the relationship between negative and positive cognition

12
Journal de thérapie comportementale et cognitive (2016) 26, 79—90 Disponible en ligne sur ScienceDirect www.sciencedirect.com ORIGINAL ARTICLE The relationship between negative and positive cognition and psychopathological states in adults aged 18 to 20 Relation entre les cognitions négatives et positives et les états psychophysiologiques chez les adultes âgés de 18 à 20 ans Muaweah Alsaleh , Romain Lebreuilly , Joelle Lebreuilly , Manuel Tostain Center for research on risks and vulnerabilities, university of Caen, department of psychology, university of Caen-Normandy, 14000 Caen, France Received 19 September 2015 ; received in revised form 23 February 2016; accepted 25 February 2016 Available online 29 April 2016 KEYWORDS Cognition; Negative thinking; Positive thinking; Psychopathological state; Depression; Anxiety; CBT Summary We have examined the relationships between positive and negative thinking and depression and anxiety. A total of 102 students have participated in this study. The Negative Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire and Positive Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire were used to evaluate negative and positive thoughts, respectively. The Beck Depression Inventory and the Beck Anxiety Inventory were used to assess psychopathological states. Depression was cor- related with negative thinking (r = 0.77; P < 0.01) and gender (r = 0.13; P < 0.05), and inversely correlated with positive thinking (r = 0.45; P < 0.01). Multiple linear regressions showed that both negative and positive thinking affected depression (F = 148.04 and F = 13.86, respectively; P < 0.01). Anxiety was correlated with negative thinking (r = 0.62; P < 0.01), and inversely cor- related with positive thinking (r = 0.31; P < 0.01) and gender (r = 0.11; P < 0.05). Depressed, non-anxious participants had higher negative thinking scores (P < 0.05) than non-depressed, non-anxious subjects who had lower positive thinking scores (P < 0.05) than non-depressed, non- anxious ones. Depression and anxiety were explained by negative thinking more than positive thinking. A decrease in depression and anxiety could be linked to a decrease in negative Corresponding author. E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M. Alsaleh), [email protected] (R. Lebreuilly), [email protected] (J. Lebreuilly), [email protected] (M. Tostain). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jtcc.2016.02.002 1155-1704/© 2016 Association Franc ¸aise de Therapie Comportementale et Cognitive. Published by Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.

Upload: others

Post on 22-Jun-2022

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

Journal de thérapie comportementale et cognitive (2016) 26, 79—90

Disponible en ligne sur

ScienceDirectwww.sciencedirect.com

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

The relationship between negative andpositive cognition and psychopathologicalstates in adults aged 18 to 20Relation entre les cognitions négatives et positives et lesétats psychophysiologiques chez les adultes âgés de 18 à20 ans

Muaweah Alsaleh ∗, Romain Lebreuilly, Joelle Lebreuilly,Manuel Tostain

Center for research on risks and vulnerabilities, university of Caen, department of psychology,university of Caen-Normandy, 14000 Caen, France

Received 19 September 2015 ; received in revised form 23 February 2016; accepted 25 February 2016Available online 29 April 2016

KEYWORDSCognition;Negative thinking;Positive thinking;Psychopathologicalstate;Depression;Anxiety;CBT

Summary We have examined the relationships between positive and negative thinking anddepression and anxiety. A total of 102 students have participated in this study. The NegativeAutomatic Thoughts Questionnaire and Positive Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire were usedto evaluate negative and positive thoughts, respectively. The Beck Depression Inventory andthe Beck Anxiety Inventory were used to assess psychopathological states. Depression was cor-related with negative thinking (r = 0.77; P < 0.01) and gender (r = 0.13; P < 0.05), and inverselycorrelated with positive thinking (r = −0.45; P < 0.01). Multiple linear regressions showed thatboth negative and positive thinking affected depression (F = 148.04 and F = 13.86, respectively;P < 0.01). Anxiety was correlated with negative thinking (r = 0.62; P < 0.01), and inversely cor-related with positive thinking (r = −0.31; P < 0.01) and gender (r = −0.11; P < 0.05). Depressed,

non-anxious participants had higher negative thinking scores (P < 0.05) than non-depressed,non-anxious subjects who had lower positive thinking scores (P < 0.05) than non-depressed, non-anxious ones. Depression and anxiety were explained by negative thinking more than positivethinking. A decrease in depression and anxiety could be linked to a decrease in negative

∗ Corresponding author.E-mail addresses: [email protected] (M. Alsaleh), [email protected] (R. Lebreuilly), [email protected] (J. Lebreuilly),

[email protected] (M. Tostain).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jtcc.2016.02.0021155-1704/© 2016 Association Francaise de Therapie Comportementale et Cognitive. Published by Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.

Page 2: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

80 M. Alsaleh et al.

thoughts rather than an increase in positive thoughts. Both negative and positive thoughts maycontribute to a more precise prediction of depression and anxiety levels.© 2016 Association Francaise de Therapie Comportementale et Cognitive. Published by ElsevierMasson SAS. All rights reserved.

MOTS CLÉSCognition ;Pensées négatives ;Pensée positive ;Étatpsychopathologique ;Dépression ;Anxiété ;Thérapiescomportementales etcognitives

Résumé La dépression se caractérise par la présence de pensées négatives, sur soi, sur lemonde et sur l’avenir, ce que Beck a appelé la triade cognitive. Les pensées automatiquesnégatives sont supposées jouer un rôle central dans le développement et la persistance de ladépression (Hollon et Kendall, 1980) et de l’anxiété (Hollon et Kendall, 1980 ; Sanne et al.,2012). Cependant, le rôle des pensées positives dans la dépression et l’anxiété n’est pas trèsévident. Notre étude a pour but d’examiner le rôle des auto-déclarations négatives et posi-tives sur le niveau d’anxiété et de dépression des étudiants. Notre échantillon se compose de102 étudiants âgés de 18 à 20 ans, déprimés, non déprimés, anxieux et non anxieux. Les résultatsde l’étude nous ont permis de mettre en évidence deux types de pensées liés aux troubles psy-chologiques. Premièrement, les pensées automatiques négatives sont fortement activées chezles étudiants déprimés, alors qu’elles le sont beaucoup moins chez les étudiants non déprimés.Deuxièmement, à l’inverse ce sont les pensées automatiques positives qui sont plus activéeschez les étudiants non déprimés par comparaison avec les étudiants déprimés. Dans notre étude,la pensée négative est positivement corrélée avec la dépression et l’anxiété (p < 0,01). Cepen-dant, la pensée positive est négativement corrélée avec la dépression et l’anxiété (p < 0,01). Lesrésultats de la régression multiple linéaire montrent que les pensées négatives et positives ontcontribué de manière significative (p < 0,001) à expliquer la variance dans la dépression. La pen-sée négative a contribué de manière significative à la variance de l’anxiété (p < 0,001), tandisque la pensée positive (p > 0,05) n’a pas eu un effet significatif. Ces résultats vont dans le sensdes recherches (Ingram et Price, 2010 ; Kendall, 1984 ; Kendall et Hollon, 1981), qui suggèrentque la présence de la pensée positive peut être moins importante que l’absence de la penséenégative dans la détermination des états psychopathologiques. En revanche, l’augmentationdes pensées positives peut être une protection importante contre les troubles psychologiques(TPSY). Ces résultats sont cohérents avec plusieurs recherches qui indiquent que le degré depensées positives est parfois le déterminant le plus important dans les TPSY et l’(in)adaptationpsychologique (Heimberg, et al., 1985). Ainsi, les étudiants déprimés et non anxieux ont plus depensées négatives (p < 0,05) et moins des pensées positives que les étudiants non déprimés etnon anxieux (p < 0,05). Notre résultats montrent que ces pensées (PP et PN) peuvent contribuerà une meilleure compréhension des troubles psychologiques des sujets ainsi qu’au développe-ment de traitements plus efficaces contre ces troubles, afin d’améliorer le bien-être et laqualité de vie des sujets. Cela est cohérent avec les résultats des études antérieures (Kendallet Hollon, 1981 ; Kendall, 1983). Enfin, la pensée négative et la pensée positive sont opposéescar elles sont corrélées négativement. Il est possible pour une personne d’avoir une et/ou deuxtypes de pensées dans une période spécifique. Cependant, les personnes ayant des penséesnégatives élevées présentent de faibles pensées positives et inversement. Selon notre étude,les pensées négatives (effet fort) et les pensées positives (effet modéré à fort) peuvent contri-buer à une meilleure prédiction des niveaux de dépression et d’anxiété. Les pensées positiveset négatives sont des facteurs déterminants des TPSY. Plus précisément, nous pouvons dire queles PP jouent un rôle protecteur, alors que les PN jouent un rôle dévastateur. Cette étude sug-gère que le manque de PP peut, ne pas être seulement, qu’un épiphénomène de la dépressionet de l’anxiété. Elle permet de se rendre compte qu’un manque de PP est tout aussi importantque la présence ou le développement d’une quantité excessive de PN. Ces pensées automa-tiques positives n’ont pas seulement pour but de limiter les émotions et les comportementsnégatifs, elles permettent aussi d’aider les sujets à éprouver davantage de bien-être. C’estl’objectif principal des recherches sur les thérapies comportementales et cognitives (TCC),aujourd’hui très actives en psychologie, notamment en psychologie cognitive. Notre rechercheconfirme l’utilité de la mise en place des outils de la TCC dans le cadre de la prévention destroubles anxieux et dépressifs dans la population étudiante où les pratiques de soins sont peudéveloppées actuellement.© 2016 Association Francaise de Therapie Comportementale et Cognitive. Publie par ElsevierMasson SAS. Tous droits reserves.

Page 3: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

vtd[

ppittstbTdseatidioe

tnttaoa(

waw

M

P

Osprws

M

Po

N(

Cognition and psychopathological states

Introduction

Cognition is the way we think about ourselves. Automaticthoughts are explored in several theories dealing with psy-chopathology [1]. Several authors have placed increasingemphasis on the role of cognition in the origin, mainte-nance, and treatment of different types of psychopathology[2,3]. Haaga, Dyck, and Ernst [4] pointed out that thistheoretical position focused on the automatic negative cog-nitions encountered in psychological disorders, whilst others[1,5] postulated that a specific relationship between pos-itive and negative cognition might be a key determinantin psychopathology. Kendall suggested that examination ofboth negative and positive cognitions could contribute to abetter understanding of the health-pathology relationship[5]. In addition, other authors have reported that a positivecognition deficit could be a facet of emotional disorders,which is as influential as excessive negative cognition [6,7].Cognition plays an important role in theories concerning thedevelopment and treatment of psychopathologies such asanxiety disorders, depression, and loss of self-esteem. Beck[2] was amazed to discover the extent to which his patients’cognitions affected both their feelings and their behaviour,and thus possibly their well-being.

Automatic thoughts are the ‘‘stream of thoughts, ideasand images which constantly accompany an individual as heor she proceeds through daily life’’ [8]. As the most superfi-cial level of cognition, it plays a critical role in CBT becauseit directly influences our emotions and behavior [9]. Thesethoughts are often easily accessible and frequently suscep-tible to modification, and are considered to be the outputof our information processing system—the product of howpeople construct and perceive their worlds [1]. Automaticthoughts are regarded as automatic because they can beinitiated by both conscious and unconscious thoughts, andthe cognitive processes that follow do not require consciouseffort [1]. Cognitive models of psychopathology [2] positthat dysfunctional cognitions directly contribute to negativeemotions. Accordingly, a common technique in traditionalcognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) is cognitive restructu-ring [10]. The goal of this process is to encourage patientsto think in more accurate and adaptive ways, which facili-tate effective problem solving and living a more satisfyinglife [11,12].

Negative thinking has been found to play a pivotal role inpsychopathological states such as depression [3,11,13—18]and anxiety [3,7,17—21]. Indeed, studies have found a con-nection between the presence of negative thoughts andanxiety in clinically anxious adults [22] and children [23].Conversely, positive thinking has been found to be negativelyrelated to psychopathological states including depression[1,13,14,17,24] and anxiety [17].

Cognition themes in depressed and/or anxious individ-uals are characterized by a loss of self-esteem, self-blame,self-criticism, and a desire to escape and commit suicide.According to Beck, depressed patients tend to distort theirexperiences in an idiosyncratic way [2]. Ingram and Wis-nicki [25] proposed that the presence of positive thinking

may be less important in determining psychopathologicalstates than the absence of negative thinking [5,26]. How-ever, when focusing on positive psychology [27,28], a second

T[t

81

iew holds that the presence of positive thinking rather thanhe presence of negative thinking may be more important inetermining both psychopathological states and well-being7].

In studies examining the cognitive model in student sam-les, the main focus has been on negative rather than onositive thinking. Kendall, 1984 [7] observed that psycholog-cal adjustments might not be related as so much to positivehinking, but to the absence of negative thinking. In addi-ion, he reported that improvements in psychopathologicaltates were linked to a reduction in negative thoughts ratherhan an increase in positive thoughts. This phenomenon haseen called the ‘‘Power of Non-negative Thinking’’ [23].houghts play an important role in the cognitive model ofevelopment and treatment of psychopathological statesuch as anxiety disorders, depression, and loss of self-steem. Cognitive therapy is based on the cognitive modelnd focuses on identifying and restructuring biased negativehoughts and interpretations. The cognitive model was orig-nally developed for adults and assumes that anxious andepressed individuals process external and internal stimulin a biased way, resulting in a variety of cognitive errors (e.g.ver-generalization, personalization, selective abstraction,tc.) [29].

The central question is therefore to determine the extento which anxiety and depression can be related to recurrentegative thinking as opposed to a deficit of positive cogni-ion. The purpose of this study was to attempt to identifyhe link between negative thinking versus positive thinkingnd several important psychological variables indicative ofne’s psychopathological state (e.g., depression, anxiety) indults aged between 18 and 20 years old in a Caen UniversityFrance) student sample.

Our main hypothesis was that negative thinking (NT)ould be positively correlated with depression and anxiety,nd our alternate hypothesis was that positive thinking (PT)ould be inversely correlated with depression and anxiety.

ethod

articipants

ne hundred and two male and female undergraduatetudents at the University of Caen aged 18—20 years oldarticipated in this study. Participation was voluntary andesponses were anonymous. The 18—20-year-old narrow ageindow was selected to control the impact of age on the

tudy’s variables.

easures

articipants’ thoughts have been evaluated at the beginningf their first year of undergraduate studies.

egative Automatic Thoughts QuestionnaireATQ-N)

he Negative Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ-N)3] was designed to measure the frequency of negativehoughts. These negative thoughts are believed to play

Page 4: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

8

atmt15tn[

mdtdaNtdtssl

hsdwtcdsa

P

Tpsiaoocndvi

i3tti9ad

BB

Tr

Edtcd3gravdiad

B

Tmcwatsaowh

P

Bpqca

tuaavSdt

Cm

R

D

T

2

n important role in the development, maintenance, andreatment of various types of psychological maladjust-ent, including depression [11] and anxiety [19]. The

hirty items of the ATQ-N are scored on a 5-point scale: = Not at all, 2 = Sometimes, 3 = Moderately often, 4 = Often, = Permanently (Always). The total score is a sum of all thehirty items. The ATQ is reported to have an excellent inter-al consistency and good concurrent validity for depression3].

The ATQ [3] is one of the most widely used instruments foreasuring automatic thoughts [30]. The original ATQ-30 wasesigned to measure the frequency of negative automatichoughts associated with depression [3,31]. Research hasemonstrated the ATQ’s discriminate validity for differenti-ting between depressed and non-depressed groups [3,32].umerous studies have supported the convergent validity ofhe ATQ via its positive correlation with various measures ofepression [33,34] and anxiety symptoms [35] and its nega-ive correlation with life satisfaction [36]; studies have alsohown support for predictive validity because of its ability toignificantly predict the onset of suicidal ideation 3 monthsater [37].

Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ) [3], whichas been developed to assess spontaneous negative self-tatements and intrusive cognitions as experienced byepressed persons. To that end, adults (college students)ere asked to recall depressing situations and then to list

heir spontaneous thoughts in those situations. The ATQonsists of items that discriminated depressed and non-epressed persons. Construct validity of the ATQ has beenupported by findings that depressed and non-depresseddults, whether clinic or non-clinic samples [31].

ositive Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire (ATQ-P)

he ATQ-P [25] was developed as the theoretical com-lement to the ATQ-N. The ATQ-P is a thirty-itemelf-report instrument that measures the frequency of pos-tive thoughts. The thirty items on the ATQ-N are scored on

5-point scale: 1 = Not at all, 2 = Sometimes, 3 = Moderatelyften, 4 = Often, 5 = Permanently. The total score is the sumf all the thirty items. The ATQ-P is reported to be reliable,apable of differentiating between psychopathological andon-psychopathological states and it is unaffected by socialesirability influences. In addition, this scale has good con-ergent and discriminatory power and is sensitive to changesn affective states [1].

Automatic thoughts may include both negative and pos-tive thinking. Ingram and Wisnicki [25] have developed a0-item Positive Automatic Thought Questionnaire (ATQ-P)o measure positive automatic thoughts as a complemento the ATQ. Like the ATQ, the ATQ-P also showed adequatenternal consistency [25] and good test-retest reliability over

weeks [1]. ATQ-P were positively correlated with positiveffect, and negatively correlated with negative affect andepressive and anxiety symptoms [16].

eck Depression Inventory, abbreviated form,

DI-13

he 13-item Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-13) is a self-eporting instrument that assesses depressive symptoms.

1aad

M. Alsaleh et al.

ach item contains four statements reflecting variousegrees of symptom severity. Participants are instructedo circle the number (ranging from zero to three, indi-ating severity) corresponding to the statement that bestescribes them. The total BDI-13 score ranges from 0 to9. The BDI-13 has demonstrated high internal consistency,ood test-retest reliability, and good construct and concur-ent validity with other common depression scales in clinicalnd non-clinical samples [38]. Compared to the originalersion, the short version differentiates depressed from non-epressed subjects and non-depressed-anxious subjects, butt does not distinguish between anxious-depressed from non-nxious-depressed subjects [39]. Participants were declaredepressed if they had a score ≥ 4 on the BDI-13 scale.

eck Anxiety Inventory (BAI)

he BAI [40] is a 21-item and self-reporting inventory foreasuring severity of anxiety. Respondents are instructed to

ircle the number representing one of the four alternatives,hich ranges from no anxiety (1 = Not at all) to severenxiety (4 = severely or ‘‘I could barely stand it’’). Theotal BAI score ranges from 21 to 84. The BAI demon-trated high internal consistency and test-retest reliabilitynd good concurrent and discriminatory validity [40]. More-ver, the BAI was carefully constructed to avoid confusionith depression. Participants were declared anxious if theyad a score ≥ 43 on the BAI scale.

rocedure and data analysis

efore completing the ATQ-N, ATQ-P, BDI and BAI, partici-ants were asked to read instructions about filling in theuestionnaire. The study received approval from the Psy-hology department’s Human Research unit (CERReV, MRSH)t Caen University (France).

The data were summarized by standard descriptive statis-ics and correlation analyses. The Student t-test has beensed for depressed, non-anxious and non-depressed, non-nxious participants. Three sets of multiple linear regressionnalyses have been defined for each of the two dependentariables (depression and anxiety): Forward, Backward,tepwise, while using the same three predictors for gen-er, positive thinking, and negative thinking (automatichoughts) in each analysis.

The SAS and R software (Version 9.3, SAS Institute Inc.,ary, NC; Version R x64 3.2.0) has been used to performultiple linear regression analyses.

esults

escriptive statistics of the sample

he sample consisted of 102 students with a mean age of

8.6 and a standard deviation of 0.7. 88 (86.3%) were femalend 14 (13.7%) were male. The mean and standard devi-tion (SD) scores for negative thinking, positive thinking,epression, and anxiety are shown in Table 1.
Page 5: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

Cognition and psychopathological states

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of the sample.Statistiques descriptives de l’échantillon.

Variable n Mean SD

Age 102 18.64 0.73Gender, n (%)

Male 14 (13.7)Female 88 (86.3)

Negative thinking 102 48.32 15.32Positive thinking 102 86.82 20.35

is

nsf

Nolstlwatest

T

A(rira(

icta

igba

Depression 102 4.92 4.30Anxiety 102 29.16 7.10

Overall correlations

The correlation coefficients were reported in Table 2. Gen-der was significantly correlated with depression and anxiety(r = 0.13 and r = −0.11, respectively; P < 0.05); these corre-lations were a negligible relationship, according the criteriar. Negative thoughts have been inversely correlated withpositive thoughts (r = −0.30; P < 0.01); this correlation wasa moderate negative relationship. Depression was positivelycorrelated with anxiety (r = 0.62; P < 0.01); this correlationwas a strong positive relationship.

Thinking and depression

Depression was positively correlated with negative thinking(r = 0.77; P < 0.01); this correlation was a higher, very strongpositive relationship. Depression was also positively corre-lated with gender (r = 0.13; P < 0.05); this correlation wasa negligible relationship. Finally, depression was inverselycorrelated with positive thinking (r = 0.45; P < 0.01); this cor-relation was a strong negative relationship, in a decreasingorder of magnitude (see Table 2).

The two variables (depression and negative thinking)increase or decrease together because they have a positivecorrelation, but increases in one variable, such as positivethinking, are associated with decreases in the other, such asdepression, because they have a negative correlation.

The effect size between depression and NT is largebecause it is greater than 0.50, the effect size betweendepression and gender is small because it is near 0.10, andthe effect size between depression and PT is large because

aa(a

Table 2 Multiple correlation coefficients for the different combiCoefficients des corrélations pour les différentes variables.

Measure Age Gender NT

Age — 0.00 −0.02

Gender — 0.08

NT —

PT

Depression

Anxiety

NT: negative thinking; PT: positive thinking.* P < 0.05 (2-tailed).

** P < 0.01 (2-tailed)

83

t is near 0.50, according to Cohen’s guidelines for the socialciences.

Multiple linear regression analyses have shown that bothegative thinking and positive thinking had a statisticallyignificant effect on depression (P < 0.001) after controllingor age. Gender did not affect depression (Table 3).

Concerning depression, adjusted R2 between gender, age,T, PT, and depression (Adj. R2) = 0.64, which means that 64%f the total variation in depression can be explained by theinear relationship between gender, age, NT, PT, and depres-ion. This adjusted R2 was smaller than multiple R2 and washe best estimate of the degree of relationship in the under-ying population (Table 3). Also the model was significantith a P-value of 2.2e−16. P-values for NA (0.0001 < 0.05)nd PA (0.0001 < 0.05) were less than 0.05, which has shownhat this result was significant (i.e. NA and PA can reallyxplain depression), while P-values for age (0.77 > 0.05) andex (0.17 > 0.05) were higher than 0.05, which means thathe result was not significant.

hinking and anxiety

nxiety was significantly correlated with negative thinkingr = 0.62; P < 0.01). This correlation was a strong positiveelationship, and inversely correlated with positive think-ng (r = −0.31; P < 0.01), which was a moderate negativeelationship, and gender (r = −0.11; P < 0.05), which was

negligible relationship in decreasing order of magnitudeTable 2).

The two variables (anxiety and negative thinking)ncrease or decrease together because they have a positiveorrelation, but increases in one variable, such as positivehinking, are associated with decreases in the other, such asnxiety, because they have a negative correlation.

The effect size between anxiety and NT is large becauset is greater than 0.50, the effect size between anxiety andender is small because it is near 0.10, and the effect sizeetween anxiety and PT is medium because it is near 0.30,ccording to Cohen’s guidelines for the social sciences.

Multiple linear regression analyses have shown that neg-

tive thinking (P < 0.001) and gender (P < 0.05) significantlyffect anxiety after controlling for age. Positive thinkingP > 0.05) did not have a statistically significant effect onnxiety (Table 4).

nations.

PT Depression Anxiety

−0.07 −0.01 0.020.01 0.13* −0.11*

−0.30** 0.77** 0.62**

— −0.45** −0.31**

— 0.62**

Page 6: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

84 M. Alsaleh et al.

Table 3 Summary of multiple linear regression analyses for variables predicting depression.Résumé de l’analyse de régression linéaire multiple pour les variables prédisant la dépression.

Step Variable Estimate Standard error t-value Pr ( > |t|)1 Age —0.104 0.3540 −0.294 0.7692 Gender 1.038 0.746 1.392 0.1673 Negative thinking 0.195 0.017 11.019 P < 0,000014 Positive thinking −0.050 0.013 −3.780 P < 0.00001

Residual standard error: 2.584 on 97 degrees of freedom; multiple R2: 0.6536; adjusted R2: 0.639; F-statistic: 45.75 on 4 and 97 DF;P-value: < 0.0001.

Table 4 Summary of multiple linear regression analyses for variables predicting anxiety.Résumé de l’analyse de régression linéaire multiple pour les variables prédisant l’anxiété.

Step Variable Estimate Standard error t-value Pr ( > |t|)1 Age 0.252 0.754 0.334 0.7392 Gender −3.231 1.591 −2.031 0.04*

3 Negative thinking 0.274 0.037 7.270 P < 0.000014 Positive thinking −0.043 0.028 −1.524 0.131

le R2

NotaPwngta(r

Nd

T(a

dnnitifnnttp(igps

Residual standard error: 5.511 on 97 degrees of freedom; multipP-value: < 0.00001.

Concerning anxiety, adjusted R2 between gender, age,T, PT, and anxiety (Adj. R2) = 0.40, which means that 40%f the total variation in anxiety can be really explained byhe linear relationship between gender, age, NT, PT, andnxiety (Table 4). Also the model was significant, with a-value of 6.359e−11. The P-values for NA (0.0001 < 0.05)ere less than 0.05, which showed that this result was sig-ificant (i.e. NA can explain anxiety), and the P-values forender (0.04 < 0.05) were less than 0.05, which showed thathis result was significant (i.e. gender can really explainnxiety), while the P-values for age (0.73 > 0.05) and PT0.13 > 0.05) were greater than 0.05, which means that theesult was not significant.

egative thinking and positive thinking inepressed and anxious participants

he average negative thinking (NT) and positive thinkingPT) scores distributed as a function of depression andnxiety are shown in Table 5. Participants were broken

st

d

Table 5 Mean and standard deviations of negative thinking and

states.Moyenne et écarts-types les scores de la pensée négative et de la

Participants Negative thinking

Mean SD

Depressed anxious 91.50 44.55

Depressed non-anxious 52.74 14.05

Anxious non-depressed — —

Non-depressed non-anxious 38.84 5.97

Total

: 0.421; adjusted R2: 0.397; F-statistic: 17.67 on 4 and 97 DF;

own into four categories: depressed anxious, depressedon-anxious, anxious non-depressed, and non-depressedon-anxious, and both variables, NT, and PT, were exam-ned in each of these groups. Participants were consideredo be depressed if they had a BDI-13 score ≥ 4 and anx-ous if the BAI score ≥ 43. According to the mean valuesor each category, depressed anxious participants had moreegative thinking and less positive thinking than depressedon-anxious subjects, while depressed non-anxious par-icipants had more negative thinking and less positivehinking than non-depressed, non-anxious ones. In our sam-le, there were no anxious, non-depressed participantsTable 5). The results showed that negative thinking scoresn depressed, non-anxious participants were significantlyreater than in the non-depressed, non-anxious partici-ants (t(98) = 5.78; P < 0.05), and that positive thinkingcores in the depressed, non-anxious participants were

ignificantly lower than in non-depressed, non-anxious par-icipants (t(98) = −2.96; P < 0.05).

In conclusion, the NT score was always higher inepressed than in non-depressed adults and in depressed

positive thinking scores according to depressive and anxious

pensée positive selon les états dépressifs et anxieux.

Positive thinking n

Mean SD

59.50 13.44 282.87 19.34 62— — 094.71 19.46 38

102

Page 7: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

Cognition and psychopathological states 85

Figure 1 Sex and NT — 1 = women; 0 = men. According to sex,NT means are the numbers next to the boxplot.Le sexe et PN — 1 = femmes ; 0 = hommes. Selon le sexe, lesmoyennes de la PN sont les numéros à côté de la boîte à mous-taches.

Figure 2 Sex and PT — 1 = women; 0 = men. According to sex,PT means are the numbers next to the boxplot.Le sexe et PP — 1 = femmes ; 0 = hommes. Selon le sexe, les

Figure 3 The confidence interval (CI) of a NT mean.L’intervalle de confiance (IC) de la moyenne de la PN.

Tt1ao

cvmmb9o

G

Td

moyennes de la PP sont les numéros à côté de la boîte à mous-taches.

anxious subjects than in depressed, non-anxious and non-depressed, non-anxious adults. In contrast, the PT score wasalways higher in non-depressed than in depressed adultsand in non-depressed, non-anxious participants than indepressed, non-anxious and depressed anxious adults.

Gender, NT, and PT

The result has shown that the males (value 0) did not have a

greater NT and PT than the females (value 1) (Figs. 1 and 2).

In addition, the mean population has a 95% chance ofbeing comprised between 45.2862 and 51.3538 (confidenceintervals = 6.0676, and 1/2 confidence intervals = 3.0338).

st

b

Figure 4 The confidence interval of a PT mean.

L’intervalle de confiance (IC) de la moyenne de la PP.

his means that, if the degree of confidence is 95% (Fig. 3),he mean NT score of the student community aged between7 and 29 years old is between 45.2862 (the minimum level)nd 51.3538 (the maximum level), whereas 5% are locatedutside of these two levels.

The mean population has a 95% chance of beingomprised between 82.7901 and 90.8499 (confidence inter-als = 8.0598, and 1/2 confidence intervals = 4.0299). Thiseans that, if the degree of confidence is 95% (Fig. 4), theean of the PT score of the community of students agedetween 17 and 29 is between 82.7901 (minimum level) and0.8499 (maximum level), whereas 5% are located outsidef these two levels.

ender, depression, and anxiety

he result has shown that the females (value 1) exhibit moreepression than the males (value 0) (Fig. 5). The result hashown that the females (value 1) exhibit less anxiety than

he males (value 0) (Fig. 6).

In addition, the mean population has a 95% chance ofeing comprised between 4.0685 and 5.7715 (confidence

Page 8: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

86 M. Alsaleh et al.

Figure 5 Sex and depression — 1 = women; 0 = men. Accordingto sex, DEP means are the numbers next to the boxplot.Le sexe et la dépression — 1 = femmes ; 0 = hommes. Selon lesexe, les moyennes de la DEP sont les numéros à côté de laboîte à moustaches.

Figure 6 Sex and anxiety — 1 = women; 0 = men. According tosex, ANX means are the numbers next to the boxplot.Le sexe et l’anxiété — 1 = femmes ; 0 = hommes. Selon le sexe,lm

iTtblo

bi

Figure 7 The confidence interval of a depression mean.L’intervalle de confiance (IC) de la moyenne de la dépression.

FL

Ttblo

D

Tcap1Tietdtdepression and anxiety.

es moyennes de l’ANX sont les numéros à côté de la boîte àoustaches.

ntervals = 1.7031, and 1/2 confidence intervals = 0.8515).his means that, if the degree of confidence is 95% (Fig. 7),he mean depression score of the student community agedetween 17 and 29 years old is between 4.0685 (minimumevel) and 5.7715 (maximum level), whereas 5% are locatedutside of these two levels.

In addition, the mean population has a 95% chance ofeing comprised between 27.7540 and 30.5660 (confidencentervals = 2.8120, and 1/2 confidence intervals = 1.4060).

ba

igure 8 The confidence interval of an anxiety mean.’intervalle de confiance (IC) de la moyenne de l’anxiété.

his means that, if the degree of confidence is 95% (Fig. 8),he mean anxiety score of the student community agedetween 17 and 29 years old is between 27.7540 (minimumevel) and 30.5660 (maximum level), whereas 5% are locatedutside of these two levels.

iscussion

he purpose of our study was to identify the link betweenognitive dimensions (both negative and positive cognition)nd several important psychological variables indicative ofsychopathological states in young adults aged between8 and 20 years old in a Caen University student sample.he study has focused on negative and positive think-

ng and their relationships with psychopathological states,specially depression and anxiety. Our hypothesis washat negative thinking would be positively correlated withepression and anxiety, and our alternate hypothesis washat positive thinking would be negatively correlated with

Age was not significantly predictive of any of the varia-les; this may be due, at least in part, to the narrowge range selected (18—20 years). The explanatory role of

Page 9: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

bN

badTg

lmca[ws

fiiiorttviria(c

cuctpccottais

hnsddpaordtD

Cognition and psychopathological states

gender was relatively minor compared to negative thinkingand positive thinking. This means that males were as likelyas females to think both negatively and positively. In compli-ance with previous findings [41], our results have shownthat gender was significantly (P < 0.05) correlated with bothdepression and anxiety, but this correlation was a negligiblerelationship, according the criteria r.

Multiple linear regression analyses have shown that thegender effect was not significant in comparison with bothtypes of thinking (P < 0.001) in depression, but was sig-nificant (P < 0.001) in comparison with positive thinking inanxiety. These results stress the importance of exploringfactors beyond gender and age when explaining individ-ual differences in psychopathological states (depression andanxiety). In addition, these results have underlined theimportance of examining negative cognition to explain psy-chological disorders.

Correlation analyses support our hypotheses that nega-tive thinking was positively correlated with psychopatho-logical state scores for anxiety and depression, while theopposite was observed for positive thinking, which was neg-atively correlated with psychopathological state scores foranxiety and depression. The correlations obtained wereconsistent with the theoretical expectations as well asprevious findings [1,4,11,17,18]. Like some other studies[11,17,18,20,24,42], our study showed that negative think-ing was positively linked with depression and anxiety.

The multiple linear regression analyses supportour hypothesis for depression but not for anxiety.Our results were similar to those of previous studies[3,13,14,17,18,20,24] as negative thinking was positivelycorrelated with depression and anxiety. However, ouralternate hypothesis was only partially verified, as positivethoughts correlated significantly with depression, but not onanxiety. The multiple linear regression results showed thatboth negative and positive thinking significantly contributed(P < 0.001) to explain the variance in depression after con-trolling for age, but gender had no effect. Conversely,negative thinking significantly contributed (P < 0.001) tothe variance in anxiety, while positive thinking did nothave a significant effect. This result is consistent withprevious proposals [5,26,43], suggesting that the presenceof positive thinking may be less important than the absenceof negative thinking in determining psychopathologicalstates.

Concerning depression, the correlation coefficientbetween NT and depression was r = 0.77, so that r2 = 0.5929,which means that 59% of the total variation in depressioncan be explained by the linear relationship between NTand depression. The correlation coefficient between PA anddepression was r = −0.45, so that r2 = 0.2025, which meansthat 20% of the total variation in depression can be explainedby the linear relationship between PT and depression. Theseresults have shown that the NT could better explain depres-sion than PT.

Concerning anxiety, the correlation coefficient betweenNT and anxiety was r = 0.62, so that r2 = 0.3844, which meansthat 38% of the total variation in anxiety can be explained

by the linear relationship between NT and anxiety. The cor-relation coefficient between PT and anxiety was r = −0.31,so that r2 = 0.0961, which means that 9.6% of the total vari-ation in anxiety can be explained by the linear relationship

tpit

87

etween PT and anxiety. These results have shown that theT could better explain anxiety than PT.

The results have shown that there is no differenceetween gender (males and females) concerning positivend negative thinking. But the results showed that there isifference between males and females concerning anxiety.he results also showed that there is difference betweenenders concerning depression.

This study had several implications. Our results under-ined that, in our study, negative thinking was relativelyore important than positive thinking in explaining psy-

hopathological states and disorders such as depression andnxiety. This is consistent with the findings of Haaga et al.,4] who suggest that the presence of negative thinking out-eighs positive thinking in determining psychopathological

tates.This means that positive thinking alone may not offer suf-

cient protection against depression and anxiety. Therefore,t is also consistent with the ‘‘power of non-negative think-ng’’ suggested by a number of theorists [23]. The severityf the depression and/or anxiety may play an importantole in determining the frequency of negative and/or posi-ive thinking. In the current non-clinical sample, we foundhat the presence of positive thinking better explained theariance in depression and anxiety than negative think-ng. Firstly, we can suppose that this also means that theeduction in depression and anxiety could be linked to anncrease in positive thoughts rather than a decrease in neg-tive thoughts. In the second time, it must pass for the NTthis also means that the reduction in depression and anxietyould be linked to a decrease in negative thoughts).

Our study has several limitations. First, as the study wasonducted in a sample of young and presumably healthyniversity students, our findings cannot be generalized tolinical populations. Secondly, we mainly focused on cogni-ive content (i.e. expressed thoughts), leaving out cognitiverocesses and other components of the cognitive model (e.g.oping ability or cognitive coping). Further studies usinglinical and/or non-clinical samples are needed to confirmur results. Another possible limitation was also related tohe single measurement of the psychological state of par-icipants; further studies could be performed to achieve

higher reliability with repeated measurements, takingnto account the learning curve for depression and anxietycales.

Our results showed that depressed, non-anxious adultsad more negative thoughts and less positive thoughts thanon-depressed, non-anxious adults. These results are con-istent with the negativity hypothesis [4], which states thatepressed people have more depressed thoughts than non-epressed people. Our results are also consistent with theositivity hypothesis, which holds that non-depressed, non-nxious adults’ thoughts may be more positive than thosef depressed, non-anxious adults. We found support for theole of negative thoughts in adults’ anxiety and depression;epressed and/or anxious adults reported more nega-ive thoughts than non-anxious and non-depressed adults.epressed and anxious adults also reported less positive

houghts than non-depressed and non-anxious adults. Oneossible factor in CBT’s effectiveness in treating depressions that depression is characterized by enhanced nega-ive information processing [2,44]. Depressed persons have
Page 10: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

8

ftidwnTsmthv

nfwhv(t

AT

CmrcptartosrtacItIa

RaT

WpaoucIaaiFtd

utqrstratro

N

Itissesniia

upihcdainn

D

T

R

215—36.[5] Kendall PC. Cognitive processes and procedures in behav-

ior therapy. In: Wilson GT, Franks CM, Brownell KD, Kendall

8

requent negative automatic thoughts about themselves,heir future, and the world [45]; these automatic thoughtsnduce depressive mood states in the cognitive model ofepression. Hollon and Kendall [3] found that personsith depression showed significantly greater numbers ofegative automatic thoughts than non-depression persons.herefore, for CBT to be effective in treating depressiveymptoms, it is important that automatic thoughts becomeore functional and positive [46]. The effectiveness of

reating depression with cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT)as been substantiated by a growing number of clinical inter-ention studies [47,48].

Finally, negative and positive thinking have oppositeatures since they are negatively correlated. It is possibleor one individual to have one and/or both types of thinkingithin a specific period of time. However, individuals withigh negative thinking do have low positive thinking, andice versa. According to this study, both negative thoughtslarge effect) and positive thoughts (small effect) may con-ribute to better predicting depression and anxiety levels.

utomatic Thought in Cognitive-Behavioralherapy (CBT)

BT is a highly effective treatment for a wide range ofental [12,49]. Cognitive-behavioral researchers have also

ecognized that the assessment of positive and negativeognitions may contribute to a better understanding ofsychopathology and the development of more effectivereatments [43,50]. Cognitive processes have been accorded

pivotal role in the etiology, treatment, maintenance, andemission of depression [51—53]. Cognitions play an impor-ant role in theories about the development and treatmentf anxiety disorders [21]. Often activated by negative andtressful life events, negative automatic thoughts are oftenepetitive and intrusive, which play a key role in precipi-ating psychopathology [54]. Negative automatic thoughtsre specific in content and unique to different psychologi-al disorders [55], such as depression [56] and anxiety [35].n CBT, negative automatic thoughts have a causal role inhe onset of negative mood and depressive symptoms [9].n contrast, positive automatic thoughts may act as a buffergainst significant distress [25].

esearch and Practice Implications of positiveutomatic thoughts for Cognitive-Behavioralherapy (CBT)

ith the increasing number of French people suffering fromsychological distress and various mental health problems,

questionnaire form of the ATQ including the evaluationf both positive and negative automatic thoughts could besed as a useful alternative in large-scale research and clini-al assessments in which multiple constructs are measured.n this sense, the ATQ-P-N, with its satisfactory reliabilitynd convergent validity, is an easy-to-use scale that can bepplied in both research and clinical settings. Furthermore,

n the clinical assessment of mental health problems forrench clients, the ATQ-P-N could be a useful supplemento current mental health measures, especially for assessingepression and anxiety. Cognitive assessment can be a

M. Alsaleh et al.

seful reference not only for clinical diagnosis but also forhe treatment evaluation of CBT. These automatic thoughtuestionnaires for French people would also advance theesearch of CBT in French communities. Specific culturallyensitive CBT intervention may be helpful for this popula-ion, such as cognitive restructuring of negative thoughtselated to depression, anxiety and states of mind [57]. Withn assessment tool, researchers would be able to evaluatehe effectiveness of CBT for French clients and explore theelationship between cognitive change and mental healthutcome measures.

on-clinical Implications

t should be noted that this study was correlational and thathe author did not assess the temporal precedence of pos-tive and negative thinking, depression, and anxiety. Thistudy showed that NT and PT can help to explain depres-ion and NT can help to explain anxiety; gender can help toxplain anxiety but cannot help to explain depression. Thistudy suggests that a lack of PT may not only be an epiphe-omenon of anxiety and depression. Although more researchs needed, it cannot be ruled out that a lack of PT is equallymportant in the development or presence of an excessivemount of NT.

To conclude, in this study an emotion questionnaire wassed to simultaneously measure the amount of negative andositive thinking in a sample of students. Negative and pos-tive thinking all seem to be related to the level of mentalealth. However, negative thinking is one of the strongestorrelations with mental health levels in non-clinical stu-ents, whereas positive thinking is related to lower levels ofnxiety and depression. Future research should clarify whichs most effective in improving mental health: restructuringegative thinking, enhancing positive thinking, or restrictingegative thinking and enhancing positive thinking.

isclosure of interest

he authors declare that they have no competing interest.

eferences

[1] Ingram RE, Kendall PC, Siegle G, Guarino J. Psychometricproperties of the positive automatic thoughts questionnaire.Psychol Assess 1995;7:495—507.

[2] Beck AT. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. NewYork: The New American Library; 1976.

[3] Hollon SD, Kendall PC. Cognitive self-statements in depression:development of an automatic thoughts questionnaire. CognitTher Res 1980;4:383—95.

[4] Haaga DA, Dyck MJ, Ernst D. Empirical status of cogni-tive theory of depression [Review]. Psychol Bull 1991;110(2):

PC, editors. Annual review of behavior therapy: theory andpractice. 9 New York: Guilford Press; 1984. p. 132—79.

[6] Kuiper NA, Derry PA. Depressed and non-depressed contentself-reference in mild depressives. J Person 1982;50:67—80.

Page 11: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

Cognition and psychopathological states

[7] Heimberg RG, Acerra MC, Holstein A. Partner similarity medi-ates interpersonal anxiety. Cognit Ther Res 1985;9:443—53.

[8] Curwen B, Palmer S, Ruddell P. Brief cognitive behaviour ther-apy. London: Sage; 2000.

[9] Beck JS. Cognitive behavioral therapy: basics and beyond. 2nded. New York: Guilford Press; 2011.

[10] Hofmann SG, Asmundson GJ. Acceptance and mindfulness-based therapy: new wave or old hat? Clin Psychol Rev2008;28(1):1—16.

[11] Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF, Emery G. Cognitive therapy ofdepression. New York: Guilford Press; 1979.

[12] Deacon BJ, Fawzy TI, Lickel JJ. Cognitive defusion versus cogni-tive restructuring in the treatment of negative self-referentialthoughts: an investigation of process and outcome. J Cogn Psy-chother 2011;25(3):218—32.

[13] Lightsey OR. ‘‘Thinking positive’’ as a stress buffer: the roleof positive automatic cognitions in depression and happiness.J Consult Psychol 1994;41:325—34.

[14] Lightsey OR. Positive automatic cognitions as moderators ofthe negative life event-dysphoria relationship. Cognit Ther Res1994;18:353—65.

[15] Muris P, Mayer B, den Adel M, Roos T, van Wamelen J. Predictorsof change following cognitive-behavioral treatment of childrenwith anxiety problems: a preliminary investigation on negativeautomatic thoughts and anxiety control. Child Psychiatry HumDev 2009;40(1):139—51.

[16] Burgess E, Haaga OA. The positive automatic thoughts ques-tionnaire (ATQ-P) and the automatic thoughts questionnaire-revised (ATQ-RP): equivalent measures of positive thinking?Cognit Ther Res 1994;18:15—23.

[17] Wong SS. Negative thinking versus positive thinking in a Sin-gaporean student sample: relationships with psychologicalwell-being and psychological maladjustment. Learn Individ Dif-fer 2012;22:76—82.

[18] McEvoy PM, Mahoney AE, Moulds ML. Are worry, rumination,and post-event processing one and the same? Developmentof the repetitive thinking questionnaire. J Anxiety Disord2010;24(5):509—19.

[19] Beck AT, Emery G, Greenberg RL. Anxiety disorders and pho-bias: a cognitive perspective. New York: Basic Books; 1985.

[20] Wong SS. The relations of cognitive triad, dysfunctional atti-tudes, automatic thoughts, and irrational beliefs with testanxiety. Curr Psychol 2008;27:177—91.

[21] Hogendoorn SM, Prins PJ, Vervoort L, Wolters LH, Nauta MH,Hartman CA, et al. Positive thinking in anxiety disordered chil-dren reconsidered. J Anxiety Disord 2012;26(1):71—8.

[22] Beazley MB, Glass CR, Chambless DL, Arnkoff DB. Cogni-tive self-statements in social phobia: a comparison acrossthree types of social situations. Cognit Ther Res 2001;25:781—99.

[23] Kendall PC, Chansky TE. Considering cognition in anxiety-disordered children. J Anxiety Disord 1991;5:167—85.

[24] Lightsey OR, Christopher JC. Stress buffers and dysphoria in anon-western population. J Couns Dev 1997;75:451—9.

[25] Ingram RE, Wisnicki KS. Assessment of positive automatic cog-nition. J Consult Clin Psychol 1988;56(6):898—902.

[26] Ingram RE, Price JM. Vulnerability to psychopathology: riskacross the lifespan. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 2010.

[27] Seligman ME, Csikszentmihalyi M. Positive psychology. An intro-duction. Am Psychol 2000;55(1):5—14.

[28] Simonton DK, Baumeister RF. Positive psychology at the sum-mit. Rev Gen Psychol 2005;9:99—102.

[29] Beck AT. The current state of cognitive therapy: a 40-year ret-rospective. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62(9):953—9.

[30] Calvete E, Connor-Smith JK. Automatic thoughts and psycho-logical symptoms: a cross-cultural comparison of American andSpanish students. Cognit Ther Res 2005;29(2):201—17.

[

89

31] Kazdin AE. Evaluation of the Automatic Thoughts Question-naire: negative cognitive processes and depression amongchildren. Psychological assessment. J Consult Clin Psychol1990;2(1):73—9.

32] Harrell TH, Ryon NB. Cognitive-behavioral assessment ofdepression: clinical validation of the Automatic Thoughts Ques-tionnaire. J Consult Clin Psychol 1983;51(5):721—5.

33] Ghassemzadeh H, Mojtabai R, Karamghadiri N, EbrahimkhaniN. Psychometric properties of a Persian-language version ofthe Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire: ATQ-Persian. Int J SocPsychiatry 2006;52(2):127—37.

34] Zettle RD, Webster BK, Gird SR, Wagener AL, Burdsal CA. Factorstructure of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire in a clinicalsample. Int J Cogn Ther 2013;6(3):280—91.

35] Aydin KB. Automatic thoughts as predictors of Turkish universitystudents’ state anxiety. Soc Behav Pers 2009;37(8):1065—72.

36] Netemeyer RG, Williamson DA, Burton S, Biswas D, Jindal S,Landreth S, et al. Psychometric properties of shortened ver-sions of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire. Educ PsycholMeas 2002;62:111—29.

37] Chioqueta AP, Stiles TC. Dimensions of the Dysfunctional Atti-tude Scale (DAS-A) and the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire(ATQ-30) as cognitive vulnerability factors in the developmentof suicide ideation. Behav Cogn Psychother 2007;35:579—89.

38] Whisman MA, Perez JE, Ramel W. Factor structure of the BeckDepression Inventory-Second Edition (BDI-II) in a student sam-ple. J Clin Psychol 2000;56(4):545—51.

39] Bruch MA, Mattia JL, Heimberg RG, Holt CS. Cognitive speci-ficity in social anxiety and depression: supporting evidence andqualifications due to affective confounding. Cognit Ther Res1993;17:1—21.

40] Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA. An inventory for mea-suring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. J Consult ClinPsychol 1988;56(6):893—7.

41] Zahn-Waxler C, Crick NR, Shirtcliff EA, Woods KE. The originsand development of psychopathology in females and males. In:Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathol-ogy: risk, disorder, and adaptation. Hoboken, New Jersey:Wiley; 2006. p. 76—138.

42] Szentagotai A, Freeman A. An analysis of the relationshipbetween irrational beliefs and automatic thoughts in predictingdistress. J Cogn Behav Psychother 2007;7:1—9.

43] Kendall PC, Hollon SD. Assessing self-referent speech: methodsin the measurement of self-statement. In: Kendall PC, Hol-lon SD, editors. Assessment strategies for cognitive-behavioralinterventions. New York: Academic Press; 1981. p. 85—118.

44] Teasdale JD. Psychological treatments for depression: how dothey work? Behav Res Ther 1985;23:157—65.

45] Beck AT. Depression: clinical, experimental, and theoreticalaspects. New York: Harper & Row; 1967.

46] Furlong M, Oei PST. Changes to automatic thoughts and dys-functional attitudes in group CBT for depression. Behav CognPsychother 2002;30:351—60.

47] Cuijpers P, van Straten A, Andersson G, van Oppen P.Psychotherapy for depression in adults: a meta-analysisof comparative outcome studies. J Consult Clin Psychol2008;76:909—22.

48] Dobson KS. A meta-analysis of efficacy of cognitive therapy fordepression. J Consult Clin Psychol 1989;57:414—9.

49] Butler AC, Chapman JE, Forman EM, Beck AT. The empirical sta-tus of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses.Clin Psychol Rev 2006;26(1):17—31.

50] Kendall PC. Methodology and cognitive-behavioral assessment.Behav Cogn Psychother 1983;11:285—301.

51] Abramson LY, Metalsky GI, Alloy LB. Hopelessness depres-sion: a theory-based subtype of depression. Psychol Rev1989;96:358—72.

Page 12: The relationship between negative and positive cognition

9

[

[

[

[

[

0

52] Alloy LB. Cognitive processes in depression. New York: Guilford;1988.

53] Beck AT. Cognitive models of depression. J Cogn Psychother1987;1:5—37.

54] Tanaka N, Uji M, Hiramura H, Chen Z, Shikai N, Kita-mura T. Cognitive patterns and depression: study of aJapanese university student population. Psychiatry Clin Neu-rosci 2006;60(3):358—64.

[

M. Alsaleh et al.

55] Purdon C. Thought suppression and psychopathology. Behav ResTher 1999;37(11):1029—54.

56] Kopala-Sibley DC, Santor DA. The mediating role of automaticthoughts in the personality-event-affect relationship. Cogn

Behav Ther 2009;38(3):153—61.

57] Alsaleh M, Lebreuilly R, Lebreuilly J, Tostain M. Cognitive bal-ance: states-of-mind model and mental health among Frenchstudents. Best Pract Ment Health 2015;11(1):42—53.