tunnel vision all in the mind at hothouse high

1
CORRESPONDENCE NATURE|Vol 436|11 August 2005 776 Harry Potter and the recessive allele SIR — We are bombarded with news of genetic discoveries on an almost daily basis, but people without a formal knowledge of heredity and genetics can have difficulty in deciphering and applying this information. Education and debate across all ages would undoubtedly help, but how can we teach children these concepts? We believe that successful lessons for younger children can be achieved using analogies of direct interest and relevance. Most children are familiar with J. K. Rowling’s stories about the young wizard Harry Potter (whose latest exploit, Harry Potter and the Half-Blood Prince, was published by Bloomsbury in July). They are set in a world like our own, but populated by a minority of people with supernatural powers (wizards and witches) and a majority of people without (muggles). Wizards or witches can be of any race, and may be the offspring of a wizard and a witch, the offspring of two muggles (‘muggle-born’), or of mixed ancestry (‘half-blood’). This suggests that wizarding ability is inherited in a mendelian fashion, with the wizard allele (W) being recessive to the muggle allele (M). According to this hypothesis, all wizards and witches therefore have two copies of the wizard allele (WW). Harry’s friends Ron Weasley and Neville Longbottom and his arch-enemy Draco Malfoy are ‘pure-blood’ wizards: WW with WW ancestors for generations back. Harry’s friend Hermione is a powerful muggle-born witch (WW with WM parents). Their classmate Seamus is a half-blood wizard, the son of a witch and a muggle (WW with one WW and one WM parent). Harry (WW with WW parents) is not considered a pure-blood, as his mother was muggle-born. There may even be examples of incomplete penetrance (Neville has poor wizarding skills) and possible mutations or questionable paternity: Filch, the caretaker, is a ‘squib’, someone born into a wizarding family but with no wizarding powers of their own. We believe that, with the use of these examples, the concepts of mendelian genetics can be introduced to children as young as five, and then built on by gradually introducing specific terms such as ‘gene’ and ‘allele’, and relating these to chromosomes and DNA. At every stage, the children’s familiarity with the Harry Potter characters can be used as a hook to engage them in discussing concepts of heredity and genetics. Jeffrey M. Craig*‡, Renee Dow†, MaryAnne Aitken†‡ *Chromosome Research, Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Royal Childrens Hospital, †Genetics Education, Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Royal Childrens Hospital, ‡Department of Paediatrics, University of Melbourne, Royal Childrens Hospital, Flemington Road, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia Tunnel vision all in the mind at Hothouse High SIR — Your News Feature “Hothouse High” (Nature 435, 874–875; 2005) quotes current students of Thomas Jefferson High School for Science and Technology as wishing they had more opportunities to take music and other non-science courses. As an alumna from the class of 1993, and a musician, I am dismayed, because this was not the case at all while I attended Jefferson. If anything, there was a strong multidisciplinary approach to teaching, which I believe actually helped those of us who went on to pursue a science career. I disagree with Chris Colin’s statement that “tunnel vision at Jefferson hindered thinking about other interests and career choices” and I suggest that the only tunnel vision at Jefferson in danger of affecting students might be their own. I was saddened by the declaration that two-thirds of the seniors at Jefferson now say they wouldn’t choose it again. Perhaps the wrong students are attending Jefferson for the wrong reasons? There is pressure, especially from parents worried about college admissions, for their children to attend Jefferson regardless of their interest in science. Not to mention that Jefferson is a public school, free to attend and free from the violence endemic to most public schools in the greater Washington DC area. Samantha G. Zeitlin Department of Medicine, Moores–UCSD Cancer Center, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, USA Misconduct: acceptable practices differ by field SIR — The US Office of Science and Technology Policy wisely states, in its definition of misconduct, that there must be a significant departure from the accepted practices of the relevant research community. In their Commentary article on the integrity of science, “Scientists behaving badly” (Nature 435, 737–738; 2005), Brian C. Martinson and colleagues use a survey that blends responses from investigators doing basic discovery science with those doing clinical research, as if the accepted and acceptable practices in these two areas did not differ markedly. However, that is not the case. For instance, in basic science, there are no specific regulations other than keeping research records (misbehaviour no. 16) for a particular time period. In clinical research, on the other hand, good laboratory practice requires certain types of record-keeping, the omission of which is not just careless but potentially sanctionable, as it would place an investigator out of compliance with Federal regulations. Also, in basic research, intuition (misbehaviour no. 15) is an important, and perhaps in the end a researcher’s best, guide to distinguishing between data and noise. In clinical research, by contrast, intuition should never be used for deciding what data can or cannot be included in an experiment. Indeed, every subject counts, and excluding subjects without appropriate documentation would be a serious and sanctionable matter. Finally, Martinson and his colleagues consider “changing the design, methodology or results of a study in response to pressure from a funding source” as a single misbehaviour (misbehaviour no. 10). They ignore the fact that changing research design and methodology — although never the results — is precisely what investigators are supposed to do, to satisfy the criticisms of scientific review groups and the funding agencies that they represent. Frederick Grinnell Ethics in Science and Medicine Program, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, 5323 Harry Hines Boulevard, Dallas, Texas 75390-9039, USA Cannabis on a downer SIR — Why did you use a down arrow to score the recent US Supreme Court decision banning users from growing cannabis at home (“Scorecard” Nature 435, 724; 2005)? The issue of medical marijuana in the United States is controversial. Although there is still no clear definitive scientific evidence for either side of the argument, you seemed to pass judgement on this issue in a one-line statement. Martin J. Neumann Department of Nuclear, Plasma and Radiological Engineering, College of Medicine, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois 61801, USA The arrows used in Sidelines are intended to record changes to the current prospects of the topic under discussion, not to pass an editorial judgement, although in this case their meaning was ambiguous — News Editor, Nature. “With the use of these examples, the concepts of mendelian genetics can be introduced to children as young as five.” — J. M Craig, R. Dow, M. A. Aitken Nature Publishing Group ©2005

Upload: samantha-g

Post on 26-Jul-2016

213 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

© 2005 Nature Publishing Group

CORRESPONDENCE NATURE|Vol 436|11 August 2005

776

Harry Potter and the recessive alleleSIR — We are bombarded with news ofgenetic discoveries on an almost daily basis,but people without a formal knowledge ofheredity and genetics can have difficulty indeciphering and applying this information.Education and debate across all ages wouldundoubtedly help, but how can we teachchildren these concepts?

We believe that successful lessons foryounger children can be achieved usinganalogies of direct interest and relevance.Most children are familiar with J. K. Rowling’sstories about the young wizard Harry Potter(whose latest exploit, Harry Potter and the Half-Blood Prince, was published byBloomsbury in July). They are set in a worldlike our own, but populated by a minority ofpeople with supernatural powers (wizardsand witches) and a majority of peoplewithout (muggles).

Wizards or witches can be of any race, andmay be the offspring of a wizard and a witch,the offspring of two muggles (‘muggle-born’),or of mixed ancestry (‘half-blood’).

This suggests that wizarding ability isinherited in a mendelian fashion, with thewizard allele (W) being recessive to themuggle allele (M). According to thishypothesis, all wizards and witches thereforehave two copies of the wizard allele (WW).Harry’s friends Ron Weasley and NevilleLongbottom and his arch-enemy DracoMalfoy are ‘pure-blood’ wizards: WW withWW ancestors for generations back. Harry’sfriend Hermione is a powerful muggle-bornwitch (WW with WM parents). Theirclassmate Seamus is a half-blood wizard, theson of a witch and a muggle (WW with oneWW and one WM parent). Harry (WW withWW parents) is not considered a pure-blood,as his mother was muggle-born.

There may even be examples of incompletepenetrance (Neville has poor wizardingskills) and possible mutations or questionablepaternity: Filch, the caretaker, is a ‘squib’,someone born into a wizarding family butwith no wizarding powers of their own.

We believe that, with the use of theseexamples, the concepts of mendelian genetics can be introduced to children asyoung as five, and then built on by graduallyintroducing specific terms such as ‘gene’ and‘allele’, and relating these to chromosomesand DNA. At every stage, the children’sfamiliarity with the Harry Potter characters

can be used as a hook to engage them indiscussing concepts of heredity and genetics.Jeffrey M. Craig*‡, Renee Dow†, MaryAnne Aitken†‡*Chromosome Research, Murdoch ChildrensResearch Institute, Royal Childrens Hospital,†Genetics Education, Murdoch ChildrensResearch Institute, Royal Childrens Hospital,‡Department of Paediatrics, University ofMelbourne, Royal Childrens Hospital, Flemington Road, Parkville, Victoria 3052, Australia

Tunnel vision all in themind at Hothouse HighSIR — Your News Feature “Hothouse High”(Nature 435, 874–875; 2005) quotes currentstudents of Thomas Jefferson High School forScience and Technology as wishing they hadmore opportunities to take music and othernon-science courses.

As an alumna from the class of 1993, and a musician, I am dismayed, because this wasnot the case at all while I attended Jefferson. Ifanything, there was a strong multidisciplinaryapproach to teaching, which I believe actuallyhelped those of us who went on to pursue ascience career. I disagree with Chris Colin’sstatement that “tunnel vision at Jeffersonhindered thinking about other interests andcareer choices” and I suggest that the onlytunnel vision at Jefferson in danger ofaffecting students might be their own.

I was saddened by the declaration that two-thirds of the seniors at Jefferson now saythey wouldn’t choose it again. Perhaps thewrong students are attending Jefferson for the wrong reasons? There is pressure,especially from parents worried about college admissions, for their children toattend Jefferson regardless of their interest in science. Not to mention that Jefferson is a public school, free to attend and free fromthe violence endemic to most public schoolsin the greater Washington DC area.Samantha G. ZeitlinDepartment of Medicine, Moores–UCSD CancerCenter, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093, USA

Misconduct: acceptablepractices differ by fieldSIR — The US Office of Science andTechnology Policy wisely states, in itsdefinition of misconduct, that there must be a significant departure from the acceptedpractices of the relevant research community.

In their Commentary article on theintegrity of science, “Scientists behavingbadly” (Nature 435, 737–738; 2005), Brian C. Martinson and colleagues use a survey that

blends responses from investigators doingbasic discovery science with those doingclinical research, as if the accepted andacceptable practices in these two areas did not differ markedly. However, that is not the case.

For instance, in basic science, there are no specific regulations other than keepingresearch records (misbehaviour no. 16) for a particular time period. In clinical research,on the other hand, good laboratory practicerequires certain types of record-keeping, the omission of which is not just careless but potentially sanctionable, as it would place an investigator out of compliance with Federal regulations.

Also, in basic research, intuition(misbehaviour no. 15) is an important, andperhaps in the end a researcher’s best, guideto distinguishing between data and noise. Inclinical research, by contrast, intuition shouldnever be used for deciding what data can orcannot be included in an experiment. Indeed,every subject counts, and excluding subjectswithout appropriate documentation wouldbe a serious and sanctionable matter.

Finally, Martinson and his colleaguesconsider “changing the design, methodologyor results of a study in response to pressure from a funding source” as a singlemisbehaviour (misbehaviour no. 10). Theyignore the fact that changing research designand methodology — although never theresults — is precisely what investigators aresupposed to do, to satisfy the criticisms ofscientific review groups and the fundingagencies that they represent. Frederick Grinnell Ethics in Science and Medicine Program,University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center,5323 Harry Hines Boulevard, Dallas, Texas 75390-9039, USA

Cannabis on a downer SIR — Why did you use a down arrow toscore the recent US Supreme Court decisionbanning users from growing cannabis athome (“Scorecard” Nature 435, 724; 2005)?

The issue of medical marijuana in theUnited States is controversial. Although there is still no clear definitive scientificevidence for either side of the argument, you seemed to pass judgement on this issue in a one-line statement.Martin J. NeumannDepartment of Nuclear, Plasma and RadiologicalEngineering, College of Medicine, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois 61801, USA

The arrows used in Sidelines are intendedto record changes to the current prospectsof the topic under discussion, not to pass an editorial judgement, although in thiscase their meaning was ambiguous — News Editor, Nature.

“With the use of these examples,the concepts of mendelian geneticscan be introduced to children asyoung as five.”

— J. M Craig, R. Dow, M. A. Aitken

11.8 Correspondence 776 NEW McP 9/8/05 3:36 PM Page 776

Nature Publishing Group© 2005