uva-dare (digital academic repository) impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in...

205
UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a pediatric brain tumor: Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention de Ruiter, M.A. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): de Ruiter, M. A. (2016). Impact of a pediatric brain tumor: Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. Download date: 04 Feb 2021

Upload: others

Post on 30-Sep-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

Impact of a pediatric brain tumor: Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocialconsequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention

de Ruiter, M.A.

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):de Ruiter, M. A. (2016). Impact of a pediatric brain tumor: Research into neurocognitive late effects andpsychosocial consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention.

General rightsIt is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s),other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulationsIf you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, statingyour reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Askthe Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam,The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.

Download date: 04 Feb 2021

Page 2: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

UitnodigingVoor het bijwonen van de

openbare verdediging van het proefschrift

Impact of a paediatric brain tumor

Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial

consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention

Door Marieke A. Montgomery-de Ruiter

***

Op vrijdag 9 december om 10.00u in de Agnietenkapel, Oudezijds Voorburgwal 229-231 te Amster-

dam

Aansluitend bent u van harte welkom op de receptie in Frenzi, Zwanenburgwal 232 te Amster-

dam (5 minuten lopen)

***

Marieke Montgomery-deRuiter19e Arundel Gardens

London W11 2LNVerenigd Koningkrijk

+447802588720

ParanimfenLotte Haverman Dominique Bol

[email protected]

Boekenlegger concept 1.indd 1 26-10-16 11:34

Marieke A. Montgomery-de Ruiter

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Impact of a Pediatric

Brain TumorResearch into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences

and the evaluation of a potential intervention

Cover FINAL.indd 1 26-10-16 11:18

Page 3: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a pediatric brain tumor

Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention

Page 4: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

COLOFON

ISBN: 978-94-6233-470-0

Cover: Taco BosLay-out en print: Gildeprint

The printing of this thesis was financially supported by:Academic Medical Center (AMC), Psychosocial Department Emma Children’s Hospital, and the Dutch Cancer Society KWF Kankerbestrijding.

Page 5: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a pediatric brain tumor

Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention

ACADEMISCH PROEFSCHRIFT

ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Universiteit van Amsterdam op gezag van de Rector Magnificus

prof. dr. ir. K.I.J. Maex ten overstaan van een door het College voor Promoties ingestelde commissie,

in het openbaar te verdedigen in de Agnietenkapel op vrijdag 9 december 2016, te 10:00 uur

door Marieke Anna de Ruiter

geboren te Nootdorp

Page 6: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

PROMOTIECOMMISSIE:

Promotores: prof. dr. M.A. Grootenhuis Universiteit van Amsterdam prof. dr. J. Oosterlaan Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam

Copromotores: prof. dr. H.N. Caron Universiteit van Amsterdam dr. A.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren Universiteit van Amsterdam

Overige leden: prof. dr. J.K. Buitelaar Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen prof. dr. G.J.L. Kaspers Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam dr. K.J. Oostrom UvA - Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam prof. dr. B.A. Schmand Universiteit van Amsterdam prof. dr. J.B. van Goudoever Universiteit van Amsterdam prof. dr. B.T. Poll-The Universiteit van Amsterdam

Faculteit der Geneeskunde

Page 7: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Voor mijn ouders

Page 8: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association
Page 9: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Chapter 1 General introduction p. 9

Chapter 2 Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric p. 23 brain tumour and its treatment: a meta-analysis Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2013, 55(5), 408-417

Chapter 3 Neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive p. 45 functioning of children treated for a brain tumor: design of a randomized controlled double-blind trial BMC Cancer 2012, 12, 581 Chapter 4 Timed performance weaknesses on computerized p. 61 tasks in pediatric brain tumor survivors: a comparison with sibling controls Child Neuropsychology 2015, epub ahead of print

Chapter 5 Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor p. 85 survivors with neurocognitive complaints Quality of Life Research 2016, 25(2), 435-446

Chapter 6 The association between the behavior rating p. 107 inventory of executive functioning and neurocognitive testing in children diagnosed with a brain tumor Submitted

Chapter 7 Neurofeedback ineffective in paediatric brain p. 125 tumour survivors: Results of a double-blind randomised placebo-controlled trial European Journal of Cancer 2016, 64, 62-73

Chapter 8 General Discussion p. 155

Summary p. 173 Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch) p. 179 List of contributions of all author p. 185 Portfolio p. 189 Financial Support p. 195 Dankwoord – Acknowledgements p. 197 Curriculum Vitae p. 201

Page 10: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

Page 11: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

1

General Introduction

Page 12: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 1

10

Page 13: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General introduction

11

1CANCER DURING CHILDHOOD

In the Netherlands, approximately 550 children are diagnosed with cancer each year. Over the last decades, impressive advances have been made in medicine and one of the most striking advances has been in the treatment of pediatric cancer. Treatment often consists of a combination of surgery, chemotherapy, and/or, radiotherapy. In the first half of the previous century, children who were diagnosed with cancer usually died within weeks. In 1947, for the first time, doctors achieved partial remission in a four-year-old girl who was diagnosed with leukemia.1 This marks the first of many medical milestones improving early detection and better treatment, which have led the survival rates of pediatric cancer patients to rise from less than 10% in the 1940’s and 1950’s to almost 80% nowadays.2 Nevertheless, despite the medical advances, pediatric cancer is still the most common cause of death by a disease in children.

PEDIATRIC BRAIN TUMORS

Brain tumors are the second most common type of cancer in children concerning almost 18% of new pediatric cancer diagnoses, after leukemia with 27% of the cases.2 In the Netherlands every year approximately 100 children are diagnosed with a brain tumor. There are many different types of brain tumors in childhood and the survival rates vary widely per tumor type. Mostly an infaust prognosis is for diffuse intrinsic pontine gliomas (DIPG), brainstem gliomas that are inoperable due to the location. The best survival is for pilocytic astrocytomas, a form of low-grade glioma (LGG), for which the survival rate is up to 95%.3 Increased survival comes at a cost. The tumor and its treatment inevitably damage healthy brain tissue. The treatment modalities are targeted to remove or destroy the tumor cells, but often healthy brain tissue and cells are unintentionally also affected.4 The brain damage from systemic treatment, e.g. chemotherapy and/or radiotherapy, typically white matter loss, can lead to a wide variety of late effects in pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTS), among which neurocognitive late effects.5

NEUROCOGNITIVE CONSEQUENCES OF A PEDIATRIC BRAIN TUMOR

Compared with survivors of other types of cancer, survivors of a brain tumor in childhood bear the greatest risk of neurocognitive impairment. Many PBTS suffer from a wide range of neurocognitive deficits, for example impaired intelligence, slower speed, and memory and attention deficits.6 Attention and intelligence have often been studied in PBTS. A

Page 14: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 1

12

major consequence of impairments in attention or intelligence is the decline in ability to acquire new skills and information, which leads to an increasing gap in the development between patients and their unaffected peers.7 For this reason, meta-analytically studying the magnitude of impairments in neurocognitive functioning, especially attention and intelligence, in PBTS is an important topic.Although neurocognitive functioning after brain tumor treatment has received intensive study, the available knowledge can be increased. In the past, studies have often focused on a limited array of neurocognitive functions, such as attention or speed,8,9 or have focused on a restricted subgroup of PBTS, such as patients with cerebellar tumors.10,11 Also, traditionally studies used paper and pencil tasks to measure neurocognitive functioning.12 Paper and pencil-based measures often unintentionally target multiple neurocognitive functions, while computerized tasks facilitate isolation of particular neurocognitive functions. Therefore Ullrich and Embry advocate the use of computerized measures, to study the neurocognitive functions in PBTS.13

PREDICTORS OF NEUROCOGNITIVE CONSEQUENCES

In addition to studying the nature and magnitude of neurocognitive problems in PBTS, it is important to understand the risk factors that are associated with and/or predict these deficits, as that may help in the development of ways to prevent damage or alleviate the consequences. The neurocognitive deficits may also have an impact on educational results, vocational success and may compromise social competence and health related quality of life (HRQOL).14 In previous studies, multiple child characteristics and medical factors have been investigated as possible predictors of late neurocognitive outcomes. It has been reported that age, gender, and treatment factors are associated with neurocognitive outcomes.15 Especially cranial irradiation is a major risk factor that has been reported to lead to worse neurocognitive late effects.16 A younger brain appears to be more vulnerable to the adverse effects of the tumor and the treatment, thus children who are diagnosed earlier in life have on average poorer neurocognitive outcomes than children who are diagnosed later.17 Furthermore, female patients tend to be more vulnerable than their male counterparts.18 In addition, presence of a pre-operative hydrocephalus, particularly when it requires a shunt, has been associated with worse neurocognitive outcome.19 Brain damage, especially white matter damage as a result of the tumor and the treatment, might be an underlying cause of the neurocognitive late effects.20 A model has been proposed, in which damage to the white matter causes processing speed deficits. These processing speed deficits in turn cascade into deficits in other neurocognitive functions,

Page 15: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General introduction

13

1intelligence, and academic achievement.17,21 Indeed, processing speed has found to be correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association between white matter integrity and reading skills.23 The same research group reported a correlation between intellectual outcomes and white matter integrity.20 Especially the young brain is vulnerable to white matter damage after treatment for a brain tumor, as the white matter in young children is still immature. Damage to neural progenitor cells, for example, caused by cranial radiation therapy, may challenge healthy age-appropriate white matter growth.24 The development of interventions to prevent or alleviate the neurocognitive consequences and could greatly impact the lives of PBTS.

PSYCHOSOCIAL CONSEQUENCES OF A PEDIATRIC BRAIN TUMOR

Psychosocial functioning is understudied in PBTS, as compared to other types of cancer. This is largely due to the fact that children with a brain tumor are often excluded from studies on childhood cancer survivors, due to small numbers of patients and the heterogeneity of tumors, treatment, and outcomes.25 The neurocognitive consequences in PBTS may depress their psychosocial functioning, i.e. their HRQOL, social competence, self-esteem, and fatigue. HRQOL comprises multiple aspects of subjective well being and functioning. In the past decades, several studies on HRQOL in PBTS have been conducted.26 Although, these studies have reported contradictory findings, with HRQOL either comparable to the general population,27 or worse HRQOL.28 For example, one study demonstrated PBTS being bullied, having problems with peers, and suffering from stressful and depressive feelings.28 A comprehensive review on social-competence found that PBTS reported deficits in this area.29 Also, according to another study, PBTS reported lower self-confidence and self-esteem compared to leukemia survivors.30 The brain tumor and the treatment frequently causes sleep deficits and decreased sleep quality in PBTS, which leads to fatigue which negatively influences daily functioning.31 Fatigue in PBTS may decrease psychosocial functioning.32,33

PBTS and their parents or teachers may differ in their view of psychosocial functioning of the PBTS, as it has been reported that proxy-report for chronically ill children often differs from self-report.34 Additional insight from different reference persons surrounding PBTS, such as parent-report and teacher-report on top of self-report, would reveal relevant information on the functioning of PBTS from different point of views and in different environments.Overall, PBTS with neurocognitive deficits seem to be at increased risk for decreased psychosocial functioning.35 Therefore it is important to study psychosocial functioning in PBTS with neurocognitive complaints.

Page 16: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 1

14

SCREENING FOR NEUROCOGNITIVE LATE EFFECTS IN PBTS

It is important to screen PBTS for neurocognitive problems regularly, to assess changes over time, in order to enable timely referral to the appropriate professional when indicated. For this reason, neurocognitive functioning of PBTS should be assessed systematically, e.g. as suggested by Wash and colleagues.36 However, extensive assessments are time consuming and costly. Preferably, PBTS would be screened regularly with a brief questionnaire and only further assessed in case of screener indication. Computerized tests and questionnaires that are designed to measure the same neurocognitive domain (e.g. attention) do differ. Computerized tests are highly structured and have the ability of measuring a specific function objectively, by using built-in control conditions that manipulate the level of difficulty for the domain of interest, while keeping other functions consistent. This leads to high experimental control and internal validity.37 On the other hand, questionnaires measure subjective functioning and may provide better ecological validity by assessing complex behavioral problems faced in daily life.38 It would be expected, however, that a computerized test and a questionnaire targeting the same neurocognitive domain show a relation.PBTS have been reported to have impaired executive functioning. The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF, Dutch version;38,39 is a questionnaire that measures behavioral executive functioning. In PBTS it has been found that one of the BRIEF scales, the working memory scale, correlates to working memory tasks,40 implying that the BRIEF can potentially be employed as a screener for deficits after a brain tumor. It would be interesting to study whether a questionnaire, the BRIEF, is correlated to tasks measuring different neurocognitive domains PBTS often show deficits, such as attention, cognitive flexibility, memory, and inhibition. Furthermore, we are interested to investigate the differences and relation between different respondents; by comparing the BRIEF parent-report to the BRIEF teacher-report. As mentioned above, different reporters see PBTS in different environments, which may lead to different views. Lastly we will explore the clinical utility of the BRIEF as a screening tool.

INTERVENTIONS FOR NEUROCOGNITIVE LATE EFFECTS IN PBTS

With the robust knowledge of the neurocognitive late effects of the tumor and the treatment in PBTS, interventions to improve the neurocognitive functioning in PBTS are warranted. However, to date there is a paucity of effective interventions.41 A few neurocognitive training programs have been developed and investigated, although the samples were small and the effectiveness modest.42–44 Butler and colleagues developed

Page 17: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General introduction

15

1a cognitive remediation program using techniques from brain injury rehabilitation, special education and clinical psychology.42 They performed a randomized controlled trial including 161 childhood cancer survivors with central nervous system involvement, aged 6-17 years. Caregivers reported improved attention and academic achievement, although the effect sizes were modest and were not found on neurocognitive functioning, which acted as primary outcome in the study. The researchers argued that the modest effects are comparable to other rehabilitation interventions for children with brain injuries. In another study, Van ‘t Hooft et al. investigated the effects of a cognitive training program on neurocognitive function in patients with acquired brain injury, aged 9-16 years, including 14 brain tumor survivors.43 The training program consisted of memory and attention exercises, in combination with cognitive behavioral training. The training showed positive effects on memory and attention functioning until six months after the training, but not on processing speed. A third study into an intervention for neurocognitive functioning of PBTS was conducted by Conklin et al.45 These researchers published a randomized controlled trial with PBTS and leukemia survivors (N=68, aged 8-16 years) on the effects of a computerized working memory training, CogMed.44 The results showed improvements directly after the intervention as compared to pre-intervention in visual working memory, but not in verbal working memory.Pharmacological interventions such as methylphenidate, have also been reported to be effective for neurocognitive deficits in PBTS.46 A drawback of pharmacotherapy, though, is the possibility of side effects, e.g. sleep disturbance, weight loss, anxiety, and sadness.47 Also, this medication does not lead to a sustained effect unless the patient continues the pharmacotherapy.

NEUROFEEDBACK

The limited current available intervention options for neurocognitive late effects in PBTS call for the search of alternatives. Neurofeedback (NF) training is a relatively new form of therapy, which has never been investigated in PBTS. NF training is an intervention based on the principles of operant conditioning. Direct feedback of the current brain activity is offered to the patient, in order to teach the patient to regulate his or her brain activity. Reinforcement may comprise seeing a movie or hearing music. The desired brain wave is determined by a quantified electro encephalogram (EEG), which is conducted prior to the training. The efficacy of NF training in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has been studied extensively and promising results have been reported in uncontrolled and controlled studies.48,49 The effects of NF training were found to be smaller in randomized controlled trials than in uncontrolled trials.50 At the start of our study, no

Page 18: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 1

16

double blind randomized controlled study had been published yet.51 However, in recent years, three double-blind and one single-blind RCTs have been published on NF training in children with ADHD.52–55 These studies found improvements over time on ADHD symptoms in the NF-group; however, similar improvements were found in the placebo feedback (PF) group, potentially reflecting non-specific treatment effects. Brain tumor survivors differ from ADHD patients, as they have structural brain damage caused by the tumor, hydrocephalus, surgery, radiotherapy and/or chemotherapy. An indication that NF training might be an effective intervention for PBTS derives from the results of studies into the effects of NF training in patients with traumatic brain injury. A review by Thornton and colleagues describes several studies with traumatic brain injury patients, reporting improved attention, cognitive flexibility, cognitive performance, and problem solving after NF training, but no control group was included in the studies reviewed.56 In our hospital, Aukema and colleagues conducted a pilot study into the feasibility of NF training on 9 nine brain tumor survivors and found that NF training was feasible in PBTS.57 All participants completed the training and would recommend it to others. Patients reported less fatigue after the training. Also processing speed improved in six out of nine patients. The results of the studies on NF training in children with ADHD and children with acquired brain injury that were published at the time our study started, as well as the results of the pilot study, identified the need for a randomized controlled trial (RCT) into the effects of NF training in PBTS.

AIMS OF THE THESIS

The focus of the present thesis is on the neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning of PBTS and investigating the efficacy of NF training to improve their functioning. Specifically, the aims of the current thesis were:- To provide a systematic review of studies into intellectual and attention functioning of

PBTS. - To outline the neurocognitive functions that might be affected after treatment for a

pediatric brain tumor, while potential predictors for neurocognitive functioning were also investigated.

- To assess the psychosocial functioning of the PBTS.- To investigate the correlation between proxy-report questionnaires (parent and teacher)

and tasks measuring neurocognitive functioning in PBTS. - To investigate the effects of NF training on neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning

in PBTS using a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trial.

Page 19: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General introduction

17

1SAMPLE AND DESIGN

This thesis describes neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning of PBTS with subjective neurocognitive complaints, as well as the results of the first double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trial investigating the efficacy of NF training in PBTS (clinicaltrials.gov NCT00961922). For this purpose, 249 PBTS were invited to participate. Participants were included if they were aged 8 to 18 years, treated for a brain tumor in the Netherlands >2 years prior to enrolment, as neurocognitive deficits often appear or increase over time. Lastly we included PBTS who suffered from neurocognitive complaints, in order to study the nature of deficits in these PBTS. Siblings of participants in the age range 8-18 were invited to participate as a control group to study the nature of the deficits.

OUTLINE OF THE THESIS

Firstly, we conducted a meta-analysis (Chapter 2) to establish the magnitude of the neurocognitive problems in PBTS. We searched for studies on intelligence and attention functioning of PBTS, as these are areas that PBTS have often been reported to have problems. Furthermore, exploratory analyses investigated the possible impact of medical risk factors on general intelligence.The problems that are reportedly experienced by PBTS require an intervention to improve neurocognitive functioning. In Chapter 3 we describe the design of the PRISMA study, a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trail to investigate the efficacy of NF training in PBTS with neurocognitive complaints. In Chapter 4 the neurocognitive functioning of the participating PBTS at enrollment in the study is compared to the functioning of a sibling control group. In Chapter 5 the baseline psychosocial functioning of the participants of PRISMA is presented, as compared to normative data. Chapter 6 describes the relationship between tasks and questionnaire measures of executive functions in PBTS. Finally, the results of the randomized controlled trial are presented in Chapter 7. The neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning of PBTS in the NF group was compared to the functioning of the PF group. The last chapter of this thesis, Chapter 8, is the summary and general discussion of the results of the studies described in this thesis.

Page 20: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 1

18

REFERENCES

1. Farber S, Diamond LK, Mercer RD, Sylvester RF, Wolff JA. Temporary remissions in acute leukemia in children produced by folic acid antagonist, 4-aminopteroyl-glutamic acid (aminopterin). N Engl J Med. 1948;238(23):787–93.

2. Howlader N, Noone A, Krapcho M, Garshell J, Miller D, Altekruse S, et al. SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975-2011, National Cancer Institute [Internet]. 2014. Available from: http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2011/

3. Wells EM, Packer RJ. Pediatric brain tumors. Contin Lifelong Learn Neurol. 2015;12(2, Neuro-oncology):373–96.

4. Han R, Yang YM, Dietrich J, Luebke A, Mayer-Proschel M, Noble M. Systemic 5-fluorouracil treatment causes a syndrome of delayed myelin destruction in the central nervous system. J Biol. 2008;7(4):12.

5. Palmer SL, Glass JO, Li Y, Ogg R, Qaddoumi I, Armstrong GT, et al. White matter integrity is associated with cognitive processing in patients treated for a posterior fossa brain tumor. Neuro Oncol. 2012;14(9):1185–93.

6. Robinson KE, Fraley CE, Pearson MM, Kuttesch JF, Compas BE. Neurocognitive Late Effects of Pediatric Brain Tumors of the Posterior Fossa: A Quantitative Review. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;1–10.

7. Mabbott DJ, Spiegler BJ, Greenberg ML, Rutka JT, Hyder DJ, Bouffet E. Serial evaluation of academic and behavioral outcome after treatment with cranial radiation in childhood. J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(10):2256–63.

8. Moyer KH, Willard VW, Gross AM, Netson KL, Ashford JM, Kahalley LS, et al. The Impact of Attention on Social Functioning in Survivors of Pediatric Acute Lymphoblastic Leukemia and Brain Tumors. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2012;59(7):1290–5.

9. Kahalley LS, Conklin HM, Tyc VL, Hudson MM, Wilson SJ, Wu S, et al. Slower processing speed after treatment for pediatric brain tumor and acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Psychooncology. 2013;9:1979–86.

10. Steinlin M, Imfeld S, Zulauf P, Boltshauser E, Lovblad KO, Ridolfi LA, et al. Neuropsychological long-term sequelae after posterior fossa tumour resection during childhood. Brain. 2003;126:1998–2008.

11. Mabbott DJ, Penkman L, Witol A. Core neurocognitive functions in children treated for posterior fossa tumors. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(2):159–68.

12. Shortman RI, Lowis SP, Penn A, Mccarter RJ, Hunt LP, Brown CC, et al. Cognitive Function in Children With Brain Tumors in the First Year After Diagnosis Compared to Healthy Matched Controls. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2013;61:464–72.

13. Ullrich NJ, Embry L. Neurocognitive dysfunction in survivors of childhood brain tumors. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2012;19(1):35–42.

14. Hobbie WL, Ogle S, Reilly M, Barakat L, Lucas MS, Ginsberg JP, et al. Adolescent and Young Adult Survivors of Childhood Brain Tumors: Life After Treatment in Their Own Words. Cancer Nurs. 2015 Apr 6;39(2):134–43.

15. Tonning Olsson I, Perrin S, Lundgren J, Hjorth L, Johanson A. Long-Term Cognitive Sequelae After Pediatric Brain Tumor Related to Medical Risk Factors, Age, and Sex. Pediatr Neurol. 2014 Oct;51(4):515–21.

16. Reddick WE, Taghipour DJ, Glass JO, Ashford J, Xiong X, Wu S, et al. Prognostic Factors that Increase the Risk for Reduced White Matter Volumes and Deficits in Attention and Learning for Survivors of Childhood Cancers. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2014;61:1074–9.

Page 21: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General introduction

19

117. Palmer SL. Neurodevelopmental impact on children treated for medulloblastoma: A review and

proposed conceptual model. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2008;14(3):203–10. 18. Mulhern RK, Merchant TE, Gajjar A, Reddick WE, Kun LE. Late neurocognitive sequelae in

survivors of brain tumours in childhood. Lancet Oncol. 2004;5(7):399–408. 19. Duffner PK. Risk factors for cognitive decline in children treated for brain tumors. Eur J Paediatr

Neurol. 2010;14(2):106–15. 20. King TZ, Wang L, Mao H. Disruption of White Matter Integrity in Adult Survivors of Childhood

Brain Tumors: Correlates with Long-Term Intellectual Outcomes. PLoS One. 2015 Jan 6;10(7):e0131744.

21. Wolfe KR, Madan-Swain A, Kana RK. Executive dysfunction in pediatric posterior fossa tumor survivors: a systematic literature review of neurocognitive deficits and interventions. Dev Neuropsychol. 2012;37(2):153–75.

22. Aukema EJ, Caan M, Oudhuis N, Majoie C, Vos F, Reneman L, et al. White matter fractional anisotropy correlates with speed of processing and motor speed in young childhood cancer survivors. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2009;74(3):837–43.

23. Smith KM, King TZ, Jayakar R, Morris RD. Reading Skill in Adult Survivors of Childhood Brain Tumor: A Theory-Based Neurocognitive Model. Neuropsychology. 2014;28(3):448–58.

24. Redmond KJ, Mahone EM, Terezakis S, Ishaq O, Ford E, McNutt T, et al. Association between radiation dose to neuronal progenitor cell niches and temporal lobes and performance on neuropsychological testing in children: a prospective study. Neuro Oncol. 2013;15(3):360–9.

25. Patenaude AF, Kupst MJ. Psychosocial functioning in pediatric cancer. J Pediatr Psychol. 2005;30(1):9–27.

26. Gudrunardottir T, Lannering B, Remke M, Taylor MD, Wells EM, Keating RF, et al. Treatment developments and the unfolding of the quality of life discussion in childhood medulloblastoma: a review. Child’s Nerv Syst. 2014 Feb 26;30(6):979–90.

27. Zebrack BJ, Gurney JG, Oeffinger K, Whitton J, Packer RJ, Mertens A, et al. Psychological outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood brain cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22(6):999–1006.

28. Aukema EJ, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Last BF, Maurice-Stam H, Grootenhuis MA. Childhood brain tumor survivors at risk for impaired health-related quality of life. J Pediatr Hematol Oncol. 2013 Nov;35(8):603–9.

29. Schulte F, Barrera M. Social competence in childhood brain tumor survivors: a comprehensive review. Support care cancer. 2010;18(12):1499–513.

30. Chou L-N, Hunter A. Factors affecting quality of life in Taiwanese survivors of childhood cancer. J Adv Nurs. 2009;65(10):2131–41.

31. Verberne LM, Maurice-Stam H, Grootenhuis M a, Van Santen HM, Schouten-Van Meeteren AYN. Sleep disorders in children after treatment for a CNS tumour. J Sleep Res. 2012;21(4):461–9.

32. Meeske KA, Patel SK, Palmer SN, Nelson MB, Parow AM. Factors Associated With Health-Related Quality of Life in Pediatric Cancer Survivors. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2007;49:298–305.

33. Daniel LC, Brumley LD, Schwartz LA. Fatigue in Adolescents With Cancer Compared to Healthy Adolescents. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2013;60:1902–7.

34. Sattoe JNT, van Staa A, Moll HA. The proxy problem anatomized: child-parent disagreement in health related quality of life reports of chronically ill adolescents. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2012;10(1):10.

35. Penn A, Shortman RI, Lowis SP, Stevens MCG, Hunt LP, Mccarter RJ, et al. Child-Related Determinants of Health-Related Quality of Life in Children With Brain Tumours 1 Year After Diagnosis. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2010;55:1377–85.

Page 22: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 1

20

36. Walsh KS, Noll RB, Annett RD, Patel SK, Patenaude AF, Embry L. Standard of Care for Neuropsychological Monitoring in Pediatric Neuro-Oncology: Lessons From the Children’s Oncology Group (COG). Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2016;63:191–5.

37. Anderson VA, Anderson P, Northam E, Jacobs R, Mikiewicz O. Relationships between cognitive and behavioral measures of executive function in children with brain disease. Child Neuropsychol. 2002 Dec;8(4):231–40.

38. Gioia G a, Isquith PK, Retzlaff PD, Espy K a. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;8(4):249–57.

39. Smidts DP, Huizinga M. BRIEF executieve functies gedragsvragenlijst: Handleiding. Amsterdam: Hogrefe Uitgevers; 2009.

40. Howarth R a, Ashford JM, Merchant TE, Ogg RJ, Santana V, Wu S, et al. The Utility of Parent Report in the Assessment of Working Memory among Childhood Brain Tumor Survivors. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19(4):380–9.

41. Olson K, Sands S a. Cognitive training programs for childhood cancer patients and survivors: A critical review and future directions. Child Neuropsychol. 2016;22(5):509–36.

42. Butler RW, Copeland DR, Fairclough DL, Mulhern RK, Katz ER, Kazak AE, et al. A multicenter, randomized clinical trial of a cognitive remediation program for childhood survivors of a pediatric malignancy. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76(3):367–78.

43. van ’t Hooft I, Andersson K, Bergman B, Sejersen T, von Wendt L, Bartfai A. Sustained favorable effects of cognitive training in children with acquired brain injuries. NeuroRehabilitation. 2007;22(2):109–16.

44. Hardy KK, Willard VW, Allen TM, Bonner MJ. Working memory training in survivors of pediatric cancer: a randomized pilot study. Psychooncology. 2013 Aug;22(8):1856–65.

45. Conklin HM, Ogg RJ, Ashford JM, Scoggins MA, Zou P, Clark KN, et al. Computerized Cognitive Training for Amelioration of Cognitive Late Effects Among Childhood Cancer Survivors: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Clin Oncol. 2015 Nov 20;33(33):3894–902.

46. Conklin HM, Reddick WE, Ashford JM, Ogg S, Howard SC, Morris EB, et al. Long-term efficacy of methylphenidate in enhancing attention regulation, social skills, and academic abilities of childhood cancer survivors. J Clin Oncol. 2010;28(29):4465–72.

47. Simonoff E, Taylor E, Baird G, Bernard S, Chadwick O, Liang H, et al. Randomized controlled double-blind trial of optimal dose methylphenidate in children and adolescents with severe attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and intellectual disability. J child Psychol psychiatry. 2013;54(5):527–35.

48. Strehl U, Leins U, Goth G, Klinger C, Hinterberger T, Birbaumer N. Self-regulation of slow cortical potentials: a new treatment for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2006;118(5):e1530–40.

49. Gevensleben H, Holl B, Albrecht B, Schlamp D, Kratz O, Studer P, et al. Neurofeedback training in children with ADHD: 6-month follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19(9):715–24.

50. Janssen TWP, Bink M, Geladé K, van Mourik R, Maras A, Oosterlaan J. A Randomized Controlled Trial Investigating the Effects of Neurofeedback, Methylphenidate, and Physical Activity on Event-Related Potentials in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2016 Jan 15;26(4):344–53.

51. Moriyama TS, Polanczyk G, Caye A, Banaschewski T, Brandeis D, Rohde L a. Evidence-based information on the clinical use of neurofeedback for ADHD. Neurotherapeutics. 2012 Jul;9(3):588–98.

Page 23: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General introduction

21

152. Perreau-Linck E, Lessard N, Levesque J, Beauregard M. Effects of neurofeed back training on

inhibitory capacities in ADHD children: A single-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study. J Neurother. 2010;14:229–42.

53. Arnold LE, Lofthouse N, Hersch S, Pan X, Hurt E, Bates B, et al. EEG neurofeedback for ADHD: double-blind sham-controlled randomized pilot feasibility trial. J Atten Disord. 2013 Jul;17(5):410–9.

54. Van Dongen-Boomsma M, Vollebregt MA, Slaats-Willemse D, Buitelaar JK. A randomized placebo-controlled trial of electroencephalographic (EEG) neurofeedback in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2013 Aug;74(8):821–7.

55. Lansbergen MM, van Dongen-Boomsma M, Buitelaar JK, Slaats-Willemse D. ADHD and EEG-neurofeedback: a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled feasibility study. J Neural Transm. 2011;118(2):275–84.

56. Thornton KE, Carmody DP. Efficacy of traumatic brain injury rehabilitation: interventions of QEEG-guided biofeedback, computers, strategies, and medications. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2008;33(2):101–24.

57. Aukema EJ, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Last BF, Breteler MHM, Hogeweg J, Grootenhuis MA. Exploring the feasibility of Neurofeedback training as a cognitive intervention for childhood brain tumor survivors: a pilot study. (Submitted).

Page 24: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

Page 25: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

2

Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain

tumour and its treatment: a meta-analysis

Marieke A. de RuiterRosa van Mourik

Antoinette Y.N. Schouten-van MeeterenMartha A. Grootenhuis

Jaap Oosterlaan

Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 2013, 55(5), 408-417

Page 26: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

24

ABSTRACT

Aims: This meta-analysis provides a systematic review of studies into intellectual and attentional functioning of paediatric brain tumour survivors (PBTS) as assessed by two widely used measures: the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd edition; WISC-III) and the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT).

Methods: Studies were located that reported on performance of PBTS (age range 6–16y). Meta-analytic effect sizes were calculated for Full-scale IQ, Performance IQ, and Verbal IQ as measured by the WISC-III, and mean hit reaction time, errors of omission, and errors of commission as measured by the CPT. Exploratory analyses investigated the possible impacts of treatment mode, tumour location, age at diagnosis, and time since diagnosis on intelligence.

Results: Twenty-nine studies were included: 22 reported on the WISC-III in 710 PBTS and seven on CPT results in 372 PBTS. PBTS performed below average (ps<0.001) on Full-scale IQ (Cohen’s d=−0.79), Performance IQ (d=−0.90), and Verbal IQ (d=−0.54). PBTS committed more errors of omission than the norm (d=0.82, p<0.001); no differences were found for mean hit reaction time and errors of commission. Cranial radiotherapy, chemotherapy, and longer time since diagnosis were associated with lower WISC-III scores (ps<0.05).

Conclusions: PBTS have seriously impaired intellectual functioning and attentiveness. Being treated with cranial radiotherapy and/or chemotherapy as well as longer time since diagnosis leads to worse intellectual functioning.

Page 27: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

25

2

INTRODUCTION

In the USA the incidence of cancer in children aged 0 to 14 years is almost three per 100 000.1 Approximately 17 to 22.5% of children with cancer have a brain tumour.1,2 Advances in medicine have led to an increasing number of children surviving cancer. The 5-year survival rate of children diagnosed with a brain tumour under the age of 15 increased from 57% for patients diagnosed from 1975 to 1977 to 74% for patients diagnosed from 1996 to 2004.3

With more children becoming long-term survivors, the need has grown to understand fully the nature and magnitude of the late effects of the tumour and treatment. Compared with survivors of other malignancies, survivors of brain tumours in childhood bear the greatest risk of neurocognitive impairment.4 Numerous studies have shown that 40 to 100% of paediatric brain tumour survivors (PBTS) show some form of neurocognitive deficit.5 Frequently reported impairments in PBTS are declining levels of general intelligence and attention deficits. Deficits in these areas can have a deleterious effect on academic achievement and psychosocial functioning.6–8

Besides the burden of the tumour itself, the treatment can contribute to neurocognitive impairments. Radiotherapy is especially considered to have an impact on neurocognitive functioning.9,10 Chemotherapy, however, has also been found to be associated with poor outcomes in PBTS.11 In addition to the treatment, tumour location can affect the neurocognitive outcome of PBTS, with infratentorial tumours being associated with worse outcomes than supratentorial tumours.12 Furthermore, age at diagnosis is known to have an impact on neurocognitive outcome.13 The young brain is especially vulnerable to the adverse effects of treatment because of the rapid cell proliferation, dendritic and axonal outgrowth, as well as myelination, which take place during infancy, childhood, and adolescence. Therefore, radiotherapy is postponed or omitted in most protocols if the child is under the age of 3 years.11 In addition, time since treatment is an important determinant of neurocognitive deficits, as the deficits often increase over time, owing to a slower rate of acquiring new skills and knowledge compared with healthy peers.13,14

The current paper reports the results of a quantitative meta-analysis, investigating the magnitude and consistency of neurocognitive deficits in PBTS. Analysis of the literature determined general intelligence and attention as two frequently studied areas of neurocognitive functioning. General intelligence provides insight into the generic cognitive functioning of the patient and is measured most often using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd edition; WISC-III).15 Attention is required to some extent for nearly all components of neurocognitive functioning and is therefore a crucial area to study thoroughly. The Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT, CPT II) is the most widely used measure for attention.16,17 Besides intelligence and attention, processing speed and working memory

Page 28: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

26

are often studied in PBTS. These areas are important in understanding the neurocognitive functioning of a patient; they are, however, beyond the scope of this meta-analysis, which focuses on the two key areas: intelligence and attention. The CPT comprises a measure for processing speed; therefore, this area is reported as well. Additionally, exploratory analyses investigated the possible impact of cranial radiotherapy, chemotherapy, tumour location, age at diagnosis, and time since diagnosis on general intelligence.

METHOD

Selection of studiesStudies were searched using the PubMed, Web of Science, and Embase computerized databases. Relevant studies were located by combining the search terms: neurocogniti*, neuropsych*, cogniti*, child*, pediatric*, tumor, tumour, cancer, neoplasm*, central nervous system, and brain.All retrieved studies were reviewed to include studies meeting the following criteria: (1) the participants included children treated for a brain tumour by neurosurgery, radiotherapy, and/or chemotherapy; (2) intelligence was assessed using the full WISC-III (as abbreviated versions might yield unreliable data) and/or attention was assessed using the CPT; (3) mean age of PBTS at assessment was between 6 years and 16 years, corresponding to the age range covered by the WISC-III; (4) the study was published in a peer-reviewed English language journal; and (5) the study was published before November 2011. The last search was performed on 25 November 2011. The reference lists of included studies were explored to locate additional potentially relevant studies for inclusion in the meta-analysis. No research protocol of the present meta-analysis exists.

Dependent variablesThe WISC-III is the most widely used intelligence test for children aged 6 to 16 years. Dependent measures include Full-scale IQ (FSIQ), Verbal IQ (VIQ), and Performance IQ (PIQ), on which normative samples obtain a mean score of 100 with a standard deviation (SD) of 15. VIQ is a measure of the ability to use and understand language. PIQ assesses perceptual reasoning. FSIQ is calculated by averaging VIQ and PIQ. Higher scores indicate better intellectual functioning. WISC-IV studies were not included because the WISC-IV does not allow calculation of VIQ and PIQ scores, and only few PBTS studies reported WISC-IV scores.18,19

The CPT is a widely used test to assess attention. In the CPT, a sequence of different letters is shown, one at a time, and the participant is instructed to press the space bar as quickly as possible without committing errors when any letter other than ‘X’ appears on the screen.

Page 29: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

27

2

Each letter is displayed for 250 ms, with different time intervals between each letter. Main dependent variables are (1) mean hit reaction time (MHRT), measuring processing speed, (2) errors of omission, measuring inattentiveness, and (3) errors of commission, measuring impulsivity.20 Scores are reported in T scores, with a mean of 50 and an SD of 10. For all CPT variables, higher scores indicate worse performance.

Quality assessmentTwo authors (MAdR and RvM) independently assessed the quality of the included studies using the Newcastle-Ottawa Scale.21 The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale assesses quality in terms of the selection of children (four criteria), comparability of study groups if applicable (one criterion), and outcome assessment (three criteria). Differences in assessment between both authors were resolved by consensus. Some criteria were not applicable to all studies; therefore we used the percentage of the applicable criteria each study met as a score.

Statistical analysesThe computer programs Comprehensive Meta-Analysis 2.222 and SPSS version 18.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA) were used for statistical analyses. Techniques by Hozo et al. were used to convert medians into means and SDs if necessary.23–27 Where studies compared two or more subgroups of PBTS, the data were aggregated into one mean and SD per study.For each of the dependent measures, effect sizes were calculated for each study separately. Effect sizes were calculated in terms of Cohen’s d, with sizes of 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80 translating into small, medium, and large effects respectively.28 Only one study used a comparison group of healthy participants;29 all other studies used normative data to interpret data derived from PBTS. For comparability, normative data were used to calculate effect sizes for all studies. For each dependent variable, an overall effect size was calculated by weighting all the effect sizes according to the sample sizes. To test whether the variability in effect sizes exceeded what could be expected from sampling error alone, Q and I2 tests of heterogeneity were conducted.30,31 That is, when homogeneously distributed, an identical underlying effect size is representative for all studies and so-called fixed effects analysis can be used for estimating the assumed common effect. If the effect sizes are heterogeneously distributed, a random effects analysis estimates the mean of distribution of effects across all studies, which yields wider confidence intervals for the combined effect size.A major concern in conducting a meta-analysis is the presence of publication bias, meaning that studies reporting non-significant results are less likely to be published, leading to erroneous inflation of meta-analytic effect sizes. The possibility of publication bias was reduced by including unpublished data.32–34 Furthermore, the possibility of publication bias was studied using two methods. First, we calculated Rosenthal’s fail-safe N, which calculates the necessary number of studies to nullify the overall effect, for each significant combined

Page 30: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

28

effect size.35 Second, the correlation between sample sizes (the number of PBTS) and effect sizes was calculated for each dependent variable. A significant negative correlation between sample sizes and effect sizes would indicate a tendency that significant results in small samples are easier to publish than non-significant results in small samples.We studied the possible moderating effects of the following variables on the study specific effect sizes for the dependent variables of the WISC-III: (1) cranial radiotherapy as measured by the percentage of patients treated with cranial radiotherapy (% cRT); (2) chemotherapy as measured by the percentage of patients treated with chemotherapy (% chemo); (3) tumour location as measured by the percentage of patients treated for infratentorial brain tumour (% infra); (4) age at diagnosis (age at dx); and (5) time since diagnosis (time since dx). The effects were analysed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis by meta-regression analyses, assessing the relationship between the moderating variables and the effect sizes on the dependent variables. For each moderating variable we calculated the proportion of variance accounted for, with 1%, 9%, and 25% being interpreted as small, moderate, and large effects respectively.28 These analyses were not conducted on the CPT, because of the limited number of studies available. Alpha was set at 0.05 in all analyses.

RESULTS

Figure 1 shows the selection of studies in a flowchart. Twenty-nine studies met inclusion criteria. Twenty-two studies reported scores on the WISC-III for a total of 710 PBTS.9,12,23–

26,29,34,36–49 Seven studies reported CPT results for a total of 372 PBTS.32,33,50–54 When two or more studies reported on the same participants, we included the most recently published study to prevent erroneously inflated homogeneity of meta-analytic results. Grill et al.44 and Kieffer-Renaux et al.47 report partly on the same participants. The most recent publication by Grill et al. reports on PIQ and VIQ, but does not report on FSIQ. The earlier publication of Kieffer-Renaux et al., however, does report on FSIQ. Therefore, the study by Grill et al. was included in the meta-analysis of PIQ and VIQ, whereas the study by Kieffer-Renaux et al. was included in the meta-analysis of FSIQ.For five of the 22 WISC-III studies we aggregated data on two or more PBTS subgroups into one mean and SD per study: (1) Patel et al.12 compared PBTS according to their tumour location; (2) Callu et al.39 compared patients with low-grade gliomas and malignant cerebellar tumours; (3) Lacaze et al.24 studied three samples of patients with optic pathway tumours who received three different treatments; (4) Kieffer-Renaux et al.47 compared patients with medulloblastoma who received two different doses of radiotherapy; and (5) Mulhern et al.48 compared patients with medulloblastoma with those having low-grade glioma.

Page 31: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

29

2

21

Figure 1. Flow chart of study selection n, number of studies; PBTS, paediatric brain tumour survivors; WISC, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; CPT, Conners’ Continuous Performance Test.

Studies identified through computerized database searching

(n=3074)

Additional studies identified through reference list searches

(n=0)

Studies after duplicates removed (n=1615)

English language studies (n=1395)

Studies excluded, reported as conference abstract or not

published in English (n=220)

Studies on PBTS (n=708)

Studies excluded, reporting on other patient group

(n=687)

Studies on WISC/CPT (n=34)

Studies included in meta-analysis (n=29)

Studies excluded, not reporting on WISC/CPT

(n=674)

Studies excluded due to overlap in participants

(n=5)

Figure 1. Flow chart of study selection n, number of studies; PBTS, paediatric brain tumour survivors; WISC, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; CPT, Conners’ Continuous Performance Test.

Page 32: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

30

Tabl

e I.

Char

acte

risti

cs o

f stu

dies

incl

uded

in th

e m

eta-

anal

ysis

Stud

y

Num

ber

of

parti

cipa

nts

and

diag

nosi

s

Trea

tmen

tLo

catio

nTi

me/

age

Qua

lity

% c

RT%

Che

mo

% In

fra

Mea

n ag

e at

dx

Tim

e si

nce

dxa

NO

S sc

ore

WIS

C-III

Haz

in e

t al.49

13

LG

G, 7

MB

3535

100

8.3

1.9

100

Pate

l et a

l.1270

BT

7671

497.

83.

410

0Sa

ury

and

Eman

uels

on36

8 M

B10

010

063

7.8

5.1

50Sa

nds

et a

l.3724

BT

29.2

100

65.4

3.0

3.3

100

Auk

ema

et a

l.296

MB

100

100

100

4.7

8.9

50Bo

nner

et a

l.3810

1 BT

74N

A56

7.0

3.9

100

Callu

et a

l.3920

HG

G, 1

9 LG

G44

3610

05.

43.

410

0Br

iere

et a

l.4012

MB,

6 G

NS

9478

NA

6.3

NA

0Ri

s et

al.26

83 L

GG

00

16N

AN

A10

0Sa

nder

s et

al.41

5 H

GG

8010

040

0.9

11.4

100

Jala

li et

al.23

7 LG

G10

0bN

A<4

2N

AN

A10

0Kh

ong

et a

l.4212

MB

100

100

100

8.5

3.4

50Be

ebe

et a

l.4392

LG

G0

010

08.

10.

410

0G

rill

et a

l.4476

PF

100

7210

05.

76.

150

Spie

gler

et a

l.3434

MB,

EP

100

7010

05.

52.

510

0La

caze

et a

l.2421

LG

G38

100

0N

AN

A10

0Pa

cker

4540

MB

100

100

100

6.0

4.0

50Ca

rey

et a

l.4615

BT

6053

NA

NA

NA

100

Kieff

er-R

enau

x et

al.47

36 M

B10

010

010

08.

05.

050

Mer

chan

t et a

l.258

GCT

100

00

NA

NA

100

Gri

ll et

al.9

19 M

B, E

P10

0N

A10

06.

15.

310

0M

ulhe

rn e

t al.48

18 M

B, 1

8 LG

G50

5010

0N

AN

A10

0

Page 33: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

31

2

Stud

y

Num

ber

of

parti

cipa

nts

and

diag

nosi

s

Trea

tmen

tLo

catio

nTi

me/

age

Qua

lity

% c

RT%

Che

mo

% In

fra

Mea

n ag

e at

dx

Tim

e si

nce

dxa

NO

S sc

ore

CPT

Butle

r et

al.50

131

BTN

SN

AN

AN

A6.

55.

350

Mab

bott

et a

l.5164

MB,

EP

50N

A50

5.8

5.6

100

Conk

lin e

t al.32

61 B

TNS

NA

NA

NA

6.5

5.0

50St

arga

tt e

t al.52

16 B

T62

6910

09.

94.

110

0Re

eves

et a

l.5338

MB

100

100

100

8.3

2.0

100

Mul

hern

et a

l.5437

BT

100

4962

NA

NA

100

Mul

hern

et a

l.3325

MB

100

NA

100

8.2

5.2

100

Age

s ar

e in

yea

rs. a

Calc

ulat

ed b

y su

btra

cting

age

at

asse

ssm

ent

and

age

at d

iagn

osis

. b St

ereo

tacti

c ra

diot

hera

py. B

TNS,

bra

in t

umou

r no

t sp

ecifi

ed; %

cR

T, p

erce

ntag

e of

pati

ents

tre

ated

with

cra

nial

rad

iatio

n th

erap

y; %

Che

mo,

per

cent

age

of p

atien

ts t

reat

ed w

ith c

hem

othe

rapy

; % in

fra,

per

cent

age

of p

atien

ts w

ith a

n in

frat

ento

rial

tum

our;

dx,

dia

gnos

is; N

OS,

New

cast

le-O

ttaw

a Sc

ale,

in p

erce

ntag

es o

f ap

plic

able

cri

teri

a th

at w

ere

met

; WIS

C-III

, W

echs

ler

Inte

llige

nce

Scal

e fo

r Ch

ildre

n (3

rd e

ditio

n).

LGG

, low

-gra

de g

liom

a; M

B, m

edul

lobl

asto

ma;

BT,

mix

ed d

iagn

osis

gro

up;

NA

, not

ava

ilabl

e;

HG

G, h

igh-

grad

e gl

iom

a; G

NS,

glio

ma

not

spec

ified

; PF,

pos

teri

or fo

ssa

tum

our;

EP,

epe

ndym

oma;

GCT

, ger

m c

ell t

umou

r; C

PT, C

onne

rs’ C

ontin

uous

Pe

rfor

man

ce T

est.

Page 34: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

32

Table I displays details of the studies incorporated in this meta-analysis. Some studies reported insufficient details to allow calculation of effect sizes. In these cases, authors were contacted to provide the missing data.32–34,50,51 For some studies, data were unavailable on one or more of the dependent variables, leading to unequal numbers of studies for these dependent variables.Figures 2 and 3 display the studies’ effect sizes as well as the overall effect sizes for each of the dependent variables and the accompanying 95% confidence intervals. Effect sizes of all dependent variables were heterogeneously distributed and a random effect analysis was used in all analyses. There was no significant association between the study quality ratings and effect sizes (all ps>0.08) for any of the dependent variables.

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IIIPBTS had lower FSIQ scores than their peers, as indicated by a combined random effect size of d=−0.79 (p<0.001), translating into a large effect. Of the 21 studies that reported on FSIQ, 14 reported scores significantly (p<0.05) below the average FSIQ of 100,9,12,26,34,36–41,45–48 whereas none of the studies reported scores significantly higher than average.PIQ scores were significantly lower in PBTS than in the normative sample, as indicated by a combined random effect size of d=−0.90 (p<0.001), again translating into a large effect. Fifteen of the 19 studies found PIQ scores of PBTS significantly (p<0.05) below average,9,12,24,26,34,36–40,43,44,47–49 whereas in none of the studies were scores significantly higher than average found. VIQ scores of PBTS were significantly below average. The combined random effect size was d=−0.54 (p<0.001), which represents a medium effect size. Eleven of the 19 studies reported scores that were significantly (p<0.05) below the mean.9,12,23,34,36–38,40,44,47,48 Eight studies reported VIQ scores that did not differ significantly from the mean of the normative sample.24–26,29,39,42,43 The combined effect size for PIQ was significantly higher than the combined effect size for VIQ (d=−0.29, p<0.001), indicating greater impairments in PIQ than in VIQ.There was no evidence for publication bias for any of the WISC-III measures, as we found high fail-safe N values and non-significant (ns) positive correlations between sample size and effect size (FSIQ: fail-safe N=871, r=0.36, ns; PIQ: fail-safe N=1030, r=0.16, ns; and VIQ: fail-safe N=406, r=0.25, ns).

Page 35: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

33

2

24

Variable Study d p-Value

FSIQ Hazin et al. 2011 -0,48 0,13Patel et al. 2011 -0,61 0,00Saury et al. 2011 -2,79 0,00Sands et al. 2010 -0,75 0,01Aukema et al. 2009 -1,15 0,06Bonner et al. 2009 -0,90 0,00Callu et al. 2009 -0,61 0,01Briere et al. 2008 -1,46 0,00Ris et al. 2008 -0,39 0,01Sanders et al. 2007 -2,45 0,00Jalali et al. 2006 -1,08 0,06Khong et al. 2006 0,15 0,72Beebe et al. 2005 -0,28 0,06Spiegler et al. 2004 -1,03 0,00Lacaze et al. 2003 -0,53 0,09Packer et al. 2002 -1,08 0,00Carey et al. 2001 -0,99 0,01Kieffer-Renaux et al. 2000 -0,96 0,00Merchant et al. 2000 -0,24 0,64Grill et al. 1999 -1,50 0,00Mulhern et al. 1999 -0,60 0,01

-0,79 0,00

Hazin et al. 2011 -0,80 0,02Patel et al. 2011 -0,79 0,00Saury et al. 2011 -2,79 0,00Sands et al. 2010 -0,65 0,03Aukema et al. 2009 -1,10 0,07Bonner et al. 2009 -1,02 0,00Callu et al. 2009 -0,74 0,00Briere et al. 2008 -1,42 0,00Ris et al. 2008 -0,48 0,00Jalali et al. 2006 -0,83 0,14Khong et al. 2006 -0,24 0,56Beebe et al. 2005 -0,33 0,02Grill et al. 2004 -1,48 0,00Spiegler et al. 2004 -1,14 0,00Lacaze et al. 2003 -0,87 0,01Kieffer-Renaux et al. 2000 -1,24 0,00Merchant et al. 2000 0,19 0,70Grill et al. 1999 -1,74 0,00Mulhern et al. 1999 -0,81 0,00

-0,90 0,00

Hazin et al. 2011 -0,10 0,76Patel et al. 2011 -0,40 0,02Saury et al. 2011 -2,26 0,00Sands et al. 2010 -0,69 0,02Aukema et al. 2009 -1,01 0,10Bonner et al. 2009 -0,64 0,00Callu et al. 2009 -0,21 0,35Briere et al. 2008 -1,18 0,00Ris et al. 2008 -0,27 0,09Jalali et al. 2006 -1,14 0,05Khong et al. 2006 0,53 0,20Beebe et al. 2005 -0,21 0,15Grill et al. 2004 -0,77 0,00Spiegler et al. 2004 -0,71 0,00Lacaze et al. 2003 -0,29 0,35Kieffer-Renaux et al. 2000 -0,59 0,01Merchant et al. 2000 -0,27 0,59Grill et al. 1999 -1,03 0,00

-0,85 0,00-0,54 0,00

-4,00 -2,00 0,00 2,00 4,00

Standard Difference and 95% CI

PIQ

VIQ

Mulhern et al. 1999Combined d

Combined d

Combined d

Figure 2. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edition (WISC-III) study results. CI, confidence interval; FSIQ, Full-scale IQ; PIQ, Performance IQ; VIQ, Verbal IQ.

Figure 2. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edition (WISC-III) study results. CI, confidence interval; FSIQ, Full-scale IQ; PIQ, Performance IQ; VIQ, Verbal IQ.

Page 36: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

34

25

-2,00 -1,00 0,00 1,00 2,00

Standard Difference and 95% CI

Butler et al. 2008 0,24 0,05Mabbott et al. 2008 0,46 0,01Conklin et al. 2007 -0,53 0,00Stargatt et al. 2007 -0,28 0,44Reeves et al. 2006 1,06 0,00Mulhern et al. 2004 0,93 0,00Mulhern et al. 2001 -0,96 0,00

0,15 0,54

Butler et al. 2008 0,09 0,46Mabbott et al. 2008 0,41 0,02Conklin et al. 2007 0,49 0,01Stargatt et al. 2007 1,29 0,00Reeves et al. 2006 1,66 0,00Mulhern et al. 2001 1,32 0,00

0,82 0,00

Butler et al. 2008 0,00 0,97Mabbott et al. 2008 -0,19 0,28Conklin et al. 2007 -0,03 0,85Stargatt et al. 2007 0,91 0,01Reeves et al. 2006 -0,50 0,03Mulhern et al. 2004 0,31 0,19Mulhern et al. 2001 0,26 0,36

0,03 0,78

Variable Study d p-Value

MHRT

EO

EC

Combined d

Combined d

Combined d

Figure 3. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT) study results. Higher scores indicate worse performance for all three dependent variables. CI, confidence interval; EC, errors of commission; EO, errors of omission; MHRT, mean hit reaction time.

Figure 3. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT) study results. Higher scores indicate worse performance for all three dependent variables. CI, confidence interval; EC, errors of commission; EO, errors of omission; MHRT, mean hit reaction time.

Conners’ Continuous Performance TestThe studies were ambiguous about the scores of PBTS on MHRT of the CPT. Three of seven studies found significantly slower MHRT,51,53,54 whereas two studies found responses of the PBTS to be significantly faster than average.32,33 Two other studies found PBTS scores in the average range.50,52 Across studies a non-significant combined random effect size of d=0.15 was found for MHRT.The number of errors of omission on the CPT committed by PBTS was higher than the normative sample, as indicated by a combined random effect size of d=0.82 (p<0.001), which is considered to be a large effect. All but one study found significantly higher errors of omission rates in PBTS than the normative sample.32,33,51–53 Fail-safe N was 64 and there was a positive non-significant correlation between sample sizes and effect sizes (r=0.85), together indicating that there was no evidence for publication bias.PBTS did not differ from the normative sample on the number of errors of commission, as indicated by a non-significant combined random effect size of d=0.03. Five of seven studies found no performance differences between PBTS and the normative sample;32,33,50,51,54 one study reported PBTS to make fewer errors of commission than the normative sample,52 and another study found that more errors were made by the PBTS than the normative sample.53

Page 37: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

35

2

Exploratory analysesTable II reports the results for the meta-regression analysis for the five moderating variables. Cranial radiotherapy was a strong predictor of lower intellectual functioning, accounting for 26%, 32%, and 19% of the variance in FSIQ, PIQ, and VIQ respectively, with cranial radiotherapy leading to lower scores as opposed to no cranial radiotherapy. Chemotherapy accounted for 22% of the variance in FSIQ and 29% of the variance in PIQ scores, with chemotherapy leading to lower scores as opposed to no chemotherapy. There was no association between chemotherapy and VIQ. Furthermore, we found no predictive value of tumour location or age at diagnosis for intelligence scores. Longer time since diagnosis, however, was highly predictive of lower scores on all WISC-III scales, accounting for large proportions of the variance (FSIQ 41%; PIQ 44%; VIQ 25%). As expected, there was a strong association between cranial radiotherapy and chemotherapy (r=0.54, p<0.05), and between age at diagnosis and time since diagnosis (r=−0.66, p<0.05), not allowing us to distinguish between the effects of these moderating variables.

Table II. Meta-regression analyses, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd edition) studies

FSIQ PIQ VIQn β R2 p n β R2 p n β R2 p

Treatment module cRT 21 −0.51 0.26 0.001 19 −0.56 0.32 <0.001 19 −0.44 0.19 0.013 Chemo 18 −0.47 0.22 0.006 16 −0.54 0.29 0.004 16 −0.36 0.13 0.090Tumour location Infra 19 −0.12 0.01 0.591 18 −0.30 0.09 0.188 18 −0.11 0.01 0.633Age at diagnosis 15 0.40 0.16 0.061 14 0.15 0.02 0.568 14 0.35 0.12 0.114Time since diagnosis

14 −0.64 0.41 <0.001 13 −0.67 0.44 <0.001 13 −0.50 0.25 0.014

FSIQ, Full-scale IQ; PIQ, Performance IQ; VIQ, Verbal IQ; n, number of studies; β, standardized Beta coefficient; R2, R squared; cRT, cranial radiation therapy; Chemo, chemotherapy; Infra, infratentorial tumour.

DISCUSSION

This meta-analysis summarized neurocognitive functioning of 710 (WISC-III) and 372 (CPT, CPT II) PBTS. We found substantial impairments in intellectual functioning and attentional abilities. PBTS scored on average −0.54SD to −0.90SD lower on the WISC-III scales than the normative sample, with PIQ scores being even more depressed than VIQ scores. The number of PBTS in this meta-analysis that were at grade level and succeeding academically is unknown. However, a large body of research has demonstrated that intellectual functioning, as assessed with intelligence tests, is a powerful predictor of academic achievement and

Page 38: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

36

vocational success.55,56 In PBTS this association was substantiated by Reddick et al.,57 who found a positive relation between intelligence scores of PBTS, as measured with the Wechsler intelligence scales, and academic achievement, as measured with the abbreviated Wechsler Individual Achievement Test. Moreover de Boer et al.58 report academic failure and lower intelligence to be risk factors for decreased employment rates in PBTS, with PBTS being nearly five times more likely to be unemployed than healthy peers.

This meta-analysis found performance intelligence to be more vulnerable to the detrimental effects of a brain tumour and its associated treatment than verbal intelligence, with the observed PIQ scores in this meta-analysis being on average −0.29SD lower than the VIQ scores. Similar findings have been reported in other populations at risk for brain damage, including children born very preterm and those with traumatic brain injury.59,60 The discrepancy between PIQ and VIQ findings in PBTS might be related to the high rate of cerebellar tumours in this group, as half of the paediatric brain tumours are located within the posterior fossa.34 PIQ subtests are of a multi-component nature and draw more heavily on motor functions, visuomotor integration, visual attention, abstract reasoning, and working memory than the VIQ subtests. Information about how many PBTS were excluded from the analyses because of a persistent motor or sensory disability was lacking. However, it is well known that many of these neurocognitive functions depend heavily on cerebellar functioning.61–63 Therefore damage to the cerebellum is expected to be related to depressed PIQ scores. Secondly, lower PIQ than VIQ scores may be related to a general slowing in information processing resulting from white matter damage. Compared with VIQ subtests, many PIQ subtests draw heavily on processing speed owing to their timed character. Although grey matter development peaks during childhood, white matter continues to develop gradually until early adulthood.59 The maturation of white matter is therefore challenged in PBTS because cancer therapy damages the healthy cells of the central nervous system. Glial progenitor cells are especially vulnerable to the effects of chemotherapy and radiotherapy.64 These cells are responsible for the formation of oligodendrocytes and astrocytes, both myelinating cells that are crucial for white matter integrity. White matter integrity plays a major role in information processing speed with decreased white matter integrity, resulting in slower processing speed.65

In the present meta-analysis, severe attentional problems in PBTS were revealed, as indicated by the high mean number of errors of omission committed by PBTS on the CPT compared with the normative sample. In addition to the errors of omission of the CPT, attention problems in PBTS have been reported using other measures of attention. PBTS perform below the norm on the Freedom of Distractibility Index, the attentional factor of the WISC-III.40 Also, measuring attention using the Gordon Diagnostic System, PBTS scores on focused attention have been reported to be almost 1SD below the mean of the normative sample

Page 39: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

37

2

and more than 1SD below the mean on selective attention.66 Other researchers have used the Conners’ Rating Scale for Parents and Teachers to identify attention deficits in PBTS; they found more than half of the patients obtained scores above the 75th centile, indicating attentional difficulties.67 Attentional abilities develop early in life and form the basis for other emerging and proliferating neurocognitive functions.59 As a result, attentional abilities are an important prerequisite for scholastic development and are strongly associated with academic achievement.59 Reddick et al.57 found worse attentional functioning in PBTS, measured with the CPT, to be associated with lower academic achievement scores, as measured with the abbreviated Wechsler Individual Achievement Test.

Interestingly, this meta-analysis found no evidence for slow processing speed in PBTS as measured with MHRT on the CPT. Individual studies revealed conflicting findings, with some researchers reporting worse performance of PBTS than the normative sample and others reporting no difference or even better performance by PBTS. Our failure to find evidence for slowed information processing as measured by the MHRT on the CPT does not fit with the frequently observed impairments in white matter integrity in PBTS. Such impairments are expected to result in slower processing speed.65 Perhaps MHRT is a limited measure for processing speed. It is possible that slowed information processing becomes evident only in tasks that place demands on the integration of multiple stimulus modalities and involve more complex mental operations, which would engage widespread neural networks in the brain. Speed on such complex tasks, for example the performance subtests of the WISC-III, draw more heavily on white matter integrity than simple stimulus response tasks such as the CPT. Indeed, several researchers have reported decreased scores of PBTS on the processing measures obtained with more complex tasks such as the speed index of the Wechsler Scale and the Trail Making Task.12,29,38

Inhibitory control seems to be spared in PBTS, as the number of errors of commission on the CPT was in the average range. This result converges with results of earlier studies into inhibitory control, which failed to find evidence for inhibitory problems.68,69 The absence of an effect on inhibitory control may be explained in terms of tumour location. Inhibition of responses is primarily mediated by the prefrontal cortex, and paediatric brain tumours in the frontal lobe are rare.59,70 On the other hand, there are a multitude of connections between the cerebellum and the frontal lobes, which can be damaged by local radiotherapy, so causing inhibitory problems.61 Also, frontal lobes are irradiated in craniospinal radiotherapy, for example for medulloblastomas. Indeed Aukema et al.29 found vulnerability of the white matter in the frontal lobes in survivors of medulloblastoma, which was associated with slower processing speed. The fact that we did not find inhibitory control problems might also be caused by the relatively slow development of the prefrontal cortex at the ages the patients were tested.59

Page 40: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

38

As described above, cancer treatment inevitably causes cell damage, challenging the normal maturation and myelination of the neural pathways in the young brain, and consequently challenging the development of cognitive, motor, behavioural, and emotional functioning.42,57,71,72 In our exploratory analyses we found that cranial radiotherapy and chemotherapy are indeed strong predictors of worse intellectual functioning. In our analyses it was impossible to make the distinction between different doses and volumes of radiotherapy, but it is known from the literature that higher doses and larger volumes are associated with worse neurocognitive outcomes.5 The combination of radiotherapy and chemotherapy in adults is believed to play an important role in neurocognitive deficits, owing to the induced damage to neural progenitor cells for hippocampal neurogenesis and the maintenance of subcortical white matter integrity.64 Moreover, we found that longer time since diagnosis is highly associated with worse intellectual outcomes. This finding is probably explained in terms of the neurocognitive problems such as reduced attentiveness, slower processing speed, and memory problems exhibited by PBTS that challenge the learning process, causing an increasing gap between PBTS and their peers in intellectual functioning. No effect was found for tumour location on intellectual functioning. Although the adverse effects of the tumour and its treatment are considered detrimental for the developing brain, interestingly no relationship was found between mean age at diagnosis and WISC-III scores of PBTS. Longer follow-ups might reveal effects of age at diagnosis.

This meta-analysis has some limitations that need to be taken into account. A limited number of studies were available for this meta-analysis. Also, the available studies did not allow a distinction to be made between the different brain tumour diagnoses, tumour locations, and treatment intensities. This may have contributed to heterogeneity in the study findings. For the quality analysis, we used the most relevant quality rating that we found. Nevertheless, not all criteria were applicable to the included studies, which potentially decreased the reliability of the quality ratings. Furthermore, it has been reported in the literature that there is an effect of sex in outcome, to the disadvantage of female children.72 However, owing to lack of variability in proportions of male and female children in the included studies, it was not possible to include sex in the analyses. It would have been interesting to compare scores of patients on the WISC-III and the CPT; unfortunately none of the included studies reported on both a full WISC-III and the CPT. In addition, with one single exception, the included studies used an uncontrolled study design or compared PBTS with other patient groups.29 Also, working memory and other important cognitive areas have not been addressed in this meta-analysis, despite their interdependence with intelligence, attention, and processing speed. Even though the WISC-III and the CPT are well-validated tests and normative data are available, the use of a healthy comparison group, matched on demographic background characteristics, would have been preferable.

Page 41: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

39

2

CONCLUSIONS

This meta-analysis highlights the negative neurocognitive sequelae of paediatric brain tumours and their treatment in terms of intellectual functioning and attentiveness. Moreover, longer time since diagnosis was found to be associated with worse intellectual functioning. Poor intellectual functioning and inattentiveness might underlie the negative outcomes of PBTS in terms of academic achievement, vocational success, and general adaptive functioning. The field is in urgent need of developing effective screening and treatments for these negative neurocognitive sequelae of PBTS.

Page 42: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

40

REFERENCES

1. Kaatsch P. Epidemiology of childhood cancer. Cancer Treat Rev. 2010;36(4):277–85. 2. Mariotto AB, Rowland JH, Yabroff KR, Scoppa S, Hachey M, Ries L, et al. Long-term survivors of

childhood cancers in the United States. Cancer Epidemiol biomarkers Prev. 2009;18(4):1033–40. 3. Ries LAG, Melbert D, Krapcho M. SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975-2005. Bethesda, MD:

National Cancer Institute; 2008. 4. Butler RW, Mulhern RK. Neurocognitive Interventions for Children and Adolescents Surviving

Cancer. J Pediatr Psychol. 2005;30(1):65–78. 5. Duffner PK. Risk factors for cognitive decline in children treated for brain tumors. Eur J Paediatr

Neurol. 2010;14(2):106–15. 6. Bhat SR, Goodwin TL, Burwinkle TM, Lansdale MF, Dahl G V, Huhn SL, et al. Profile of daily

life in children with brain tumors: an assessment of health-related quality of life. J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(24):5493–500.

7. Koch S V, Kejs AM, Engholm G, Johansen C, Schmiegelow K. Educational attainment among survivors of childhood cancer: a population-based cohort study in Denmark. Br J Cancer. 2004;91(5):923–8.

8. Zebrack BJ, Gurney JG, Oeffinger K, Whitton J, Packer RJ, Mertens A, et al. Psychological outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood brain cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22(6):999–1006.

9. Grill J, Kieffer Renaux V, Bulteau C, Viguier D, Levy-Piebois C, Sainte-Rose C, et al. Long-term intellectual outcome in children with posterior fossa tumors according to radiation doses and volumes. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 1999;45(1):137–45.

10. Conklin HM, Li C, Xiong X, Ogg RJ, Merchant TE. Predicting change in academic abilities after conformal radiation therapy for localized ependymoma. J Clin Oncol. 2008;26(24):3965–70.

11. Rutkowski S, Bode U, Deinlein F, Ottensmeier H, Warmuth-Metz M, Soerensen N, et al. Treatment of early childhood medulloblastoma by postoperative chemotherapy alone. N Engl J Med. 2005;352(10):978–86.

12. Patel SK, Mullins WA, O’Neil SH, Wilson K. Neuropsychological differences between survivors of supratentorial and infratentorial brain tumours. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2011;55(1):30–40.

13. Ris MD, Packer RJ, Goldwein J, Jones-Wallace D, Boyett JM. Intellectual outcome after reduced-dose radiation therapy plus adjuvant chemotherapy for medulloblastoma: a Children’s Cancer Group study. J Clin Oncol. 2001;19(15):3470–6.

14. Palmer SL, Goloubeva O, Reddick WE, Glass JO, Gajjar A, Kun LE, et al. Patterns of intellectual development among survivors of pediatric medulloblastoma: a longitudinal analysis. J Clin Oncol. 2001;19(8):2302–8.

15. Wechsler D. Wechsler’s Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd Edition. London: Harcourt Brace and Company; 1992.

16. Conners CK. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT). Canada: Multi-Health Systems; 1995. 17. Conners CK. Conners’ Continuous Performance Test (CPT II). New York: Multi-Health Systems;

2002. 18. Hardy KK, Willard VW, Bonner MJ. Computerized cognitive training in survivors of childhood

cancer: A pilot study. J Pediatr Oncol Nurs. 2011;28(1):27–33. 19. O’Neil S, Ji L, Buranahirun C, Azoff J, Dhall G, Khatua S, et al. Neurocognitive outcomes in

pediatric and adolescent patients with central nervous system germinoma treated with a strategy of chemotherapy followed by reduced-dose and volume irradiation. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2011;57(4):669–73.

Page 43: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

41

2

20. Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, Norms and Commentary. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford university press; 2006.

21. Wells GA, Shea B, O’Connell D, Robertson J, Peterson J, Welch V, et al. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for Assessing the Quality of Nonrandomized Studies in Meta-Analysis. 2010.

22. Borenstein M, Rothstein H. Comprehensive meta-analysis, a computer program for research synthesis. 2nd ed. Englewood, NJ: Biostat Inc; 1999.

23. Jalali R, Goswami S, Sarin R, More N, Siddha M, Kamble R. Neuropsychological status in children and young adults with benign and low-grade brain tumors treated prospectively with focal stereotactic conformal radiotherapy. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2006;66(4 SUPPL.):S14–9.

24. Lacaze E, Kieffer V, Streri A, Lorenzi C, Gentaz E, Habrand J-L, et al. Neuropsychological outcome in children with optic pathway tumours when first-line treatment is chemotherapy. Br J Cancer. 2003;89(11):2038–44.

25. Merchant TE, Sherwood SH, Mulhern RK, Rose SR, Thompson SJ, Sanford RA, et al. CNS germinoma: disease control and long-term functional outcome for 12 children treated with craniospinal irradiation. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2000;46(5):1171–6.

26. Ris MD, Beebe DW, Armstrong FD, Fontanesi J, Holmes E, Sanford RA, et al. Cognitive and adaptive outcome in extracerebellar low-grade brain tumors in children: a report from the Children’s Oncology Group. J Clin Oncol. 2008;26(29):4765–70.

27. Hozo SP, Djulbegovic B, Hozo I. Estimating the mean and variance from the median, range, and the size of a sample. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2005;5(1):13.

28. Cohen J. The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: a review. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1962;65:145–53.

29. Aukema EJ, Caan M, Oudhuis N, Majoie C, Vos F, Reneman L, et al. White matter fractional anisotropy correlates with speed of processing and motor speed in young childhood cancer survivors. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2009;74(3):837–43.

30. Gliner JA, Morgan GA, Harmon RJ. Meta-analysis: formulation and interpretation. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;42(11):1376–9.

31. Huedo-Medina TB, Sanchez-Meca J, Marin-Martinez F, Botella J. Assessing heterogeneity in meta-analysis: Q statistic or I2 index? Psychol Methods. 2006;11(2):193–206.

32. Conklin HM, Khan RB, Reddick WE, Helton S, Brown R, Howard SC, et al. Acute neurocognitive response to methylphenidate among survivors of childhood cancer: a randomized, double-blind, cross-over trial. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(9):1127–39.

33. Mulhern RK, Palmer SL, Reddick WE, Glass JO, Kun LE, Taylor J, et al. Risks of young age for selected neurocognitive deficits in medulloblastoma are associated with white matter loss. J Clin Oncol. 2001;19:472–9.

34. Spiegler BJ, Bouffet E, Greenberg ML, Rutka JT, Mabbott DJ. Change in neurocognitive functioning after treatment with cranial radiation in childhood. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22(4):706–13.

35. Rosenthal R. Writing meta-analystic reviews. Psychol Bull. 1995;118(2):183–92. 36. Saury JMG, Emanuelson I. Cognitive Consequences of the Treatment of Medulloblastoma

Among Children. Pediatr Neurol. 2011;44(1):21–30. 37. Sands SA, Oberg JA, Gardner SL, Whiteley JA, Glade-Bender JL, Finlay JL. Neuropsychological

functioning of children treated with intensive chemotherapy followed by myeloablative consolidation chemotherapy and autologous hematopoietic cell rescue for newly diagnosed CNS tumors: An analysis of the head start II survivors. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2010;54(3):429–36.

38. Bonner MJ, Hardy KK, Willard VW, Gururangan S. Additional Evidence of a Nonverbal Learning Disability in Survivors of Pediatric Brain Tumors. Child Heal Care. 2009;38(1):49–63.

39. Callu D, Viguier D, Laroussinie F, Puget S, Boddaert N, Kieffer V, et al. Cognitive and academic outcome after benign or malignant cerebellar tumor in children. Cogn Behav Neurol. 2009;22(4):270–8.

Page 44: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 2

42

40. Briere M-EE, Scott JG, McNall-Knapp RY, Adams RL. Cognitive outcome in pediatric brain tumor survivors: delayed attention deficit at long-term follow-up. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2008;50(2):337–40.

41. Sanders RP, Kocak M, Burger PC, Merchant TE, Gajjar A, Broniscer A. High-grade astrocytoma in very young children. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2007;49(7):888–93.

42. Khong P-L, Leung LHT, Fung ASM, Fong DYT, Qiu D, Kwong DLW, et al. White matter anisotropy in post-treatment childhood cancer survivors: preliminary evidence of association with neurocognitive function. J Clin Oncol. 2006;24(6):884–90.

43. Beebe DW, Ris MD, Armstrong FD, Fontanesi J, Mulhern RK, Holmes E, et al. Cognitive and adaptive outcome in low-grade pediatric cerebellar astrocytomas: Evidence of diminished cognitive and adaptive functioning in national collaborative research studies (CCG 9891/POG 9130). J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(22):5198–204.

44. Grill J, Viguier D, Kieffer V, Bulteau C, Sainte-Rose C, Hartmann O, et al. Critical risk factors for intellectual impairment in children with posterior fossa tumors: the role of cerebellar damage. J Neurosurg. 2004;101(2 Suppl):152–8.

45. Packer RJ. Radiation-induced neurocognitive decline: the risks and benefits of reducing the amount of whole-brain irradiation. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep. 2002;2(2):131–3.

46. Carey ME, Barakat LP, Foley B, Gyato K, Phillips PC. Neuropsychological functioning and social functioning of survivors of pediatric brain tumors: Evidence of nonverbal learning disability. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;7(4):265–72.

47. Kieffer-Renaux V, Bulteau C, Grill J, Kalifa C, Viguier D, Jambaque I. Patterns of neuropsychological deficits in children with medulloblastoma according to craniospatial irradiation doses. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2000;42(11):741–5.

48. Mulhern RK, Reddick WE, Palmer SL, Glass JO, Elkin TD, Kun LE, et al. Neurocognitive deficits in medulloblastoma survivors and white matter loss. Ann Neurol. 1999;46(6):834–41.

49. Hazin I, Dellatolas G, Garcia D, Pedrosa F, Pedrosa A. Intellectual impairment after treatment for medulloblastoma and astrocytoma in childhood: the Brazilian experience. J Pediatr Hematol Oncol. 2011;33(7):506–15.

50. Butler RW, Copeland DR, Fairclough DL, Mulhern RK, Katz ER, Kazak AE, et al. A multicenter, randomized clinical trial of a cognitive remediation program for childhood survivors of a pediatric malignancy. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76(3):367–78.

51. Mabbott DJ, Penkman L, Witol A. Core neurocognitive functions in children treated for posterior fossa tumors. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(2):159–68.

52. Stargatt R, Rosenfeld J V, Maixner W, Ashley DM. Multiple factors contribute to neuropsychological outcome in children with posterior fossa tumors. Dev Neuropsychol. 2007;32(2):729–48.

53. Reeves CB, Palmer SL, Reddick WE, Merchant TE, Buchanan GM, Gajjar A, et al. Attention and memory functioning among pediatric patients with medulloblastoma. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31(3):272–80.

54. Mulhern RK, White HA, Glass JO, Kun LE, Leigh L, Thompson SJ, et al. Attentional functioning and white matter integrity among survivors of malignant brain tumors of childhood. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:180–9.

55. Deary IJ, Strand S, Smith P, Fernandes C. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence. 2007;35(1):13–21.

56. Watkins MW, Lei P, Canivez GL. Psychometric intelligence and achievement: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Intelligence. 2007;35(1):59–68.

57. Reddick WE, White H a, Glass JO, Wheeler GC, Thompson SJ, Gajjar A, et al. Developmental model relating white matter volume to neurocognitive deficits in pediatric brain tumor survivors. Cancer. 2003;97(10):2512–9.

Page 45: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurocognitive consequences of a brain tumor

43

2

58. de Boer AGEM, Verbeek JH a M, van Dijk FJH. Adult survivors of childhood cancer and unemployment: A metaanalysis. Cancer. 2006;107(1):1–11.

59. Anderson V, Northam E, Hendy J, Wrennall J. Developmental neuropsychology; A clinical approach. East Sussex: Psychology Press Ltd; 2001.

60. Johnson S. Cognitive and behavioural outcomes following very preterm birth. Semin fetal neonatal Med. 2007;12(5):363–73.

61. Cantelmi D, Schweizer TA, Cusimano MD. Role of the cerebellum in the neurocognitive sequelae of treatment of tumours of the posterior fossa: an update. Lancet Oncol. 2008;9(6):569–76.

62. Salman MS. The cerebellum: it’s about time! But timing is not everything--new insights into the role of the cerebellum in timing motor and cognitive tasks. J Child Neurol. 2002 Jan 1;17(1):1–9.

63. Copeland DR, DeMoor C, Moore III B, Ater J. Neurocognitive development of children after a cerebellar tumor in infancy: A longitudinal study. J Clin Oncol. 1999;17(11):3476–86.

64. Monje M, Dietrich J. Cognitive side effects of cancer therapy demonstrate a functional role for adult neurogenesis. Behav Brain Res. 2012;227:376–9.

65. Dockstader C, Gaetz W, Rockel C, Mabbott DJ. White matter maturation in visual and motor areas predicts the latency of visual activation in children. Hum Brain Mapp. 2012;33:179–91.

66. Dennis M, Hetherington CR, Spiegler BJ. Memory and attention after childhood brain tumors. Med Pediatr Oncol. 1998;Suppl 1:25–33.

67. Netson KL, Conklin HM, Ashford JM, Kahalley LS, Wu S, Xiong X. Parent and Teacher Ratings of Attention during a Year-Long Methylphenidate Trial in Children Treated for Cancer. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010;36(4):438–50.

68. Kahalley LS, Conklin HM, Tyc VL, Wilson SJ, Hinds PS, Wu S, et al. ADHD and secondary ADHD criteria fail to identify many at-risk survivors of pediatric ALL and brain tumor. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2011;57(1):110–8.

69. Kiehna EN, Mulhern RK, Li C, Xiong X, Merchant TE. Changes in attentional performance of children and young adults with localized primary brain tumors after conformal radiation therapy. J Clin Oncol. 2006;24(33):5283–90.

70. Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Taylor E. Right inferior prefrontal cortex mediates response inhibition while mesial prefrontal cortex is responsible for error detection. Neuroimage. 2003;20(1):351–8.

71. Han R, Yang YM, Dietrich J, Luebke A, Mayer-Proschel M, Noble M. Systemic 5-fluorouracil treatment causes a syndrome of delayed myelin destruction in the central nervous system. J Biol. 2008;7(4):12.

72. Mulhern RK, Merchant TE, Gajjar A, Reddick WE, Kun LE. Late neurocognitive sequelae in survivors of brain tumours in childhood. Lancet Oncol. 2004;5(7):399–408.

Page 46: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

Page 47: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

3

Neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive

functioning of children treated for a brain tumor:

design of a randomized controlled double-blind trial

Marieke A. de RuiterAntoinette Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren

Rosa van MourikTieme W.P. Janssen

Juliette E.M. GreidanusJaap Oosterlaan

Martha A. Grootenhuis

BMC Cancer 2012, 12:581

Page 48: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 3

46

ABSTRACT

Background: Neurotoxicity caused by treatment for a brain tumor is a major cause of neurocognitive decline in survivors. Studies have shown that neurofeedback may enhance neurocognitive functioning. This paper describes the protocol of the PRISMA study, a randomized controlled trial to investigate the efficacy of neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive functioning in children treated for a brain tumor.

Methods/design: Efficacy of neurofeedback will be compared to placebo training in a randomized controlled double-blind trial. A total of 70 brain tumor survivors in the age range of 8 to 18 years will be recruited. Inclusion also requires caregiver-reported neurocognitive problems and being off treatment for more than two years. A group of 35 healthy siblings will be included as the control group. On the basis of a qEEG patients will be assigned to one of three treatment protocols. Thereafter patients will be randomized to receive either neurofeedback training (n=35) or placebo training (n=35). Neurocognitive tests, and questionnaires administered to the patient, caregivers, and teacher, will be used to evaluate pre- and post-intervention functioning, as well as at 6-month follow-up. Siblings will be administered the same tests and questionnaires once.

Discussion: If neurofeedback proves to be effective for pediatric brain tumor survivors, this can be a valuable addition to the scarce interventions available to improve neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT00961922

Page 49: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Design RCT

47

3

BACKGROUND

As a result of improved treatment, the survival rate of children diagnosed with a brain tumor has increased considerably.1 As a consequence, neurocognitive long-term effects of the tumor and the treatment are reported more often, including deficits in attention, processing speed, and memory.2–4 Radiotherapy, chemotherapy, tumor location, and longer time since diagnosis are related to worse neurocognitive functioning.5,6 A major consequence of these impairments is the decline in ability to acquire new skills and information, which leads to an increasing gap in the development between patients and their peers. This, in turn, has its impact on educational results, vocational success and may compromise social competence and quality of life.7

Butler and Mulhern have emphasized that interventions should be developed to improve neurocognitive functioning and subsequently improve future perspectives of these children.8 Interventions that are considered relevant for survivors with cancer-related brain injury are cognitive remediation and pharmacotherapy.9,10 A cognitive remediation program, using techniques from three disciplines: brain injury rehabilitation, special education and clinical psychology, has been developed and tested by Butler and colleagues.9 Participants in the randomized controlled trial were 161 survivors of a childhood cancer, whose malignancy and/or treatment involved the central nervous system. The results showed improvements in caregiver reported attention and academic achievement, although the effect sizes were modest. Van ‘t Hooft et al. have investigated the effects of a cognitive training program on neurocognitive function with a randomized controlled trial, enrolling 38 patients with acquired brain injury, including 14 brain tumor survivors.10 The training program consisted of memory and attention exercises, in combination with cognitive behavioral training. The children in the treatment group showed sustained positive effects on memory and attention functioning until six months after the training, but not on processing speed.Regarding pharmacotherapy, it has been suggested that survivors of childhood cancer may benefit from stimulant medication as used in the treatment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Attention deficits in survivors of brain tumors are likely to improve by methylphenidate. Mulhern and colleagues found improvements of attention in 37 long term survivors of a malignant brain tumor after methylphenidate.11 In a randomized placebo-controlled double-blinded trial including 32 survivors of a brain tumor (n=25) or acute lymphoblastic leukemia (n=7), Thompson et al. found that methylphenidate led to improved sustained attention.12 A drawback of pharmacotherapy is the possibility of side effects, e.g. sleep disturbance, weight loss, anxiety, and sadness.13 Also, this medication does not lead to a sustained effect unless the patient continues the pharmacotherapy.The limited current available options warrant the search for alternatives. Neurofeedback is a relatively new form of therapy, which has never been investigated in pediatric brain tumor

Page 50: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 3

48

survivors. Neurofeedback is a behavioral intervention that is based on the principles of operant conditioning. During the therapy the patient is presented with real-time feedback on his or her brainwaves, as measured by one or more electrodes on the scalp. The patient is reinforced when the brain produces a certain desired wave. Reinforcement may comprise seeing a movie or hearing music. The desired brain wave is determined by a quantified electro encephalogram (qEEG), which is conducted prior to the training.The effects of neurofeedback have been discovered serendipitously by Sterman, when cats having received feedback of 12–15 Hz on the motor cortex showed to be less susceptible to epileptic seizures.14 There is a large body of scientific research documenting the effectiveness of neurofeedback for the treatment of diverse pathological conditions as summarized in comprehensive reviews, including ADHD, traumatic brain injury and schizophrenia.15–19

Strehl et al. showed that children with ADHD were able to learn to regulate their brain activity by neurofeedback.20 After training, significant improvements in behavior, attention, and IQ scores were found. All changes proved to be stable at six months follow-up after the end of training. Hodgson et al. conclude in their meta-analysis on nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD that neurofeedback resulted in significant improvements of DSM-IV symptoms of ADHD, neurocognitive functioning and behavior.18 In a comparative study researchers found that the positive effects of neurofeedback for children with ADHD were superior to a computerized attention training at six months follow up.21 However, to date there is a lack of published studies that employ a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind design when investigating neurofeedback.22

Brain tumor survivors differ from ADHD patients, as they have structural brain damage caused by the tumor, surgery, radiotherapy and/or chemotherapy. An indication that neurofeedback might be effective in pediatric brain tumor survivors may be derived from the results of studies into the effects of neurofeedback in patients with traumatic brain injury. A review of Thornton and colleagues23 describes a total of 44 studies (12 RCT, 16 comparative, 16 correlation) with traumatic brain injury patients reporting improved attention, cognitive flexibility, cognitive performance, and problem solving after neurofeedback, providing strong initial support for the idea that neurofeedback could be used in patients with structural brain damage. Subsequently, Aukema and colleagues conducted a pilot study into the feasibility of neurofeedback on 9 brain tumor survivors in our hospital.24 This study demonstrated that it was feasible to use neurofeedback with brain tumor survivors. All participants completed the training and were positive about the training they received, as they would recommend it to others. Patients reported decreased subjective fatigue after the training. Also the test results showed that processing speed improved in 6 out of 9 patients. These findings warranted the set up of a larger study into the effectiveness of neurofeedback for pediatric brain tumor survivors.

Page 51: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Design RCT

49

3

The current paper describes the protocol of the PRISMA study (pediatric research on improving speed, memory, and attention); a randomized controlled double-blind trial, approved by the medical ethical committee of the Academic Medical Centre in Amsterdam. The primary aim of the PRISMA study is to investigate the efficacy of neurofeedback for improving neurocognitive functioning after treatment for a pediatric brain tumor. Secondary, we hypothesize that subsequent to the expected neurocognitive changes achieved with neurofeedback, children will experience improved psychosocial functioning.25 Neurocognitive functioning will be investigated by tests administered to the patient. Psychosocial functioning will be measured using patient-reported as well as caregiver and teacher reported questionnaires. Assessments will take place pre and post training, as well as six months post training, in order to examine the long-term effects of the training. Comparing the effects of neurofeedback to placebo feedback will assess efficacy of neurofeedback. Pre training results obtained with the brain tumor survivors will be compared to a control group of healthy siblings, to assess the level of dysfunction on the measures used in this study.

METHODS

Study designThis study is a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind trial, to investigate whether neurofeedback improves neurocognitive functioning in children who have received treatment for a brain tumor (trial number clinicaltrials.gov NCT00961922). After enrollment, patients will be randomized into two groups: (1a) the experimental group, receiving neurofeedback, and (1b) the placebo group, receiving placebo training. In addition, (2) a control group of healthy siblings is included; this group will not receive any training. If effectiveness of neurofeedback is demonstrated after completion of the study, patients in the placebo group will be given the opportunity to receive neurofeedback.

ParticipantsEligible for inclusion are patients in the Netherlands, aged 8 to 18 years, who finished treatment for a brain tumor at least two years prior to enrolment and who suffer from problems in neurocognitive functioning. Problems in neurocognitive functioning include attention problems, problems with information processing speed and/or memory problems as assessed by caregiver report. Exclusion criteria are premorbid diagnosis of ADHD or ADD, a mental or physical condition that prohibits neurocognitive assessment and insufficient mastery of the Dutch language. Siblings, aged between 8 and 18 years, form the control group.

Page 52: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 3

50

InterventionThe neurofeedback training is performed at home or school using a Dell notebook (Inspiron N5030, 15.6 inch screen), with BioExplorer software, version 1.5 installed, and a portable Brainquiry PET neurofeedback device.26,27 Reinforcement is provided by a self-selected movie that will be presented on the screen if the brain produces the desired activity, as detected by an electrode placed at Cz (see Figure 1). Each patient receives two sessions weekly for 15 weeks, 30 sessions in total. Each session takes 39 minutes to administer, divided in ten blocks of three-minutes training, alternated with one-minute breaks. In the breaks the patient will be instructed to sit quietly with the eyes closed.The neurofeedback sessions are hosted by extensively trained research assistants who have successfully completed a full day schooling session on administration of the neurofeedback training in accordance with detailed standard operating procedures. During the first neurofeedback session, the research assistant will be accompanied by one of the researchers to ensure adherence to the standard operating procedures. After each session, the research assistant is required to fill out a checklist providing information about the training that includes items on start and finish time of the training, duration of the session, selected movie, alertness of the patient, and any deviations from the standard procedures. Checklists are e-mailed to the researchers on a weekly basis.

40

Nose

CzC3

Fcz

C4

Cpz

Figure 1. EEG locations. Patients in PRISMA are trained on location Cz. Location Cz is the location exactly halfway between the nasion (the bridge of the nose) and the inion (the most prominent point on the lower rear of the skull) and halfway between the two ears.

Figure 1. EEG locations. Patients in PRISMA are trained on location Cz. Location Cz is the location exactly halfway between the nasion (the bridge of the nose) and the inion (the most prominent point on the lower rear of the skull) and halfway between the two ears.

Page 53: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Design RCT

51

3

Neurofeedback treatment modulesThe neurofeedback treatment modules were developed in the software program BioExplorer. To increase comparability, we decided to develop three standard treatment modules based on the qEEGs from the pilot study,24 rather than designing an individualized treatment module for each participant. The three treatment modules are (1) beta 1 up training, (2) sensory motor rhythm (SMR) up/beta 1 down training, and (3) beta 1 down training. The qEEG of the patient determines the most suitable of the three neurofeedback treatment modules. The mean Z-score for the power in the beta 1 frequency band (15–20 Hz) for the electrodes on locations Fcz, Cz, C3, C4 and Cpz are calculated (see Figure 1). For SMR no Z-scores are provided in the brain resource report. SMR power is calculated and p-values are obtained over the average of 9 electrodes (F3, Fz, F4, C3, Cz, C4, P3, Pz and P4). The beta 1 up training is given if the beta 1 power is within the normal range (within 1 standard deviation from the mean) or lowered (more than 1 standard deviation below the mean). The SMR up/beta 1 down training is chosen if the beta 1 power is elevated (more than 1 standard deviation above the mean) and SMR (12–15 Hz) is within the normal range (p>0.05) or lowered (p<0.05). The beta 1 down training is chosen if the patient has beta spindles, ascertained by an EEG specialist via observation, or if both beta 1 is elevated (more than 1 standard deviation above the mean) and SMR power is elevated (p<0.05). Three identical placebo treatment modules were matched to the three neurofeedback treatment modules; beta 1 up placebo, SMR up/beta 1 down placebo and beta 1 down placebo. In the placebo treatment modules, the provided reward of a movie is not based on the desired brain waves from the patient, but on the ‘random signal generator’ that is incorporated in the BioExplorer software.All treatment modules are set at an automatic threshold of 80% reward. This means that the threshold of reward is regularly adjusted in a way that the child sees the movie approximately 80% of the time and 20% of the time the screen of the laptop briefly turns black. Elevated muscular tension and electrical noise from surrounding devices (e.g. a lamp) can decrease the quality of the training; therefore we employ a threshold of 10 μV for muscular tension (> 55 Hz) and 50 μV for noise (range 48–52 Hz). If the muscular tension and/or the noise reach above the threshold, the movie is interrupted and the computer makes a beeping sound. The training will not continue until the muscular tension and/or noise are brought back under the threshold. This applies to the neurofeedback treatment modules as well as the placebo treatment modules.

RandomizationThe three neurofeedback treatment modules and three placebo treatment modules have been designed to have the exact same appearance on the screen of the notebook, in order to be indistinguishable during training. The six treatment modules have randomly been

Page 54: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 3

52

assigned a number (treatment module 1–6). J.O., one of the members of the research team, holds the key to the codes; the other members are blinded. Another member of the research team analyzes the qEEG and informs J.O. which of the three neurofeedback treatment modules is indicated according to the protocol (see Figure 2). J.O. is then responsible for randomizing the patient into the actual neurofeedback or the placebo training using a randomization table generated by SPSS. For stratfication purposes, randomization takes place after selection of the most appropriate neurofeedback treatment module. After randomization, J.O. notifies M.d.R. of the assigned treatment module (treatment module 1–6) and M.d.R. sends the assigned treatment module via email to the designated research assistant providing the neurofeedback training. Due to his not-blind status, J.O. is not involved in training any patients or the processing of the data.

42

Sent recruitment letter

Screened for participationOnline screening

Refused participation

Non respons

Inclusion

ExclusionNo reported neurocognitive problemsPremorbid diagnosis AD/HDPhysical or mental disability that makes testing impossible

Randomisation

Selection most favorable training based on qEEG

Beta 1 up SMR up Spindles down

Randomisation Randomisation

Training Placebo Training Placebo Training Placebo

T1 assessmentNeurocognitive assessmentOnline questionnaires (patient, parent, teacher)qEEG

T0 assessmentNeurocognitive assessmentOnline questionnaires (patient, parent, teacher)qEEG

T2 assessmentNeurocognitive assessmentOnline questionnaires (patient, parent, teacher)qEEG

Month 0

Patients8 – 18 years oldat least 2 yrs post treatment

Month 1

Months 2 - 5

Month 6

Month 12

Control group Sibling8-18 years old

T0 assessmentNeurocognitive assessmentOnline Questionnaires (sibling)

Figure 2. Flow chart of the PRISMA study. Figure 2. Flow chart of the PRISMA study.

Page 55: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Design RCT

53

3

PROCEDURE

RecruitmentFive out of seven Dutch University hospitals accepted the invitation to join the study. Participating hospitals are the Emma children’s hospital/Academic Medical Centre in Amsterdam, VU medical centre in Amsterdam, university medical centre Utrecht in Utrecht, St. Radboud university medical centre in Nijmegen and university medical centre Maastricht in Maastricht. A letter via their oncologist or psychologist informs patients and their caregivers about the study. Interested caregivers will be provided with a screening questionnaire concerning their child’s neurocognitive functioning (including items on attention functioning, memory and speed) and exclusion criteria (e.g. premorbid diagnosis of ADHD or ADD) to verify eligibility of the patient. If eligible for inclusion, the patient is invited for the pre training assessment. If applicable, a sibling will also be invited for assessment to participate in the control group. On the day of the pre training assessment, the informed consent form is signed by caregivers, the patient, and if applicable, the sibling.

AssessmentAssessments are conducted at one of the three cooperating EEG centers in the Netherlands; Pels institute in Amsterdam, Brainfact in Amsterdam, and EEG resource institute in Nijmegen. Patients are assessed on three occasions: pre training (T0), directly post training (T1), and six months post training (T2); see Figure 2. Assessments include neurocognitive testing, questionnaires filled out by patient, caregiver, and teacher, and a qEEG, see Table 1. Assessment of the siblings occurs only once, and is identical to the assessments used in patients with the exception of questionnaires filled out by parents and teachers.

qEEGAn EEG is recorded from the patients at three time points. A Quick-cap with NuAmp 10–20 electrodes international system from neuroscan is used, with 28 channels;40 Fp1, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FC3, FCz, FC4, T3, C3, Cz, C4, T4, CP3, CPz, CP4, T5, P3, Pz, P4, T6, O1, Oz and O2. During the first three minutes an eyes-open resting EEG is registered, in the consecutive three minutes an eyes-closed resting EEG. After the resting EEG, event related potentials are measured during an odd ball and a go-nogo task. The Brain Resource International Database (BRID),41,42 comprising EEG power spectra of over 4.000 healthy controls, provides normative data to quantify the EEG (qEEG) and obtain Z-scores for the participants in the current study.

Neurocognitive testsTo objectify the primary hypothesis of the study, that neurofeedback will improve neurocognitive functioning, different neurocognitive domains are assessed. The tests are

Page 56: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 3

54

conducted by one of the researchers or extensively trained research assistants and take approximately two and a half hours. Based on literature describing late effects in brain tumor patients, the following neurocognitive domains were targeted for assessment:43 attention, processing speed, memory, intellectual functioning, inhibition, and visuomotor integration. Well-validated computerized and pencil-and-paper tests were selected to provide a comprehensive assessment of neurocognitive functioning before the training and the efficacy of neurofeedback (see Table 1).

QuestionnairesOur secondary hypothesis regards the impact of neuropsychological performance on psychosocial functioning.25,44 Based on studies reports, the following domains are assessed using questionnaires: social/emotional functioning, self-esteem, and health related quality of life. Because of the reported decrease in fatigue after training in the pilot study, we also included questionnaires assessing fatigue and sleep disturbance.24 In addition, two questionnaires on attention and executive functioning were added to assess caregiver and teacher rated neurocognitive functioning. Widely used, reliable, and validated questionnaires were selected in order to assess the domains of interest, as well as the effect of neurofeedback on these domains (see Table 1). Questionnaires were administered to either patient, caregivers or teacher, if applicable. The online questionnaires take approximately 30 minutes to fill out. In addition, as an interim measurement, caregivers fill in the attention questionnaire (SWAN)38 one extra time, after the first 10 sessions of the patient.

Power calculationPower calculations used the neurocognitive measures as primary outcome measures. The calculations were done in the statistical program nQuery Advisor.45 We expect that the neurofeedback will have a medium (d=0.5) to large effect (d=0.8) on neurocognitive functioning as measured at T1 and compared to T0, based on improvements found in children with ADHD who were trained with neurofeedback and on the improvements found in learning-impaired childhood cancer survivors treated with methylphenidate.12,15 Given an effect size of 0.6 with alpha set at 0.05 (one-sided) and a power of 0.80, a minimum of 35 patients is required in both the neurofeedback group and the placebo group.

Page 57: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Design RCT

55

3

Tabl

e 1.

Out

com

es, m

easu

res

and

to w

hom

it is

adm

inis

tere

d

Neu

roco

gniti

ve a

sses

smen

t M

easu

rem

ent

T0T1

T2Att

entio

nAtt

entio

n N

etw

ork

task

(AN

T) 28

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Pa

tient

(8-1

8)

Proc

essi

ng s

peed

Base

line

Spee

d A

NT

28Pa

tient

/sib

ling

(8-1

8)Pa

tient

(8-1

8)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

M

emor

yVi

sual

Seq

uenc

ing

task

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Pa

tient

(8-1

8)

Dig

it Sp

an (a

ge a

ppro

pria

te W

echs

ler

scal

e) 29

,30

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Pa

tient

(8-1

8)

Inte

llect

ual f

uncti

onin

gA

bbre

viat

ed W

ISC-

III*

29Pa

tient

/sib

ling

(8-1

6)-

Patie

nt (8

-16)

A

bbre

viat

ed W

AIS

-III*

30Pa

tient

/sib

ling

(17-

18)

-Pa

tient

(17-

18)

Inhi

bitio

nSt

op S

igna

l tas

k 31

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Pa

tient

(8-1

8)

Visu

omot

or in

tegr

ation

Trac

king

and

Pur

suit

task

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Que

stion

naire

sSo

cial

/em

otion

al fu

nctio

ning

SDQ

chi

ld v

ersi

on 32

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Pa

tient

(8-1

8)

SDQ

par

ent v

ersi

on 32

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Care

give

r (8

-18)

SDQ

teac

her

vers

ion

32Te

ache

r (8

-18)

Teac

her

(8-1

8)Te

ache

r (8

-18)

Self

este

emD

utch

ver

sion

SPP

C 33

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-11)

Patie

nt (8

-11)

Patie

nt (8

-11)

Dut

ch v

ersi

on S

PPA

34Pa

tient

/sib

ling

(12-

18)

Patie

nt (1

2-18

)Pa

tient

(12-

18)

Hea

lth re

late

d qu

ality

of l

ifeKi

dscr

een

27 35

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Pa

tient

(8-1

8)

Fatig

ueCI

S 36

Patie

nt/s

iblin

g (8

-18)

Patie

nt (8

-18)

Pa

tient

(8-1

8)

Slee

p di

sord

erSD

SC 37

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Atten

tion

SWA

N 38

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Exec

utive

func

tioni

ngBR

IEF

pare

nt v

ersi

on 39

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Care

give

r (8

-18)

Care

give

r (8

-18)

BR

IEF

teac

her

vers

ion

39Te

ache

r (8

-18)

Teac

her

(8-1

8)Te

ache

r (8

-18)

Not

e. In

telle

ctua

l fun

ction

ing

is a

sses

sed

at T

0 an

d T2

but

not

at T

1. S

iblin

gs a

re a

sses

sed

at T

0 on

ly.

*The

follo

win

g su

btas

ks w

ere

adm

inis

tere

d: A

rith

meti

c, S

imila

ritie

s, B

lock

Des

ign,

and

Pic

ture

Com

pleti

on.

SDQ

= S

tren

gths

and

Diffi

culti

es Q

uesti

onna

ires;

SPP

C/SP

PA =

Sel

f Per

cepti

on P

rofil

e fo

r Chi

ldre

n/A

dole

scen

ts; C

IS =

Che

cklis

t Ind

ivid

ual S

tren

gth;

Sle

ep

Dis

turb

ance

Sca

le fo

r Ch

ildre

n; S

WA

N =

Str

engt

hs a

nd W

eakn

esse

s of

AD

HD

-sym

ptom

s an

d N

orm

al-b

ehav

ior;

BRI

EF =

Beh

avio

r Ra

ting

Inve

ntor

y of

Ex

ecuti

ve F

uncti

onin

g; W

ISC-

III =

Wec

hsle

r Int

ellig

ence

Sca

le fo

r Chi

ldre

n –

Third

ver

sion

; WA

IS-II

I = W

echs

ler A

dult

Inte

llige

nce

Scal

e –

Third

ver

sion

.

Page 58: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 3

56

Statistical analysesIntention-to-treat analyses will be conducted. Because of possible withdrawal before treatment starts, dropouts during the study, failure to fill out questionnaires, or research procedure violations, missing data will occur. Imputation of missing values will be carried out as much as possible to make intention-to-treat analyses feasible. Missing data will be imputed using Imputation and Variance Estimation Software.46

Prior to the training (T0) we will assess differences between patients and siblings on neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning, using mixed modelling. Subsequently, we will conduct multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) to determine the effect of neurofeedback post-treatment (T1) on the primary and secondary outcomes, comparing the patients in the neurofeedback group to the patients in the placebo group. To control for possible differences in neurocognitive functioning prior to the training, T0 data will be included in the model as covariate. Finally, we will use repeated measures analysis for group (neurofeedback and placebo) x time (T0, T1, and six months follow-up, T2) to investigate the changes over time. To examine the possible effects of patient characteristics on the efficacy of the neurofeedback, the following variables will be assessed as covariates in the MANOVA and repeated measures analyses: age at assessment, age at diagnosis, diagnosis, time since diagnosis, and treatment modalities. All analyses will be conducted using SPSS. A P-value <0.05 will be considered significant.

DISCUSSION

This article describes the design of the PRISMA study, a randomized controlled trial investigating the efficacy of neurofeedback in pediatric brain tumor survivors with neurocognitive problems. Although neurocognitive problems in pediatric oncology survivors are reported in numerous studies, empirically validated interventions addressing these deficits are scarce. There is growing evidence for neurofeedback as a valuable treatment in different brain disorders.15,21 Our study is the first to investigate the efficacy of neurofeedback in pediatric brain tumor survivors using a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind trial, comparing neurofeedback to placebo training. By setting an automatically adjusted threshold of feedback as opposed to a manually adjusted threshold, we enabled blinding the trainers; trainers were not required to monitor the brain activity of the patient during the sessions. Furthermore, we ensure the standardization of the neurofeedback treatment by employing carefully instructed research assistants providing the neurofeedback treatment. At the same time we increased the feasibility for the patients, by administering the training at the patients’ home or school. In addition, the effects of neurofeedback on neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning are thoroughly investigated by using well-validated paradigms

Page 59: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Design RCT

57

3

and psychometrically sound questionnaires administered to patient, caregiver and teacher. Lastly, we have included a control group of healthy siblings, to compare performance of the brain tumor survivors to children without a history of a brain tumor.The design of PRISMA has some methodological pitfalls to take into account. Because of time factors and the population, this study might be at risk for losing patients during the treatment phase and during follow-up. With five year survival rates of approximately 65%, some patients may relapse[47], or they may discontinue their participation in the study. We increase comparability by employing three different training modules; however, the training might be less effective than an individualized training. The groups receiving each of the three treatment modules are small. Also, the group of brain tumor patients is heterogeneous, e.g. in terms of tumor diagnosis, tumor location, age at diagnosis, treatment, time since diagnosis, and time since end of treatment. It is well documented that these variables play an important role in neurocognitive outcomes. These heterogeneities may be reflected in our results.In conclusion, if neurofeedback proves to be effective in improving neurocognitive deficits after treatment for a brain tumor, this would be a valuable addition to the currently available effective interventions for this vulnerable group of pediatric brain tumor survivors.

Page 60: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 3

58

REFERENCES

1. Mariotto AB, Rowland JH, Yabroff KR, Scoppa S, Hachey M, Ries L, et al. Long-term survivors of childhood cancers in the United States. Cancer Epidemiol biomarkers Prev. 2009;18(4):1033–40.

2. Mabbott DJ, Penkman L, Witol A. Core neurocognitive functions in children treated for posterior fossa tumors. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(2):159–68.

3. Mulhern RK, Palmer SL, Merchant TE, Wallace D, Kocak M, Brouwers P, et al. Neurocognitive consequences of risk-adapted therapy for childhood medulloblastoma. J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(24):5511–9.

4. Zebrack BJ, Gurney JG, Oeffinger K, Whitton J, Packer RJ, Mertens A, et al. Psychological outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood brain cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22(6):999–1006.

5. Duffner PK. Risk factors for cognitive decline in children treated for brain tumors. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2010;14(2):106–15.

6. de Ruiter MA, van Mourik R, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Grootenhuis MA, Oosterlaan J. Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatment: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(5):408–17.

7. Carey ME, Barakat LP, Foley B, Gyato K, Phillips PC. Neuropsychological functioning and social functioning of survivors of pediatric brain tumors: Evidence of nonverbal learning disability. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;7(4):265–72.

8. Butler RW, Mulhern RK. Neurocognitive Interventions for Children and Adolescents Surviving Cancer. J Pediatr Psychol. 2005;30(1):65–78.

9. Butler RW, Copeland DR, Fairclough DL, Mulhern RK, Katz ER, Kazak AE, et al. A multicenter, randomized clinical trial of a cognitive remediation program for childhood survivors of a pediatric malignancy. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76(3):367–78.

10. van ’t Hooft I, Andersson K, Bergman B, Sejersen T, von Wendt L, Bartfai A. Sustained favorable effects of cognitive training in children with acquired brain injuries. NeuroRehabilitation. 2007;22(2):109–16.

11. Mulhern RK, White HA, Glass JO, Kun LE, Leigh L, Thompson SJ, et al. Attentional functioning and white matter integrity among survivors of malignant brain tumors of childhood. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:180–9.

12. Thompson SJ, Leigh L, Christensen R, Xiong X, Kun LE, Heideman RL, et al. Immediate neurocognitive effects of methylphenidate on learning-impaired survivors of childhood cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2001;19(6):1802–8.

13. Simonoff E, Taylor E, Baird G, Bernard S, Chadwick O, Liang H, et al. Randomized controlled double-blind trial of optimal dose methylphenidate in children and adolescents with severe attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and intellectual disability. J child Psychol psychiatry. 2013;54(5):527–35.

14. Sterman M. Basic concepts and clinical findings in the treatment of seizure disorders with EEG operant conditioning. Clin Electroencephalogr. 2000;31:45–55.

15. Heinrich H, Gevensleben H, Strehl U. Annotation: neurofeedback - train your brain to train behaviour. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(1):3–16.

16. Hirshberg LM, Chiu S, Frazier J a. Emerging brain-based interventions for children and adolescents: overview and clinical perspective. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2005;14(1):1–19.

17. Arns M, Ridder S De, Strehl U, Breteler MHM, Coenen A. Efficacy of Neurofeedback Treatment in ADHD: the Effects on Inattention, Impulsivity and Hyperactivity: a Meta-Analysis. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2009;40(3):180–9.

Page 61: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Design RCT

59

3

18. Hodgson K, Hutchinson AD, Denson L. Nonpharmacological Treatments for ADHD: A Meta-Analytic Review. J Atten Disord. 2012;XX(X):1–8.

19. Gevensleben H, Rothenberger A, Moll GH, Heinrich H. Neurofeedback in children with ADHD: validation and challenges. Expert Rev Neurother. 2012;12(4):447–60.

20. Strehl U, Leins U, Goth G, Klinger C, Hinterberger T, Birbaumer N. Self-regulation of slow cortical potentials: a new treatment for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2006;118(5):e1530–40.

21. Gevensleben H, Holl B, Albrecht B, Schlamp D, Kratz O, Studer P, et al. Neurofeedback training in children with ADHD: 6-month follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19(9):715–24.

22. Lofthouse N, Arnold LE, Hersch S, Hurt E, DeBeus R. A review of neurofeedback treatment for pediatric ADHD. J Atten Disord. 2012;16(5):351–72.

23. Thornton KE, Carmody DP. Efficacy of traumatic brain injury rehabilitation: interventions of QEEG-guided biofeedback, computers, strategies, and medications. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback. 2008;33(2):101–24.

24. Aukema EJ, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Last BF, Breteler MHM, Hogeweg J, Grootenhuis MA. Exploring the feasibility of Neurofeedback training as a cognitive intervention for childhood brain tumor survivors: a pilot study. (Submitted).

25. Schulte F, Barrera M. Social competence in childhood brain tumor survivors: a comprehensive review. Support care cancer. 2010;18(12):1499–513.

26. BioExplorer [Internet]. [cited 2012 Jul 19]. Available from: http://www.cyberevolution.com/27. BrainQuiry [Internet]. [cited 2012 Jul 19]. Available from: http://www.brainquiry.com/28. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI, Petersen SE. Testing the efficiency and

independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(3):340–7. 29. Wechsler D. Wechsler’s Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd Edition. London: Harcourt Brace and

Company; 1992. 30. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological

Corporation; 1997. 31. Logan GD, Cowan WB. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control.

Psychol Rev. 1984;91(3):295–327. 32. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol

Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–6. 33. Veerman JW, Straathof MAE, Treffers PDA, van den Bergh BRH ten BL. Handleiding bij de

Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Kinderen, CBSK (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Children – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997.

34. Treffers P, Goedhart A, Veerman J, van den Bergh B, Ackaert L, de Rycke L. Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Adolescenten, CBSA (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Adolescents – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002.

35. Ravens-Sieberer U, Auquier P, Erhart M, Gosch A, Rajmil L, Bruil J, et al. The KIDSCREEN-27 quality of life measure for children and adolescents: psychometric results from a cross-cultural survey in 13 European countries. Qual life Res. 2007;16(8):1347–56.

36. Stulemeijer M, de Jong LWAM, Fiselier TJW, Hoogveld SWB, Bleijenberg G. Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2005;330(7481):14.

37. Bruni O, Ottaviano S, Guidetti V, Romoli M, Innocenzi M, Cortesi F, et al. The Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC). Construction and validation of an instrument to evaluate sleep disturbances in childhood and adolescence. J Sleep Res. 1996;5(4):251–61.

Page 62: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 3

60

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

38. Polderman TJC, Derks EM, Hudziak JJ, Verhulst FC, Posthuma D, Boomsma DI. Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(11):1080–7.

39. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. TEST REVIEW Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6(3):235–8.

40. Neuroscan [Internet]. [cited 2012 Sep 7]. Available from: http://www.neuroscan.com/41. Gordon E, Cooper N, Rennie C, Hermens D, Williams LM. Integrative Neuroscience: The Role of

a Standardized Database. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2005;36(2):64–75. 42. Brain Resource [Internet]. [cited 2012 Sep 5]. Available from: http://www.brainresource.com/43. Mulhern RK, Merchant TE, Gajjar A, Reddick WE, Kun LE. Late neurocognitive sequelae in

survivors of brain tumours in childhood. Lancet Oncol. 2004;5(7):399–408. 44. Bhat SR, Goodwin TL, Burwinkle TM, Lansdale MF, Dahl G V, Huhn SL, et al. Profile of daily

life in children with brain tumors: an assessment of health-related quality of life. J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(24):5493–500.

45. Elashoff JD. nQuery Advisor Version 7.0 User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA; 2007. 46. IVEware: Imputation and Variance Estimation Software [Internet]. [cited 2012 Aug 15]. Available

from: http://www.isr.umich.edu/src/smp/ive/

Page 63: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

4

Timed performance weaknesses on computerized

tasks in pediatric brain tumor survivors:

a comparison with sibling controls

Marieke A. de Ruiter Martha A. Grootenhuis

Rosa van MourikHeleen Maurice-StamMarinus H.M. Breteler

Corrie Gidding Laura R. Beek

Bernd GranzenDannis G. van Vuurden

Antoinette Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren Jaap Oosterlaan

Child Neuropsychology 2015, epub ahead of print

Page 64: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

62

ABSTRACT

Background: With more children surviving a brain tumor, insight into the late effects of the disease and treatment is of high importance. This study focused on profiling the neurocognitive functions that might be affected after treatment for a pediatric brain tumor, using a broad battery of computerized tests. Predictors that may influence neurocognitive functioning were also investigated.

Methods: A total of 82 pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTSs) aged 8–18 years (M = 13.85, SD = 3.15, 49% males) with parent-reported neurocognitive complaints were compared to a control group of 43 siblings (age M = 14.27, SD = 2.44, 40% males) using linear mixed models. Neurocognitive performance was assessed using measures of attention, processing speed, memory, executive functioning, visuomotor integration (VMI), and intelligence. Tumor type, treatment, tumor location, hydrocephalus, gender, age at diagnosis, and time since diagnosis were entered into regression analyzes as predictors for neurocognitive functioning.

Results: The PBTSs showed slower processing speeds and lower intelligence (range effect sizes .71–.82,p < .001), as well as deficits in executive attention, short-term memory, executive functioning, and VMI (range effect sizes .40–.57, p < .05). Older age at assessment was associated with better neurocognitive functioning (B = .450, p < .001) and younger age at diagnosis was associated with lower intelligence (B = .328, p < .05). Medical risk factors, e.g., hydrocephalus, did not show an association with neurocognitive functioning.

Conclusions: Late effects in PBTSs include a broad range of neurocognitive deficits. The results suggest that even PBTSs that were traditionally viewed as low risk for neurocognitive problems (e.g., surgery only, no hydrocephalus) may suffer from decreased neurocognitive functioning.

Page 65: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

63

4

INTRODUCTION

A brain tumor is the second most frequently occurring type of childhood malignancy.1 During the last decades, the treatment of childhood brain tumors has improved considerably, leading to substantially lower mortality rates. Increased survival has, however, not come without cost. Many pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTSs) experience neurocognitive late effects.2 The tumor and its treatment inevitably damage healthy brain tissue, leading to a wide range of neurocognitive deficits.3 Neurocognitive deficits have deteriorating effects on the psychosocial development and academic achievement of PBTSs.4 Although neurocognitive functioning after brain tumor treatment has been subject of many studies, the focus is often on a limited array of neurocognitive functions.5,6 Studies that do include a more comprehensive assessment of neurocognitive functions have mostly focused on a restricted subgroup of PBTSs, e.g., posterior fossa tumor patients, preventing the results from being generalized to the PBTS population, although this may aid a better understanding of the impact on specific subgroups.7,8 Other studies used paper and pencil tasks to measure neurocognitive functioning.9,10 Paper-and-pencil-based measures often unintentionally target multiple neurocognitive functions within one instrument, such as visuomotor abilities, while computerized tasks facilitate the isolation of individual neurocognitive functions. This may be done by either systematically manipulating the load on the function of interest while holding all other function demands constant (e.g., the demand on motor functioning in a visuomotor integration [VMI] task is kept constant, while the demand on VMI is increased by manipulating speed and structure; see De Kieviet et al., 2013)11 or by the use of built-in control conditions allowing control over performance on functions that are not of interest (e.g., with regard to an inhibition task, the demand on processing speed and motor response are controlled for in order to isolate response inhibition; see De Zeeuw et al., 2008).12 Furthermore, computerized tasks are highly standardized and thereby increase accuracy in administration and scoring, allow a more accurate response-time recording, and reduce examiner burden with regard to assessment and scoring. Computerized tasks also facilitate the development of alternative versions and distribution of the tasks with software.13 To the best of our knowledge, this study is among the first to focus on a broad range of neurocognitive deficits using mainly computerized measures in PBTSs with varied etiological and treatment backgrounds.In addition to studying the occurrence and magnitude of neurocognitive deficits after a pediatric brain tumor, defining predictors of these deficits is of great clinical relevance. Our recent quantitative meta-analysis found that young age at diagnosis, radiotherapy, chemotherapy, and longer time since diagnosis were associated with worse neurocognitive outcomes in terms of intelligence and attention.14 In addition, female gender and complications such as hydrocephalus have been found to be associated with worse neurocognitive functioning.15

Page 66: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

64

In the present study, we employed computerized tests to improve understanding of the profile of neurocognitive deficits displayed by PBTSs. The performance of PBTSs with subjective neurocognitive complaints was compared to that of sibling controls. Because neurocognitive functions are known to be highly influenced by genetic factors, sibling controls are preferred as a comparison group over, for example, classmates.16,17 The use of sibling controls also controls for socioeconomic background. Groups were compared on a broad range of neurocognitive functions including attention, processing speed, memory, inhibition, VMI, and intelligence. All tasks, except for the intelligence measures, were designed to measure one single neurocognitive function, thereby controlling for other possible functions using tasks with built-in control conditions or tasks that parametrically manipulate the difficulty level of the neurocognitive function of interest. We hypothesized that PBTSs would perform worse than sibling controls on all measures of neurocognitive functioning. This study also addressed the predictive value of gender, tumor type, tumor location, treatment, hydrocephalus, age at diagnosis, and time since diagnosis for neurocognitive outcome, hypothesizing that male gender, low-grade tumor, infratentorial tumor, treatment with surgery only, no hydrocephalus, older age at diagnosis and shorter time since diagnosis would lead to better functioning.14,15

METHOD

ParticipantsEligible for inclusion were children treated for a brain tumor in the Netherlands, aged 8–18 years, who finished treatment at least 2 years prior to enrollment and who suffered from neurocognitive complaints as reported by parents. Children with a premorbid diagnosis of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD) were excluded. Furthermore, exclusion criteria were a mental or physical condition that restricts possibilities for neurocognitive assessment (e.g., blindness) or insufficient mastery of the Dutch language. Siblings, aged 8–18 years, formed the control group. Only siblings mentally and physically able to participate in the assessment and who had a sufficient mastery of the Dutch language were included. Data included in this manuscript were obtained in compliance with the regulations of the Academic Medical Center Amsterdam and the Helsinki Declaration.

ProcedureData collection took place between January 2010 and August 2012 as part of the PRISMA study, a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind trial to investigate the effects of neurofeedback on neurocognitive functioning of PBTSs.18 The present paper reports data on neurocognitive functioning at study entry. The study protocol was approved by the

Page 67: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

65

4

Medical Ethical Committee of the Academic Medical Center Amsterdam and registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT00961922).Five out of seven Dutch university hospitals accepted the invitation to join the study: Emma Children’s Hospital/Academic Medical Center Amsterdam, the VU Medical Center Amsterdam, the University Medical Center Utrecht, the Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, and the Maastricht University Medical Center. A letter informed children and their parents about the study. Interested parents were provided with an online screening questionnaire concerning inclusion and exclusion criteria. Children eligible for inclusion were invited for baseline assessment. If available, siblings were invited to participate as controls in the study. Informed consent was signed by parents and participating children. Testing took place in a quiet room by trained examiners using standardized instructions. The total testing time was approximately two and a half hours. Tests were administered in a fixed order for both groups. Frequent breaks were taken to avoid fatigue.

NEUROCOGNITIVE OUTCOME MEASURES

AttentionThe Attention Network Task (ANT) is a computerized task designed to measure the functioning of three independent neural networks that enable alerting, orienting, and executive attention. Reliability and validity has been established.19,20 In this study, we used a child-friendly adaptation of the ANT.21

In the ANT, children are required to push as quickly and accurately as possible one of two buttons corresponding to the location of a target stimulus presented at either the left- or right-hand side of a computer screen. There are four trial types: (1) neutral trials, in which a neutral cue appears in the center of the screen preceding the target stimulus; (2) alerting trials, in which no cue is provided before the onset of the target stimulus; (3) orienting trials, in which a central cue points to the location of the subsequently presented target stimulus; and (4) executive trials, in which the central cue points to the location opposite the subsequently presented target stimulus (causing slower reaction times due to the necessary shift in attention). The task consists of 312 trials, randomly presenting the four trial types. The efficiency of the alerting, orienting, and executive attention networks was assessed by contrasting the mean reaction times of the correct responses for each of the three corresponding trials (alerting, orienting, and executive) with the mean reaction time of the correct responses on the neutral trials.In addition, the intraindividual coefficient of variation across all trials was used as a measure of maintenance of attention. The intraindividual coefficient of variation is defined as the standard deviation of reaction times on correct responses divided by the mean reaction time of correct responses.12

Page 68: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

66

Processing speedProcessing speed was measured by the mean reaction time on correct responses on the neutral ANT trials.21

Memory: Visual short-term memoryA computerized version of a visual sequencing task was used to measure visual short-term memory. Psychometric properties have been reported in several studies.21,22 During a trial, a sequence of yellow circles appears one by one in a 4×4 grid. The participant is required to repeat the sequence by tapping the correct locations on a touch screen. The difficulty level is manipulated by increasing the number of circles to be repeated (2–9 circles), with each difficulty level comprising two trials. If a participant makes an error on both trials of a difficulty level, the task is terminated. The total number of correct trials was used as the outcome variable.

Memory: Auditory short-term memoryAuditory short-term memory was measured by the forward items of the Digit Span task of the age-appropriate Wechsler scale (the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children –Third Edition [WISC-III] or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition [WAIS-III]23,24). In this task, participants are required to repeat verbally presented sequences of digits, increasing in length (2–9 digits). The task is terminated after an error on both items of a difficulty level. The number of correct forward items (i.e., the raw score associated with Digit Span forward) on the Digit Span task was used as the outcome variable.

Executive functioning: Working memoryThe backward items on the Digit Span task of the age-appropriate Wechsler scale were used as a measure of working memory.23,24 During the backward items, the participants are asked to repeat verbally presented sequences of digits, increasing in length (2–9 digits), in the reverse order, requiring simultaneous storage and processing of information.25 The task is terminated after an error on both items of a difficulty level. The total number of correct items (i.e., the raw score associated with Digit Span backward) was used as the outcome variable, with the number of correct forward items as covariate.

Executive functioning: Response inhibitionThe Stop Signal task was used to assess response inhibition. Reliability and validity have been established in several studies.26,27 The task presents participants with two kinds of trials: go trials and stop trials. The participant is required to press as quickly and accurately as possible one of two buttons corresponding to the location of a target stimulus presented on either the left- or right-hand side of a computer screen. On stop trials, the stimulus is

Page 69: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

67

4

followed by a stop signal—a cross, superimposed on the stimulus—requiring participants to inhibit the response. The time between stimulus and stop signal (stop signal delay) is varied to accomplish on average 50% successful inhibition on stop trials. The stop signal reaction time (SSRT), measuring the speed of the inhibitory response, was calculated by subtracting the mean stop signal delay from the mean reaction time on the correct go trials, and served as the outcome variable.

VMIA computerized tracking and pursuit task was used to measure VMI,28 which had been adapted and extended for use with children.11 The psychometric properties are detailed in de Sonneville (1999).29 The task comprises a structured part and an unstructured part. In both parts, participants are required to follow the trail of a moving caterpillar with a computer pencil in their preferred hand. During the structured part, the caterpillar moves in a fixed circular way around the screen. In the unstructured part of the task, the caterpillar moves in random directions around the screen. Both parts consist of six trials of 30 seconds each, with increasing speed levels (40–140 mm/s). For each trial, accuracy was calculated as the mean distance (in mm) between the position of the pencil and the caterpillar. The accuracy scores on the structured and unstructured part of the six different speed levels were used as measures of VMI.

IntelligenceFull Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ) was estimated using a four-subtask short form of the age-appropriate WISC-III or WAIS-III23,24 containing the subtests Arithmetic, Similarities, Block Design, and Picture Completion (see Sattler, 1992).30 Satisfactory reliability (Cronbach’s alphas >.80) and validity (rs with full test >.77) have been reported for this short form for both the WISC-III and the WAIS-III.30

Medical and demographic dataFor participating PBTSs, information from the medical records was provided by the oncologist and included tumor type and grade (high-grade/low-grade tumor, based on histopathology), type of treatment (surgery only versus chemotherapy and/or radiotherapy with or without surgery), tumor location (supratentorial/infratentorial, based on MRI) and history of hydrocephalus (based on MRI), age at diagnosis, and time since diagnosis. Information on demographics was provided by the parents (gender, parental country of origin, and the highest level of parental education assessing socioeconomic status). Medical and demographic data were also collected for a subsample of non-participants (45 of 71 non-participants) to study selective dropout. For non-participants, the “age at assessment” was defined as the difference between birth date and the average assessment date of participating PBTSs.

Page 70: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

68

Statistical analysesAll analyses were conducted using SPSS version 20.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Differences between PBTSs and controls on age at assessment and demographical data were tested with an independent-samples t-test and chi-square test, respectively. Differences between participating and non-participating PBTSs on demographical and medical data were tested with an independent-samples t-test (age at assessment, age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis), a chi-square test (gender, tumor type, tumor location, treatment, hydrocephalus), or a binomial test (diagnosis). Linear mixed models were used to evaluate differences in neurocognitive functioning between survivors and controls, controlling for age.These analyses accounted for the dependency of the data of PBTSs and controls from the same families and in the case of repeated measurements (ANT and VMI task), also for the dependency of the data within subjects. In all models, neurocognitive function was predicted by group (survivors versus controls) and age as fixed factors with a random intercept for “family”. Additionally, in the model of the data derived from the ANT assessing the efficiency of the alerting, orienting, and executive attention networks, trial type was included as a within-subject factor. Group and the two-way interaction group × trial type were included as fixed factors. This two-way interaction was used to test differences in the efficiency of the three attention networks between PBTSs and controls, contrasting the performance on neutral trials to the performance on the alerting, orienting, and executive trials. Likewise, in the model of the VMI task, structure and speed were added as within-subject factors. Group, structure, speed, the two-way interaction structure × speed, and all interactions with group were included as fixed factors. These interactions were used to test the differential effects of structure and speed between PBTSs and controls.Effect sizes were calculated in terms of Cohen’s d by dividing the difference in mean score between PTBSs and controls by the pooled standard deviation with values of .20, .50, and .80 considered as small, medium, and large effects, respectively.31 Regression analyses were used to study the predictors of neurocognitive functioning. To limit the number of outcomes (criterion variables), a “summary factor of neurocognitive functioning” was constructed. We grouped all neurocognitive measures into one aggregated score using principal component analysis (PCA). PCA with oblique rotation was performed on those neurocognitive outcomes on which patients and controls differed significantly, except intelligence (see below). For the VMI task, only the highest speed level of the unstructured part was included (this variable was regarded as most representative of the data collected with this task). The resulting one factor harbored the variance common to all neurocognitive measures. These factor scores were used to analyze risk factors for poor neurocognitive functioning. As age plays an important role in performance on neurocognitive tasks, age at assessment was entered in the first step in the regression model, followed by all other predictor variables: tumor type (high-grade versus low-grade tumor), type of treatment

Page 71: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

69

4

(surgery only versus chemotherapy and/or radiotherapy with/without surgery), tumor location (supratentorial versus infratentorial), history of a hydrocephalus, gender, age at diagnosis, and time since diagnosis. Due to the fact that intelligence scores are age corrected, a separate regression analysis was performed with estimated FSIQ as the outcome, in which age at assessment was not entered. A p-value <.05 was considered significant.

RESULTS

ParticipantsThe final sample in the present study included 82 PBTSs and 43 controls (for a flowchart of inclusion, see Figure 1). Of the survivors, 20 received special education (24%) and 39 have been held back a class in the past (48%). Data on age at assessment, demographic characteristics, and disease characteristics are displayed in Table 1. PBTSs and controls did not differ in age at assessment, gender, country of origin of parents, or highest level of parental education (p > .324).Participating and non-participating PBTSs did not differ in age at assessment (p > .062), gender (p > .062), or tumor location (p > .116), but they did differ with regard to the distribution of tumor types (p < .01) as well as the grade of tumor, with relatively more high-grade tumors in the participants than in the non-participants (p < .05). The participants were on average younger at diagnosis (p < .05), and time since diagnosis was longer (p < .05) than in the case of the non-participants. More participating PBTSs had undergone radiotherapy (p < .05) and chemotherapy (p < .05) than non-participating PBTSs.

58

Figure 1. Flowchart of inclusion. Note. AD/HD = attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder; PBTSs = Pediatric brain tumor survivors.

Figure 1. Flowchart of inclusion.Note. AD/HD = attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder; PBTSs = Pediatric brain tumor survivors.

Page 72: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

70

Table 1. Demographics and medical information of Pediatric Brain Tumor Survivors (PBTSs) and sibling controls.

PBTSN = 82

Controls N = 43

M SD M SDAge Age at assessment (years) 13.85 3.15 14.27 2.44Age diagnosis (years) 6.87 3.77 n/a n/aTime since diagnosis (years) 6.98 3.57 n/a n/a

n % n %Gender Males 40 49 17 40Country of origin motherNetherlands 71 87 37 86Other 11 13 9 14Country of origin fatherNetherlands 73 89 40 93Other 9 11 3 7Highest education parenta Low or Intermediate 39 48 23 54High 43 52 20 46Tumor type and gradeHigh grade 34 42 n/a n/aMedulloblastoma 12 15 n/a n/aSupratentorial PNET 8 10 n/a n/aEpendymoma 5 6 n/a n/aAstrocytoma gr III 5 6 n/a n/aGerm cell tumor 4 5 n/a n/aLow Grade 48 59 n/a n/aLow grade glioma 35 43 n/a n/aCraniopharyngioma 7 9 n/a n/aPlexus papilloma 6 7 n/a n/aTreatmentRadiotherapy 34 42 n/a n/aChemotherapy 35 43 n/a n/aSurgeryb 72 88 n/a n/aOtherc 2 2 n/a n/aLocationSupratentorial 46 56 n/a n/aInfratentorial 36 44 n/a n/aHydrocephalusYes 39 48 n/a n/aNo 43 52 n/a n/a

Note. aHighest level of education of father or mother is reported, where Low or Intermediate = primary education, general secondary education, and secondary vocational education and High = Higher vocational education and university; b37 patients were treated with surgery only; cBiopsy only, n = 1; cerebrospinal fluid pressure relief only, n =1. M = Mean; n/a = Not applicable; PBTSs = Pediatric brain tumor survivors; PNET = Primitive neuroectodermal tumor; SD = Standard deviation. PBTSs and controls did not differ in terms of age at assessment, gender, country of origin of parents, and highest level of parental education.

Page 73: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

71

4

Neurocognitive outcomesThe results of the neurocognitive measures are displayed in Table 2 and Figure 2.

AttentionNo group differences were found for alerting (d = .07, p = .799) and orienting attention (d = .43, p = .104), but PBTSs showed a less efficient executive attention network than the controls, as reflected in a greater difference between mean reaction times on correct neutral and executive trials (d = .47, p < .01). PBTSs showed poorer maintenance of their attention than controls, as reflected by the higher intraindividual coefficient of variation (d = .40, p < .05).

Processing speedInformation processing speed was slower in PBTSs compared to controls, as measured by the mean reaction time of correct responses on the neutral trials of the ANT (d = .74, p < .001).

Memory: Visual short-term memoryPBTSs had a lower number of correct items on the visual sequencing task than the controls (d = .56, p < .01), reflecting worse visual short-term memory performance.

Memory: Auditory short-term memoryPBTSs completed fewer forward items correctly on the Digit Span task (d = .57, p < .01), indicative of poorer auditory short-term memory.

Executive functioning: Working memoryThe total number of correct backward items on the Digit Span task, controlling for the correct number of forward items, was lower in the PBTSs than in the control group (d = .41, p < .05), indicating poorer working memory.

Executive functioning: Response inhibitionSSRT scores were higher in PBTSs than the controls (d = .40, p < .05), reflecting slower inhibitory processes.

Page 74: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

72

Table 2. Neurocognitive outcomes of the Pediatric Brain Tumor Survivors (PBTSs) compared to sibling controls.

PBTSN = 82

Sibling controlsN = 43

Group differences

Measure M SD M SD d p AttentionANT alerting attention 53.91 31.45 55.99 20.68 .07 .799ANT orienting attention 40.58 34.61 27.30 22.66 .43 .104ANT executive attention 41.67 56.55 18.74 27.93 .47* .005ANT intraindividual coefficient .23 .07 .20 .05 .40* .028Processing speedANT mean reaction time 442.84 167.00 340.46 54.88 .74* <.001MemoryVisual short-term memory 13.59 4.39 15.84 3.19 .56* .001Auditory short-term memory 7.83 2.06 8.95 1.75 .57* .004Executive functioningWorking memory 5.26 2.22 6.14 1.93 .41* .037Stop task response inhibitiona 261.61 69.89 235.65 55.62 .40* .034IntelligenceEstimated FSIQ 89.71 18.67 103.49 12.57 .82* <.001

Note.*p < .05; an = 78 for PBTSs and n = 39 for sibling controls. ANT = Attention Network Task; FSIQ = Full Scale IQ; M = Mean; PBTSs = Pediatric brain tumor survivors; SD = Standard deviation. For all outcomes, except for intelligence, raw scores are reported and age was entered as a covariate in the analyses. For intelligence, age-standardized scores are given. Effect sizes “d” were calculated using the pooled SD of both groups. The results for the VMI task are displayed in Figure 2 and described in the text.

63

Figure 2. Results for the VMI task for pediatric brain tumor survivors and sibling controls for both the structured and unstructured parts.

0

5

10

15

20

25

40 60 80 100 120 140

Acc

urac

y (m

m)

Speed (mm/s)

PBTSsunstructuredSiblingsunstructuredPBTSs structured

Siblingsstructured

Figure 2. Results for the VMI task for pediatric brain tumor survivors and sibling controls for both the structured and unstructured parts.

Page 75: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

73

4

VMIThere was a significant main effect of group, with PBTSs overall being less accurate in following the caterpillar than controls, F(1,46) = 17.307, p < .001, indicative of poorer VMI. Accuracy decreased with higher speed in both sibling controls and PBTSs, F(5,1256) = 2305, p < .001, but the decrease in accuracy with higher speed levels was stronger in PBTSs than in sibling controls, according to the significant two-way interaction effect of group × speed, F(5,1256) = 7.176, p < .001. VMI performance was worse in the unstructured condition than in the structured condition, F(1,1260) = 4095, p < .001, and the decrease in accuracy with higher speed levels was the strongest in the unstructured condition, as shown by the significant two-way interaction effect of speed × structure, F(5,1256) = 298, p < .001. However, these effects of structure and speed × structure were not different for PBTSs and controls, as indicated by the non-significant two-way (group × structure, F(1,1265) = 1.981,p > .05) and three-way (group × structure × speed, F(5,1256) = 0.616, p > .05) interaction effects.

IntelligencePBTSs had lower estimated FSIQs compared to controls (d = .82, p < .001).

Predictors of neurocognitive outcomeAll outcomes on which PBTSs and controls appeared to differ, except intelligence, were entered in a principal components analysis. For the VMI task, the highest speed level of the unstructured part was included. The principal components analysis yielded one factor that explained 49% of the variance of all outcome variables entered. The factor loadings for each variable on the factor are provided in Table 3. The loadings are all .60 or higher and the average loading is .70. According to MacCallum, Widaman, Zhang, and Hong (1999) and MacCallum, Widaman, Preacher, and Hong (2001) this justifies performing the principal component analysis, given our sample size.32,33 We interpret the resulting factor as primarily reflecting neurocognitive processing speed, since all tasks except for the span tasks had a timing factor. Also, almost all the variables entered into the PCA strongly correlated with the ANT mean reaction time, with correlations ranging from r = .53 (working memory) to r = .69 (ANT intraindividual coefficient). Together, these findings strengthen the idea that the scores on our factor mainly represent neurocognitive processing speed. A higher score on the factor represents better neurocognitive functioning. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) was calculated as. 78, showing that our available data fulfill criteria for PCA. In fact, the ratio of the squared correlation between the variables and the squared partial correlation between the variables of .78 can be described as good.34

Page 76: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

74

Table 3. Factor loadings per variable of the Principal Component Analysis (PCA).

Measure Factor loadingANT executive attention .71ANT intraindividual coefficient .65ANT mean reaction time .87Visual short-term memory -.74Auditory short-term memory -.72Working memory -.60Stop task response inhibition .70Visuomotor integration (VMI) .61Eigenvalue 3.95% variance 49%

Note. ANT = Attention network task. All outcomes on which PBTSs and controls appeared to differ, except intelligence, were entered in the PCA. For VMI, the highest speed level of the unstructured part was included. N = 74 applies to the entire table/PCA.

Table 4. Regression analysis of medical and child factors on neurocognitive functioning of Pediatric Brain Tumor Survivors (PBTSs).

Neurocognitive summaryfactor (n = 74)

Estimated FSIQ(n = 82)

B p B p

Step 1Age assessment (years) 0.499 <.001 - -F 23.932 <.001 - -r2 0.249 - - -Step 2Age assessment (years) 0.450 <.001 - -Tumor grade -0.061 .635 -0.027 .831Treatment 0.030 .819 -0.137 .282Location tumor 0.014 .901 0.212 .052Hydrocephalus 0.081 .453 0.039 .718Gender 0.110 .325 -0.061 .573Age diagnosis (years) 0.144 .232 0.328 .003F change 0.675 0.670 2.895 .014r2 change 0.043 - 0.188 -

Note: B = Standardized regression coefficient; FSIQ = Full scale IQ; Gender = males (1) versus females (2); Hydrocephalus = No (history of) hydrocephalus (1) versus (history of) hydrocephalus (2); Location of tumor = Infratentorial (1) versus supratentorial (2); Neurocognitive summary factor = Factor derived from the PCA; Treatment = Surgery, biopsy or cerebrospinal fluid pressure relief only (1) versus radiotherapy and/or chemotherapy with or without surgery (2); Tumor grade = Low-grade tumor (1) versus high-grade tumor (2). Time since diagnosis was excluded from the analysis due to multicollinearity.

Page 77: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

75

4

Table 4 displays the results of the regression analysis predicting the neurocognitive summary factor as well as estimated FSIQ. Age at assessment was significantly associated with the neurocognitive summary factor, with older children performing better than younger children (p < .001). No other predictors significantly contributed to the neurocognitive summary factor. For estimated FSIQ it was found that age at diagnosis positively predicts estimated FSIQ, indicating that children who are older at diagnosis have a higher estimated FSIQ (p < .05). None of the other risk factors included in the model contributed significantly to estimated FSIQ.

DISCUSSION

This study investigated a broad range of neurocognitive functions in PBTSs with subjective neurocognitive complaints as compared to sibling controls. The results demonstrate that PBTSs have problems of medium (d = −.40) to large (d = −.82) size across the whole range of examined neurocognitive functions, including executive attention, maintenance of attention, processing speed, visual and auditory short-term memory, working memory, inhibition, VMI, and estimated FSIQ. Furthermore, older PBTSs performed better on the tasks than younger PBTSs did. Also, PBTSs who were younger when they were diagnosed had lower estimated IQ scores than PBTSs who were older at diagnosis.In line with research on childhood cancer survivors and (subsamples of) PBTSs we found worse attention performance compared to controls.35–37 Our study is novel in the use of the ANT, which adds to the existing literature by studying the functionality of three independent attention networks: alerting, orienting, and executive attention. We found that PBTSs show more difficulty resolving contradictory information than controls, as reflected by a less efficient executive attention network. Furthermore, maintenance of attention in our PBTSs sample was worse compared to the controls. As maintaining attention and ignoring irrelevant cues are crucial skills for acquiring new information, this finding might be related to the decrease in school functioning seen over time in PBTSs who received cranial irradiation.38 Regarding memory and working memory, previous study results consistently showed visual memory and working memory deficits; however, previous work was inconsistent regarding auditory memory, with findings varying from normal memory to a strongly diminished memory function.39–43 According to the most influential model of working memory proposed by Baddeley,44 working memory comprises three subsystems: the visuospatial sketchpad maintaining visual information, the phonological loop maintaining auditory information, and the central executive controlling and allowing the manipulation of the information contained in the visuospatial sketchpad and phonological loop. In PBTSs, hippocampal volume could be an underlying factor in these deficits, as it has been reported that hippocampal volumes

Page 78: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

76

are positively associated with auditory attention in the context of a verbal memory task in PBTSs, especially in patients treated with radiotherapy.45 In our study, PBTSs demonstrated more difficulty in keeping both visual and auditory information in their short-term memory as compared to controls, suggesting an underlying problem in the visuospatial sketchpad and phonological loop, respectively. We also found evidence for deficits in the central executive, since PBTSs performed worse than controls on the Digit Span backward condition (requiring the manipulation of verbal information, assessing functioning of the central executive), even after controlling for performance on the forward condition (assessing phonological loop functioning). Together, our findings suggest impairments in all aspects of working memory, a neurocognitive function that has been found highly predictive of IQ and academic performance.46

PBTSs showed worse response inhibition skills than the controls, contrary to what we found in a meta-analysis.14 Response inhibition is an important skill for social functioning, with impaired inhibition being associated with social problems.47

Results in VMI when measured with a pen and paper task (Beery Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration)48 have been ambiguous.49–54 As stated in the introduction, computerized tasks allow the workload for a specific function to be increased while maintaining other conditions as equal. If an increasing difficulty level leads to greater performance decrements in PBTSs then this would pinpoint a deficit in VMI not confounded by other functions necessary to conduct the task. With the computerized VMI task, we were able to demonstrate that the performance of PBTSs decreased to a greater extent with an increase in VMI workload in terms of speed than the performance of controls, illustrating a poorer ability to rapidly integrate visual information in order to guide a motor response. Compared to the structured condition, the unstructured condition resulted in poorer task performance, indicating that performance became worse when participants were required to trace a target following an unpredictable course as compared to a target following a predictable course. However this effect was not different for PBTSs and controls, suggesting that predictability of the visuomotor response does not explain poor visuomotor performance in PBTSs, but that these problems rather result from deficits in VMI speed. Impairments in VMI speed may cause difficulty in writing ability under conditions of time pressure, which often exists in the school context. Poor writing skills may affect schoolwork and is associated with poor school performance and low self-esteem.55 Previously, studies have reported that medical factors such as hydrocephalus, young age at diagnosis, or cranial irradiation had a negative effect on neurocognitive functioning in medulloblastoma and ependymoma survivors.37,56–59 In our sample of PBTSs with neurocognitive complaints, none of the medical risk factors were predictive of neurocognitive functioning. Our finding suggests that even children who do not carry the commonly reported risk factors for poor neurocognitive outcomes, such as hydrocephalus, young

Page 79: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

77

4

age at diagnosis, or cranial irradiation, may still be at risk in terms of their neurocognitive functioning. We were surprised to find this, as often in the literature an association is found between medical risk factors and neurocognitive functioning.14 It would have been interesting to analyze risk factors for poor neurocognitive functioning using each of the neurocognitive measures separately. However, given the sample size and the number of tests, it was not possible to correct for multiple testing. Alternatively, to reduce the chance of type I error, we grouped all neurocognitive measures into one aggregated score using PCA. The resulting one factor harbored the variance common to all neurocognitive measures. PCA also allows a reduction of error variance, since scores on a factor derived from PCA have higher reliability compared to the variables contributing to this factor.60 It must be noted that the participants were younger at diagnosis, had a longer time since diagnosis and had more often had chemotherapy and radiotherapy than the non-participants, which possibly caused the group of participants to have relatively worse neurocognitive functioning than the non-participants. This may have increased homogeneity and therefore contributed to the lack of association we found between the risk factors and neurocognitive functioning. We did not find evidence for a possible moderating effect of age on treatment, as we found a small and non-significant correlation between both age and age at diagnosis and radiation therapy. It must be noted that in the Netherlands, children diagnosed with a brain tumor at very young age (<3 years) are not treated with radiotherapy. Our study was limited to PBTSs with subjective neurocognitive complaints, which may have limited the range of scores on our neurocognitive function measures. However, this interpretation seems unlikely given the spread observed in our neurocognitive function measures. Although numerous studies have reported correlations between risk factors and neurocognitive functioning, we are not the first study to report no significant correlation with neurocognitive functioning; e.g., Ris et al.59 and Butler et al.35 did not find a relationship between gender and neurocognitive outcome, Grill et al.61 found no significant association between tumor location and intelligence in posterior fossa survivors, and Riva et al.62 and Mulhern, Fairclough, and Ochs did not find a significant association between cognitive outcomes and treatment intensity (chemotherapy and cranial irradiation, respectively).63

In line with previous research, we found that PBTSs have slower reaction times than controls.7,43,64,65 Moreover, the profile of neurocognitive functions shows that tasks which are timed or draw more heavily on information processing speed, such as reaction time and estimated FSIQ, all seem to be more affected than untimed tasks, such as performance on memory tasks. This finding is in accordance with the fact that estimated FSIQ was found to be significantly correlated with processing speed (r = −.45,p < .001). White matter damage might play a crucial role in information processing speed. Palmer and Wolfe et al. suggest a model in which neurological damage, and in particular damage to the white matter resulting from the tumor and the treatment of the tumor, is the underlying cause of processing

Page 80: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

78

speed deficits.47,66 These processing speed deficits in turn cascade into deficits in other neurocognitive functions, intelligence, and academic achievement. Indeed, several studies have found slower processing speed to be associated with damaged white matter tracts in PBTSs.67,68 Also, it has been reported that processing speed mediates the association between white matter integrity and reading skills.69 Another study by the same research group reported intellectual outcomes as being associated with white matter integrity.70 White matter is vulnerable to the negative effects of treatment for a brain tumor, especially in young children, whose white matter is still immature. Damage to neural progenitor cells, for example, caused by cranial radiation therapy, may challenge healthy age-appropriate white matter growth.71 In our sample, 42% of the PBTSs underwent irradiation as treatment. However, in additional exploratory analyses we did not find any differences on any of the neurocognitive domains studied between the irradiated and the non-irradiated group, or between groups receiving different radiation doses and locations: (1) no irradiation; (2) local irradiation only, excluding supratentorial ≥50 gray; (3) craniospinal irradiation ≥23 gray and <36; (4) craniospinal irradiation ≥36 gray; and (5) local irradiation supratentorial ≥50 gray. Further study is clearly warranted.The neurocognitive deficits we have found in our sample of PBTSs are in line with the problems these children experience in daily life. In our sample, 48% have repeated a class and 24% of the PBTSs received special education. This underscores the importance of support for the PBTSs with neurocognitive problems. Teachers and parents need to be informed of possible late effects and taught how to assist PBTSs suffering from poor neurocognitive functioning, for example by giving these children more time to complete tasks. Furthermore, future studies should use more fine-grained measures of medical predictors, rather than dichotomous measures. It would also be interesting to test whether white matter integrity, as assessed using diffusion tensor imaging, mediates the association between medical risk factors and neurocognitive functioning, as has been found in reading skills.69 Lastly, there is a clear call for the development of (non-medical) interventions targeting neurocognitive late effects. Some interesting studies are currently being carried out, including a study using working memory training (CogMed) and neurofeedback training (PRISMA study).18,72

This study has some limitations that should be taken into account. Firstly, the results cannot be generalized beyond PBTSs with neurocognitive complaints, as study inclusion was based on the presence of parent-reported neurocognitive problems in the PBTSs and participants were also invited for participation in a treatment study (ClinicalTrials.gov, NCT00961922). Secondly, the heterogeneous group of PBTSs harbors mutually interacting risk factors, which make it difficult to investigate the influence of earlier described predictors of neurocognitive outcome. Another limitation is that only PBTSs who were willing to participate in the PRISMA study were included. This inclusion process may have led to an underestimation of the impairments, as some children declined participation due to severe neuropsychological

Page 81: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

79

4

impairments. On the other hand, this may have led to an overestimation of neurocognitive impairments, as children without complaints did not participate. Also, the non-participants were on average older at diagnosis, had a shorter time since diagnosis, and were less often treated with chemotherapy and radiotherapy. These differences suggest that a more severely impaired group of PBTSs participated in our study, which may have led to an overestimation of the neurocognitive problems faced by the general population of PBTSs. There are also advantages inherent to the current study’s design: it targeted a broad range of neurocognitive functions using measures with built-in control conditions designed to isolate each neurocognitive function, computerized tests were used to enhance the reliability of measurement, and the inclusion of siblings as control group allowed for controlling genetic factors and socioeconomic background.To summarize, PBTSs with neurocognitive complaints exhibit a broad range of neurocognitive late effects. The results suggest that even PBTSs that have traditionally been viewed as at low risk of developing neurocognitive problems (e.g., surgery only, no hydrocephalus) may suffer from decreased neurocognitive functioning.

Page 82: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

80

REFERENCES

1. Kaatsch P. Epidemiology of childhood cancer. Cancer Treat Rev. 2010;36(4):277–85. 2. Mulhern RK, Merchant TE, Gajjar A, Reddick WE, Kun LE. Late neurocognitive sequelae in

survivors of brain tumours in childhood. Lancet Oncol. 2004;5(7):399–408. 3. Turner CD, Rey-Casserly C, Liptak CC, Chordas C. Late effects of therapy for pediatric brain tumor

survivors. J Clin Oncol. 2009;24(11):1455–63. 4. Reddick WE, White H a, Glass JO, Wheeler GC, Thompson SJ, Gajjar A, et al. Developmental

model relating white matter volume to neurocognitive deficits in pediatric brain tumor survivors. Cancer. 2003;97(10):2512–9.

5. Kahalley LS, Wilson SJ, Tyc VL, Conklin HM, Hudson MM, Wu S, et al. Are the psychological needs of adolescent survivors of pediatric cancer adequately identified and treated ? Psychooncology. 2013;22:447–58.

6. Moyer KH, Willard VW, Gross AM, Netson KL, Ashford JM, Kahalley LS, et al. The Impact of Attention on Social Functioning in Survivors of Pediatric Acute Lymphoblastic Leukemia and Brain Tumors. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2012;59(7):1290–5.

7. Mabbott DJ, Penkman L, Witol A. Core neurocognitive functions in children treated for posterior fossa tumors. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(2):159–68.

8. Steinlin M, Imfeld S, Zulauf P, Boltshauser E, Lovblad KO, Ridolfi LA, et al. Neuropsychological long-term sequelae after posterior fossa tumour resection during childhood. Brain. 2003;126:1998–2008.

9. Briere M-EE, Scott JG, McNall-Knapp RY, Adams RL. Cognitive outcome in pediatric brain tumor survivors: delayed attention deficit at long-term follow-up. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2008;50(2):337–40.

10. Shortman RI, Lowis SP, Penn A, Mccarter RJ, Hunt LP, Brown CC, et al. Cognitive Function in Children With Brain Tumors in the First Year After Diagnosis Compared to Healthy Matched Controls. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2013;61:464–72.

11. De Kieviet JF, Stoof CJJ, Geldof CJ a, Smits N, Piek JP, Lafeber HN, et al. The crucial role of the predictability of motor response in visuomotor deficits in very preterm children at school age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(7):624–30.

12. de Zeeuw P, Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Bijlhout J, König C, Post Uiterweer A, Papanikolau A, et al. Inhibitory performance, response speed, intraindividual variability, and response accuracy in ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(7):808–16.

13. Ullrich NJ, Embry L. Neurocognitive dysfunction in survivors of childhood brain tumors. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2012;19(1):35–42.

14. de Ruiter MA, van Mourik R, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Grootenhuis MA, Oosterlaan J. Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatment: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(5):408–17.

15. Duffner PK. Risk factors for cognitive decline in children treated for brain tumors. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2010;14(2):106–15.

16. Bartels M, Rietveld MJH, Van Baal GCM, Boomsma DI. Genetic and environmental influences on the development of intelligence. Behav Genet. 2002;32(4):237–49.

17. Burt C. The genetic determination of differences in intelligence: a study of monozygotic twins reared together and apart. Br J Psychol. 1966;2:137–53.

18. de Ruiter MA, Schouten-Van Meeteren AYN, Van Mourik R, Janssen TWP, Greidanus JEM, Oosterlaan J, et al. Neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive functioning of children treated for a brain tumor: design of a randomized controlled double-blind trial. BMC Cancer. 2012;12:581.

Page 83: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

81

4

19. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage. 2005;26(2):471–9.

20. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI, Petersen SE. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(3):340–7.

21. de Kieviet JF, van Elburg RM, Lafeber HN, Oosterlaan J. Attention problems of very preterm children compared with age-matched term controls at school-age. J Pediatr. 2012;161(5):824–9.

22. Nutley SB, Söderqvist S, Bryde S, Humphreys K, Klingberg T. Measuring working memory capacity with greater precision in the lower capacity ranges. Dev Neuropsychol. 2010;35(1):81–95.

23. Wechsler D. Wechsler’s Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd Edition. London: Harcourt Brace and Company; 1992.

24. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation; 1997.

25. Miyake A. Individual differences in working memory: introduction to the special section. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001;130(2):163–8.

26. Kindlon D, Mezzacappa E, Earls F. Psychometric Properties of Impulsivity Measures: Temporal Stability, Validity and Factor Structure. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995 May 7;36(4):645–61.

27. Logan GD, Cowan WB. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychol Rev. 1984;91(3):295–327.

28. Wolpert DM. Computational approaches control. Trends Cogn Sci. 1997;1(6):209–16. 29. De Sonneville LM. Amsterdam Neuropsychological Task: A computer-aided assessment program.

In: Brinker BPLM Den, Beek PJ, Brand AN, Maarse SJ, Mulder LJM, editors. Cognitive Ergonomics, Clinical Assessment and Computer-assisted Learning (Computers in Psychology, 6). 6th ed. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1999. p. 204–17.

30. Sattler JM. Assessment of children. 3rd ed. San Diego, CA: Jerome M, Sattler Publisher, Inc.; 1992.

31. Cohen J. The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: a review. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1962;65:145–53.

32. Maccallum RC, Widaman KF, Zhang S, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis. Psychol Methods. 1999;4(1):84–99.

33. MacCallum RC, Widaman KF, Preacher KJ, Hong S. Sample size in factor analysis: The role of model error. Multivariate Behav Res. 2001;36(4):611–37.

34. Hutcheson GD, Sofroniou N. The Multivariate Social Scientist: Introductory Statistics Using Generalized Linear Models. London: Sage; 1999.

35. Butler RW, Fairclough DL, Katz ER, Kazak AE, Noll RB, Thompson RD, et al. Intellectual functioning and multi-dimensional attentional processes in long-term survivors of a central nervous system related pediatric malignancy. Life Sci. 2013;93(17):611–6.

36. Kiehna EN, Mulhern RK, Li C, Xiong X, Merchant TE. Changes in attentional performance of children and young adults with localized primary brain tumors after conformal radiation therapy. J Clin Oncol. 2006;24(33):5283–90.

37. Mulhern RK, White HA, Glass JO, Kun LE, Leigh L, Thompson SJ, et al. Attentional functioning and white matter integrity among survivors of malignant brain tumors of childhood. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:180–9.

38. Mabbott DJ, Spiegler BJ, Greenberg ML, Rutka JT, Hyder DJ, Bouffet E. Serial evaluation of academic and behavioral outcome after treatment with cranial radiation in childhood. J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(10):2256–63.

39. Conklin HM, Ashford JM, Howarth R a, Merchant TE, Ogg RJ, Santana VM, et al. Working Memory Performance among Childhood Brain Tumor Survivors. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2012;18(6):996–1005.

Page 84: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 4

82

40. Dennis M, Hetherington CR, Spiegler BJ. Memory and attention after childhood brain tumors. Med Pediatr Oncol. 1998;Suppl 1:25–33.

41. Maddrey AM, Bergeron JA, Lombardo ER, McDonald NK, Mulne AF, Barenberg PD, et al. Neuropsychological performance and quality of life of 10 year survivors of childhood medulloblastoma. J Neurooncol. 2005;72(3):245–53.

42. Reeves CB, Palmer SL, Reddick WE, Merchant TE, Buchanan GM, Gajjar A, et al. Attention and memory functioning among pediatric patients with medulloblastoma. J Pediatr Psychol. 2006;31(3):272–80.

43. Spiegler BJ, Bouffet E, Greenberg ML, Rutka JT, Mabbott DJ. Change in neurocognitive functioning after treatment with cranial radiation in childhood. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22(4):706–13.

44. Baddeley A. Working memory. Philos Trans R Soc London. 1983;203(1110):311–24. 45. Jayakar R, King TZ, Morris R. Hippocampal Volume and Auditory Attention on a Verbal Memory

Task with Adult Survivors of Pediatric Brain Tumor. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(2):303–19. 46. Shelton JT, Elliott EM, Matthews RA, Hill BD, Gouvier WD. The relationships of working memory,

secondary memory, and general fluid intelligence: working memory is special. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2010;36(3):813–20.

47. Wolfe KR, Madan-Swain A, Kana RK. Executive dysfunction in pediatric posterior fossa tumor survivors: a systematic literature review of neurocognitive deficits and interventions. Dev Neuropsychol. 2012;37(2):153–75.

48. Beery K. The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration with supplemental developmental tests of visual-perception integration and motor coordination: administration, scoring and teaching manual. 4th ed. Parsippany, NJ: Modern Curriculum Press; 1997.

49. Aarsen FK, Paquier PF, Arts W-FM, Van Veelen ML, Michiels E, Lequin M, et al. Cognitive deficits and predictors 3 years after diagnosis of a pilocytic astrocytoma in childhood. J Clin Oncol. 2009;27(21):3526–32.

50. Beebe DW, Ris MD, Armstrong FD, Fontanesi J, Mulhern RK, Holmes E, et al. Cognitive and adaptive outcome in low-grade pediatric cerebellar astrocytomas: Evidence of diminished cognitive and adaptive functioning in national collaborative research studies (CCG 9891/POG 9130). J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(22):5198–204.

51. Bonner MJ, Hardy KK, Willard VW, Gururangan S. Additional Evidence of a Nonverbal Learning Disability in Survivors of Pediatric Brain Tumors. Child Heal Care. 2009;38(1):49–63.

52. Carpentieri SC, Waber DP, Pomeroy SL, Scott RM, Goumnerova LC, Kieran MW, et al. Neuropsychological functioning after surgery in children treated for brain tumor. Neurosurgery. 2003;52(6):1348–56.

53. Mabbott DJ, Monsalves E, Spiegler BJ, Bartels U, Janzen L, Guger S, et al. Longitudinal evaluation of neurocognitive function after treatment for central nervous system germ cell tumors in childhood. Cancer. 2011;117(23):5402–11.

54. Ris MD, Beebe DW, Armstrong FD, Fontanesi J, Holmes E, Sanford RA, et al. Cognitive and adaptive outcome in extracerebellar low-grade brain tumors in children: a report from the Children’s Oncology Group. J Clin Oncol. 2008;26(29):4765–70.

55. Henderson SE, Hall D. Concomitants of Clumsiness in Young Schoolchildren. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2008 Nov 12;24(5):448–60.

56. Hardy KK, Bonner MJ, Willard VW, Watral MA, Gururangan S. Hydrocephalus as a possible additional contributor to cognitive outcome in survivors of pediatric medulloblastoma. Psychooncology. 2008;17(11):1157–61.

57. Merchant TE, Lee H, Zhu J, Xiong X, Wheeler G, Phipps S, et al. The effects of hydrocephalus on intelligence quotient in children with localized infratentorial ependymoma before and after focal radiation therapy. J Neurosurg. 2004;101(2 Suppl):159–68.

Page 85: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Timed performance weaknesses in PBTS

83

4

58. Mulhern RK, Palmer SL, Merchant TE, Wallace D, Kocak M, Brouwers P, et al. Neurocognitive consequences of risk-adapted therapy for childhood medulloblastoma. J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(24):5511–9.

59. Ris MD, Walsh K, Wallace D, Armstrong FD, Holmes E, Gajjar A, et al. Intellectual and Academic Outcome Following Two Chemotherapy Regimens and Radiotherapy for Average-Risk Medulloblastoma: COG A9961. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2013;60:1350–7.

60. Stevens JP. Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (4th ed.). Applied multivariate statistics for the social sciences (4th ed.). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Erlbaum; 2002.

61. Grill J, Viguier D, Kieffer V, Bulteau C, Sainte-Rose C, Hartmann O, et al. Critical risk factors for intellectual impairment in children with posterior fossa tumors: the role of cerebellar damage. J Neurosurg. 2004;101(2 Suppl):152–8.

62. Riva D, Massimino M, Giorgi C, Nichelli F, Erbetta A, Usilla A, et al. Cognition before and after chemotherapy alone in children with chiasmatic-hypothalamic tumors. J Neurooncol. 2009 Mar;92(1):49–56.

63. Mulhern RK, Fairclough D, Ochs J. A prospective comparison of neuropsychologic performance of children surviving leukemia who received 18-Gy, 24-Gy, or no cranial irradiation. J Clin Oncol. 1991;9(8):1348–56.

64. Conklin HM, Ashford JM, Di Pinto M, Vaughan CG, Gioia G a, Merchant TE, et al. Computerized assessment of cognitive late effects among adolescent brain tumor survivors. J Neurooncol. 2013;113(2):222–340.

65. Padovani L, Nicolas A, Constine LS, Muracciole X, André N. Neurocognitive function after radiotherapy for paediatric brain tumours. Nat Rev Neurol. 2012;8(10):578–88.

66. Palmer SL. Neurodevelopmental impact on children treated for medulloblastoma: A review and proposed conceptual model. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2008;14(3):203–10.

67. Aukema EJ, Caan M, Oudhuis N, Majoie C, Vos F, Reneman L, et al. White matter fractional anisotropy correlates with speed of processing and motor speed in young childhood cancer survivors. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys. 2009;74(3):837–43.

68. Palmer SL, Glass JO, Li Y, Ogg R, Qaddoumi I, Armstrong GT, et al. White matter integrity is associated with cognitive processing in patients treated for a posterior fossa brain tumor. Neuro Oncol. 2012;14(9):1185–93.

69. Smith KM, King TZ, Jayakar R, Morris RD. Reading Skill in Adult Survivors of Childhood Brain Tumor: A Theory-Based Neurocognitive Model. Neuropsychology. 2014;28(3):448–58.

70. King TZ, Wang L, Mao H. Disruption of White Matter Integrity in Adult Survivors of Childhood Brain Tumors: Correlates with Long-Term Intellectual Outcomes. PLoS One. 2015 Jan 6;10(7):e0131744.

71. Redmond KJ, Mahone EM, Terezakis S, Ishaq O, Ford E, McNutt T, et al. Association between radiation dose to neuronal progenitor cell niches and temporal lobes and performance on neuropsychological testing in children: a prospective study. Neuro Oncol. 2013;15(3):360–9.

72. Sung L, Zaoutis T, Ullrich NJ, Johnston D, Dupuis L, Ladas E. Children’ s Oncology Group’s 2013 Blueprint for Research: Cancer Control and Supportive Care. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2013;60:1027–30.

Page 86: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

Page 87: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

5

Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor

survivors with neurocognitive complaints

Marieke A. de RuiterAntoinette Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren

Dannis G. van VuurdenHeleen Maurice-Stam

Corrie GiddingLaura R. Beek

Bernd GranzenJaap Oosterlaan

Martha A. Grootenhuis

Quality of Life Research 2016, 25(2), 435-446

Page 88: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

86

ABSTRACT

Aim: With more children surviving a brain tumor, neurocognitive consequences of the tumor and its treatment become apparent, which could affect psychosocial functioning. The present study therefore aimed to assess psychosocial functioning of pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTS) in detail.

Methods: Psychosocial functioning of PBTS (8–18 years) with parent-reported neurocognitive complaints was compared to normative data on health-related quality of life (HRQOL), self-esteem, psychosocial adjustment, and executive functioning (one-sample t tests) and to a sibling control group on fatigue (independent-samples t test). Self-, parent-, and teacher-report questionnaires were included, where appropriate, providing complementary information.

Results: Eighty-two PBTS (mean age 13.4 years, SD 3.2, 49 % males) and 43 healthy siblings (mean age 14.3, SD 2.4, 40 % males) were included. As compared to the normative population, PBTS themselves reported decreased physical, psychological, and generic HRQOL (d = 0.39–0.62, p < 0.008). Compared to siblings, increased fatigue-related concentration problems (d = 0.57, p < 0.01) were reported, although self-reported self-esteem and psychosocial adjustment seemed not to be affected. Parents of PBTS reported more psychosocial (d = 0.81, p < 0.000) and executive problems (d = 0.35–0.43, p < 0.016) in their child than parents of children in the normative population. Teachers indicated more psychosocial adjustment problems for female PBTS aged 8–11 years than for the female normative population (d = 0.69, p < 0.025), but they reported no more executive problems.

Conclusions: PBTS with parent-reported neurocognitive complaints showed increased psychosocial problems, as reported by PBTS, parents, and teachers. Systematic screening of psychosocial functioning is necessary so that tailored support from professionals can be offered to PBTS with neurocognitive complaints.

Page 89: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

87

5

INTRODUCTION

Due to developments in the medical field, survival rates in children with a brain tumor have increased drastically to over 74 %.1 These successes have led to a growing number of pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTS). Tumor- and treatment-induced brain injury exerts negative effects on neurocognitive functions, such as attention, processing speed, and memory.2 As a result, 40–100 % of PBTS suffer from neurocognitive decline.3 The decline in neurocognitive functioning appears to increase when the children grow older, resulting in an increasing gap between the PBTS and their peers.4–6 Consequently, children treated for a brain tumor may experience lower academic achievements, resulting in lower vocational success, and decreased psychosocial functioning compared to their healthy peers later in life.7–9

To date, studies on psychosocial functioning of PBTS are relatively scarce as compared to other types of cancer, especially because children with a brain tumor have often been excluded from studies, due to their atypical outcomes, i.e., they seem to suffer from more serious problems on a variety of domains (e.g., neurocognitive, social, and adjustment problems) than other pediatric cancer survivors.9 The studies with PBTS found in the literature focused on health-related quality of life (HRQOL), social competence, self-esteem, and fatigue. Attention for HRQOL, a multidimensional construct covering perceived physical, emotional, mental, social, and behavioral components of well-being and functioning,10 in PBTS has started to emerge in the past decades.11 However, no studies to our knowledge have focused on PBTS with neurocognitive complaints. The results of the studies on HRQOL in PBTS in general were contradictory, with HRQOL comparable to the general population,12 or worse HRQOL in several domains.13 PBTS reported being bullied, encountering problems with peers, and suffering from stressful and depressive feelings. The researchers mention late effects of the cancer treatment as a possible cause of the decreased HRQOL scores. Decreased neurocognitive functioning was found to be associated with worse HRQOL in PBTS 1 year after treatment.14

The literature on self-esteem in PBTS is scarce; however, social competence, an aspect of self-esteem which may predict psychosocial functioning, has been investigated in PBTS.15 In a comprehensive review on social competence, it was concluded that PBTS experienced deficits in this area.16 In a cross-sectional study, PBTS reported lower social competence than healthy peers and patients with a pediatric brain tumor during treatment, indicative of a decline of social competence of PBTS over time.17 Furthermore, PBTS experienced more problems with self-confidence and self-esteem compared to leukemia survivors.18

Fatigue is a common adverse effect of cancer treatment.19,20 In addition, due to the nature of their disease, PBTS frequently experience sleep problems and decreased sleep quality, leading to fatigue and negatively influencing daily functioning.21 Fatigue in childhood cancer survivors and PBTS is associated with worse psychosocial functioning.22,23

Page 90: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

88

The influence of executive deficits on psychosocial functioning has been acknowledged.24 Executive functions, an umbrella term for mental skills concerning planning, behavioral control, and self-regulation, such as attention control, cognitive flexibility, and goal setting are critical skills to function properly in society.24 Executive functions are often reported to be affected in PBTS.25–27

Since psychosocial functioning is important but understudied in PBTS as compared to other types of cancer,9,28 we aimed to investigate various domains of psychosocial functioning of PBTS who suffer from parent-reported neurocognitive complaints: HRQOL, self-esteem, psychosocial adjustment, executive functioning, and fatigue. Based on the previous, we can conclude that it is especially important to study psychosocial functioning of PBTS who suffer from neurocognitive problems, as the literature indicated that patients with neurocognitive problems are vulnerable to psychosocial problems. We take multiple informants (self-, parent-, and teacher report) into account, providing complementary information on how PBTS function, both at home and at school, investigating psychosocial functioning of PBTS who suffer from parent-reported neurocognitive complaints. We hypothesize that PBTS experience decreased HRQOL, self-esteem, psychosocial adjustment, executive functioning, and increased fatigue as reported by PBTS themselves, their parents, and/or teachers.

METHODS

Procedures and participantsData collection took place between January 2010 and August 2012, as part of the PRISMA study, a randomized placebo-controlled double-blind trial to investigate whether neurofeedback can improve neurocognitive functioning in PBTS.29 Eligible for inclusion were children treated for a brain tumor in the Netherlands, aged 8–18 years, who finished treatment >2 years prior to enrollment and who suffered from neurocognitive complaints as reported by a parent on a screening questionnaire, assessing attention, speed, information processing and memory as well as exclusion criteria. Children with a premorbid diagnosis of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, a mental or physical condition prohibiting neurocognitive assessment, or insufficient mastery of the Dutch language were excluded from the study.PBTS (n = 249) who were treated in one of the participating Dutch hospitals (Emma Children’s Hospital/Academic Medical Center Amsterdam, VU University Medical Center Amsterdam, University Medical Center Utrecht, St. Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, and University Medical Center Maastricht) and their parents received a letter via their pediatric oncologist or psychologist informing them about the PRISMA study. Additionally, three patients from other hospitals made contact via email about participation, after they learned about the study.

Page 91: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

89

5

Of the PBTS, 89 (35 %) did not meet inclusion criteria and 71 (29 %) declined participation (‘non-participants’) (see Fig.1 for reasons). Parents of PBTS willing to participate (n = 92, 37 %) were provided with an online screening questionnaire concerning their child’s neurocognitive functioning, in order to verify eligibility. Ten PBTS (4 %) were excluded after online screening.

79

Fig. 1. Flowchart of inclusion. Note. Self-reported questionnaires were completed by 81 PBTS and 40 sibling controls. Parent data were available for all 82 PBTS, and teacher data were available for 73 of the included PBTS

Figure 1. Flowchart of inclusion. Note. Self-reported questionnaires were completed by 81 PBTS and 40 sibling controls. Parent data were available for all 82 PBTS, and teacher data were available for 73 of the included PBTS

If the included PBTS had a sibling in the age range from 8 to 18 years, he or she was invited via telephone to participate in the control group for the fatigue outcome measure. Siblings were not considered to be optimal as a control group for the other psychosocial outcomes since the cancer diagnosis of the sibling could affect the scores on psychosocial functioning.30,31

Informed consent was obtained from the included 82 PBTS (33 %) and 43 siblings. Subsequently, PBTS, parents, siblings, and the case where parents and PBTS gave permission (n = 76), teachers of PBTS, were sent the questionnaires via email. For parent-report questionnaires, the primary caregiver was asked to fill out the questionnaire. For the teacher-report questionnaires, the parent was asked to indicate which teacher was most suitable to fill out questionnaires about the functioning of the child.The study protocol was approved by the Medical Ethics Committee of the Academic Medical Center Amsterdam and was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (NCT00961922).

Demographic and medical characteristicsParents of participating PBTS supplied information on gender and demographics (the parental country of origin and the highest level of parental education). Medical characteristics were taken from the medical records and included tumor histopathology and grade, type

Page 92: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

90

of treatment (surgery only vs. chemotherapy and/or radiotherapy with/without surgery), tumor location (supratentorial vs. infratentorial) and prior hydrocephalus, age at diagnosis, and time since diagnosis.Medical and demographic data were also collected for a subsample of non-participants (45 out of 71 non-participants) to study selection bias at inclusion. As the non-participants declined participation, they were not assessed. To compare the age of participants and non-participants, ‘age at assessment’ for non-participants was calculated as the difference between the birth date and the average assessment date of participating PBTS.

Outcome measuresIt is well-known that proxy report (parent/teacher) on the functioning of chronically ill children often yields discrepancies with self-report, although results of studies have been contradictory.32 For this reason, we included a combination of self-report, parent-report, and teacher-report questionnaires.

Self-reportHRQOL - The Kidscreen-27 was administered to evaluate HRQOL in children by means of 27 items, scored on a 5-point Likert scale, divided over 5 dimensions: physical well-being, psychological well-being, autonomy and parents, peers and social support, and school environment.33 In addition, a Generic score was calculated by summing the ten items that comprise the Kidscreen-10, a shorter version of the Kidscreen, derived from the Kidscreen-27.34 Raw scores for each dimension were transformed into T values with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 in a European sample.T values of a Dutch normative sample are available. Higher scores indicated better HRQOL. The Kidscreen-27 and Kidscreen-10 have good levels of validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha normative samples >0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS 0.71–0.88).33,34 The Dutch normative sample did not differ in age and gender distribution from the PBTS group (p > 0.10).

Self-esteem - We used the self-perception profile for children (SPPC, age 8–12) and adolescents (SPPA, age 12–18) to investigate self-esteem.35–37 The SPPC consists of 36 items, divided into six scales: scholastic competence, social acceptance, athletic competence, physical appearance, behavioral conduct, and global self-worth. The adolescent version (SPPA) contains 35 items and comprises one additional scale: close friendship. Each item was presented on a 4-point Likert scale, with higher scores indicating stronger self-esteem. The SPPC and SPPA have acceptable to good validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch manual >0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS 0.62–0.91).36,37 The manual provided mean scores for males and females separately. For comparison with the total group of PBTS, scores of males and females in the normative population were combined after weighting by the gender

Page 93: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

91

5

distribution in the PBTS group. Age was not taken into account, as the SPPC and SPPA have separate norms based on age.

Psychosocial adjustment - The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) was used to assess psychosocial adjustment.38,39 The items were scored on a 3-point Likert scale. A total problem score was calculated by adding the scores of 20 items, with a higher score indicating more problems. The SDQ total problem score has good validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch controls = 0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS = 0.77).39 Dutch norms were available for children aged 11–16 years; therefore, analyses on the SDQ were restricted to PBTS aged 11–16 years. The gender distribution did not differ between the Dutch normative group and the PBTS, but mean age of the normative population was lower than of the PBTS. However, since total problem score is not affected by age, age was not taken into account in the analysis.

Fatigue - Fatigue was measured with the checklist individual strength (CIS).40 The CIS is a questionnaire that measures fatigue-related problems and consists of 20 items, scored on a 7-point Likert scale. The four subscales were subjective fatigue, concentration, motivation, and physical activity. A total score was calculated by summing up all items. Higher scores indicated more fatigue-related problems. The CIS has good reliability, with Cronbach’s alpha of the sibling controls and the PBTS 0.72–0.94. The data collected in the sibling control group were used for comparison with the PBTS. The sibling control group and the PBTS did not differ significantly (p > 0.10) in gender and age.

Parent reportPsychosocial adjustment - The SDQ was used to measure the parental perspective of PBTS’ psychosocial adjustment (see ‘Self-report’ for the description of the questionnaire). Reliability of the total problem score is good (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch controls = 0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS = 0.77).39 PBTS were compared to a Dutch normative sample of children aged 8–16. The Dutch normative sample did not differ in age and gender from the PBTS (p > 0.10).

Executive functioning - Parents rated their child’s behavioral executive functioning using the behavior rating inventory of executive function (BRIEF).41 The BRIEF contains 75 items, scored on a 3-point Likert scale. The scores were summarized in eight scales (inhibit, shift, emotional control, initiate, working memory, plan/organize, organization of materials, and monitor), two indices (behavioral index and metacognition index), and a total score. The raw scores of the scales and indices were transformed into age- and gender-specific standardized T scores, as provided in the manual, with a mean of 50 and a standard

Page 94: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

92

deviation of 10. Higher scores indicated more problems. Validity and reliability range from good to excellent, with Cronbach’s alphas reported in the manual between 0.78 and 0.96 and Cronbach’s alphas of the PBTS between 0.66 and 0.94.42

Teacher reportPsychosocial adjustment - The SDQ was used to measure the teacher perspective of psychosocial adjustment of the PBTS (see ‘Self-report’ for the description of the questionnaire). The reliability of the total problem score of the teacher report is reported to be good (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch controls = 0.88; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS = 0.77).39 A Dutch normative population of children aged 8–11 was available; therefore, the answers from teachers of PBTS within that age range were analyzed. The total problem score was analyzed separately for females and males, because the PBTS sample had more females than the normative population. The mean age of the Dutch normative population did not differ from the mean age of the PBTS aged 8–11.

Executive functioning- The BRIEF teacher-report version measures executive functioning of PBTS in the school situation (see ‘Parent-report’ for the description of the BRIEF). Validity and reliability are good to excellent, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from 0.88 to 0.98 as reported in the manual and between 0.82 and 0.97 of the PBTS.42

Statistical analysesAll analyses were conducted using SPSS version 20.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). To be able to detect possible confounders, one-sample t tests (age) and binomial tests (gender) were performed to test differences between PBTS and the normative population. Independent-samples t tests (age) and Chi-square tests (gender, country of birth, education) were used to test differences between PBTS and sibling controls. A p value of <0.10 was considered statistically significant for these analyses.Differences between the participating PBTS and the subsample of 45 non-participating PBTS were tested with one-sample t tests (age at assessment, age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis), binomial tests (gender, tumor grade, tumor location, treatment, and hydrocephalus), or Chi-square test (tumor type).One-sample t tests were used to evaluate differences between PBTS and the normative population regarding self-reported HRQOL, self-esteem, and regarding psychosocial adjustment, and proxy-reported psychosocial adjustment and executive functioning. Self-reported fatigue was analyzed with independent-samples t test (PBTS vs. sibling controls).Effect sizes were calculated in terms of Cohen’s d, with 0.20, 0.50, and 0.80, reflecting small, medium, and large effect sizes, respectively.43 To adjust for multiple testing, Bonferroni correction was applied to the significance levels, as follows: HRQOL and self-esteem

Page 95: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

93

5

8–11 years 0.05/6 = 0.008; self-esteem 12–18 years 0.05/7 = 0.007; fatigue 0.05/5 = 0.01; indices/total executive functioning 0.05/3 = 0.016; scales executive functioning 0.05/8 = 0.006. Differences with p values <0.05 in combination with effect size >0.35 were considered to be trends.

RESULTS

ParticipantsThe inclusion flowchart is depicted in Fig. 1. One enrolled PBTS, three enrolled siblings, and three teachers of enrolled PBTS did not complete the questionnaires. Self-report data were therefore available for 81 PBTS, 40 siblings, and teacher-report data for 73 PBTS.Characteristics of the participating PBTS, the sibling control group and the non-participating PBTS are depicted in Table 1. Regarding the demographics, 20 of the participating PBTS received special education (24 %) and 39 have been held back a class (48 %). The participating and non-participating PBTS were comparable in age at assessment, gender, and education (p > 0.062). Participants and non-participants did not differ in tumor location, but they did differ with regard to the distribution of tumor grade, with more high-grade tumors in the participants than in the non-participants (p < 0.05). The participants were younger at diagnosis (p < 0.05) and had a longer interval past diagnosis than the non-participants (p < 0.05). More participating than non-participating PBTS underwent radiotherapy (p < 0.05) and chemotherapy (p = 0.001). The participating PBTS and the sibling control group were comparable in age, gender, parental country of origin, and the highest level of parental education (p > 0.324).

OutcomesIn Tables 2 and 3, the results of the self-report and proxy-report questionnaires’ analyses are displayed, respectively. Figure 2 is a graphical summary of the results (effect sizes) from Tables 2 and 3, showing the profile of psychosocial functioning in PBTS. For self-esteem, average effect sizes are depicted, weighted by the number of PBTS who completed the SPPC and SPPA.

Page 96: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

94

Table 1. Demographics and medical information of participating pediatric brain tumor survivors, sibling controls, and non-participating pediatric brain tumor survivors

PBTS participants Controls PBTS non-participantsn = 82 n = 43 n = 45M SD M SD M SD

Age Age at assessment 13.85 3.15 14.27 2.44 14.28 3.04 Age diagnosis 6.87 3.77 – – 8.23 3.95 Time since diagnosis 6.98 3.57 – – 6.05 3.31

n % n % n %Gender Boys 40 49 17 40 26 58Country of origin mother Netherlands 71 87 37 86 n/a n/a Other 11 13 9 14 n/a n/aCountry of origin father Netherlands 73 89 40 93 n/a n/a Other 9 11 3 7 n/a n/aHighest education parenta

Low or Intermediate 39 48 23 54 n/a n/a High 43 52 20 46 n/a n/aEducation Regular education 62 80 n/a n/a 28 62 Special education 20 20 n/a n/a 11 24 Unknown 0 0 n/a n/a 6 13Tumor type and grade High grade 34 42 – – 13 29 Medulloblastoma 12 15 – – 6 13 Supratentorial PNET 8 10 – – 2 4 Ependymoma 5 6 – – 2 4 Astrocytoma gr III 5 6 – – 1 2 Germ cell tumor 4 5 – – 2 4 Low grade 48 59 – – 32 71 Low grade glioma 35 43 – – 26 58 Craniopharyngioma 7 9 – – 5 11 Plexus papilloma 6 7 – – 1 2Treatment Radiotherapy 34 42 – – 14 31 Chemotherapy 35 43 – – 12 27 Surgeryb 72 88 – – 41 91 Other 2 2 – – 1 2 Biopsy only 1 1 – – 1 2 CSF pressure relief only 1 1 – – 0 0

Page 97: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

95

5

n % n % n %Location Supratentorial 46 56 – – 22 49 Infratentorial 36 44 – – 23 51Hydrocephalus Yes 39 48 – – n/a n/a No 43 52 – – n/a n/a

The information was available for 45 of 71 non-participanting PBTS. The siblings did not differ significantly from the participating PBTS on any of the variables. The non-participanting PBTS differed from the participants on age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis, tumor type, tumor grade, radiotherapy and chemotherapyPBTS pediatric brain tumor survivors, M mean, SD standard deviation, n/a not available* p < .05; ** p < .001aHighest education of father or mother is reported: Low or Intermediate = Primary education, general secondary education and secondary vocational education; High = Higher vocational education and universityb37 patients were treated with surgery only

Page 98: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

96

Table 2. Psychosocial functioning of the pediatric brain tumor survivors compared to the controls; self-report

Measure nPBTS Controls Group differencesM SD M SD d p

HRQOL—KIDSCREEN-27 33/Kidscreen-10 34

Physical well-being 81 46.69 9.69 52.88 10.02 0.62 <0.001 Psychological well-being 81 49.09 9.09 52.79 9.46 0.39 <0.001 Autonomy and parents 81 51.99 8.21 53.95 9.51 0.21 0.035 Peers and social support 81 49.11 10.94 52.36 9.04 0.36 0.009 School environment 78 51.08 8.70 53.06 9.71 0.20 0.049 Generic (Kidscreen-10) 78 49.55 8.32 54.10 10.40 0.44 <0.001Self-esteem—SPPC (8–12)37 Scholastic competence 24 14.79 3.79 16.60 3.46 0.52 0.028 Social acceptance 24 18.33 3.91 17.55 3.68 0.21 0.334 Athletic competence 24 16.08 4.24 17.88 3.24 0.56 0.049 Physical appearance 24 20.67 3.50 19.24 3.95 0.36 0.058 Behavioral conduct 24 20.29 3.58 17.72 2.85 0.91 0.002 Global self-worth 24 20.63 3.32 19.55 3.06 0.35 0.126Self-esteem—SPPA (12–18)38 Scholastic competence 57 13.23 2.93 13.88 2.51 0.26 0.097 Social acceptance 57 15.14 2.97 15.34 2.73 0.07 0.607 Athletic competence 57 12.37 4.26 13.74 3.35 0.41 0.018 Physical appearance 57 13.67 3.50 13.82 3.27 0.05 0.742 Behavioral conduct 57 15.49 3.67 14.24 2.88 0.44 0.013 Global self-worth 57 16.47 3.52 17.08 2.80 0.22 0.196 Close friendship 57 15.28 3.71 15.52 2.84 0.08 0.634Psychosocial adjustment—SDQ (11–16)39

Total problem score 48 10.02 5.09 9.90 4.90 0.02 0.870Fatigue—CIS41 a

Subjective fatigue 76 23.57 11.16 20.53 10.75 0.25 0.168 Concentration 76 19.09 7.78 14.45 7.19 0.57 0.003 Motivation 76 11.29 4.91 9.82 4.87 0.27 0.133 Physical activity 76 9.27 5.00 6.97 4.16 0.45 0.011 Total score 76 63.23 21.80 51.76 21.88 0.47 0.010

Significant differences after Bonferroni correction are presented in bold. Effect sizes ‘d’ were calculated by dividing the difference in mean score between the PBTS and the normative population or sibling controls by the pooled standard deviation. Lower scores reflect worse HRQOL and Self-Esteem. Higher scores reflect more problems on Psychosocial adjustment and fatiguePBTS pediatric brain tumor survivors, HRQOL health related quality of life, M mean, SD standard deviationa Sibling controls. n = 40

Page 99: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

97

5

Table 3. Psychosocial functioning of the pediatric brain tumor survivors compared to the controls; proxy report

nPBTS Controls Group differencesM SD M SD d p

Parent reportPsychosocial adjustment—SDQ (8–16)39 Total problem score 67 11.01 5.16 6.70 5.30 0.81 <0.001Behavioral executive functioning—BRIEF43

Behavioral index 82 53.48 11.22 50.00 10.00 0.35 0.002 Metacognition index 82 54.09 8.11 50.00 10.00 0.41 <0.001 Total score 82 54.29 8.45 50.00 10.00 0.43 <0.001Teacher reportPsychosocial adjustment—SDQ (8–12)39 Total problem score males 9 10.33 9.44 9.10 6.60 0.19 0.582 Total problem score females 21 9.71 5.60 5.80 5.70 0.69 0.004Behavioral executive functioning—BRIEF43 Behavioral index 73 50.89 12.41 50.00 10.00 0.09 0.511 Metacognition index 73 51.89 13.90 50.00 10.00 0.19 0.249 Total score 73 51.29 12.72 50.00 10.00 0.13 0.395

Significant differences after Bonferroni correction are presented in bold. Effect sizes ‘d’ were calculated by dividing the difference in mean score between the PBTS and the normative population by the pooled standard deviation. Higher scores reflect worse psychosocial adjustment and behavioral functioningPBTS pediatric brain tumor survivors, M mean, SD standard deviation

Page 100: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

98

89

Fi

g. 2

Pr

ofile

of

psyc

hoso

cial

fun

ctio

ning

in p

edia

tric

bra

in t

umor

sur

vivo

rs in

sta

ndar

dize

d ef

fect

siz

es a

s co

mpa

red

to t

he m

ean

of t

he c

ontr

ol g

roup

(0.

00).

*S

igni

fica

nt d

iffe

renc

e be

twee

n PB

TS a

nd c

ontr

ols

afte

r th

e Bo

nfer

roni

cor

rect

ion,

eff

ect

size

s pr

esen

ted

in red

and

bold.

Note.

Eff

ect

size

s ‘d

’ w

ere

calc

ulat

ed u

sing

the

poo

led

stan

dard

dev

iati

on.

Scor

es h

ave

been

adj

uste

d in

a w

ay t

hat

for

all d

omai

ns,

low

er s

core

s re

flec

t w

orse

psy

chos

ocia

l fu

ncti

onin

g. F

or s

elf-

este

em,

wei

ghte

d av

erag

e ef

fect

siz

es o

f th

e SP

PC a

nd S

PPA

are

depi

cted

. Fo

r te

ache

r re

port

of

psyc

hoso

cial

adj

ustm

ent

(SD

Q),

sc

ores

for

mal

es a

nd f

emal

es a

re r

epor

ted

sepa

rate

ly d

ue t

o m

ore

fem

ales

in o

ur s

ampl

e as

com

pare

d to

the

con

trol

gro

up.

HRQ

OL

= he

alth

-rel

ated

qu

alit

y of

life

, ph

ysic

al =

phy

sica

l wel

l-be

ing

subs

cale

of

the

Kids

cree

n-27

, ps

ych

= ps

ycho

logi

cal w

ell-

bein

g su

bsca

le o

f th

e Ki

dscr

een-

27,

auto

nom

y =

auto

nom

y an

d pa

rent

s su

bsca

le o

f th

e Ki

dscr

een-

27,

peer

s =

peer

s an

d so

cial

sup

port

sub

scal

e of

the

Kid

scre

en-2

7, s

choo

l = s

choo

l en

viro

nmen

t su

bsca

le o

f th

e Ki

dscr

een-

27,

gene

ric

= ge

neri

c he

alth

-rel

ated

qua

lity

of li

fe s

ubsc

ale

of t

he K

idsc

reen

-10,

sch

olas

tic

= sc

hola

stic

co

mpe

tenc

e su

bsca

le o

f th

e SP

PC/S

PPA,

soc

ial =

soc

ial a

ccep

tanc

e su

bsca

le o

f th

e SP

PC/S

PPA,

ath

leti

c =

athl

etic

com

pete

nce

subs

cale

of

the

SPPC

/SPP

A,

appe

aran

ce =

phy

sica

l app

eara

nce

subs

cale

of

the

SPPC

/SPP

A, c

ondu

ct =

beh

avio

ral c

ondu

ct s

ubsc

ale

of t

he S

PPC/

SPPA

, se

lf-w

orth

= g

loba

l sel

f-w

orth

su

bsca

le o

f th

e SP

PC/S

PPA,

fri

ends

hip

= cl

ose

frie

ndsh

ip s

ubsc

ale

of t

he S

PPA,

psy

adj

= p

sych

osoc

ial a

djus

tmen

t, t

otal

sco

re S

DQ

, fa

tigu

e =

subj

ecti

ve

fati

gue

subs

cale

of

the

CIS,

con

cent

r =

conc

entr

atio

n su

bsca

le o

f th

e CI

S, m

otiv

atio

n =

mot

ivat

ion

subs

cale

of

the

CIS,

act

ivit

y =

phys

ical

act

ivit

y su

bsca

le

of t

he C

IS,

beha

vior

al E

F =

beha

vior

al e

xecu

tive

fun

ctio

ning

BRI

EF,

beha

vior

al =

beh

avio

ral r

egul

atio

n in

dex

of t

he B

RIEF

, m

etac

og =

met

acog

niti

on in

dex

of t

he B

RIEF

. (C

olor

fig

ure

onlin

e)

Figu

re 2

. Pr

ofile

of p

sych

osoc

ial f

uncti

onin

g in

ped

iatr

ic b

rain

tum

or s

urvi

vors

in s

tand

ardi

zed

effec

t siz

es a

s co

mpa

red

to th

e m

ean

of th

e co

ntro

l gro

up (0

.00)

. *S

igni

fican

t di

ffere

nce

betw

een

PBTS

and

con

trol

s aft

er t

he B

onfe

rron

i cor

recti

on, e

ffect

siz

es p

rese

nted

in r

ed a

nd b

old.

Not

e. E

ffect

siz

es ‘d

’ wer

e ca

lcul

ated

usi

ng t

he p

oole

d st

anda

rd d

evia

tion.

Sco

res

have

bee

n ad

just

ed in

a w

ay t

hat

for

all d

omai

ns, l

ower

sco

res

refle

ct w

orse

psy

chos

ocia

l fu

nctio

ning

. For

self-

este

em, w

eigh

ted

aver

age

effec

t siz

es o

f the

SPPC

and

SPPA

are

dep

icte

d. Fo

r tea

cher

repo

rt o

f psy

chos

ocia

l adj

ustm

ent (

SDQ

), sc

ores

fo

r mal

es a

nd fe

mal

es a

re re

port

ed se

para

tely

due

to m

ore

fem

ales

in o

ur sa

mpl

e as

com

pare

d to

the

cont

rol g

roup

. HRQ

OL

= he

alth

-rel

ated

qua

lity

of

life,

phy

sica

l = p

hysi

cal w

ell-b

eing

sub

scal

e of

the

Kids

cree

n-27

, psy

ch =

psy

chol

ogic

al w

ell-b

eing

sub

scal

e of

the

Kids

cree

n-27

, aut

onom

y =

auto

nom

y an

d pa

rent

s su

bsca

le o

f th

e Ki

dscr

een-

27, p

eers

= p

eers

and

soc

ial s

uppo

rt s

ubsc

ale

of t

he K

idsc

reen

-27,

sch

ool =

sch

ool e

nviro

nmen

t su

bsca

le o

f th

e Ki

dscr

een-

27, g

ener

ic =

gen

eric

hea

lth-r

elat

ed q

ualit

y of

life

sub

scal

e of

the

Kid

scre

en-1

0, s

chol

astic

= s

chol

astic

com

pete

nce

subs

cale

of

the

SPPC

/SPP

A, s

ocia

l = s

ocia

l acc

epta

nce

subs

cale

of t

he S

PPC/

SPPA

, ath

letic

= a

thle

tic c

ompe

tenc

e su

bsca

le o

f the

SPP

C/SP

PA, a

ppea

ranc

e =

phys

ical

ap

pear

ance

sub

scal

e of

the

SPPC

/SPP

A, c

ondu

ct =

beh

avio

ral c

ondu

ct s

ubsc

ale

of th

e SP

PC/S

PPA

, sel

f-w

orth

= g

loba

l sel

f-w

orth

sub

scal

e of

the

SPPC

/SP

PA, f

rien

dshi

p =

clos

e fr

iend

ship

sub

scal

e of

the

SPP

A, p

sy a

dj =

psy

chos

ocia

l adj

ustm

ent,

tot

al s

core

SD

Q, f

atigu

e =

subj

ectiv

e fa

tigue

sub

scal

e of

the

CIS

, con

cent

r =

conc

entr

ation

sub

scal

e of

the

CIS

, moti

vatio

n =

moti

vatio

n su

bsca

le o

f the

CIS

, acti

vity

= p

hysi

cal a

ctivi

ty s

ubsc

ale

of t

he C

IS,

beha

vior

al E

F =

beha

vior

al e

xecu

tive

func

tioni

ng B

RIEF

, beh

avio

ral =

beh

avio

ral r

egul

ation

inde

x of

the

BRI

EF, m

etac

og =

met

acog

nitio

n in

dex

of t

he

BRIE

F. (C

olor

figu

re o

nlin

e)

Page 101: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

99

5

Self-reportHRQOL - PBTS showed significantly worse HRQOL than the normative sample (p < 0.008) on 2 subscales of the Kidscreen-27: physical well-being and psychological well-being, and on the generic scale (medium-to-large effect sizes). A tendency toward lower HRQOL in PBTS than the norm was found for peers and social support (p < 0.05; medium effect size).

Self-esteem - PBTS aged 8–11 years obtained significantly higher behavioral conduct scores compared to the normative population (p < 0.008, large effect size), indicating higher self-esteem regarding their behavior; PBTS aged 12–18 tended toward higher self-esteem on this scale (p < 0.05, medium effect size). A trend toward lower self-esteem in PBTS aged 8–11 was found for scholastic competence and for athletic competence, and also for athletic competence of PBTS aged 12–18 (p < 0.05, medium effect size). No differences between the normative population and the PBTS were observed on the other scales.

Psychosocial adjustment - PBTS between 11 and 16 years of age did not experience more psychosocial adjustment problems than the normative population as shown by their total problem score of the SDQ.

Fatigue - PBTS reported more concentration problems than the sibling control group (p < 0.01, medium effect size). A trend toward decreased physical activity in PBTS compared to the sibling control group was found as well as a trend toward a higher total scale compared to the siblings (p < 0.05, medium effect sizes), indicating more fatigue-related problems. The PBTS did not differ from the siblings on subjective fatigue and motivation problems.

Parent reportPsychosocial adjustment - The parent-reported total problem score of psychosocial adjustment (SDQ) was higher in the PBTS than in the norm (p < 0.001, large effect size), indicating more problems in psychosocial adjustment.

Executive functioning - Parents of PBTS considered their children’s behavioral expressions of executive functioning to be significantly worse than parents in the normative population. More specifically, PBTS had lower scores regarding the two indices and the total score (p < 0.016; medium effect sizes). Subsequent analyses showed worse functioning on the scales’ emotional control, and on initiate and working memory (p < 0.006; ds 0.47, 0.59, and 0.71, respectively). No significant differences were found on the other subscales.

Page 102: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

100

Teacher reportPsychosocial adjustment - For the female PBTS aged 8–11 years (n = 9), teachers reported significantly higher total problem scores (SDQ) than the norm (p < 0.01, large effect size). No difference was found between the male PBTS (n = 21) and the males in the normative population.

Executive functioning - According to the teacher report, no differences were found between the PBTS and the normative population on the indices and the total score of the BRIEF.

DISCUSSION

This study provides the first multidimensional (self-, parent- and teacher report) view of psychosocial functioning of PBTS with parent-reported neurocognitive complaints. The multidimensional approach is an advantage of the study because of the symptom burden of patients and complexity of their social situation. PBTS showed decreased psychosocial functioning on a number of the tested domains: self-reported HRQOL and fatigue, parent-reported psychosocial adjustment and executive functioning, and teacher-reported psychosocial adjustment for females only. These results are in line with a study by Meeske et al.,20 who reported PBTS to exhibit problems in physical, social, psychosocial, school, cognitive domains, and fatigue. The decreased HRQOL scores of PBTS on psychological well-being may be caused by the neurocognitive complaints from which they suffer. This is supported by the trend we found toward lower self-esteem regarding scholastic competence the PBTS show and by the literature.13 This should be further studied in future studies. However, despite the neurocognitive complaints, PBTS functioned within normal ranges in several psychosocial domains or showed only trends toward worse functioning: self-reported self-esteem and psychosocial adjustment, and teacher-reported executive functioning and psychosocial adjustment for males.Physical functioning was specifically compromised in PBTS. Besides worse physical HRQOL, a tendency toward decreased self-reported athletic competence (domain of self-esteem) and decreased physical activity (domain of the fatigue questionnaire) was observed. It is known that PBTS are at increased risk of functional impairments, which is related to physical self-esteem.44 It is important that professionals working with PBTS are aware of these possible late effects and monitor physical well-being in relation to self-esteem and HRQOL.Regarding self-esteem, no problems other than the trends toward physical-related and scholastic-related problems were seen. PBTS behavioral conduct scores were even better than the norm. This positive finding has been observed previously, e.g., after a trauma,45 but has recently also received attention in pediatric oncology literature.46 It has been attributed, among other factors, to the resilience of the PBTS and posttraumatic growth.

Page 103: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

101

5

The PBTS in our sample did not report more psychosocial adjustment problems as assessed with the SDQ than their peers, in contrast with parents and teachers who did report psychosocial adjustment problems in PBTS. This finding is not surprising, as in the literature it has been found that both healthy children and childhood cancer survivors typically report different levels of psychosocial problems than their parents and/or teachers.47 Some studies found child-reported levels of problems to be higher than parent- and/or teacher-reported levels,48 while other studies found the opposite.49 Sato et al.50 concluded that parent and child ratings are influenced by different factors. Among others, parents’ perception was influenced by their level of distress, whereas the child’s perception tended to be dependent on trait anxiety. Others found that childhood cancer survivors may report less psychosocial problems influenced by social desirability, stress-related growth, or a positive coping strategy.51,52 In this study, the diagnosis and treatment resulting neurocognitive consequences might have led to increased parental distress, causing parents to report more problems than their children. Another possible reason for the discrepancy between the observed scores of the different informants in our study is the age difference in self-reports versus parent and teacher reports concerning psychosocial adjustment: Due to age-restricted normative data, the results on the self-reported SDQ were based on PBTS aged 11–16 (n = 48), whereas parent- and teacher-reported results were based on PBTS aged 8–16 (n = 67) and 8–12 (n = 30), respectively.Teachers reported no executive problems in PBTS, whereas parents did, especially regarding emotional control, initiation, and working memory. This discrepancy could be the result of the ‘observation environment.’ Teachers observed the PBTS in a school environment, which is more structured than the home situation. Possibly, the problems parents saw at home did not exist in the same way in structured settings like school. Turner and colleagues describe problems of PBTS to increase as they leave the structured school environment.53 This implies that PBTS may benefit from a structured environment. Another reason for the difference between the parent and teacher perspective could be that they have a different reference background. Parents know the child’s premorbid functioning, whereas teachers have the behavior of classmates as a reference. A large proportion of the children in our sample were in special education (24 %), where many classmates suffered from chronic conditions, which could also affect psychosocial functioning.54

This study has some limitations to take into account. The results are not generalizable to the PBTS population as a whole, since PBTS in this study were selected on the basis of parent-reported neurocognitive problems and the willingness to participate in a study of a treatment aimed to improve neurocognitive functioning. This may have led to an overestimation of the psychosocial problems. It is easy to consider this a non-representative sample, but we have to take into account that many children with a brain tumor suffer from neurocognitive problems (40–100 %). Therefore, this study sheds light on a vulnerable group of PBTS.

Page 104: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

102

Awareness for their psychosocial functioning from a complementary perspective is of utmost importance. Another limitation of the study is that normative data were not available for all questionnaires within all age groups. So for some outcomes, especially the SDQ, comparison with the normative population was possible for only small subgroups of PBTS. This limits the reliability and generalizability of the results. For this reason, we would like to urge future studies to aim at collecting norm data for broader age ranges. Nevertheless, this study adds to the existing knowledge as it provides a broad, multidimensional profile of functioning of PBTS with neurocognitive complaints, based on multi-informants.Better insight into psychosocial functioning in the growing group of PBTS with neurocognitive complaints will help professionals to identify those patients susceptible to developing psychosocial problems. Timely identification is important to prevent problems from escalating. Screening for possible psychosocial late effects should be done in a systematic way, preferably by using the perspective of the patient, parent, and teacher. In daily clinical practice, patient- and/or parent-reported outcomes (PROs) are recommended, because this will increase awareness of and attention for psychosocial functioning during routine checkups. Increased awareness can improve provision of aftercare.55 Furthermore, providing tailored support to this group of vulnerable children is necessary. Interventions for PBTS with (imminent) psychosocial problems should be aimed at improving HRQOL, coping with fatigue, and providing structure in daily life.

Page 105: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

103

5

REFERENCES

1. Howlader N, Noone A, Krapcho M, et al. SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975-2011, National Cancer Institute. 2014. Available at: http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2011/.

2. Tonning Olsson I, Perrin S, Lundgren J, Hjorth L, Johanson A. Long-Term Cognitive Sequelae After Pediatric Brain Tumor Related to Medical Risk Factors, Age, and Sex. Pediatr Neurol. 2014;51(4):515–521. doi:10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2014.06.011.

3. Glauser TA, Packer RJ. Cognitive deficits in long-term survivors of childhood brain tumors. Child’s Nerv Syst. 1991;7:7–12.

4. Stargatt R, Rosenfeld J V, Anderson V, Hassall T, Maixner W, Ashley D. Intelligence and adaptive function in children diagnosed with brain tumour during infancy. J Neurooncol. 2006;80(3):295–303.

5. Saury JMG, Emanuelson I. Cognitive Consequences of the Treatment of Medulloblastoma Among Children. Pediatr Neurol. 2011;44(1):21–30.

6. de Ruiter MA, van Mourik R, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Grootenhuis MA, Oosterlaan J. Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatment: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(5):408–417. doi:10.1111/dmcn.12020.

7. Ellenberg L, Liu Q, Gioia GA, et al. Neurocognitive status in long-term survivors of childhood CNS malignancies: a report from the Childhood Cancer Survivor Study. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(6):705–717.

8. Wakefield CE, McLoone J, Goodenough B, Lenthen K, Cairns DR, Cohn RJ. The psychosocial impact of completing childhood cancer treatment: a systematic review of the literature. J Pediatr Psychol. 2010;35(3):262–74. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsp056.

9. Patenaude AF, Kupst MJ. Psychosocial functioning in pediatric cancer. J Pediatr Psychol. 2005;30(1):9–27.

10. www.kidscreen.org. 2015. Available at: http://www.kidscreen.org/english/measurement-model/. Accessed April 14, 2015.

11. Gudrunardottir T, Lannering B, Remke M, et al. Treatment developments and the unfolding of the quality of life discussion in childhood medulloblastoma: a review. Child’s Nerv Syst. 2014;30(6):979–990. doi:10.1007/s00381-014-2388-5.

12. Zebrack BJ, Gurney JG, Oeffinger K, et al. Psychological outcomes in long-term survivors of childhood brain cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22(6):999–1006.

13. Aukema EJ, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Last BF, Maurice-Stam H, Grootenhuis MA. Childhood brain tumor survivors at risk for impaired health-related quality of life. J Pediatr Hematol Oncol. 2013;35(8):603–609. doi:10.1097/MPH.0b013e31829b7ec6.

14. Penn A, Shortman RI, Lowis SP, et al. Child-Related Determinants of Health-Related Quality of Life in Children With Brain Tumours 1 Year After Diagnosis. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2010;55:1377–1385.

15. Harter S. Self-perception profile for children. Denver, CO: University of Denver; 1985.16. Schulte F, Barrera M. Social competence in childhood brain tumor survivors: a comprehensive

review. Support care cancer. 2010;18(12):1499–1513.17. Hardy KK, Willard VW, Watral MA, Bonner MJ. Perceived social competency in children with brain

tumors: comparison between children on and off therapy. J Pediatr Oncol Nurs. 2010;27(3):156–163. doi:10.1177/1043454209357918.

18. Chou L-N, Hunter A. Factors affecting quality of life in Taiwanese survivors of childhood cancer. J Adv Nurs. 2009;65(10):2131–2141. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2648.2009.05078.x.

Page 106: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 5

104

19. Campos M, Hassan B, Riechelmann R, del Giglio A. Cancer-related fatigue: a review. Rev Assoc Med Bras. 2011;57(2):206–214.

20. Meeske K, Katz ER, Palmer SN, Burwinkle T, Varni JW. Parent proxy-reported health-related quality of life and fatigue in pediatric patients diagnosed with brain tumors and acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Cancer. 2004;101(9):2116–25. doi:10.1002/cncr.20609.

21. Verberne LM, Maurice-Stam H, Grootenhuis M a, Van Santen HM, Schouten-Van Meeteren AYN. Sleep disorders in children after treatment for a CNS tumour. J Sleep Res. 2012;21(4):461–469. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2869.2011.00971.x.

22. Meeske KA, Patel SK, Palmer SN, Nelson MB, Parow AM. Factors Associated With Health-Related Quality of Life in Pediatric Cancer Survivors. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2007;49:298–305. doi:10.1002/pbc.

23. Daniel LC, Brumley LD, Schwartz LA. Fatigue in Adolescents With Cancer Compared to Healthy Adolescents. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2013;60:1902–1907. doi:10.1002/pbc.

24. Beauchamp MH, Anderson V. SOCIAL: an integrative framework for the development of social skills. Psychol Bull. 2010;136(1):39–64. doi:10.1037/a0017768.

25. Aarsen FK, Paquier PF, Arts W-FM, et al. Cognitive deficits and predictors 3 years after diagnosis of a pilocytic astrocytoma in childhood. J Clin Oncol. 2009;27(21):3526–3532.

26. Maddrey AM, Bergeron JA, Lombardo ER, et al. Neuropsychological performance and quality of life of 10 year survivors of childhood medulloblastoma. J Neurooncol. 2005;72(3):245–53. doi:10.1007/s11060-004-3009-z.

27. Wolfe KR, Madan-Swain A, Kana RK. Executive dysfunction in pediatric posterior fossa tumor survivors: a systematic literature review of neurocognitive deficits and interventions. Dev Neuropsychol. 2012;37(2):153–75. doi:10.1080/87565641.2011.632462.

28. Bhat SR, Goodwin TL, Burwinkle TM, et al. Profile of daily life in children with brain tumors: an assessment of health-related quality of life. J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(24):5493–5500.

29. de Ruiter MA, Schouten-Van Meeteren AYN, Van Mourik R, et al. Neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive functioning of children treated for a brain tumor: design of a randomized controlled double-blind trial. BMC Cancer. 2012;12:581.

30. Houtzager BA, Grootenhuis MA, Caron HN, Last BF. Quality of life and psychological adaptation in siblings of paediatric cancer patients, 2 years after diagnosis. Psychooncology. 2004;13(8):499–511. doi:10.1002/pon.759.

31. Alderfer M a, Long K a, Lown EA, et al. Psychosocial adjustment of siblings of children with cancer: a systematic review. Psychooncology. 2010;19(8):789–805. doi:10.1002/pon.1638.

32. Eiser C, Varni JW. Health-related quality of life and symptom reporting: Similarities and differences between children and their parents. Eur J Pediatr. 2013;172(10):1299–1304. doi:10.1007/s00431-013-2049-9.

33. Ravens-Sieberer U, Auquier P, Erhart M, et al. The KIDSCREEN-27 quality of life measure for children and adolescents: psychometric results from a cross-cultural survey in 13 European countries. Qual life Res. 2007;16(8):1347–56. doi:10.1007/s11136-007-9240-2.

34. Ravens-Sieberer U, Erhart M, Rajmil L, et al. Reliability, construct and criterion validity of the KIDSCREEN-10 score: a short measure for children and adolescents’ well-being and health-related quality of life. Qual Life Res. 2010;19(10):1487–500. doi:10.1007/s11136-010-9706-5.

35. Harter S. Social-Perception Profile for Children. Denver, CO: University of Denver; 1985.36. Veerman JW, Straathof MAE, Treffers PDA, van den Bergh BRH ten BL. Handleiding bij de

Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Kinderen, CBSK (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Children – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997.

37. Treffers P, Goedhart A, Veerman J, van den Bergh B, Ackaert L, de Rycke L. Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Adolescenten, CBSA (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Adolescents – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002.

Page 107: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Psychosocial effects of pediatric brain tumors

105

5

38. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–6.

39. van Widenfelt BM, Goedhart AW, Treffers PD a, Goodman R. Dutch version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;12(6):281–9. doi:10.1007/s00787-003-0341-3.

40. Stulemeijer M, de Jong LWAM, Fiselier TJW, Hoogveld SWB, Bleijenberg G. Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2005;330(7481):14. doi:10.1136/bmj.38301.587106.63.

41. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. TEST REVIEW Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6(3):235–238.

42. Smidts DP, Huizinga M. BRIEF executieve functies gedragsvragenlijst: Handleiding. Amsterdam: Hogrefe Uitgevers; 2009.

43. Cohen J. The statistical power of abnormal-social psychological research: a review. J Abnorm Soc Psychol. 1962;65:145–153.

44. Boman KK, Hörnquist L, De Graaff L, Rickardsson J, Lannering B, Gustafsson G. Disability, body image and sports/physical activity in adult survivors of childhood CNS tumors: population-based outcomes from a cohort study. J Neurooncol. 2013;112(1):99–106. doi:10.1007/s11060-012-1039-5.

45. Blix I, Birkeland MS. Posttraumatic growth and centrality of event: A longitudinal study in the aftermath of the 2011 Oslo bombing. - PubMed - NCBI. 2015;7(1):18–23. Available at: https://c44wl11i7e8zqde2rbj4elx.sec.amc.nl/pubmed/25793589. Accessed April 14, 2015.

46. Zebrack BJ, Stuber ML, Meeske K a, et al. Perceived positive impact of cancer among long-term survivors of childhood cancer: a report from the childhood cancer survivor study. Psychooncology. 2012;21(6):630–9. doi:10.1002/pon.1959.

47. Sattoe JNT, van Staa A, Moll HA. The proxy problem anatomized: child-parent disagreement in health related quality of life reports of chronically ill adolescents. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2012;10(1):10. doi:10.1186/1477-7525-10-10.

48. Theunissen NC, Vogels TG, Koopman HM, et al. The proxy problem: child report versus parent report in health-related quality of life research. Qual Life Res. 1998;7(5):387–97.

49. Chang P-C, Yeh C-H. Agreement between child self-report and parent proxy-report to evaluate quality of life in children with cancer. Psychooncology. 2005;14(2):125–34. doi:10.1002/pon.828.

50. Sato I, Higuchi A, Yanagisawa T, et al. Factors influencing self- and parent-reporting health-related quality of life in children with brain tumors. Qual life Res. 2013;22(1):185–201. doi:10.1007/s11136-012-0137-3.

51. O’Leary TE, Diller L, Recklitis CJ. The effects of response bias on self-reported quality of life among childhood cancer survivors. Qual life Res. 2007;16(7):1211–1220.

52. Liptak C, Manley P, Recklitis CJ. The feasibility of psychosocial screening for adolescent and young adult brain tumor survivors: the value of self-report. J cancer Surviv. 2012;6(4):379–87. doi:10.1007/s11764-012-0237-7.

53. Turner CD, Rey-Casserly C, Liptak CC, Chordas C. Late effects of therapy for pediatric brain tumor survivors. J Clin Oncol. 2009;24(11):1455–1463.

54. Van Cleave J, Davis MM. Bullying and peer victimization among children with special health care needs. Pediatrics. 2006;118(4):e1212–9. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-3034.

55. Engelen V, Detmar S, Koopman H, et al. Reporting Health-Related Quality of Life Scores to Physicians During Routine Follow-Up Visits of Pediatric Oncology Patients: Is It Effective? Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2012;58:766–774. doi:10.1002/pbc.

Page 108: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

Page 109: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

6

The association between the behavior rating inventory

of executive functioning and neurocognitive testing in

children diagnosed with a brain tumor

Marieke de VriesMarieke A. de Ruiter

Kim J. OostromAntoinette Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren

Heleen Maurice-StamJaap Oosterlaan

Martha A. Grootenhuis

Submitted for publication

Page 110: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

108

ABSTRACT

Aim: Pediatric Brain Tumor Survivors (PBTS) suffer from cognitive late effects, such as worse Executive Functioning (EF), which might worsen over time and influence daily life. It is important to screen for these late effects. While EF-tasks measure cognitive (dys)function, questionnaires (e.g., the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function; BRIEF) assess daily life consequences of cognitive (dys)function. We explored the suitability of the BRIEF as screener for EF-problems in PBTS by assessing the relationship between the BRIEF and EF-tasks. We also investigated the association between the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher, and explored the clinical utility.

Methods: Eighty-two PBTS (8-18 years) were assessed with EF-tasks measuring attention (Attention Network Test; ANT), cognitive flexibility (ANT), inhibition (Stop Signal Task; SST), visual memory (Visual Sequencing Task; VST), and working memory (WM; Digit Span). The BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher measured daily EF-problems. We studied the Pearson’s correlations between the BRIEF and EF-tasks, and between BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher.

Results: The BRIEF-Parent related poorly to EF-tasks (rs<.26, ps>.01), but several subscales of the BRIEF-Teacher related significantly to some EF-tasks (WM-scale, Monitor-scale, Behavioral Regulation Index, Total score, and Meta-cognition Index (rs>.31, ps<.01)). Only the Inhibit-scales of the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher correlated significantly (r=.33, p<.01). Children with clinically elevated scores on BRIEF-scales that correlated with EF-tasks, performed worse on all EF-tasks than children without clinically elevated BRIEF-scores (ds 0.56–1.23, ps<.05).

Conclusions: Although subscale-scores might capture separate functions insufficiently, Total and Index-scores may capture general EF. The BRIEF-Teacher might better screen EF in PBTS than the BRIEF-Parent.

Page 111: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

BRIEF-EF task association in PBTS

109

6

INTRODUCTION

Survival rates of children who have been treated for a brain tumor have increased in the last decades due to improved diagnostic and neurosurgical techniques, radiation therapy and chemotherapy. The 5-year survival rate has risen from 57 % (1975-1977) up to 74 % (2004-2010).1 This increasing survival rate comes with a cost; childhood brain tumors and their treatment have long lasting developmental consequences, particularly in the cognitive domain.2 The brain tumor and its treatment may provoke a cascade of cognitive consequences, with immediate consequences followed by impaired cognitive skills that have yet to emerge, leading to the children to ‘grow into deficit’.3

To screen for changes in cognitive functioning over time, it is important that PBTS undergo cognitive assessment systematically to obtain information on the nature, severity, and course of cognitive consequences.4 ‘Screening’ protocols based on international requirements as formulated in Société Internationale d’Oncologie Pédiatrique (SIOP) and the Children’s Oncology Group (COG) include comprehensive cognitive assessment. However, as this is time and resource intensive, this is not always feasible. It might be preferable to conduct brief, repeated assessments,4 or screen all PBTS regularly with a questionnaire, and extensively examine only those children who report cognitive problems. Tasks and questionnaires that measure cognitive functioning cannot simply replace each other. EF tasks are designed to measure cognitive (dys)function, whereas questionnaires assess the consequences of the (dys)function in daily life. However, to reliably screen for cognitive (dys)function with a questionnaire, the outcomes of questionnaire should correlate to cognitive tasks for which psychometric properties have been determined.PBTS are known to show deficits in executive functioning (EF).5 Executive functions are those cognitive functions that are needed to adapt behavior to varying demands of the environment, and are comprised of a multitude of cognitive functions, including working memory (WM), inhibition, and cognitive flexibility. The Dutch Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF)6,7 is a standardized questionnaire to measure daily life EF. Findings on the correlation between the BRIEF and EF tasks vary widely (0 – 50% significant correlations, r = 0 – .48) in both clinical (neurological or psychiatric) and healthy control groups.8 However, the BRIEF-WM parent-report scale correlates significantly to WM tasks in PBTS.9 Given that EF demands differ per context, e.g. home versus school, it is particularly interesting to study the relation between the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher. Teachers might be better able to evaluate EF than parents, as EF correlates to academic skills,10 and teachers have a professional background to evaluate academic skills. Indeed, BRIEF-Teacher scores appear strongly correlated to school functioning, while small to medium-sized associations have been reported between BRIEF-Parent scores and school functioning.7

Page 112: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

110

We aimed to study whether the BRIEF-Parent, and/or BRIEF-Teacher are adequate screening instruments for EF problems in PBTS. To do so, we first studied the correlations between the BRIEF (Parent and Teacher) and a battery of EF tasks measuring attention, cognitive flexibility, inhibition, visual-, and verbal memory, on which PBTS show difficulties.11 Secondly, we examined the correlation and tested group differences between the scores on the BRIEF-Parent and the BRIEF-Teacher. Thirdly we explored the clinical utility of the findings by testing whether children who obtained clinically elevated scores (T score > 65) on either the BRIEF-Parent or BRIEF-Teacher, also scored worse on the EF tasks.

METHODS

ParticipantsEighty-two children (8-18 years) diagnosed with a brain tumor participated in the cohort of the PRISMA study. Inclusion criteria for the PRISMA study consisted of having finished treatment at least two years prior, being able (mentally and physically) to undergo cognitive assessment, and parent-reported cognitive problems (a score > 14 on the attention scale of the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale [DBDRS],12 or if parents indicated at least two problems with respect to attention, memory, speed, and information processing). Exclusion criteria were a premorbid attention disorder diagnosis or being non-fluent in Dutch language. See Table 1 for demographic and medical details.

ProcedureThe data collection of the current cross-sectional study was part of the baseline assessment of a randomized controlled trial on neurofeedback for PBTS.13 Data collection took place between 2010 and 2012. The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Committee of the Academic Medical Center (METC 09/137). Children were recruited through 5 Dutch academic hospitals (Emma Children’s Hospital/Academic Medical Center Amsterdam, the Vrije Universiteit Medical Center Amsterdam, the University Medical Center Utrecht, the Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, and the Maastricht University Medical Center). After receiving an information letter, parents were asked to fill out online screening questionnaires (see Participants) concerning inclusion and exclusion criteria. Informed consent was signed by the parents and children. Next, parents filled out the BRIEF online and indicated which teacher was most suitable to evaluate the child’s functioning; usually a mentor or otherwise the teacher spending most time with the children. Teachers were contacted by email, and filled out the BRIEF online. Children’s EFs were tested in a quiet room by trained examiners, and tests were administered in a fixed order. The total testing time was approximately 150 minutes, and regular breaks were taken to avoid fatigue.

Page 113: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

BRIEF-EF task association in PBTS

111

6

Table 1. Participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics

Characteristics N PercentageGender male/female 40/42 49/51Medical information High grade/low grade tumor 34/48 41.5/58.5 Hydrocephalus 39 48Location Infratentorial 36 44 Supratentorial 46 56Tumor type and gradeHigh grade 34 42 Medulloblastoma 12 15 Supratentorial PNET 8 10 Ependymoma 5 6 Astrocytoma gr III 5 6 Germ cell tumour 4 5Low grade 48 59 Low Grade Glioma 35 43 Craniopharyngioma 7 9 Plexus Papilloma 6 7Treatment Radiotherapy 35 43 Chemotherapy 35 43 Surgery 72 88 Biopsy 19 23 Surgery/biopsy/pressure relief only 39 48

Mean Standard Deviation

Range

Age at diagnosis 6.9 3.8 0.4 - 14.2Age at assessment 13.8 3.2 8.0 - 18.8Time since diagnosis 7.0 3.6 2.1 - 17.0Verbal Intelligence Quotient 91.0 16.1 50.7 - 117.4Performance Intelligence Quotient 91.0 18.9 46.0 - 133.0Total Intelligence Quotient 89.7 18.7 40.5 - 125.5

Page 114: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

112

MEASURES

EF questionnairesTo measure EF problems in daily life the Dutch parent and teacher version of the BRIEF6 were administered. The BRIEF (75 items, 3-point Likert scale) includes a Total score and two indices, subdivided into eight subscales. The Behavior Regulation index consists of the subscales Inhibit, Shift, and Emotional control. The Meta-Cognition Index consists of Initiate, WM, Plan/Organize, Organization of Materials, and Monitor. The BRIEF parent and teacher version has good test-retest stability and high internal consistency in the general population (Cronbach’s αs BRIEF-Parent .78-.96; BRIEF-Teacher .88—.98),7 as well as in PBTS (Cronbach’s αs BRIEF-Parent .66-.94; BRIEF-Teacher .82-.97).13,14 The BRIEF is validated with the Child Behavior Checklist,15 the Disruptive Behavior Disorder rating scale,12 and the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children.16,17 Moreover, both convergent and divergent validity were adequate and in the expected direction (medium to large correlations with the Child Behavior Checklist15 and the Disruptive Behavior Disorders Rating Scale).18 The age-, and gender standardized T-scores on the subscales, Index scores, and Total score were used as outcome measures. Higher scores indicate more problems. A T-score > 65 is considered a clinically elevated score.7

EF tasksAttention and cognitive flexibilityTo measure several components of attention we administered an adapted version of the Attention Network Test ANT.19 The ANT measures three subdomains of attention: Alerting (maintaining an alert state), Orienting (selecting information from sensory input), and Executive control (resolving conflict among responses).19 In the current version,20 on each trial a picture of a ship appears on the right or the left side of the computer screen and the child has to respond by pressing the corresponding key. In the different trials, the ship is preceded by different cues. The different trial types measure the before mentioned subdomains of attention; 1) A palm tree appears before the ship appears (neutral cue); 2) No cue, only the ship appears (alerting; the participant has to stay alert without any triggers); 3) A parrot pointing in the direction where the ship will appear precedes the appearance of the ship (orienting cue) and 4) A parrot pointing in the opposite direction of where the ship will appear precedes the appearance of the ship (executive cue). The difference in reaction times between the neutral cue (1) and other cues is used as a measure of the subdomains of attention. The ANT consists of 312 trials. The standard deviation (SD) divided by mean reaction time of the alerting trials (2) is used as a measure of inattention (the intra individual coefficient of variation [IICV]), and the reaction time on the executive trials is used as a measure of cognitive flexibility. The ANT can be considered a valid measure of

Page 115: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

BRIEF-EF task association in PBTS

113

6

the three different subdomains of attention, as the three corresponding distinct subcortical anatomical networks are activated during task performance.21 Higher scores indicate worse functioning.

InhibitionTo measure inhibition an adaptation of the Stop Signal Task22 was administered. The Stop Signal Task is a cognitive inhibition task (as compared to a motivational inhibition task)23 with a medium to high temporal stability, and discriminates well between typically developing children and children with an impulse control disorder. A picture of an airplane appears on the computer screen, facing left or right. The child has to press a corresponding key. In 25 % of the trials a cross on top of the airplane indicates that the response has to be inhibited.24 The time between stimulus and stop signal (stop signal delay) is adapted to the child’s performance throughout the task, to accomplish about 50% correct stop trials. The outcome measure is the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT); the time the child needs to inhibit his/her response. De Zeeuw (2008) explains: ‘SSRT was estimated using the race model in which the go process and the inhibitory process are conceived of as competing processes (see De Zeeuw, 200825 for more details). Whether a response will be executed or inhibited in a stop trial depends on which of these processes “wins” the race. In the case in which 50% of stop trials result in successful stopping, the mean stop signal delay is where both the stop and the go processes have equal probability of “winning” the race (i.e., the mean go process duration [mean reaction time] and the sum of the mean stop signal delay plus the duration of the stop process [SSRT] are approximately equal). It follows that SSRT can be calculated using the equation SSRT = mean reaction time - mean stop signal delay.’24 A higher score indicates worse inhibition.

Visual short-term memoryTo measure visual short-term memory the valid and reliable Visual Sequencing Task26 was administered to the children. In a four by four grid a sequence of dots lights up and has to be repeated on a touch screen. The length of the sequence ranges from two to nine, with four trials per sequence, divided into two difficulty levels; a level of two relatively simple trials and a level of two relatively difficult trials. After an error on both trials of the same difficulty level, the task is terminated. The total number of correctly repeated sequences was the outcome measure, hence, a higher score indicates a better visual memory.

Verbal (working) memory Digit span from the WISC-III or WAIS-III27–29 was administered to measure verbal WM. Digit span correlates significantly with other verbal WM tasks 30. The participant is asked to verbally repeat sequences of digits (2–9 digits, increasing in length), first in the same order,

Page 116: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

114

and next in the reverse order. The task is terminated after two errors on a difficulty level. The total number of correctly repeated sequences (raw score) was used as the outcome measure. Higher scores indicate a better WM.

Statistical analysesTo control for possible non-response bias, we compared BRIEF-Parent, BRIEF-Teacher, and EF task scores of children whose data was complete with children whose data was incomplete using T-tests. For the main analyses we firstly tested whether the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher correlated with the ANT-IICV (attention), Stop Signal Task SSRT (inhibition), Visual Sequencing (visual short-term memory), Digit Span (WM), and ANT-Executive (cognitive flexibility). We were particularly interested in the Pearson’s correlations between BRIEF-scales and EF tasks that aim to measure the same construct; 1) the Inhibit scale, and the Stop Signal Task, 2) the Shift scale, and the ANT-Executive, 3) the WM scale, and Visual Sequencing and Digit Span, and 4) the Monitor scale, and the ANT-IICV. Moreover, we were interested in the Pearson’s correlations of the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher Index and Total scores with the EF tasks. Secondly, we compared the BRIEF-Parent subscale-, Index-, and Total scores with the BRIEF-Teacher subscale-, Index-, and Total scores using Pearson’s correlations. In addition, a repeated measures ANOVA was performed to study the difference between parent and teacher reports while correcting the analyses for dependency of the parent and teacher reports. Considering the large number of comparisons, we set the alpha at p ≤ .01 to control for Type I errors. Finally, we explored the clinical utility of the findings with independent sample t-tests. We tested whether children who showed any clinically elevated score (T score > 65) on the Index or Total scales of either the BRIEF-Parent or the BRIEF-Teacher, also scored higher on the EF tasks than children without any clinically elevated score. Moreover, we studied EF task performance of children with versus children without T scores > 65 on those BRIEF scales that correlated significantly with the EF tasks. For these explorative analyses we did not correct for multiple testing.

RESULTS

Response BRIEF teacher ratings were missing for nine children, and five children did not complete the Stop Signal Task.11 Children with missing teacher data for the BRIEF scored significantly worse on the parent version of the BRIEF-Parent Monitor scale (T = 2.8, p = .01), and children

Page 117: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

BRIEF-EF task association in PBTS

115

6

who did not complete the Stop Signal Task scored worse on the ANT-IICV (T = 2.5, p = .02), and Digit span (T = -2.3, p = .03).

Correlation of BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher with EF tasks The BRIEF-Parent Total score, Index scores, nor subscales were significantly related to any of the EF tasks (rs -.24–.26, ps >.01).With respect to expected relations between BRIEF-scales and EF tasks that aim to measure the same construct, the BRIEF-Teacher Inhibit scale was not significantly related to the Stop Signal Task and the Shift scale was not significantly related to the ANT-Executive (ps > .01), but the WM scale was significantly related to the Visual Sequencing (r=-.31) and Digit span (r=-.36), and the Monitor scale was significantly related to the ANT-IICV (r=.35). The Behavioral Regulation and the Total scale were significantly related to the ANT-IICV and Digit span (rs > .31). The Meta-cognition Index was significantly related to the ANT-IICV (r=.41) and the Stop Signal Task (r=.31). Notably, the ANT-IICV was significantly related to all BRIEF-Teacher scales except Shift, Emotional control and Organization of materials; and the BRIEF–Teacher WM-scale was significantly related to all tasks except the ANT-Executive. See Table 2 for all correlations. (Note that all EF tasks were also significantly interrelated rs > .35, p < .01)

Page 118: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

116

Table 2. Correlation between questionnaires and tasks measuring neurocognitive functioning

TasksQuestionnaires Attention

ANT-IICV1,aInhibition Stop

Signal Task SSRT2,a

Visual memory Visual

Sequencing b

Working memory

Digit Span3,b

Cognitive flexibility ANT-

Executive4,a

BRIEF parent (N=82) r r (N = 77) r r rScalesInhibit -.01 -.05 .04 .02 -.14Shift .02 .03 -.01 -.02 .01Emotional control -.09 -.15 .00 .10 -.19Initiate -.07 -.11 -.05 -.02 .11Working memory .22 .07 -.12 -.24 .17Plan/organize -.08 -.10 .23 .12 -.06Organization of materials -.21 -.21 .26 .21 -.24Monitor -.11 .03 .19 .01 .03Index and Total scoresBehavioral Regulation Index -.04 -.08 .02 .04 -.14Meta-cognition Index -.04 -.08 .12 .01 .01Total score -.04 -.10 .09 .03 -.06BRIEF teacher (N=73) (N = 68)ScalesInhibit .31** .06 -.20 -.20 .07Shift .28 .12 -.27 -.27 .20Emotional control .21 -.04 -.20 -.30 .10Initiate .45** .31 -.31** -.27 .19Working memory .47** .38** -.31** -.36** .24Plan/organize .36** .20 -.19 -.18 .14Organization of materials .21 .04 -.06 -.13 .14Monitor .35** .275 -.25 -.25 .17Index and Total scoresBehavioral Regulation Index .32** .05 -.26 -.31** .17Meta-cognition Index .41** .31** -.27 -.29 .22Total score .41** .22 -.29 -.32** .22

Relation between scores on the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-TeacherOnly the Inhibition scales of the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher were significantly related (r = .33, p < .01). The BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher scores did not significantly differ on any of the scales, Indexes, nor on the Total scale. See Table 3 for all results.

Page 119: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

BRIEF-EF task association in PBTS

117

6

EF task performance of children with clinical versus non-clinical scores on the BRIEFWe compared children who had a clinically elevated score (T > 65) on one or more of the BRIEF-Parent or BRIEF-Teacher Index scores or Total score, with children who did not have a clinically elevated score on any of these scales. There were no significant differences in EF task performance between children with and without a clinical score (see Table 4). Based on the correlational findings (Table 3), we explored whether children who had clinically elevated scores (T > 65) on the BRIEF-Teacher WM scale, Index scores, or Total score (the scales that correlated best with the EF tasks) differed in EF task performance from children who did not have clinically elevated scores on these scales. Children with a clinically elevated score performed significantly worse on all EF tasks than children who did not have clinically elevated scores on these scales (ds 0.56 – 1.23, ps < .05; see Table 4).

Table 3. Comparison and correlation of BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher T scores

BRIEF-Parent BRIEF-Teacher(N=73)

M SD M SD F (1,72) ηp2 r

ScalesInhibit 51.73 11.03 48.29 9.58 6.00 .08 .33**

Shift 52.49 9.17 51.79 13.24 0.13 .00 -.06Emotional control 54.75 12.01 52.78 13.86 0.75 .01 -.12Initiate 55.85 8.69 52.10 15.78 3.36 .05 .07Working memory 56.75 9.39 53.58 15.49 2.79 .04 .22Plan/organize 51.71 8.21 50.53 13.00 0.41 .01 -.04Organization of materials 48.42 10.12 49.78 11.67 0.56 .01 -.01Monitor 50.97 8.84 50.82 12.12 0.01 .00 .09

Index and Total scoresBehavioral Regulation Index 53.70 11.32 50.89 12.41 1.87 .03 -.09Meta-cognition Index 53.51 7.99 51.89 13.90 0.75 .01 .02Total 53.77 8.38 51.21 12.74 1.92 .03 -.08

Note Higher scores indicate more executive functioning problems, ** p <.01, ηp2 partial eta squared

effect size: .01 = small, .06 = medium, .14 = large (Cohen, 1988), effect size of r: .1 = small, .3 = medium, .5 = large (Cohen, 1988)

Page 120: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

118

Tabl

e 4.

Com

pari

son

of E

F ta

sk p

erfo

rman

ce o

f chi

ldre

n w

ith a

nd w

ithou

t clin

ical

sco

res

on th

e BR

IEF-

Pare

nt o

r BR

IEF-

Teac

her

Tota

l or

Inde

x sc

ores

Inde

x sc

ores

or

Tota

l sco

re B

RIEF

-Par

ent/

BRI

EF-T

each

erW

orki

ng m

emor

y sc

ale,

Inde

x sc

ores

, or

Tota

l sco

re B

RIEF

-Tea

cher

Non

clin

ical

N =

45

Clin

ical

5 (T>6

5)N

=31

t (74

)6d

7N

on c

linic

alN

= 5

6Cl

inic

al8 (T

>65)

N =

17

t (71

)9d3

Task

sM

SDM

SDM

SDM

SD

Atten

tion

AN

T-IIC

V1 00.

220.

060.

240.

08-1

.27

0.28

0.21

0.06

0.29

0.07

-4.4

5***

1.23

Inhi

bitio

n St

op S

igna

l Tas

k SS

RT1 1

256.

011 2

68.5

525

3.40

1 352

.80

0.17

0.04

244.

241 4

53.0

629

4.36

1 584

.83

-2.7

6**

0.71

Visu

al m

emor

y Vi

sual

Seq

uenc

ing1 6

13.4

44.

0413

.58

4.68

-0.1

40.

0314

.14

4.32

10.9

43.

512.

79**

0.81

Wor

king

mem

ory

Dig

it Sp

an1 7

13.4

43.

4912

.48

4.29

1.07

0.25

13.9

13.

5010

.29

3.96

3.62

**0.

97

Cogn

itive

flex

ibili

ty A

NT-

Exec

utive

1 838

.11

40.6

540

.00

48.3

3-0

.18

0.04

31.9

539

.19

58.2

854

.01

-2.2

1*0.

56

Page 121: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

BRIEF-EF task association in PBTS

119

6

DISCUSSION

We studied the correlation between parent- and teacher-rated EF problems, and test results on experimental, yet appropriate neuropsychological tasks, designed to assess core executive functions. The aim of our study was to explore whether EF in PBTS can be reliably screened by means of parent or teacher questionnaires, which are less expensive and therefore more feasible than extensive neuropsychological assessment. Parents’ ratings of EF problems were poorly correlated to the EF test performances, but the teachers’ ratings showed several significant correlations of medium size. Correlations between test performances and questionnaire ratings were highest for the compound Total Score on the BRIEF-Teacher. Parents’ and teachers’ ratings were poorly interrelated, though the means of their ratings were not significantly different either. Interestingly, children who showed clinically elevated scores on the BRIEF-Teacher WM scale, Index scale, and/or on the Total scale, performed significantly worse on all EF tasks than children who did not show clinically elevated scores on these scales. The lack of significant relations between parent rated EFs and EF tasks is notable, particularly as parents appear to report more EF problems as compared to the normative population.14 One might expect that parents from PBTS could evaluate their child’s EF problems relatively well, as there is often a considerable difference in EF from before diagnosis to after treatment. In contrast, parents from children with a heritable condition, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),31 might consider EF problems less remarkable as they might exist since young age, and often are also seen in other family members. There are a number of explanations for the current poor relation between parent rated EF problems and children’s performance on EF tasks. Firstly, as all currently studied children had parent reported cognitive problems (such as affected attention, memory, speed, and/or information processing, or a clinical score on the attention scale of the DBD-RS), the variance in the parent ratings might have been too low to find correlations with other measures. However, this explanation seems unlikely as the standard deviations (SDs) of the BRIEF in the current study (7.99-12.01) are in line with the BRIEF T-score SDs (10). Secondly, parents of PBTS might condone deviant behavior more easily. This alteration in internal standards, known as response shift,32,33 might make parents less apt to rate their child’s EF reliably. On the other hand, teachers might also be prone to response shift, when they know what the child has been through in the past with their illness and the treatment. Finally, besides the advantages of computerized measures (such as ruling out the influence of the assessor,34 and precise, standardized, and reliably measuring EF), EF tasks may lack ecological validity compared to questionnaires. However, this also applies to teacher reports. The current findings confirm that experienced EF problems in daily life, particularly in the home situation, do not necessarily immediately derive from an assumed underlying cognitive (dys)function as measured with an EF task.

Page 122: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

120

The relation between teacher EF ratings and EF tasks was somewhat stronger. Teachers might be more competent to rate a child’s behavior, as they have other children in the classroom to compare the child with. This might particularly be true in the current sample, were 24% of the children received special education, with relatively small classes and specialized teachers (although these teachers might be more familiar with problem behavior, as expressed by other children within special education, and their evaluation might be biased). Moreover, teachers observe the child in an academic environment, and specific cognitive functions, such as focused attention, might largely influence schoolwork, but might not notably influence the home situation. Indeed, the BRIEF-Teacher more strongly relates to school functioning than the BRIEF-Parent,7 and the discrepancy between the parent and teacher reports might actually be meaningful.35

There are some notable patterns in the BRIEF-Teacher. Firstly, many subscales of the BRIEF-Teacher were related to the attention task, even though the BRIEF does not include a specific attention scale (and the currently used Monitor scale as a measure of attention is hence negotiable). The current findings suggest that EF problems as reported by teachers might largely be influenced by a child’s attention abilities. Secondly, the BRIEF-Teacher WM scale was related to several EF tasks, and this scale might therefore be considered as a more broad EF measure, or daily experienced WM might be influenced by several underlying EFs. Thirdly, children with clinically elevated scores on the WM-, Index-, or Total scale performed worse on all EF tasks with medium to large effect sizes (note that this was not found with the BRIEF Parent). This suggests that the BRIEF-Teacher might be useful to screen for EF problems, although the explorative character of these findings require replication. Different informants give distinct information and the use of a specific informant might depend on the goal of an assessment. The current findings suggest that EFs might be better assessed by teachers, but parent ratings should not be ruled out. As the BRIEF-Parent might not be the best measure of EFs in PBTS, alternative questionnaires could be explored to tap EF at home. Questionnaires that could be explored as possible screeners are Pediatric Perceived Cognitive Functioning (Peds-PCF) parent- or self-report 36, the Childhood Executive Functioning Inventory (CHEXI) parent-report,37 or the Subjective Awareness of Neuropsychological Deficits for Children (SAND-C) self-report,38 Self-reports might also give important, distinctive information. As Achenbach,39 and Toplak8 suggested, it is important to consult multiple informants to get a good view on the behavior, and cognitive functioning of children or adolescents.

Page 123: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

BRIEF-EF task association in PBTS

121

6

CONCLUSIONS

The current findings suggest that the BRIEF-Teacher might be a better screening tool for EFs in PBTS than the BRIEF-Parent. The BRIEF-Teacher does not seem specific enough to capture particular cognitive functions based on the subscales, but the Total and Index scores may be useful measures of general EF functioning. As a subclinical score on the BRIEF-Parent does not indicate that there are no EF problems, solely relying on parent ratings of EF as a screening of EF problems seems unreliable. Procedures to achieve structural screening of PBTS are important to explore, and it is relevant to explore alternative questionnaires.

Page 124: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

122

REFERENCES

1. Howlader N, Noone A, Krapcho M, et al. SEER Cancer Statistics Review, 1975-2011, National Cancer Institute. 2014. Available at: http://seer.cancer.gov/csr/1975_2011/.

2. de Ruiter MA, van Mourik R, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, Grootenhuis MA, Oosterlaan J. Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatment: a meta-analysis. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(5):408–417. doi:10.1111/dmcn.12020.

3. Anderson V, Northam E, Wrennall J. Developmental neuropsychology; A clinical approach. East Sussex: Psychology Press Ltd; 2014.

4. Walsh KS, Noll RB, Annett RD, Patel SK, Patenaude AF, Embry L. Standard of Care for Neuropsychological Monitoring in Pediatric Neuro-Oncology: Lessons From the Children’s Oncology Group (COG). Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2016;63:191–195. doi:10.1002/pbc.

5. Netson KL, Ashford JM, Skinner T, et al. Executive dysfunction is associated with poorer health-related quality of life in pediatric brain tumor survivors. J Neurooncol. 2016;128(2):313–21. doi:10.1007/s11060-016-2113-1.

6. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. TEST REVIEW Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6(3):235–238.

7. Smidts DP, Huizinga M. BRIEF executieve functies gedragsvragenlijst: Handleiding. Amsterdam: Hogrefe Uitgevers; 2009.

8. Toplak ME, West RF, Stanovich KE. Practitioner review: do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J child Psychol psychiatry. 2013;54(2):131–43. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12001.

9. Howarth R a, Ashford JM, Merchant TE, et al. The Utility of Parent Report in the Assessment of Working Memory among Childhood Brain Tumor Survivors. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19(4):380–389. doi:10.1017/S1355617712001567.

10. Best JR, Miller PH, Naglieri JA. Relations between Executive Function and Academic Achievement from Ages 5 to 17 in a Large, Representative National Sample. Learn Individ Differ. 2011;21(4):327–336. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007.

11. de Ruiter MA, Grootenhuis MA, van Mourik R, et al. Timed performance weaknesses on computerized tasks in pediatric brain tumor survivors: A comparison with sibling controls. Child Neuropsychol. 2015;53(9):1689–99. doi:10.1080/09297049.2015.1108395.

12. Oosterlaan J, Bayens D, Scheres A, Antrop J, Roeyers H, Sergeant JA. Handleiding van de VvGK6-16 vragenlijst voor gedragsproblemen bij kinderen van 6 tot en met 16 jaar [manual for the DBDRS disruptive behavior disorder rating scale for children from 6 to 16 years old]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Harcourt Assessment BV; 2008.

13. Ruiter MA de, Oosterlaan J, Antoinette Yvonne Narda Schouten-van Meeteren, et al. Neurofeedback ineffective in pediatric brain tumor survivors: Results of a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trial. Eur J Cancer. 2016;64:62–73. doi:10.1007/s13398-014-0173-7.2.

14. de Ruiter MA, Schouten-van Meeteren AYN, van Vuurden DG, et al. Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor survivors with neurocognitive complaints. Qual life Res. 2016;25(2):435–46. doi:10.1007/s11136-015-1091-7.

15. Verhulst FC, van der Ende J, Koot HM. Manual for the child behavior checklist: 4-18. Dutch Department of Child Psychiatry, Sophia Children’s Hospital Rotterdam; 1996.

16. Ferdinand RF, Van der Ende J. Diagnostic interview schedule for children IV parent-version. Rotterdam: Erasmus University Rotterdam, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry; 1998.

Page 125: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

BRIEF-EF task association in PBTS

123

6

17. Shaffer DFRCP, Schwab-stone M, Fisher P, et al. The Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children-Revised Version (DISC-R): I. Preparation, Field Testing, Interrater Reliability, and Acceptability. J Am Acad Child. 1993;32(3):643–650.

18. Pelham WJ, Gnagy E, Greenslade K, Milich R. Teacher ratings of DSM-III-R symptoms for the disruptive behavior disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31(2):210–218. doi:10.1097/00004583-199203000-00006.

19. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI, Petersen SE. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(3):340–7. doi:10.1162/089892902317361886.

20. de Kieviet JF, van Elburg RM, Lafeber HN, Oosterlaan J. Attention problems of very preterm children compared with age-matched term controls at school-age. J Pediatr. 2012;161(5):824–9. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.05.010.

21. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage. 2005;26(2):471–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004.

22. Logan GD, Cowan WB. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psychol Rev. 1984;91(3):295–327. doi:10.1037//0033-295X.91.3.295.

23. Kindlon D, Mezzacappa E, Earls F. Psychometric Properties of Impulsivity Measures: Temporal Stability, Validity and Factor Structure. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995;36(4):645–61. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb02319.x.

24. de Zeeuw P, Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Bijlhout J, et al. Inhibitory performance, response speed, intraindividual variability, and response accuracy in ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(7):808–16. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318172eee9.

25. Oosterlaan J, Logan GD, Sergeant J a. Response inhibition in AD/HD, CD, comorbid AD/HD + CD, anxious, and control children: a meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1998;39(3):411–25.

26. Nutley SB, Söderqvist S, Bryde S, Humphreys K, Klingberg T. Measuring working memory capacity with greater precision in the lower capacity ranges. Dev Neuropsychol. 2010;35(1):81–95. doi:10.1080/87565640903325741.

27. Kort W, Schittekatte M, Compaan EL, et al. WISC-III NL. handleiding. nederlandse bewerking. London: The Psychological Corporation; 2002.

28. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2005.

29. Wechsler D. Wechsler’s Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd Edition. London: Harcourt Brace and Company; 1992.

30. Gathercole SE, Pickering SJ. Assessment of working memory in six- and seven-year-old children. J Educ Psychol. 2000;92(2):377–390. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.92.2.377.

31. Faraone S V, Doyle AE. Genetic influences on attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2000;2(2):143–146.

32. Schwartz CE, Bode R, Repucci N, Becker J, Sprangers MAG, Fayers PM. The clinical significance of adaptation to changing health: A meta-analysis of response shift. Qual Life Res. 2006;15(9):1533–1550. doi:10.1007/s11136-006-0025-9.

33. Schwartz CE, Andresen EM, Nosek MA, Krahn GL. Response Shift Theory: Important Implications for Measuring Quality of Life in People With Disability. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2007;88(4):529–536. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2006.12.032.

34. Schatz P, Browndyke J. Applications of computer-based neuropsychological assessment. J Head Trauma Rehabil. 2002;17(5):395–410. doi:10.1097/00001199-200210000-00003.

35. De Los Reyes A, Thomas S a, Goodman KL, Kundey SM a. Principles underlying the use of multiple informants’ reports. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:123–49. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185617.

Page 126: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 6

124

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

36. Lai JS, Butt Z, Zelko F, et al. Development of a parent-report cognitive function item bank using item response theory and exploration of its clinical utility in computerized adaptive testing. J Pediatr Psychol. 2011;36(7):766–779. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsr005.

37. Thorell LB, Nyberg L. The childhood executive functioning inventory (CHEXI): a new rating instrument for parents and teachers. Dev Neuropsychol. 2008;33(4):536–552. doi:10.1080/87565640802101516.

38. Hufford BJ, Fastenau PS. Development and validation of the Subjective Awareness of Neuropsychological Deficits Questionnaire for Children (SAND-C). J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2005;27(3):255–77. doi:10.1080/13803390490515478.

39. Achenbach TM, Edelbrock C, Howell CT. Empirically based assessment of the behavioral/emotional problems of 2- and 3- year-old children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1987;15(4):629–50. doi:10.1007/BF00917246.

Page 127: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

7

Neurofeedback not effective in pediatric brain tumor

survivors: Results of a double-blind randomized

placebo-controlled trial

Marieke A. de Ruiter Jaap Oosterlaan

Antoinette Y.N. Schouten-van MeeterenHeleen Maurice-StamDannis G. van Vuurden

Corrie GiddingLaura R. Beek

Bernd GranzenHuib N. Caron

Martha A. Grootenhuis

European Journal of Cancer 2016, 64, 62-73

Page 128: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

126

ABSTRACT

Background: Many paediatric brain tumour survivors (PBTS) suffer from neurocognitive impairments. Promising effects of neurofeedback (NF) on neurocognitive functioning have been reported, however research into NF for PBTS has not been conducted. We investigated the effects of NF on neurocognitive functioning in PBTS using a double-blind randomised placebo-controlled trial with a parallel-group design (Pediatric Research on Improving Speed, Memory, and Attention; the PRISMA study).

Methods: Eligible for inclusion were PBTS with neurocognitive complaints, aged 8–18 years, >2 years post-treatment. They were recruited from five medical centres in the Netherlands. A randomisation table assigned participants to 30 sessions (two per week) of either NF or placebo feedback (PF) (ratio 1:1). Participants, parents, trainers, and researchers handling the data were blinded to group assignment. Participants were assessed pre-, post- and 6 months post-training to determine whether NF training would lead to improved functioning as compared with PF training. Primary outcome measures were attention, processing speed, memory, executive functioning, visuomotor integration, and intelligence. Linear mixed models analyses were used to test differences between NF and PF training over time.

Results: A total of 82 children were enrolled (mean age 13.9 years, standard deviation = 3.2, 49% males); 80 participants were randomised (NF: n = 40, PF n = 40); 71 participants completed the training (NF: n = 34, PF: n = 37); 68 participants completed training and 6 months post-training assessment (NF: n = 33, PF: n = 35). Similar improvements were found over time for the two treatment groups on the primary outcomes (all p’s > 0.15).

Conclusion: Results indicated no specific treatment-effects of NF on neurocognitive functioning of PBTS.

Page 129: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

127

7

INTRODUCTION

Paediatric brain tumour survivors (PBTS) often experience neurocognitive impairments, with attention being identified as a core problem.1 Neurocognitive impairments, in turn, lead to difficulties in acquiring new skills and knowledge. Ultimately this may lead to decreased academic and vocational achievements and may challenge social functioning and health related quality of life (HRQOL).2 So far, few interventions targeting neurocognitive impairments in PBTS have proven to be effective. Some training programs have been developed and investigated in the past, however, the studied groups were small and the efficacy limited.3,4 A recent study reported promising results of a computerised cognitive training as compared to a waiting list control group.5 The group who received the training showed improvements in neurocognitive functioning as compared to the control group, with large effect sizes. Due to the lack of a placebo control group, non-specific treatment-effects cannot be ruled out. Pharmacological interventions such as methylphenidate, have proven to be effective for neurocognitive deficits in PBTS.6 However, adverse side-effects have been reported, such as insomnia and loss of appetite.6

Neurofeedback (NF) is an intervention based on the principles of operant conditioning. In order to regulate brain activity, direct visual or auditory feedback of the brain activity is offered. Brain activity may be described in different frequency bands as depicted by a quantified electroencephalogram (qEEG), associated with different mental states. Beta frequencies (>12 Hz) are associated with a state of outwardly focused concentration.7 Reinforcing beta frequencies in healthy students leads to increased attention and arousal.8 The efficacy of NF in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has been studied extensively and promising results have been reported in uncontrolled and controlled studies.9,10 However, numerous reviews conclude that more blinded placebo-controlled randomised trials are warranted to establish efficacy of NF over non-specific treatment-effects.11,12 NF has not yet been studied in PBTS.This paper describes the results of the first double-blind randomised placebo-controlled trial investigating the efficacy of NF in PBTS (Pediatric Research on Improving Speed, Memory, and Attention; the PRISMA-study). It was hypothesised that PBTS receiving NF would improve more on objective measures of neurocognitive functioning than PBTS who received placebo feedback (PF). Secondary, it was expected that PBTS in the NF group would experience enhanced subjective functioning regarding neurocognitive and psychosocial outcomes.

Page 130: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

128

METHODS

Study designThis study used a double-blind parallel placebo-controlled design, with participants randomised 1:1 to either NF or PF. Participants of this study were recruited from five participating Dutch University hospitals: Emma Children’s Hospital/Academic Medical Center Amsterdam, VU Medical Center Amsterdam, University Medical Center Utrecht, Radboud University Medical Center Nijmegen, and Maastricht University Medical Center. Three children from other medical centres contacted us about participation independently. The study was approved by the local medical ethical committee (#W13_183). Data included in this manuscript were obtained in compliance with regulations of the Academic Medical Center Amsterdam and the Helsinki Declaration.

ParticipantsParticipants were eligible for inclusion if they were treated for a brain tumour more than 2 years prior to enrolment, aged 8–18 years, and suffered from parent-reported neurocognitive complaints. Interested parents were provided with a screening questionnaire concerning their child’s neurocognitive functioning, including the attention scale of the disruptive behaviour disorders rating scale (DBDRS),13 and items regarding problems with attention, memory, information processing, and speed. If the score on the attention scale of the DBDRS was 14 (subclinical functioning) or higher and/or the parents indicated at least two problems, children were considered eligible. Exclusion criteria were a premorbid diagnosis of ADHD (the current study focused on cancer related impairments), a mental or physical condition that restricted neurocognitive assessment (e.g. blindness), and insufficient mastery of the Dutch language.Potentially eligible children and their parents were informed about the study by their oncologist or psychologist and provided with written information on the study. Eligible children were subsequently invited for the first assessment. Written informed consent was obtained prior to the first assessment.

Randomisation and maskingRandomisation was done after selection of the most appropriate NF module based on the individual qEEG (Fig. 1). Three randomisation tables were generated using SPSS (fixed block size of four), one for each of the three NF modules. To ensure blinding, the three NF modules and three accompanying PF modules (Fig. 1) had identical interfaces and were randomly assigned a number between one and six as file name. Research team-member JO held the key to the codes of the training modules.

Page 131: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

129

7

One research team member analysed the qEEG and informed JO which NF module would be indicated. JO then randomised the participant to receive NF or PF, and notified MdR of the assigned, blinded, module (module 1–6; Fig. 1). MdR sent the module via email to the trainer. JO was not involved in training or testing participants or analysing the data. Participants, their parents, trainers and researchers handling the data were blinded regarding assignment of the participants until the analyses were finished.

118

Fig. 1. Procedure of the PRISMA study. Beta 1 up = reinforcing beta 1 frequency (15–20 Hz); SMR up = reinforcing sensory motor rhythm (12–15 Hz), with beta 1 inhibition; spindles down = beta 1 inhibition in case of beta spindles; qEEG = quantitative electroencephalogram.

Figure 1. Procedure of the PRISMA study. Beta 1 up = reinforcing beta 1 frequency (15–20 Hz); SMR up = reinforcing sensory motor rhythm (12–15 Hz), with beta 1 inhibition; spindles down = beta 1 inhibition in case of beta spindles; qEEG = quantitative electroencephalogram.

InterventionThe intervention consisted of 30 sessions of 30 min, twice weekly at home or school. Each session was divided in ten times 3 min training, alternated with 1-min breaks, when the participant was required to sit with the eyes closed. All participants were trained with an electrode on location Cz with a portable Brainquiry PET NF device and a laptop equipped with BioExplorer software version 1.5. The design of the intervention was comparable to the majority of previous studies into the effects of NF.12

After comparison of the participant’s qEEG to a commercially available normative database (Brain Resource,14 data of >4000 healthy subjects, aged 6–100 years), standardised criteria determined the most appropriate NF module:8 (1) reinforcing beta 1 (15–20 Hz) when beta

Page 132: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

130

1 power <1 standard deviation (SD) below the normative age and gender specific mean; (2) reinforcing sensory motor rhythm (SMR, 12–15 Hz), with beta 1 inhibition when beta 1 power >1SD above the normative mean and SMR <1SD above normative mean; and (3) beta 1 inhibition in case of beta spindles, or if beta 1 was >1SD above the normative mean and SMR power was >1SD above the normative mean. A PF module was designed, which provided feedback based on the ‘random signal generator’ of the BioExplorer software.All modules (NF and PF) were set to provide positive reinforcement 80% and negative reinforcement 20% of the time. A self-selected movie was used as positive reinforcement. A silent black screen was used as negative reinforcement. Positive reinforcement was based on an individually determined threshold, which was calculated from the amplitude of the brainwave frequency of interest (e.g. beta 1). The threshold was adjusted automatically during the session, based on the individual brain activity of the previous 30 s. This threshold was used to deliver positive reinforcement during the session. A threshold of 10 μV for muscular tension (>55 Hz) and 50 μV for noise (range 48–52 Hz) was used. If the muscular tension and/or the noise reached above the threshold, the movie stopped and the software produced a beeping sound until the muscular tension and/or noise were below the threshold again. This applied to the NF and PF modules.The training was conducted by carefully trained trainers, according to an extensive standard operating procedure. After each session, the trainer filled out a checklist, providing information about the quality of the training; the duration, the time, the movie, the alertness, and anything aberrant from the standard procedure. The checklist was emailed weekly to the researchers. Assessment of the checklist did not show any major deviations of the procedure.

ProceduresParticipants were assessed pre-training (T0), post-training (T1), and at 6-months follow-up (T2); see Fig. 1. Neurocognitive testing and qEEGs were conducted at three locations, using identical equipment, software and procedures. Questionnaires were completed online at home.

OUTCOME MEASURES

Primary outcomesWell-validated, mostly computerised, (adaptations of) widely used tests were selected to provide a comprehensive assessment of neurocognitive functioning, according to recent recommendations:15 attention,16 processing speed,16 memory,17–19 executive functioning (EF),20 visuomotor integration (VMI),21 and intellectual functioning (Table 1).17,18 For a detailed description of the tests, see Appendix.

Page 133: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

131

7

Secondary outcomesSelf-, parent- and/or teacher-questionnaires assessed HRQOL,22 social-emotional functioning,23 self-esteem,24,25 fatigue,26 behavioural EF,27 attention in daily life,28 and sleep disturbance.29 Widely used, reliable, and validated questionnaires were selected (Table 1). For a detailed description of the questionnaires, see Appendix.

Medical and demographic characteristicsParents of participants provided information on demographics, medical data were obtained from the medical records; see Table 2. Limited medical and demographic data were available for a subsample of non-participants (n = 45): age at assessment, age at diagnosis, time since diagnosis, gender, tumour histopathology and grade, type of treatment, and tumour location.

Statistical analysesSample size calculation was based on previous research on NF in children with ADHD, using nQuery Advisor.30 With an effect size of 0.60, alpha set at 0.05 and a power of 0.80, a minimum of 35 patients were required in both groups.30 Analyses were conducted using SPSS version 20.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, United States of America). Outliers on outcome measures were rescaled according to Tabachnick and Fidell.31 There were no missing data on any of the dependent measures. Independent-samples t-test and chi-square test were used to test differences on demographic and medical differences between: (1) participants and non-participants, (2) NF and PF, and (3) dropouts and non-dropouts. For (2) and (3), baseline differences on outcome measures were tested using one-sample t-tests.The short term (T1) and long term (T2) effects of NF were tested using linear mixed models analysis, accounting for the dependency of the data within subjects. Data of participants were included in the linear mixed models analyses if participants had completed the training as well as the pre-training and one post-training assessment. Differences between NF and PF at T0 regarding primary and secondary outcomes were controlled for by the use of a random intercept. For each outcome measure (except for the attention network task, ANT, and VMI task), a linear mixed model was fitted with a random intercept and fixed slopes for training (NF versus PF), time (T1 versus T0 and T2 versus T0) and the interaction term training × time. Training × time interaction effects tested the effects of NF compared to PF.To analyse the effects of NF on the efficiency of three attention networks, ANT-trial type (Table 1) was added to the model as a repeated factor. The three-way interaction training × time × trial type was used to establish effects of NF. To analyse the effects of NF on VMI, structure and speed (Table 1) were added to the model as repeated factors. The three-way interactions of training × time × structure and training × time × speed were used to establish effects of NF.

Page 134: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

132

Tabl

e 1.

Out

com

es a

nd m

easu

res.

Prim

ary

outc

omes

Mea

sure

men

tSu

bjec

tD

escr

iptio

nD

epen

dent

var

iabl

esAtt

entio

nAtt

entio

n ne

twor

k ta

sk

(AN

T)Ch

ildCo

mpu

teri

sed

resp

onse

task

with

a

neut

ral c

ondi

tion

and

thre

e co

nditi

ons

to m

easu

re th

e th

ree

netw

orks

Ale

rting

, ori

entin

g an

d ex

ecuti

ve a

tten

tion

b

Proc

essi

ng s

peed

Base

line

spee

d A

NT

Child

Neu

tral

con

ditio

n A

NT

Mea

n re

actio

n tim

eM

emor

yVi

sual

seq

uenc

ing

task

Child

Man

ually

repe

ating

a s

eque

nce

of

circ

les

Num

ber

of c

orre

ct it

ems

Dig

it sp

an fo

rwar

d (W

ISC/

WA

IS)

Child

Verb

ally

repe

ating

a s

eque

nce

of d

igits

fo

rwar

dN

umbe

r of

cor

rect

item

s

Exec

utive

func

tioni

ng:

Inhi

bitio

nSt

op s

igna

l tas

kCh

ildRe

spon

se in

hibi

tion

task

Stop

sig

nal r

eacti

on ti

me

Wor

king

mem

ory

Dig

it sp

an b

ackw

ard

(WIS

C/W

AIS

)Ch

ildVe

rbal

ly re

peati

ng a

seq

uenc

e of

dig

its

back

war

dsN

umbe

r of

cor

rect

item

s

Visu

omot

or

inte

grati

onTr

acki

ng a

nd p

ursu

it ta

skCh

ildFo

llow

ing

a m

ovin

g ta

rget

on

a sc

reen

Acc

urac

y c (s

truc

ture

, spe

ed)

Inte

llect

ual

func

tioni

ngA

bbre

viat

ed W

ISC-

III a

Child

(8–1

6)IQ

test

FSIQ

, PIQ

and

VIQ

Abb

revi

ated

WA

IS-II

I aCh

ild (1

7–18

)IQ

test

FSIQ

, PIQ

and

VIQ

Seco

ndar

y ou

tcom

esRe

spon

dent

Des

crip

tion

Dep

ende

nt v

aria

bles

HRQ

OL

Kids

cree

n 27

Child

Que

stion

naire

Phys

ical

wel

l-bei

ng, p

sych

olog

ical

wel

l-be

ing,

aut

onom

y an

d pa

rent

s, p

eers

an

d so

cial

sup

port

, sch

ool e

nviro

nmen

t,

gene

ric

Soci

al-e

moti

onal

fu

nctio

ning

SDQ

chi

ld v

ersi

onCh

ildQ

uesti

onna

ireTo

tal p

robl

em-s

core

SDQ

par

ent v

ersi

onPa

rent

Que

stion

naire

Tota

l pro

blem

-sco

reSD

Q te

ache

r ve

rsio

nTe

ache

rQ

uesti

onna

ireTo

tal p

robl

em-s

core

Page 135: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

133

7

Prim

ary

outc

omes

Mea

sure

men

tSu

bjec

tD

escr

iptio

nD

epen

dent

var

iabl

esSe

lf-es

teem

SPPC

Child

(8–1

1)Q

uesti

onna

ireSc

hola

stic

com

pete

nce,

soc

ial a

ccep

tanc

e,

athl

etic

com

pete

nce,

phy

sica

l app

eara

nce,

be

havi

oura

l con

duct

, clo

se fr

iend

ship

(S

PPA

onl

y), g

loba

l sel

f-w

orth

SPPA

Child

(12–

18)

Que

stion

naire

See

SPPC

Fatig

ueCI

SCh

ildQ

uesti

onna

ireSu

bjec

tive

fatig

ue, c

once

ntra

tion,

m

otiva

tion,

phy

sica

l acti

vity

, tot

al s

core

Exec

utive

fu

nctio

ning

BRIE

F pa

rent

ver

sion

Pare

ntQ

uesti

onna

ireBe

havi

oura

l ind

ex, m

etac

ogni

tion

inde

x,

tota

l sco

reBR

IEF

teac

her

vers

ion

Teac

her

Que

stion

naire

See

BRIE

F pa

rent

ver

sion

Atten

tion

daily

life

SWA

NPa

rent

Que

stion

naire

Atten

tion

defic

it, h

yper

activ

ity/i

mpu

lsiv

itySl

eep

dist

urba

nce

SDSC

Pare

ntQ

uesti

onna

ireIn

itiati

ng/m

aint

aini

ng s

leep

, exc

essi

ve

som

nole

nce,

tota

l sco

re

Not

es. I

ntel

lect

ual f

uncti

onin

g is

ass

esse

d pr

e-tr

aini

ng a

t T0

and

6 m

onth

s aft

er tr

aini

ng a

t T2

but n

ot d

irect

ly a

fter

trai

ning

at T

1. A

NT

= att

entio

n ne

twor

k ta

sk; B

RIEF

= b

ehav

iour

ratin

g in

vent

ory

of e

xecu

tive

func

tioni

ng; C

IS =

che

cklis

t ind

ivid

ual s

tren

gth;

FSI

Q =

full

scal

e IQ

; HRQ

OL

= he

alth

rela

ted

qual

ity

of li

fe; P

IQ =

per

form

ance

IQ; S

DQ

= s

tren

gths

and

diffi

culti

es q

uesti

onna

ires;

SD

SC =

sle

ep d

istu

rban

ce s

cale

for

child

ren;

SPP

C/SP

PA =

sel

f per

cepti

on

profi

le fo

r ch

ildre

n/ad

oles

cent

s; S

WA

N =

str

engt

hs a

nd w

eakn

esse

s of

AD

HD

-sym

ptom

s an

d no

rmal

-beh

avio

ur; V

IQ =

ver

bal I

Q; W

ISC-

III =

Wec

hsle

r in

telli

genc

e sc

ale

for

child

ren

– th

ird v

ersi

on; W

AIS

-III =

Wec

hsle

r ad

ult i

ntel

ligen

ce s

cale

– th

ird v

ersi

on.

a Th

e fo

llow

ing

subt

asks

wer

e ad

min

iste

red:

ari

thm

etic,

sim

ilari

ties,

blo

ck d

esig

n, a

nd p

ictu

re c

ompl

etion

.b Th

e effi

cien

cy o

f the

ale

rting

, ori

entin

g, a

nd e

xecu

tive

atten

tion

netw

orks

wer

e as

sess

ed b

y co

ntra

sting

the

mea

n re

actio

n tim

es o

f cor

rect

resp

onse

s on

ale

rting

, ori

entin

g an

d ex

ecuti

ve tr

ials

resp

ectiv

ely,

to th

e m

ean

reac

tion

time

of th

e co

rrec

t res

pons

es to

neu

tral

tria

ls.

c Th

e ac

cura

cy s

core

s on

two

part

s (s

truc

ture

d an

d un

stru

ctur

ed) o

n si

x di

ffere

nt s

peed

leve

ls w

ere

used

as

mea

sure

s of

vis

uom

otor

inte

grati

on.

Page 136: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

134

Table 2. Demographics and medical characteristics of participants who completed the training.

Neurofeedback N = 34

Placebo-feedback N = 37

Mean SD Mean SDAge and timeAge at assessment 14.45 2.99 13.45 3.28Age diagnosis 6.81 3.65 7.42 4.09Time since diagnosis 7.64 4.04 6.03 2.99

N % N %GenderBoys 16 47 19 51Girls 18 53 18 49Tumour gradeHigh grade 11 32 17 46Low grade 23 68 20 54TreatmentSurgery a 17 50 18 49Chemotherapy/Radiotherapy b 17 50 19 51LocationSupratentorial 20 59 20 54Infratentorial 14 41 17 46HydrocephalusYes 18 53 14 38No 16 47 23 62Country of origin motherNetherlands 28 82 33 89Other 6 18 4 11Country of origin fatherNetherlands 30 88 32 86Other 4 12 5 14Highest parental education c

Low or intermediate 14 41 19 51High 20 59 18 49

Notes. None of the group differences reached significance (p > 0.05). High grade = medulloblastoma, supratentorial primitive neuroectodermal tumour (PNET), ependymoma, astrocytoma grade III, germ cell tumour; low grade = low grade glioma, craniopharyngioma, plexus papilloma; SD = standard deviation.a Surgery, biopsy or cerebrospinal fluid pressure relief onlyb Chemotherapy and/or radiotherapy with or without surgeryc Based on the parent with the highest education: low – intermediate = primary education, general secondary education and secondary vocational education; high = higher vocational education and university.

Page 137: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

135

7

We also tested the possibility that beneficial effects of NF would be evident for the reinforcing beta 1 module (power did not allow examining the other two NF modules). This analysis included 55 participants (77%) who were allocated to receive the reinforcing beta 1 module (NF: n = 28, PF: n = 27).For primary outcomes, p-values of <0.05 were considered significant. For secondary outcomes, the p-value was set on <0.01 due to the large number of outcomes.An independent data monitoring safety board (DMSB) was installed to receive reports on serious adverse events (SAE), defined as seizures that are not controllable with conventional antiepileptic drugs and which require admittance to an intensive care unit. In case the total number of dropouts was >10% or the number of SAE was >5% of participants, the DMSB would receive information about which module (NF or PF) participants received and decide whether the study should be terminated. The trial is registered at clinicaltrials.gov: NCT00961922.32

RESULTS

ParticipantsA total of 82 participants enrolled in the study (mean age 13.9 years, SD = 3.2, 49% males), 80 were randomised (NF: n = 40, PF: n = 40, refused qEEG: n = 2), 71 completed training and T1 (mean number of sessions = 30, range 28–33, NF: n = 34, PF: n = 37) and were included into the analyses, and 68 also completed T2 (NF: n = 33, PF: n = 35). Post-training and follow-up data were lacking for 8 of the 9 participants who dropped out. The analyses based on intention-to-treat (including one single subject for whom data were available) yielded highly similar results as the analysis without the data of the drop-out (data not shown).Fig. 2 shows the flow diagram. Recruitment took place between November 2, 2009 and May 3, 2012. The assessments and intervention took place between January 5, 2010 and December 4, 2013.

Page 138: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

136

125

Fig. 2. Flow diagram. Beta 1 up = reinforcing beta 1 frequency (15–20 Hz); SMR up = reinforcing sensory motor rhythm (12–15 Hz), with beta 1 inhibition; spindles down = beta 1 inhibition in case of beta spindles; qEEG = quantitative electroencephalogram.

Figure 2. Flow diagram. Beta 1 up = reinforcing beta 1 frequency (15–20 Hz); SMR up = reinforcing sensory motor rhythm (12–15 Hz), with beta 1 inhibition; spindles down = beta 1 inhibition in case of beta spindles; qEEG = quantitative electroencephalogram.

Page 139: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

137

7

Demographic and medical variables are displayed in Table 2. Participants did not differ from non-participants regarding age at assessment, gender and tumour location (p > 0.05). The percentage of high-grade tumours, radiotherapy, and chemotherapy was higher in the participants (p < 0.05). Compared with non-participants, participants were younger at diagnosis (p < 0.05) and time since diagnosis was longer (p < 0.05). There was no difference between the dropouts (n = 11) and the non-dropouts at T0 (n = 71) on the demographic and medical variables and the primary outcomes (p > 0.05). On the secondary outcomes at T0, teachers reported more problems for dropouts (n = 7) on two of three behavioural EF scales and on social-emotional functioning (p < 0.01) than for non-dropouts (n = 66). No other differences were found (p > 0.01).Regarding the primary neurocognitive outcomes (Table 3) no beneficial effects of NF were found; all interaction effects involving training × time, were not significant (p > 0.15). However, significant effects of time were found for processing speed (p < 0.001), visual short-term memory (p < 0.001), working memory (p = 0.01), and intelligence (FSIQ: p < 0.001; PIQ: p = 0.01; VIQ: p = 0.01), indicating that both groups improved over time.Regarding secondary outcomes (Table 4), NF revealed no advantage over PF. Several significant effects of time suggested improvements on the ‘autonomy and parents’ subscale of the self-report HRQOL (p = 0.002; data not shown), self- and parent-report social-emotional functioning (p = 0.001), the ‘behavioural conduct’ subscale of self-report self-esteem (p < 0.001; data not shown), the ‘concentration’ subscale of self-report fatigue (p = 0.001; data not shown), and parent-report EF (p < 0.001).No SAEs were reported and <10% participants dropped out.The exploratory analyses on reinforcing beta 1 yielded similar results: no significant intervention effects on neurocognitive outcomes. Only one secondary outcome measure showed significant training × time effects for NF resulting in worse self-reported self-esteem (subscale ‘athletic competence’, p = 0.005; data not shown).

Page 140: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

138

Tabl

e 3.

Pri

mar

y, n

euro

cogn

itive

, out

com

es o

f par

ticip

ants

at T

0, T

1, T

2: n

euro

feed

back

ver

sus

plac

ebo-

feed

back

.

Neu

rofe

edba

ck (N

F)

Plac

ebo-

feed

back

(PF)

Tim

e

Trai

ning

*tim

e

T0

N =

34

T1

N =

34

T2

N =

33

T0

N =

37

T1

N =

37

T2

N =

35

Ove

rall

T0–T

1T0

–T2

Ove

rall

T0–T

1 N

F ve

rsus

PF

T0–T

2 N

F ve

rsus

PF

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

p-Va

lue

ββ

p-Va

lue

ββ

Atten

tion

a0.

780.

70A

NT

aler

ting

atten

tion

c51

.75

(28.

57)

48.4

8 (3

5.21

)54

.38

(32.

30)

56.2

7 (3

2.32

)59

.76

(25.

80)

63.4

9 (2

9.55

)−0

.01

0.06

−0.1

0−0

.05

AN

T or

ienti

ng

atten

tion

−40.

40

(30.

31)

−43.

13

(23.

92)

−41.

46

(19.

33)

−39.

32

(36.

59)

−40.

46

(29.

28)

−40.

91

(24.

93)

−0.0

20.

000.

010.

02

AN

T ex

ecuti

ve

atten

tion

c28

.12

(32.

29)

30.8

3 (3

0.49

)34

.20

(26.

50)

45.7

4 (4

3.09

)28

.86

(33.

99)

32.5

5 (3

3.07

)0.

030.

070.

120.

14

AN

T in

trai

ndiv

idua

l co

effici

ent c

0.21

(0

.06)

0.21

(0

.05)

0.20

(0

.05)

0.23

(0

.06)

0.22

(0

.06)

0.21

(0

.06)

0.06

0.01

−0.1

40.

540.

180.

19

Proc

essi

ng s

peed

AN

T m

ean

reac

tion

time

c40

1.42

(1

04.9

2)36

8.51

(9

9.83

)33

8.24

(8

7.73

)43

4.40

(1

27.6

5)38

6.79

(1

06.1

8)35

9.23

(9

5.89

)<0

.001

−0.2

9**

−0.5

4***

0.53

0.11

0.12

Mem

ory

Visu

al s

hort

-ter

m

mem

ory

14.4

4 (4

.57)

14.7

6 (4

.78)

15.7

9 (4

.93)

13.0

3 (4

.04)

14.3

5 (4

.10)

14.5

4 (4

.11)

<0.0

010.

080.

31**

0.22

−0.2

3−0

.04

Aud

itory

sho

rt-t

erm

m

emor

y7.

97

(2.0

1)8.

09

(1.8

8)7.

91

(1.9

4)7.

95

(1.9

0)8.

00

(2.0

3)8.

00

(1.9

9)0.

750.

08−0

.01

0.91

0.06

−0.0

1

Exec

utive

func

tioni

ngW

orki

ng m

emor

y5.

50

(2.0

9)6.

21

(2.3

6)6.

36

(2.3

3)5.

30

(2.3

3)5.

59

(2.3

0)5.

80

(2.7

6)0.

010.

34*

0.41

**0.

480.

210.

20

Stop

task

resp

onse

in

hibi

tion

c23

5.56

(4

9.54

)24

4.02

(3

9.14

)23

2.30

(3

6.13

)26

7.49

(6

2.47

)d26

7.25

(5

6.29

)24

5.74

(5

0.38

) e0.

010.

12−0

.05

0.15

0.15

0.37

Page 141: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

139

7

Neu

rofe

edba

ck (N

F)

Plac

ebo-

feed

back

(PF)

Tim

e

Trai

ning

*tim

e

T0

N =

34

T1

N =

34

T2

N =

33

T0

N =

37

T1

N =

37

T2

N =

35

Ove

rall

T0–T

1T0

–T2

Ove

rall

T0–T

1 N

F ve

rsus

PF

T0–T

2 N

F ve

rsus

PF

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

M (S

D)

p-Va

lue

ββ

p-Va

lue

ββ

Inte

llige

nce

FSIQ

91.1

8 (1

9.44

)

b95

.16

(19.

22)

90.5

1 (1

7.24

)

b92

.71

(18.

18)

0.00

1b

0.22

**0.

22b

0.12

PIQ

93.2

1 (2

1.29

)

b96

.73

(20.

00)

91.0

0 (1

6.72

)

b93

.23

(18.

11)

0.01

b0.

19*

0.60

b0.

07

VIQ

91.3

3 (1

5.63

)

b95

.06

(15.

46)

92.3

9 (1

5.69

)

b94

.06

(16.

46)

0.01

b0.

25**

0.16

b0.

18

Not

es. D

ata

of p

artic

ipan

ts w

ere

incl

uded

in th

e lin

ear m

ixed

mod

els

anal

yses

if p

artic

ipan

ts h

ad c

ompl

eted

the

trai

ning

as

wel

l as

the

pre-

trai

ning

and

on

e po

st-t

rain

ing

asse

ssm

ent.

Pla

cebo

-fee

dbac

k w

as th

e re

fere

nce

cate

gory

in th

e an

alys

es.

For

all o

utco

mes

, exc

ept

for

inte

llige

nce,

raw

sco

res

are

repo

rted

. For

inte

llige

nce,

age

sta

ndar

dise

d sc

ores

are

pro

vide

d. S

core

s ba

sed

on r

eacti

on

times

are

in m

s (A

NT,

sto

p ta

sk).

Out

com

es w

ere

anal

ysed

aft

er t

rans

form

ation

into

sta

ndar

d no

rmal

sco

res,

exp

ress

ing

devi

ation

s fr

om t

he o

vera

ll m

ean

at b

asel

ine

(T0)

, so

that

the

coeffi

cien

ts c

ould

be

inte

rpre

ted

as s

tand

ardi

sed

β co

effici

ents

. Hig

her s

core

s re

flect

bett

er fu

nctio

ning

, unl

ess

stat

ed

othe

rwis

e.Sc

ores

of

the

visu

omot

or in

tegr

ation

tas

k ar

e no

t di

spla

yed,

res

ults

of

the

anal

yses

are

des

crib

ed in

the

tex

t. F

or in

telli

genc

e, W

ISC

and

WA

IS s

core

s w

ere

com

bine

d. A

NT

= att

entio

n ne

twor

k ta

sk; F

SIQ

= fu

ll sc

ale

IQ; M

= m

ean;

PIQ

= p

erfo

rman

ce IQ

; SD

= s

tand

ard

devi

ation

; T0

= ba

selin

e; T

1 =

post

-tr

aini

ng; T

2 =

6-m

onth

follo

w-u

p; V

IQ =

ver

bal I

Q; W

ISC

= W

echs

ler

inte

llige

nce

scal

e; W

AIS

= W

AIS

-III =

Wec

hsle

r ad

ult

inte

llige

nce

scal

e. *

p <

0.05

; **

p <

0.01

; ***

p <

0.00

repo

rted

the

in c

olum

n Tr

aini

ng ×

tim

e ar

e ba

sed

on th

e th

ree-

way

inte

racti

on T

ime

× Tr

aini

ng ×

AN

T-co

nditi

on.

a β

repo

rted

the

in c

olum

n Ti

me

are

base

d on

the

two-

way

inte

racti

on T

ime

× A

NT-

cond

ition

.b N

ot a

sses

sed/

not a

naly

sed

c Lo

wer

sco

res

refle

ct b

etter

func

tioni

ng.

d N

= 3

4e N

= 3

3

Page 142: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

140

Tabl

e 4.

Sec

onda

ry o

utco

mes

of p

artic

ipan

ts a

t T0,

T1,

T2:

neu

rofe

edba

ck v

ersu

s pl

aceb

o-fe

edba

ck.

Neu

rofe

edba

ck (N

F)

Plac

ebo-

feed

back

(PF)

Tim

e

Trai

ning

× ti

me

T0 N

= 3

4T1

N =

33

T2 N

= 3

2T0

N =

36

T1 N

= 3

7T2

N =

33

Ove

rall

T0–T

1T0

–T2

Ove

rall

T0–T

1 N

F ve

rsus

PF

T0–T

2 N

F ve

rsus

PF

Self-

repo

rtM

(SD

)M

(SD

)M

(SD

)M

(SD

)M

(SD

)M

(SD

)p-

valu

βp-

valu

βH

RQO

LG

ener

ic a

48.2

7 (7

.10)

b48

.50

(6.9

9)c

51.0

9 (7

.54)

d49

.18

(6.4

4)50

.40

(7.6

8)e

49.8

8 (7

.80)

b0.

20−0

.02

0.33

0.35

−0.1

90.

20

Soci

al-e

moti

onal

func

tioni

ngTo

tal p

robl

em-s

core

9.62

(4

.36)

8.64

(4

.49)

7.97

(4

.15)

10.6

4 (5

.42)

9.38

(4

.79)

8.61

(4

.33)

0.00

1−0

.16

−0.2

80.

790.

110.

09

Self-

este

emG

loba

l sel

f-w

orth

a52

.47

(34.

50)

48.6

4 (3

2.66

)49

.13

(32.

06)

57.9

2 (2

9.33

)58

.92

(32.

05)

62.1

5 (3

1.82

)0.

96−0

.12

−0.1

30.

36−0

.19

−0.2

7

Fatig

ueTo

tal s

core

60.9

7 (2

2.52

)b60

.06

(17.

74)b

61.8

1 (2

2.49

)64

.73

(21.

46)f

59.9

2 (2

0.77

)63

.48

(22.

24)

0.34

0.02

0.13

0.26

0.30

0.28

Pare

nt-r

epor

tN

= 3

4N

= 3

3N

= 3

3N

= 3

7N

= 3

6N

= 3

4So

cial

-em

otion

al fu

nctio

ning

Tota

l pro

blem

-sco

re10

.50

(4.3

1)9.

03

(4.6

3)8.

94

(4.8

3)11

.22

(5.1

8)10

.06

(4.5

4)9.

12

(4.7

0)0.

001

−0.2

7−0

.31

0.79

−0.0

50.

08

Exec

utive

func

tioni

ngBe

havi

oura

l ind

ex52

.32

(9.6

8)49

.85

(11.

61)

49.7

6 (1

1.35

)55

.19

(12.

56)

51.7

2 (1

1.14

)51

.18

(10.

94)

0.00

1−0

.22

−0.2

30.

900.

060.

05

Met

acog

nitio

n in

dex

52.6

8 (8

.85)

49.9

1 (9

.21)

50.7

0 (9

.23)

55.4

1 (7

.80)

53.2

2 (7

.51)

52.9

1 (9

.86)

0.00

4−0

.32

−0.2

60.

90−0

.06

0.04

Tota

l sco

re52

.71

(8.3

7)49

.70

(9.9

1)50

.33

(9.8

1)55

.68

(8.9

8)52

.89

(8.2

6)52

.50

(9.8

6)<0

.001

−0.3

4**

−0.2

90.

93−0

.03

0.04

Page 143: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

141

7

Atten

tion

in d

aily

life

Atten

tion

defic

it a

0.34

(0

.99)

0.14

(0

.91)

0.33

(1

.01)

0.77

(0

.74)

0.54

(0

.74)

0.41

(0

.71)

0.02

−0.2

3−0

.06

0.12

0.03

0.36

Hyp

erac

tivity

/im

puls

ivity

a−0

.65

(0.9

2)−0

.77

(0.9

5)−0

.60

(1.0

0)−0

.09

(0.8

4)−0

.18

(0.7

9)−0

.16

(0.6

8)0.

46−0

.16

−0.0

10.

86−0

.07

0.05

Slee

p di

sord

ers

Tota

l sco

re40

.18

(7.7

3)39

.39

(8.4

0)39

.00

(7.7

8)40

.54

(6.4

4)40

.47

(6.6

2)39

.65

(6.7

6)0.

41−0

.08

−0.1

20.

82−0

.10

0.02

Teac

her-

repo

rtN

= 3

2N

= 3

1N

= 2

8N

= 3

4N

= 3

3N

= 3

0So

cial

-em

otion

al fu

nctio

ning

Tota

l pro

blem

-sco

re8.

91

(4.7

7)8.

58

(5.6

3)8.

29

(4.7

9)9.

74

(6.1

8)9.

18

(5.1

5)8.

47

(4.4

6)0.

22−0

.12

−0.1

90.

920.

08−0

.00

Exec

utive

func

tioni

ngBe

havi

oura

l ind

ex48

.81

(10.

83)

46.7

0 (1

0.44

)c46

.36

(9.5

6)48

.94

(8.4

3)50

.45

(11.

04)

51.6

9 (1

4.07

)d0.

46−0

.30

−0.3

50.

03−0

.31

−0.6

6**

Met

acog

nitio

n in

dex

48.4

4 (1

1.71

)45

.27

(10.

70)c

47.9

6 (1

2.25

)51

.79

(10.

45)

52.1

2 (1

1.31

)51

.24

(10.

94)d

0.12

−0.3

5−0

.16

0.31

−0.2

9−0

.04

Tota

l sco

re48

.47

(11.

79)

45.4

3 (1

0.48

)c47

.46

(11.

40)

50.6

5 (9

.64)

51.4

2 (1

1.31

)51

.34

(12.

41)d

0.15

−0.3

5−0

.21

0.28

−0.3

1−0

.24

Not

es. D

ata

of p

artic

ipan

ts w

ere

incl

uded

in th

e lin

ear m

ixed

mod

els

anal

yses

if p

artic

ipan

ts h

ad c

ompl

eted

the

trai

ning

as

wel

l as

the

pre-

trai

ning

and

on

e po

st-t

rain

ing

asse

ssm

ent.

Pla

cebo

-fee

dbac

k w

as th

e re

fere

nce

cate

gory

in th

e an

alys

es.

For

HRQ

OL,

sel

f-es

teem

and

exe

cutiv

e fu

ntion

ing

stan

dard

ised

sco

res

are

repo

rted

, fo

r ot

her

mea

sure

s ra

w s

core

s ar

e pr

ovid

ed.

Out

com

es w

ere

anal

ysed

aft

er tr

ansf

orm

ation

into

stan

dard

nor

mal

scor

es, e

xpre

ssin

g de

viati

ons f

rom

the

over

all m

ean

at b

asel

ine

(T0)

, so

that

the

coeffi

cien

ts c

ould

be

inte

rpre

ted

as s

tand

ardi

sed

β co

effici

ents

. Low

er s

core

s re

flect

bett

er fu

nctio

ning

, unl

ess

stat

ed o

ther

wis

e. P

-val

ues

of <

0.01

are

con

side

red

sign

ifica

nt.

For

Self-

Este

em, S

PPC

and

SPPA

sco

res

wer

e co

mbi

ned.

HRQ

OL

= he

alth

rel

ated

qua

lity

of li

fe;

M =

mea

n; S

D =

sta

ndar

d de

viati

on;

T0 =

bas

elin

e;

T1 =

pos

t-tr

aini

ng; T

2 =

6 m

onth

s fo

llow

-up.

**p

< 0.

01.

a H

ighe

r sc

ores

refle

ct b

etter

func

tioni

ng.

b N

= 3

2.c N

= 3

0.d N

= 2

9.e N

= 3

6.f N

= 3

3

Page 144: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

142

DISCUSSION

The results of this first double-blind randomised placebo-controlled trial, aimed at investigating the efficacy of NF for PBTS, reveal no positive effects for NF over PF on any of the primary and secondary outcomes. In both groups, PBTS improved over time on the majority of primary and some secondary outcome measures, with small to medium effects. However, improvements over time have also been found in the four placebo-controlled studies in children with ADHD, potentially reflecting non-specific treatment-effects.33–36

The sound design of our study allows us to conclude that NF is not effective in improving neurocognitive functioning in PBTS. In agreement with our findings, the effects of NF in children with ADHD were found to be smaller in randomised controlled trials than in uncontrolled trials, and in double-blind trials no beneficial effects of NF in patients with ADHD were obtained, although similar to our study, improvements over time were found.12 In addition, our study had sufficient power to detect effects of medium size. Furthermore, a 6-month follow-up assessment was included to rule out a potential sleeper effect and lastly, dropout was low without evidence for selective dropout.The design of the NF and PF modules paralleled the mainstream of previous studies into the effects of NF. The NF modules used in this study were aimed at the beta 1 frequency, associated with focused attention. Attention has been identified as a fundamental neurocognitive domain that underlies higher order domains, such as working memory, intelligence and academic functioning.37 Reinforcing beta 1 in combination with theta inhibition has often been used for ADHD.38 It has been argued that treatment-induced white matter impairments cause neurocognitive deficits in PBTS and also that NF enhances white matter associated with increased attention, as was shown in a placebo-controlled trial where they reinforced beta 1 in healthy students.39 We found no evidence of an effect of NF on attention but information on white matter of the participants is lacking. The number of sessions of NF in our study has yielded large positive effects in earlier controlled studies performed in children with ADHD.38 We cannot rule out that PBTS would benefit from a more intensive training, however, in children and adults with traumatic brain injury, whose brain damage is of structural nature as in PBTS, similar numbers of sessions produced positive outcomes.40 It could be argued that using automatically adjusted thresholds is less effective than manually adjusted, however, a recent study did not find larger effect sizes when thresholds were switched from automatically to manually adjusted.35 Giving 80% positive feedback, as we did, has been criticised as possibly too high for optimal learning.41 However, the optimal percentage of feedback to optimise learning principles has yet to be investigated.41,42

The improvements we found over time in both training groups have also been reported in other NF studies,33–36 and in other intervention studies.43 The improvements may be a result

Page 145: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

143

7

of non-specific treatment-effects, including the influence of positive expectations about the efficacy of the intervention, a substantial risk in a ‘high-tech’ setting like NF. Also personal attention of the trainer, the structured setting of the sessions, requiring participants to focus their attention for 30 min twice a week are potential non-specific treatment-effects. Regression to the mean, a well-known phenomenon in behavioural sciences, could also have caused the improvements.44 Another reason might be found in 1-min breaks, sitting with the eyes closed after every 3-min training block. This may have given the intervention an unintended mindfulness-like character, for which some evidence has been found for children in improving behavioural and psychosocial functioning.45 The year time between the T0 and T2 assessments may also have played a role in some improvements, due to maturation. Although it has been suggested that PBTS learn at a slower rate than their peers, (‘growing into deficit’),46 we did not find evidence for this, as the scores of PBTS improved also on age-corrected intelligence scores. It could not be ruled out that the improvements over time could have resulted from practice-effects, since most tests were repeated three times in a year’s time. To minimise the chance of practice-effects, neurocognitive measures with high test-retest validity were used, such as the inhibition task.20 Furthermore, intelligence was not tested after the intervention, increasing the interval between the intelligence tests to 1 year.There are some limitations that should be taken into account when interpreting the results of this study. Patients were selected based on parent-reported complaints, not based on objective measures. This recruitment process could have led to inclusion of PBTS without ‘room for improvement’ in neurocognitive functioning. However, the results of the objective neurocognitive tests at T0 showed more problems in PBTS than in sibling controls, with effect sizes ranging from 0.40 to 0.82 (p < 0.05).47 Also, participants were more likely than non-participants to have high-grade tumours, to have been treated with radiotherapy and chemotherapy, and were on average younger at diagnosis with a longer time since diagnosis. This suggests that participants may have been at greater risk for neurocognitive late effects than non-participants. This might in turn limit the generalisation of the findings to a lower risk cohort. Regarding the intervention, a recent development is the application of transfer sessions, in which the participant does not receive feedback, while trying to maintain the desired brain activity to enhance generalisation from the training setting into daily life.42 We did not include this type of session.Concluding, the results of this double-blind randomised placebo-controlled trial show that NF does not have favourable effects on neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning in PBTS as compared with PF. Both NF and PF were found to improve neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning, possibly due to non-specific treatment-effects. Thus far positive findings have been reported for several interventions, especially methylphenidate and the Cogmed training. However, interventions aiming to improve neurocognitive functioning of

Page 146: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

144

PBTS remain to be developed that lack the possible side-effects of medication, remain to be studied using double-blind randomised placebo-controlled trials, to rule out confounding of results by non-specific training-effects.

Page 147: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

145

7

REFERENCES

1. Mulhern RK, White HA, Glass JO, et al. Attentional functioning and white matter integrity among survivors of malignant brain tumors of childhood. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2004;10:180–189.

2. Turner CD, Rey-Casserly C, Liptak CC, Chordas C. Late effects of therapy for pediatric brain tumor survivors. J Clin Oncol. 2009;24(11):1455–1463.

3. Butler RW, Copeland DR, Fairclough DL, et al. A multicenter, randomized clinical trial of a cognitive remediation program for childhood survivors of a pediatric malignancy. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76(3):367–378. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.367.

4. van ’t Hooft I, Andersson K, Bergman B, Sejersen T, von Wendt L, Bartfai A. Sustained favorable effects of cognitive training in children with acquired brain injuries. NeuroRehabilitation. 2007;22(2):109–116.

5. Conklin HM, Ogg RJ, Ashford JM, et al. Computerized Cognitive Training for Amelioration of Cognitive Late Effects Among Childhood Cancer Survivors: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Clin Oncol. 2015;33(33):3894–3902. doi:10.1200/JCO.2015.61.6672.

6. Conklin HM, Khan RB, Reddick WE, et al. Acute neurocognitive response to methylphenidate among survivors of childhood cancer: a randomized, double-blind, cross-over trial. J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(9):1127–1139. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsm045.

7. Hammond DC. What Is Neurofeedback? J Neurother. 2006;10(4):25–36. doi:10.1300/J184v10n04.

8. Egner T, Gruzelier JH. EEG Biofeedback of low beta band components: Frequency-specific effects on variables of attention and event-related brain potentials. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004;115:131–139. doi:10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00353-5.

9. Strehl U, Leins U, Goth G, Klinger C, Hinterberger T, Birbaumer N. Self-regulation of slow cortical potentials: a new treatment for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics. 2006;118(5):e1530–e1540. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-2478.

10. Gevensleben H, Holl B, Albrecht B, et al. Neurofeedback training in children with ADHD: 6-month follow-up of a randomised controlled trial. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19(9):715–724. doi:10.1007/s00787-010-0109-5.

11. Holtmann M, Sonuga-Barke E, Cortese S, Brandeis D. Neurofeedback for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2014;23(4):789–806. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2014.05.006.

12. Moriyama TS, Polanczyk G, Caye A, Banaschewski T, Brandeis D, Rohde L a. Evidence-based information on the clinical use of neurofeedback for ADHD. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9(3):588–598. doi:10.1007/s13311-012-0136-7.

13. Oosterlaan J, Bayens D, Scheres A, Antrop J, Roeyers H, Sergeant JA. Handleiding van de VvGK6-16 vragenlijst voor gedragsproblemen bij kinderen van 6 tot en met 16 jaar [manual for the DBDRS disruptive behavior disorder rating scale for children from 6 to 16 years old]. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Harcourt Assessment BV; 2008.

14. Brain Resource. Available at: http://www.brainresource.com/. Accessed September 5, 2012.15. Ullrich NJ, Embry L. Neurocognitive dysfunction in survivors of childhood brain tumors. Semin

Pediatr Neurol. 2012;19(1):35–42. doi:10.1016/j.spen.2012.02.014.16. de Kieviet JF, van Elburg RM, Lafeber HN, Oosterlaan J. Attention problems of very preterm

children compared with age-matched term controls at school-age. J Pediatr. 2012;161(5):824–9. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.05.010.

17. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2005.

Page 148: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

146

18. Wechsler D. Wechsler’s Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd Edition. London: Harcourt Brace and Company; 1992.

19. Nutley SB, Söderqvist S, Bryde S, Humphreys K, Klingberg T. Measuring working memory capacity with greater precision in the lower capacity ranges. Dev Neuropsychol. 2010;35(1):81–95. doi:10.1080/87565640903325741.

20. Oosterlaan J, Logan GD, Sergeant J a. Response inhibition in AD/HD, CD, comorbid AD/HD + CD, anxious, and control children: a meta-analysis of studies with the stop task. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1998;39(3):411–25.

21. De Kieviet JF, Stoof CJJ, Geldof CJ a, et al. The crucial role of the predictability of motor response in visuomotor deficits in very preterm children at school age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(7):624–30. doi:10.1111/dmcn.12125.

22. Ravens-Sieberer U, Auquier P, Erhart M, et al. The KIDSCREEN-27 quality of life measure for children and adolescents: psychometric results from a cross-cultural survey in 13 European countries. Qual life Res. 2007;16(8):1347–56. doi:10.1007/s11136-007-9240-2.

23. van Widenfelt BM, Goedhart AW, Treffers PD a, Goodman R. Dutch version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;12(6):281–9. doi:10.1007/s00787-003-0341-3.

24. Veerman J, Straathof M, Treffers P, van den Bergh B, ten Brink L. Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Kinderen, CBSK (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Children – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997.

25. Treffers P, Goedhart A, Veerman J, van den Bergh B, Ackaert L, de Rycke L. Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Adolescenten, CBSA (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Adolescents – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002.

26. Stulemeijer M, de Jong LWAM, Fiselier TJW, Hoogveld SWB, Bleijenberg G. Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2005;330(7481):14. doi:10.1136/bmj.38301.587106.63.

27. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. TEST REVIEW Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6(3):235–238.

28. Polderman TJC, Derks EM, Hudziak JJ, Verhulst FC, Posthuma D, Boomsma DI. Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(11):1080–7. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01783.x.

29. Bruni O, Ottaviano S, Guidetti V, et al. The Sleep Disturbance Scale for Children (SDSC). Construction and validation of an instrument to evaluate sleep disturbances in childhood and adolescence. J Sleep Res. 1996;5(4):251–61.

30. Elashoff JD. nQuery Advisor Version 7.0 User’s Guide. Los Angeles, CA; 2007.31. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics. 6th editio. Boston MA: Pearson; 2013.

doi:10.1037/022267.32. de Ruiter MA, Schouten-Van Meeteren AYN, Van Mourik R, et al. Neurofeedback to improve

neurocognitive functioning of children treated for a brain tumor: design of a randomized controlled double-blind trial. BMC Cancer. 2012;12:581.

33. Perreau-Linck E, Lessard N, Levesque J, Beauregard M. Effects of neurofeed back training on inhibitory capacities in ADHD children: A single-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study. J Neurother. 2010;14:229–242. doi:10.1080/10874208.2010.501514.

34. Arnold LE, Lofthouse N, Hersch S, et al. EEG neurofeedback for ADHD: double-blind sham-controlled randomized pilot feasibility trial. J Atten Disord. 2013;17(5):410–419. doi:10.1177/1087054712446173.

Page 149: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

147

7

35. Van Dongen-Boomsma M, Vollebregt MA, Slaats-Willemse D, Buitelaar JK. A randomized placebo-controlled trial of electroencephalographic (EEG) neurofeedback in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2013;74(8):821–827. doi:10.4088/JCP.12m08321.

36. Lansbergen MM, van Dongen-Boomsma M, Buitelaar JK, Slaats-Willemse D. ADHD and EEG-neurofeedback: a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled feasibility study. J Neural Transm. 2011;118(2):275–284. doi:10.1007/s00702-010-0524-2.

37. Palmer SL. Neurodevelopmental impact on children treated for medulloblastoma: A review and proposed conceptual model. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2008;14(3):203–210. doi:10.1002/ddrr.32.

38. Arns M, Ridder S De, Strehl U, Breteler MHM, Coenen A. Efficacy of Neurofeedback Treatment in ADHD: the Effects on Inattention, Impulsivity and Hyperactivity: a Meta-Analysis. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2009;40(3):180–189.

39. Ghaziri J, Tucholka A, Larue V, et al. Neurofeedback training induces changes in white and gray matter. Clin EEG Neurosci. 2013;44(4):265–72. doi:10.1177/1550059413476031.

40. Simkin DR, Thatcher RW, Lubar J. Quantitative EEG and neurofeedback in children and adolescents: anxiety disorders, depressive disorders, comorbid addiction and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and brain injury. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 2014;23(3):427–64. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2014.03.001.

41. Arns M, Heinrich H, Strehl U. Evaluation of neurofeedback in ADHD: the long and winding road. Biol Psychol. 2014;95:108–15. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.013.

42. Vollebregt MA, van Dongen-Boomsma M, Slaats-Willemse D, Buitelaar JK. What future research should bring to help resolving the debate about the efficacy of EEG-neurofeedback in children with ADHD. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8(May):321. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00321.

43. de Vries M, Prins PJM, Schmand B a., Geurts HM. Working memory and cognitive flexibility-training for children with an autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(5):566–576. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12324.

44. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.; 2013.

45. Black DS, Milam J, Sussman S. Sitting-meditation interventions among youth: a review of treatment efficacy. Pediatrics. 2009;124(3):e532–41. doi:10.1542/peds.2008-3434.

46. Ris MD, Walsh K, Wallace D, et al. Intellectual and Academic Outcome Following Two Chemotherapy Regimens and Radiotherapy for Average-Risk Medulloblastoma: COG A9961. Pediatr Blood Cancer. 2013;60:1350–1357. doi:10.1002/pbc.

47. de Ruiter MA, Grootenhuis MA, van Mourik R, et al. Timed performance weaknesses on computerized tasks in pediatric brain tumor survivors: A comparison with sibling controls. Child Neuropsychol. 2015;53(9):1689–99. doi:10.1080/09297049.2015.1108395.

Page 150: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

148

APPENDIX CHAPTER 7 – PRIMARY AND SECONDARY OUTCOME MEASURES

Primary outcome measuresAttention. The Attention Network Task is a computerized task that is designed to measure functioning of three independent neural networks that enable alerting, orienting, and executive attention. Reliability and validity has been established.1,2 In this study, we used a child-friendly adaptation of the Attention Network Task.3

In the Attention Network Task, children are required to push as quickly and accurately as possible one of two buttons corresponding to the location of a target stimulus presented at either the left or right side of a computer screen. There are four trial types: (1) neutral trials with a neutral cue in the center of the screen preceding the target stimulus; (2) alerting trials, with no cue provided before onset of the target stimulus; (3) orienting trials with a central cue pointing to the location of the subsequently presented target stimulus; and (4) executive trials in which the central cue points to the location opposite of the subsequently presented target stimulus (causing slower reaction times, due to the necessary shift in attention). The task consists of 312 trials, randomly presenting the four trial types. The efficiency of the alerting, orienting, and executive attention networks was assessed by contrasting the mean reaction times of correct responses on the alerting, orienting and executive trials respectively, to the mean reaction time of the correct responses to the neutral trials. In addition, the intraindividual coefficient of variation across all trials was used as measure of maintenance of attention. Intraindividual coefficient of variation is defined as standard deviation of reaction times on correct responses, divided by the mean reaction time of correct responses.4

Processing speed. Processing speed was measured by the mean reaction time on correct responses on the neutral Attention Network Task trials.3

Memory: visual short-term memory. A computerized version of a visual sequencing task was used to measure visual short-term memory. Psychometric properties have been reported in several studies.3,5 During a trial, a sequence of yellow circles appears one by one in a 4x4-grid. The participant is required to repeat the sequence by tapping the correct locations on a touch screen. Difficulty level is manipulated by increasing the number of circles to be repeated (2-9 circles), with each difficulty level comprising two trials. If a participant makes an error on both trials of a difficulty level, the task is terminated. The total number of correct trials was used as the outcome variable.

Page 151: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

149

7

Memory: auditory short-term memory. Auditory short-term memory was measured by forward items of the Digit Span task of the age appropriate Wechsler Intelligence scale (Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, WISC-III, or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, WAIS-III).6,7 In this task, participants are required to repeat verbally presented sequences of digits, increasing in length (2-9 digits). The task is terminated after an error on both items of a difficulty level. The number correct forward items (i.e. the raw score associated with Digit Span forward) on the Digit Span task was used as the outcome variable.

Executive functioning: working memory. The backward items on the Digit Span task of the age appropriate Wechsler Intelligence scale were used as a measure of working memory.6,7 During the backward items, the participants are asked to repeat verbally presented sequences of digits, increasing in length (2-9 digits), in the reverse order, requiring simultaneous and storage and processing of information.8 The task is terminated after an error on both items of a difficulty level. The total number of correct items (i.e. the raw score associated with Digit Span backward) was used as the outcome variable, with the number of correct forward items as covariate.

Executive functioning: response inhibition. The Stop Signal task was used to assess response inhibition. Reliability and validity have been established in several studies.9 The task presents participants with two kinds of trials; go trials and stop trials. The participant is required to press as quickly and accurately as possible one of two buttons corresponding to the location of a target stimulus presented either at the left or right side of a computer screen. On stop trials, the stimulus is followed by a stop signal, a cross superimposed on the stimulus, requiring participants to inhibit the response. The time between stimulus and stop signal (stop signal delay) is varied to accomplish on average 50% successful inhibition on stop trials. Stop signal reaction time (SSRT), measuring the speed of the inhibitory response, was calculated by the mean reaction time on correct go trials minus mean stop signal delay, and served as the outcome variable.

VMI. We used a computerized tracking and pursuit task to measure VMI,10 which had been adapted and extended for use with children.11 Psychometric properties have been detailed in de Sonneville (1999).12 The task comprises two parts, a structured and an unstructured part. In both parts, participants are required to follow the trail of a moving caterpillar with a computer pencil in their preferred hand. During the structured part, the caterpillar moves in a fixed circular way over the screen. In the unstructured part of the task, the caterpillar moves in random directions over the screen. Both parts consist of six trials of 30 seconds each, with increasing speed levels (40-140 mm/s). For each trial, accuracy was calculated as the mean distance (in mm) between the position of the pencil and the caterpillar. The

Page 152: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

150

accuracy scores on the structured and unstructured part of the six different speed levels were used as measures of VMI.

Intelligence. Estimated Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ), Verbal IQ (VIQ), and Performance IQ (PIQ) were was estimated using a four-subtask short form of the age appropriate WISC-III or WAIS-III6,7 containing the subtests Arithmetic, Similarities, Block Design and Picture Completion (see Sattler).13 Satisfactory reliability (Cronbach’s Alphas>.80) and validity (rs with full test >.77) have been reported for this short form for both the WISC-III and the WAIS-III.13

Secondary outcome measuresSelf-reportHRQOL. The Kidscreen-27 was administered to evaluate HRQOL in children by means of 27 items, scored on a 5-point Likert-scale, divided over 5 dimensions: Physical Well-being, Psychological Well-being, Autonomy and Parents, Peers and Social Support, and School Environment.14 In addition, a Generic score was calculated by summing the ten items that comprise the Kidscreen-10, a shorter version of the Kidscreen, derived from the Kidscreen-27.15 Raw scores for each dimension were transformed into T-values with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10 in a European sample. T-values of a Dutch normative sample are available. Higher scores indicated better HRQOL. The Kidscreen-27 and Kidscreen-10 have good levels of validity and reliability (Chronbach’s alphas normative samples >0.70; Cronbach’s alphas PBTS 0.71-0.88).14,15 The Dutch normative sample did not differ in age and gender distribution from the PBTS group (p>0.10).

Self-esteem. We used the Self Perception Profile for Children (SPPC, age 8-12) and Adolescents (SPPA, age 12-18) to investigate self-esteem.16–18 The SPPC consists of 36 items, divided into six scales: Scholastic Competence, Social Acceptance, Athletic Competence, Physical Appearance, Behavioral Conduct and Global Self-worth. The adolescent version (SPPA) contains 35 items and comprises one additional scale: Close Friendship. Each item was presented on a 4-point Likert-scale, with higher scores indicating stronger self-esteem. The SPPC and SPPA have acceptable to good validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alphas Dutch manual >0.70; Cronbach’s alphas PBTS 0.62-0.91).17,18 The manual provided mean scores for males and females separately. For comparison with the total group of PBTS, scores of males and females in the normative population were combined after weighting by the gender distribution in the PBTS group. Age was not taken into account, as the SPPC and SPPA have separate norms, based on age.

Page 153: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

151

7

Psychosocial adjustment. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) was used to assess psychosocial adjustment.19,20 The items were scored on a 3-point Likert-scale. A Total Problem-score was calculated by adding the scores of 20 items, with a higher score indicating more problems. The SDQ Total Problem-score has good validity and reliability (Cronbach’s alphas Dutch controls =0.70; Cronbach’s alphas PBTS =0.77).20 Dutch norms were available for children aged 11-16 years; therefore analyses on the SDQ were restricted to PBTS aged 11-16 years. The gender distribution did not differ between the Dutch normative group and the PBTS, but mean age of the normative population was lower than of the PBTS. However, since Total Problem-score is not affected by age, age was not taken into account in the analysis.

Fatigue. Fatigue was measured with the Checklist Individual Strength (CIS).21 The CIS is a questionnaire that measures fatigue-related problems and consists of 20 items, scored on a 7-point Likert-scale. The four subscales were Subjective Fatigue, Concentration, Motivation and Physical Activity. A Total Score was calculated by summing up all items. Higher scores indicated more fatigue-related problems. The CIS has good reliability, with Cronbach’s alphas of the sibling controls and the PBTS 0.72-0.94. The data collected in the sibling control group were used for comparison with the PBTS. The sibling control group and the PBTS did not differ significantly (p>0.10) in gender and age.

Parent-reportPsychosocial adjustment. The SDQ was used to measure the parental perspective of PBTS’ psychosocial adjustment; see “Self-report” for the description of the questionnaire. Reliability of the Total Problem-score is good (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch controls =0.70; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS=0.77).20 PBTS were compared to a Dutch normative sample of children aged 8-16. The Dutch normative sample did not differ in age and gender from the PBTS (p>0.10).

Executive functioning. Parents rated their child’s behavioral executive functioning using the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF).22 The BRIEF contains 75 items, scored on a 3-point Likert-scale. The scores were summarized in 8 scales (Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Organization of Materials, and Monitor), two indices (behavioral and metacognition index), and a Total Score. The raw scores of the scales and indices were transformed into age and gender specific standardized T-scores, as provided in the manual, with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Higher scores indicated more problems. Validity and reliability range from good to excellent, with Cronbach’s alphas reported in the manual between 0.78 and 0.96 and Cronbach’s alphas of the PBTS between 0.66 and 0.94.23

Page 154: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

152

Teacher-reportPsychosocial adjustment. The SDQ was used to measure the teacher perspective of psychosocial adjustment of the PBTS; see “Self-report” for the description of the questionnaire. The reliability of the Total Problem-score of the teacher-report is reported to be good (Cronbach’s alpha Dutch controls=0.88; Cronbach’s alpha PBTS=0.77).20 A Dutch normative population of children aged 8-11 was available, therefore the answers from teachers of PBTS within that age range were analyzed. The Total Problem-score was analysed separately for females and males, because the PBTS sample had more females than the normative population. The mean age of the Dutch normative population did not differ from the mean age of the PBTS aged 8-11.

Executive functioning. The BRIEF teacher-report version measures executive functioning of PBTS in the school situation; see “Parent-report” for the description of the BRIEF. Validity and reliability are good to excellent, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from 0.88 to 0.98 as reported in the manual and between 0.82 and 0.97 of the PBTS.23

Page 155: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Neurofeedback in PBTS: Results RCT

153

7

REFERENCES

1. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI. The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage. 2005;26(2):471–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004.

2. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI, Petersen SE. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(3):340–7. doi:10.1162/089892902317361886.

3. de Kieviet JF, van Elburg RM, Lafeber HN, Oosterlaan J. Attention problems of very preterm children compared with age-matched term controls at school-age. J Pediatr. 2012;161(5):824–9. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.05.010.

4. de Zeeuw P, Aarnoudse-Moens CS, Bijlhout J, et al. Inhibitory performance, response speed, intraindividual variability, and response accuracy in ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(7):808–16. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e318172eee9.

5. Nutley SB, Söderqvist S, Bryde S, Humphreys K, Klingberg T. Measuring working memory capacity with greater precision in the lower capacity ranges. Dev Neuropsychol. 2010;35(1):81–95. doi:10.1080/87565640903325741.

6. Wechsler D. Wechsler’s Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd Edition. London: Harcourt Brace and Company; 1992.

7. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III). San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 2005.

8. Miyake A. Individual differences in working memory: introduction to the special section. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001;130(2):163–8.

9. Kindlon D, Mezzacappa E, Earls F. Psychometric Properties of Impulsivity Measures: Temporal Stability, Validity and Factor Structure. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1995;36(4):645–61. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb02319.x.

10. Wolpert DM. Computational approaches control. Trends Cogn Sci. 1997;1(6):209–216.11. De Kieviet JF, Stoof CJJ, Geldof CJ a, et al. The crucial role of the predictability of motor

response in visuomotor deficits in very preterm children at school age. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(7):624–30. doi:10.1111/dmcn.12125.

12. De Sonneville LM. Amsterdam Neuropsychological Task: A computer-aided assessment program. In: Brinker BPLM Den, Beek PJ, Brand AN, Maarse SJ, Mulder LJM, eds. Cognitive Ergonomics, Clinical Assessment and Computer-assisted Learning (Computers in Psychology, 6). 6th ed. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1999:204–217.

13. Sattler JM. Assessment of children. 3rd ed. San Diego, CA: Jerome M, Sattler Publisher, Inc.; 1992.

14. Ravens-Sieberer U, Auquier P, Erhart M, et al. The KIDSCREEN-27 quality of life measure for children and adolescents: psychometric results from a cross-cultural survey in 13 European countries. Qual life Res. 2007;16(8):1347–56. doi:10.1007/s11136-007-9240-2.

15. Ravens-Sieberer U, Erhart M, Rajmil L, et al. Reliability, construct and criterion validity of the KIDSCREEN-10 score: a short measure for children and adolescents’ well-being and health-related quality of life. Qual Life Res. 2010;19(10):1487–500. doi:10.1007/s11136-010-9706-5.

16. Harter S. Social-Perception Profile for Children. Denver, CO: University of Denver; 1985.17. Veerman J, Straathof M, Treffers P, van den Bergh B, ten Brink L. Handleiding bij de Competentie

Belevingsschaal voor Kinderen, CBSK (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Children – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 1997.

18. Treffers P, Goedhart A, Veerman J, van den Bergh B, Ackaert L, de Rycke L. Handleiding bij de Competentie Belevingsschaal voor Adolescenten, CBSA (Manual of the Self Perception Profile for Adolescents – Dutch version). Amsterdam, Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger; 2002.

Page 156: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 7

154

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

19. Goodman R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–6.

20. van Widenfelt BM, Goedhart AW, Treffers PD a, Goodman R. Dutch version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2003;12(6):281–9. doi:10.1007/s00787-003-0341-3.

21. Stulemeijer M, de Jong LWAM, Fiselier TJW, Hoogveld SWB, Bleijenberg G. Cognitive behaviour therapy for adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome: randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 2005;330(7481):14. doi:10.1136/bmj.38301.587106.63.

22. Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Guy SC, Kenworthy L. TEST REVIEW Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function. Child Neuropsychol. 2000;6(3):235–238.

23. Smidts DP, Huizinga M. BRIEF executieve functies gedragsvragenlijst: Handleiding. Amsterdam: Hogrefe Uitgevers; 2009.

Page 157: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

8

General Discussion

Page 158: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 8

156

Page 159: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General Discussion

157

8

AIMS

This thesis describes the PRISMA study; investigating neurocognitive and psychosocial late effects after a pediatric brain tumor and the efficacy of neurofeedback (NF) training as a possible intervention. In this final chapter, the results will be summarized and discussed. Furthermore, limitations of the study, clinical implications and suggestions for future research will be described.

MAIN FINDINGS

For an overview of the studies in this thesis, please see Table 1.

Neurocognitive late effectsThis thesis clearly shows that many PBTS suffer from substantial neurocognitive late effects on many domains. First, we conducted a meta-analysis, described in Chapter 2, to determine the extent of the neurocognitive problems. We summarized results of 22 studies concerning intelligence and 7 studies on attention of pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTS). The results of the meta-analysis show that the PBTS had significantly worse scores on IQ measures and worse attention as compared to normative data. Participants who were treated with radiotherapy and/or chemotherapy had lower intelligence scores than participants who had been treated with surgery only. Also, PBTS with a longer interval between diagnosis and treatment had lower intelligence scores than those with a shorter interval.In Chapter 4, the results of pre-intervention neurocognitive functioning of a sample of PBTS with neurocognitive complaints are presented. A total of 82 PBTS and 43 sibling controls were assessed. Neurocognitive functioning was studied using a broad battery of computerized tests. PBTS showed problems in various domains: slower processing speed, lower intelligence, and more deficits in attention, short-term memory, executive functions, and visuomotor integration compared to the sibling control group.

Page 160: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 8

158

Tabl

e 1.

Mai

n fin

ding

s de

scri

bed

in th

is th

esis

Chap

ter

Shor

t titl

eSa

mpl

eM

etho

dsO

utco

me

mea

sure

sTi

me

poin

tM

ain

findi

ngs

2M

eta-

anal

ysis

of

inte

llige

nce

and

atten

tion

in P

BTS

Inte

llige

nce:

22

stud

ies

on 7

10

PBTS

Atten

tion:

7

stud

ies

on 3

72

PBTS

Aver

age

age

6-16

ye

ar

Scor

es P

BTS

vs

norm

ative

dat

aIn

telli

genc

e: W

ISC-

IIIAtt

entio

n: C

PT, C

PT II

n/a

PBTS

sho

w m

ediu

m to

larg

e de

pres

sion

s in

IQ.

PBTS

sho

w la

rge-

size

d in

crea

ses

in c

omm

issi

on

erro

rs, i

ndic

ating

inatt

entiv

enes

s.Ra

diot

hera

py, c

hem

othe

rapy

, and

long

er ti

me

sinc

e di

agno

sis

are

rela

ted

to w

orse

inte

llect

ual o

utco

me.

4N

euro

cogn

itive

fu

nctio

ning

in

PBTS

PBTS

n=8

2, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

3.85

(S

D=3

.15)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=4

0/42

Sibl

ing

cont

rol

grou

p n=

43, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

4.27

(S

D=2

.44)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=1

7/26

T0 s

core

s PB

TS

vs s

core

s si

blin

g co

ntro

l gro

up

Atten

tion

(AN

T)Pr

oces

sing

spe

ed (A

NT)

Visu

al m

emor

y (v

isua

l seq

uenc

ing

task

)A

udito

ry m

emor

y (D

igit

Span

forw

ard

WIS

C/W

AIS

) W

orki

ng m

emor

y (D

igit

Span

bac

kwar

d W

ISC/

WA

IS)

Resp

onse

inhi

bitio

n (S

top

sign

al ta

sk)

Visu

omot

or in

tegr

ation

(Tra

ckin

g an

d Pu

rsui

t ta

sk)

Inte

llige

nce

(abb

revi

ated

WIS

C-III

/WA

IS-II

I)

T0PB

TS h

ave

prob

lem

s of

med

ium

to la

rge

effec

t siz

e on

the

who

le ra

nge

of e

xam

ined

neu

roco

gniti

ve

func

tions

. O

lder

PBT

S at

ass

essm

ent p

erfo

rmed

bett

er o

n th

e ta

sks

than

you

nger

PBT

S di

d.N

one

of th

e m

edic

al r

isk

fact

ors

wer

e pr

edic

tive

of

neur

ocog

nitiv

e fu

nctio

ning

.PB

TS w

ho w

ere

youn

ger

whe

n th

ey w

ere

diag

nose

d ha

d lo

wer

esti

mat

ed IQ

-sco

res

than

PBT

S w

ho w

ere

olde

r at

dia

gnos

is.

5Ps

ycho

soci

al

func

tioni

ng in

PB

TS

PBTS

n=8

2, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

3.85

(S

D=3

.15)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=4

0/42

Sibl

ing

cont

rol

grou

p n=

43, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

4.27

(S

D=2

.44)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=1

7/26

T0 s

core

s PB

TS

vs s

core

s si

blin

g co

ntro

l gro

up o

r no

rmati

ve d

ata

HRQ

OL

(Kid

scre

en-2

7, s

elf-

repo

rt)

Soci

al-e

moti

onal

func

tioni

ng (S

DQ

sel

f-,

pare

nt- a

nd te

ache

r-re

port

)Se

lf-es

teem

(SPP

C/SP

PA, s

elf-

repo

rt)

Fatig

ue (C

IS, s

elf-

repo

rt)

Exec

utive

func

tioni

ng (B

RIEF

-Par

ent a

nd B

RIEF

-Te

ache

r)Att

entio

n da

ily li

fe (S

WA

N, p

aren

t-re

port

)Sl

eep

dist

urba

nce

(SD

SC, p

aren

t-re

port

)

T0PB

TS s

how

ed d

ecre

ased

psy

chos

ocia

l fun

ction

ing

on

HRQ

OL,

soc

ial-e

moti

onal

func

tioni

ng, f

atigu

e, a

nd

exec

utive

func

tioni

ng.

Des

pite

the

neur

ocog

nitiv

e co

mpl

aint

s, P

BTS

func

tione

d w

ithin

nor

mal

rang

es in

man

y ps

ycho

soci

al d

omai

ns o

r sh

owed

onl

y tr

ends

tow

ards

w

orse

func

tioni

ng.

Phys

ical

sub

scal

es p

sych

osoc

ial f

uncti

onin

g w

ere

spec

ifica

lly c

ompr

omis

ed in

PBT

S.PB

TS b

ehav

iora

l con

duct

sco

res,

a s

ubsc

ale

of s

elf-

este

em, w

ere

high

er th

an in

the

norm

ative

sam

ple.

PBTS

did

not

repo

rt s

ocia

l-em

otion

al p

robl

ems,

w

here

as p

aren

ts a

nd te

ache

rs d

id.

Teac

hers

repo

rted

no

exec

utive

pro

blem

s in

PBT

S,

whe

reas

par

ents

did

.

Page 161: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General Discussion

159

8

Tabl

e 1.

Mai

n fin

ding

s de

scri

bed

in th

is th

esis

Chap

ter

Shor

t titl

eSa

mpl

eM

etho

dsO

utco

me

mea

sure

sTi

me

poin

tM

ain

findi

ngs

2M

eta-

anal

ysis

of

inte

llige

nce

and

atten

tion

in P

BTS

Inte

llige

nce:

22

stud

ies

on 7

10

PBTS

Atten

tion:

7

stud

ies

on 3

72

PBTS

Aver

age

age

6-16

ye

ar

Scor

es P

BTS

vs

norm

ative

dat

aIn

telli

genc

e: W

ISC-

IIIAtt

entio

n: C

PT, C

PT II

n/a

PBTS

sho

w m

ediu

m to

larg

e de

pres

sion

s in

IQ.

PBTS

sho

w la

rge-

size

d in

crea

ses

in c

omm

issi

on

erro

rs, i

ndic

ating

inatt

entiv

enes

s.Ra

diot

hera

py, c

hem

othe

rapy

, and

long

er ti

me

sinc

e di

agno

sis

are

rela

ted

to w

orse

inte

llect

ual o

utco

me.

4N

euro

cogn

itive

fu

nctio

ning

in

PBTS

PBTS

n=8

2, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

3.85

(S

D=3

.15)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=4

0/42

Sibl

ing

cont

rol

grou

p n=

43, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

4.27

(S

D=2

.44)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=1

7/26

T0 s

core

s PB

TS

vs s

core

s si

blin

g co

ntro

l gro

up

Atten

tion

(AN

T)Pr

oces

sing

spe

ed (A

NT)

Visu

al m

emor

y (v

isua

l seq

uenc

ing

task

)A

udito

ry m

emor

y (D

igit

Span

forw

ard

WIS

C/W

AIS

) W

orki

ng m

emor

y (D

igit

Span

bac

kwar

d W

ISC/

WA

IS)

Resp

onse

inhi

bitio

n (S

top

sign

al ta

sk)

Visu

omot

or in

tegr

ation

(Tra

ckin

g an

d Pu

rsui

t ta

sk)

Inte

llige

nce

(abb

revi

ated

WIS

C-III

/WA

IS-II

I)

T0PB

TS h

ave

prob

lem

s of

med

ium

to la

rge

effec

t siz

e on

the

who

le ra

nge

of e

xam

ined

neu

roco

gniti

ve

func

tions

. O

lder

PBT

S at

ass

essm

ent p

erfo

rmed

bett

er o

n th

e ta

sks

than

you

nger

PBT

S di

d.N

one

of th

e m

edic

al r

isk

fact

ors

wer

e pr

edic

tive

of

neur

ocog

nitiv

e fu

nctio

ning

.PB

TS w

ho w

ere

youn

ger

whe

n th

ey w

ere

diag

nose

d ha

d lo

wer

esti

mat

ed IQ

-sco

res

than

PBT

S w

ho w

ere

olde

r at

dia

gnos

is.

5Ps

ycho

soci

al

func

tioni

ng in

PB

TS

PBTS

n=8

2, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

3.85

(S

D=3

.15)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=4

0/42

Sibl

ing

cont

rol

grou

p n=

43, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

4.27

(S

D=2

.44)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=1

7/26

T0 s

core

s PB

TS

vs s

core

s si

blin

g co

ntro

l gro

up o

r no

rmati

ve d

ata

HRQ

OL

(Kid

scre

en-2

7, s

elf-

repo

rt)

Soci

al-e

moti

onal

func

tioni

ng (S

DQ

sel

f-,

pare

nt- a

nd te

ache

r-re

port

)Se

lf-es

teem

(SPP

C/SP

PA, s

elf-

repo

rt)

Fatig

ue (C

IS, s

elf-

repo

rt)

Exec

utive

func

tioni

ng (B

RIEF

-Par

ent a

nd B

RIEF

-Te

ache

r)Att

entio

n da

ily li

fe (S

WA

N, p

aren

t-re

port

)Sl

eep

dist

urba

nce

(SD

SC, p

aren

t-re

port

)

T0PB

TS s

how

ed d

ecre

ased

psy

chos

ocia

l fun

ction

ing

on

HRQ

OL,

soc

ial-e

moti

onal

func

tioni

ng, f

atigu

e, a

nd

exec

utive

func

tioni

ng.

Des

pite

the

neur

ocog

nitiv

e co

mpl

aint

s, P

BTS

func

tione

d w

ithin

nor

mal

rang

es in

man

y ps

ycho

soci

al d

omai

ns o

r sh

owed

onl

y tr

ends

tow

ards

w

orse

func

tioni

ng.

Phys

ical

sub

scal

es p

sych

osoc

ial f

uncti

onin

g w

ere

spec

ifica

lly c

ompr

omis

ed in

PBT

S.PB

TS b

ehav

iora

l con

duct

sco

res,

a s

ubsc

ale

of s

elf-

este

em, w

ere

high

er th

an in

the

norm

ative

sam

ple.

PBTS

did

not

repo

rt s

ocia

l-em

otion

al p

robl

ems,

w

here

as p

aren

ts a

nd te

ache

rs d

id.

Teac

hers

repo

rted

no

exec

utive

pro

blem

s in

PBT

S,

whe

reas

par

ents

did

.

6A

ssoc

iatio

n BR

IEF

with

ne

uroc

ogni

tive

task

s

PBTS

n=8

2, a

ge

in y

ears

M=1

3.85

(S

D=3

.15)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=4

0/42

T0 s

core

s PB

TS, B

RIEF

qu

estio

nnai

re v

s ne

uroc

ogni

tive

task

s

Inatt

entio

n (IC

V A

NT)

Wor

king

mem

ory

(Dig

it Sp

an W

ISC/

WA

IS)

Resp

onse

inhi

bitio

n (S

top

sign

al ta

sk)

Visu

al m

emor

y (v

isua

l seq

uenc

ing

task

)Ex

ecuti

ve a

tten

tion

(exe

cutiv

e ne

twor

k A

NT)

BRIE

F-Pa

rent

and

BRI

EF-T

each

er

T0N

o si

gnifi

cant

ass

ocia

tions

wer

e fo

und

betw

een

the

BRIE

F-Pa

rent

and

the

neur

ocog

nitiv

e ta

sks.

We

foun

d a

sign

ifica

nt re

latio

n be

twee

n th

e BR

IEF-

Teac

her

Wor

king

Mem

ory

scor

es, i

ndex

-sco

res

and

Tota

l sc

ores

with

neu

roco

gniti

ve ta

sks.

No

sign

ifica

nt c

orre

latio

ns w

ere

foun

d be

twee

n BR

IEF-

Pare

nt a

nd B

RIEF

-Tea

cher

7Ra

ndom

ized

co

ntro

lled

tria

l on

neur

ofee

dbac

k in

PBT

S

PBTS

n=7

1

NF:

n=3

4, a

ge in

ye

ars

M=1

4.45

(S

D=2

.99)

, mal

es/

fem

ales

n=1

6/18

PF: n

=37,

age

in

year

s M

=13.

45

(SD

=3.2

8), m

ales

/fe

mal

es n

=19/

18

T1 a

nd T

2 da

ta

of P

BTS

rece

ivin

g ne

urof

eedb

ack

trai

ning

vs

PBTS

re

ceiv

ing

plac

ebo

feed

back

tr

aini

ng,

cont

rolli

ng fo

r T0

sc

ores

Neu

roco

gniti

ve fu

nctio

ning

(see

Cha

pter

4)

Psyc

hoso

cial

func

tioni

ng (s

ee C

hapt

er 5

)T0

, T1,

T2

Neu

rofe

edba

ck tr

aini

ng d

id n

ot h

ave

a fa

vora

ble

effec

t on

neur

ocog

nitiv

e fu

nctio

ning

in P

BTS

as

com

pare

d w

ith p

lace

bo-f

eedb

ack

trai

ning

. N

euro

feed

back

trai

ning

did

not

hav

e a

favo

rabl

e eff

ect o

n ps

ycho

soci

al fu

nctio

ning

in P

BTS

as

com

pare

d w

ith p

lace

bo-f

eedb

ack

trai

ning

. N

euro

cogn

itive

and

psy

chos

ocia

l fun

ction

ing

of P

BTS

in b

oth

trai

ning

gro

ups

wer

e fo

und

to h

ave

impr

oved

ov

er ti

me,

but

no

diffe

renc

e be

twee

n th

e gr

oups

was

ob

serv

ed.

Effec

tive

inte

rven

tions

aim

ed a

t im

prov

ing

neur

ocog

nitiv

e an

d ps

ycho

soci

al fu

nctio

ning

of P

BTS

rem

ain

to b

e de

velo

ped.

Not

e. T

he f

ollo

win

g su

btas

ks o

f th

e W

ISC/

WA

IS w

ere

adm

inis

tere

d: A

rith

meti

c, S

imila

ritie

s, B

lock

Des

ign,

and

Pic

ture

Com

pleti

on.

AN

T=Att

entio

n N

etw

ork

Task

; BR

IEF=

Beha

vior

Rati

ng In

vent

ory

of E

xecu

tive

Func

tioni

ng; C

IS=C

heck

list I

ndiv

idua

l Str

engt

h; C

PT=C

onne

r’s

Conti

nuou

s Pe

rfor

man

ce T

est;

HRQ

OL=

Hea

lth R

elat

ed Q

ualit

y O

f Life

; M=M

ean;

ICV=

Intr

aind

ivid

ual c

oeffi

cien

t of v

aria

tion;

NF=

Neu

rofe

edba

ck tr

aini

ng; P

BTS=

Pedi

atri

c Br

ain

Tum

or S

urvi

vors

; PF=

Plac

bo fe

edba

ck tr

aini

ng; S

D=S

tand

ard

Dev

iatio

n; S

DQ

=Str

engt

hs a

nd D

ifficu

lties

Que

stion

naire

s; S

DSC

=Sle

ep D

istu

rban

ce S

cale

for

Chi

ldre

n; S

PPC/

SPPA

=Sel

f Pe

rcep

tion

Profi

le f

or C

hild

ren/

Ado

lesc

ents

; SW

AN

=Str

engt

hs a

nd W

eakn

esse

s of

AD

HD

-sym

ptom

s an

d N

orm

al-b

ehav

ior;

T0=

Pre-

inte

rven

tion

asse

ssm

ent;

T1=

Post

-inte

rven

tion

asse

ssm

ent;

T2=

6 M

onth

s po

st-

inte

rven

tion

asse

ssm

ent;

WIS

C-III

= W

echs

ler

Inte

llige

nce

Scal

e fo

r Ch

ildre

n –

Third

ver

sion

; WA

IS-II

I = W

echs

ler

Adu

lt In

telli

genc

e Sc

ale

– Th

ird v

ersi

on.

Page 162: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 8

160

Findings of our study seem to be in line with the hierarchical conceptual model of neurocognitive functions, that has been suggested by Palmer and colleagues.1 In this model, processing speed is a function that underlies other neurocognitive functions, and via attention and working memory, also underlies intelligence and academic achievement. In our study, slower processing speed appeared to be an underlying factor for many of the neurocognitive late effects, as scores of PBTS were more depressed on timed tasks as compared with untimed tasks. The low scores on intelligence we found in our sample of PBTS might in part, be a result of slower processing speed. Lower intelligence is a strong predictor of school achievement and vocational success in the general population, as has also been shown in PBTS.2–5 Therefore, lower processing speed and intelligence scores may have long lasting and far-reaching consequences for PBTS. Predictors that may influence neurocognitive functioning of PBTS were also examined. Older age at assessment was associated with better neurocognitive functioning and younger age at diagnosis with lower intelligence. Medical risk factors, such as hydrocephalus or types of treatment, were not associated with worse neurocognitive functioning.

Executive functioningExecutive functions (EF) are called ‘higher order functions’ of the brain and comprise multi facetted neurocognitive functions, such as planning and working memory, and are mainly mediated by the frontal lobe. EF are important functions for daily living, especially in goal directed behavior.6 In PBTS, these functions are often depressed as a result of the tumor and/or the treatment.7 In our study, we included different ways to measure EF including performance based neurocognitive tasks and both parent- and teacher-rated EF using the BRIEF questionnaire (Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Functions).8 On the neurocognitive tasks, PBTS showed worse working memory and attention functioning as compared to the sibling control group. These findings might be related to the decrease in scholastic performance seen over time in PBTS, as these executive skills are crucial for learning.9 Regarding the BRIEF questionnaire (Chapter 5), there was a discrepancy between parent-report and teacher-report. Overall, parents reported more executive problems than the teachers did. In Chapter 6, we tested the association between the BRIEF-questionnaire and neurocognitive tasks, to identify whether the BRIEF might be suitable as a screening tool to detect children that are at risk for deficits. If the BRIEF would show significant correlations with neurocognitive tasks, this would increase the feasibility to screen all PBTS for neurocognitive problems more frequently, since screening costs and time spent on testing are reduced dramatically.

Page 163: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General Discussion

161

8

There were no significant correlations between the BRIEF-Parent and the neurocognitive tasks, while the BRIEF-Teacher did reveal that inattention as measured by the ANT was correlated to the Monitor scale and working memory as measured by the Digit Span of the age appropriate Wechsler intelligence scale was related to the Working Memory scale. Also, there was no significant correlation between the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher, and the outcomes did not differ significantly either. From our data, it can be concluded that teacher-reported EF problems is a better predictor of neurocognitive problems in PBTS than parent-reported EF problems. However, we should be aware that teachers report on the school situation and thus might not pick up problems as perceived at home. Also, it is important for a more comprehensive assessment of possible problems to include multiple respondents, as they might complement each other. Therefore, we would advise to use a parent-report questionnaire in addition to the BRIEF-Teacher as a screening tool for neurocognitive functioning in PBTS.

Psychosocial functioningNeurological late effects in PBTS may contribute to lower psychosocial functioning, especially social competence may be affected as reported in a review paper of Fuemmeler.10 But the review by Fuemmeler and a review by Nelson et al. both conclude that the findings are mixed as to whether and how psychosocial functioning is affected.10,11 Chapter 5 reports the psychosocial functioning of the 82 PBTS at baseline, which was assessed using self-, parent- and/or teacher-report questionnaires. The PBTS suffered from more psychosocial problems, health related quality of life, fatigue, and behavioral executive functioning problems compared to a sibling control group (n = 43) or normative data, as reported by themselves, parents, and teachers. One of the main findings of our study on psychosocial functioning was the decreased subjective physical functioning. PBTS reported worse physical health related quality of life (HRQOL), a trend toward decreased athletic competence and decreased physical activity. PBTS often suffer from physical impairments, which may be related to lower physical self-esteem.12 Health care professionals working with PBTS should be aware of possible physical problems and accompanying self-esteem and HRQOL problems.

Multiple respondents’ dilemmaThe multidimensional approach we used, including multiple respondents, proved interesting also with respect to some discrepancies we found. The PBTS in our sample did not report psychosocial problems, but their parents and teachers did report such problems. Furthermore there was a discrepancy between teachers and parents regarding their reports on EF. Teachers reported no problems in PBTS, whereas parents did report problems on both the Behavioral index and the Metacognition index, as well as the Total Problems scale of the BRIEF questionnaire.

Page 164: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 8

162

Self-report, parent-report, and teacher-report scores might all be influenced by different factors. Parents’ perception of the child’s psychosocial functioning has been found to be influenced by their level of distress, whereas the child’s perception may be related to trait anxiety.13 Other possibilities are that childhood cancer survivors report less psychosocial problems than peers due to social desirability or positive coping.14 Age of the children could also play a role in the discrepancies in the scores of different respondents: the available normative data was not appropriate for our sample. The age-bands of the normative data differed for the different respondents.The difference in parent- and teacher-report results regarding executive functioning might also be due to ‘observation environment.’ Teachers observe PBTS in a classroom situation, which is often more structured than the home situation. Possibly, some of the problems which parents observe at home, might not be as striking in structured settings like school.15 This leads us to believe that PBTS might benefit from a structured environment. Parents and teachers also have a different frame of reference when perceiving the PBTS. Parents know the premorbid functioning or compare the child to healthy siblings, while teachers might be more prone to compare the child with peers. As many of our participants attended special education (24%), often classmates suffered from chronic conditions, which could also have affected teachers’ judgment of psychosocial functioning. The discrepancies between different respondents and the possible contributing factors illustrate the value of a multidimensional approach when investigating psychosocial functioning in PBTS.

InterventionThe neurocognitive deficits exhibited by PBTS call for an intervention to improve neurocognitive functioning. At present there are few effective interventions for PBTS.16 The interventions that have shown some efficacy in PBTS are a cognitive remediation program,17 a program that combines memory and attention exercises with cognitive behavioral training,18 a computerized working memory training, CogMed,19 and methylphenidate.20 NF training has shown promising effects in the past in other patient groups, e.g. children with ADHD,21 however, more recent studies did not show convincing effects of NF training in this group of patients.22,23

RCTThe results of the randomized controlled trial (RCT) on NF training in PBTS are discussed in Chapter 7. We examined the effects of NF training on neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning, by randomizing 80 participating PBTS to receive NF training (feedback based on the brain activity) or to receive placebo-feedback training (PF; random feedback, based on a built-in signal generator of the software), and comparing the performance of the two training groups regarding neurocognitive functioning as primary outcome and psychosocial functioning as secondary outcome.

Page 165: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General Discussion

163

8

The results reveal no positive effects for NF training over PF training on any of the primary and secondary outcomes. In both groups, PBTS improved over time on the majority of primary and some secondary outcome measures, with small to medium effects (effect sizes between d =.19 and d = .54). However, no differences between the two groups were found. This finding is in line with recent double-blind RCTs that did not find an effect of NF training as compared to PF training in children with ADHD.22–26 Based on characteristics of our design and the parallel of findings in randomized controlled trials with NF training in children with ADHD, we believe we can conclude the NF training is neither effective in improving neurocognitive functioning nor in improving psychosocial functioning in PBTS. But what could have caused these improvements observed over time, if not the NF training? Improvements over time have been reported previously in trials investigating NF training,23–26 and in other interventions.27 Non-specific treatment-effects, such as positive expectations, regression to the mean or even the minutes of rest during the training sessions, could all have caused the improvements.28 The time that passed between the assessments may also contribute due to maturation effects, although the improvements were also found on age-corrected scores. It may also be speculated that with the structured frequent sessions we have trained focused attention, increasing functioning overall on neurocognitive domains.Interestingly, attention did not improve over time, even though the NF training in present RCT focused on the beta 1 brain wave frequency, a frequency band that is strongly associated with attentional abilities. The fact that we did not find that NF training improved attention might also be related to the fact that the attention problems in our sample of PBTS were not pronounced enough. Perhaps there was not much room for improvement in attention functioning, or the selected task was not sensitive to the specific attention problems of the PBTS. Also the majority of the participants, parents, and teachers reported that they did not think the training had been effective. Surprisingly, the percentage of participants and teachers that did report a potential benefit of the training, was even slightly but not significantly higher in the PF group than in the NF group, further strengthening our believe that the NF training was not effective in our group of PBTS.

Key messages:- PBTS have more neurocognitive problems than the sibling control group- Even PBTS without traditional risk factors, such as cranial irradiation and young age

at diagnosis, may suffer from neurocognitive late effects- PBTS with neurocognitive complaints suffer from more psychosocial problems than

healthy controls, as reported by themselves, parents, and teachers. Specifically physical aspects seem to be affected, as reported by PBTS themselves.

- Parents report more EF problems in PBTS than teachers.

Page 166: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 8

164

- Teacher-reported EF of PBTS shows some correlations with neurocognitive tasks and parent-reported EF does not.

- NF training is not more effective in PBTS than PF training in improving neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning.

Limitations and strengthsThere are some limitations that should be taken into account when interpreting the results of this study. Regarding the sample, the results can not be generalized to PBTS without neurocognitive complaints, as study inclusion was based on the presence of parent-reported neurocognitive problems. Furthermore, after enrolment, parents reported larger (executive function) deficits than teachers did. Consequently, as inclusion was based on parent-report only, we might have included participants that had only little neurocognitive deficits as measured by standardized tests. The results in Chapter 4 of this thesis contradicts this though, as the PBTS in our sample had worse neurocognitive functioning as compared to the sibling control group. The participants bore more risk factors for neurocognitive problems than non-participants. The participants were on average younger at diagnosis, had longer time since diagnosis and had more often had chemotherapy and radiotherapy than the non-participants. This seemingly higher risk for neurocognitive deficits, suggests that a more severely impaired group of PBTS participated in our study. This was expected, as the participants were included based on parent-reported neurocognitive complaints.Regarding the intervention, increased effectiveness of the NF training might be seen when using newer NF approaches. Recent approaches include transfer sessions, in which the participant receives no feedback, while trying to continue to show the required brain activity. This is done to increase generalization from the training into daily life.29

Also, we used a 30 s epoch to adjust the threshold in the NF sessions, while a longer threshold of e.g. three minutes, or manually adjusted threshold might have led to a steeper learning curve than the PF training. Regarding the outcome measurements we chose to use mainly experimental tasks that isolate a single neurocognitive domain, but these tasks are not used in clinical practice or many other brain tumor studies. Thus, for comparability reasons, it would have been interesting to see how the participants performed on widely-used multifaceted tasks that are common in clinical practice, such as the CPT (Conners’ continuous performance task) for attention or the trail-making task, for executive functioning. The usage of experimental tasks made it more difficult to compare our results to other studies. Our study also has strengths. With the broad test battery that was used, we were able to assess a wide variety of possible neurocognitive and psychosocial problems. Also, the use of objective and subjective tests, as well as multiple informants, provided us with interesting

Page 167: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General Discussion

165

8

complementary information from different perspectives. The experimental tasks that were used had the advantage that they isolated the neurocognitive functions we were interested in, using built-in control conditions. Also, test reliability was increased by the use of computerized tests, versus traditional pencil-and-paper tasks. For the neurocognitive tasks and the fatigue questionnaire, a sibling control group was included. This had the advantage over a non-related control group or normative data that it allowed controlling for genetic factors and socio-economic background. The use of automatic thresholds for the NF training enabled us to conduct a double blind placebo controlled RCT preventing selection bias. Furthermore, using portable NF devices it was possible to train the participants at a time and location that was convenient for them. We believe this increased the commitment of the participants, reflected by the low attrition.

FUTURE RESEARCH AND CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

Education and information on late effects for health care providers, parents and teachers It is evident, from our study and other studies, that many PBTS suffer from neurocognitive and psychosocial late effects. This emphasizes the importance for parents and teachers of PBTS to be aware of the potential consequences of the tumor and the treatment. The Internet could be a good source of information; websites such as www.skion.nl (Dutch) or www.cancer.org (English) provide an overview of possible late effects of cancer treatment. Dutch parents might also www.vokk.nl a useful website, it provides all kinds of information for parents and families of children with cancer.Besides education on which late effects might occur after successful treatment for a pediatric brain tumor, parents and teachers of PBTS should also be instructed on how to support PBTS that are suffering from poor neurocognitive functioning. From our results, it appears that PBTS with neurocognitive problems might benefit from a structured environment. Another conclusion that could be drawn from our findings is that some PBTS might profit from extra time in the classroom to complete exams or tasks.Furthermore, it is essential that health care providers working with PBTS know about potential neurocognitive and psychosocial late effects. An important finding in our study on psychosocial functioning of PBTS, was a decrease on multiple physical aspects of psychosocial functioning, such as self-esteem and HRQOL. This could be related to physical late effects of the tumor or the treatment, such as weight gain, hair loss or scars. Health care providers should be aware of these potential problems. Psychosocial interventions might be supportive in increasing self-esteem and HRQOL.

Page 168: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 8

166

Regular screening in clinical practices to monitor neurocognitive functioningScreening can help identify possible problems. As a screening tool to assess neurocognitive functioning in PBTS, the BRIEF-Parent might not be the best option because this questionnaire did not show significant correlations with neurocognitive tasks. The BRIEF-Teacher could potentially be used as a screener, as it did show some significant correlations with PBTS neurocognitive functioning. However, in addition to the BRIEF-Teacher, parent reported questionnaires should be explored, as teachers might not be able to detect problems that manifest at home. Alternatives that could be explored include the parent-reported PedsPCF (Pediatric Perceived Cognitive Functioning) item-bank or the CCSS-NCQ (Childhood Cancer Survivors Study – Neurocognitive Questionnaire).30,31

By monitoring PBTS regularly, problems can be identified in time and the PBTS may be referred to receive appropriate care before problems escalate. Therefore, we would like to advocate using standardized monitoring systems to screen PBTS for problems, e.g. by using patient- and/or parent-reported outcomes (PROs). This could be done in clinical practices where PBTS are seen regularly. A fruitful way to conduct screening without increasing assessment time of the professional or burden for the patient is by using the KLIK system, an online system to monitor functioning of patients over time.39

Development of interventions to decrease late effectsIt is of utmost importance to develop and further study interventions to decrease neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences in PBTS. The treatment of neurocognitive deficits requires a specific program, targeting processing speed and working memory. With CogMed a good start has been made. CogMed is a working-memory training with promising results shown in recently published randomized controlled trial. A group of childhood cancer survivors was trained with CogMed and compared to a waiting list group.19 The results showed large improvements, with effect sizes of up to .84, in neurocognitive functioning after the training as compared to the wait list control group. The researchers did not see the purpose of adding an active control group, which limits the possibility to eliminate non-specific training effects. Furthermore, the costs for the training can be substantial. Besides working-memory training, mindfulness might be an optional intervention for PBTS to train attention. The improvements after the NF and PF training could have been due to the one-minute breaks during the training. During the training sessions, the participant was asked to sit still with eyes closed for one minute after every three minutes of training. This was similar in both training groups. These breaks may have given the training sessions and unintentional mindfulness-like character, which potentially contributed to the improvements in neurocognitive functioning. With TVs, computers, smartphones, and tablets with Internet available everywhere, people might take less time to sit quietly and be mindful in the

Page 169: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General Discussion

167

8

moment, reflecting their day. This lack of mindfulness in daily life might interfere with the processing of experiences and attention span. Mindfulness has shown preliminary favorable effects in children and adults with attention problems.32 Interventions to improve psychosocial functioning should also be pursued, given the psychosocial problems reported by the PBTS, their parents, and their teachers. Regarding the physical HRQOL problems reported by the PBTS, exercise based interventions should be explored, as suggested by previous studies.33,34 Physical exercise has been found to improve overall QOL, happiness, fitness and decrease depression in adult cancer survivors.35 Mindfulness or meditation in young adult cancer survivors has been explored in combination with an exercise program.36 This study showed improved physical activity, fitness and a trend towards improved mood in the young adult cancer survivors who received the mediation and exercise program as compared to a wait list control group.36 A recently updated Cochrane review summarized physical exercise related intervention studies (randomized and non-randomized controlled trials) for childhood cancer survivors.37 Small benefits for HRQOL were found, among other outcomes. However, the studies included small numbers of patients, and therefore the authors conclude that more studies are warranted to be conclusive.

CONCLUSION

In summary, the results of this study show that PBTS suffer from a wide range of considerable neurocognitive and psychosocial late effects. Results suggest that even PBTS that were traditionally viewed as having low risk for neurocognitive problems (e.g. surgery only, no prior hydrocephalus) may suffer from decreased neurocognitive functioning. The randomized controlled trial in this thesis convincingly shows that NF training does not have a favorable effect on neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning in PBTS above and beyond PF training. Interestingly, the NF as well as the PF training group improved regarding neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning, possibly due to non-specific training effects related to aspects of mindfulness. Continuous efforts are necessary to develop and test effective interventions for neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning of PBTS, as impairments are associated with decreased academic outcomes and vocational success. Some potentially useful programs are being developed and investigated, such as the working-memory training CogMed. Centers for pediatric neuro-oncology should incorporate monitoring of neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning. Conducting appropriate questionnaires via digital monitoring systems could increase feasibility. This will help professionals to identify those patients with specific deficits, who require advice for their home and schooling environment and to timely refer them when necessary.

Page 170: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 8

168

REFERENCES

1. Palmer SL. Neurodevelopmental impact on children treated for medulloblastoma: A review and proposed conceptual model. Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2008;14(3):203–210. doi:10.1002/ddrr.32.

2. de Boer AGEM, Verbeek JH a M, van Dijk FJH. Adult survivors of childhood cancer and unemployment: A metaanalysis. Cancer. 2006;107(1):1–11. doi:10.1002/cncr.21974.

3. Deary IJ, Strand S, Smith P, Fernandes C. Intelligence and educational achievement. Intelligence. 2007;35(1):13–21.

4. Reddick WE, White H a, Glass JO, et al. Developmental model relating white matter volume to neurocognitive deficits in pediatric brain tumor survivors. Cancer. 2003;97(10):2512–2519. doi:10.1002/cncr.11355.

5. Watkins MW, Lei P, Canivez GL. Psychometric intelligence and achievement: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Intelligence. 2007;35(1):59–68.

6. Toplak ME, West RF, Stanovich KE. Practitioner review: do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J child Psychol psychiatry. 2013;54(2):131–43. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12001.

7. Wolfe KR, Walsh KS, Reynolds NC, et al. Executive functions and social skills in survivors of pediatric brain tumor. Child Neuropsychol. 2013;19(4):370–84. doi:10.1080/09297049.2012.669470.

8. Gioia G a, Isquith PK, Retzlaff PD, Espy K a. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in a clinical sample. Child Neuropsychol. 2002;8(4):249–257. doi:10.1076/chin.8.4.249.13513.

9. Mabbott DJ, Spiegler BJ, Greenberg ML, Rutka JT, Hyder DJ, Bouffet E. Serial evaluation of academic and behavioral outcome after treatment with cranial radiation in childhood. J Clin Oncol. 2005;23(10):2256–2263. doi:10.1200/JCO.2005.01.158.

10. Fuemmeler BF, Elkin TD, Mullins LL. Survivors of childhood brain tumors: behavioral, emotional, and social adjustment. Clin Psychol Rev. 2002;22(4):547–85.

11. Baron Nelson M, Compton P, Patel SK, et al. Central Nervous System Injury and Neurobiobehavioral Function in Children With Brain Tumors: A Review of the Literature. Cancer Nurs. 2012;36(2):31–47. doi:10.1097/NCC.0b013e31825d1eb0.

12. Boman KK, Hörnquist L, De Graaff L, Rickardsson J, Lannering B, Gustafsson G. Disability, body image and sports/physical activity in adult survivors of childhood CNS tumors: population-based outcomes from a cohort study. J Neurooncol. 2013;112(1):99–106. doi:10.1007/s11060-012-1039-5.

13. Sato I, Higuchi A, Yanagisawa T, et al. Factors influencing self- and parent-reporting health-related quality of life in children with brain tumors. Qual life Res. 2013;22(1):185–201. doi:10.1007/s11136-012-0137-3.

14. Linley PA, Joseph S. Positive change following trauma and adversity: a review. J Trauma Stress. 2004;17(1):11–21. doi:10.1023/B:JOTS.0000014671.27856.7e.

15. Turner CD, Rey-Casserly C, Liptak CC, Chordas C. Late effects of therapy for pediatric brain tumor survivors. J Clin Oncol. 2009;24(11):1455–1463.

16. Castellino SM, Ullrich NJ, Whelen MJ, Lange BJ. Developing interventions for cancer-related cognitive dysfunction in childhood cancer survivors. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2014;106(8). doi:10.1093/jnci/dju186.

17. Butler RW, Copeland DR, Fairclough DL, et al. A multicenter, randomized clinical trial of a cognitive remediation program for childhood survivors of a pediatric malignancy. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2008;76(3):367–378. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.76.3.367.

Page 171: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

General Discussion

169

8

18. van ’t Hooft I, Andersson K, Bergman B, Sejersen T, von Wendt L, Bartfai A. Sustained favorable effects of cognitive training in children with acquired brain injuries. NeuroRehabilitation. 2007;22(2):109–116.

19. Conklin HM, Ogg RJ, Ashford JM, et al. Computerized Cognitive Training for Amelioration of Cognitive Late Effects Among Childhood Cancer Survivors: A Randomized Controlled Trial. J Clin Oncol. 2015;33(33):3894–3902. doi:10.1200/JCO.2015.61.6672.

20. Conklin HM, Reddick WE, Ashford JM, et al. Long-term efficacy of methylphenidate in enhancing attention regulation, social skills, and academic abilities of childhood cancer survivors. J Clin Oncol. 2010;28(29):4465–4472. doi:10.1200/JCO.2010.28.4026.

21. Moriyama TS, Polanczyk G, Caye A, Banaschewski T, Brandeis D, Rohde L a. Evidence-based information on the clinical use of neurofeedback for ADHD. Neurotherapeutics. 2012;9(3):588–598. doi:10.1007/s13311-012-0136-7.

22. Janssen TWP, Bink M, Geladé K, van Mourik R, Maras A, Oosterlaan J. A Randomized Controlled Trial Investigating the Effects of Neurofeedback, Methylphenidate, and Physical Activity on Event-Related Potentials in Children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2016;26(4):344–353. doi:10.1089/cap.2015.0144.

23. Van Dongen-Boomsma M, Vollebregt MA, Slaats-Willemse D, Buitelaar JK. A randomized placebo-controlled trial of electroencephalographic (EEG) neurofeedback in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Psychiatry. 2013;74(8):821–827. doi:10.4088/JCP.12m08321.

24. Perreau-Linck E, Lessard N, Levesque J, Beauregard M. Effects of neurofeed back training on inhibitory capacities in ADHD children: A single-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled study. J Neurother. 2010;14:229–242. doi:10.1080/10874208.2010.501514.

25. Arnold LE, Lofthouse N, Hersch S, et al. EEG neurofeedback for ADHD: double-blind sham-controlled randomized pilot feasibility trial. J Atten Disord. 2013;17(5):410–419. doi:10.1177/1087054712446173.

26. Lansbergen MM, van Dongen-Boomsma M, Buitelaar JK, Slaats-Willemse D. ADHD and EEG-neurofeedback: a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled feasibility study. J Neural Transm. 2011;118(2):275–284. doi:10.1007/s00702-010-0524-2.

27. de Vries M, Prins PJM, Schmand B a., Geurts HM. Working memory and cognitive flexibility-training for children with an autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(5):566–576. doi:10.1111/jcpp.12324.

28. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.; 2013.

29. Vollebregt MA, van Dongen-Boomsma M, Slaats-Willemse D, Buitelaar JK. What future research should bring to help resolving the debate about the efficacy of EEG-neurofeedback in children with ADHD. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8(May):321. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00321.

30. Kenzik KM, Huang I-C, Brinkman TM, et al. The Childhood Cancer Survivor Study-Neurocognitive Questionnaire (CCSS-NCQ) revised: item response analysis and concurrent validity. Neuropsychology. 2015;29(1):31–44. doi:10.1037/neu0000095.

31. Lai J-S, Zelko F, Krull KR, et al. Parent-reported cognition of children with cancer and its potential clinical usefulness. Qual Life Res. 2013. doi:10.1007/s11136-013-0548-9.

32. Mitchell JT, Zylowska L, Kollins SH. Mindfulness meditation training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adulthood: Current empirical support, treatment overview, and future directions. Cogn Behav Pract. 2015;22(2):172–191. doi:10.1016/j.cbpra.2014.10.002.

33. Ness KK, Morris EB, Nolan VG, Howell CR, Gilchrist LS. Physical Performance Limitations Among Adult Survivors of Childhood Brain Tumors. 2010:3034–3044. doi:10.1002/cncr.25051.

34. Piscione PJ, Bouffet E, Mabbott DJ, Shams I, Kulkarni A V. Physical functioning in pediatric survivors of childhood posterior fossa brain tumors. Neuro Oncol. 2014;16(1):147–155. doi:10.1093/neuonc/not138.

Page 172: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Chapter 8

170

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

35. Courneya KS, Sellar CM, Stevinson C, et al. Randomized controlled trial of the effects of aerobic exercise on physical functioning and quality of life in lymphoma patients. J Clin Oncol. 2009;27(27):4605–4612. doi:10.1200/JCO.2008.20.0634.

36. Rabin C, Pinto B, Fava J. Randomized Trial of a Physical Activity and Meditation Intervention for Young Adult Cancer Survivors. J Adolesc Young Adult Oncol. 2016;5(1):41–47. doi:10.1089/jayao.2015.0033.

37. Braam KI, van der Torre P, Takken T, Veening MA, van Dulmen-den Broeder E, Kaspers GJ. Physical exercise training interventions for children and young adults during and after treatment for childhood cancer. Cochrane Libr. 2016;(3). doi:10.1002/14651858.CD008796.pub3.www.cochranelibrary.com.

38. Ogrim G, Hestad K a. Effects of neurofeedback versus stimulant medication in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a randomized pilot study. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2013;23(7):448–57. doi:10.1089/cap.2012.0090.

39. Haverman L, van Oers H, Limperg P, et al. Implementation of Electronic Patient Reported Outcomes in Pediatric Daily Clinical Practice: The KLIK Experience. Clin Pr pediatr psychol. 2014;2(1):50–67. doi:10.1037/cpp0000043.

Page 173: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Cover concept 2.indd 1 21-10-16 15:04

Summary

Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch)

List of contributions of all author

Portfolio

Financial Support

Dankwoord – Acknowledgements

Curriculum Vitae

Page 174: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Summary

172

Page 175: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Summary

173

S

SUMMARY

Summary of ‘Impact of a pediatric brain tumor: Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention’

Each year about 120 children are diagnosed with a brain tumor in the Netherlands (source: SKION). Fortunately, earlier detection and improved treatment options lead to an increasing number of children who survive the disease and its treatment. Survival differs per brain tumor type, but overall the five-year survival rate is up to almost 80%. However, the tumor and its treatment have been known to cause damage to the healthy tissue in the brain, which may cause a range of neurocognitive late effects. For example, many pediatric brain tumor survivors (PBTS) suffer from lower intelligence, which in turn may affect their quality of life. With more children surviving, the consequences of damage to this growing group become apparent. With this, also the need grows to fully understand the extent of the neurocognitive late effects.

The First Chapter forms a general introduction to this thesis. It outlines the possible neurocognitive late effects PBTS may suffer from after being treated for a brain tumor. Impairments such as slower processing speed may cause to PBTS perform worse in school than peers. The neurocognitive late effects PBTS suffer as a result of the tumor and/or the treatment may lead to worse psychosocial functioning. In order to timely identify neurocognitive problems, it is important to regularly test PBTS. A suitable screener would increase feasibility for monitoring PBTS. Subsequently available interventions for PBTS with neurocognitive problems are described, such as medication. Neurofeedback (NF) training is introduced as a possible addition to the current options.

The aims of the current thesis were:- To provide a systematic review of studies into intellectual and attention functioning of

PBTS. - To outline the neurocognitive functions that might be affected after treatment for a

pediatric brain tumor, while potential predictors for neurocognitive functioning were also investigated.

- To assess the psychosocial functioning of the PBTS.- To investigate the correlation between proxy-report questionnaires (parent and teacher)

and tasks measuring neurocognitive functioning in PBTS. - To investigate the effects of NF training on neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning

in PBTS using a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trial.

Page 176: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Summary

174

With Chapter 2, we were interested in the magnitude of the neurocognitive late effects in PBTS. To determine this, we included 29 trials into a meta-analysis: 22 studies on intelligence of 710 PBTS and seven on attention of 372 PBTS. The results show seriously declined intelligence and attention scores as compared to healthy peers. Risk factors for lower intelligence scores were radiation and/or chemotherapy and longer time since diagnosis.

The problems experienced by PBTS require an intervention to improve neurocognitive functioning. To date, few effective interventions for neurocognitive late effects in PBTS are available. The current options are cognitive remediation training, working memory training or pharmacological interventions. These options have some limitations, such as side effects of the pharmacological treatments or small effect sizes. Neurofeedback (NF) training could be an interesting addition to the existing interventions. NF training is based on operant conditioning, in which the participant acquired control over the brain waves measured on an EEG. NF training showed promising effects in other patient groups, such as children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Chapter 3 describes the design of the PRISMA study; a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trial to investigate the effects of NF training on neurocognitive late effects in PBTS. The study was set up from the Emma Children’s Hospital/Academic Medical Center and we collaborated with four other Dutch University Medical Centers (UMC): the VUmc Amsterdam, UMC Utrecht, Radboud UMC Nijmegen, and Maastricht UMC. The intervention consisted of 30 sessions of NF or placebo-feedback (PF) training. For this, PBTS (8-18 years> 2 years after treatment) were invited to participate if they had neurocognitive complaints. Their neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning was tested before the intervention (baseline), immediately after and 6 months after the intervention. To gain insight into their functioning at baseline, we compared them with a sibling control group. A total of 82 PBTS (average age 13.9 years) and 43 siblings as control group (mean age 14.3 years) were enrolled into the PRISMA-study.

In Chapters 4 & 5, the baseline data of the PRISMA-study are presented. The neurocognitive functioning of the enrolled PBTS with neurocognitive complaints was compared to the functioning of a sibling control group in Chapter 4. Tests of attention, processing speed, memory, visuomotor integration, executive functioning, and intellectual functioning were used to create a detailed picture of the neurocognitive functioning. We also investigated the association between neurocognitive functioning and medical risk factors. PBTS showed that they had more difficulties than the sibling control group in the majority of the assessed domains. Especially tasks that were time-critical seemed to differentiate between PBTS and the sibling control group, with PBTS performing worse. There was no correlation between the medical risk factors and neurocognitive functioning. This suggests that even PBTS without the traditional medical risk factors for neurocognitive late effects, such as hydrocephalus, early age at diagnosis or cranial radiation, are at risk of neuro-cognitive problems.

Page 177: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Summary

175

S

The neurocognitive late effects PBTS suffer as a result of the tumor and/or the treatment may lead to worse psychosocial functioning in PBTS. Studies on different aspects of psychosocial functioning have been contradictory, some reporting no psychosocial problems and other studies reporting lower psychosocial functioning in PBTS. Therefore, studying psychosocial functioning, especially of PBTS with neurocognitive complains, is warranted. Psychosocial functioning can be measured using self-report or proxy-report, e.g. parent-report or teacher-report. These respondents have a different frame of reference, which may result in differing insights. By including several respondents, a more complete picture of the psychosocial functioning of the PBTS can be created. Psychosocial functioning of the PBTS with neurocognitive complaints was assessed using various self-, parent- and/or teacher-report questionnaires: health related quality of life (HRQOL), self-esteem, social-emotional functioning, fatigue, sleep disturbance, attention in daily life, and behavioral executive functioning (EF). The functioning was compared to normative data or the sibling control group (fatigue only). The results are described in Chapter 5. PBTS subjectively suffered more from social-emotional problems, fatigue, and behavioural executive functioning problems, as reported by themselves, parents and teachers. Especially physical aspects of psychosocial functioning appeared to be depressed. However, the difference between the PBTS and the normative data was less than a standard deviation, and therefore the scores appear to be within the normal ranges of psychosocial functioning.

The neurocognitive and psychosocial late effects can worsen over time as a result of difficulty to process new information and learn new skills. Therefore, it is important to monitor changes in neurocognitive functioning by testing PBTS regularly. Neurocognitive assessment is time consuming and expensive. A screener could be a cheaper, faster alternative to assess PBTS. The BRIEF (Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function) questionnaire could be a possible screener tool, as it is designed to assess behavioral executive functions in daily life. In Chapter 6, the utility of the BRIEF questionnaire as a possible screener for executive function problems in PBTS was researched. The correlation between the BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher questionnaire and neurocognitive tasks measuring the same executive functions. The BRIEF-Parent seems unsuitable as a screener as the scores did not correlated to the tasks. The BRIEF-Teacher showed some significant correlations with EF tasks. PBTS with clinically elevated BRIEF-Teacher scores had worse scores on all executive function tasks, as compared to PBTS without elevated scores. The BRIEF-Parent and BRIEF-Teacher were not correlated to each other. The BRIEF-Teacher seems to be a better tool for screening executive functions in PBTS than the BRIEF-Parent, however, other questionnaires should be explored in the future.

Page 178: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Summary

176

Chapter 7 describes the results of the PRISMA-study. We examined the effects of NF training by treating half of the participants with NF training (on the basis of the brain activity) and the other half with placebo-feedback (PF) training (random), and comparing the two groups. Of the 82 PBTS who completed the baseline assessment, 80 PBTS were randomized to receive NF or PF training; 71 PBTS completed the training and post-training assessment; 68 PBTS completed the training and the 6 months post-training assessment. The results show no difference between the two groups. In short, we have found no positive effect of NF training on the functioning of PBTS above and beyond PF training. Based on these results, we cannot advise NF training for neurocognitive problems after treatment for a brain tumor.

In Chapter 8, a general discussion of the main findings of the studies is presented.

The key messages are:

- PBTS have more neurocognitive problems than the sibling control group- Even PBTS without traditional risk factors, such as cranial irradiation and young age

at diagnosis, may suffer from neurocognitive late effects- PBTS with neurocognitive complaints suffer from more psychosocial problems than

healthy controls, as reported by themselves, parents, and teachers. Specifically physical aspects seem to be affected, as reported by PBTS themselves.

- Parents report more EF problems in PBTS than teachers.- Teacher-reported EF of PBTS shows some correlations with neurocognitive tasks

and parent-reported EF does not.- NF training is not more effective in PBTS than PF training in improving neurocognitive

and psychosocial functioning.

We also reflect on the limitations of the studies in this thesis. As PBTS could only participate if they had neurocognitive problems, the results cannot be generalized to the entire group of PBTS. Also, the epoch to adjust the threshold of feedback in the training was short. Further, there were no ‘transfer sessions’, which are sessions to increase generalization from the training into daily life.Recommendations for future research and clinical practice are made based on the findings in this thesis. Parents, teachers and health care providers that work with PBTS should be well aware of the possible late effects. Also, suggestions for parents and teachers of PBTS are made. They should provide structure for the PBTS and if indicated give extra time for completing a task.We, furthermore, stress the importance of monitoring neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning of PBTS, in order to timely identify problems, before they intensify. The BRIEF-

Page 179: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Summary

177

S

Teacher could be used as a screening tool, although other options should be explored. Some recommendations are made for systems that could increase feasibility of the monitoring, such as the KLIK-system. This is an online system to monitor patients over time using Patient or Parent Reported Outcomes (PROs). Lastly, the quest for effective interventions to improve neurocognitive and psychosocial functioning of PBTS should continue. An intervention for neurocognitive functioning should be targeted at processing speed and working memory. CogMed and Mindfulness are mentioned as possible contenders. Regarding psychosocial functioning, physical exercises should be explored, as the psychosocial problems reported by PBTS seemed to be mainly physical, and also because exercises have been found to improve quality of life and increase happiness.

In summary, the results of this study show that PBTS have neurocognitive and psychosocial late effects as a result of the treatment. It is important to monitor these deficits in order to timely refer PBTS to proper care if indicated. New interventions that might be effective to decrease neurocognitive problems in PBTS should be developed and studied.

Page 180: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch)

178

Page 181: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch)

179

S

SUMMARY IN DUTCH

Samenvatting van ‘Impact of a pediatric brain tumor: Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention’

Elk jaar worden in Nederland ongeveer 120 kinderen gediagnosticeerd met een hersentumor (bron: SKION). Vroege diagnose en betere behandeling hebben ertoe geleid dat een groeiend aantal kinderen de ziekte en de behandeling overleven. Het overlevingspercentage verschilt per soort hersentumor, maar over het algemeen is het vijf-jaar-overlevingspercentage bijna 80%. Helaas is bekend dat de tumor en de behandelingen ook het gezonde weefsel in de hersenen kunnen beschadigen, wat tot neurocognitieve problemen kan leiden. Zo hebben veel kinderen na (behandeling van) een hersentumor een lagere intelligentie dan gezonde leeftijdsgenoten, wat een negatief effect kan hebben op hun kwaliteit van leven.Met de stijging van het aantal kinderen dat een hersentumor overleeft, worden de neurocognitieve gevolgen van de tumor en de behandeling ook steeds duidelijker. Hiermee groeit de noodzaak om de omvang en de aard van de neurocognitieve problemen nog beter in kaart te brengen.

In het eerste hoofdstuk, een algemene inleiding op dit proefschrift, worden de mogelijke neurocognitieve en psychosociale gevolgen van een hersentumor op de kinderleeftijd beschreven. Problemen zoals een tragere verwerkingssnelheid kunnen ervoor zorgen dat deze kinderen slechter presteren op school dan leeftijdsgenoten. Deze problemen kunnen leiden tot problemen in psychosociaal functioneren. Het is van belang dat kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor regelmatig worden getest, zodat problemen tijdig kunnen worden ontdekt. Een goede screener zou hierbij kunnen helpen. Vervolgens worden de beschikbare interventies, zoals medicatie, voor kinderen met neurocognitieve problemen na behandeling voor een hersentumor beschreven. Neurofeedback (NF) training wordt geïntroduceerd als mogelijke aanvulling op de huidige interventies.

We hadden voor dit proefschrift de volgende doelen voor ogen:- Een systematisch overzicht geven van studies naar intelligentie en aandacht van kinderen

die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor- De neurocognitieve functies die mogelijk zijn aangedaan na behandeling van een

hersentumor op kinderleeftijd meten, alsmede mogelijke voorspellers voor neurocognitief functioneren

- Het psychosociaal functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor onderzoeken.

Page 182: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch)

180

- De correlatie meten tussen de proxy - vragenlijsten (ouder en docent) en taken die het neurocognitief functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor testen.

- De effecten van NF training op de het neurocognitief en psychosociaal functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor te onderzoeken met behulp van een dubbelblinde gerandomiseerde placebo-gecontroleerde studie.

In Hoofdstuk 2 worden de resultaten van de meta-analyse naar de omvang van de neurocognitieve problemen van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor beschreven. In de meta-analyse zijn 29 studies geïncludeerd: 22 studies naar intelligentie van in totaal 710 kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor en zeven studies naar de aandacht van in totaal 372 kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor. De uitkomsten laten zien dat kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor een aanzienlijk lagere intelligentie en meer aandachtsproblemen hebben in vergelijking met gezonde leeftijdsgenoten. Risicofactoren voor lagere intelligentiescores waren: bestraling en / of chemotherapie en langere tijd sinds diagnose.

De neurocognitieve problemen die kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor laten zien, vragen om een interventie om hun functioneren te verbeteren. Er zijn momenteel weinig effectieve interventies beschikbaar om het neurocognitief functioneren van deze kinderen te verbeteren. Huidige opties zijn cognitieve training, een geheugentraining, een werkgeheugentraining of medicijnen. Deze opties hebben een aantal beperkingen, zoals bijwerkingen van medicijnen, bovendien lieten studies slechts kleine effecten zien. NF training is een mogelijk interessante aanvulling op de bestaande interventies. NF training is gebaseerd op operante conditionering: de deelnemer leert controle te krijgen over de hersengolven gemeten met een EEG. NF training liet veelbelovende effecten zien bij andere patiëntengroepen, zoals kinderen met aandachttekort-hyperactiviteitstoornis (ADHD). Hoofdstuk 3 beschrijft de opzet van de PRISMA-studie; een dubbelblinde gerandomiseerde placebo-gecontroleerde studie naar de effecten van NF training op de neurocognitieve problemen bij kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor. Het onderzoek is opgezet vanuit het Emma Kinderziekenhuis/AMC en er werd samengewerkt met het VUmc Amsterdam, UMC Utrecht, Radboud UMC Nijmegen en Maastricht UMC. De training bestond uit 30 sessies van NF of placebo-feedback (PF) training. Hiervoor werden kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor (8-18 jaar en > 2 jaar na de behandeling) uitgenodigd om deel te nemen als ze neurocognitieve klachten hadden. Hun neurocognitieve en psychosociale functioneren werd getest voor de interventie (voormeting), direct erna en 6 maanden na de interventie. Om inzicht te krijgen in hun functioneren voor de interventie, werden ze vergeleken met een controlegroep van broers en zussen. In totaal werden 82

Page 183: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch)

181

S

kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor (gemiddelde leeftijd 13,9 jaar) en 43 broers en zussen (gemiddelde leeftijd 14,3 jaar) geïncludeerd in de PRISMA-studie.

De resultaten van de voormeting van de PRISMA-studie worden in de hoofdstukken 4 & 5 beschreven. In Hoofdstuk 4 werd het neurocognitief functioneren van de kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor vergeleken met het functioneren van de controlegroep. Aandacht, verwerkingssnelheid, geheugen, visuomotorische integratie, executieve functies, en intellectueel functioneren werden getest om een gedetailleerd beeld te krijgen van het neurocognitief functioneren. We onderzochten ook de samenhang tussen neurocognitief functioneren en medische risicofactoren, zoals radiotherapie en tumorgraad. kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor bleken slechter te presteren dan de controlegroep op het merendeel van de onderzochte gebieden. Vooral op taken waarbij tijd een rol speelde, was er een substantieel verschil tussen kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor en de controlegroep, waarbij kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor slechter presteerden. Er was geen correlatie tussen de medische risicofactoren en neurocognitief functioneren. Dit suggereert dat zelfs kinderen zonder de klassieke medische risicofactoren voor neurocognitieve late effecten, zoals hydrocefalus, jonge leeftijd bij diagnose of bestraling op het hoofd, toch risico lopen op neurocognitieve problemen.De neurocognitieve problemen bij kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor als gevolg van de tumor en / of de behandeling kunnen leiden tot problemen in het psychosociaal functioneren. Eerdere studies over het psychosociaal functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor lieten tegenstrijdige resultaten zien. Sommige studies vonden geen psychosociale problemen en andere studies vonden dat kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor meer psychosociale problemen lieten zien dan leeftijdsgenoten. Daarom is het van belang om het psychosociaal functioneren van deze kinderen te onderzoeken, met name van de kinderen met neurocognitieve klachten. Psychosociaal functioneren kan worden gemeten met behulp van zelf-rapportage of proxy-rapportage vragenlijsten, bijvoorbeeld ingevuld door de ouder of docent. Deze respondenten hebben allemaal een eigen referentiekader, wat tot verschillende inzichten kan leiden. Door de informatie van verschillende respondenten te verzamelen, ontstaat een completer beeld van het psychosociaal functioneren van een kind. In Hoofdstuk 5 werd het psychosociaal functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor onderzocht met behulp van diverse vragenlijsten, ingevuld door verschillende respondenten: het kind zelf, een ouder en een docent. Met de vragenlijsten werd gezondheids-gerelateerde kwaliteit van leven, zelfvertrouwen, sociaal-emotioneel functioneren, vermoeidheid, slaapstoornissen, aandacht, en executief functioneren (EF) gemeten. De uitkomsten werden vergeleken met normatieve data of de controlegroep van broers en zussen (alleen voor vermoeidheid). Kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor hadden significant meer last van

Page 184: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch)

182

psychosociale problemen, zoals gerapporteerd door henzelf, ouders en leerkrachten. Vooral op fysieke aspecten van psychosociaal functioneren scoorden ze lager. Echter, het verschil tussen kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor en normatieve data was minder dan een standaarddeviatie en dus lijken de scores binnen de grenzen van normaal psychosociaal functioneren te vallen.

Vaak verergeren de neurocognitieve problemen bij kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor, door moeite met het verwerken van nieuwe informatie of het leren nieuwe vaardigheden. Daarom is het belangrijk om veranderingen in het neurocognitief functioneren door de tijd te volgen, door kinderen regelmatig te testen. Een neuropsychologisch onderzoek kost veel tijd en is duur. Een screeningsvragenlijst zou een goedkoper, sneller alternatief kunnen zijn om het functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor te onderzoeken. De BRIEF (Behavior Rating inventaris van Executive Function) vragenlijst zou een optie kunnen zijn, omdat de lijst is ontworpen om problemen met executieve functies in het dagelijks leven te meten. In Hoofdstuk 6 werd de bruikbaarheid van de BRIEF-vragenlijst onderzocht als mogelijke screener voor executieve problemen bij kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor. De correlatie tussen de BRIEF-Ouder en BRIEF-Docent vragenlijst en neurocognitieve taken die EF meten (aandacht, cognitieve flexibiliteit, inhibitie, visueel geheugen, en werkgeheugen) werd bekeken. De BRIEF-Ouder lijkt niet geschikt als screener, omdat de scores niet correleerden met de EF taken. De BRIEF-Docent liet wel enkele significante correlaties zien. Kinderen met klinisch verhoogde BRIEF-Docent scores hadden slechtere scores op alle EF taken, in vergelijking met kinderen zonder verhoogde scores. De BRIEF-Ouder en BRIEF-Docent waren niet gecorreleerd met elkaar. Uit de resultaten lijkt de BRIEF-Docent een beter instrument voor het screenen van EF bij kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor dan de BRIEF-Ouder, maar andere vragenlijsten zouden moeten worden overwogen in de toekomst.

Hoofdstuk 7 beschrijft de resultaten van de PRISMA-studie. We onderzochten de effecten van NF training door de deelnemers met behulp van loting in twee groepen te verdelen. De ene helft van de deelnemers kreeg NF training (op basis van de hersenactiviteit) en de andere helft placebo-feedback (PF) training (met willekeurige feedback). Van de 82 kinderen die de voormeting deden, werden 80 kinderen gerandomiseerd om NF dan wel PF training te ontvangen. 71 kinderen maakten de training af en namen deel aan de nameting. Uiteindelijk werd na 6 maanden de nameting gedaan bij 68 kinderen. De resultaten lieten geen verschil zien tussen de twee trainingsgroepen. Kortom, we hebben niet gevonden dat NF training het functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor meer verbetert dan PF training. Op basis van deze resultaten kunnen we niet adviseren om NF training aan te bieden aan kinderen met neurocognitieve problemen na behandeling voor een hersentumor.

Page 185: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch)

183

S

In Hoofdstuk 8 wordt een algemene discussie van de belangrijkste bevindingen van het onderzoek gepresenteerd.

De belangrijkste boodschappen zijn:- Kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor hebben meer neurocognitieve

problemen dan de controlegroep van broers en zussen- Zelfs kinderen zonder de bekende risicofactoren, zoals bestraling van het hoofd en

jonge leeftijd bij diagnose, kunnen last hebben van neurocognitieve problemen- Kinderen met neurocognitieve klachten na behandeling voor een hersentumor

hebben meer last van psychosociale problemen dan gezonde controles, zoals gerapporteerd door zichzelf, ouders en docenten. Vooral fysieke aspecten lijken een probleem te zijn, zoals gerapporteerd door henzelf.

- Ouders rapporteren meer EF problemen bij kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor dan docenten.

- Door de docent gerapporteerde EF van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor toont enkele correlaties met neurocognitieve taken, door de ouder gerapporteerde EF niet.

- NF training is niet effectiever in het verbeteren van het neurocognitief en psychosociaal functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor dan PF training.

Deze studie heeft ook enkele beperkingen. Aangezien kinderen alleen konden deelnemen aan de studie als zij neurocognitieve problemen hadden, kunnen de resultaten niet worden gegeneraliseerd naar de hele groep van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor. Ook was de tijdspanne waarop de drempelwaarde van feedback werd aangepast vrij kort. Binnen de interventie werden er geen ‘transfer sessies’ gedaan, dit zijn sessies om overdracht van de training naar het dagelijks leven te vergroten. Diverse aanbevelingen voor toekomstig onderzoek en de klinische praktijk worden gegeven op basis van de bevindingen in dit proefschrift. Ouders, docenten en zorgverleners die met kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor werken moeten goed worden geïnformeerd over de mogelijke late effecten. Ook worden er suggesties voor de omgangswijze van ouders en leerkrachten van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor gedaan. Het is bijvoorbeeld belangrijk dat zij structuur aanbrengen voor de kinderen en dat zij, zo nodig, extra tijd geven voor een taak. Het is van groot belang om het neurocognitief en psychosociaal functioneren van kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor te monitoren door hen regelmatig te screenen op eventuele problemen. Zo kunnen problemen tijdig worden geïdentificeerd. De BRIEF-Docent kan worden gebruikt als een screener, hoewel ook andere vragenlijsten zouden moeten

Page 186: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Samenvatting (Summary in Dutch)

184

worden onderzocht. We doen een aanbeveling voor systemen die de uitvoerbaarheid van het monitoren kunnen verhogen, zoals het KLIK-systeem. Dit is een online systeem om patiënten te monitoren over verloop van tijd met behulp van door de patiënt of ouder gerapporteerde uitkomsten. Tenslotte is het belangrijk dat de zoektocht naar effectieve interventies om neurocognitieve en psychosociale problemen te verminderen bij kinderen na behandeling voor een hersentumor, doorgaat. Een interventie voor het verminderen van neurocognitieve problemen van kinderen na behandeling van een hersentumor zou vooral gericht moeten zijn op verwerkingssnelheid en werkgeheugen. Cogmed en Mindfulness worden genoemd als mogelijke opties. Wat betreft het psychosociaal functioneren kan lichaamsbeweging als interventie worden onderzocht, omdat de psychosociale problemen die door kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor werden gerapporteerd vooral van fysieke aard lijken te zijn, en ook omdat in eerdere studies is gevonden dat lichaamsbeweging de stemming en de kwaliteit van leven kan verbeteren.

Samenvattend laten de resultaten van deze studie zien dat kinderen die zijn behandeld voor een hersentumor neurocognitieve en psychosociale late effecten hebben als gevolg van de tumor en / of behandeling. Het is belangrijk om deze problemen te monitoren zodat deze kinderen tijdig kunnen worden verwezen naar de juiste zorg als dat nodig is. Nieuwe interventies die mogelijk effectief zijn bij het verminderen van neurocognitieve problemen van kinderen na behandeling voor een hersentumor dienen te worden ontwikkeld en onderzocht als aanvulling op de huidige opties.

Page 187: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

List of contributions of all author

185

L

LIST OF CONTRIBUTIONS OF ALL AUTHORS Neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive functioning of children treated for a brain tumor: design of a randomized controlled double-blind trial BMC Cancer 12:581, 2012Chapter 2

M.A. de Ruiter conception and design of the study, wrote the first draft, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionA.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren conception and design of the study, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionR. van Mourik conception and design of the study, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionT.W.P Janssen critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionJ.E.M. Greidanus data acquisition, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionJ. Oosterlaan conception and design of the study, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionM.A. Grootenhuis conception and design of the study, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version

Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour and its treatment: a meta-analysis Dev Med Child Neurol 55:408–417, 2013Chapter 3

M.A. de Ruiter conception and design of the study, data acquisition, analyses, and interpretation, wrote the first draft, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionR. van Mourik conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionA.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version M.A. Grootenhuis conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionJ. Oosterlaan conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version

Page 188: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

List of contributions of all author

186

Timed performance weaknesses on computerized tasks in pediatric brain tumor survivors: a comparison with sibling controls. J Child Neuropsych, Epub ahead of print, 2015Chapter 4

M.A. de Ruiter conception and design of the study, data acquisition, analyses, and interpretation, wrote the first draft, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionM.A. Grootenhuis conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionR. van Mourik conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionH. Maurice-Stam conception and design of the study, analyses and interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionM.H.M. Breteler critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version C. Gidding provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionL.R. Beek provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionB. Granzen provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionD.G. van Vuurden provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionA.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version J. Oosterlaan conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version

Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor survivors with neurocognitive complaints Qual Life Res 25(2):435-446, 2015Chapter 5

M.A. de Ruiter conception and design of the study, data acquisition, analyses, and interpretation, wrote the first draft, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version

Page 189: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

List of contributions of all author

187

L

A.Y.N. Schouten-van conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionD.G. van Vuurden provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionH. Maurice-Stam conception and design of the study, analyses and interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionC. Gidding provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionL.R. Beek provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionB. Granzen provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionJ. Oosterlaan conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionM.A. Grootenhuis conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version

The association between the behavior rating inventory of executive functioning and neurocognitive testing in children diagnosed with a brain tumor SubmittedChapter 6

M. de Vries data analyses, and interpretation, wrote the first draft, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionM.A. de Ruiter conception and design of the study, data acquisition, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionK.J. Oostrom conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionA.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionH. Maurice-Stam conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionJ. Oosterlaan conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionM.A. Grootenhuis conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version

Page 190: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

List of contributions of all author

188

Neurofeedback ineffective in pediatric brain tumor survivors: Results of a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trial Eur J Cancer 64:62-73, 2015Chapter 7

M.A. de Ruiter conception and design of the study, data acquisition, analyses, and interpretation, wrote the first draft, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionJ. Oosterlaan conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionA.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionH. Maurice-Stam conception and design of the study, analyses and interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final versionD.G. van Vuurden provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionC. Gidding provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionL.R. Beek provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionB. Granzen provided us with potential patients from their medical centres and provided us with the medical data of the participants and non-participants, approved the final versionH.N. Caron conception and design of the study, approved the final versionM.A. Grootenhuis conception and design of the study, interpretation of data, critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content, approved the final version

Page 191: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Portfolio

189

P

PORTFOLIO

AMC Graduate School for Medical Sciences PhD PortfolioSummary of PhD training. Teaching and parameters of esteemPhD student: Marieke Montgomery-de RuiterPhD period: December 2008 - December 2016Supervisors: Prof dr MA Grootenhuis, Prof dr J OosterlaanCo-supervisors: Prof dr HN Caron, dr AYN Schouten-van Meeteren

1. PhD training Year Workload (ECTS)

General courses - Basic course AMC World of Science 2008 - 0.7- Reference Manager 2009 - 0.1- Better use of PubMed 2009 - 0.1 - Scientific writing in English 2009 - 1.5- Practical Biostatistics 2010 - 1.1- BROK: Basic course in Legislation and Organisation 2010 - 1.0 for Clinical researchers

Seminars, workshops and master classes- Masterclass ‘How to publish in the Journal of Pediatric 2012 - 0.1 Psychology’ by dr. L. Barakat- Elsevier workshop ‘Author and Reviewer’ 2013 - 0.1- CV workshop by dr. K. Fijnvandraat 2013 - 0.1

PresentationsThe efficacy of neurofeedback to improve attention and memory in childhood brain tumour survivors: a randomized controlled trial. - Poster presentation Dutch Society for Psychosocial 2010 - 0.5 Oncology (NVPO), Utrecht, the Netherlands - Poster presentation International Society for Pediatric 2010 - 0.5 Oncology (SIOP), Boston, USA- Poster presentation Emma Children’s Hospital 2010 - 0.5 scientific symposium, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Page 192: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Portfolio

190

Neurocognitive consequences of treatment for pediatric brain tumors: a meta-analysis. - Oral presentation Pediatric Neuro Oncological Platform 2010 - 0.5 (KNOP) research symposium, Amsterdam, the Netherlands - Poster presentation International Symposium on 2010 - 0.5 Pediatric Neuro- Oncology (ISPNO) Vienna, Austria- Poster presentation Dutch Pediatric Psychology 2011 - 0.5 Network (PPN) symposium, Amsterdam, the Netherlands - Poster presentation Emma Children’s Hospital scientific symposium, 2011 - 0.5 Amsterdam, the Netherlands - Oral presentation; NVPO, Utrecht, the Netherlands 2012 - 0.5

Attention networks in pediatric brain tumor survivors - Oral presentation ISPNO, Toronto, Canada 2012 - 0.5- Poster presentation SIOP, London, England 2012 - 0.5- Poster presentation 2nd Amsterdam Pediatric 2013 - 0.5 Symposium (AKS). Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Neurocognitive profile of pediatric brain tumor survivors: a comparison with siblings- Oral presentation multidisciplinary meeting KNOP, the 2012 - 0.5 Netherlands- Poster presentation PPN symposium, Amsterdam, the 2014 - 0.5 Netherlands- Poster presentation SIOP, Toronto, Canada 2014 - 0.5

Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor survivors with neurocognitive complaints- Poster presentation PPN symposium, Amsterdam, the 2014 - 0.5 Netherlands- Poster presentation SIOP, Toronto, Canada 2014 - 0.5

Neurofeedback in children treated for a brain tumor: Results of a randomized controlled double-blind trial- Oral presentation IPOS world congress, Washington, 2015 - 0.5 USA- Poster presentation AKS, Amsterdam, the Netherlands 2015 - 0.5

Page 193: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Portfolio

191

P

(Inter-)national conferences

- Emma Children’s hospital (EKZ) Scientific Symposium 2010, 2011 0.5- International Society for Pediatric Oncology (SIOP) 2010, 2014 2- Pediatric Neuro Oncological Platform (KNOP) research 2010 0.25 Symposium- International Symposium on Pediatric Neuro- Oncology 2010, 2012 2 (ISPNO)- Dutch Society for Psychosocial Oncology (NVPO) 2011, 2012 0.5- Pediatric Psychological Network (PPN) Symposium 2011, 2014 0.5- Amsterdam Pediatric Symposium (AKS) 2013, 2015 0.5- International Psycho Oncology Society (IPOS) World 2015 1- Congress

2. Teaching Year Workload (ECTS)

Lecturing- University of Leiden, guest lecture on neurofeedback 2010 0.5 training for the course Intervention Strategies in Clinical Neuropsychology in the Master Clinical Neuropsychology- University of Amsterdam, guest lectures on 2013-2014 2 psychosocial care and age appropriate communication with patients for residents in the Academic Medical Centre- VU University, guest lecture on the (neuro) 2014 0.5 psychological late effects of brain cancer and cancer treatment for the Master Clinical Neuropsychology

Supervising - Zeliha Pekcan, master thesis Clinical Neuropsychology, 2012 1 VU University, ‘Attention functioning of childhood brain tumor survivors compared to their siblings’ - Leontine Stolk, master thesis Psychology, University 2013 1 of Amsterdam, ‘De invloed van de behandeling van een hersentumor op responsinhibitie en snelheid van informatieverwerking’ - Dora Csermak, master thesis Clinical Neuropsychology, 2014 1 VU University, ‘General well-being after a brain

Page 194: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Portfolio

192

tumour: the influence of psychosocial problems, fatigue and self-esteem on the quality of life of Dutch paediatric brain tumour survivors’ - Arnout Smit, master thesis Clinical Neuropsychology, 2014 1 VU University, ‘Pediatric brain tumors and IQ: The effects of age at diagnosis, radiotherapy, and chemotherapy - Sander Schippers, master thesis Clinical 2015 1 Neuropsychology, VU University, ‘Effects of neurofeedback on visuomotor functioning in children with a brain tumor

3. Parameters of Esteem Year

Scholarship- Spinoza scholarship (UvA) €400 for ISPNO 2012 in Toronto, CA.

4. Publications Year

Peer reviewed

Neurofeedback to improve neurocognitive functioning of 2012children treated for a brain tumor: design of a randomizedcontrolled double-blind trialM.A. de Ruiter, A.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren, R. van Mourik, T.W.P Janssen, J.E.M. Greidanus, J. Oosterlaan, M.A. Grootenhuis BMC Cancer

Neurocognitive consequences of a paediatric brain tumour 2013and its treatment: a meta-analysisM.A. de Ruiter, R. van Mourik, A.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren, M.A. Grootenhuis, J. Oosterlaan Dev Med Child Neurol

Page 195: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Portfolio

193

P

Volume of white matter hyperintensities is an independent 2014predictor of intelligence quotient and processing speed in children with sickle cell disease.V. van der Land, C.T. Hijmans, M.A. de Ruiter, H.J. Mutsaerts, M.H. Cnossen, M. Engelen, C.B. Majoie, A.J. Nederveen, M.A. Grootenhuis, K. Fijnvandraat Br J Haematol

Timed performance weaknesses on computerized tasks in 2015pediatric brain tumor survivors: a comparison with sibling controls.M.A. de Ruiter, M.A. Grootenhuis, R. van Mourik, H. Maurice-Stam, M.H.M. Breteler, C. Gidding, L.R. Beek, B. Granzen, D.G. van Vuurden, A.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren,J. Oosterlaan J Child Neuropsych

Psychosocial profile of pediatric brain tumor survivors 2015with neurocognitive complaintsM.A. de Ruiter, A.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren, D.G. van Vuurden, H. Maurice-Stam, C. Gidding, L.R. Beek, B. Granzen, J. Oosterlaan, M.A. Grootenhuis Qual Life Res

Neurofeedback ineffective in pediatric brain tumor 2015 survivors: Results of a double-blind randomized placebo-controlled trialM.A. de Ruiter, J. Oosterlaan, A.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren, H. Maurice-Stam, D.G. van Vuurden, C. Gidding, L.R. Beek, B. Granzen, H.N. Caron, M.A. Grootenhuis Eur J Cancer

The association between the behavior rating inventory of 2016executive functioning and neurocognitive testing in children diagnosed with a brain tumorM. de Vries, M.A. de Ruiter, K.J. Oostrom, A.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren, H. Maurice-Stam, J. Oosterlaan, M.A. Grootenhuis Submitted

Page 196: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Financial Support

194

Page 197: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Financial Support

195

F

FINANCIAL SUPPORT

This work was supported by the Dutch Cancer Society KWF Kankerbestrijding under grant number UVA 2008-4013; and the Tom Voûte Fund, Part of the Dutch Children Cancer Free Foundation, KiKa under grant number SKK-PRISMA.

Page 198: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Dankwoord – Acknowledgements

196

Page 199: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Dankwoord – Acknowledgements

197

D

DANKWOORD

Bijna, bijna, bijna is het boekje dan echt af! Bijna acht jaar, 82 patiënten, 266 neuropsychologische onderzoeken, meer dan 2300 neurofeedback sessie en 30 trainers die samen zoveel kilometers maakten dat zij meer dan twee keer de wereld rond reisden voor de neurofeedback trainingen! Graag bedank ik hier de mensen die mij op allerlei manieren geholpen en gesteund hebben bij het tot stand komen van dit proefschrift.

Allereerst bedank ik alle deelnemers aan het onderzoek en hun ouders. Zonder hen zou er geen PRISMA-onderzoek zijn gekomen. De deelnemers ondergingen 15 weken trainingen en kwamen drie keer naar een EEG-centrum voor een hele dag vol testen. Een aantal broers en zussen deden als controlegroep belangeloos mee aan het onderzoek. En de ouders, docenten en kinderen vulden drie keer vragenlijsten in.

Prof. dr. MA Grootenhuis, lieve Martha, dank je wel voor je begeleiding de afgelopen jaren. We hebben heel wat meegemaakt door de jaren heen, met ups en downs, zowel in mijn privéleven als in het onderzoek. Dank voor al je medeleven, begrip en begeleiding door de jaren heen! En ondanks hier en daar een slapeloze nacht, is de PRISMA-studie er toch maar gekomen! Er deden kinderen mee, er werden trainers op pad gestuurd en de data werd verzameld! Je was een betrokken, enthousiaste begeleider en bevlogen in je rol als professor in de pediatrische psychologie. Naast dat je een bevlogen professor bent overdag, hou jij van een feestje in de avond. Of het nou een zomaar-zomer-feest is of een gala op een boot tijdens een congres in Washington, jij houdt van dansen! Ik denk niet dat veel promovendi zo’n gezellige begeleider hebben!

Prof. dr. J Oosterlaan, beste Jaap, jouw begeleiding startte eigenlijk al een jaar voor mijn promotietraject, toen je mijn scriptiebegeleider was. We halen net niet een tinnen jubileum! Door de jaren heen heb ik je leren kennen als een begeleider met enorm veel neuropsychologische kennis en een scherpe, kritische blik. Je was nooit te beroerd om naar nog een versie van een artikel te kijken, ‘je bent er echt bijna’. Dit heeft de kwaliteit van mijn scriptie en mijn proefschrift zeker verhoogd! Bedankt!

Dr. A.Y.N. Schouten-van Meeteren, lieve Netteke, wat ben jij een enorm betrokken, warm mens, zowel in je werk als arts en onderzoeker, als privé. Zoals die avond dat je me bij je thuis uitnodigde om mee te eten met jouw familie. Zodat je me daarna kon helpen de meta-analyse, een artikel waar ik enorm mee aan het stoeien was, af te ronden. Naast dat je enorm betrokken bent, heb jij ook een geheugen waarvan ik onder de indruk ben. Jij onthoudt de kleinste details van al je patiënten, maar ook van het onderzoek waar je bij betrokken bent. Ik vond je een erg fijne begeleider, dank je wel!

Page 200: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Dankwoord – Acknowledgements

198

Prof. dr. H.N. Caron, beste Huib, bedankt dat je eerst als promotor en later als co-promotor betrokken wilde zijn bij dit project. Je startte als promotor, maar hebt van plek geruild met Martha toen zij professor werd. Dank voor je bijdrage aan de PRISMA-studie.

Dr. H. Maurice-Stam, lieve Heleen, jij bent de laatste jaren van onschatbare waarde gebleken bij de analyses van PRISMA. Samen gingen we op zoek naar de juiste methode om de RCT te analyseren. Maar ik ben je vooral enorm dankbaar voor het uitvoeren van de eerste analyses hiervan, toen bleek dat het deblinderen van de onderzoeksgroepen niet meer ging lukken voordat ik op zwangerschapsverlof ging en ik deze niet zou kunnen doen. Wat heb jij een werk verricht voor de PRISMA-studie!

De leden van de promotiecommissie, Prof. dr. J.K. Buitelaar, prof. dr. G.J.L. Kaspers, dr. K.J. Oostrom, prof. dr. B.A. Schmand, prof. dr. J.B. van Goudoever en prof. dr. B.T. Poll-The, bedank ik graag voor het beoordelen van dit proefschrift op de wetenschappelijke kwaliteit.

Lieve Juliette, wat was jij een fijne onderzoeksassistent. Jij hield het overzicht over alle afspraken, patiënten, welke trainer of tester waar wanneer moest zijn. Al leverde de omvang van het project ons af en toe hartkloppingen op, het kwam toch allemaal goed! De patiënten en student-testers kwamen allemaal op de juiste dag op je juiste locatie aan. De gang van zake bespraken we tijdens onze overlegjes op het voetenplein met een cappuccino erbij. En als er wat te vieren was gingen we naar de Keet voor een biertje!

Graag bedank ik ook alle de co-auteurs van de artikelen in dit proefschrift voor het meelezen en waardevolle feedback. Corrie, Bernd, Dannis en Laura, hartelijk dank voor de hulp bij het werven van patiënten en het verschaffen van medische informatie. Rosa, behalve co-auteur heb jij ook enorm bijgedragen aan het ontstaan van PRISMA en bij het ontwerpen van de neurofeedback designs.

Jorn, Rien en Suzan, bedankt voor het uitvoeren van alle EEGs, en het ter beschikking stellen van ruimte om de neuropsychologische testen af te nemen. Naast nuttig was het bij jullie alle drie ook altijd gezellig met een kopje thee en een praatje. Rien, hartelijk dank voor de ondersteuning bij het gebruik van de neurofeedback apparaten, het ontwerpen van de neurofeedback designs, advies en de gezelligheid!

Bij de PRISMA-studie waren 5 masterstudenten betrokken die hun scriptie hebben geschreven met PRISMA-data. Zeliha, Leontine, Dora, Arnout en Sander, bedankt voor de inzet bij het testen van de patiënten, het analyseren en het schrijven! Verder hebben 30 neurofeedback trainers heel Nederland door gereisd om alle deelnemers op school of thuis

Page 201: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Dankwoord – Acknowledgements

199

D

twee keer per week te trainen. Met name Diane wil ik hier graag bedanken, want zij heeft in haar auto ruim 53000 km gereden voor meer dan 550 trainingen van 20 patiënten!

Collega’s en oud-collega’s van de Psychosociale afdeling van het Emma Kinderziekenhuis bedank ik graag voor de fijne tijd! Marga en Michal, zonder jullie op het secretariaat geen PSA! Bedankt voor alle ondersteuning en de gezellige praatjes als ik de post kwam halen op het secretariaat.

En dan natuurlijk de Basement, zowel oud als nieuw: Madelon, Channa, Vivian, Eefje, Janneke, Lotte, JP, Sasja, Hedy, Perrine, Florrie, Anouck, Charlotte, Juliette en Marieke de Vries! Een gezelligere kelder dan ons plekkie op K01-139 kan ik me niet bedenken. Ik zit er al even niet meer, maar denk graag terug aan de wandelingen naar de ap, de lunches in de eerste zonnestralen buiten, de gebbetjes en de potjandosie op kamertje 1! Amai, dat was een tantoe fissa, fy fan! Gelukkig zit ik nog in de basement-app om af en toe een woensdag zaagmansgrapje te kunnen maken! Dat maakte het thuis schrijven veel gezelliger!

Basement-buren, inmiddels zitten we allemaal op andere plekken en is de basement leeg, maar wat was het leuk om af en toe bij elkaar binnen te kunnen lopen!

Liefste paranimfen, wat fijn dat jullie straks naast me willen zitten op 9 december. Lieve D, je bent mijn oudste vriendinnetje, al ruim 25 jaar! Na de basisschool en middelbare school gingen we elk onze eigen kant op, maar we verloren elkaar nooit uit het oog als vriendinnen. Ik kijk uit naar de Toekomst met jou ;) Op naar de volgende 25 jaar vriendschap!Lieve Lot, mijn allesie, wat hadden we ook al weer allemaal hetzelfde? We startten als collega’s, maar werden al snel fiets-buddies, goede vriendinnetjes en later zelfs huisgenootjes. 15 maart 2013 mocht ik jouw paranimf zijn, ik ben heel blij dat jij nu de mijne bent!

Lieve vriendinnen en vrienden die ik over vele jaren heb verzameld, jullie hebben niet allemaal direct bijgedragen aan dit boekje, maar maken mijn leven leuker, vrolijker, gezelliger! Bedankt dat jullie er zijn! Lieve club, bedankt voor de mooie vakanties en de interesse in wat ik nou eigenlijk allemaal aan het doen ben… Lieve Sen, je bent altijd zo betrokken en geïnteresseerd, fijn dat we ondanks de afstand zoveel contact blijven houden! Chris en Jor, wat fijn om af en toe onze onderzoeken te bespreken met een burger met een biertje! Taac, behalve goede gesprekken tijdens onze wandelingen en onze vaste woensdagavonden, heb je ook de website voor PRISMA en de cover van dit boekje ontworpen, dank je wel!! Lieve Evelien, met jou begon onze passie voor de neuropsychologie, in de UB samen studeren en gefascineerd raken van de wondere wereld van de hersenen. Elske, Ivo en Charlotte, het begon met het organiseren van een Nerdenbal en groeide uit in spelletjesavonden en wekelijkse pubquizzen in Thijssen. Als vierde ben ik nu eindelijk ook (bijna) klaar!

Page 202: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9

R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Dankwoord – Acknowledgements

200

My dearest Monty-family, thank you so much for your never ending interest in how my project is progressing. I am blessed to have you around me, and am thrilled that we are all so much closer to each other now that we moved to London.

Lieve familie, wat zijn we inmiddels een flinke groep met alle gezinnen bij elkaar. Het is een gezellige chaos van 10 volwassenen en 11 kinderen. Wat heerlijk om zo’n grote maar vooral ook fijne familie te hebben! Lieve Sas, Max en Jits, lieve brussen, ik ben enorm blij met jullie en met onze jaarlijkse brussendag. Ik blijf altijd jullie kleine zusje en ik zou t niet anders willen! Lieve pap en mam, bedankt voor jullie veilige basis, het vertrouwen en de vrijheid die jullie me altijd hebben gegeven. En mam, bedankt voor je grenzeloze eigenwijsheid, waaraan ik te danken heb dat ik er überhaupt ben! Jij durfde te vertrouwen op je gevoel dat het goed zat met de baby in je buik, ondanks dat de artsen dachten dat het nooit veel soeps kon worden. Pap, wat gezellig dat je voor je werk regelmatig in de buurt moet zijn en dan altijd even langskomt! Het is altijd fijn met je over mijn promotieonderzoek te praten, ondanks dat we heel andere vakgebieden hebben, jij in de technische natuurkunde en ik in de neuropsychologie. Een promotietraject verloopt toch redelijk hetzelfde. Ik vind het bijzonder dat ik na over-opa, opa en papa nu de eerste vrouw ben die promoveert in de familie! Lieve James, you are my favourite! Thank you for your ‘Impact’ on my PhD! I am so lucky to have found you, my compatible component! Liefste Hanna en Emma, jullie maken mij blij! Ik geniet zo van jullie. I am the luckiest girl in the world!

Page 203: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

R1R2R3R4R5R6R7R8R9R10R11R12R13R14R15R16R17R18R19R20R21R22R23R24R25R26R27R28R29R30R31R32R33R34R35R36R37R38R39

Curriculum Vitae

201

CV

CURRICULUM VITAE

Marieke Anna de Ruiter werd op 8 november 1982 geboren als jongste in een gezin van vier kinderen in Nootdorp, waar zij ook haar jeugd doorbracht. Voor haar middelbare school fietste zij naar het nabijgelegen Voorburg, waar zij in 2001 haar gymnasium diploma behaalde aan het Huygens Lyceum. Na het eindexamen vertrok Marieke naar Groningen om psychologie te studeren aan de Rijksuniversiteit Groningen. In haar derde studiejaar volgde zij enkele keuzevakken aan de Uppsala Universitet in Uppsala, Zweden, waar ze ook een taalcursus Zweeds deed. Marieke koos Hersenen en Gedrag als studierichting voor haar bacheloropleiding, aangezien hersenen en vooral de invloed van hersenschade op bepaalde functies haar fascineerde. Na het behalen van haar bachelor besloot zij de masteropleiding Klinische Neuropsychologie te volgen aan de Vrije Universiteit in Amsterdam, omdat deze opleiding de kans bood een neurologisch vak te volgen aan de geneeskunde faculteit. In 2008 behaalde zij haar master.Na haar afstuderen wilde Marieke even iets heel anders doen, voordat ze op zoek zou gaan naar een baan als promovendus. Daarom solliciteerde ze bij Transavia.com en werd aangenomen om een aantal maanden als stewardess naar verschillende bestemmingen te vliegen. Tijdens deze zomer werd zij aangenomen voor een promotieplek bij de Psychosociale afdeling van het Emma Kinderziekenhuis, om te onderzoeken wat de effecten zijn van neurofeedback training bij kinderen die neurocognitieve problemen hebben als gevolg van (de behandeling voor) een hersentumor; de PRISMA-studie. Maar voordat zij haar promotieonderzoek startte, was er nog tijd om een aantal weken door zuidoost Azië te backpacken. De dag nadat ze terugkwam in Nederland was haar eerste werkdag. Ze werkte in het Emma Kinderziekenhuis met veel plezier in de kelder aan haar promotieonderzoek, en maakte het proefschrift later in haar eigen tijd af. Marieke woont tegenwoordig in Londen, Engeland, met haar man James en hun twee dochtertjes, Hanna en Emma.

Page 204: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association
Page 205: UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Impact of a ... · correlated with white matter integrity in PBTS.5,22 Also, in a study by Smith et al, processing speed mediated the association

UitnodigingVoor het bijwonen van de

openbare verdediging van het proefschrift

Impact of a paediatric brain tumor

Research into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial

consequences and the evaluation of a potential intervention

Door Marieke A. Montgomery-de Ruiter

***

Op vrijdag 9 december om 10.00u in de Agnietenkapel, Oudezijds Voorburgwal 229-231 te Amster-

dam

Aansluitend bent u van harte welkom op de receptie in Frenzi, Zwanenburgwal 232 te Amster-

dam (5 minuten lopen)

***

Marieke Montgomery-deRuiter19e Arundel Gardens

London W11 2LNVerenigd Koningkrijk

+447802588720

ParanimfenLotte Haverman Dominique Bol

[email protected]

Boekenlegger concept 1.indd 1 26-10-16 11:34

Marieke A. Montgomery-de Ruiter

Impact of a Pediatric Brain Tum

or | Marieke A. M

ontgomery-de Ruiter

Impact of a Pediatric

Brain TumorResearch into neurocognitive late effects and psychosocial consequences

and the evaluation of a potential intervention

Cover FINAL.indd 1 26-10-16 11:18