the prosodic word and accent patterns in swedish - s u/prosodichierarch... · the prosodic word and...

77
The prosodic word and accent patterns in Swedish Tomas Riad, Stockholm University Workshop on the Prosodic Hierarchy, September 5, 2008 University of Tromsø

Upload: lamthien

Post on 11-Mar-2019

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

The prosodic word and accent patterns in Swedish

Tomas Riad, Stockholm UniversityWorkshop on the Prosodic Hierarchy,

September 5, 2008University of Tromsø

Distribution of accent varies in interesting ways

Conditions: –

Room

Locality–

Initiality

Presence of stress–

Presence of morphological information

or prosodic information

What is the relevance of Pω

structure?

Empirical area

Interaction of Lexical accent and Auftakt (anacrusis) exposes several properties.

The pattern is interesting in itself.

Looking at Pω

structure may give us hints regarding the relationship between lexical and postlexical accent.

Prosodic words in Swedish

Culminativity:–

One phonological stress within the minimal Pω

One main-stress within the maximal Pω

Rhythmic grouping:–

One can tell trochaic and iambic rhythm apart(ˈtetra-)(ˌvinet) vs

(ˈbäck-)(raˌvinen)

.] [x [. x’the tetrapak wine’

’the brook ravine’

Prosodic words in Swedish

Syllabification:–

Minimal Pω

is the domain for syllabification

Accent:–

accent 2 assignment would also seem to take place within the Pω

lexically and post-lexically•

But how?

Privative distinction•

lexical tone

+ prominence tone

= accent 2

prominence tone

+ Ø

= accent 1

Accent 1 Accent 2

ˈs y r a k ˈe l a k ǀ ǀ L*H L% H*LoHL%

Lahiri et al. (2005): accent 1 is specified•

Morén (2006): neither accent is specified

Riad (1996 et seq.): accent 2 is specified•

Bruce (1977 et seq.): both are specified, but 2 is marked.

I will look for support that accent 2 is morphologically specified, mostly in suffixes.

Plan•

Interaction of lexical accent 2 (specified) and Auftakt/anacrusis (initiality challenged) in lexical accent assignment.

Support for the lexical specification of tone in suffixes.

Prosodic structure of words.•

Integrate the analysis of accent variation with Pω-structure.

Simplex polysyllabic forms

Accent 2 Accent 1σˈσ Ø ka1ˈmel, gar1ˈdin

ˈσσ 2ˈsommar, 2ˈbyx-or, 2ˈtvätt-a

1ˈtango, 1ˈfänrik, 1ˈfeg-is

ˈσσσ 2ˈlöp-are, 2ˈglad-are 1ˈsyfilis, 1ˈKanada

σˈσσ ve2ˈrand-a, go2ˈrill-a bog1ˈser-a, bo1ˈlero, be-1ˈtal-a

σσˈσσ kara2ˈok-e, pape2ˈgoj-a mili1ˈtär-er,

σσˈσσσ speci2ˈal-are speci1ˈell-are

Patterns

Most syllabic suffixes correlate with accent 2 in canonical contexts (ˈσ-σ); a few don’t (-is)

It’s mostly suffixes that carry lexical accent information (and that are stable across dialects, Bruce 1998): -are2

, -a2

, -e2

, -ning2

, -ig2

, -nad2

, -ar2

, -or2

, - ande2

, …

Patterns

Unsuffixed Auftakt forms get accent 1mi1ˈnister, bo1ˈlero, an1ˈsjovis

Suffixed Auftakt forms variably get accent 1 or 2 bog1ˈser-a,

go2ˈrill-a

There’s a distinction between suffixes vis- à-vis Auftakt.

Central Swedish ”strong acc 2 suffixes”

-areN

, -aN

, -eN

,-orPl

, -orN

, -skaN

, -igA

Internal Prosodic Auftakt

(ana- crusis)

”weak acc 2 suffixes”

-ingN

, -andeVAN

, -arPl

, -aV

, -arV, -ligA

, -arecomp

, -astsup

a ˈglad-are ‘happier’ 2

b speciˈell-are ‘more special’

1 2

c ˈlöp-are ’runner’ 2

d speciˈal-are ‘a special’ 2 1

e bogs-ˈer-a ‘to tow’ 1 2

f bogs-ˈer-ing ‘towing’ 1 2

g bogs-ˈer-are ‘tower’ 2 1

”Strong”

>> Auftakt >> ”Weak”

”Weak”

and ”strong”

asymmetry correlates roughly with ”verbal”

and ”nominal”

(cf.

Smith 2001, Kabak & Plank ms. 2007)

”verbal” ”nominal”2ˈtal-a2

2ˈtal-are2

bog1ˈser-a2

bog2ˈser-are2

han1ˈter-ande2

han2ˈter-are2

han1ˈter-ing2

kara2ˈok-e2

dis1ˈträ-are2

ve2ˈrand-a2

pru1ˈdent-lig2

ge2ˈlé-ig2

”strong acc 2 suffixes”

Internal Prosodic Auftakt

”weak acc 2 suffixes”

Central Swedish -areN

, -skaN

, -eN

, -aN

, -orPl

, -orN

, -igA

-ingN

, -andeVAN

, -arPl

, -aV

, -arV, -ligA

-arecomp

, -astsup

North Swedish -areN

, -skaN

, -eN:Lat

-ingN

, -andeVAN

, -arPl

, -aV

, -arV, -ligA

-arecomp

, -astsup

-aN

, -orPl

, -orN

, -igA

North Swedish

ve1ˈrand-ago1ˈrill-orpro1ˈfessorge1ˈlé-ig

re2ˈal-e kara1ˈok-e

So

Internal prosodic Auftakt, i.e. initiality of stress or not interacts with

lexical specification of tone (=morphology) in determining accent in simplex forms.

Unstressed prefixes be- and för- represent External Auftakt.

Unstressed prefixes be- and för-Central Swedish

External Prosodic Auftakt

Incorporation of be- & för-

”strong acc 2 suffixes”-areN

, -aN

, -eN

, -orPl

,-orN

, -skaN

, -igA

InternalProsodic Auftakt (ana-

crusis)

”weak acc 2 suffixes”

-ingN

, -andeVAN

, -arPl

, -aV

, -arV, -ligA-arecomp

, -astsup

a be-ˈtal-a ‘pay’ 1 2

b för-ˈräd-are ‘traitor’

1 2

c för-ˈakt-lig ’despicable’

1 2

d be-ˈständ-ig ‘lasting’

1 2

Internal

Auftakt and External

Auftakt interact differently with accent-2-inducing suffixes.

External

Auftakt always inhibits accent 2 in simplex forms. …hm…

The presence of two stresses always yields accent 2, however.

Two stresses yield post-lexical accent 2

Central Swedish

Two stress

es yield

post- lexical

acc 2

External Prosodi

c Auftakt.Incorpora

tion of be- & för-

”strong acc 2 suffixes”-areN

, -aN

, - eN

, -orPl

,-orN

, -skaN

, -igA

Inter nal

Pros odic

Aufta kt

”weak acc 2 suffixes”

-ingN

, -andeVAN

, -arPl

, -aV

, -arV

,-ligA

-arecomp

, -astsup

e ˈmellan-ˌmål ’snack’

2

f ˈmellan-ˌmåls- ˌbull-e

‘snack bun’

2 2

g be-ˈtal-kaˌnal ‘pay channel’

2 1

So, compounds will always get accent 2 in this dialect

Properties•

Lexical tone must associate to a stressed syllable (premise)

Initiality•

Post-lexical accent 2 targets the first

stress

with the word tone (always) •

We get lexical accent 2 canonically when stress is at the left edge.

In SSw compounds (Skåne) initiality matters for post-lexical accent, too.

Kristoffersen (2000, 257)

Tonal Foot is a trochee in which the lexical tone must be initial.

Metrical Foot•

The TF occurs at some edge of a Pω

This captures the fact that accent 2 occurs in the initial or the penultimate syllable.(not quite surface true for Sw: pape2ˈgoj-or2

-na)

LocalityLocal

Non-local

2ˈnunn-a2

’nun/s’

1ˈoper-a2

’opera/s’2ˈnunn-or2

1ˈoper-or21ˈbil ’car/s’

1ˈkaktus ’cactus/es’

2ˈbil-ar2

1ˈkaktus-ar2

In SSw compounds (Skåne) locality matters for post-lexical accent, too, in the form of a stress clash requirement.

Prosodic hierarchy

What does the prosodic hierarchy look like at this level?

go2ˈrill-a

ω

2ˈlär-are

ω

ω ω ω

ω

2ˈmellan-ˌmåls-ˌbulle

be-2ˈtal-kaˈnal

ω ω

ω

ω

σ

σ för-1ˈtal-a

ω

ω

bo1ˈlero

ω

Prosodic wordssimplex

compound

?

’to slander’ ’snack bun’

’pay channel’

’teacher’

The Pω

is recursive (Ito & Mester 2006, Kabak & Revithiadou 2007)

Non-isomorphism: Prosodic structure often doesn’t reflect

morphological structure be-jaka ‘acknowledge’ jaka ‘say yea’

be-frynda ‘make friends’ †frynda be-segla ‘seal’ †segel ‘seal’ be-tala ‘pay’ tala ‘speak’ för-leda ‘lead astray’ leda ‘lead’ för-gråa ’become grey’ grå ’grey’ för-spröda ’become brittle’ spröd ’brittle’ för-gubbning ’geezering’ gubbe ’geezer’

(be-(tal-a)ω)ω’

(för-(gubb-ning)ω)ω’ Evidence of some productivity

Non-isomorphism

Morphologically left-branching:[varg-skinn-s]-mössa ‘wolf skin hat’

Morphologically right-branching:Barn-[skinn-mössa] ‘children’s skin/leather hat’

Prosodically identical:2((Hˈvarg)(ˌskinns)(LHˌmössa)ω)ω2((Hˈbarn)(ˌskinn)(LHˌmössa)ω)ω

Non-isomorphism

Also true of phrases included in compounds (more later)

Vi ska gå

i första ((maj)-(tåget)ω)ω[ ]NP

… and in syntax/prosody: embedded/flat(This is the cát)(that chased the rát)(that ate the mált)[ [ [ [ [ [ ]]]]]]

Diagnosing the minimal PωSyllabification appears to be the indicator of

minimal Pω

in ordinary compounding in Gmc. (cf. Hall and Kleinhenz 1999), etc.

Onset challenge

(ˈhärm)ω(ˌapa)ω härm.a.pa *här.ma.pa compound (favoˈrit)ω(antipaˌti)ω fa.vo.rit.an.ti.pa.ti *fa.vo.ri.tan.ti.pa.ti (ˈBred)ω(ˌäng)ω bred.äng *bre.däng (ˈsokrates)ω(ˌanda)ω so.kra.tes.an.da *so.kra.te.san.da (ˈvan)ω(ˌartad)ω van.a.ʈad *va.na.ʈad derivation (ˈfuling)ω *ful.ing fu.ling (ˈhärmande)ω *härm.an.de här.man.de derivation/inflection (ˈgrinar)ω *grin.ar gri.nar inflection

Diagnosing PωBehaviour of (unstressed) ”strong”

and

”weak”

suffixes is identical regarding syllabification

They are all internal to Pωmin

(2ˈgla.d-a.re)ω

(2ˈba.g-a.re)ω’happier’

’baker’,

(spe.ci.1ˈel.l-a.re)ω

vs. (spe.ci.2ˈa.l-a.re)ω’more special’

’a special’

speciˈalarespeciˈellare

ω

Initiality: distance•

Pattern dependent on suffix

rather than on

prosodic form lexical encoding of tones

(for1ˈmell-are)ω

vs. (lun2ˈdell-are)ω’more formal’, ’song by Lundell’

(speci1ˈell-are)ω

vs. (speci2ˈal-are)ω’more special’, ’a special’

(traditio1ˈnell-are)ω

vs (instrumen2ˈtal-are)ω’more traditional’, ’instrumental piece of music’

Measured distance seems unimportant

Diagnosing Pω’

Incorporation in simplex morphological context

(be-1(ˈtala2

)ω’

(för-1(ˈära2

)ω’

σ be- tala

ω

ω’

Prefixes be- and för- are not

part of the minimal Pω

Syllabification in ω

and ω’Onset challenge again:för-.ä.r-a rather than *fö.r-är.-a

’bestow’

or *fö.r-ä.r-athus:(för.-(ä.r-a)ω

)ω’

rather than *(för.-(är.)ω

-a)ω’(be.-(ta.l-a)ω

)ω’

’pay’

or *(fö.r-ä.r-a)ω

Not so for ha.v-an.de ’pregnant’, hå.r-ig ’hairy’

The test is somewhat unstraightforward, but it works in slow motion.

Initiality: barrier

Barrier: Ø

vs. (ω

(bog2ˈser-are2

vs.

(be-1(ˈtal-are2

)ω’

’tower’

’payer’(mo2ˈras-ig2

vs.

(be-1(ˈhör-ig2

)ω’

’”morassy”’

’eligible’(ge2ˈlé-ig2

vs.

(för-1(ˈsikt-ig2

)ω’

’jellyish’

’careful’

Evidence for incorporation as such: 1. Simplex incorporation in phrasal context

(många2

)

a. Rostbiff och lax – det är (för 1(många2

)ω’läckerheter’roast beef and salmon –

that’s too many

delicacies’b. Rostbiff och lax – det är för 2(många2

)ωläckerheter’roast beef and salmon –

that is delicacies for

many

(people)’

Example from Malmgren (1992)

2. Incorporation is morpheme dependentbe-, för-, but not des- or kon-de.sin.fi.ce.ra ;

des-infi2cer-are2

de.sin.te.gre.ra ;

des-inte2grer-are2

kon-1ter-a2

~kon-2ter-are2

för ’too’, but not för ’for, to’

2. Incorporation is morpheme dependent

Some sensitivity to the head word in phrasal incorporation (Sara Myrberg, p.c.):

för 1liten2

, för 1stora2

, för 1vanlig2

’ordinary’but

för 2/1egen2

’odd’, för 2/1dyig2

’muddy’

för 2ˈfårˌaktig ’too sheepish’, för 2ˈhemˌtrevlig ’too cosy’

(*för 1ˈfårˌaktig)

So no incorporation into compounds

3. Norwegian ”stress retraction”……is also sensitive to the difference between ω

and ω’.

Kristoffersen (2000, 165)protes1ˈtere > 2ˈprotesˌterebekka1ˈsin > 2ˈbekkaˌsin ’snipe’selek1ˈsjon > 2ˈselekˌsjonbe1ˈtong > 2ˈbeˌtong ’concrete’be-1ˈtone > 2ˈbe-ˌtone ’stress’

This phenomenon is better thought of as (pseudo-) compound formation.

3. Norwegian ”stress retraction”…More general versions of this phenomenon

occur, e.g. Ärtemark, Dalsland (Noreen 1915, 39):2ˈbe-ˌsked ’word’

be-1ˈdrövlig ’terrible’

2ˈge-ˌmen ’simple’

för-1ˈstå ’understand’2ˈKatˌrina, 2ˈfranˌsoser ’venereal disease’, etc.

Cf. alsopoˈlice > ˈpoˌliceDeˈtroit > ˈDeˌtroit

So,

This motivates a structure where be- and för- are differently connected to the Pω, than internal Auftakt.

How now is the structure related to accent?

Lahiri et al. (2005), Wetterlin (2007): Accent 1 is specified

…and accent 2 is default.A central argument for them is precisely the

behaviour of be- and för-words: If 2tal-a has accent 2, why doesn’t be-1tal-a have accent 2?

Their answer: lexical specification of be-1

, för-1

.and lexical specification always dominates.

Structure-based analysis•

I have suggested a difference in prosodic structure.

Now we must make that structure responsible for the accent pattern, rather than the prefixes themselves.

NB. There are no equivalent prefixes that induce/admit accent 2, so we might as well get the accent behaviour from structure rather than specification.

Structure-based analysis•

Prima facie problem (for this perspective): (be-1(tal-a2

)), (för-1(gör-are2

)), etc. seem to fulfill the central criteria: stress is initial in ω, and locality is met.

Yet, neither strong nor weak suffixes appear to be able to realize their lexical accent….hm…

We return to the issue of accent in betala after discussing compounds:

Unstressed be- and för- can incorporate into compounds as stressed.

4. Prosodic incorporation of be- and för- in compounds

2ˈför-ˌan-ˌleda ’occasion, cause’2ˈför-ˌor-ˌsaka ’cause’2ˈför-ˌo-ˌrätta ‘cause injustice to; insult’2ˈför-ˌöd-ˌmjuka ’humiliate’2ˈför-ˌall-ˌmänliga ’make public’

(~0för-ˈall-ˌmänliga)

2ˈbe-ˌled-ˌsaga ’accompany’

(~0be-ˈled-ˌsaga)2ˈbe-ˌar-ˌbeta ’work, tr.’

Meanings are often causativeThe pattern is marginal, and doesn’t take place in phrases

(*ˈför-ˌfår-ˌaktig ’too sheepish’)

Incorporation into compounds only happens with full Pω

((ˈöd)ω

(ˈmjuka)ω

)ωmax

0för

X

((ˈöd)ω

(ˈmjuka)ω

)ωmax

unstressed prefix

(ˈför)ω

((ˈöd)ω

(ˈmjuka)ω

)ωmax create prosodic word

2((ˈför)ω

(ˈöd)ω

(ˈmjuka)ω

)ωmax incorporate

2((ˈbe)ω

(ˈled)ω

(ˈsaga)ω

)ωmax

Exhaustive parsing•

Incorporation of the unstressed prefixes into compounds indicates that compounds are exhaustively

parsed into lower projections of the

Pω.

( ω ω ω )ωmax

((för)(an)(leda))

( ω ω ω ω’ )ωmax

((hem)(kun)(skaps)(be(tyg)))

Cf. phrasal layering forcing ω-status onto function words phrase-finally (Selkirk 1996, Itô & Mester 2007, 200ff)

(He wanted [tu]ω

/*[tə])Φ

but he couldn’t(What did you look [æt]ω

/*[ət])Φ

yesterday

incorporation into compounds also takes place in phrasal prosody (in some dialects), in so-called particle verbs.

Particle verbs in Nw and NSw

2ˈkomme + 1ˈover > 0komm1ˈover or [2ˈkomˌmover]2ˈkomme + 2ˈetter > 0komm2ˈetter or [2ˈkomˌmetter]1ˈfinner + 1ˈut > 0finner1ˈut or [2ˈfinneˌrut]

Kristoffersen (2000, 288)

jag ska 2((ˈslå)ω(ˌin)ω)ω an åt de2((ˈskick)ω(ˌå)ω)ω den 2((ˈfå)ω(ˌut)ω)ω den

Görel Sandström (p.c.)

0gå ˈut

Ф

ω ω>

ω ω

ˈgå ˌut

ω

Compounds

Compounds and similar structures are Pωmax

2((ˈhärm)ω

(ˌapa)ω

)ωmax

’copy cat’2((ˈmellan)ω

(ˌmåls)ω

(ˌbulle)ω

)ωmax

’snack bun’

2((ˈvan)ω

(ˌartad)ω

)ωmax

’delinquent’

Compounds

CSw Compounds get accent 2 post-lexically –

two stresses accent 2

pitch events at first and last stress–

Lexical tonal information is superseded

Morphological composition is superseded

Compounds

ˈtull+ˈpack+ˈhus+ˈkarl-s+ˈlag

ω ω ω ω ω

ω

H* -----------------------------------L*HL%

H* ------

L*HL%

be-ˈtal-kaˈnal

ω ω

ω’

ωmax

σ

ˈbe-ˈled-ˈsaga

ω ω ω

ωmax

H* ---------

L*HL%

The prosodic shape of compounds indicates a simple prosodic structure.

ˈtull+ˈpack+ˈhus+ˈkarl-s+ˈlag

ω ω ω ω ω

ω

’customs packing house men’s (work)team’

H* -----------------------------------L*HL%

Stresses associated with pitch accents are prominent, whereas intervening stresses are not.

Unlike Danish, English …

Danish, mainstress[ˈarmˌbånds]ˌur ’wrist watch’

but(ˌ)stats-[ˈbolig-ˌfonden] ‘state dwelling trust’

(ˌ)kalve-[ˈnyre-ˌsteg] ‘veal kidney steak’

English, main, secondary, tertiary…

stress[[ˈLaw deˌgree] [ˈˈlanguage re0

quirement]]

… and some tonal dialects

Sunnmøre (Abrahamsen 2003: 193ff.), prominence tone.ˈdame-[Hˌhår-ˌpynt] ‘hair adornment for

women’[ˈdame-ˌhår]-Hˌpynt ‘adornment made of

women’s hair’ˈherre-[Hˌpels-ˌhuve] ‘fur hat for men’[ˈherre-ˌpels]-Hˌhuve ‘hat made of man fur’

… and some tonal dialects

Malmö

(Bruce 1974, Riad 2003) accent2[ˈmaskˌros]ˌbrand ‘dandelion fire’1ˈklot-[ˌarm-ˌband]-

‘ball bracelet’

ω ω ω

ω

2ˈmask-ˌros-ˌbrand

ω

ω ω ω

ω

1ˈklot-ˌarm-ˌband

ω

Compounds with lexicalized phrases•

Compounds seem not to prosodically incorporate larger units, although the morphology does.

ω ω

Där går första-maj-tåget

ωmax

’There is the 1st of May procession’

Cpd and lexicalized phrases

Data from Masja Koptjevskaja-Tamm (p.c.)ˈAstrid ’A.’0Astrid ˈLindgren ’A.L.’0Astrid ˈLindgren-ˈböcker ’A.L. books’

0Gustav ˈAdolf-ˈdagen ’the G.A. day’0Drottning ˈSilvia-ˈbrytningen ’Queen S. accent’0Helan och ˈHalvan-parˈtiet ’The Laurel and Hardy party’0Romeo och ˈJulia-förˈälskelsen ’The R. and J. love story’0duktiga ˈAnnika-synˈdromet ’The good Annika syndrome’0Svarte ˈPetter-ˈrollen ’The Black Peter/sheep role’min Me0nuhin och Grapˈpelli-ˈskiva ’my M. and G. record’

lexicalized phrases

0första ˈmaj-ˈtåget’the 1st of May procession’

my I-can’t-be0lieve-it’s-not-ˈbutter ˈsandwich

Putative structures((0första ˈmaj)ω

(ˈtåget)ω

)ωmax

?(0första (ˈmaj)ω

(ˈtåget)ω

)ωmax0första ((ˈmaj)ω

(ˈtåget)ω

)ωmax*

lexicalized phrases

0första ((ˈmaj)ω

(ˈtåget)ω

)ωmax

Test with derivational endings ”strong”

and ”weak” which are demonstrably sensitive to Auftakt.

Hon är en riktig första 2(maj-are2

”strong”’She is a real 1st of May-person’

Vi ska första 2(maj-a2

hela dagen. ”weak”’We’re going to (do) 1st of May (things) all day’

lexicalized phrases

By transitivity, the same structure should apply in compounds.

0första ((ˈmaj)ω

(ˈtåget)ω

)ωmax0Helan och ((ˈHalvan-)ω

(parˈtiet)ω

)ωmax

min Me0nuhin och ((Grapˈpelli-)ω

(ˈskiva)ω

)ωmax

Summing up•

Lexical accent assignment is sensitive to a distance to the left edge for the ”weak”

accent-2-

inducing suffixes (bog1ser-a vs bog2ser-are).

Lexical accent assignment is sensitive to an intervening Pω-barrier between the stress and the left edge, also for the ”strong”

accent-2-

inducing suffixes (för-1för-a, för-1för-are). …hm…

Postlexical accent 2 assignment is insensitive to both distance and barriers.

Proposal for be- and för-

Lexical tonal information is only present/visible when ωmin

= ωmax

Any expansion of Pω

entails invisibility of lexical tones ( post-lexicon)

Therefore, unless accent 2 can be motivated post-lexically, we should expect

accent 1 (= no lexical tone) in forms like be-tala and för-föra.

Proposal for be- and för-•

Lexical information is no longer relevant at the post-lexical level:

2(ˈsommar2

)

2((ˈmid)(ˌsommar2

)(ˌdansen))2(ˈför-a2

) (för-1(ˈföra2

))

Unifying property of compounds and be- and för- formations: They’re projections of ω

”ert”

inert

Conditions on post-lexical accent 2 (or why betala can’t get accent 2)

Postlexical accent 2 is the result of two pitch accents being assigned to one maximal prosodic word: –

One final phrasal accent (L*H, the prominence tone, the focus tone of Bruce 1977 and others),

one initial word tone (H*).

TBU is a stressed syllable (head of ω)two TBUs required for postlexical accent

2, viz. a compound.

ˈtull+ˈpack+ˈhus+ˈkarl-s+ˈlag

ω ω ω ω ω

ω

H* -------------------------------L*HL%

Association is required of both tones.

Cf. Gussenhoven 2005 for English phrases

Align(Ф,T*,Rt): The right edge of every Ф

coincides with a pitch

accent

Align(Ф,T*,Left): The left edge of every Ф

coincides with a pitch

accent

The result for CSw is a ”compound rule”, however.

Asymmetry:

In simplex as in compounds, the word tone of accent 2 is always associated. (Unassociated = unrealized.)

The prominence tone can be bumped by the lexical

word tone

(H*) and remain

unassociated in simplex, •

but the prominence tone can’t be bumped by the postlexical

word tone

(H*) (in CSw)

Accent 1 Accent 2

ˈs y r a k ˈe l a k ǀ ǀ L*H L% H*LoHL%

’angry’ ’mean’

Accent 2, compound

ˈm e l l a n ˌm å l ˈm e l l a n ˌm å l ǀ ǀ ǀ H* L*HL% H*LoH L%

’snack’

*

Lexical word tone may bump the prominence tone,

But postlexical word tone may not

Therefore, post-lexical accent 2 is not an option in betala.

Accent 1 Accent 2

b e ˈt a l a b e ˈt a l a ǀ ǀ L*H L% H*LoHL%

’pay’

*

Recursivity and layering

Ito & Mester 2007: •

Recursivity from the ω

and up

Strict Layering from the ω

and upSomewhat challenged in Swedish

No Recursivity from the Ft and down•

Unstrict layering from the Ft and down

σ för- ˈtala

ω

ω’

σ för 1ˈmånga

ω

ω’

σ för 2ˈmånga

ω

Ф

ω ω ω

ω

2ˈför-ˌan-ˌleda

ω ω

ˈför-ˌmånga

ω*

ω ω

2ˈför-ˌtala

ω*

σ gå 1ˈetter

ω

ω’*

0gå 2ˈetter

Ф

ω ω

2ˈgå-ˌetter

ω

ω ω

a

bc

d

Layers identified for CSw

min ω: syllabification domain, one stress•

max ω: exhaustive parsing into ω

(stressing be-, för-), domain of post-lexical accent 2.

Adjunction of unstressed be- and för- to min ω

indicate an intermediate expansion:

ω’

The connection between tonal grammar and accentual

distribution in compounds•

CSw and other dialects have so-called connective accent in compounds

SSw and other dialects don’t

The prominence

tone

is left- aligned

The prominence

tone

is associated and right-

aligned (and left-aligned, too, in most of them)

Left-alignment only of prominence tone admits accent variation in compounds

Interaction of prosodic factors: clash, Auftakt, locality,…

With morphological factors: linking element, morphological grouping, lexical tonal specification of first member, word class, …

South less South

even less

South

Central Sw.

nonce compounds Malmö 1973

K-stad 1973

H-stad 1973

Sthlm 1973

a. pros. non-local ˈtaxi-ˈgris 1 1 1 2 b. linking-s, morph. non-

ˈskog-s-ˈhals 1 1 1 (48%)

local, pros. local ˈskog-s-ˈhals 2 (52%) 2 c. Auftakt baˈnan-ˈkust 1 1 (35%)

pros. local baˈnan-ˈkust 2 (65%) 2 2 d. pros. non-local ˈlax-choˈklad 1 2 2 2

e. participle ˈin-ˈkläckt 1 1 2 2

f. monosyll., ˈklot-[ˈarm-ˈband] 1 (68%)

morph. non-local, pros. local

ˈklot-[ˈarm-ˈband] 2 (32%) 2 2 2

g. l,r,n pros. local ˈcykel-ˈplank (l,r,n) 1 (43%)

or non-local) ˈcykel-ˈplank (l,r,n) 2 (57%) 2 2 2 h. clash, local ˈblod-ˈprins 2 2 2 2

i. clash, local [ˈmask-ˈros]- ˈbrand 2 2 2 2 j. lex. spec. ˈsommar2-ˈträsk 2 2 2 2

Some references

Abrahamsen, Jardar E. 2003. Ein vestnorsk intonasjonsfonologi. NTNU, Trondheim.Bruce, Gösta. 1974. Tonaccentregler för sammansatta ord i några sydsvenska stadsmål,

in: Platzack, C. (ed.): Svenskans beskrivning 8, 62–75.Bruce, Gösta. 1977. Swedish word accents in sentence perspective. (Travaux de l’institut

de linguistique de Lund 12) CWK Gleerup, Lund.Bruce, Gösta. 1998. Allmän och svensk prosodi. Praktisk Lingvistik 16. Lund University.Elert, Claes-Christian. 1970. Ljud och ord i svenskan. Uppsala, Almqvist & Wiksell.Ito, Junko & Armin Mester. 2007. Categories and Projections in Prosodic Structure. OCP

4, Rhodes.Ito, Junko & Armin Mester. 2006. Prosodic Adjunction in Japanese

Compounds. Proceedings of FAJL 4, Osaka, 2006

Kristoffersen, Gjert. 2000. The Phonology of Norwegian. (The phonology of the world’s languages.) Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Lahiri, Aditi, Allison Wetterlin & Elisabet Jönsson-Steiner. 2005. Lexical specification of tone in North Germanic. Nordic Journal of Linguistics 28, 1, 61–96.

Malmgren, Sven Göran. 1992. Om tonaccent i centralsvenskt standardspråk –

och i svenska ordböcker. Svenskans beskrivning 19, 206–214. Lund University press.

Morén, Bruce. 2006. Central Swedish noun inflection and prosody: synchrony at its best (and no underlying tones!). Rough draft for project group (nov. 2006), CASTL, Tromsø.

Noreen, Erik. 1915. Ärtemarksmålets ljudlära I. Norstedts, Stockholm.

Riad, Tomas. 1998b. Towards a Scandinavian accent typology, in: Kehrein, W. & Wiese, R. (eds.) Phonology and Morphology of the Germanic Languages, 77–109. (Linguistische Arbeiten 386) Niemeyer, Tübingen.

Riad, Tomas. 2006a. Scandinavian accent typology. Sprachtypol. Univ. Forsch. (STUF), Berlin 59 (2006) 1, 36–55.

Riad, Tomas. 2003. Diachrony of the Scandinavian accent typology, in: Fikkert, P. & Jacobs, H. (eds.) Development in Prosodic Systems (Studies in Generative Grammar 58). Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 91–144.

Riad, Tomas. 2008. Prosodi i svenskans ordbildning. Ms. Stockholm university.Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1996. The prosodic structure of function words. In Morgan,

James L. & Katherine Demuth (eds.) Signal to syntax. Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ. 187–213

Smith. Jennifer L. 2001. Lexical Category and Phonological Contrast. In R. Kirchner, J. Pater, and W. Wikely (eds.). PETL 6: Proceedings of the Workshop on the Lexicon in Phonetics and Phonology. Edmonton: University of Alberta, 61–72.

Wetterlin, Allison. 2007. The Lexical specification of Norwegian tonal word accents. Ph. D. diss. Konstanz.