training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a uk survey

16
21 © 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare The Old School, Brewhouse Hill, Wheathampstead, Hertfordshire AL4 8AN, UK Animal Welfare 2007, 16: 21-36 ISSN 0962-7286 Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey MJ Prescott* and HM Buchanan-Smith National Centre for the Replacement, Refinement and Reduction of Animals in Research (NC3Rs), 20 Park Crescent, London W1B 1AL, UK Department of Psychology, University of Stirling, Stirling FK9 4LA, Scotland, UK * Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints: [email protected] Abstract Training using positive reinforcement is increasingly recognised as a valuable tool for the humane and effective management and use of laboratory-housed non-human primates. We utilised a mixed-mode questionnaire to survey use of training and other learning processes (socialisation, habituation and desensitisation) in over half of UK establishments using and breeding primates. The survey demonstrated that there is widespread awareness of training as a refinement technique and appreciation of its diverse benefits, but training is not used as widely or as fully as it might be. This is due to real constraints (principally staff and time and a lack of confi- dence in ability to train), and perceived constraints (such as a supposed lack of published information on how to train and assess- ment of the benefits, and an overestimation of the time investment needed). There is also considerable variation between establishments in the purposes of training and techniques used, with a reliance on negative reinforcement in some. We conclude that there is opportunity for refinement of common scientific, veterinary and husbandry procedures (such as blood and urine collection, injection, capture from the group and weighing) through use of positive reinforcement training, especially when combined with appro- priate socialisation, habituation and desensitisation. We end this paper with recommendations on best practice, training techniques and staff education. Keywords: animal welfare, habituation, positive reinforcement, refinement, socialisation, training Introduction Laboratory-housed non-human primates may experience a range of potential stressors, including capture, cage-change, physical and chemical restraint, injection and venepuncture. Training them to co-operate, using positive reinforcement training (PRT) techniques which reward desired behaviour (see Table 1), is one means of significantly reducing the adverse impact of such procedures upon them, and is recommended as good practice by many legislative and professional guidelines (eg Home Office 1989; International Primatological Society 1989; National Research Council 1998; Laboratory Animal Science Association/Medical Research Council 2004; Medical Research Council 2004). Published literature demonstrates that primates can be trained to co-operate with a wide variety of procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) and that they maintain a high degree of reliability in participating in such events, which is particularly important when working to fixed-time points for sampling (Schapiro 2000; Prescott & Buchanan-Smith 2003). Trained animals show reductions in cortisol levels, stress-related behaviours, stress-related abortions, physical resistance to handling, and fear responses such as fear-grinning, screaming and acute diarrhoea (Elvidge et al 1976; Mitchell et al 1980; Moseley & Davis 1989; Reinhardt et al 1990; Reinhardt 1991, 2003; Luttrell et al 1994; Bassett et al 2003; McKinley et al 2003; Videan et al 2005). Training can also enhance the care and well-being of captive primates, for example, by providing a means to reduce abnormal behaviour, enhance positive social interaction and facilitate health inspection and treatment (Laule et al 2003). Refinement techniques that reduce or eliminate adverse effects for animals used in scientific research (Russell & Burch 1959) not only benefit animal welfare, but can also enhance the quality of the research. Suffering in animals can result in physiological responses that are, at least, likely to increase variability in experimental data and, at worst, may invalidate the research; but training can minimise such responses (Schnell & Gerber 1997; Schapiro 2000; Reinhardt 1991, 2003, 2004; Hassimoto et al 2004; Lambeth et al 2004; Schapiro et al 2005). Techniques that reduce sources of variability also have the potential of reducing the number of animals required in a given protocol (Biological Council 1992; Brockway et al 1993; Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002). Training can also have significant benefits for staff by improving the ease, speed and safety with which procedures can be performed (Heath 1989; Bloomsmith 1992; Luttrell Universities Federation for Animal Welfare Science in the Service of Animal Welfare

Upload: phungxuyen

Post on 01-Jan-2017

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

21

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal WelfareThe Old School Brewhouse Hill WheathampsteadHertfordshire AL4 8AN UK

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36ISSN 0962-7286

Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part 1 a UK survey

MJ Prescottdagger and HM Buchanan-SmithDagger

dagger National Centre for the Replacement Refinement and Reduction of Animals in Research (NC3Rs) 20 Park Crescent LondonW1B 1AL UKDagger Department of Psychology University of Stirling Stirling FK9 4LA Scotland UK Contact for correspondence and requests for reprints markprescottnc3rsorguk

Abstract

Training using positive reinforcement is increasingly recognised as a valuable tool for the humane and effective management and useof laboratory-housed non-human primates We utilised a mixed-mode questionnaire to survey use of training and other learningprocesses (socialisation habituation and desensitisation) in over half of UK establishments using and breeding primates The surveydemonstrated that there is widespread awareness of training as a refinement technique and appreciation of its diverse benefits buttraining is not used as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to real constraints (principally staff and time and a lack of confi-dence in ability to train) and perceived constraints (such as a supposed lack of published information on how to train and assess-ment of the benefits and an overestimation of the time investment needed) There is also considerable variation betweenestablishments in the purposes of training and techniques used with a reliance on negative reinforcement in some We conclude thatthere is opportunity for refinement of common scientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such as blood and urine collectioninjection capture from the group and weighing) through use of positive reinforcement training especially when combined with appro-priate socialisation habituation and desensitisation We end this paper with recommendations on best practice training techniquesand staff education

Keywords animal welfare habituation positive reinforcement refinement socialisation training

IntroductionLaboratory-housed non-human primates may experience arange of potential stressors including capture cage-changephysical and chemical restraint injection and venepunctureTraining them to co-operate using positive reinforcementtraining (PRT) techniques which reward desired behaviour(see Table 1) is one means of significantly reducing theadverse impact of such procedures upon them and isrecommended as good practice by many legislative andprofessional guidelines (eg Home Office 1989International Primatological Society 1989 NationalResearch Council 1998 Laboratory Animal ScienceAssociationMedical Research Council 2004 MedicalResearch Council 2004) Published literature demonstratesthat primates can be trained to co-operate with a widevariety of procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) and that theymaintain a high degree of reliability in participating in suchevents which is particularly important when working tofixed-time points for sampling (Schapiro 2000 Prescott ampBuchanan-Smith 2003) Trained animals show reductions incortisol levels stress-related behaviours stress-relatedabortions physical resistance to handling and fearresponses such as fear-grinning screaming and acutediarrhoea (Elvidge et al 1976 Mitchell et al 1980 Moseley

amp Davis 1989 Reinhardt et al 1990 Reinhardt 1991 2003Luttrell et al 1994 Bassett et al 2003 McKinley et al 2003Videan et al 2005) Training can also enhance the care andwell-being of captive primates for example by providing ameans to reduce abnormal behaviour enhance positivesocial interaction and facilitate health inspection andtreatment (Laule et al 2003)Refinement techniques that reduce or eliminate adverseeffects for animals used in scientific research (Russell ampBurch 1959) not only benefit animal welfare but can alsoenhance the quality of the research Suffering in animals canresult in physiological responses that are at least likely toincrease variability in experimental data and at worst mayinvalidate the research but training can minimise suchresponses (Schnell amp Gerber 1997 Schapiro 2000Reinhardt 1991 2003 2004 Hassimoto et al 2004Lambeth et al 2004 Schapiro et al 2005) Techniques thatreduce sources of variability also have the potential ofreducing the number of animals required in a given protocol(Biological Council 1992 Brockway et al 1993 ScientificCommittee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002)Training can also have significant benefits for staff byimproving the ease speed and safety with which procedurescan be performed (Heath 1989 Bloomsmith 1992 Luttrell

Universities Federation for Animal Welfare Science in the Service of Animal Welfare

22 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

et al 1994 Reinhardt 1997 2003 McKinley et al 2003 Iliffet al 2004) Furthermore the additional time that staff spendwith the primates and the need for individual recognitionand close observation of animal behaviour means that thetrainer develops a relationship with each individualanimal which can be beneficial for animal welfare (eg byreducing the occurrence of abnormal behaviour orthrough a reduction of fear of humans and improvedability to cope with routine husbandry procedures (Bayneet al 1993 Waitt et al 2002 Bassett et al 2003 Baker2004 McKinley 2004 Bourgeois amp Brent 2005) This isespecially likely to be the case when PRT is used Thebuilding of a primate-trainer bond has also been reportedto raise staff morale and lead to positive changes in theattitude of staff to the animals involved (Bayne et al 1993Bloomsmith et al 1997 Bayne 2002)Given the potential benefits of training primates to animalsscience and staff one might expect training to be used in alllaboratories that use and breed primates However imple-menting training may be difficult in some cases Laule et al(2003) write that staff may have to work with manyanimals and populations may change frequently shortnotice may be given for research programmes allowinglittle time for training of animals and the research mayinvolve situations where PRT is difficult to implementwithin restrictions on enrichment options social interac-tions physical activity and food amounts In addition thereare few opportunities for staff education about traininganimals and worries about knowing how to train can havea stalling effect If solutions can be found to suchconstraints on use of training then this could result insignificant reductions in animal stress and improvements inanimal welfare and ultimately better researchHere we report the results of a survey designed to evaluatethe extent to which primates are trained in a range of UKresearch and breeding establishments designated under theAnimals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The survey wasinitiated as part of a programme of work on refinement bythe Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals(RSPCA) and following discussions with the UniversitiesFederation for Animal Welfare Pharmaceutical Housing andHusbandry Steering Committee (UFAW PHHSC) and thePrimate Sub-Committee of the Animal ProceduresCommittee The survey also explored socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation (see Table 1) sincethe term lsquotrainingrsquo is used colloquially in the UK to refer tothese learning processes as well as to the shaping of ananimalrsquos behaviour so that it actively responds in a way thatis desired by the trainer These processes have also beenreported to improve the well-being of captive primates(Moseley amp Davis 1989 Bayne et al 1993 Heath 1989Bloomsmith et al 1999 Laule et al 2003 Baker 2004)The key objectives of the survey werebull to create a database of current knowledge and practice oftraining socialisation with humans habituation and desen-sitisationbull to identify good practice

bull to identify obstacles and solutions to further uptake ofthese processesThe overall aim was to use the results of the survey to facil-itate application of these processes to refine the use andbreeding of laboratory-housed primates

Materials and methodsBetween April and December 2004 15 establishments werevisited comprising six universities three government estab-lishments or pharmaceutical companies three contractresearch organisations (CROs) and three breeding establish-ments This represented over half of all designated estab-lishments using primates at that time and includes mostmajor users Thirteen establishments housed macaques(rhesus macaque Macaca mulatta long-tailed macaque Mfascicularis stump-tailed macaque M arctoides) and fiveestablishments housed common marmosets (Callithrixjacchus)The survey took the form of a face-to-face interview basedon a mixed-mode questionnaire This allowed for morecomprehensive and accurate data collection than is possiblewith a postal-based survey method A total of 32 peopleparticipated in the survey including scientists animal tech-nicians Named Animal Care and Welfare Officers NamedVeterinary Surgeons and facility managers Participants hadprimary responsibility for co-ordinating and conducting thetraining socialisation habituation and desensitisation of theprimates or else had primary responsibility for the welfareof the primates Fixed and open response questions wereused to collect quantitative and qualitative data on trainingknowledge and practice The Likert technique (Likert 1932)was used to measure attitudes The terms in Table 1 weredefined and explained before the interview Preliminaryresults of the survey for some questions on training only arereported in Prescott et al (2005b)

Results and discussion

Incidence of training socialisation habituation anddesensitisationTable 2 shows incidence of training socialisation habitua-tion and desensitisation broken down by establishmenttype A total of 11 out of 15 establishments train primatesalthough sometimes using negative reinforcement training(NRT) (see Table 1) Of these 11 six have formal trainingprogrammes where records are kept These are Universitiesconducting physiological and psychological research whereprimates are trained to perform tasks to generate data andsuch training is integral to the research The other establish-ments train on an ad hoc basis for some species groups ofanimals and procedures onlyOne of the first steps in training animals is to socialise themwith humans so that interactions that occur during trainingcan be as stress free as possible Socialisation with humansalso facilitates monitoring of health and welfare without theneed to capture animals and remove them from their enclo-sures We therefore included socialisation with humans inour definition (which normally refers only to conspecifics

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 23

see Table 1) All 15 establishments felt that primatesbecome familiar with human behaviour as an inevitableconsequence of their housing and husbandry in captivityHowever four establishments (two CROs and two breedingestablishments) have a formal socialisation programmewhere time is set aside for positive interaction betweenhumans and animals and records are kept Formalprogrammes are beneficial to ensure that socialisation withhumans does not slip when staff is busy and the records canbe reviewed to check the progress of individual animals andthe programme as a wholeAll establishments with the exception of two breedingestablishments use habituation and all save one breedingestablishment and two universities use desensitisationThese universities felt that their management and use ofprimates is such that uncomfortable or aversive experiencesfor the animals are avoided and therefore use of desensitisa-tion is not necessaryOpportunity for and feasibility of use of the learningprocesses and in particular training depends on a numberof factors such as the numbers of animals held used orbred staff to animal ratio age of animals at first use andduration of their use and group size We were interested inlooking for gross patterns between these factors (Table 3)and use of the training (Table 2) Universities typically havesmall numbers and groups of animals that are used forresearch projects lasting many years and high staff toanimal ratios One might expect these characteristics tofacilitate use of training and this appears to be the caseHowever all three CROs surveyed also utilise training

primarily of macaques to enter a transport cage Thisfinding is encouraging given that CROs hold large numbersof animals and use large numbers quickly (eg short-termtoxicology studies) and at a young age They do howeverhave a relatively high staff to animal ratio which mayassist trainingBreeding establishments may not use training and habitua-tion because they do not conduct many scientific procedures(two breeding establishments conduct a limited amount ofresearch or testing) hold large numbers of animalssometimes in large groups and have a relatively low staff toanimal ratio However there is opportunity for training andhabituation of animals in breeding establishments toencourage co-operation with husbandry and veterinaryprocedures particularly for breeding animals that are keptfor a long time (eg Reinhardt 1990 Luttrell et al 1994) Onemacaque breeding establishment has now begun to trainanimals to stand for injectionFive customers of breeding establishments reported thatthey were keen for them to invest more in socialisationhabituation and training of primates destined for use inresearch so that the animals are more tractable and lessstressed when issued for use with the consequence that theresearch can progress more quickly and easily Moreovertemperament has been found to correlate with trainingsuccess for rhesus macaques (Coleman et al 2005) Itshould therefore be possible for breeding establishments toscreen primates to be assigned to research projects in whichthey will be trained with the goal of obtaining the bestcandidates for those studies Socialisation habituation and

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 1 Definitions of terms used in the survey

1 Note that these processes may occur concurrently2 Defined in the Oxford English Dictionary Online as ldquoThe process of forming associations or of adapting oneself to them especially theprocess whereby an individual acquires the modifications of behaviour and the values necessary for the stability of the social group ofwhich he is or becomes a memberrdquo

Term DefinitionTraining1 The shaping of the behaviour of a primate so that it actively responds in a way that is desired by the train-

er (eg offers a limb for injection stands on a weighing scale waits and allows subordinate individuals tofeed uninterrupted)

Socialisation1 2 The process by which a primate learns how to successfully interact with members of its own species andwith other species (eg humans) with which it co-habits

Habituation1 The waning of a response as a result of repeated stimulation but not fatigue This kind of learning is ofimportance in familiarising a primate with aspects of the environment to which it is inconvenient for it toreact to It is of value in encouraging primates to ignore non-threatening stimuli (eg the sound of clippersrestraint in a sling confinement in a transport container)

Desensitisation1 Systematically pairing positive reward directly with an uncomfortable or aversive experience or stimulus inorder to reduce any associated fear or anxiety response

Positive reinforcement Frequency of a behaviour is increased because something positive is obtained on its performance (eg foodtreat verbal praise tactile contact)

Negative reinforcement Frequency of a behaviour is increased because something negative is removed on its performance (eg cagesqueeze-back mechanism)

Punishment Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something negative is introduced on its performance (egverbal command lsquoNorsquo)

24 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

training programmes in conjunction with temperamenttesting are becoming an accepted part of the preparation oflaboratory dogs for their life on study (Heath et al 2002Adams et al 2004) Dogs that have undergone limitedsocialisation show greater variation in behaviour (includingduring mock procedures) compared with dogs that haveexperienced intensive socialisation (Boxall et al 2004)Decisions about whether to utilise training to refine a partic-ular procedure often depend on a cost-benefit analysisweighing on the one hand the potential for animal sufferingassociated with the traditional method and on the otherhand the likely staff and time investment involved with thetraining Different types of establishment may reachdifferent decisions depending on the frequency with whichanimals undergo the procedure and the method used Forexample the frequency with which blood is taken frommacaques ranged from once per lifetime involving a cagesqueeze-back mechanism andor anaesthesia (most univer-sities and breeding establishments) to up to 24 times in a48 h or much longer period using manual restraint (CROs)Time before data collection begins is short for CROs (mean= 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks for marmosets) incontrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks for macaques)(Table 6) We believe training for co-operation to be aworthwhile investment in the latter case to reduce animalstress and data variability

Moreover depending on their respective responsibilitiesdifferent types of staff may reach different decisions on theneed for animals to be trained Animal technicians andscientists are the staff most responsible for driving trainingat the establishments surveyed (Table 4) It is surprising thatfew establishments identified the Home Office Inspectorateand local ethical review process (ERP) as drivers of animaltraining given the responsibilities of the Inspectorate andERP for reviewing research proposals involving animalsencouraging the widest possible application of the 3Rs andadvising on high standards of animal care and welfare(Home Office 2000)

Constraints on trainingAll establishments believe training can be a refinementEleven out of 15 establishments have acknowledged this forover 10 years and eight for over 15-25 years Thirteen outof 15 establishments agree that training of primates is afeasible concept for them one disagrees and one neitheragrees nor disagrees ndash largely because of time constraintsOf the 13 that agree training is feasible three do not trainpresently so there may be opportunity for refinement atthese establishments Furthermore all of the establishmentswho do not train identified instances where training wouldbenefit animals and staff so again there may be opportunityfor refinement at these establishments if they can overcomethe constraints they face

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 2 Incidence of training socialisation habituation and desensitisation by establishment type

Learningprocess

University (n = 6)

Government or phar-maceutical (n = 3)

Contractresearch (n = 3)

Breeding (n = 3)

Total(n = 15)

Formal programme

Training 6 2 3 0 11 611

Socialisation withhumans

6 3 3 3 15 415

Habituation 6 3 3 1 13 413

Desensitisation 4 3 3 2 12 -

Table 3 Characteristics of establishments surveyed

Characteristic University (n = 6)

Government or pharmaceutical (n = 3)

Contract research(n = 3)

Breeding (n = 3)

Number of animals held (range) 2-15 148-380 109-678 220-860

Number of animals used peryear1 (range)

05-5 15-35 150-654 0-30

Number of animals bred peryear2 (range)

0-1 0-106 na 30-400

Mean staff to animal ratio 12 145 120 170

Age at first use (range) 3-24 months 3-24 months 12-36 months 18-24 months

Duration of use (range) 3 months - 10years

3 months - 7 years 1 week - 1 year 15-20 years (breeding) 1 week - 2years (research)

Group size (range) 1-5 1-30 1-30 1-100

1 euthanased2 production

Training non-human primates a UK survey 25

Reported constraints on training are shown in Table 5 thereare both real and perceived constraints For example fiveestablishments including one that trains cite lack of staff asa constraint and some establishments reportedly lack timebefore data collection begins andor confidence in theirability to train In addition establishments report a paucityof information on how to train and reliable assessment ofthe benefits and some overestimate the time investmentinvolved in training Arguably the problem is not lack ofpublished information on these points but lack ofknowledge about where to find it (see Prescott et al 2005a)CROs (mean = 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks formarmosets) in contrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks formacaques) (Table 6) In addition CROs have larger andmore batches of animals per year These factors maketraining programmes at CROs more resource intensiveMoreover CROs use younger animals sourced fromoverseas breeding establishments Habituation desensitisa-tion and training were reported as being difficult with younganimals if they are poorly socialised with humans because

they become easily stressed and will not take foodHowever macaques at CROs can be and are trained espe-cially during long-term studies where studies are staggeredor where the animals are imported well in advance ofstudies to be held as stock and then used sequentiallyEstablishments reported variation in the speed with whichanimals acclimatised following relocation and transportsuch that their temperament and behaviour were suitable fortraining more than 4 weeks was reported as necessary insome cases Breeding in-house and frequent and timelycommunication between breederssuppliers and users ofprimates will help facilitate acclimatisation socialisationhabituation and training

Resources reported as required in order for establishmentsto begin to train or to expand existing training programmesare shown in Table 7 These map quite closely to theconstraints reported in Table 5 The second part of this paperaims to help facilitate use of training by improving access tosome of these resources (see Prescott et al 2005a)

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 4 Drivers of training as described by those surveyed

Driver Total (n = 15)

Animal technicians 8

Scientists 6

Veterinarians 4

Scientific literature 3

Needs of the experiment 3

Animal welfare organisations 3

Ethical review process 2

Home Office Inspectorate 2Legislation 1Professional guidelines 1Training specialists 1

Customers of breeding establishments 1Culture of care 1

Table 5 Constraints on training as described by those surveyed

Constraint Do train (n = 15)

Do not train have tried(n = 2)

Do not train havenot tried (n = 15)

Total (n = 15)

Paucity of information on how to train 3 1 2 6

Lack of staff 1 1 3 5

Perceived time investment 2 - 1 3

Lack of time before data collection 2 - 1 3

Lack of assessment of benefits - 1 2 3

Lack of confidence in ability to train 2 - 1 3

Possible effects on animal health 1 - - 1Concern about staff health and safety - - 1 1

Concern about effects on scientific validity - - - -

Concern about effect on animal behaviour - - - -

26 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Purposes of trainingThe purposes for which establishments wish to train andwhat they train for presently are shown in Table 8Establishments would like to train macaques for injectionvenepuncture and topical application and marmosets forrestraint and urine sampling because the animals oftenundergo these procedures There are published papersavailable for these procedures and others many withtraining protocols (see Prescott et al 2005a) Furthermoresome establishments already train for these purposes Thisillustrates that there is a need for better communicationbetween establishments for sharing of information onrefinement techniques

For instance we are aware of 10 published papers ontraining macaques to voluntarily offer a limb forvenepuncture Reinhardt (2003) has shown a two-foldincrease in serum cortisol with the traditional manualrestraint method and no increase in serum cortisol with analternative trained method using a combination of PRTand NRT The initial time investment in training(mean = 38 minutes per animal) quickly pays off in a safehandling procedure that no longer requires a secondperson to control the animal while the blood sample istaken Laule et al (2003) give a protocol that utilises PRTonly and does not feature the cage squeeze-backmechanism utilised in Reinhardtrsquos protocol

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 6 Source and arrivals of animals at establishment surveyed

1 Breeding stock may be occasionally supplemented with new animals to increase genetic diversity

Macaques (n =13 ) Marmosets (n = 5)

University Government orpharmaceutical

Contractresearch

Breeding university Government orpharmaceutical

Contractreserarch

breeding

Source of animals

Bred in-house1 1 2 - 2 - 3 - 1

UK breeding establishment

5 - - - - - 1 -

Overseas breedingestablishment

- - 3 - - - - -

Arrivals from externalestablishments

Size of batch 2-4 (mean = 24)

- 4-60 (mean = 31)

- - - 10-52 -

Number of batchesper year

0-1 (mean = 1)

- 3-12 (mean = 63)

- - - 3 -

Age on arrival(months)

18-36 (mean = 228)

- 12-36 (mean = 18)

- - - 14-18 -

Length of acclimati-sation period beforestudy begins (weeks)

24-48 (mean = 36)

- 3-8 (mean = 52)

- - - 6 -

Table 7 Resource requirements for training as described by those surveyed

Resource Total (n = 15)

Audio-visual materials on how to train effectively 6

Written guidance on how to train effectively 6

In person demonstration of training practice 6

More staff 6

More time before studies begin 5

Access to a primate behaviour specialist 5

Wider access to reference material 5

Dedicated staff-training course with practical component 5

More money 3

Face-to-face discussion with experts 2

Secure database on training 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 27

With regard to husbandry procedures nine establishmentstrain their macaques for co-operation with capture shiftinglocation and weighing This involves using a combinationof PRT and NRT to train animals to enter a transport cageor in two cases a primate chair Once trained animals canbe caught swiftly and without undue distress (Reinhardt1992) which can then facilitate veterinary and breedingmanagement husbandry and research Use of training forthis purpose in the UK is in contrast to a 1989 United Statescensus conducted at the National Institutes of Health whichreported that ldquoonly 9 of (56) scientists interviewedcurrently train their animals to enter transport cagesrdquo(Bayne 1989 cited in Reinhardt 1991) The frequency with

which macaques are captured varies from five times per day(CRO) to once every 6 months (University) depending onthe purpose of capture (eg weighing or procedures) age ofthe animal and individual study requirementsFew establishments train marmosets (25) compared withmacaques (913) yet staff members have a high desire totrain marmosets particularly for husbandry and healthprocedures Marmosets may be trained less because of theirsmall size such that they can be easily handled andtherefore present less of a danger to personnel In additionfewer marmosets may be trained because of other consider-ations for example compared to macaques marmosetcolonies are often larger marmosets have shorter life spans

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 8 Purposes of training as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Purpose Already train Would like to train Already train Would like to train

Scientific procedures

Venepuncture 2 2 - 1Injection - 6 - -Urine sampling - 1 1 2

Saliva sampling - - - -

Faecal sampling - 1 - -

Topical application 1 2 1 -

Restraint 6 (chair) 1 - 3

Oral administration 2 - - 2

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Touch screen 2 - 1 -

Lever press 1 - 1 -

Joystick 1 - - -

Eye tracking 1 - - -

Finger press 1 - - -

Husbandry procedures

Weighing 9 1 1 2

Shifting location 9 - 1 1

Collar cleaning - - - 1

Capture 9 1 - 2

Separation 3 - - 1

Station - 2 - 3

Cooperative feeding - 1 - 1

Health procedures

Palpation - 1 - 2

Stethoscope - - - 2Joint manipulation 1 (hand) - - 2

Infant care - - - 2

Mouth inspectionteeth cleaning - 1 - 3

Temperature - 1 - 2

Ultrasound - - - 2

X-ray - - - -

28 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

and large series of blood samples cannot be taken from thesame individual (Buchanan-Smith et al 2003a b) Howeverthere is growing evidence that marmosets can be trained toco-operate in a range of tasks using only PRT and with noneed for food or water management (eg McKinley et al2003 Scott et al 2003 Smith et al 2004) The typical tasksinclude training to co-operate in scientific procedures andtests of cognitive ability and to facilitate husbandry andveterinary procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) Forexample marmosets have been trained to stand on a balancefor in home-cage weighing and to urinate into a collectionvial on request (McKinley et al 2003) One advantage ofthis training is that it avoids the need for capture andrestraint which can be stressful for primates (Reinhardtet al 1990 1995 National Research Council 1998 Saucedaamp Schmidt 2000)Only one establishment of the five housing marmosets hastrained its marmosets for in home-cage weighing At theremainder marmosets are caught for weighing (one perweek to once per month) or scientific procedures (daily orless frequently) either by chasing them into a nest box or byhand using a gauntlet or surgical glove Although someanimals if exposed often enough to capture and restraintappear to acquiesce andor tolerate this practice there arephysiological data which demonstrate that restraint canremain stressful for marmosets and macaques even whenthe animals are habituated to the procedure over a longperiod (Mann 1991 Morrow-Tesch et al 1993 Schnell ampGerber 1997) this can have implications for the dataobtained from such animals

Training practice and staff educationJunior and senior animal technicians and scientists are theindividuals most commonly responsible for determining theneed to train primates and for conducting training of theanimals (Table 9) Regarding techniques used to train thereare a variety of schedules of behaviour modification (seeTable 1) Positive reinforcement is generally considered themost humane of these options (Laule 1999 Laule et al2003 Pryor 2002) It involves voluntary co-operation forpleasurable reward rather than coercion via the threat of anegative event or experience and therefore gives the animalgreater control over the interaction which is a desirablescenario for the well-being of captive animals (Weiss 1968)Nine out of 15 establishments use both positive andnegative reinforcement sometimes in the context of thesame procedure and three establishments use negative rein-forcement only (see Table 10) which is a cause for concernNo establishments used clicker-training We recommendthat training methods should be based on positive reinforce-ment and that negative reinforcement should only be usedwhen positive alternatives have been shown to be ineffec-tive If NRT must be used (eg for training aversive proce-dures) it should be used in combination with PRT(McKinley 2004)The most common positive reinforcers are verbal praise andfood (Table 11) although the efficacy of verbal praise as areinforcer has not been shown Preferred foods can behighly motivating but foods used for training need to becounted in the nutritional content of the diet (Scott 1990)and junk food should be avoided The most common

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 9 Responsibilities for training (n = 15) as described by those surveyed

Technique used Total (n = 15)Positive and negative reinforcement 9

Negative reinforcement only 3Food or water management 4

Positive reinforcement only 2

Negative punishment1 2Positive punishment2 1Clicker training -

Responsible for determining the needto train primates

Responsible for training primates

Junior animal technician 10 12Senior animal technician 12 7Scientist 8 8

Veterinarian 5 -Ethical review process 3 -Customer of breeding establishment 2

Table 10 Techniques used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something pleasant is removed on its performance 2 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something unpleasant is introduced on its performance

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 2: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

22 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

et al 1994 Reinhardt 1997 2003 McKinley et al 2003 Iliffet al 2004) Furthermore the additional time that staff spendwith the primates and the need for individual recognitionand close observation of animal behaviour means that thetrainer develops a relationship with each individualanimal which can be beneficial for animal welfare (eg byreducing the occurrence of abnormal behaviour orthrough a reduction of fear of humans and improvedability to cope with routine husbandry procedures (Bayneet al 1993 Waitt et al 2002 Bassett et al 2003 Baker2004 McKinley 2004 Bourgeois amp Brent 2005) This isespecially likely to be the case when PRT is used Thebuilding of a primate-trainer bond has also been reportedto raise staff morale and lead to positive changes in theattitude of staff to the animals involved (Bayne et al 1993Bloomsmith et al 1997 Bayne 2002)Given the potential benefits of training primates to animalsscience and staff one might expect training to be used in alllaboratories that use and breed primates However imple-menting training may be difficult in some cases Laule et al(2003) write that staff may have to work with manyanimals and populations may change frequently shortnotice may be given for research programmes allowinglittle time for training of animals and the research mayinvolve situations where PRT is difficult to implementwithin restrictions on enrichment options social interac-tions physical activity and food amounts In addition thereare few opportunities for staff education about traininganimals and worries about knowing how to train can havea stalling effect If solutions can be found to suchconstraints on use of training then this could result insignificant reductions in animal stress and improvements inanimal welfare and ultimately better researchHere we report the results of a survey designed to evaluatethe extent to which primates are trained in a range of UKresearch and breeding establishments designated under theAnimals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The survey wasinitiated as part of a programme of work on refinement bythe Royal Society for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals(RSPCA) and following discussions with the UniversitiesFederation for Animal Welfare Pharmaceutical Housing andHusbandry Steering Committee (UFAW PHHSC) and thePrimate Sub-Committee of the Animal ProceduresCommittee The survey also explored socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation (see Table 1) sincethe term lsquotrainingrsquo is used colloquially in the UK to refer tothese learning processes as well as to the shaping of ananimalrsquos behaviour so that it actively responds in a way thatis desired by the trainer These processes have also beenreported to improve the well-being of captive primates(Moseley amp Davis 1989 Bayne et al 1993 Heath 1989Bloomsmith et al 1999 Laule et al 2003 Baker 2004)The key objectives of the survey werebull to create a database of current knowledge and practice oftraining socialisation with humans habituation and desen-sitisationbull to identify good practice

bull to identify obstacles and solutions to further uptake ofthese processesThe overall aim was to use the results of the survey to facil-itate application of these processes to refine the use andbreeding of laboratory-housed primates

Materials and methodsBetween April and December 2004 15 establishments werevisited comprising six universities three government estab-lishments or pharmaceutical companies three contractresearch organisations (CROs) and three breeding establish-ments This represented over half of all designated estab-lishments using primates at that time and includes mostmajor users Thirteen establishments housed macaques(rhesus macaque Macaca mulatta long-tailed macaque Mfascicularis stump-tailed macaque M arctoides) and fiveestablishments housed common marmosets (Callithrixjacchus)The survey took the form of a face-to-face interview basedon a mixed-mode questionnaire This allowed for morecomprehensive and accurate data collection than is possiblewith a postal-based survey method A total of 32 peopleparticipated in the survey including scientists animal tech-nicians Named Animal Care and Welfare Officers NamedVeterinary Surgeons and facility managers Participants hadprimary responsibility for co-ordinating and conducting thetraining socialisation habituation and desensitisation of theprimates or else had primary responsibility for the welfareof the primates Fixed and open response questions wereused to collect quantitative and qualitative data on trainingknowledge and practice The Likert technique (Likert 1932)was used to measure attitudes The terms in Table 1 weredefined and explained before the interview Preliminaryresults of the survey for some questions on training only arereported in Prescott et al (2005b)

Results and discussion

Incidence of training socialisation habituation anddesensitisationTable 2 shows incidence of training socialisation habitua-tion and desensitisation broken down by establishmenttype A total of 11 out of 15 establishments train primatesalthough sometimes using negative reinforcement training(NRT) (see Table 1) Of these 11 six have formal trainingprogrammes where records are kept These are Universitiesconducting physiological and psychological research whereprimates are trained to perform tasks to generate data andsuch training is integral to the research The other establish-ments train on an ad hoc basis for some species groups ofanimals and procedures onlyOne of the first steps in training animals is to socialise themwith humans so that interactions that occur during trainingcan be as stress free as possible Socialisation with humansalso facilitates monitoring of health and welfare without theneed to capture animals and remove them from their enclo-sures We therefore included socialisation with humans inour definition (which normally refers only to conspecifics

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 23

see Table 1) All 15 establishments felt that primatesbecome familiar with human behaviour as an inevitableconsequence of their housing and husbandry in captivityHowever four establishments (two CROs and two breedingestablishments) have a formal socialisation programmewhere time is set aside for positive interaction betweenhumans and animals and records are kept Formalprogrammes are beneficial to ensure that socialisation withhumans does not slip when staff is busy and the records canbe reviewed to check the progress of individual animals andthe programme as a wholeAll establishments with the exception of two breedingestablishments use habituation and all save one breedingestablishment and two universities use desensitisationThese universities felt that their management and use ofprimates is such that uncomfortable or aversive experiencesfor the animals are avoided and therefore use of desensitisa-tion is not necessaryOpportunity for and feasibility of use of the learningprocesses and in particular training depends on a numberof factors such as the numbers of animals held used orbred staff to animal ratio age of animals at first use andduration of their use and group size We were interested inlooking for gross patterns between these factors (Table 3)and use of the training (Table 2) Universities typically havesmall numbers and groups of animals that are used forresearch projects lasting many years and high staff toanimal ratios One might expect these characteristics tofacilitate use of training and this appears to be the caseHowever all three CROs surveyed also utilise training

primarily of macaques to enter a transport cage Thisfinding is encouraging given that CROs hold large numbersof animals and use large numbers quickly (eg short-termtoxicology studies) and at a young age They do howeverhave a relatively high staff to animal ratio which mayassist trainingBreeding establishments may not use training and habitua-tion because they do not conduct many scientific procedures(two breeding establishments conduct a limited amount ofresearch or testing) hold large numbers of animalssometimes in large groups and have a relatively low staff toanimal ratio However there is opportunity for training andhabituation of animals in breeding establishments toencourage co-operation with husbandry and veterinaryprocedures particularly for breeding animals that are keptfor a long time (eg Reinhardt 1990 Luttrell et al 1994) Onemacaque breeding establishment has now begun to trainanimals to stand for injectionFive customers of breeding establishments reported thatthey were keen for them to invest more in socialisationhabituation and training of primates destined for use inresearch so that the animals are more tractable and lessstressed when issued for use with the consequence that theresearch can progress more quickly and easily Moreovertemperament has been found to correlate with trainingsuccess for rhesus macaques (Coleman et al 2005) Itshould therefore be possible for breeding establishments toscreen primates to be assigned to research projects in whichthey will be trained with the goal of obtaining the bestcandidates for those studies Socialisation habituation and

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 1 Definitions of terms used in the survey

1 Note that these processes may occur concurrently2 Defined in the Oxford English Dictionary Online as ldquoThe process of forming associations or of adapting oneself to them especially theprocess whereby an individual acquires the modifications of behaviour and the values necessary for the stability of the social group ofwhich he is or becomes a memberrdquo

Term DefinitionTraining1 The shaping of the behaviour of a primate so that it actively responds in a way that is desired by the train-

er (eg offers a limb for injection stands on a weighing scale waits and allows subordinate individuals tofeed uninterrupted)

Socialisation1 2 The process by which a primate learns how to successfully interact with members of its own species andwith other species (eg humans) with which it co-habits

Habituation1 The waning of a response as a result of repeated stimulation but not fatigue This kind of learning is ofimportance in familiarising a primate with aspects of the environment to which it is inconvenient for it toreact to It is of value in encouraging primates to ignore non-threatening stimuli (eg the sound of clippersrestraint in a sling confinement in a transport container)

Desensitisation1 Systematically pairing positive reward directly with an uncomfortable or aversive experience or stimulus inorder to reduce any associated fear or anxiety response

Positive reinforcement Frequency of a behaviour is increased because something positive is obtained on its performance (eg foodtreat verbal praise tactile contact)

Negative reinforcement Frequency of a behaviour is increased because something negative is removed on its performance (eg cagesqueeze-back mechanism)

Punishment Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something negative is introduced on its performance (egverbal command lsquoNorsquo)

24 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

training programmes in conjunction with temperamenttesting are becoming an accepted part of the preparation oflaboratory dogs for their life on study (Heath et al 2002Adams et al 2004) Dogs that have undergone limitedsocialisation show greater variation in behaviour (includingduring mock procedures) compared with dogs that haveexperienced intensive socialisation (Boxall et al 2004)Decisions about whether to utilise training to refine a partic-ular procedure often depend on a cost-benefit analysisweighing on the one hand the potential for animal sufferingassociated with the traditional method and on the otherhand the likely staff and time investment involved with thetraining Different types of establishment may reachdifferent decisions depending on the frequency with whichanimals undergo the procedure and the method used Forexample the frequency with which blood is taken frommacaques ranged from once per lifetime involving a cagesqueeze-back mechanism andor anaesthesia (most univer-sities and breeding establishments) to up to 24 times in a48 h or much longer period using manual restraint (CROs)Time before data collection begins is short for CROs (mean= 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks for marmosets) incontrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks for macaques)(Table 6) We believe training for co-operation to be aworthwhile investment in the latter case to reduce animalstress and data variability

Moreover depending on their respective responsibilitiesdifferent types of staff may reach different decisions on theneed for animals to be trained Animal technicians andscientists are the staff most responsible for driving trainingat the establishments surveyed (Table 4) It is surprising thatfew establishments identified the Home Office Inspectorateand local ethical review process (ERP) as drivers of animaltraining given the responsibilities of the Inspectorate andERP for reviewing research proposals involving animalsencouraging the widest possible application of the 3Rs andadvising on high standards of animal care and welfare(Home Office 2000)

Constraints on trainingAll establishments believe training can be a refinementEleven out of 15 establishments have acknowledged this forover 10 years and eight for over 15-25 years Thirteen outof 15 establishments agree that training of primates is afeasible concept for them one disagrees and one neitheragrees nor disagrees ndash largely because of time constraintsOf the 13 that agree training is feasible three do not trainpresently so there may be opportunity for refinement atthese establishments Furthermore all of the establishmentswho do not train identified instances where training wouldbenefit animals and staff so again there may be opportunityfor refinement at these establishments if they can overcomethe constraints they face

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 2 Incidence of training socialisation habituation and desensitisation by establishment type

Learningprocess

University (n = 6)

Government or phar-maceutical (n = 3)

Contractresearch (n = 3)

Breeding (n = 3)

Total(n = 15)

Formal programme

Training 6 2 3 0 11 611

Socialisation withhumans

6 3 3 3 15 415

Habituation 6 3 3 1 13 413

Desensitisation 4 3 3 2 12 -

Table 3 Characteristics of establishments surveyed

Characteristic University (n = 6)

Government or pharmaceutical (n = 3)

Contract research(n = 3)

Breeding (n = 3)

Number of animals held (range) 2-15 148-380 109-678 220-860

Number of animals used peryear1 (range)

05-5 15-35 150-654 0-30

Number of animals bred peryear2 (range)

0-1 0-106 na 30-400

Mean staff to animal ratio 12 145 120 170

Age at first use (range) 3-24 months 3-24 months 12-36 months 18-24 months

Duration of use (range) 3 months - 10years

3 months - 7 years 1 week - 1 year 15-20 years (breeding) 1 week - 2years (research)

Group size (range) 1-5 1-30 1-30 1-100

1 euthanased2 production

Training non-human primates a UK survey 25

Reported constraints on training are shown in Table 5 thereare both real and perceived constraints For example fiveestablishments including one that trains cite lack of staff asa constraint and some establishments reportedly lack timebefore data collection begins andor confidence in theirability to train In addition establishments report a paucityof information on how to train and reliable assessment ofthe benefits and some overestimate the time investmentinvolved in training Arguably the problem is not lack ofpublished information on these points but lack ofknowledge about where to find it (see Prescott et al 2005a)CROs (mean = 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks formarmosets) in contrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks formacaques) (Table 6) In addition CROs have larger andmore batches of animals per year These factors maketraining programmes at CROs more resource intensiveMoreover CROs use younger animals sourced fromoverseas breeding establishments Habituation desensitisa-tion and training were reported as being difficult with younganimals if they are poorly socialised with humans because

they become easily stressed and will not take foodHowever macaques at CROs can be and are trained espe-cially during long-term studies where studies are staggeredor where the animals are imported well in advance ofstudies to be held as stock and then used sequentiallyEstablishments reported variation in the speed with whichanimals acclimatised following relocation and transportsuch that their temperament and behaviour were suitable fortraining more than 4 weeks was reported as necessary insome cases Breeding in-house and frequent and timelycommunication between breederssuppliers and users ofprimates will help facilitate acclimatisation socialisationhabituation and training

Resources reported as required in order for establishmentsto begin to train or to expand existing training programmesare shown in Table 7 These map quite closely to theconstraints reported in Table 5 The second part of this paperaims to help facilitate use of training by improving access tosome of these resources (see Prescott et al 2005a)

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 4 Drivers of training as described by those surveyed

Driver Total (n = 15)

Animal technicians 8

Scientists 6

Veterinarians 4

Scientific literature 3

Needs of the experiment 3

Animal welfare organisations 3

Ethical review process 2

Home Office Inspectorate 2Legislation 1Professional guidelines 1Training specialists 1

Customers of breeding establishments 1Culture of care 1

Table 5 Constraints on training as described by those surveyed

Constraint Do train (n = 15)

Do not train have tried(n = 2)

Do not train havenot tried (n = 15)

Total (n = 15)

Paucity of information on how to train 3 1 2 6

Lack of staff 1 1 3 5

Perceived time investment 2 - 1 3

Lack of time before data collection 2 - 1 3

Lack of assessment of benefits - 1 2 3

Lack of confidence in ability to train 2 - 1 3

Possible effects on animal health 1 - - 1Concern about staff health and safety - - 1 1

Concern about effects on scientific validity - - - -

Concern about effect on animal behaviour - - - -

26 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Purposes of trainingThe purposes for which establishments wish to train andwhat they train for presently are shown in Table 8Establishments would like to train macaques for injectionvenepuncture and topical application and marmosets forrestraint and urine sampling because the animals oftenundergo these procedures There are published papersavailable for these procedures and others many withtraining protocols (see Prescott et al 2005a) Furthermoresome establishments already train for these purposes Thisillustrates that there is a need for better communicationbetween establishments for sharing of information onrefinement techniques

For instance we are aware of 10 published papers ontraining macaques to voluntarily offer a limb forvenepuncture Reinhardt (2003) has shown a two-foldincrease in serum cortisol with the traditional manualrestraint method and no increase in serum cortisol with analternative trained method using a combination of PRTand NRT The initial time investment in training(mean = 38 minutes per animal) quickly pays off in a safehandling procedure that no longer requires a secondperson to control the animal while the blood sample istaken Laule et al (2003) give a protocol that utilises PRTonly and does not feature the cage squeeze-backmechanism utilised in Reinhardtrsquos protocol

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 6 Source and arrivals of animals at establishment surveyed

1 Breeding stock may be occasionally supplemented with new animals to increase genetic diversity

Macaques (n =13 ) Marmosets (n = 5)

University Government orpharmaceutical

Contractresearch

Breeding university Government orpharmaceutical

Contractreserarch

breeding

Source of animals

Bred in-house1 1 2 - 2 - 3 - 1

UK breeding establishment

5 - - - - - 1 -

Overseas breedingestablishment

- - 3 - - - - -

Arrivals from externalestablishments

Size of batch 2-4 (mean = 24)

- 4-60 (mean = 31)

- - - 10-52 -

Number of batchesper year

0-1 (mean = 1)

- 3-12 (mean = 63)

- - - 3 -

Age on arrival(months)

18-36 (mean = 228)

- 12-36 (mean = 18)

- - - 14-18 -

Length of acclimati-sation period beforestudy begins (weeks)

24-48 (mean = 36)

- 3-8 (mean = 52)

- - - 6 -

Table 7 Resource requirements for training as described by those surveyed

Resource Total (n = 15)

Audio-visual materials on how to train effectively 6

Written guidance on how to train effectively 6

In person demonstration of training practice 6

More staff 6

More time before studies begin 5

Access to a primate behaviour specialist 5

Wider access to reference material 5

Dedicated staff-training course with practical component 5

More money 3

Face-to-face discussion with experts 2

Secure database on training 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 27

With regard to husbandry procedures nine establishmentstrain their macaques for co-operation with capture shiftinglocation and weighing This involves using a combinationof PRT and NRT to train animals to enter a transport cageor in two cases a primate chair Once trained animals canbe caught swiftly and without undue distress (Reinhardt1992) which can then facilitate veterinary and breedingmanagement husbandry and research Use of training forthis purpose in the UK is in contrast to a 1989 United Statescensus conducted at the National Institutes of Health whichreported that ldquoonly 9 of (56) scientists interviewedcurrently train their animals to enter transport cagesrdquo(Bayne 1989 cited in Reinhardt 1991) The frequency with

which macaques are captured varies from five times per day(CRO) to once every 6 months (University) depending onthe purpose of capture (eg weighing or procedures) age ofthe animal and individual study requirementsFew establishments train marmosets (25) compared withmacaques (913) yet staff members have a high desire totrain marmosets particularly for husbandry and healthprocedures Marmosets may be trained less because of theirsmall size such that they can be easily handled andtherefore present less of a danger to personnel In additionfewer marmosets may be trained because of other consider-ations for example compared to macaques marmosetcolonies are often larger marmosets have shorter life spans

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 8 Purposes of training as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Purpose Already train Would like to train Already train Would like to train

Scientific procedures

Venepuncture 2 2 - 1Injection - 6 - -Urine sampling - 1 1 2

Saliva sampling - - - -

Faecal sampling - 1 - -

Topical application 1 2 1 -

Restraint 6 (chair) 1 - 3

Oral administration 2 - - 2

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Touch screen 2 - 1 -

Lever press 1 - 1 -

Joystick 1 - - -

Eye tracking 1 - - -

Finger press 1 - - -

Husbandry procedures

Weighing 9 1 1 2

Shifting location 9 - 1 1

Collar cleaning - - - 1

Capture 9 1 - 2

Separation 3 - - 1

Station - 2 - 3

Cooperative feeding - 1 - 1

Health procedures

Palpation - 1 - 2

Stethoscope - - - 2Joint manipulation 1 (hand) - - 2

Infant care - - - 2

Mouth inspectionteeth cleaning - 1 - 3

Temperature - 1 - 2

Ultrasound - - - 2

X-ray - - - -

28 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

and large series of blood samples cannot be taken from thesame individual (Buchanan-Smith et al 2003a b) Howeverthere is growing evidence that marmosets can be trained toco-operate in a range of tasks using only PRT and with noneed for food or water management (eg McKinley et al2003 Scott et al 2003 Smith et al 2004) The typical tasksinclude training to co-operate in scientific procedures andtests of cognitive ability and to facilitate husbandry andveterinary procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) Forexample marmosets have been trained to stand on a balancefor in home-cage weighing and to urinate into a collectionvial on request (McKinley et al 2003) One advantage ofthis training is that it avoids the need for capture andrestraint which can be stressful for primates (Reinhardtet al 1990 1995 National Research Council 1998 Saucedaamp Schmidt 2000)Only one establishment of the five housing marmosets hastrained its marmosets for in home-cage weighing At theremainder marmosets are caught for weighing (one perweek to once per month) or scientific procedures (daily orless frequently) either by chasing them into a nest box or byhand using a gauntlet or surgical glove Although someanimals if exposed often enough to capture and restraintappear to acquiesce andor tolerate this practice there arephysiological data which demonstrate that restraint canremain stressful for marmosets and macaques even whenthe animals are habituated to the procedure over a longperiod (Mann 1991 Morrow-Tesch et al 1993 Schnell ampGerber 1997) this can have implications for the dataobtained from such animals

Training practice and staff educationJunior and senior animal technicians and scientists are theindividuals most commonly responsible for determining theneed to train primates and for conducting training of theanimals (Table 9) Regarding techniques used to train thereare a variety of schedules of behaviour modification (seeTable 1) Positive reinforcement is generally considered themost humane of these options (Laule 1999 Laule et al2003 Pryor 2002) It involves voluntary co-operation forpleasurable reward rather than coercion via the threat of anegative event or experience and therefore gives the animalgreater control over the interaction which is a desirablescenario for the well-being of captive animals (Weiss 1968)Nine out of 15 establishments use both positive andnegative reinforcement sometimes in the context of thesame procedure and three establishments use negative rein-forcement only (see Table 10) which is a cause for concernNo establishments used clicker-training We recommendthat training methods should be based on positive reinforce-ment and that negative reinforcement should only be usedwhen positive alternatives have been shown to be ineffec-tive If NRT must be used (eg for training aversive proce-dures) it should be used in combination with PRT(McKinley 2004)The most common positive reinforcers are verbal praise andfood (Table 11) although the efficacy of verbal praise as areinforcer has not been shown Preferred foods can behighly motivating but foods used for training need to becounted in the nutritional content of the diet (Scott 1990)and junk food should be avoided The most common

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 9 Responsibilities for training (n = 15) as described by those surveyed

Technique used Total (n = 15)Positive and negative reinforcement 9

Negative reinforcement only 3Food or water management 4

Positive reinforcement only 2

Negative punishment1 2Positive punishment2 1Clicker training -

Responsible for determining the needto train primates

Responsible for training primates

Junior animal technician 10 12Senior animal technician 12 7Scientist 8 8

Veterinarian 5 -Ethical review process 3 -Customer of breeding establishment 2

Table 10 Techniques used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something pleasant is removed on its performance 2 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something unpleasant is introduced on its performance

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 3: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

Training non-human primates a UK survey 23

see Table 1) All 15 establishments felt that primatesbecome familiar with human behaviour as an inevitableconsequence of their housing and husbandry in captivityHowever four establishments (two CROs and two breedingestablishments) have a formal socialisation programmewhere time is set aside for positive interaction betweenhumans and animals and records are kept Formalprogrammes are beneficial to ensure that socialisation withhumans does not slip when staff is busy and the records canbe reviewed to check the progress of individual animals andthe programme as a wholeAll establishments with the exception of two breedingestablishments use habituation and all save one breedingestablishment and two universities use desensitisationThese universities felt that their management and use ofprimates is such that uncomfortable or aversive experiencesfor the animals are avoided and therefore use of desensitisa-tion is not necessaryOpportunity for and feasibility of use of the learningprocesses and in particular training depends on a numberof factors such as the numbers of animals held used orbred staff to animal ratio age of animals at first use andduration of their use and group size We were interested inlooking for gross patterns between these factors (Table 3)and use of the training (Table 2) Universities typically havesmall numbers and groups of animals that are used forresearch projects lasting many years and high staff toanimal ratios One might expect these characteristics tofacilitate use of training and this appears to be the caseHowever all three CROs surveyed also utilise training

primarily of macaques to enter a transport cage Thisfinding is encouraging given that CROs hold large numbersof animals and use large numbers quickly (eg short-termtoxicology studies) and at a young age They do howeverhave a relatively high staff to animal ratio which mayassist trainingBreeding establishments may not use training and habitua-tion because they do not conduct many scientific procedures(two breeding establishments conduct a limited amount ofresearch or testing) hold large numbers of animalssometimes in large groups and have a relatively low staff toanimal ratio However there is opportunity for training andhabituation of animals in breeding establishments toencourage co-operation with husbandry and veterinaryprocedures particularly for breeding animals that are keptfor a long time (eg Reinhardt 1990 Luttrell et al 1994) Onemacaque breeding establishment has now begun to trainanimals to stand for injectionFive customers of breeding establishments reported thatthey were keen for them to invest more in socialisationhabituation and training of primates destined for use inresearch so that the animals are more tractable and lessstressed when issued for use with the consequence that theresearch can progress more quickly and easily Moreovertemperament has been found to correlate with trainingsuccess for rhesus macaques (Coleman et al 2005) Itshould therefore be possible for breeding establishments toscreen primates to be assigned to research projects in whichthey will be trained with the goal of obtaining the bestcandidates for those studies Socialisation habituation and

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 1 Definitions of terms used in the survey

1 Note that these processes may occur concurrently2 Defined in the Oxford English Dictionary Online as ldquoThe process of forming associations or of adapting oneself to them especially theprocess whereby an individual acquires the modifications of behaviour and the values necessary for the stability of the social group ofwhich he is or becomes a memberrdquo

Term DefinitionTraining1 The shaping of the behaviour of a primate so that it actively responds in a way that is desired by the train-

er (eg offers a limb for injection stands on a weighing scale waits and allows subordinate individuals tofeed uninterrupted)

Socialisation1 2 The process by which a primate learns how to successfully interact with members of its own species andwith other species (eg humans) with which it co-habits

Habituation1 The waning of a response as a result of repeated stimulation but not fatigue This kind of learning is ofimportance in familiarising a primate with aspects of the environment to which it is inconvenient for it toreact to It is of value in encouraging primates to ignore non-threatening stimuli (eg the sound of clippersrestraint in a sling confinement in a transport container)

Desensitisation1 Systematically pairing positive reward directly with an uncomfortable or aversive experience or stimulus inorder to reduce any associated fear or anxiety response

Positive reinforcement Frequency of a behaviour is increased because something positive is obtained on its performance (eg foodtreat verbal praise tactile contact)

Negative reinforcement Frequency of a behaviour is increased because something negative is removed on its performance (eg cagesqueeze-back mechanism)

Punishment Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something negative is introduced on its performance (egverbal command lsquoNorsquo)

24 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

training programmes in conjunction with temperamenttesting are becoming an accepted part of the preparation oflaboratory dogs for their life on study (Heath et al 2002Adams et al 2004) Dogs that have undergone limitedsocialisation show greater variation in behaviour (includingduring mock procedures) compared with dogs that haveexperienced intensive socialisation (Boxall et al 2004)Decisions about whether to utilise training to refine a partic-ular procedure often depend on a cost-benefit analysisweighing on the one hand the potential for animal sufferingassociated with the traditional method and on the otherhand the likely staff and time investment involved with thetraining Different types of establishment may reachdifferent decisions depending on the frequency with whichanimals undergo the procedure and the method used Forexample the frequency with which blood is taken frommacaques ranged from once per lifetime involving a cagesqueeze-back mechanism andor anaesthesia (most univer-sities and breeding establishments) to up to 24 times in a48 h or much longer period using manual restraint (CROs)Time before data collection begins is short for CROs (mean= 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks for marmosets) incontrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks for macaques)(Table 6) We believe training for co-operation to be aworthwhile investment in the latter case to reduce animalstress and data variability

Moreover depending on their respective responsibilitiesdifferent types of staff may reach different decisions on theneed for animals to be trained Animal technicians andscientists are the staff most responsible for driving trainingat the establishments surveyed (Table 4) It is surprising thatfew establishments identified the Home Office Inspectorateand local ethical review process (ERP) as drivers of animaltraining given the responsibilities of the Inspectorate andERP for reviewing research proposals involving animalsencouraging the widest possible application of the 3Rs andadvising on high standards of animal care and welfare(Home Office 2000)

Constraints on trainingAll establishments believe training can be a refinementEleven out of 15 establishments have acknowledged this forover 10 years and eight for over 15-25 years Thirteen outof 15 establishments agree that training of primates is afeasible concept for them one disagrees and one neitheragrees nor disagrees ndash largely because of time constraintsOf the 13 that agree training is feasible three do not trainpresently so there may be opportunity for refinement atthese establishments Furthermore all of the establishmentswho do not train identified instances where training wouldbenefit animals and staff so again there may be opportunityfor refinement at these establishments if they can overcomethe constraints they face

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 2 Incidence of training socialisation habituation and desensitisation by establishment type

Learningprocess

University (n = 6)

Government or phar-maceutical (n = 3)

Contractresearch (n = 3)

Breeding (n = 3)

Total(n = 15)

Formal programme

Training 6 2 3 0 11 611

Socialisation withhumans

6 3 3 3 15 415

Habituation 6 3 3 1 13 413

Desensitisation 4 3 3 2 12 -

Table 3 Characteristics of establishments surveyed

Characteristic University (n = 6)

Government or pharmaceutical (n = 3)

Contract research(n = 3)

Breeding (n = 3)

Number of animals held (range) 2-15 148-380 109-678 220-860

Number of animals used peryear1 (range)

05-5 15-35 150-654 0-30

Number of animals bred peryear2 (range)

0-1 0-106 na 30-400

Mean staff to animal ratio 12 145 120 170

Age at first use (range) 3-24 months 3-24 months 12-36 months 18-24 months

Duration of use (range) 3 months - 10years

3 months - 7 years 1 week - 1 year 15-20 years (breeding) 1 week - 2years (research)

Group size (range) 1-5 1-30 1-30 1-100

1 euthanased2 production

Training non-human primates a UK survey 25

Reported constraints on training are shown in Table 5 thereare both real and perceived constraints For example fiveestablishments including one that trains cite lack of staff asa constraint and some establishments reportedly lack timebefore data collection begins andor confidence in theirability to train In addition establishments report a paucityof information on how to train and reliable assessment ofthe benefits and some overestimate the time investmentinvolved in training Arguably the problem is not lack ofpublished information on these points but lack ofknowledge about where to find it (see Prescott et al 2005a)CROs (mean = 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks formarmosets) in contrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks formacaques) (Table 6) In addition CROs have larger andmore batches of animals per year These factors maketraining programmes at CROs more resource intensiveMoreover CROs use younger animals sourced fromoverseas breeding establishments Habituation desensitisa-tion and training were reported as being difficult with younganimals if they are poorly socialised with humans because

they become easily stressed and will not take foodHowever macaques at CROs can be and are trained espe-cially during long-term studies where studies are staggeredor where the animals are imported well in advance ofstudies to be held as stock and then used sequentiallyEstablishments reported variation in the speed with whichanimals acclimatised following relocation and transportsuch that their temperament and behaviour were suitable fortraining more than 4 weeks was reported as necessary insome cases Breeding in-house and frequent and timelycommunication between breederssuppliers and users ofprimates will help facilitate acclimatisation socialisationhabituation and training

Resources reported as required in order for establishmentsto begin to train or to expand existing training programmesare shown in Table 7 These map quite closely to theconstraints reported in Table 5 The second part of this paperaims to help facilitate use of training by improving access tosome of these resources (see Prescott et al 2005a)

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 4 Drivers of training as described by those surveyed

Driver Total (n = 15)

Animal technicians 8

Scientists 6

Veterinarians 4

Scientific literature 3

Needs of the experiment 3

Animal welfare organisations 3

Ethical review process 2

Home Office Inspectorate 2Legislation 1Professional guidelines 1Training specialists 1

Customers of breeding establishments 1Culture of care 1

Table 5 Constraints on training as described by those surveyed

Constraint Do train (n = 15)

Do not train have tried(n = 2)

Do not train havenot tried (n = 15)

Total (n = 15)

Paucity of information on how to train 3 1 2 6

Lack of staff 1 1 3 5

Perceived time investment 2 - 1 3

Lack of time before data collection 2 - 1 3

Lack of assessment of benefits - 1 2 3

Lack of confidence in ability to train 2 - 1 3

Possible effects on animal health 1 - - 1Concern about staff health and safety - - 1 1

Concern about effects on scientific validity - - - -

Concern about effect on animal behaviour - - - -

26 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Purposes of trainingThe purposes for which establishments wish to train andwhat they train for presently are shown in Table 8Establishments would like to train macaques for injectionvenepuncture and topical application and marmosets forrestraint and urine sampling because the animals oftenundergo these procedures There are published papersavailable for these procedures and others many withtraining protocols (see Prescott et al 2005a) Furthermoresome establishments already train for these purposes Thisillustrates that there is a need for better communicationbetween establishments for sharing of information onrefinement techniques

For instance we are aware of 10 published papers ontraining macaques to voluntarily offer a limb forvenepuncture Reinhardt (2003) has shown a two-foldincrease in serum cortisol with the traditional manualrestraint method and no increase in serum cortisol with analternative trained method using a combination of PRTand NRT The initial time investment in training(mean = 38 minutes per animal) quickly pays off in a safehandling procedure that no longer requires a secondperson to control the animal while the blood sample istaken Laule et al (2003) give a protocol that utilises PRTonly and does not feature the cage squeeze-backmechanism utilised in Reinhardtrsquos protocol

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 6 Source and arrivals of animals at establishment surveyed

1 Breeding stock may be occasionally supplemented with new animals to increase genetic diversity

Macaques (n =13 ) Marmosets (n = 5)

University Government orpharmaceutical

Contractresearch

Breeding university Government orpharmaceutical

Contractreserarch

breeding

Source of animals

Bred in-house1 1 2 - 2 - 3 - 1

UK breeding establishment

5 - - - - - 1 -

Overseas breedingestablishment

- - 3 - - - - -

Arrivals from externalestablishments

Size of batch 2-4 (mean = 24)

- 4-60 (mean = 31)

- - - 10-52 -

Number of batchesper year

0-1 (mean = 1)

- 3-12 (mean = 63)

- - - 3 -

Age on arrival(months)

18-36 (mean = 228)

- 12-36 (mean = 18)

- - - 14-18 -

Length of acclimati-sation period beforestudy begins (weeks)

24-48 (mean = 36)

- 3-8 (mean = 52)

- - - 6 -

Table 7 Resource requirements for training as described by those surveyed

Resource Total (n = 15)

Audio-visual materials on how to train effectively 6

Written guidance on how to train effectively 6

In person demonstration of training practice 6

More staff 6

More time before studies begin 5

Access to a primate behaviour specialist 5

Wider access to reference material 5

Dedicated staff-training course with practical component 5

More money 3

Face-to-face discussion with experts 2

Secure database on training 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 27

With regard to husbandry procedures nine establishmentstrain their macaques for co-operation with capture shiftinglocation and weighing This involves using a combinationof PRT and NRT to train animals to enter a transport cageor in two cases a primate chair Once trained animals canbe caught swiftly and without undue distress (Reinhardt1992) which can then facilitate veterinary and breedingmanagement husbandry and research Use of training forthis purpose in the UK is in contrast to a 1989 United Statescensus conducted at the National Institutes of Health whichreported that ldquoonly 9 of (56) scientists interviewedcurrently train their animals to enter transport cagesrdquo(Bayne 1989 cited in Reinhardt 1991) The frequency with

which macaques are captured varies from five times per day(CRO) to once every 6 months (University) depending onthe purpose of capture (eg weighing or procedures) age ofthe animal and individual study requirementsFew establishments train marmosets (25) compared withmacaques (913) yet staff members have a high desire totrain marmosets particularly for husbandry and healthprocedures Marmosets may be trained less because of theirsmall size such that they can be easily handled andtherefore present less of a danger to personnel In additionfewer marmosets may be trained because of other consider-ations for example compared to macaques marmosetcolonies are often larger marmosets have shorter life spans

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 8 Purposes of training as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Purpose Already train Would like to train Already train Would like to train

Scientific procedures

Venepuncture 2 2 - 1Injection - 6 - -Urine sampling - 1 1 2

Saliva sampling - - - -

Faecal sampling - 1 - -

Topical application 1 2 1 -

Restraint 6 (chair) 1 - 3

Oral administration 2 - - 2

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Touch screen 2 - 1 -

Lever press 1 - 1 -

Joystick 1 - - -

Eye tracking 1 - - -

Finger press 1 - - -

Husbandry procedures

Weighing 9 1 1 2

Shifting location 9 - 1 1

Collar cleaning - - - 1

Capture 9 1 - 2

Separation 3 - - 1

Station - 2 - 3

Cooperative feeding - 1 - 1

Health procedures

Palpation - 1 - 2

Stethoscope - - - 2Joint manipulation 1 (hand) - - 2

Infant care - - - 2

Mouth inspectionteeth cleaning - 1 - 3

Temperature - 1 - 2

Ultrasound - - - 2

X-ray - - - -

28 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

and large series of blood samples cannot be taken from thesame individual (Buchanan-Smith et al 2003a b) Howeverthere is growing evidence that marmosets can be trained toco-operate in a range of tasks using only PRT and with noneed for food or water management (eg McKinley et al2003 Scott et al 2003 Smith et al 2004) The typical tasksinclude training to co-operate in scientific procedures andtests of cognitive ability and to facilitate husbandry andveterinary procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) Forexample marmosets have been trained to stand on a balancefor in home-cage weighing and to urinate into a collectionvial on request (McKinley et al 2003) One advantage ofthis training is that it avoids the need for capture andrestraint which can be stressful for primates (Reinhardtet al 1990 1995 National Research Council 1998 Saucedaamp Schmidt 2000)Only one establishment of the five housing marmosets hastrained its marmosets for in home-cage weighing At theremainder marmosets are caught for weighing (one perweek to once per month) or scientific procedures (daily orless frequently) either by chasing them into a nest box or byhand using a gauntlet or surgical glove Although someanimals if exposed often enough to capture and restraintappear to acquiesce andor tolerate this practice there arephysiological data which demonstrate that restraint canremain stressful for marmosets and macaques even whenthe animals are habituated to the procedure over a longperiod (Mann 1991 Morrow-Tesch et al 1993 Schnell ampGerber 1997) this can have implications for the dataobtained from such animals

Training practice and staff educationJunior and senior animal technicians and scientists are theindividuals most commonly responsible for determining theneed to train primates and for conducting training of theanimals (Table 9) Regarding techniques used to train thereare a variety of schedules of behaviour modification (seeTable 1) Positive reinforcement is generally considered themost humane of these options (Laule 1999 Laule et al2003 Pryor 2002) It involves voluntary co-operation forpleasurable reward rather than coercion via the threat of anegative event or experience and therefore gives the animalgreater control over the interaction which is a desirablescenario for the well-being of captive animals (Weiss 1968)Nine out of 15 establishments use both positive andnegative reinforcement sometimes in the context of thesame procedure and three establishments use negative rein-forcement only (see Table 10) which is a cause for concernNo establishments used clicker-training We recommendthat training methods should be based on positive reinforce-ment and that negative reinforcement should only be usedwhen positive alternatives have been shown to be ineffec-tive If NRT must be used (eg for training aversive proce-dures) it should be used in combination with PRT(McKinley 2004)The most common positive reinforcers are verbal praise andfood (Table 11) although the efficacy of verbal praise as areinforcer has not been shown Preferred foods can behighly motivating but foods used for training need to becounted in the nutritional content of the diet (Scott 1990)and junk food should be avoided The most common

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 9 Responsibilities for training (n = 15) as described by those surveyed

Technique used Total (n = 15)Positive and negative reinforcement 9

Negative reinforcement only 3Food or water management 4

Positive reinforcement only 2

Negative punishment1 2Positive punishment2 1Clicker training -

Responsible for determining the needto train primates

Responsible for training primates

Junior animal technician 10 12Senior animal technician 12 7Scientist 8 8

Veterinarian 5 -Ethical review process 3 -Customer of breeding establishment 2

Table 10 Techniques used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something pleasant is removed on its performance 2 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something unpleasant is introduced on its performance

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 4: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

24 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

training programmes in conjunction with temperamenttesting are becoming an accepted part of the preparation oflaboratory dogs for their life on study (Heath et al 2002Adams et al 2004) Dogs that have undergone limitedsocialisation show greater variation in behaviour (includingduring mock procedures) compared with dogs that haveexperienced intensive socialisation (Boxall et al 2004)Decisions about whether to utilise training to refine a partic-ular procedure often depend on a cost-benefit analysisweighing on the one hand the potential for animal sufferingassociated with the traditional method and on the otherhand the likely staff and time investment involved with thetraining Different types of establishment may reachdifferent decisions depending on the frequency with whichanimals undergo the procedure and the method used Forexample the frequency with which blood is taken frommacaques ranged from once per lifetime involving a cagesqueeze-back mechanism andor anaesthesia (most univer-sities and breeding establishments) to up to 24 times in a48 h or much longer period using manual restraint (CROs)Time before data collection begins is short for CROs (mean= 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks for marmosets) incontrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks for macaques)(Table 6) We believe training for co-operation to be aworthwhile investment in the latter case to reduce animalstress and data variability

Moreover depending on their respective responsibilitiesdifferent types of staff may reach different decisions on theneed for animals to be trained Animal technicians andscientists are the staff most responsible for driving trainingat the establishments surveyed (Table 4) It is surprising thatfew establishments identified the Home Office Inspectorateand local ethical review process (ERP) as drivers of animaltraining given the responsibilities of the Inspectorate andERP for reviewing research proposals involving animalsencouraging the widest possible application of the 3Rs andadvising on high standards of animal care and welfare(Home Office 2000)

Constraints on trainingAll establishments believe training can be a refinementEleven out of 15 establishments have acknowledged this forover 10 years and eight for over 15-25 years Thirteen outof 15 establishments agree that training of primates is afeasible concept for them one disagrees and one neitheragrees nor disagrees ndash largely because of time constraintsOf the 13 that agree training is feasible three do not trainpresently so there may be opportunity for refinement atthese establishments Furthermore all of the establishmentswho do not train identified instances where training wouldbenefit animals and staff so again there may be opportunityfor refinement at these establishments if they can overcomethe constraints they face

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 2 Incidence of training socialisation habituation and desensitisation by establishment type

Learningprocess

University (n = 6)

Government or phar-maceutical (n = 3)

Contractresearch (n = 3)

Breeding (n = 3)

Total(n = 15)

Formal programme

Training 6 2 3 0 11 611

Socialisation withhumans

6 3 3 3 15 415

Habituation 6 3 3 1 13 413

Desensitisation 4 3 3 2 12 -

Table 3 Characteristics of establishments surveyed

Characteristic University (n = 6)

Government or pharmaceutical (n = 3)

Contract research(n = 3)

Breeding (n = 3)

Number of animals held (range) 2-15 148-380 109-678 220-860

Number of animals used peryear1 (range)

05-5 15-35 150-654 0-30

Number of animals bred peryear2 (range)

0-1 0-106 na 30-400

Mean staff to animal ratio 12 145 120 170

Age at first use (range) 3-24 months 3-24 months 12-36 months 18-24 months

Duration of use (range) 3 months - 10years

3 months - 7 years 1 week - 1 year 15-20 years (breeding) 1 week - 2years (research)

Group size (range) 1-5 1-30 1-30 1-100

1 euthanased2 production

Training non-human primates a UK survey 25

Reported constraints on training are shown in Table 5 thereare both real and perceived constraints For example fiveestablishments including one that trains cite lack of staff asa constraint and some establishments reportedly lack timebefore data collection begins andor confidence in theirability to train In addition establishments report a paucityof information on how to train and reliable assessment ofthe benefits and some overestimate the time investmentinvolved in training Arguably the problem is not lack ofpublished information on these points but lack ofknowledge about where to find it (see Prescott et al 2005a)CROs (mean = 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks formarmosets) in contrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks formacaques) (Table 6) In addition CROs have larger andmore batches of animals per year These factors maketraining programmes at CROs more resource intensiveMoreover CROs use younger animals sourced fromoverseas breeding establishments Habituation desensitisa-tion and training were reported as being difficult with younganimals if they are poorly socialised with humans because

they become easily stressed and will not take foodHowever macaques at CROs can be and are trained espe-cially during long-term studies where studies are staggeredor where the animals are imported well in advance ofstudies to be held as stock and then used sequentiallyEstablishments reported variation in the speed with whichanimals acclimatised following relocation and transportsuch that their temperament and behaviour were suitable fortraining more than 4 weeks was reported as necessary insome cases Breeding in-house and frequent and timelycommunication between breederssuppliers and users ofprimates will help facilitate acclimatisation socialisationhabituation and training

Resources reported as required in order for establishmentsto begin to train or to expand existing training programmesare shown in Table 7 These map quite closely to theconstraints reported in Table 5 The second part of this paperaims to help facilitate use of training by improving access tosome of these resources (see Prescott et al 2005a)

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 4 Drivers of training as described by those surveyed

Driver Total (n = 15)

Animal technicians 8

Scientists 6

Veterinarians 4

Scientific literature 3

Needs of the experiment 3

Animal welfare organisations 3

Ethical review process 2

Home Office Inspectorate 2Legislation 1Professional guidelines 1Training specialists 1

Customers of breeding establishments 1Culture of care 1

Table 5 Constraints on training as described by those surveyed

Constraint Do train (n = 15)

Do not train have tried(n = 2)

Do not train havenot tried (n = 15)

Total (n = 15)

Paucity of information on how to train 3 1 2 6

Lack of staff 1 1 3 5

Perceived time investment 2 - 1 3

Lack of time before data collection 2 - 1 3

Lack of assessment of benefits - 1 2 3

Lack of confidence in ability to train 2 - 1 3

Possible effects on animal health 1 - - 1Concern about staff health and safety - - 1 1

Concern about effects on scientific validity - - - -

Concern about effect on animal behaviour - - - -

26 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Purposes of trainingThe purposes for which establishments wish to train andwhat they train for presently are shown in Table 8Establishments would like to train macaques for injectionvenepuncture and topical application and marmosets forrestraint and urine sampling because the animals oftenundergo these procedures There are published papersavailable for these procedures and others many withtraining protocols (see Prescott et al 2005a) Furthermoresome establishments already train for these purposes Thisillustrates that there is a need for better communicationbetween establishments for sharing of information onrefinement techniques

For instance we are aware of 10 published papers ontraining macaques to voluntarily offer a limb forvenepuncture Reinhardt (2003) has shown a two-foldincrease in serum cortisol with the traditional manualrestraint method and no increase in serum cortisol with analternative trained method using a combination of PRTand NRT The initial time investment in training(mean = 38 minutes per animal) quickly pays off in a safehandling procedure that no longer requires a secondperson to control the animal while the blood sample istaken Laule et al (2003) give a protocol that utilises PRTonly and does not feature the cage squeeze-backmechanism utilised in Reinhardtrsquos protocol

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 6 Source and arrivals of animals at establishment surveyed

1 Breeding stock may be occasionally supplemented with new animals to increase genetic diversity

Macaques (n =13 ) Marmosets (n = 5)

University Government orpharmaceutical

Contractresearch

Breeding university Government orpharmaceutical

Contractreserarch

breeding

Source of animals

Bred in-house1 1 2 - 2 - 3 - 1

UK breeding establishment

5 - - - - - 1 -

Overseas breedingestablishment

- - 3 - - - - -

Arrivals from externalestablishments

Size of batch 2-4 (mean = 24)

- 4-60 (mean = 31)

- - - 10-52 -

Number of batchesper year

0-1 (mean = 1)

- 3-12 (mean = 63)

- - - 3 -

Age on arrival(months)

18-36 (mean = 228)

- 12-36 (mean = 18)

- - - 14-18 -

Length of acclimati-sation period beforestudy begins (weeks)

24-48 (mean = 36)

- 3-8 (mean = 52)

- - - 6 -

Table 7 Resource requirements for training as described by those surveyed

Resource Total (n = 15)

Audio-visual materials on how to train effectively 6

Written guidance on how to train effectively 6

In person demonstration of training practice 6

More staff 6

More time before studies begin 5

Access to a primate behaviour specialist 5

Wider access to reference material 5

Dedicated staff-training course with practical component 5

More money 3

Face-to-face discussion with experts 2

Secure database on training 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 27

With regard to husbandry procedures nine establishmentstrain their macaques for co-operation with capture shiftinglocation and weighing This involves using a combinationof PRT and NRT to train animals to enter a transport cageor in two cases a primate chair Once trained animals canbe caught swiftly and without undue distress (Reinhardt1992) which can then facilitate veterinary and breedingmanagement husbandry and research Use of training forthis purpose in the UK is in contrast to a 1989 United Statescensus conducted at the National Institutes of Health whichreported that ldquoonly 9 of (56) scientists interviewedcurrently train their animals to enter transport cagesrdquo(Bayne 1989 cited in Reinhardt 1991) The frequency with

which macaques are captured varies from five times per day(CRO) to once every 6 months (University) depending onthe purpose of capture (eg weighing or procedures) age ofthe animal and individual study requirementsFew establishments train marmosets (25) compared withmacaques (913) yet staff members have a high desire totrain marmosets particularly for husbandry and healthprocedures Marmosets may be trained less because of theirsmall size such that they can be easily handled andtherefore present less of a danger to personnel In additionfewer marmosets may be trained because of other consider-ations for example compared to macaques marmosetcolonies are often larger marmosets have shorter life spans

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 8 Purposes of training as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Purpose Already train Would like to train Already train Would like to train

Scientific procedures

Venepuncture 2 2 - 1Injection - 6 - -Urine sampling - 1 1 2

Saliva sampling - - - -

Faecal sampling - 1 - -

Topical application 1 2 1 -

Restraint 6 (chair) 1 - 3

Oral administration 2 - - 2

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Touch screen 2 - 1 -

Lever press 1 - 1 -

Joystick 1 - - -

Eye tracking 1 - - -

Finger press 1 - - -

Husbandry procedures

Weighing 9 1 1 2

Shifting location 9 - 1 1

Collar cleaning - - - 1

Capture 9 1 - 2

Separation 3 - - 1

Station - 2 - 3

Cooperative feeding - 1 - 1

Health procedures

Palpation - 1 - 2

Stethoscope - - - 2Joint manipulation 1 (hand) - - 2

Infant care - - - 2

Mouth inspectionteeth cleaning - 1 - 3

Temperature - 1 - 2

Ultrasound - - - 2

X-ray - - - -

28 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

and large series of blood samples cannot be taken from thesame individual (Buchanan-Smith et al 2003a b) Howeverthere is growing evidence that marmosets can be trained toco-operate in a range of tasks using only PRT and with noneed for food or water management (eg McKinley et al2003 Scott et al 2003 Smith et al 2004) The typical tasksinclude training to co-operate in scientific procedures andtests of cognitive ability and to facilitate husbandry andveterinary procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) Forexample marmosets have been trained to stand on a balancefor in home-cage weighing and to urinate into a collectionvial on request (McKinley et al 2003) One advantage ofthis training is that it avoids the need for capture andrestraint which can be stressful for primates (Reinhardtet al 1990 1995 National Research Council 1998 Saucedaamp Schmidt 2000)Only one establishment of the five housing marmosets hastrained its marmosets for in home-cage weighing At theremainder marmosets are caught for weighing (one perweek to once per month) or scientific procedures (daily orless frequently) either by chasing them into a nest box or byhand using a gauntlet or surgical glove Although someanimals if exposed often enough to capture and restraintappear to acquiesce andor tolerate this practice there arephysiological data which demonstrate that restraint canremain stressful for marmosets and macaques even whenthe animals are habituated to the procedure over a longperiod (Mann 1991 Morrow-Tesch et al 1993 Schnell ampGerber 1997) this can have implications for the dataobtained from such animals

Training practice and staff educationJunior and senior animal technicians and scientists are theindividuals most commonly responsible for determining theneed to train primates and for conducting training of theanimals (Table 9) Regarding techniques used to train thereare a variety of schedules of behaviour modification (seeTable 1) Positive reinforcement is generally considered themost humane of these options (Laule 1999 Laule et al2003 Pryor 2002) It involves voluntary co-operation forpleasurable reward rather than coercion via the threat of anegative event or experience and therefore gives the animalgreater control over the interaction which is a desirablescenario for the well-being of captive animals (Weiss 1968)Nine out of 15 establishments use both positive andnegative reinforcement sometimes in the context of thesame procedure and three establishments use negative rein-forcement only (see Table 10) which is a cause for concernNo establishments used clicker-training We recommendthat training methods should be based on positive reinforce-ment and that negative reinforcement should only be usedwhen positive alternatives have been shown to be ineffec-tive If NRT must be used (eg for training aversive proce-dures) it should be used in combination with PRT(McKinley 2004)The most common positive reinforcers are verbal praise andfood (Table 11) although the efficacy of verbal praise as areinforcer has not been shown Preferred foods can behighly motivating but foods used for training need to becounted in the nutritional content of the diet (Scott 1990)and junk food should be avoided The most common

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 9 Responsibilities for training (n = 15) as described by those surveyed

Technique used Total (n = 15)Positive and negative reinforcement 9

Negative reinforcement only 3Food or water management 4

Positive reinforcement only 2

Negative punishment1 2Positive punishment2 1Clicker training -

Responsible for determining the needto train primates

Responsible for training primates

Junior animal technician 10 12Senior animal technician 12 7Scientist 8 8

Veterinarian 5 -Ethical review process 3 -Customer of breeding establishment 2

Table 10 Techniques used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something pleasant is removed on its performance 2 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something unpleasant is introduced on its performance

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 5: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

Training non-human primates a UK survey 25

Reported constraints on training are shown in Table 5 thereare both real and perceived constraints For example fiveestablishments including one that trains cite lack of staff asa constraint and some establishments reportedly lack timebefore data collection begins andor confidence in theirability to train In addition establishments report a paucityof information on how to train and reliable assessment ofthe benefits and some overestimate the time investmentinvolved in training Arguably the problem is not lack ofpublished information on these points but lack ofknowledge about where to find it (see Prescott et al 2005a)CROs (mean = 5 weeks for macaques and 6 weeks formarmosets) in contrast to universities (mean = 36 weeks formacaques) (Table 6) In addition CROs have larger andmore batches of animals per year These factors maketraining programmes at CROs more resource intensiveMoreover CROs use younger animals sourced fromoverseas breeding establishments Habituation desensitisa-tion and training were reported as being difficult with younganimals if they are poorly socialised with humans because

they become easily stressed and will not take foodHowever macaques at CROs can be and are trained espe-cially during long-term studies where studies are staggeredor where the animals are imported well in advance ofstudies to be held as stock and then used sequentiallyEstablishments reported variation in the speed with whichanimals acclimatised following relocation and transportsuch that their temperament and behaviour were suitable fortraining more than 4 weeks was reported as necessary insome cases Breeding in-house and frequent and timelycommunication between breederssuppliers and users ofprimates will help facilitate acclimatisation socialisationhabituation and training

Resources reported as required in order for establishmentsto begin to train or to expand existing training programmesare shown in Table 7 These map quite closely to theconstraints reported in Table 5 The second part of this paperaims to help facilitate use of training by improving access tosome of these resources (see Prescott et al 2005a)

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 4 Drivers of training as described by those surveyed

Driver Total (n = 15)

Animal technicians 8

Scientists 6

Veterinarians 4

Scientific literature 3

Needs of the experiment 3

Animal welfare organisations 3

Ethical review process 2

Home Office Inspectorate 2Legislation 1Professional guidelines 1Training specialists 1

Customers of breeding establishments 1Culture of care 1

Table 5 Constraints on training as described by those surveyed

Constraint Do train (n = 15)

Do not train have tried(n = 2)

Do not train havenot tried (n = 15)

Total (n = 15)

Paucity of information on how to train 3 1 2 6

Lack of staff 1 1 3 5

Perceived time investment 2 - 1 3

Lack of time before data collection 2 - 1 3

Lack of assessment of benefits - 1 2 3

Lack of confidence in ability to train 2 - 1 3

Possible effects on animal health 1 - - 1Concern about staff health and safety - - 1 1

Concern about effects on scientific validity - - - -

Concern about effect on animal behaviour - - - -

26 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Purposes of trainingThe purposes for which establishments wish to train andwhat they train for presently are shown in Table 8Establishments would like to train macaques for injectionvenepuncture and topical application and marmosets forrestraint and urine sampling because the animals oftenundergo these procedures There are published papersavailable for these procedures and others many withtraining protocols (see Prescott et al 2005a) Furthermoresome establishments already train for these purposes Thisillustrates that there is a need for better communicationbetween establishments for sharing of information onrefinement techniques

For instance we are aware of 10 published papers ontraining macaques to voluntarily offer a limb forvenepuncture Reinhardt (2003) has shown a two-foldincrease in serum cortisol with the traditional manualrestraint method and no increase in serum cortisol with analternative trained method using a combination of PRTand NRT The initial time investment in training(mean = 38 minutes per animal) quickly pays off in a safehandling procedure that no longer requires a secondperson to control the animal while the blood sample istaken Laule et al (2003) give a protocol that utilises PRTonly and does not feature the cage squeeze-backmechanism utilised in Reinhardtrsquos protocol

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 6 Source and arrivals of animals at establishment surveyed

1 Breeding stock may be occasionally supplemented with new animals to increase genetic diversity

Macaques (n =13 ) Marmosets (n = 5)

University Government orpharmaceutical

Contractresearch

Breeding university Government orpharmaceutical

Contractreserarch

breeding

Source of animals

Bred in-house1 1 2 - 2 - 3 - 1

UK breeding establishment

5 - - - - - 1 -

Overseas breedingestablishment

- - 3 - - - - -

Arrivals from externalestablishments

Size of batch 2-4 (mean = 24)

- 4-60 (mean = 31)

- - - 10-52 -

Number of batchesper year

0-1 (mean = 1)

- 3-12 (mean = 63)

- - - 3 -

Age on arrival(months)

18-36 (mean = 228)

- 12-36 (mean = 18)

- - - 14-18 -

Length of acclimati-sation period beforestudy begins (weeks)

24-48 (mean = 36)

- 3-8 (mean = 52)

- - - 6 -

Table 7 Resource requirements for training as described by those surveyed

Resource Total (n = 15)

Audio-visual materials on how to train effectively 6

Written guidance on how to train effectively 6

In person demonstration of training practice 6

More staff 6

More time before studies begin 5

Access to a primate behaviour specialist 5

Wider access to reference material 5

Dedicated staff-training course with practical component 5

More money 3

Face-to-face discussion with experts 2

Secure database on training 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 27

With regard to husbandry procedures nine establishmentstrain their macaques for co-operation with capture shiftinglocation and weighing This involves using a combinationof PRT and NRT to train animals to enter a transport cageor in two cases a primate chair Once trained animals canbe caught swiftly and without undue distress (Reinhardt1992) which can then facilitate veterinary and breedingmanagement husbandry and research Use of training forthis purpose in the UK is in contrast to a 1989 United Statescensus conducted at the National Institutes of Health whichreported that ldquoonly 9 of (56) scientists interviewedcurrently train their animals to enter transport cagesrdquo(Bayne 1989 cited in Reinhardt 1991) The frequency with

which macaques are captured varies from five times per day(CRO) to once every 6 months (University) depending onthe purpose of capture (eg weighing or procedures) age ofthe animal and individual study requirementsFew establishments train marmosets (25) compared withmacaques (913) yet staff members have a high desire totrain marmosets particularly for husbandry and healthprocedures Marmosets may be trained less because of theirsmall size such that they can be easily handled andtherefore present less of a danger to personnel In additionfewer marmosets may be trained because of other consider-ations for example compared to macaques marmosetcolonies are often larger marmosets have shorter life spans

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 8 Purposes of training as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Purpose Already train Would like to train Already train Would like to train

Scientific procedures

Venepuncture 2 2 - 1Injection - 6 - -Urine sampling - 1 1 2

Saliva sampling - - - -

Faecal sampling - 1 - -

Topical application 1 2 1 -

Restraint 6 (chair) 1 - 3

Oral administration 2 - - 2

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Touch screen 2 - 1 -

Lever press 1 - 1 -

Joystick 1 - - -

Eye tracking 1 - - -

Finger press 1 - - -

Husbandry procedures

Weighing 9 1 1 2

Shifting location 9 - 1 1

Collar cleaning - - - 1

Capture 9 1 - 2

Separation 3 - - 1

Station - 2 - 3

Cooperative feeding - 1 - 1

Health procedures

Palpation - 1 - 2

Stethoscope - - - 2Joint manipulation 1 (hand) - - 2

Infant care - - - 2

Mouth inspectionteeth cleaning - 1 - 3

Temperature - 1 - 2

Ultrasound - - - 2

X-ray - - - -

28 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

and large series of blood samples cannot be taken from thesame individual (Buchanan-Smith et al 2003a b) Howeverthere is growing evidence that marmosets can be trained toco-operate in a range of tasks using only PRT and with noneed for food or water management (eg McKinley et al2003 Scott et al 2003 Smith et al 2004) The typical tasksinclude training to co-operate in scientific procedures andtests of cognitive ability and to facilitate husbandry andveterinary procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) Forexample marmosets have been trained to stand on a balancefor in home-cage weighing and to urinate into a collectionvial on request (McKinley et al 2003) One advantage ofthis training is that it avoids the need for capture andrestraint which can be stressful for primates (Reinhardtet al 1990 1995 National Research Council 1998 Saucedaamp Schmidt 2000)Only one establishment of the five housing marmosets hastrained its marmosets for in home-cage weighing At theremainder marmosets are caught for weighing (one perweek to once per month) or scientific procedures (daily orless frequently) either by chasing them into a nest box or byhand using a gauntlet or surgical glove Although someanimals if exposed often enough to capture and restraintappear to acquiesce andor tolerate this practice there arephysiological data which demonstrate that restraint canremain stressful for marmosets and macaques even whenthe animals are habituated to the procedure over a longperiod (Mann 1991 Morrow-Tesch et al 1993 Schnell ampGerber 1997) this can have implications for the dataobtained from such animals

Training practice and staff educationJunior and senior animal technicians and scientists are theindividuals most commonly responsible for determining theneed to train primates and for conducting training of theanimals (Table 9) Regarding techniques used to train thereare a variety of schedules of behaviour modification (seeTable 1) Positive reinforcement is generally considered themost humane of these options (Laule 1999 Laule et al2003 Pryor 2002) It involves voluntary co-operation forpleasurable reward rather than coercion via the threat of anegative event or experience and therefore gives the animalgreater control over the interaction which is a desirablescenario for the well-being of captive animals (Weiss 1968)Nine out of 15 establishments use both positive andnegative reinforcement sometimes in the context of thesame procedure and three establishments use negative rein-forcement only (see Table 10) which is a cause for concernNo establishments used clicker-training We recommendthat training methods should be based on positive reinforce-ment and that negative reinforcement should only be usedwhen positive alternatives have been shown to be ineffec-tive If NRT must be used (eg for training aversive proce-dures) it should be used in combination with PRT(McKinley 2004)The most common positive reinforcers are verbal praise andfood (Table 11) although the efficacy of verbal praise as areinforcer has not been shown Preferred foods can behighly motivating but foods used for training need to becounted in the nutritional content of the diet (Scott 1990)and junk food should be avoided The most common

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 9 Responsibilities for training (n = 15) as described by those surveyed

Technique used Total (n = 15)Positive and negative reinforcement 9

Negative reinforcement only 3Food or water management 4

Positive reinforcement only 2

Negative punishment1 2Positive punishment2 1Clicker training -

Responsible for determining the needto train primates

Responsible for training primates

Junior animal technician 10 12Senior animal technician 12 7Scientist 8 8

Veterinarian 5 -Ethical review process 3 -Customer of breeding establishment 2

Table 10 Techniques used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something pleasant is removed on its performance 2 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something unpleasant is introduced on its performance

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 6: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

26 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Purposes of trainingThe purposes for which establishments wish to train andwhat they train for presently are shown in Table 8Establishments would like to train macaques for injectionvenepuncture and topical application and marmosets forrestraint and urine sampling because the animals oftenundergo these procedures There are published papersavailable for these procedures and others many withtraining protocols (see Prescott et al 2005a) Furthermoresome establishments already train for these purposes Thisillustrates that there is a need for better communicationbetween establishments for sharing of information onrefinement techniques

For instance we are aware of 10 published papers ontraining macaques to voluntarily offer a limb forvenepuncture Reinhardt (2003) has shown a two-foldincrease in serum cortisol with the traditional manualrestraint method and no increase in serum cortisol with analternative trained method using a combination of PRTand NRT The initial time investment in training(mean = 38 minutes per animal) quickly pays off in a safehandling procedure that no longer requires a secondperson to control the animal while the blood sample istaken Laule et al (2003) give a protocol that utilises PRTonly and does not feature the cage squeeze-backmechanism utilised in Reinhardtrsquos protocol

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 6 Source and arrivals of animals at establishment surveyed

1 Breeding stock may be occasionally supplemented with new animals to increase genetic diversity

Macaques (n =13 ) Marmosets (n = 5)

University Government orpharmaceutical

Contractresearch

Breeding university Government orpharmaceutical

Contractreserarch

breeding

Source of animals

Bred in-house1 1 2 - 2 - 3 - 1

UK breeding establishment

5 - - - - - 1 -

Overseas breedingestablishment

- - 3 - - - - -

Arrivals from externalestablishments

Size of batch 2-4 (mean = 24)

- 4-60 (mean = 31)

- - - 10-52 -

Number of batchesper year

0-1 (mean = 1)

- 3-12 (mean = 63)

- - - 3 -

Age on arrival(months)

18-36 (mean = 228)

- 12-36 (mean = 18)

- - - 14-18 -

Length of acclimati-sation period beforestudy begins (weeks)

24-48 (mean = 36)

- 3-8 (mean = 52)

- - - 6 -

Table 7 Resource requirements for training as described by those surveyed

Resource Total (n = 15)

Audio-visual materials on how to train effectively 6

Written guidance on how to train effectively 6

In person demonstration of training practice 6

More staff 6

More time before studies begin 5

Access to a primate behaviour specialist 5

Wider access to reference material 5

Dedicated staff-training course with practical component 5

More money 3

Face-to-face discussion with experts 2

Secure database on training 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 27

With regard to husbandry procedures nine establishmentstrain their macaques for co-operation with capture shiftinglocation and weighing This involves using a combinationof PRT and NRT to train animals to enter a transport cageor in two cases a primate chair Once trained animals canbe caught swiftly and without undue distress (Reinhardt1992) which can then facilitate veterinary and breedingmanagement husbandry and research Use of training forthis purpose in the UK is in contrast to a 1989 United Statescensus conducted at the National Institutes of Health whichreported that ldquoonly 9 of (56) scientists interviewedcurrently train their animals to enter transport cagesrdquo(Bayne 1989 cited in Reinhardt 1991) The frequency with

which macaques are captured varies from five times per day(CRO) to once every 6 months (University) depending onthe purpose of capture (eg weighing or procedures) age ofthe animal and individual study requirementsFew establishments train marmosets (25) compared withmacaques (913) yet staff members have a high desire totrain marmosets particularly for husbandry and healthprocedures Marmosets may be trained less because of theirsmall size such that they can be easily handled andtherefore present less of a danger to personnel In additionfewer marmosets may be trained because of other consider-ations for example compared to macaques marmosetcolonies are often larger marmosets have shorter life spans

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 8 Purposes of training as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Purpose Already train Would like to train Already train Would like to train

Scientific procedures

Venepuncture 2 2 - 1Injection - 6 - -Urine sampling - 1 1 2

Saliva sampling - - - -

Faecal sampling - 1 - -

Topical application 1 2 1 -

Restraint 6 (chair) 1 - 3

Oral administration 2 - - 2

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Touch screen 2 - 1 -

Lever press 1 - 1 -

Joystick 1 - - -

Eye tracking 1 - - -

Finger press 1 - - -

Husbandry procedures

Weighing 9 1 1 2

Shifting location 9 - 1 1

Collar cleaning - - - 1

Capture 9 1 - 2

Separation 3 - - 1

Station - 2 - 3

Cooperative feeding - 1 - 1

Health procedures

Palpation - 1 - 2

Stethoscope - - - 2Joint manipulation 1 (hand) - - 2

Infant care - - - 2

Mouth inspectionteeth cleaning - 1 - 3

Temperature - 1 - 2

Ultrasound - - - 2

X-ray - - - -

28 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

and large series of blood samples cannot be taken from thesame individual (Buchanan-Smith et al 2003a b) Howeverthere is growing evidence that marmosets can be trained toco-operate in a range of tasks using only PRT and with noneed for food or water management (eg McKinley et al2003 Scott et al 2003 Smith et al 2004) The typical tasksinclude training to co-operate in scientific procedures andtests of cognitive ability and to facilitate husbandry andveterinary procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) Forexample marmosets have been trained to stand on a balancefor in home-cage weighing and to urinate into a collectionvial on request (McKinley et al 2003) One advantage ofthis training is that it avoids the need for capture andrestraint which can be stressful for primates (Reinhardtet al 1990 1995 National Research Council 1998 Saucedaamp Schmidt 2000)Only one establishment of the five housing marmosets hastrained its marmosets for in home-cage weighing At theremainder marmosets are caught for weighing (one perweek to once per month) or scientific procedures (daily orless frequently) either by chasing them into a nest box or byhand using a gauntlet or surgical glove Although someanimals if exposed often enough to capture and restraintappear to acquiesce andor tolerate this practice there arephysiological data which demonstrate that restraint canremain stressful for marmosets and macaques even whenthe animals are habituated to the procedure over a longperiod (Mann 1991 Morrow-Tesch et al 1993 Schnell ampGerber 1997) this can have implications for the dataobtained from such animals

Training practice and staff educationJunior and senior animal technicians and scientists are theindividuals most commonly responsible for determining theneed to train primates and for conducting training of theanimals (Table 9) Regarding techniques used to train thereare a variety of schedules of behaviour modification (seeTable 1) Positive reinforcement is generally considered themost humane of these options (Laule 1999 Laule et al2003 Pryor 2002) It involves voluntary co-operation forpleasurable reward rather than coercion via the threat of anegative event or experience and therefore gives the animalgreater control over the interaction which is a desirablescenario for the well-being of captive animals (Weiss 1968)Nine out of 15 establishments use both positive andnegative reinforcement sometimes in the context of thesame procedure and three establishments use negative rein-forcement only (see Table 10) which is a cause for concernNo establishments used clicker-training We recommendthat training methods should be based on positive reinforce-ment and that negative reinforcement should only be usedwhen positive alternatives have been shown to be ineffec-tive If NRT must be used (eg for training aversive proce-dures) it should be used in combination with PRT(McKinley 2004)The most common positive reinforcers are verbal praise andfood (Table 11) although the efficacy of verbal praise as areinforcer has not been shown Preferred foods can behighly motivating but foods used for training need to becounted in the nutritional content of the diet (Scott 1990)and junk food should be avoided The most common

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 9 Responsibilities for training (n = 15) as described by those surveyed

Technique used Total (n = 15)Positive and negative reinforcement 9

Negative reinforcement only 3Food or water management 4

Positive reinforcement only 2

Negative punishment1 2Positive punishment2 1Clicker training -

Responsible for determining the needto train primates

Responsible for training primates

Junior animal technician 10 12Senior animal technician 12 7Scientist 8 8

Veterinarian 5 -Ethical review process 3 -Customer of breeding establishment 2

Table 10 Techniques used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something pleasant is removed on its performance 2 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something unpleasant is introduced on its performance

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 7: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

Training non-human primates a UK survey 27

With regard to husbandry procedures nine establishmentstrain their macaques for co-operation with capture shiftinglocation and weighing This involves using a combinationof PRT and NRT to train animals to enter a transport cageor in two cases a primate chair Once trained animals canbe caught swiftly and without undue distress (Reinhardt1992) which can then facilitate veterinary and breedingmanagement husbandry and research Use of training forthis purpose in the UK is in contrast to a 1989 United Statescensus conducted at the National Institutes of Health whichreported that ldquoonly 9 of (56) scientists interviewedcurrently train their animals to enter transport cagesrdquo(Bayne 1989 cited in Reinhardt 1991) The frequency with

which macaques are captured varies from five times per day(CRO) to once every 6 months (University) depending onthe purpose of capture (eg weighing or procedures) age ofthe animal and individual study requirementsFew establishments train marmosets (25) compared withmacaques (913) yet staff members have a high desire totrain marmosets particularly for husbandry and healthprocedures Marmosets may be trained less because of theirsmall size such that they can be easily handled andtherefore present less of a danger to personnel In additionfewer marmosets may be trained because of other consider-ations for example compared to macaques marmosetcolonies are often larger marmosets have shorter life spans

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 8 Purposes of training as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Purpose Already train Would like to train Already train Would like to train

Scientific procedures

Venepuncture 2 2 - 1Injection - 6 - -Urine sampling - 1 1 2

Saliva sampling - - - -

Faecal sampling - 1 - -

Topical application 1 2 1 -

Restraint 6 (chair) 1 - 3

Oral administration 2 - - 2

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Touch screen 2 - 1 -

Lever press 1 - 1 -

Joystick 1 - - -

Eye tracking 1 - - -

Finger press 1 - - -

Husbandry procedures

Weighing 9 1 1 2

Shifting location 9 - 1 1

Collar cleaning - - - 1

Capture 9 1 - 2

Separation 3 - - 1

Station - 2 - 3

Cooperative feeding - 1 - 1

Health procedures

Palpation - 1 - 2

Stethoscope - - - 2Joint manipulation 1 (hand) - - 2

Infant care - - - 2

Mouth inspectionteeth cleaning - 1 - 3

Temperature - 1 - 2

Ultrasound - - - 2

X-ray - - - -

28 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

and large series of blood samples cannot be taken from thesame individual (Buchanan-Smith et al 2003a b) Howeverthere is growing evidence that marmosets can be trained toco-operate in a range of tasks using only PRT and with noneed for food or water management (eg McKinley et al2003 Scott et al 2003 Smith et al 2004) The typical tasksinclude training to co-operate in scientific procedures andtests of cognitive ability and to facilitate husbandry andveterinary procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) Forexample marmosets have been trained to stand on a balancefor in home-cage weighing and to urinate into a collectionvial on request (McKinley et al 2003) One advantage ofthis training is that it avoids the need for capture andrestraint which can be stressful for primates (Reinhardtet al 1990 1995 National Research Council 1998 Saucedaamp Schmidt 2000)Only one establishment of the five housing marmosets hastrained its marmosets for in home-cage weighing At theremainder marmosets are caught for weighing (one perweek to once per month) or scientific procedures (daily orless frequently) either by chasing them into a nest box or byhand using a gauntlet or surgical glove Although someanimals if exposed often enough to capture and restraintappear to acquiesce andor tolerate this practice there arephysiological data which demonstrate that restraint canremain stressful for marmosets and macaques even whenthe animals are habituated to the procedure over a longperiod (Mann 1991 Morrow-Tesch et al 1993 Schnell ampGerber 1997) this can have implications for the dataobtained from such animals

Training practice and staff educationJunior and senior animal technicians and scientists are theindividuals most commonly responsible for determining theneed to train primates and for conducting training of theanimals (Table 9) Regarding techniques used to train thereare a variety of schedules of behaviour modification (seeTable 1) Positive reinforcement is generally considered themost humane of these options (Laule 1999 Laule et al2003 Pryor 2002) It involves voluntary co-operation forpleasurable reward rather than coercion via the threat of anegative event or experience and therefore gives the animalgreater control over the interaction which is a desirablescenario for the well-being of captive animals (Weiss 1968)Nine out of 15 establishments use both positive andnegative reinforcement sometimes in the context of thesame procedure and three establishments use negative rein-forcement only (see Table 10) which is a cause for concernNo establishments used clicker-training We recommendthat training methods should be based on positive reinforce-ment and that negative reinforcement should only be usedwhen positive alternatives have been shown to be ineffec-tive If NRT must be used (eg for training aversive proce-dures) it should be used in combination with PRT(McKinley 2004)The most common positive reinforcers are verbal praise andfood (Table 11) although the efficacy of verbal praise as areinforcer has not been shown Preferred foods can behighly motivating but foods used for training need to becounted in the nutritional content of the diet (Scott 1990)and junk food should be avoided The most common

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 9 Responsibilities for training (n = 15) as described by those surveyed

Technique used Total (n = 15)Positive and negative reinforcement 9

Negative reinforcement only 3Food or water management 4

Positive reinforcement only 2

Negative punishment1 2Positive punishment2 1Clicker training -

Responsible for determining the needto train primates

Responsible for training primates

Junior animal technician 10 12Senior animal technician 12 7Scientist 8 8

Veterinarian 5 -Ethical review process 3 -Customer of breeding establishment 2

Table 10 Techniques used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something pleasant is removed on its performance 2 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something unpleasant is introduced on its performance

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 8: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

28 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

and large series of blood samples cannot be taken from thesame individual (Buchanan-Smith et al 2003a b) Howeverthere is growing evidence that marmosets can be trained toco-operate in a range of tasks using only PRT and with noneed for food or water management (eg McKinley et al2003 Scott et al 2003 Smith et al 2004) The typical tasksinclude training to co-operate in scientific procedures andtests of cognitive ability and to facilitate husbandry andveterinary procedures (see Prescott et al 2005a) Forexample marmosets have been trained to stand on a balancefor in home-cage weighing and to urinate into a collectionvial on request (McKinley et al 2003) One advantage ofthis training is that it avoids the need for capture andrestraint which can be stressful for primates (Reinhardtet al 1990 1995 National Research Council 1998 Saucedaamp Schmidt 2000)Only one establishment of the five housing marmosets hastrained its marmosets for in home-cage weighing At theremainder marmosets are caught for weighing (one perweek to once per month) or scientific procedures (daily orless frequently) either by chasing them into a nest box or byhand using a gauntlet or surgical glove Although someanimals if exposed often enough to capture and restraintappear to acquiesce andor tolerate this practice there arephysiological data which demonstrate that restraint canremain stressful for marmosets and macaques even whenthe animals are habituated to the procedure over a longperiod (Mann 1991 Morrow-Tesch et al 1993 Schnell ampGerber 1997) this can have implications for the dataobtained from such animals

Training practice and staff educationJunior and senior animal technicians and scientists are theindividuals most commonly responsible for determining theneed to train primates and for conducting training of theanimals (Table 9) Regarding techniques used to train thereare a variety of schedules of behaviour modification (seeTable 1) Positive reinforcement is generally considered themost humane of these options (Laule 1999 Laule et al2003 Pryor 2002) It involves voluntary co-operation forpleasurable reward rather than coercion via the threat of anegative event or experience and therefore gives the animalgreater control over the interaction which is a desirablescenario for the well-being of captive animals (Weiss 1968)Nine out of 15 establishments use both positive andnegative reinforcement sometimes in the context of thesame procedure and three establishments use negative rein-forcement only (see Table 10) which is a cause for concernNo establishments used clicker-training We recommendthat training methods should be based on positive reinforce-ment and that negative reinforcement should only be usedwhen positive alternatives have been shown to be ineffec-tive If NRT must be used (eg for training aversive proce-dures) it should be used in combination with PRT(McKinley 2004)The most common positive reinforcers are verbal praise andfood (Table 11) although the efficacy of verbal praise as areinforcer has not been shown Preferred foods can behighly motivating but foods used for training need to becounted in the nutritional content of the diet (Scott 1990)and junk food should be avoided The most common

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 9 Responsibilities for training (n = 15) as described by those surveyed

Technique used Total (n = 15)Positive and negative reinforcement 9

Negative reinforcement only 3Food or water management 4

Positive reinforcement only 2

Negative punishment1 2Positive punishment2 1Clicker training -

Responsible for determining the needto train primates

Responsible for training primates

Junior animal technician 10 12Senior animal technician 12 7Scientist 8 8

Veterinarian 5 -Ethical review process 3 -Customer of breeding establishment 2

Table 10 Techniques used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something pleasant is removed on its performance 2 Frequency of a behaviour is decreased because something unpleasant is introduced on its performance

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 9: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

Training non-human primates a UK survey 29

negative reinforcers are use of a cage squeeze-backmechanism to encourage animals to come to the front of acage and to be restrained sight of a net and chasing byhumans all of which can be avoided with PRTEstablishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interaction where possibleThere is little consistency in the provision of educationalopportunities for learning about training primates Staffdevelopment at seven establishments does not includeinformation about training primates whereas at sevenothers this information is included as part of on-the-jobtraining (Table 12) Members of staff learn how to trainprimates most often through word of mouth At only twoestablishments have staff attended a dedicated course ontraining animals However 14 establishments indicatedthat they would be willing to fund attendance on a UKcourse of this kindGenerally up to four staff members per establishment areinvolved in training the animals although usually there isone main trainer Animals are trained singly in pairs or in

groups in sessions ranging from lt 15 minutes to over2 hours (depending on the purpose of training) Differencesin the ease of training were reported for animals of differentage sex temperament species source past experience andin different group sizes and housing types In additionestablishments reported a number of ways to improve theefficiency of training programmes Although these aresubjective reports UFAW PHHSC funded research isunderway to identify ways to optimise the time investmentinvolved and make training more effective includingamong other things the effect of animal age sex andtemperament on the time investment required for trainingwith positive reinforcement (Bowell et al 2004)

Costs and benefits of trainingCosts and benefits of training reported by those establish-ments that train are given in Table 13 Generally speakingthe largest cost of training is the initial time investment ineducating staff and implementing the behaviour modifica-tion process However there is every indication that thisinvestment will be recouped within a short period and thatit is more than outweighed by the benefits to primates staff

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 11 Reinforcers and punishments used to train as described by those surveyed

1 Fresh fruit dried fruit vegetables seeds nuts pulses cereals primate treats banana flavoured pellets chocolate sweets rice paperfor macaques rusk marshmallow for marmosets2 Ribenareg for macaques banana milkshake for marmosets3 Although reported as a negative reinforcer human presence can have a positive or negative effect on animal welfare depending on theactions of the human

Reinforcerpunishment Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)

Positive reinforcement

Verbal praise 6 3

Food1 7 1

Tactile contact with humans (petting) 5 -

Fluid2 3 1

Contact with conspecifics 3 -

Negative reinforcement

Cage squeeze-back mechanism 9 -

Sight of net 5 2

Chasing by human 3 1

Presence of human in front or back or inside of home enclosure3 2 2

Loud stern voice 1 2

Noise from banging on enclosure fittings 2 -

Movement of human limbs or hands - 2

Rattling of keys - 1

Tap on restraint chair 1 -

Positive punishment

Verbal command lsquoNorsquo 1 -

Squirt of water - 1 (when fighting)

Negative punishment

Time out from reward 2 1

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 10: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

30 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 12 Training practice and staff education as described by those surveyed

1 Usually there is one main trainer Manipulanda objects that are manipulated by the primatersquos hands

Total (n = 15)Does staff development include training of primates If so howNo 7On-the-job training 7Attendance on a dedicated course 2Modular training under A(SP)A 1IAT course 1Visits to other establishments 1What resources are used to train people to train primatesWord of mouth (colleagues) 10Scientific literature 3Attendance on a dedicated course 2Attendance at scientific meetings 1Professional guidelines -Advice from a training specialist -Advice from HO inspector -If there was a UK course on training primates who should attendAnimal technicians 8 Senior animal technicians 7All staff working with primates 5 Veterinarians 2Scientists 1

Number of primates in a training session1 102 53-6 67-30 1Number of trainers involved1

1 42-4 65-12 1Length of training session (minutes)

lt 15 5

15-120 2gt 120 6Have you encountered any differences in the ease of training

Sex 5Temperament 3Group size 2Source 2Species 1Age 1Housing 1Past experience of the animal 1Have you identified any ways of improving the success and efficiency of your training programme (open question)

Different reinforcers for individual animals according to their preference 1Formal training for trainers 1Some animals work better after feeding - they are less distracted frustrated and aggressive 1Trainers with a good awareness of primate behaviour 1Good communication between trainers 1Adapted cages chairs and jackets to make them more comfortable for the animals 1Pair housing ndash animals are more relaxed 1Begin socialisation with humans early in life 1Begin training early in life with young animals 1Change task manipulanda in order to keep the animalsrsquo attention 1

Water management with water under complete control 1

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 11: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

Training non-human primates a UK survey 31

and science in terms of improved animal welfare facilitatedmanagement and reduction in the variability of researchdata (Table 13 Thurston 1992 cited in Desmond amp Laule1994 Schnell amp Gerber 1997 McKinley et al 2003Savastano et al 2003 Schapiro et al 2003 Reinhardt 2003)No costs to primates were reported from training with PRTUnfortunately there is a lack of objective quantification ofboth costs and benefits of training by establishments butsuch data are increasingly available in the scientific litera-ture (see Prescott amp Buchanan-Smith 2003) Five establish-ments commented that well-socialised and trained animalsyield more andor better quality data relative to untrainedanimals which could lead to reduction in the number ofanimals used although they had not attempted to quantifythis Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured This enablesthe progress of the programme to be monitored and can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedOnly two of the 11 establishments that train have publisheddetails of their use of training and it is very rare formethods of training to be included in the methods section ofmainstream science journals Establishments should shareinformation on training attempts (failures and successes) by

documenting and publishing their work and by visitingother establishments This will help identification andimplementation of humane training methods and hencerefinement Where possible publications should includedetails of the training techniques protocols cues andrewards housing and husbandry time investment effective-ness and potential for reduction of animal numbers

Socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisationTable 14 shows the importance of socialisation withhumans habituation and desensitisation as rated by partici-pants in the survey These processes underpin and comple-ment training efforts (Scott 1991 Laule et al 1996 Laule1999 McKinley et al 2003) and were most often rated asbeing of lsquohighrsquo importance both for primate well-being andfor efficient use of primates The majority of participantsfelt that these processes resulted in calmer less fearfulanimals experimental data from which are likely to be moreconsistent and meaningful Macaques and marmosets are most often socialised withhumans by animal technicians interacting positively withthe primates around routine husbandry throughout theanimalsrsquo lives (Table 15) However four macaque and three

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

Table 13 Costs and benefits of training (n = 11) as described by those surveyed

Costs Benefits

To primates Hunger or thirst when food or water management is used (1)Distress when negative reinforcement isused (1)

Less stress for the animals (11)Enrichment through greater mental stimulation and control (4)More satisfactory human-animal interaction(animal well-being) (3)Reduced opportunity for injury during procedure (1)

To staff Time investment (3)Emotional upset when familiar animals areeuthanased (1)Money for staff training courses (1)

Less stress for staff (4)Increased staff morale (3)Improved efficiency of procedure (3)Reduced concern over health and safety ofstaff (3)

To science Time investment (1) Better quality data (7)Able to use animals for longer (2)More data per animal (2)Reduction in number of animals used (1)

Table 14 Importance of socialisation with humans habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Macaques (13) Marmosets (5)Low Medium High Low Medium High

For primate well-being

Socialisation with humans - 1 10 - - 5

Habituation 1 1 9 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 1 5 1 - 4

For use of primates

Socialisation with humans - - 10 - - 4

Habituation 1 - 11 1 - 3

Desensitisation 1 2 7 1 1 3

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 12: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

32 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Table 15 Socialisation with humans as described by those surveyed

Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Responsible for socialising primates with humansAnimal technician 10 4Senior animal technician 6 5Scientist 4 1

Veterinarian - -Time when socialisation is conductedRoutine husbandry 10 4Routine husbandry and dedicated session 4 3Number of dedicated sessions per week 1 - 7 (mean = 5) 7 (mean = 7)Length of each session (minutes) 5 - 60 (mean = 35) 5 - 60 (mean = 27)Kind of human animal interaction involved -

Feeding 8 5Play 6 2Grooming 6 1

Location where socialisation takes place

Home enclosure 10 5Indoor play area 1 -Outdoor enclosure 1 -Other 1 (restraint chair) -Do staff members go into the animalrsquos enclosures Yes 3 3No 7 2Typical group size during interaction 1 ndash 30 (mean = 5) 2 ndash 100 (mean = 12)

Table 16 Purposes of and responsibilities for habituation and desensitisation as described by those surveyed

Habituation DesensitisationPurpose Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5) Macaques (n = 13) Marmosets (n = 5)Scientific procedures

Restraint in chair 7 - 2 -

Venepuncture 2 - 1 3Oral gavage 1 - 2 1Oral capsule 1 - 1 -Injection 1 - 1 -Jacket 1 - - -Cage squeeze-back mechanism 1 - - -

Mask for inhalation 1 - - -

Nasal administration 1 - 1 1Procedure room 1 - - -

Generation of data on cognitive tasks

Neck bar on chair 3 - - -Testing room 2 - 1 -Arm sleeve 2 - - -

Head fixation 1 - - -Electrodes on skin 1 - 1 -Novel manipulanda 1 - - -Touch screen 1 - - -Husbandry and health proceduresTransport box 4 - - -Weighing - 1 - 2Swap infants for hand rearing - 1 - -Other

Human visitors - - - 2Handling - 1 - 1Responsiblity for habituation

Animal technician 7 - 6 4Senior animal technician 2 1 3 3Scientist 4 1 3 -Veterinarian 2 - 1 -Are records kept

Yes 5 1 2 1No 4 - 5 3

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 13: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

Training non-human primates a UK survey 33

marmoset units have dedicated socialisation sessions inaddition to routine husbandry which involve techniciansfeeding playing with andor grooming the animals for onaverage around 30 minutes five to seven days per week Ifthe health status of the animals is known and they are freefrom zoonotic disease dedicated sessions take placewithin the animalsrsquo home enclosures Health and safetyconcerns such as bites and scratches are classified as occu-pational hazardsAt establishments where habituation takes place it is animaltechnicians that are responsible for it (Table 16) Sometimesrecords are kept as part of a daybook Habituation isgenerally not used for marmosets although at one breedingestablishment these animals are habituated to handlingweighing and swapping of infants for rotational hand-rearing At seven establishments macaques are habituated torestraint in a lsquoprimate chairrsquo and a few other establishmentshabituate macaques to a variety of other stimuli and situa-tions There is however a lack of consistency betweenestablishments which is difficult to understand given thathabituation is relatively low cost and easy to implement Forexample only one of the 13 establishments using macaquesrequires that animals be habituated to a transport containerbefore national or international transport This is done byplacing an identical container in their home enclosureseveral weeks prior to transport (Swallow et al 2005Wolfensohn amp Honess 2005)Habituation to procedures typically involves introducing theanimals to increasing degrees of restraint andor stimuluscomplexity gradually over a period of days For example inthe case of a macaque used in an inhalation study theanimal might first be habituated to restraint in a primatechair This may involve placing the chair in the animalrsquoshome enclosure for a few days so that heshe has the oppor-tunity to explore the apparatus and become familiar with itThe animal is then restrained in the chair for short periodsof time (minutes to hours) the length of which are graduallyincreased say over one week to that required for the studyNext the animal is habituated to a face mask while sitting inthe chair then the mask with positive pressure then aerosoldelivery through the mask and finally aerosol delivery ofthe test substance The intention is that by introducing theanimal to successive approximations of the procedures day1 of study is likely to be less stressful for the animal and thedata obtained more reliable Some establishments desensitise primates to a variety ofuncomfortable or aversive procedures equipment andsituations usually by providing food rewards during orafterwards (Table 16) However overall incidence ofdesensitisation is low and there is little consistency inits use between establishments and species Again it ismost often technicians who are responsible for desensi-tising primates

Conclusion and animal welfare implicationsThe survey demonstrates that there is widespread awarenessin the UK research community of training as a refinement

and appreciation of its diverse benefits but training is notused as widely or as fully as it might be This is due to realconstraints (principally a lack of staff and time and a lack ofconfidence in ability to train) but also perceivedconstraints which can be overcome by information sharingand education (such as a supposed lack of published infor-mation on how to train and assessment of the benefits andan overestimation of the time investment needed) It is clearthen that there is opportunity for refinement of commonscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures (such asblood and urine collection injection capture from the groupand weighing) through use of positive reinforcementtraining especially when combined with appropriate social-isation with humans habituation and desensitisationIn order to take advantage of this opportunity for refinementfacility managers and principal investigators must ensureappropriate staff levels and sufficient time for training beforestudies begin and consider how they can best support theirstaff to work with co-operative trained animals rather thanresisting fearful ones In addition published information andguidance on training must be made more readily available to allthose who use primates in research and testing To assist estab-lishments in achieving these goals part 2 of this paper (Prescottet al 2005a) includes a tabulated literature review of primatetraining a detailed sample training protocol and guidance ondeveloping and implementing a training programme based onPRT including resource and personnel requirements

Recommendationsbull The possibility of training primates to co-operate withscientific veterinary and husbandry procedures as a lessstressful alternative to traditional methods should always beconsidered when planning a research project and re-assessed during the life of the projectbull Regulators (eg UK Home Office Inspectors and localethical review processes) should take an active role inpromoting use of training as a proven refinementbull Facility managers and principal investigators should bereceptive to animal technicians who identify a need fortraining and should support them in this regardbull Primate behaviour and animal training specialists shouldtake the lead in making published information on trainingprimates more widely available and in challenging miscon-ceptions about the resource implications of trainingbull From the standpoint of animal welfare training methodsshould be based on positive reinforcement (reward)Negative reinforcement should only be used when positivealternatives have been shown to be ineffective If NRT mustbe used (eg for training aversive procedures or when verylittle training time is available) it should be used in combi-nation with PRTbull Establishments should create an inventory of all currentinteractions with primates and move to a PRT-based systemof interactionbull Training programmes should be planned carefully Adviceon application of learning processes should be sought froma primate behaviour andor animal training specialist

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 14: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

34 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure appropriate staff levels for training programmes tobe carried out optimally (eg by reorganising daily workroutines) bull Facility managers and principal investigators shouldensure sufficient time for training before studies begin (egby breeding in-house or importing animals well in advanceof studies and holding them as stock)bull There should be more frequent and timely communicationand co-ordination between breeders and users of primatesso that animals can be better prepared for the challenges oflife in a laboratory environment and for the specific proce-dures that they are used forbull As a first step to training animals should be socialisedwith humans and habituated to accept food treats from thehands of staff as part of a formal socialisation programmebull Habituation and desensitisation should be used morewidely particularly in the early phases of trainingprogrammes By making stimuli less fearful or stressfulthese processes can increase the animalrsquos willingness tocooperate with training and thereby achieve training goalsmore quicklybull Training programmes should be implemented andexecuted in such a way that the results (benefits costs andeffectiveness) can be quantitatively measured The progressof the programme should be monitored as this can helpjustify to senior management the time investment involvedbull Establishments should share information on trainingattempts (failures and successes) by documenting andpublishing their work and by visiting other establishmentsWhere possible publications should include details of thetraining techniques protocols cues and rewards housingand husbandry time investment effectiveness and potentialfor improved validity of scientific findings and reduction ofanimal numbersbull All staff including management should appreciate thatthe provision of consistent socialisation habituation andtraining is as important as all other husbandry activitybull All animal care staff (including veterinarians and principalinvestigators) should be trained in order to ensure that all oftheir actions make a positive and consistent contributiontowards any socialisation habituation or trainingprogramme Failure to do so can result in unintentional rein-forcement of undesirable behaviour causing misunder-standing and frustration for both primates and staffmembers Such training should include guidance on usingoperant conditioning and interpreting primate behaviour

AcknowledgementsWe gratefully acknowledge all establishments and individ-uals that participated in the survey and members of theUFAW PHHSC who commented on the survey questionsThanks to Anita Rennie (AER) for conducting two inter-views in Scotland MJP was employed by the RSPCA and isnow at the NC3Rs HMB-S was supported by a grant fromthe European Commission (QLRT-2001-00028)

ReferencesAdams KM Navarro AM Hutchinson EK and Weed JL2004 A canine socialization and training program at the NationalInstitutes of Health Lab Animal 33 32-36Baker KC 2004 Benefits of human interaction for socially housedchimpanzees Animal Welfare 13 239-245Bassett L Buchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and SmithTE 2003 Effects of training on stress-related behavior of thecommon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) in relation to coping withroutine husbandry procedures Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 221-233Bayne KA 2002 Development of the human-research animalbond and its impact on animal well-being Institute for LaboratoryAnimal Research 43 4-9Bayne KA Dexter SL and Strange GM 1993 The effects offood treat provisioning and human interaction on the behavioralwell-being of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) ContemporaryTopics in Laboratory Animal Science 32 6-9Biological Council 1992 Guidelines on the Handling andTraining of Laboratory Animals UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UKBloomsmith M 1992 Chimpanzee training and behavioralresearch A symbiotic relationship American Association ofZoological Parks and Aquariums Annual Conference Proceedingspp 403-410 American Association of Zoological Parks andAquariums Toronto Ontario CanadaBloomsmith MA Lambeth SP Stone AM and Laule GE1997 Comparing two types of human interaction as enrichmentfor chimpanzees American Journal of Primatology 42 96 (Abstract)Bloomsmith MA Baker KC Ross SK and Lambeth SP1999 Comparing animal training to non-training human interac-tion as environmental enrichment for chimpanzees AmericanJournal of Primatology 49 35-36Bourgeois SR and Brent L 2005 Modifying the behaviour ofsingly caged baboons evaluating the effectiveness of four enrich-ment techniques Animal Welfare 14 71-81Bowell V Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2004 Theeffect of animal age sex and temperament on the time investmentrequired for positive reinforcement training of common mar-mosets Folia Primatologica 75(S1) 359-360Boxall J Heath S Bate S and Brautigam J 2004 Modernconcepts of socialisation for dogs Implications for their behav-iour welfare and use in scientific procedures Alternatives toLaboratory Animals 32(S2) 81-93 httpaltwebjhsphedupublica-tionsjournalsatla32_supboxallp1pdf Brockway BP Hassler CR and Hicks N 1993 Minimizingstress during physiological monitoring In Niemi SM Willson JE(eds) Refinement and Reduction in Animal Testing pp 56-69Scientists Center for Animal Welfare Bethesda MD USABuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J and Prescott MJ 2003aPositive reinforcement training with New World primates JointLASALAVA Meeting Animal Training as Refinement Basis andBenefits 27 June 2003 Ware Hertfordshire UKBuchanan-Smith HM McKinley J Bassett L Morris KRennie A and Prescott MJ 2003b The potential uses of posi-tive reinforcement training in marmosets European MarmosetResearch Group UK Meeting 23-24 July 2003 Cambridge UKColeman K Tully LA and McMillan JL 2005 Temperamentcorrelates with training success in adult rhesus macaquesAmerican Journal of Primatology 65 63-71Desmond T and Laule G 1994 Use of positive reinforcementtraining in the management of species for reproduction ZooBiology 13 471-477

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 15: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

Training non-human primates a UK survey 35

Elvidge H Challis JRG Robinson JS Roper C andThorburn GD 1976 Influence of handling and sedation on plas-ma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Journal ofEndocrinology 70 325-326Hassimoto M Harada T and Harada T 2004 Changes inhematology biochemical values and restraint ECG of rhesusmonkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclima-tion Journal of Medical Primatology 33 175-186Heath M 1989 The training of cynomolgus monkeys and how thehuman-animal relationship improves with environmental enrich-ment Animal Technology 40 11-22 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioat40heathhtml Heath SE Thomas J and Deegan C 2002 Preparing laborato-ry beagles for their life as a working dog Fourth World Congress onAlternatives and Animal Use in the Life Sciences 11-15 August 2002New Orleans Louisiana USAHome Office 1989 Code of Practice for the Housing and Care ofAnimals Used in Scientific Procedures HMSO London httpsci-enceandresearchhomeofficegovukanimalresearchpublica-tionspublicationscode-of-practiceHome Office 2000 Guidance on the Operation of the Animals(Scientific Procedures) Act 1986 The Stationary Office Londonhttpwwwarchiveoff icial-documentscoukdocumenthoc321321-00htm Iliff SA Friscino BH and Anderson LC 2004 Refinements ofstudy design using positive reinforcement training in macaques FoliaPrimatologica 74(S1) 282-283International Primatological Society 1989 IPS Internationalguidelines for the acquisition care and breeding of nonhuman pri-mates Primate Report 25 3-27 httpwwwenvirocozavervetcare_and_breedinghtm Laboratory Animal Science AssociationMedical ResearchCouncil 2004 Principles of Best Practice in the Breeding of Macaquesand Marmosets for Scientific Purposes A Statement by the LaboratoryAnimal Science Association and the Medical Research Councilhttpwwwlasacoukposition_papersLASACBPAR20breed-ing20of20primatespdfLambeth SP Hau J Perlman JE Martino MA Bernacky BJand Schapiro SJ 2004 Positive reinforcement training affectshematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees(Pan troglodytes) American Journal of Primatology 62(S1) 37-38Laule G 1999 Training laboratory animals In Poole T (ed) UFAWHandbook on the Care and Management of Laboratory Animals Edition7 Volume 1 ndash Terrestrial Vertebrates pp 21-27 Blackwell ScienceOxford UKLaule GE Thurston RH Alford PL and Bloomsmith MA1996 Training to reliably obtain blood and urine samples from a dia-betic chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) Zoo Biology 15 587-591Laule GE Bloomsmith MA and Schapiro SJ 2003 The use ofpositive reinforcement training techniques to enhance the caremanagement and welfare of laboratory primates Journal of AppliedAnimal Welfare Science 6 163-173Likert R 1932 A technique for the measurement of attitudesArchives of Psychology 140 1-55Luttrell L Acker L Urben M and Reinhardt V 1994 Training alarge troop of rhesus macaques to co-operate during catchingAnalysis of the time investment Animal Welfare 3 135-140httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbiblioaw5trainhtm Mann WA Welzel G and Kinter LB 1991 Determination ofresting blood pressure in unrestrained cynomolgus monkeys usingimplanted telemetric transmitters The Toxicologist 11 335(Abstract)

McKinley J 2004 Training in a laboratory environment Methodseffectiveness and welfare implications of two species of primateUnpublished PhD thesis University of Stirling Scotland UKMcKinley J Buchanan-Smith HM Bassett L and Morris K2003 Training common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) to co-operateduring routine laboratory procedures Ease of training and timeinvestment Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 209-220Medical Research Council 2004 Best practice in the accommoda-tion and care of primates used in scientific procedures Medical ResearchCouncil London UK httpwwwmrcacukUtilitiesDocumentrecordindexhtmd=MRC002395Mitchell DS Wigodsky HS Peel HH and McCaffrey TA1980 Operant conditioning permits voluntary non-invasive meas-urement of blood pressure in conscious unrestrained baboons(Papio cynocephalus) Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation12 492-298Morrow-Tesch JL McGlone JJ and Norman RL 1993Consequences of restraint stress on natural killer cell activitybehavior and hormone levels in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulat-ta) Psychoendocrinology 18 383-395Moseley JR and Davis JA 1989 Psychological enrichment techniquesand New World monkey restraint device reduce colony managementtime Lab Animal 18 31-33National Research Council 1998 The Psychological Well-being of Nonhuman Primates National Academy PressWashington DC USA httpwwwnapeducatalog4909htmltocPrescott MJ and Buchanan-Smith HM 2003 Training non-human primates using positive reinforcement techniquesGuest editorsrsquo introduction Journal of Applied Animal WelfareScience 6 157-161Prescott MJ Bowell VA and Buchanan-Smith HM2005a Training laboratory-housed non-human primates part2 Resources for developing and implementing training pro-grammes Animal Technology and Welfare 4(3) 133-148Prescott MJ Buchanan-Smith HM and Rennie A 2005bTraining of laboratory-housed non-human primates in the UKAnthrozooumls 18(3) 288-303Pryor K 2002 Donrsquot Shoot the Dog The new art of teaching and train-ing Revised Edition Ringpress Books Gloucestershire UKReinhardt V 1990 Avoiding undue stress Catching individual ani-mals in groups of laboratory rhesus monkeys Lab Animal 19 52-53httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliola-avoidhtm Reinhardt V 1991 Impact of venipuncture on physiologicalresearch conducted in conscious macaques Journal of ExperimentalAnimal Science 34 212-217 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_ani-malsbiblioes34-2~1htmReinhardt V 1992 Transport-cage training of caged rhesusmacaques Animal Technology 43 57-61 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbiblioat57htmReinhardt V 1997 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate dur-ing handling procedures A review Animal Technology 48 55-73httpwwwbrowneduResearchPrimatelpn36-4htmlReinhardt V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques dur-ing blood collection Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6 189-197 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliojaaws11htmlReinhardt V 2004 Common husbandry-related variables in bio-medical research with animals Laboratory Animals 38 213-235Reinhardt V Cowley D Scheffler J Vertein R andWegner F 1990 Cortisol response of female rhesus monkeys tovenipuncture in homecage versus venipuncture restraint appara-tus Journal of Medical Primatology 19 601-606 httpwwwawion-lineorgLab_animalsbibliojmp19htm

Animal Welfare 2007 16 21-36

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Page 16: Training laboratory-housed non-human primates, part 1: a UK survey

36 Prescott and Buchanan-Smith

Reinhardt V Liss C and Stephens C 1995 Restraint methods oflaboratory non-human primates a review Animal Welfare 4 221-238Russell WMS and Burch RL 1959 The Principles of HumaneExperimental Technique Methuen London UKhttpaltwebjhsphedupublicationshumane_exphet-tochtmSauceda R and Schmidt MG 2000 Refining macaque handlingand restraint techniques Lab Animal 29 47-49Savastano G Hanson A and McCann C 2003 The develop-ment of an operant conditioning training programme for NewWorld primates at the Bronx Zoo Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 247-261Schapiro SJ 2000 A few new developments in primate housingand husbandry Scandinavian Journal of Laboratory Animal Science 27103-110 httpbiomedicumuteesjlas27_2_103pdfSchapiro SJ Bloomsmith MA and Laule GE 2003 Positivereinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primatebehavior Quantitative assessments of effectiveness Journal ofApplied Animal Welfare Science 6 175-187Schapiro SJ Perlman JE Thiele E and Lambeth S 2005Training nonhuman primates to perform behaviours useful in bio-medical research Lab Animal 34 37-42Scientific Committee on Animal Health and Welfare 2002The Welfare on Non-Human Primates Used in Research Health andConsumer Protection Directorate-General EuropeanCommission Brussels Belgium httpwwwaisalorgpagesdoc_variout83_enpdfSchnell CR and Gerber P 1997 Training and remote monitoringof cardiovascular parameters in non-human primates PrimateReport 49 61-70 httpwwwawionlineorgLab_animalsbibliopr49-6~1htm

Scott L 1990 Training non-human primates meeting theirbehavioural needs In UFAW (ed) Animal training A review andcommentary pp 129-133 UFAW WheathampsteadHertfordshire UK Scott L 1991 Environmental enrichment for single housed com-mon marmosets In Box HO (ed) Primate Responses toEnvironmental Change pp 265-274 Chapman and Hall London UKScott L Pearce P Fairhall S Muggleton N and Smith J2003 Training nonhuman primates to cooperate with scientific pro-cedures in applied biomedical research Journal of Applied AnimalWelfare Science 6 199-207Smith TE McCallister JM Gordon SJ and Whittikar M2004 Quantitative data on training New World primates to urinateAmerican Journal of Primatology 64 83-93Swallow J Anderson D Buckwell AC Harris T Hawkins PKirkwood J Lomas M Meacham S Peters A Owen SPrescott M Quest R Sutcliffe R and Thompson K 2005Guidance for the transport of laboratory animals Laboratory Animals39 1-39Videan EN Fritz J Murphy J Howell S and Heward CB2005 Does training chimpanzees to present for injection lead toreduced stress Laboratory Primate Newsletter 44 1-2Waitt C Buchanan-Smith HM and Morris K 2002 The effectsof caretaker-primate relationships on primates in the laboratoryJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 5 309-319Weiss JM 1968 Effects of coping responses on stress Journal ofComparative Physiology and Psychology 65 251-260Wolfensohn S and Honess P 2005 Handbook of PrimateHusbandry and Welfare Blackwell Publishing Ltd Oxford UK

copy 2007 Universities Federation for Animal Welfare