thesis with questionnaire

32
The influence of personality on career success: An empirical study ERASMUS UNIVERSITY ROTTERDAM Erasmus School of Economics Department of Economics Master Thesis – Economics of Management and Organisations Supervisor: Dr. S.V. Kapoor Name: L.B. de Gruijter Student no: 332965 E-mail address: [email protected]

Upload: faraliza-jumayleeza

Post on 28-Jan-2016

24 views

Category:

Documents


4 download

DESCRIPTION

about international marketing

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Thesis With Questionnaire

The influence of personality on career

success: An empirical study

ERASMUS UNIVERSITY ROTTERDAM

Erasmus School of Economics

Department of Economics

Master Thesis – Economics of Management and Organisations

Supervisor: Dr. S.V. Kapoor

Name: L.B. de Gruijter

Student no: 332965

E-mail address: [email protected]

Page 2: Thesis With Questionnaire

I

Abstract

This thesis studies the relation between the “Big Five” personality traits (openness to

experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness and neuroticism) and career

success. Career success is divided into income and job satisfaction to be able to study the

differences in the relation of these two types of career success. Cross-sectional data of the

Longitudinal Internet Studies for the Social sciences panel (LISS) is used to investigate the

possible relation between personality and career success.

Results obtained from 1940 Dutch workers reveal that conscientious, extraverted, calm and

disagreeable workers report to have higher incomes. Furthermore, it is shown that

conscientious, agreeable and calm workers report to have higher job satisfaction. It is found

that demography-related, human capital related and profession related variables are

associated with income and job satisfaction. Additionally, women are found to have higher

incomes than men for equal levels of the traits openness to experience and neuroticism.

Page 3: Thesis With Questionnaire

II

Contents

Abstract .................................................................................................................................................... I

Contents .................................................................................................................................................. II

List of tables and figures ........................................................................................................................ III

Introduction ............................................................................................................................................. 1

Related literature .................................................................................................................................... 3

Career success ..................................................................................................................................... 3

The Big Five personality traits ............................................................................................................. 3

Personality and career success............................................................................................................ 4

Data and methodology ............................................................................................................................ 6

Dependent variables ........................................................................................................................... 6

Independent variables ......................................................................................................................... 7

Other career success predictors .......................................................................................................... 7

Methodological approach ................................................................................................................. 10

Empirical results .................................................................................................................................... 11

Effect of personality on income ........................................................................................................ 11

Effect of personality on job satisfaction ............................................................................................ 13

Discussion .......................................................................................................................................... 15

Practical implications......................................................................................................................... 16

Limitations ......................................................................................................................................... 16

Extension – Male and Female career success differences .................................................................... 19

Conclusions ............................................................................................................................................ 23

Appendices ............................................................................................................................................ 24

Bibliography ........................................................................................................................................... 26

Page 4: Thesis With Questionnaire

III

List of tables and figures

Table 1: Variable definitions.

Table 2: Descriptive statistics: Means and standard deviations of the variables.

Table 3: Correlations between the Big Five personality traits and the dependent variables.

Table 4: OLS estimates: The effect of personality on income.

Table 5: OLS estimates: The effect of personality on job satisfaction.

Table 6: Testing for nonlinear effect of personality on career success.

Table 7: Male – Female differences in personality traits and income.

Table 8: Male – Female differences in personality traits and job satisfaction.

Figure 1: Means of the different traits between the sexes.

Page 5: Thesis With Questionnaire

1

Introduction

What factors lead some employees to be more successful in their careers than others? This

interesting and important question is just partly answered through prior research. In this

thesis the effect of personality on income and job satisfaction is investigated. The “Big Five”

personality traits are used for measuring personality, to determine the possible relation with

career success. The Big Five personality traits are: openness to experience,

conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness and neuroticism (Costa and McCrae, 1992).

Prior empirical research shows that a person with more human capital1 has on average

more career success than other persons (Hanushek and Woessmann, 2008) (Boissiere et al.,

1985). Other evidence shows that demography-related factors like age, sex and race are

predictors of career success. However, these factors explain just a small part2 of the

difference in career success between people (Bowles et al., 2001). There is less evidence

about the effect of non-cognitive skills3 on career success. Knowing what affects income and

job satisfaction is important and relevant for employees who want to increase their own

welfare. If an employee is able to recognize his/her own personality he/she can select

him/herself in certain jobs. Furthermore, employees who are more satisfied with their jobs

can be more beneficial for organisations (Freeman, 1978) (Delfgaauw, 2007). This makes it

beneficial for organisations to select the right employees for the right jobs.

The main purpose of this thesis is to examine the effects of the Big Five personality traits on

career success. In this thesis income and job satisfaction are used to measure career success.

The second objective is to explore the effect of demography-related, human capital related

and profession related variables on the above mentioned relationship. The research

question for this thesis is:

What is the role of personality towards differences in career success between employees and

why?

1The collective skills, knowledge, or other intangible assets of individuals that can be used to create

economic value for the individuals, their employers, or their community (Oxford Dictionary). 2 Judge et al. (1995) found that these factors explain 31% of the variance in income and 15.5% of the variance

in job satisfaction in the US. Levin & Plug (1999) found that 35% of the variance in income could be explained by these factors in the Netherlands. 3 Non-cognitive skills are any skills that are not cognitive and include emotional maturity, empathy,

interpersonal skills and verbal and non-verbal communication.

Page 6: Thesis With Questionnaire

2

In this thesis use is made of data of the LISS (Longitudinal Internet Studies for the Social

sciences) panel administered by CentERdata (Tilburg University, The Netherlands), a

representative sample of Dutch individuals who participate in monthly internet surveys. The

questionnaire used, includes 50 items from the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP)

(Goldberg et al. 2006) to measure the Big Five personality traits. Previously, hardly any

research is performed that simultaneously investigates intrinsic career success (job

satisfaction) and extrinsic career success (income); this thesis aims to fill this void. As an

extension to this thesis, a distinction between men and women is made. Gender-personality

interaction terms are included to test whether there are gender specific effects of

personality on career success. This is done to determine whether men and women face

different returns for the same personality traits. Osborne (2000) shows, for instance, that

women are punished in the wage setting for being aggressive, while men are punished for

withdrawal. This is the first study that studies this effect on job satisfaction while the effect

already has been tested on income.

The results show that conscientious workers report higher career success and more neurotic

workers report lower career success. Agreeable workers report to have a lower income but a

higher job satisfaction. Extraverted workers report to have a higher income but no relation

with job satisfaction is found. Remarkable is that workers who are more open to experience

report to have higher incomes and have less job satisfaction. The relation of some of the

personality traits on career success changes when demography-related, human capital

related and profession related variables are added. Additionally, it is found that women earn

more for the traits openness to experience and neuroticism than men with the same level of

these traits.

Page 7: Thesis With Questionnaire

3

Related literature

Career success

Career success is most commonly defined as: “… the extrinsic and intrinsic outcomes or

achievements individuals have accumulated as a result of their work experiences” (Judge et

al., 1995). Looking at the extrinsic outcomes that individuals have accumulated there is focus

on objectively and externally observable outcomes like salary or promotions (London and

Stumpf, 1982). For the intrinsic achievements that individuals have accumulated there is

focus on subjectively reported outcomes like career satisfaction or job satisfaction

(Greenhaus, Parasuraman, and Wormley, 1990). It is important to use both to measure

career success because they measure different aspects of career success and can be

influenced by different factors (Bray and Howard, 1980).

The Big Five personality traits

The objective of this thesis is to find effects of personality on career success. Personality is

measured using the Big Five personality traits. The five traits are openness to experience,

conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness and neuroticism. The questions asked to

measure the traits are found in appendix 1. Digman (1990) wrote about the emergence of

the Big Five personality traits and described them as follows:

Openness to experience includes a person’s curiosity and the tendency for seeking and

appreciating new experiences and novel ideas. People who score high are inventive and

curious. People who score low are more consistent and cautious.

Conscientiousness indicates the individual’s willingness to follow rules and schedules,

persistence, and the extent to which individuals are hardworking, organised, detailed, and

dependable, as opposed to lazy, disorganised and unreliable. People who score high are

more efficient and organized where low scoring people are more likely to be easy-going and

careless.

Extraversion encompasses the preference for human contact, attention and the wish to

inspire other people. People who score high tend to be outgoing and energetic and people

who score low are more solitary and reserved.

Agreeableness is the willingness to help other people and to act in accordance with other

person’s interests. Individuals who score high are more friendly and compassionate where a

low score means a more analytical and detached person.

Page 8: Thesis With Questionnaire

4

Neuroticism indicates adjustment versus emotional stability and addresses the degree to

which the individual is insecure, anxious, depressed and emotional rather than calm, self-

confident and cool. People who score high tend to be sensitive and nervous while people

who score low are more secure and confident.

The traits are commonly used by researchers to measure personality in a wide range of

social disciplines (Mount and Barrick, 1995). They occur in different cultures and languages

which indicates that it is possible to investigate cross country differences (Heine and Buchtel,

2009). Another feature of the traits is that they tend to be stable during adulthood (Costa

and McCrae, 1997). Despite the usefulness of the traits, the traits do not fully capture

personality. They ignore aspects like religiosity, sense of humour, honesty and risk-taking.

The traits should therefore be viewed as broad factors underlying a number of related

personality facets. Hendriks et al. (1999) tested the scale in multiple ways using various

datasets. They used three alternative Big Five measures to test validity and a test-retest

procedure was done in order to test stability in the assessments. They conclude that the IPIP

is a reliable, valid and efficient questionnaire to measure the Big Five personality traits.

Personality and career success

Prior literature shows that people who score high on conscientiousness and extraversion and

low on neuroticism and agreeableness score higher on extrinsic and intrinsic career success.

(Judge et al., 1999; Judge et al., 2002; Seibert and Kraimer., 2001). The exact hypotheses and

arguments for them are now presented.

Judge et al., (1999) shows that there is a positive relationship between conscientiousness

and career success. Conscientiousness is linked with being efficient, hard-working and

organized and those aspects proved to have a positive influence on extrinsic career success.

Conscientiousness leads to a higher possibility of satisfying work rewards like payment or

promotions (extrinsic) and recognition, respect and feelings of personal accomplishments

(intrinsic) (Organ and Lingl., 1995). The first hypothesis is that conscientious workers earn

more and are also more satisfied with their job.

Harrell and Alpert (1989) found that extraversion has a positive influence on income. They

argue that extravert people have more social contacts at work. This can help them to get a

promotion and to get higher incomes. Headey and Wearing (1989) found that extravert

Page 9: Thesis With Questionnaire

5

employees had higher job satisfaction. They argue that extraverts are more positive by their

nature and they generalized this to job satisfaction. So, the second hypothesis is that

extravert people earn more and are more satisfied with their jobs.

There are mixed results concerning the effect of agreeableness on career success. On the

one hand agreeableness can be rewarded by employers because these people are more

likely to give positive response to employers (Salgado, 1997). On the other hand more

agreeable people are less likely to claim higher wages and more likely to get overruled by

others. This is the reason that people who are more agreeable are more likely to have lower

incomes and also have less joy in their work (Johnson, 1997; Judge et al., 1999). The main

consensus regarding agreeableness is that it relates negatively to career success. The third

hypothesis is that agreeable people have lower incomes and are less satisfied with their jobs.

Barrick and Mount (1991) and Salgado (1997) found that neuroticism is negatively related to

job performance across occupations. Therefore it is expected that neuroticism leads to lower

wages. Prior literature also suggests that neuroticism leads to lower intrinsic career success

(Smith et al., 1983). This is due to the fact that more neuroticism leads to persons

overreacting to situations at work which is generalized to intrinsic career success. Therefore,

the fourth hypothesis is that people who score high on neuroticism earn less and have lower

job satisfaction.

There is not much empirical evidence that shows that people who are open to experience

have more or less career success. So, there is no hypothesis for the trait openness to

experience.

Prior research shows that demography-related variables like age, gender and marital status

affect career success (Judge et al., 1999). Other research shows that the human capital

related variable “level of education” also affect career success (Hanushek and Woessmann,

2008) (Boissiere et al., 1985). Finally, it has been shown that there is a difference in career

success between profession related variables. These include: working in the public or private

sector, tenure, type of profession and the number of employees working in an organisation

(Seibert and Kramer, 2001).

Page 10: Thesis With Questionnaire

6

Data and methodology

In this thesis data of the LISS (Longitudinal Internet Studies for the Social sciences) panel

administered by CentERdata (Tilburg University, The Netherlands) is used. “The LISS panel is

a representative sample of Dutch individuals who participate in monthly Internet surveys.

The panel is based on a true probability sample of households drawn from the population

register. Households that could not otherwise participate are provided with a computer and

Internet connection. A longitudinal survey is fielded in the panel every year, covering a large

variety of domains including work, education, income, housing, time use, political views,

values and personality (LISS, 2014)” Data is used from three studies of the LISS panel. The

first one is the study background variables of April 2012. This study includes variables like:

age, gender, income and marital status. The second study is work and schooling of April 2012

where variables like type of education, type of profession and working in the public or

private sector are included. The final study used is the personality study which includes five

waves every April between 2008 and 2012. In this study questions are asked that measure

the Big Five personality traits. Not every person filled in this questionnaire every year. For

this reason the means over the measured years is used as measure for the traits. Costa and

McCrae (1997) found that the Big Five personality traits do not change over time despite the

fact that people accumulate a lifetime of experiences. After combining the data of the

different studies, 1940 household members are left. The data includes people of 16 years4

and older. People who are actually working at the time of the questionnaire are selected.

Dependent variables

The first dependent variable is net monthly income. The question: “How much is your

personal net monthly income?” is asked. When people answer with “I don’t know”, a second

question is asked with income in different categories5. The mean of these categories is then

used as input in the variable personal net monthly income. There is no data on the number

of promotions of individuals so in this thesis only income is used to measure extrinsic career

success. The second dependent variable is job satisfaction and is measured with five

4 There is consensus that personality is not changing after the age of 30 (Caspi and Roberts, 1999). It is tested

for workers above 30 and no differences were found in the results compared with workers above 16, so the regressions are not presented. 5 The categories are: €500 or less, €501 to €1000, €1001 to €1500, €1501 to €2000, €2001 to 2500, €2501 to

€3000, €3001 to €3500, €3501 to €4000, €4001 to 4500, €4501 to €5000, €5001 to €7500, more than €7501 and I really don’t know.

Page 11: Thesis With Questionnaire

7

different questions. These questions asked employees to report their satisfaction with

various facets of their jobs (salary, working hours, type of work, atmosphere among your

colleagues, your current work). The answer categories range from 0 (not at all satisfied) to

10 (completely satisfied). A new variable is created that sums up the outcomes to measure

the job satisfaction of each individual with a range from 0 to 10. To test for the reliability of

the measuring questions Cronbach’s alpha (Cronbach, 1951) is used. For job satisfaction

Cronbach’s alpha is 0.83 which indicates a good reliability6.

Independent variables

The Big Five personality traits

Personality is measured using 50 items from the International Personality Item Pool (IPIP)

(Goldberg et al. 2006). For every personality trait 10 questions were asked starting with the

phrase: “Please use the rating scale below to describe how accurately each statement

describes you” (Appendix 1). The answer categories range from 1 (very inaccurate) to 5 (very

accurate). The Cronbach’s alpha for the five traits are: 0.77 for openness to experience, 0.77

for conscientiousness, 0.87 for extraversion, 0.80 for agreeableness and 0.88 for neuroticism

which indicates a good reliability of the answers.

Other career success predictors

The effect of other personal characteristics is used to measure the relation between the Big

Five personality traits and career success. These variables are divided into two categories.

The first category includes the demography-related variables: age, gender and marital status

and are not likely to be correlated with personality. The second category includes the human

capital related variable: education and the profession related variables: type of profession,

tenure, sector and number of employees. A possible problem with these variables is that

they may be affected by personality and vice versa which can mediate the effects of

personality on career success. That is why hierarchical multiple regressions 7 are used to

measure the difference on the personality traits from the other career success predictors.

Table 1 lists the definitions for all variables. In table 2 the descriptive statistics of the

variables are listed.

6 Cronbach's alpha is a way to determine whether multiple items may be combined to form a scale. This is

assessed on the basis of the correlation of the various items. A value of 0.7 to 0.8 is an acceptable value for Cronbach’s alpha and higher is better (Field, 2009). 7 Described in the next section: methodological approach.

Page 12: Thesis With Questionnaire

8

Table 1: Variable definitions

Variable Definition

Net monthly income Measured in Euros.

Job satisfaction Measured from 0, completely dissatisfied, to 10, completely

satisfied.

Openness to experience Measured from 1, totally open, to 10, completely closed

Conscientiousness Measured from 1 totally disorganized, to 10, completely

conscientious.

Extraversion Measured from 1 totally introvert, to 10, completely extravert.

Agreeableness Measured from 1 totally disagreeable, to 10, completely

agreeable.

Neuroticism Measured from 1 totally calm/relaxed, to 10, completely

nervous/ high-strung.

Gender Dummy =1 if the worker is male.

Age Age of the worker in years.

Age2 Age of the worker in years squared.

Married Dummy = 1 if the worker is married.

Tenure Number of years with the current employer.

Tenure2 Number of years with the current employer squared.

Number of employees Number of employees working in the company.

Education8 (i)=Low education, (ii)=Middle education, (iii)=Higher education.

Profession9 (i)=Higher professions (ii)=Intermediate professions (iii)=Other

professions.

Sector Dummy = 1 if the worker works in the private sector.

8 The level of education is measured by the highest level of education completed by the respondents in six

categories from primary school to university education. The answers are coded as low, middle and high education. (i) Low education means primary education, vmbo (intermediate secondary education, US: junior high school) or mbo (intermediate vocational education, US: junior college). (ii) Middle education consists of havo/vwo (higher secondary education/preparatory university education, US: senior high school) and hbo (higher vocational education, US: college). (iii) High education is WO (university). (i) Low education is the omitted dummy variable. 9 The type of profession the respondents work in is measured by the question: “What is your current

profession?” The nine answer categories were clustered in three new categories. (i) Higher professions include higher academic or independent profession (e.g. architect, physician, scholar, academic instructor, and engineer) and higher supervisory profession (e.g. manager, director, owner of large company, supervisory civil servant). (ii) Intermediate professions include intermediate academic or independent profession (e.g. teacher, artist, nurse, social worker, policy assistant) and intermediate supervisory or commercial profession (e.g. head representative, department manager, and shopkeeper). (iii) Other professions include other mentally intensive work (e.g. administrative assistant, accountant, sales assistant, family carer), skilled and supervisory manual work (e.g. car mechanic, foreman, electrician), semi-skilled manual work (e.g. driver, factory worker), unskilled and trained manual work (e.g. cleaner, packer) and agrarian profession (e.g. farm worker, independent agriculturalist). The category (iii) other professions is the omitted dummy variable in the regression.

Page 13: Thesis With Questionnaire

9

Table 2: Descriptive statistics: Means and standard deviations of the variables.

Variable: Mean Std. Deviation

Net monthly income10 1758.07 860.565

Job satisfaction 7.256 1.318

Openness to experience 6.458 0.922

Conscientiousness 6.595 0.988

Extraversion 5.628 1.232

Agreeableness 6.904 0.935

Neuroticism 4.683 1.235

Gender 0.483 0.500

Age 42.760 11.499

Married 0.582 0.493

Tenure 12.262 10.617

Number of employees 310.680 924.317

Middle education 0.348 0.476

High education 0.113 0.317

Intermediate profession 0.239 0.427

Higher profession 0.088 0.283

Sector 0.391 0.488

Variables Gender, Married, Middle education, High education, Intermediate profession, Higher profession and Sector are

dummy variables.

10

The average income of an employee in the Netherlands is €20.740,- a year (CBS, 2012). This means that the average net monthly income is €1728,-. The difference of €30,- is caused by the categorized income variable used.

Page 14: Thesis With Questionnaire

10

Methodological approach

A cross-sectional hierarchical multiple regression analyses is performed first. In a hierarchical

multiple regression the independent variables are put in the model after each other. First

the variables are put in the model of which the unique contribution wants to be known and

after that the other variables are put into the regression. In the first regression only the Big

Five personality traits are used to measure whether there is any effect on career success. In

the second regression the demography-related variables are added to the regression to see

whether they affect career success and to see if the personality variables will change. Finally,

the human capital related variables and the job related variables are included to see their

influence on career success. It the last regression the variables lower education and lower

profession are omitted because they are the base case for the other dummy variables of

education and profession. The hypotheses are tested simultaneously with three main

models. These models are the same for income and job satisfaction with the only difference

that income is also regressed on job satisfaction like Judge et al., (1995) did. This is done to

measure the intrinsic job satisfaction independent of extrinsic career outcomes. The base

models are:

1.

2.

In the regression is the constant term, P, are the Big Five personality traits, X, are the

control variables and is the error term.

Page 15: Thesis With Questionnaire

11

Empirical results

First the bivariate correlations between the variables are examined. Appendix 2 contains the

intercorrelations between all the main variables. Table 2 shows the intercorrelations

between the Big Five personality traits with income and job satisfaction. In italics the range

of the intercorrelations found in previous studies is mentioned (Boudreau et al., 2001; Judge

et al., 1999; Judge et al., 2002; Seibert and Kraimer., 2001). Excluding the correlation

between openness and income the general pattern of correlations fall in the range

mentioned in previous studies. However, there is no hypothesis for the trait openness to

experience and career success.

Table 3: Correlations between the Big Five personality traits and the dependent variables.

Income Job satisfaction

Openness to experience 0.24 (-0.08; 0.04) 0.06 (-0.01; 0.13)

Conscientiousness 0.05 (-0.03; 0.34) 0.15 (0.12; 0.20)

Extraversion 0.07 (0.08; 0.24) 0.11 ( 0.12: 0.27)

Agreeableness -0.12( -0.12; -0.06) 0.12 (-0.26; 0.31)

Neuroticism -0.22 (-0.32;-0.08) -0.27 (-0.56; -0.21)

Effect of personality on income

In table 4 the regressions of personality on income are found. In the first regression it is seen

that all variables have the expected hypothesised signs. However, only agreeableness and

neuroticism have a significant influence in this regression. When somebody is one point

more agreeable or neurotic he/she is expected to earn €207,- and €116,- less respectively on

average. A striking result is that openness to experience has a positive influence on income.

If an employee is one more point open to experience he/she is expected to earn €227,15

more on average.

After including the demography-related variables (regression (ii) of table 4) into the income

regression it is observed that there are no changes in the signs of the variables. However,

being one point more extraverted increases someone’s wage now with €34 on average. It is

also seen that females have €577,- less income on average as males. The variable age and

Page 16: Thesis With Questionnaire

12

age2 show the expected concave function. This means that when people get older the effect

of age on income decreases.

In the last regression (iii) the human capital related and profession related variables are

added to see the effect of personality on income. It is seen that openness to experience has

no longer a significant influence on income. On the other hand it is seen that one more point

of conscientiousness on average leads to an increase of €24,- in income. The coefficients of

being extraverted, agreeable and neurotic still have the same signs. The coefficient

extraversion increased and those of agreeableness and neuroticism decreased. Married

employees have on average €93,- less monthly income. The variable tenure and tenure2

show the same relation to income as age. The function of tenure is concave which implies

that when people work longer in a company the rise of their income decreases. If an

employee has followed middle/high education he/she will earn €295,-/€708,- more on

average each month. The same effect is seen with the intermediate/high profession where

someone earns €237,-/€686,- more on average. Finally, working in the public sector

decreases someone’s income with €109,- on average.

Table 4: OLS estimates: The effect of personality on income

Variable (i) (ii) (iii)

Openness to experience 227.15*** (20.77) 185.37*** (19.45) -2.71 (18.11)

Conscientiousness 10.13 (18.85) 15.16 (17.49) 24.34* (14.89)

Extraversion 13.28 (15.77) 33.88** (14.56) 38.81*** (12.45)

Agreeableness -206.72*** (20.88) -91.73*** (20.91) -64.43*** (17.87)

Neuroticism -115.60*** (14.73) -56.16*** (13.88) -31.99 ***(11.84)

Gender

-577.37*** (35.96) -543.10*** (31.37)

Age

26.16* (10.60) 35.03*** (9.42)

Age2

-0.14 (0.12) -0.25** (0.11)

Married -44.15 (34.77) -93.34***(29.68)

Tenure

11.22** (4.45)

Tenure2 -0.18 (0.12)

Number of employees

0.03** (0.02)

Page 17: Thesis With Questionnaire

13

Middle education

294.61*** (31.92)

High education

707.60*** (54.13)

Intermediate profession

236.56*** (31.46)

Higher profession

685.90*** (47.99)

Sector

-109.36*** (29.66)

(Constant) 2173.13***(205.021) 681.78*** (293.94) 874.53*** (252.04)

Adjusted R2 0.134 0.273 0.476

Notes: N=1940; Unstandardized coefficients with standard errors in italics.

***, **, * denotes statistically significant differences at 1%, 5% and 10% level respectively.

Effect of personality on job satisfaction

Table 5 contains the regressions of personality on job satisfaction. From the first regression

it is seen that being one point more conscientious and agreeable increases one’s job

satisfaction with 1.01% and 1.03% on average. Being open to experience and being neurotic

decreases one’s job satisfaction with 0.5% and 2.27% on average.

Looking at the effect of personality on job satisfaction after the inclusion of demography-

related variables, it is seen that there are no major changes (regression (ii) of table 5). The

personality variables still have the same signs and the coefficients are just changing slightly.

Age and gender do not affect job satisfaction but being married increases somebody’s job

satisfaction with 2.24 % on average

Considering the last regression with human capital related variables and profession related

variables included it is seen that there are still no major changes in the personality traits. All

variables that had an effect in the previous regressions still have a comparable effect. When

looking at the control variables it is seen that being a female has a positive significant effect

on job satisfaction of 1.38% on average. The profession related variables also have a

significant influence on job satisfaction. Working in the public sector increases one job

satisfaction with 1.28% on average. Finally, having a higher income increases job satisfaction.

Page 18: Thesis With Questionnaire

14

Table 5: OLS estimates: The effect of personality on job satisfaction

Variable (i) (ii) (iii)

Openness to experience -0.055* (.031) -0.037 (.032) -0.098*** (.034)

Conscientiousness 0.101*** (.028) 0.088*** (.028) 0.088*** (.028)

Extraversion 0.008 (.024) 0.009 (.024) 0.006 (.024)

Agreeableness 0.103***(.032) 0.092*** (.034) 0.107*** (.034)

Neuroticism -0.227*** (.022) -0.220*** (.023) -0.205*** (.023)

Gender

0.023 (.059) 0.138** (.064)

Age

-0.003 (.017) 0.002 (.018)

Age2

-0.000 (0) 0.000 (0)

Married 0.224***(.057) 0.218*** (.056)

Tenure

-0.007 (.008)

Tenure2 0.000 (0)

Number of employees

0.000 (0)

Middle education

-0.010 (.062)

High Education

0.040 (.107)

Intermediate Profession

0.63 (.061)

Higher Profession

0.163* (.096)

Sector

0.128** (.057)

Income 0.000*** (0)

(Constant) 7.366***(.309) 7.274*** (.479) 7.042*** (.479)

Adjusted R2 0.075 0.082 0.104

Notes: N=1936; Unstandardized coefficients with standard errors in italics (Beta of income =0.127).

***, **, * denotes statistically significant differences at 1%, 5% and 10% level respectively.

Page 19: Thesis With Questionnaire

15

Discussion

The first hypothesis states that conscientiousness is positively related to income and job

satisfaction. Looking at the results it is seen that in both regressions conscientiousness is

positively related to career success. For the job satisfaction regression it is seen that even

after including the control variables, conscientiousness has a significant positive influence on

job satisfaction. This means that being efficient, hard-working and organized leads to higher

job satisfaction on average. For the income regressions, only in the third regression with all

control variables conscientiousness has a (weakly) significant influence on income. The

hypothesis that conscientiousness has positive influence on income is partially supported

and the positive influence on job satisfaction is supported.

The second hypothesis states that extraversion is positively related to career success.

Looking at the income regressions it is seen that only after including the control variables

extraversion has a significant (positive) influence on income. In the regressions of job

satisfaction it is seen that there is no significant influence of extraversion. The hypothesis

that extraversion has positive influence on income is partially supported and the positive

influence on job satisfaction is not supported.

The third hypothesis states that agreeableness is negatively related to career success.

Looking at the regressions of income it is seen that in all three regressions agreeableness has

a negative significant influence on income. However, looking at the regressions of

agreeableness on job satisfaction it is seen that agreeableness has a positive significant

influence in each regression. The hypothesis that agreeableness has negative influence on

income is supported and the negative influence on job satisfaction is rejected.

The final hypothesis states that neuroticism is negatively related to career success. In the

regressions of income it is seen that in all three regressions neuroticism has a negative

significant effect on income. This is the same for the effect from neuroticism on job

satisfaction. The hypothesis that neuroticism has negative influence on income and job

satisfaction is supported.

Page 20: Thesis With Questionnaire

16

Practical implications

A first practical implication is one for the individual. If an individual wants to be more

successful in his or her career he or she could look at the own personal traits. He or she can

then easily see whether his or her traits are favourable for a successful career. If not, that

person could adjust the traits. For example, an individual high in neuroticism may have had

his or her career inhibited by being too worried, stressed and upset. If that person can adjust

these items he or she can get more successful. Even if one cannot change these traits, at

least knowing which strengths and weaknesses someone’s personality has, can help.

Secondly, there is a practical implication for organisations. If an individual is successful in his

or her career it is more likely that this individual is also more successful in his or her job.

Organisations can then select the individuals with the right personality traits.

A final practical implication is there for policy makers, parents and teachers. If these people

want to give children a better career, they can emphasize the personality traits that can lead

to a successful career.

Limitations

Although the results of this thesis are comparable to results obtained by previous studies,

they should be treated with care. It is possible that there are problems due to response

biases, non-linearity bias and reversed causation.

It is possible that people who are more open to experience are more likely to fill in the

questionnaires. Also people who are more conscientiousness may fill in the questionnaires

with more care than others. A last point is that people fill in the questionnaires themselves

and can over or underestimate their own personality factors. These response biases may

influence the validity but are nearly impossible to exclude from such questionnaires

(Furnham, 1986). The fact that the LISS panel uses a large set of people and pays those

people to fill in the questionnaires helps to limit these biases.

The second possible bias is that the relationship between personality and career success can

be non-linear. It is possible that the labour market only values moderately neurotic people

and punishes neurotic people and non-neurotic people. In table 6 non-linear personality

effects are tested using dummy variables for the top and bottom 10% of the distribution. It is

seen that not all the dummy variables are unequal to zero. However, for the variables that

had a significant influence in the previous regressions, it is found that those dummy

Page 21: Thesis With Questionnaire

17

variables are significant and show a linear pattern. It is concluded that for these variables a

linear pattern is a good indication of the whole relationship.

Table 6: Testing for nonlinear effect of personality on career success

Income Job satisfaction

Openness Bottom 10% -294.55 *** (55.02) 0.059 (.082)

Top 10% 454.28*** (62.51) -0.079 (.396)

Conscientiousness Bottom 10% -8.51 (63.54) -0.203** (.094)

Top 10% -88.51 (51.99) 0.194** (.012)

Extroversion Bottom 10% -25.36 (58.14) -0.105 (.086)

Top 10% 96.01 (60.52) 0.05 (.09)

Agreeableness Bottom 10% 283.20*** (63.68) -0.198** (.095)

Top 10% -283.55*** (61.73) 0.28*** (.092)

Neuroticism Bottom 10% 223.68*** (58.72) 0.476*** (.087)

Top 10% -311.76*** (63.11) -0.518*** (.093)

Adjusted R2 0.088 0.053 Unstandardized coefficients with standard errors in italics. ***, **, * denotes statistically significant differences at 1%, 5% and 10% level respectively. The (omitted) reference categories are the percentiles between 10% and 90%.

Another concern is the one of reversed causality. It is possible that not only the personality

traits affect income and job satisfaction but that these relationships also work the other way

around because they are simultaneously determined. This problem can be even more

significant after including the human capital related and profession related variables because

they can be influenced by personality, income and job satisfaction and vice versa. Because of

the possibility that the personality traits capture both cause and effect of the relationship,

the coefficients can be biased upwardly. That is the reason to look at the coefficients as

upper bounds of the actual personality effects. Fortunately, previous research on the

longitudinal stability of the personality traits found that there is a causal relation from

personality to career success and not the reverse (Judge et al., 1999) which supports the

validity of the results of this paper.

It would be interesting for future research to look at other places and times to look whether

the results hold. Thereby, it will be interesting to include variables like number of

Page 22: Thesis With Questionnaire

18

promotions, unemployment duration, place of the company, working hours and occupation

type. These variables were not available in this dataset but may describe career success in a

different way.

Page 23: Thesis With Questionnaire

19

Extension – Male and Female career success differences

Now that the main results of this thesis are investigated, it is interesting to look at the

differences between men and women. Everybody knows the saying: “Men are from Mars,

Women are from Venus.” The origin of this saying includes the difference in personality

between the sexes (Feingold, A. 1994). In this extension it is examined which differences in

personality between men and women are a cause of the differences in career success

between them. In figure 1 the means of the personality traits of men and women are shown.

An independent sample test shows that only for the trait extraversion the sexes do not

differ11.

Figure 1: Means of the different traits between the sexes.

11

For extraversion p=0.102. For the other traits p=0.000

Page 24: Thesis With Questionnaire

20

Mueller and Plug (2006) found that women receive higher returns than man for the traits

introversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, neuroticism and openness to experience.

When Mueller and Plug used more control variables they found that only agreeableness had

a significant difference between men and women. Nyhus and Pons (2005) found that women

have a significant higher return than man if they are less neurotic. However, the women had

a significant lower return when they are as agreeable or extravert as the men. These papers

have different results and do not look at the differences in job satisfaction. However, this

thesis uses about the same control variables as Mueller and Plug (2006) did. That is why the

hypothesis is that women have higher incomes than men for introversion, agreeableness,

conscientiousness, neuroticism and openness to experience.

As far as known there is no previous literature about the interaction effect between gender

and personality with the effect on job satisfaction. Despite the fact that there is no

hypothesis for this effect it is tested to see if there occurs any effect. To test the income

hypothesis and the effect on job satisfaction, five new interaction variables are created with

each personality trait times the variable female and the results are presented in table 7 and

table 8.

Table 7: Male – Female differences in personality traits and income.

Variable (i) (ii) (iii)

Openness 142.20*** (26.78) 151.49*** (26.33) -52.89** (23.78)

Conscientiousness 35.72 (25.68) 22.37 (25.25) 42.69** (21.50)

Extraversion 3.78 (20.35) 18.15 (19.99) 45.93*** (17.15)

Agreeableness -39.87 (27.97) -61.80** (27.55) -57.93**(23.52)

Neuroticism -94.76*** (20.25) -96.55*** (19.87) -58.27***(16.97)

Female -959.06** (403.88) -1006.82*** (395.59) -1030.83***(336.13)

Female * Openness 69.20* (39.47) 69.01* (38.66) 106.58***(33.00)

Female * Conscientiousness -6.827 (35.38) -12.40 (34.65) -35.60 (29.46)

Female * Extraversion 28.29 (29.55) 31.30 (28.95) -19.47 (24.75)

Female * Agreeableness -68.77 (42.66) -67.77 (41.79) -13.05 (35.70)

Female * Neuroticism 58.14** (28.18) 79.03***(27.71) 51.32** (23.59)

Page 25: Thesis With Questionnaire

21

Adjusted R2 0.246 0.277 0.479

Controls

Demography-related X X

Human capital & Profession related X

Notes: Unstandardized coefficients with standard errors in italics.

***, **, * denotes statistically significant differences at 1%, 5% and 10% level respectively.

In the all three regressions of table 7 it is seen that the interaction terms of female with

openness and neuroticism have a significant influence. The positive sign implies that women

who have 1 point more in those traits earn €69,- and €58,- more on average than men who

have the same level of these traits. The hypothesis is therefore partly rejected for extrinsic

career success. Women earn significantly more for the traits openness to experience and

neuroticism. There is no significant return for the traits extraversion, conscientiousness and

agreeableness.

In the first two columns of table 8 it is seen that the interaction term female with

neuroticism gender has a significant influence. This implies that women who are 1 more

point neurotic have about 1 percent higher job satisfaction on average than men with the

same level of neuroticism. In the third regression there is no significant effect left of this

interaction term. There was no hypothesis to test the effect on job satisfaction and there is

only a significant effect for the interaction term female with neuroticism. It may be

interesting for future research to examine this interaction term with job satisfaction but it is

very likely that no significant patterns are found as in this thesis.

Table 8: Male – Female differences in personality traits and job satisfaction.

Variable (i) (ii) (iii)

Openness -0.029 (0.044) -0.014 (0.044) -0.076* (0.046)

Conscientiousness 0.101** (0.042) 0.91** (0.042) 0.92** (0.042)

Extraversion 0.023 (0.034) 0.024 (0.034) 0.03 (0.033)

Agreeableness 0.12*** (0.046) 0.111** (0.046) 0.115** (0.046)

Neuroticism -0.29*** (0.033) -0.283*** (0.033) -0.253*** (0.033)

Female -0.101 (0.667) -0.069 (0.666) 0.174 (0.657)

Female * Openness -0.91 (0.065) -0.086 (0.065) -0.094 (0.065)

Female * Conscientiousness 0.045 (0.058) 0.043 (0.058) 0.040 (0.057)

Page 26: Thesis With Questionnaire

22

Female * Extraversion -0.007 (0.049) -0.009 (0.049) -0.033 (0.048)

Female * Agreeableness 0.002 (0.071) -0.004 (0.070) 0.025 (0.070)

Female * Neuroticism 0.094* (0.047) 0.093** (0.047) 0.069 (0.046)

Adjusted R2 0.090 0.097 0.130

Controls

Demography-related X X

Human capital & Profession related X

Notes: Unstandardized coefficients with standard errors in italics.

***, **, * denotes statistically significant differences at 1%, 5% and 10% level respectively.

Page 27: Thesis With Questionnaire

23

Conclusions

In this thesis the influence of personality on career success is examined with new insights.

The expectation was that there are personality traits that influence income and job

satisfaction. Therefore, the research question for this thesis was:

What is the role of personality towards differences in career success between employees and

why?

Hierarchical linear OLS regressions were used to answer this research question. To answer

the first part of this question; it can definitely be stated that personality has a significant

effect on career success. Looking at the second part of this question it is shown that a

conscientious, extravert, disagreeable and less neurotic individual has on average more

income. Secondly, it is seen that a closed, less neurotic, conscientious agreeable person has

more satisfaction in their job after. Furthermore, it was shown that there are demography

related, human capital related and profession related variables that also influence career

success. Many of these findings are supported by previous studies. However, the personality

trait agreeableness has a significant positive influence on job satisfaction where it was

expected to find a negative relation. Another point is that openness to experience seems to

have a significant influence on career success where there was no relation expected. Finally

it is found that there are different traits for men and women that are important for their

career success.

This thesis has contributed to the examination of influence of personality on career success.

This study is one of the first that uses a large, nationally representative sample with high-

quality measures of personality traits combined with demography-related, human capital

related and profession related variables to explain career success. The other main

contribution of this thesis to the existing literature is that it has examined a combination of

sociological and economic factors to explain career success and not just one of these factors.

Thereby, this is one of the first papers that has examined differences in personality between

men and women and career success.

Page 28: Thesis With Questionnaire

24

Appendices

Appendix 1: The International Personality Item Pool

Extraversion Agreeableness Openness

Conscientiousness Neuroticism

I am the life of the party. I am interested in people. I have a rich vocabulary. I am always prepared. I am easily disturbed.

I don't mind being the centre of

attention.

I sympathize with others' feelings. I have a vivid imagination. I pay attention to details. I change my mood a lot.

I feel comfortable around people. I have a soft heart. I have excellent ideas. I get chores done right away I get irritated easily.

I start conversations. I take time out for others. I am quick to understand things. I like order. I get stressed out easily.

I talk to a lot of different people at

parties.

I feel others' emotions. I use difficult words. I follow a schedule. I get upset easily.

I don't talk a lot. (reversed) I make people feel at ease. I spend time reflecting on things. I am exacting in my work I have frequent mood swings.

I keep in the background.

(reversed)

I am not really interested in

others. (reversed)

I am full of ideas.

I leave my belongings around.

(reversed)

I often feel blue.

I have little to say. (reversed)

I insult people. (reversed) I am not interested in

abstractions. (reversed)

I make a mess of things.

(reversed)

I worry about things.

I don't like to draw attention to

myself. (reversed)

I am not interested in other

people's problems. (reversed)

I do not have a good imagination.

(reversed)

I often forget to put things back in

their proper place. (reversed)

I am relaxed most of the time

(reversed)

I am quiet around strangers.

(reversed)

I feel little concern for others.

(reversed)

I have difficulty understanding

abstract ideas. (reversed)

I shirk my duties. (reversed)

I seldom feel blue. (reversed)

Page 29: Thesis With Questionnaire

25

Appendix 2: Correlations between the variables

Inc Job O C E A N Age Gen Mar Ten Num Pub Mid Hie Int Hip

Income 1

Job satisfaction 0,18 1

Openness 0,24 0,06 1

Conscientiousness 0,05 0,15 0,24 1

Extraversion 0,07 0,11 0,34 0,07 1

Agreeableness -0,12 0,12 0,28 0,3 0,32 1

Neuroticism -0,22 -0,27 -0,17 -0,18 -0,23 -,030 1

Age of the household member 0,21 0,09 -0,03 0,15 -0,08 0,08 -0,09 1,00

Gender -0,38 0,00 -0,07 0,12 0,03 0,38 0,19 -0,03 1,00

Married 0,10 0,11 -0,06 0,09 -0,01 0,01 -0,07 0,38 -0,04 1,00

Tenure 0,14 0,11 -0,08 0,08 -0,08 0,00 -0,07 0,54 -0,10 0,21 1,00

Number of employees 0,14 0,04 0,14 0,06 0,03 0,03 -0,07 0,02 -0,07 -0,03 0,09 1,00

Public sector -0,01 0,09 0,06 0,03 0,02 0,13 -0,02 0,12 0,16 0,03 0,15 0,11 1,00

Middle education 0,13 0,03 0,18 0,06 0,04 0,09 -0,04 0,00 0,05 -0,02 -0,03 0,05 0,12 1,00

High education 0,33 0,06 0,26 0,01 0,04 0,00 -0,05 -0,04 -0,01 0,00 -0,10 0,10 0,07 -0,26 1,00

Intermediate profession 0,01 0,08 0,04 0,09 0,03 0,11 -0,04 0,08 0,12 0,00 0,06 -0,03 0,22 0,13 -0,06 1,00

Higher profession 0,33 0,11 0,22 0,03 0,08 -0,03 -0,10 0,03 -0,09 0,04 -0,03 0,12 0,04 0,08 0,33 -0,17 1,00

Note: Correlations in italics are non-significant (p>0.05); correlations in bold are significant at p<0.001 (two-tailed tests)

Page 30: Thesis With Questionnaire

26

Bibliography

Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job

performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26.

Black, K. R. (1994). Personality screening in employment. American Business Law

Journal, 32(1), 69-124.

Boissiere, M., Knight, J. B., & Sabot, R. H. (1985). Earnings, schooling, ability, and cognitive

skills. The American Economic Review, 1016-1030.

Bowles, S., Gintis, H., & Osborne, M. (2001). The determinants of earnings: A behavioral

approach. Journal of Economic Literature, 39(4), 1137-1176.

Bray, D. W., & Howard, A. (1980). Career success and life satisfactions of middle-aged

managers. In L. A. Bond & J. C. Rosen (Eds.), Competence and coping during adulthood (pp.

258–287). Hanover, NH: Univ. Press of New England.

Boudreau, J. W., Boswell, W. R., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Effects of personality on executive

career success in the United States and Europe. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 58(1), 53-81.

Caspi, A., & Roberts, B. W. (2001). Personality development across the life course: The

argument for change and continuity. Psychological Inquiry, 12(2), 49-66.

Costa, P.T.,Jr. & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO

Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources

Costa, P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1997). Longitudinal stability of adult personality. In R. Hogan, J.

A. Johnson, & S. R. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology. San Diego: Academic

Press.

Cronbach, Lee J. 1951. “Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Tests.”

Psychometrika, Vol. 16, No. 3 (September), pp. 297–334.

Delfgaauw, J. (2007), The Effect of Job Satisfaction on Job Search: Not Just Whether, But Also

Where, Labour Economics, 14, pp. 299-317.

Digman, J. M. (1990). Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model.Annual

review of psychology, 41(1), 417-440.

Page 31: Thesis With Questionnaire

27

Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in personality: a meta-analysis. Psychological

bulletin, 116(3), 429.

Field, A., (2009), Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. Los Angeles: Sage

Freeman, R. B. (1978), Job Satisfaction as an Economic Variable, American Economic Review,

68(2), pp. 135-141.

Furnham, A (1986) Response bias, social desirability and dissimulation. Personality and

individual differences 7. 385-400

Goldberg, L. R., J. A. Johnson, H. W. Eber, R. Hogan, M. C. Ashton, C. R. Cloninger, and H. G.

Gough. 2006. ‘‘The International Personality Item Pool and the Future of Public-Domain

Personality Measures.’’ Journal of Research in Personality 40: 40-96.

Greenhaus, J.H., Parasuraman, S., & Wormley, W.M. (1990). Effects of race on organizational

experiences, job performance evaluations, and career outcomes. Academy of Management

Journal, 33, 64-86.

Hanushek, E. A., & Woessmann, L. (2008). The role of cognitive skills in economic

development. Journal of Economic literature, 607-668.

Heine, S. J., & Buchtel, E. E. (2009). Personality: The universal and the culturally

specific. Annual Review of psychology, 60, 369-394.

Hendriks, A. A. J., Hofstee, W. K. B., De Raad, B., & Angleitner, A. (1999). The five-factor personality inventory (FFPI). Personality and Individual Differences, 27, 307–325.

Human capital. 2013. In oxforddictionaries.com Retrieved March 20,2014.

Johnson, J. A. (1997). Seven social performance scales for the California Psychological

Inventory. Human Performance, 10, 1–30.

Judge, T. A., Cable, D. M., Boudreau, J. W., & Bretz, R. D. (1995). An empirical investigation of

the predictors of executive career success. Personnel Psychology, 48, 485–519

Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The Big Five personality

traits and career success across the life span. Personnel Psychology, 52, 621–652.

Judge, T. A., Heller, D., & Mount, M. K. (2002). Five-factor model of personality and job

satisfaction: a meta-analysis. Journal of applied psychology, 87(3), 530.

Page 32: Thesis With Questionnaire

28

Levin, J., & Plug, E. (1999). Instrumenting education and the returns to schooling in the Netherlands. Labour Economics, 6, 521–534. London, M., & Stumpf, S.A. (1982). Managing careers. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley

Mount, M. K., & Barrick, M. R. (1995). The big five personality dimensions: Implications for

research and practice in human resources management. In G. R. Ferris & Rollins (Eds.),

Research in personnel and human resources management (Vol. 13, pp.153–200). Greenwich,

CT: JAI Press.

Mueller, G., & Plug, E. (2006). Estimating the effect of personality on male and female

earnings. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 3-22.

Nyhus, E. K., & Pons, E. (2005). The effects of personality on earnings. Journal of Economic

Psychology, 26(3), 363-384.

Organ, D. W., & Lingl, A. (1995). Personality, satisfaction, and organizational

citizenship behavior. Journal of Social Psychology, 135, 339–350.

Osborne, M. (2000). The power of personality: Labor market rewards and the transmission

of earnings. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Massachusetts, Amherst, USA.

Podsakoff, P. M.,&Organ, D.W. (1986). Self-reports in organization research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12, 531–544. Salgado, J. F. (1997). The five factor model of personality and job performance in the

European community. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, 30–43.

Seibert, S. E., & Kraimer, M. L. (2001). The five-factor model of personality and career

success. Journal of vocational behavior, 58(1), 1-21.

Smith, C. A., Organ, D. W., & Near, J. P. (1983). Organizational citizenship behavior: Its nature

and antecedents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68, 653–663.