the long-term effects of the context/keyword a}id … · to either a rote or a context/keyword...

7
Lenguas Modernas 28-29 (2001 -2N3), 2O5 -21 I Universidad de Chile THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORD A}ID ROTE METHODS ON THE RECEPTIVE AND PRODUCTIVE LEARNING OF EFL VOCABULARY- Jesús Rrvls Univcrsidad de Oriente MÁxrrr,ro RoonÍcur,z Universidad dc Los Andes This paper reports on an experiment in which the long-term effects of the rote and the combined context/keyword methods on the receptive and productive leaming of 15 English nouns by a group of college EFL learners were compared. Participants' receptive and productive recall were assessed eithcr immediately aftcr the lreatment or a week later. The results showed that both methods were equally effectivc in enhancing recall of the Spanish word givcn its English equivalent (receptive recall), but that rote ied to superior recall of English equivalents of Spanish words (productive recall). Lack of an optimal time to creatc and rehearse the images and students' maturity and expcrience as EFL learners are discussed as possible factors diminishing the effect of the context/ keyword method. In the reccnt past, some memory researchers (e.g., Pressley et al. i 987, Sadoski and Paivio 2001) have advanced theoretical rationales to argue about the enormous potcntial of combining contcxt and thc keyword method for thc learning ol foreign language (FL) vocabulary. Empirical rcsearch (Brown and Perry 1991, Rodíguez and Sadoski 2000), albcit sparse, has consistcntly provided strong cmpirical cvidence supporting thc predictcd power of thc combined method. Brown and Perry (1991) compared the effccts of context, keyword, and the combincd contcxt/keyword. Thcy found that the combined contcxt/kcyword method 'The findings reported he¡e are based on the h¡st author's Master's thesis in ESL, which was completed under the guidance of üe second author and submitted to La Universidad de Oriente, Cumaná in October 2001. The authors are grateful to Mark Sadoski for his insightful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. Correspondence conceming this article may be addressed to Miíximo Rodíguez, Departamento de l-enguas Modernas, Universidad de Los Andes, Trujillo, Venezuela. <[email protected]>

Upload: others

Post on 22-Mar-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORD A}ID … · to either a rote or a context/keyword leaming condition and to either an immediatc or delayed recall condition. Thc experiment

Lenguas Modernas 28-29 (2001 -2N3), 2O5 -21 IUniversidad de Chile

THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORDA}ID ROTE METHODS ON THE RECEPTIVE

AND PRODUCTIVE LEARNING OF EFL VOCABULARY-

Jesús RrvlsUnivcrsidad de Oriente

MÁxrrr,ro RoonÍcur,zUniversidad dc Los Andes

This paper reports on an experiment in which the long-term effects of the rote andthe combined context/keyword methods on the receptive and productive leamingof 15 English nouns by a group of college EFL learners were compared.Participants' receptive and productive recall were assessed eithcr immediatelyaftcr the lreatment or a week later. The results showed that both methods wereequally effectivc in enhancing recall of the Spanish word givcn its Englishequivalent (receptive recall), but that rote ied to superior recall of Englishequivalents of Spanish words (productive recall). Lack of an optimal time tocreatc and rehearse the images and students' maturity and expcrience as EFLlearners are discussed as possible factors diminishing the effect of the context/keyword method.

In the reccnt past, some memory researchers (e.g., Pressley et al. i 987, Sadoski andPaivio 2001) have advanced theoretical rationales to argue about the enormouspotcntial of combining contcxt and thc keyword method for thc learning ol foreignlanguage (FL) vocabulary. Empirical rcsearch (Brown and Perry 1991, Rodíguezand Sadoski 2000), albcit sparse, has consistcntly provided strong cmpirical cvidencesupporting thc predictcd power of thc combined method.

Brown and Perry (1991) compared the effccts of context, keyword, and thecombincd contcxt/keyword. Thcy found that the combined contcxt/kcyword method

'The findings reported he¡e are based on the h¡st author's Master's thesis in ESL, which was completedunder the guidance of üe second author and submitted to La Universidad de Oriente, Cumaná in October2001. The authors are grateful to Mark Sadoski for his insightful comments on an earlier version of thismanuscript.

Correspondence conceming this article may be addressed to Miíximo Rodíguez, Departamento de l-enguasModernas, Universidad de Los Andes, Trujillo, Venezuela. <[email protected]>

Page 2: THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORD A}ID … · to either a rote or a context/keyword leaming condition and to either an immediatc or delayed recall condition. Thc experiment

206 LENGUAS MODERNAS 28-29, 2OOI -2C[3

produced better results than either context or the keyword method alone in allmcasures, although thc only statistically significant diffcrcnce was found bctweenthc two keyword conditions on a multiplc-choice test given onc day after thctrcatment.

Morc recently, Rodíguez and Sadoski (2000) extended Brown and Perry'sfindings by including the rote mcthod into their research design. They found thatthe combined context/keyword method was superior to the keyword method inimmediatc recall and to kcyword, contcxt, and rote methods after one week, as

cvidenccd by thc imprcssive effect sizes obtaincd. Particularly intcresting was thcvcry low rate of forgetting exhibited by thc combined context/keyword methodrclative to that of thc other methods.

So far, the existing evidcnce indicatcs that the combined context/keyword methodis morc cffcctivc than othcr FL vocabulary mel.hods. Its supcriority has been observedwith both high school and collcgc studcnts cnrollcd in regular classcs of English as

a loreign language (EFL) and in expcrimcnts in which rctcntion interval wasmanipulatcd as eithcr a within-participants or a bctween-participants factor.

Howevcr, all that thc cvidencc to datc amounts to is that the combined context/keyword method is effective in rcceptivc lcarning, that is, recalling the translationsof thc foreign words. No evidcnce on its effectivcness in the othcr direction (i.e.,productive learning) has been rcported yct although a study by Moore and Surber(1992) yiclded very promising findings. In that study, thc context and the keywordmethods were equally effective and superior to a no-strategy control in bothimmediatc and delayed productivc learning of German vocabulary.

Thc prcsent study sought to extcnd the lindings on the combined context/keywordmethod by examining whcther its effcctivencss holds for both receptive andproductivc learning of forcign languagc vocabulary. It involved college studentswho had bcen studying EFL lor at lcast six ycars. Further, thc present study contrastedthc combincd context/keyword method and the rote method. Thc rote method wasincludcd as control becausc it had comparcd favorably with the keyword mcthod inscveral cxpcrimcnts assessing cithcr rcccpLivc (e.g., Thomas and Wang 1996, Wangct al. 1992, Wang ct al. 1993) or productivc FL vocabulary learning (e.g., Ellis andBcaton 1993).

Msrsoo

D e sign and partic ipant s

In the present study a 2 (treatment: context/keyword, rote) x 2 (retention interval:immediatc, delayed) between-participants design for both receptive and productiverecall was used. One hundred and twenty-four first-year Spanish-speakingundergraduates from four EFL classes at a northern university in Venezuelacompleted the cued-recall tests. Two EFL instructors also participated in the study.Each instructor taught the experimental words in the two learning conditions andadministered the cued-recall tests.

Page 3: THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORD A}ID … · to either a rote or a context/keyword leaming condition and to either an immediatc or delayed recall condition. Thc experiment

J. Rivas y M. Rodríguez / The Long-term Effects of the ContexVKeyword 201

Materials

A modified version of Rodríguez and Sadoski's (2000) list of 15 English nouns was

used in thc present study. Two of their cxperimental words: costord and hnrdlewere replaced by thcir two practice words: empennage and poteen. Moreovcr, fournew words: lock, carÍ, pot, and tap were included as practice words. The words

werc presented on transparency on an overhead projector. Different transparencies

wcre prcpared for each treatment condition. For the rote condition, each transparency

included thc list of English words and their Spanish dcfinitions. For thc context/keyword condition, each transparency included four English words, thcir keywords,

and 12 senl.ences in which the English words werc used ( thrcc sentences for each

word). Furthermore, participants in this condition were given verbal dcscriptions ofthc researcher-devcloped interactive images. For example, for the English wordskillet (sartén), the keyword esqueleto (skclcton) and the image of a skeletonfryingbones in afrying pan (un esqueleto friendo huesos cn un sartén) were provided. Atotal of 18 transparencies (6 for the training phase and 12 for thc iearning phasc)

was uscd (see Procedure below). Two cued-recall tests (one rcceptivc and one

productivc) were administered to all the students. Thc reccptive cued-recall test

consisted ol a list of 15 English words with blank spaces provided to writc thcSpanish dcfinitions. The productive cued-recall test had an idcntical format with thclist that included the 15 Spanish definitions and the blank spaccs for students towrite the corresponding English words.

Procedure

All phases of the cxperiment were conducted by thc EFL instructors under thcsupervision ofone ofthe researchcrs. Studcnts in each class wcrc randomly assigncd

to either a rote or a context/keyword leaming condition and to either an immediatcor delayed recall condition. Thc experiment took place in thcir classrooms duringthc regular class timc. The instructors described thc appropriate procedurcs for thcmethod and used the four practicc words to help studcnts bccome familiar with it.To ensurc comprehension, all thc descriptions and instructions wcre givcn in Spanish.

The practice words were presented one at a time. The total prescntation timc was

held constant across thc lcarning conditions. Participants in thc rotc condition wercpaccd through four randomized lists ofthc practicc words at a ratc of l5 scconds pcrword. Thcy were instructed to repeat the English word and their Spanish definitionseveral times in order to learn the pair. Participants in the context/keyword conditionwere paced through two randomized lists of words aL arate of 30-second intervals.First, they were directed to read the three sentences in which the English wordappeared in capital letters and to infer its meaning. Once the meaning was determined,they were instructed to look at the keyword and to notice the orthographic and

acoustic similarity of it with the English word. Then they were given the interactivcimage and asked to picture that image in their minds. Immediately after the practicesession, all the participants took a recall test of the four practice items.

Page 4: THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORD A}ID … · to either a rote or a context/keyword leaming condition and to either an immediatc or delayed recall condition. Thc experiment

208 LENGUAS MODERNAS 28.29, 2OOI.2W3

Then students were asked to leam the 15 experimental words using the samemethod they had used during the practice session. An incidental learning approachwas used. Presentation of the experimental words was identical to that of the practicewords. Following study and a 2-min filler task, participants in the immediate retentioncondition completed both thc receptive and productive cued-recall tests (5 min each).To control for order effect, the tests were counterbalanced. Participants in the delayedcondition were administered thc cucd-recall tests a week later and therefore werenot administcred the filler task.

Resulrs

Both receptive and productivc cucd recall data were submitted to a 2 (treatment:repetition and conl.ext/keyword) x 2 (retcntion interval: immediate and a l-weekdelay) betwccn-participants ANOVA using the general linear model (GLM)procedure. The cxpcrimcnt-wise alpha levcl was set at .05. The means, sl.andard

deviations, and group ns arc prescntcd in Tablc 1.

Table 1. Means, Standard Dcviations, and Group ns

for Reccptive and Productive Cued Recall

Receptive Cued Recall

A statistically signiñcant main cflcct was found for rcl.cnl.ion intcrval, F(1, 120) =i 6.35, p <.0001. No statistically significant cffccts wcrc found for cithcr trcatment,

E( 1 , 120) = 1.26, p>.26 or thc intcraction bctwcen trcatmcnt and rctcntion interval,F(1, 120) - .05, B >.82. Thc tcst of homogcncity of variancc proved statisticallynon-significant, F(3, 120) = .43, p>.72.

Productive Cued RecallA statistically significant main effect was found for treatment, E(1, 120) - 5.84, p<.02, indicating that students in thc rote condition wcre superior to those in thecontext/keyword condition. No statistically significant effects were found for eitherretention interval, F(1, 120) = 1.56, L>.21 or the treatment by retention intervalinteraction, E(1, 120) = .17, g>.68. The test forthe homogeneity of variance provedstatistically non-significant, E(3, 120) = .23, p>.87.

Receptive Rccall Productive Recall

Immediate Dclayed Immediatc Dclayed

Treatment MSDnMSDnMSDnMSDn

Rote 11.11 3.41 36 8.55 3.81 31 6.29 3.11 36 5.14 4.M 3l

Context/keyword 10.42 3.80 33 7.50 4.10 ,lA4.29 4.06 JJ 3.73 3.81 24

Maximum score = 15

Page 5: THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORD A}ID … · to either a rote or a context/keyword leaming condition and to either an immediatc or delayed recall condition. Thc experiment

J. Rivas y M. Rodríguez / The Long-term Effects of the Context/Keyword 209

Although overall performance of the rote students was statistically superior to

that o[ the context/kcyword students, further analyses for immediate and dclayedcued recall yielded a statistically significant difference bctween the groups forimmediate recall,1(67) =2.14, p <.05, but not for delayed recall, !(53) = 1.31, p >.14. Effect sizes of .52 and.36 standard deviation units were obtained respectivelyfor immediate and delayed recall. Thesc effect sizes are medium (Cohen 1988).

Dlscusslox

The present study compared the long-term effect of the combincd contcxt/keywordmethod with that of rote on receptive and productive learning of English vocabularyunder classroom conditions by EFL collcge students. Results on receptive recallindicatcd that the performancc of the students in the combincd contcxt/keywordcondition was comparable with that of those in thc rote condition (69.47Vo vs.74.47Vofor immediate recall and 50Vo vs. 5JVc for delaycd recall, respectively). As can be

seen, both mcthods proved very effective in helping students to remember Spanishdclinitions quite wcll ovcr time.

These resuits are consistent. with those of previous studies by Brown and Perry(1991) and Rodríguezand Sadoski (2000) in that they showcd that the combinedcontext/keyword enhanced students' recall of definitions. Particularly intercsting is

the comparison with those of Rodríguez and Sadoski (2000). Thesc researchersfound that students using thc combined context/keyword werc able to recall about75Vc of the dcfinitions immediately and about 68Va after 1 week. Additionally, theyfound that in delayed recall, the combined contcxt/keyword method was, on aver-age, 1.5 times bettcr than the rote method.

In the pressnt study, however, the fact that the context/kcyword method was

comparable to the rote method on delayed rccall is at odds with Rodríguez and

Sadoski's findings. Comparison of the forgetting rates in the two experimcnts clarifiesthe discrepant result. In the prescnt study, the forgetting rate of the rote method wascomparabic to that ol the context/kcyword mcthod (17 .47 Vo vs. 19.47Vc, rcspectively),while in Rodríguez and Sadoski, it was almost thrce times greater (22.2Vo vs.8.2Vo).Hcnce, in thc prescnt study thc forgetting ratc of thc rote mcthod dccreascd byalmost onc-lourth, while that of thc context/keyword incrcascd by morc than 100%.

Nonetheless, it is intcresting to point out that the present study bcars importantmethodologicai differences with that of Rodíguez and Sadoski (2000). First and

foremost, in the prcsent study, prcsentation of each experimental word in the context/keyword condition lasted 60 seconds (2 passes at 30-s intervals) as opposed to about120 seconds in Rodríguez and Sadoski (2000). Given the lengthy procedureassociated with this learning condition (i.e., reading of the three sentences, inferenceof the word's meaning, presentation of the keyword and of the oral image), therefollows that students in the present study had vcry little time to create and rehearse

the images. Since creation and rehearsal of the images have been claimed to be

responsible for sustaining performance of keyword learners (Atkinson 1975), thenlack of an optimal time to do it may account at least in part for the steeper forgetting

Page 6: THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORD A}ID … · to either a rote or a context/keyword leaming condition and to either an immediatc or delayed recall condition. Thc experiment

210 LENGUAS MODERNAS 28.29. 2OOI -2M3

rate exhibited by the context/keyword method. However, what an optimal time forthe context/keyword method might be is of course an empirical matter.

Second, the participants in the present study were considerably more mature andexperienced EFL leamers. They were collcge studcnts who had studied EFL for atleast six years as opposed to the ninth graders in Rodíguez and Sadoski who hadstudied EFL for a little more than two years. Previous research (i.e., Cohen andAphek 1981, Kasper and Glass 1988) has shown that expericnced learners have

developed successful methods and strategics, including some mnemonic-based, forlearning foreign vocabulary. Thus, it is likcly that students in the rotc condition mayhave spontaneously used some mnemonic-based strategy for lcarning the Englishvocabulary. Conversely, somc students in the context/keyword condition may have

not faithfully followcd thc procedures associated with thc context/keyword mcthodand may havc resortcd instcad to other strategies, which had proven successful inthe pasL. Howcvcr, bccause no self-report data werc collected in the prcscnt study,this rcmains an opcn qucstion.

Results on productive rccall indicated that students in the rotc condition were

ablc to recall considcrably morc cxpcrimcntal words givcn their Spanish definitionsthan those in the context/kcyword condition (41.93Vo vs.28.6OVc and 3429% vs.24.877c for immediate and delayed recall, respectivcly). Additionally, thcy indicatcdthat students in eithcr trcatmcnt condition found productivc recall morc troublcsomethan receptive recall, a finding consistent with thosc of prcvious rcsearch (e.g., Ellisand Beaton 1993, dc Groot and Kcijzer 2000, Gruneberg and Pascoe 1996, Halletal. 1981, Moore and Surbcr 1992, Presslcy et al. 1980). Sevcral explanations forthe superior performance with rcceptivc testing have been advanccd, includingavailability of nativc languagc (NL) and FL words, inhcrent differenccs bctweencomprehension and production tasks, and the activation patterns produced by the

NL word and thc ncwly lcarncd FL word ([or a discussion of thcsc accounts, scc dc

Groot and Kcijzer 2000).

Bccausc thc prcscnt study was thc first attempt to assess thc cffcctivcncss of thc

context/keyword mcthod on productivc lcarning of foreign vocabulary. comparisonof its cffcct with that of prcvious rcscarch is not possiblc. Howcvcr, thc lindingabout the cffcctivcncss of- thc rotc mcthod was consistcnt with that of Ellis and

Beaton (1993).

In sum, thc rcsults ol lhc prcscnt study indicatcd that thc rotc and thc contcxL/

keyword mcthods wcrc cqually cffcctive in cnhancing rcccptivc rccall, but that rotewas superior to context/keyword in facilitating productivc recall. However, whetherstudents' maturity and experience as EFL learners and the lack of an optimal timc tocreate and rehearse their images worked independently or in combination to diminishthe long-term effect of the context/keyword method on receptive and productivelearning of foreign vocabulary warrants further research.

Page 7: THE LONG-TERM EFFECTS OF THE CONTEXT/KEYWORD A}ID … · to either a rote or a context/keyword leaming condition and to either an immediatc or delayed recall condition. Thc experiment

J. Rivas y M. Rodríguez / The Long-term Effects of the ContexlKeyword 2t1

RrpsneNcss

Arrrrsor-, R. C. 1975. Mnemotechnics in second-language learning. American Psychologist 30:82 1 -828.

Bnow^-, T. S. r"..o F. L. Prnnv Jn. 1991 . A comparison of three leaming strategies for ESL vocabulary

acquisition. TESOL Quarterly 25: 655-670.

Corros, A. D. eso E. Apnr--r. 198 1 . Retcntion of second-language vocabulary over time: investigatingthe role of mnemonic associations. System 8'. 221-235.

Cour^-, J. 1988. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2d cd.). Hillsdale, NJ:

Lawrcnce Erlbaum Associates.

or Gnoor, A. M. B. A¡'.D R. KL,uzr,R. 2000. What is hard to learn is easy to forget: the roles ol wordconcretencss, cognate status, and word frequency in foreign-language vocabulary Iearning andforgetting. l,anguage Learning 50: 1-56.

Ellrs, N. C. ex» A. Br,rros. 1993. Factors affecting leaming of foreign language vocabulary:imagcry keyword mediators and phonological short-term memory. The Quarrerly Joumal ofExperimental P sycholo gy 46: 533-558.

Gnuxunutc, M. M. r¡..» K. Pescor-. 1996. The effectiveness of the keyword method forrcceptiveand productive foreign vocabulary lcaming in the elderly. Contemporary Educational Psychology2l: 102-109.

Hru-1, J. W., K. P. Wu-sox, nxo R. Pirrr¡nsr¡s. 1981. Mnemotechnics: some limitations of themnemonic keyword method for the study of foreign language vocabulary . Journnl of EducationalPsychology 73: 345-357.

Kesprn, L. F. .rso A. L Gr-¡ss. 1988. An extension of the keyword method facilitates the acquisitionof simple Spanish sentenccs. Applied Cognitive Psychology 2: 137-146.

Moorrr., J. C. e¡-o J. R. SuRgtR. 1 992. Effects of context and keyword methods on second languagevocabulary acquisition. Contemporart Educational Psychologlt 17 : 286-292.

Ptr¡ssl.r'.y, M., J. R. Lr,vlx, J. W. Her-r., G. E. Mn-ltn, ¡rx» J. K. BuRRv. 1980. The keyword methodand foreign word acquisition. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Learning andMemory 6: 163-173.

Pm,sst-r.:v, M., J. R. Lr.-vrx, e^'.l; M. A. McD¡su,r-. 1987. Remcmbering versus inferring what a wordmcans: mnemonics and contextual approaches. In M. G. Mckeown and M. E. Curtis (Eds.),

The nature of vocabulary acquisilion.Po. 107 -127 . Hilisdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

RoonÍcut,z, M. rxo M. Sroosrr. 2000. The effects of rotc, context, keyword, and context/kcywordon retention of vocabulary in EFL classes. Language Learning 50: 385-4 1 2.

S¡roosxr, M. exn A. P,uvro. 2001. Imagery and text: a dual coding theory o{ reading and writing.Mahwah. NJ: Erlbaum.

THoves, M. H. exo A. Y. We^-c. 1996. Lcarning by thc keyword mnemonic: looking for long-termbcnefits. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 2:330-342.

Waxr;, A. Y., M. H. Tlrov¡.s, ¡rn J. A. Our,r-r-r.r'rr-.. 1992. Kcyword mnemonic and retention ofsecond-language vocabulary words. Joumal of Educational Psycholog.t 84 520-528.

WaNc, A. Y., M. H. THoves, C. M. Ixzex¿., nNo L. J. Pnlv¡cr-:p¡o. 1993. Long-term retention underconditions of intentional leaming and the keyword mnemonic. Bulletin of Psy-chonomic Society31:545-547.