J. (2006). Sensory integration and the perceptual experience of persons with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 77-90. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0044-3

Iarocci, G., Burack, J. A., Shore, D. I., Mottron, L., & Enns, J. T. (2006). Global-local visual processing in high functioning children with autism: Structural vs. implicit task biases. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 117-129.

Klein, C. L. (2006). The role of attention in implicit learning in ASD. Unpublished master’s thesis, University of Alabama, Tuscaloosa, Alabama, Dept Psychology.

Klinger, L. G., & Dawson, G. (2001). Prototype formation in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 13(1), 111-124. doi:10.1017/S0954579401001080

Page 55: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

47

Klinger, L. G., Klinger, M. R., & Pohlig, R. L. (2007). Implicit learning impairments in autism spectrum disorders. In J. M. Pérez, P. M. Gonzalez, M. L. Comi & C. Nieto (Eds.), New developments in autism: The future is today (pp. 76-103). London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.

Kuhl, P. K. (2009). Linking infant speech perception to language acquisition: Phonetic learning predicts language growth. In J. Colombo, P. McCardle, L. Freund, J. Colombo, P. McCardle, L. Freund (Eds.) ,Infant pathways to language: Methods, models, and research disorders (pp. 213-244). New York, NY US: Psychology Press.

Lam, K. S., & Aman, M. G. (2007). The repetitive behavior scale-revised: Independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 855-866.

Landry, R., & Bryson, S.E. (2004). Impaired disengagement of attention in young children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1115–1122.

Leader, G., Loughnane, A., McMoreland, C., & Reed, P. (2008). The effect of stimulus salience on over-selectivity. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0626-y

Leekam, S. R., López, B., & Moore, C. (2000). Attention and joint attention in preschool children with autism. Developmental Psychology, 36(2), 261-273. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.2.261

Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H. J., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule--generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205-223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947

Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview--revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659-685. doi:10.1007/BF02172145

Mottron, L., & Burack, J. A. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In J. A. Burack, et al. (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research. (pp. 131-148). Mahwah, NJ US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Mottron, L., & Burack, J. (2006). Autism: A different perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 1-3. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0048-z

Barber, A., Klinger, L., O’Kelley, S., Holtzclaw, T., & Klinger, M. (2011). Exploring the relationship between language and repetitive behaviors in school age children with ASD. Unpublished manuscript.

Page 56: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

48

O'Riordan, M., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 54(4), 961-979. doi:10.1080/02724980042000543

Pierce C.A., Block, C.A. & Aguinis, H. (2004). Cautionary note on reporting eta-squared values from multifactor ANOVA designs. Educational and Psychological Measurement 64(6).

Plaisted, K. C., O'Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39(5), 765-775. doi:10.1017/S0021963098002601

Renner, P., Klinger, L. G., & Klinger, M. R. (2006). Exogenous and endogenous attention orienting in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychology, 12(4-5), 361-382. doi:10.1080/09297040600770753

Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2001). A deficit in shifting attention present in high-functioning autism but not Asperger’s disorder. Autism, 5, 67–80.

Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(6), 631-642. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000006000.38991.a7

Senju, A., Tojo, Y., Dairoku, H., & Hasegawa, T. (2004). Reflexive orienting in response to eye gaze and an arrow in children with and without autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 445–458.

Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24(4), 613-620. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1983.tb00137.x

Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34 (8), 1351-1364.

Takeuchi, A. H., & Hulse, S. H. (1993). Absolute pitch. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 345-361. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.345

Taylor, N., Isaac, C., & Milne, E. (2010). A comparison of the development of audiovisual integration in children with autism spectrum disorders and typically developing children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(11), 1403-1411. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1000-4

Townsend, J., Harris, N. S., & Courchesne, E. (1996). Visual attention abnormalities in autism: Delayed orienting to location. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 541–550.

Turner, M. (1997). Executive dysfunction and repetitive behavior. In J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as

an Executive Disorder. (pp. 57-100). Oxford University Press.

Page 57: VISUAL AND AUDITORY SENSITIVITY IN AUTISM SPECTRUM ...acumen.lib.ua.edu/content/u0015/0000001/0000587/u0015_0000001_0000587.pdfIn the field of autism, reports of superior performance

49

Turner, M. (1999). Annotation: Repetitive behaviour in autism: A review of psychological research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 40(6), 839-849.

Wainwright, J. A., & Bryson, S. E. (1996). Visual-spatial orienting in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 423–438.

Wainwright-Sharp, J., & Bryson, S. E. (1993). Visual orienting deficits in high-functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(1), 1-13. doi:10.1007/BF01066415

Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

Wegener, D., Ehn, F., Aurich, M. K., Galashan, F. O., & Kreiter, A. K. (2008). Feature-based attention and the suppression of non-relevant object features. Vision Research, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.08.021

Werker, J. F., & Fennell, C. T. (2009). Infant speech perception and later language acquisition: Methodological underpinnings. In J. Colombo, P. McCardle, L. Freund, J. Colombo, P. McCardle, L. Freund (Eds.) , Infant pathways to language: Methods, models, and research disorders (pp. 85-98). New York, NY US: Psychology Press.