the intersection of narrative policy analysis and policy change theory - mark k. mcbeth, elizabeth...

Upload: sepehrasap

Post on 02-Jun-2018

222 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    1/22

    The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy

    Change Theory

    Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan, Ruth J. Arnell, and Paul L. Hathaway

    Narrative policy analysis and policy change theory rarely intersect in the literature. This research

    proposes an integration of these approaches through an empirical analysis of the narrative political

    strategies of two interest groups involved in policy debate and change over an eight-year period in theGreater Yellowstone Area. Three research questions are explored: (i) Is it possible to reconcile these

    seemingly disparate approaches? (ii) Do policy narrative strategies explain how interest groups expand

    or contain policy issues despite divergent core policy beliefs? (3) How does this new method of analysis

    add to the literature? One hundred and five documents from the Greater Yellowstone Coalition and the

    Blue Ribbon Coalition were content analyzed for policy narrative strategies: identification of winners

    and losers, diffusion or concentration of costs and benefits, and use of condensation symbols, policy

    surrogates, and science. Five of seven hypotheses were confirmed while controlling for presidential

    administration and technical expertise. The results indicate that interest groups do use distinctive

    narrative strategies in the turbulent policy environment.

    KEY WORDS: Advocacy Coalition Framework, Greater Yellowstone Area, interest groups, narrativepolicy analysis, policy change

    Introduction

    Researchers in the field of public policy theory seek to explain the divergent

    characteristics of policy change, namely equilibrium and radical change. Why does

    the public undergo alterations in how they understand policy problems and why do

    policy issues that remain static for many years suddenly become dynamic? Three

    theories have dominated the literature over the past decade: Kingdons (1995)

    policy streams, Baumgartner and Jones (1993) punctuated equilibrium, andAdvocacy Coalition Framework (ACF). These authors individually seek to build a

    theory of policy change that stands up to the rigor of empirical analyses. In this

    study, we posit a methodological innovation in the area of policy change by intro-

    ducing an integration of narrative policy analysis (NPA) into the traditional policy

    change theory. This integration is accomplished through a systematic study of the

    strategic nature of policy narratives. The results help to further explain policy change

    and the role that various groups play in prompting policy change or maintenance of

    the status quo.

    The Policy Studies Journal, Vol. 35, No. 1, 2007

    87

    0190-292X 2007 The Policy Studies Journal

    Published by Blackwell Publishing. Inc., 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA, and 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford, OX4 2DQ.

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    2/22

    During the last two decades, the work of social constructionists in the field of

    NPA (e.g., Fischer & Forrester, 1993; Roe, 1994; Stone, 2002) has developed concur-

    rently with that of policy change theorists. NPA focuses on the centrality of narra-

    tives in understanding policy issues, problems, and definitions and does so without

    the grand theoretical aspirations of the more traditional policy change works. One ofthe most developed works in the narrative genre is that of Deborah Stone (2002),

    whosePolicy Paradoxis an NPA gold mine of mini-theories about agenda setting,

    issue and problem definition, and policy dynamics. The centerpiece of Stones work

    is the use of literary devices such as characters, plots, colorful language, and meta-

    phors to analyze policy narratives. In particular, the storytellers political tactics are

    revealed in how they construct who wins and who loses in a policy story (or who

    reaps the benefits and pays the costs), how they characterize policy issues and their

    opposition, and how they either entangle policies in larger cultural issues or alter-

    natively try to ground such issues in the certainty of scientifically deduced numbers

    and facts. Ultimately, Stone (p. 229) asserts that the goal of this strategic problem

    definition is to portray a political problem so that ones favored course of action

    appears to be in the broadest public interest.

    With some exceptions (Baumgartner, 1989; Baumgartner & Jones, 1993, pp. 279;

    Hajer, 1993; Radaelli, 1999; Schneider & Ingram, 2005), NPA and the policy change

    literature rarely intersect. The exclusion of narratives from the grand theories of

    policy change is grounded in the belief that narratives are value-based random

    garble. Sabatier (2000, p. 138), for example, argues that constructivists have dem-

    onstrated very little concern with being sufficiently clear to be proven wrong and

    that their lack of clarity leads him to have no interest in popularizing their posi-tion. We argue that narratives are the lifeblood of politics. Narratives are both the

    visible outcome of differences in policy beliefs (McBeth, Shanahan, & Jones, 2005)

    and the equally visible outcome of political strategizing. Both policy beliefs and

    political strategies, as found in policy narratives, are not random occurrences. Policy

    beliefs are arguably stable, and political strategies are predictable.

    NPA and Policy Change Theory

    Sabatier and Jenkins-Smith (1993, p. 16) outline premises for their ACF, for

    which we assert that narrative theory can serve a methodological role. First, theyclaim that policy change must be analyzed over time, a decade or longer; narratives

    are written words that can easily be documented and tracked through a temporal

    perspective. Second, they purport that policy change can be understood through the

    examination of political subsystems (advocacy coalitions) that seek to influence

    governmental decisions. Other research (McBeth et al., 2005) has discovered that the

    narratives generated by political subsystems in the polity at large, not just in the

    legislative arena, also contain stable core policy beliefs and are a legitimate source of

    policy change analysis.

    The work of Baumgartner and Jones (1993) is also essential for a study of

    narratives and policy change. They point out that, at any particular time, an interest

    group is part of a winning policy monopoly or they are part of an out-of-power

    88 Policy Studies Journal, 35:1

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    3/22

    minority coalition. However, with wicked problems (Rittel & Webber, 1973),

    where rationality and science are minimized in importance, winning and losing

    is more of a perception than necessarily a reality. Wicked problems resist resolu-

    tion by appeal to the facts (Schon & Rein, 1994, p. 4) and beliefs are grounded in

    competing cultural norms (Wood & Doan, 2003, p. 641). Jenkins-Smith and Sabatier(1993, p. 49) furthermore assert that when core beliefs are at stake, competing sides

    will defend their own belief systems and attack the belief systems of the opposition.

    Yet, as later hypothesized by Sabatier and Jenkins-Smith (1999, p. 124), through the

    development of technical expertise, coalitions move toward policy learning. Because

    of the intense value-based conflict between competing groups, policy narratives are

    an important element of study for wicked problems and add to the ability of more

    traditional policy change theories to understand the strategic representation of

    values in framing the conflict.

    Interest groups attempt to maintain, demonstrate, and increase their political

    power as they seek to win a favorable policy. Furthermore, whether an interest group

    perceives themselves as winning or losing on a policy issue greatly influences how

    they play politics. According to Schattschneider (1960, p. 16), winning groups try to

    restrict participation (issue containment) in a policy issue by limiting the scope of

    the conflict whereas losing groups try to widen participation (issue expansion) in

    a policy issue. Such a conclusion is reinforced in a wide variety of literature (e.g.,

    Baumgartner, 1989; Cobb & Elder, 1983; Baumgartner & Jones, 1993). While Radaelli

    (1999, p. 674) concludes that narratives are understood within broader political

    dynamics, the unanswered questions are how do the policy narratives of interest

    groups play into the equation of issue containment and issue expansion in wickedpolicy problems and how do these narratives play into the role of policy change (or

    lack of change, thus contributing to policy intractability)? Our methodology,

    drawing on the insights of NPA, Baumgartner, Jones, Sabatier and Jenkins-Smith,

    allows us to answer these questions.

    Primary Beliefs and Political Strategies

    We assert that interest group narratives possess both primary beliefs and politi-

    cal strategies. This differs slightly from Sabatier and Jenkins-Smiths (1999, p. 122)

    view of policy beliefs. They contend that policy beliefs are composed of core beliefsthat remain stable and secondary beliefs that are more susceptible to change. The

    same principle of core beliefs and secondary beliefs can be applied to the study of

    policy narratives. When we read a policy narrative regarding an environmental

    issue, part of the narrative consists of underlying beliefs in such issues as federalism,

    science, and the relationship between humans and nature. These are primary policy

    beliefs held by interest groups, and narratives reveal that they tend to be stable over

    time (McBeth et al., 2005).

    We argue that narratives also possess political strategies and that these elements

    are much more dynamic. In contrast to Sabatier and Jenkins-Smith (1993: 3031),

    who define secondary beliefs as instrumental decisions of policy implementation, we

    assert that in a policy narrative, the secondary political strategies (not necessarily

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 89

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    4/22

    beliefs) include rhetorical devices outlined by Deborah Stone (2002), among others.

    Political strategies are an important and perhaps underdeveloped element of the

    ACF. As Brown and Stewart (1993, p. 101) argue, the study of policy change must

    focus on tactics employed by policy advocates. As found in narratives, these tactics

    or strategies shift depending on whether or not the coalition perceives itself aswinning. Competing policy narratives incorporate strategies such as identification of

    winners and losers, framing who benefits and who sustains costs in the policy

    conflict, the use of condensation symbols, the wrapping of issues in larger policy

    surrogates, and the use of scientific uncertainty. In turn, the choice of narrative

    strategy is driven by the groups perception of whether it is winning or losing on the

    policy issue. The analysis of both primary beliefs as defined by Sabatier and Jenkins-

    Smith (1999, p. 122) and political strategies (as informed by Stone) is an unexplored

    area in which the two fields intersect and strengthen each other. While traditional

    policy change theory can show that groups act strategically, our methodology draws

    on NPA to showhow groups act strategically through narratives.

    Political Narrative Strategies

    Five narrative strategies are defined in the succeeding discussion. These political

    strategies are hypothesized to contain the issue if the group is winning or to expand

    the issue if the group is losing.

    1. Identifying Winners and Losers. As part of issue containment and expansion, inter-

    est groups will strategically include or exclude mention of specific winners andlosers. Interest groups that perceive themselves as winning on a policy issue are

    more likely to identify specific winners in their policy narratives, whereas interest

    groups that perceive that they are losing on a policy issue are more likely to identify

    specific losers. Winning strategies attempt to contain the issue by illustrating that

    the status quo is positive and no change is necessary. In Baumgartner and Joness

    (1993) terminology, these groups attempt to preserve the current image of a policy

    problem simply because this policy frame has helped to achieve the status of a policy

    monopoly. The goal is to maintain a minimum winning coalition (Riker, 1962);

    expanding the coalition would necessarily entail compromises in policy beliefs and

    outcomes, which, in turn, weakens the power of the members of the policymonopoly. On the other hand, losing groups identify losers in the policy conflict in

    the hope of mobilizing opposition in order to change the status quo. Stone (2002,

    p. 228) argues that both sides try to amass the most power and that it is the loser

    who seeks to bring in outside help.

    2. Construction of Benefits and Costs. Baumgartner and Jones (1993, p. 19) contend

    that losing groups seek to redefine issues in ways that will mobilize indifferent

    citizens and groups in the hope that this mobilization will destabilize policy equi-

    librium. This expansion of an issue to heightened general attention is pivotal in

    policy change (Jones & Baumgartner, 1993, p. 20). In terms of narrative theory, when

    a competing interest group is losing, they use their policy narratives to attempt to

    90 Policy Studies Journal, 35:1

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    5/22

    reallocate attention and expand the issue by diffusing costs and concentrating ben-

    efits. This strategy makes it appear that only a few (if any) groups are benefiting from

    the status quo while many groups are paying the costs. This tactic attempts to

    mobilize the public and bring new players into a coalition. Conversely, when a group

    is winning, they are much more likely to contain the issue by diffusing benefits andconcentrating costs on a small group. This narrative strategy seeks to maintain the

    status quo and to restrict a wide-scale mobilization.

    3. The Use of Condensation Symbols. Jones and Baumgartner (1993, p. 26) argue that

    every public policy problem is usually understood, even by the politically sophis-

    ticated, in simplified and symbolic terms. Stone (2002, p. 137) asserts more directly

    that symbolic representation is the essence of problem definition in politics. Thus,

    we can hypothesize that interest groups that are winning or losing on a policy issue

    will use condensation symbols or a language that reduce[s] complicated concepts

    into simple, manageable, or memorable forms (Achter, 2004, p. 315). Interest groups

    will use condensation symbols to define the policy issue and to characterize their

    opponents. We argue that winning groups have fewer incentives to use condensation

    symbols because doing so might invoke unintended consequences such as riling of

    the opposition. Losing groups, however, have tremendous incentives to negatively

    portray both the issue and their opponents through the use of condensation symbols.

    Again, their goal is to both rally their troops and call in additional reinforcements by

    expanding the scope of the conflict.

    4. The Policy Surrogate. In his discussion of the many causes of wicked resource-based policy conflict, Nie (2003) suggests that a key cause of conflict is the policy

    surrogate. Nie (p. 314) argues that relatively straightforward policy problems can

    be turned wicked when they are used by political actors as a surrogate to debate

    larger and more controversial problems. For environmental policy issues in the

    Western United States, this means that issues like bison management and snowmo-

    bile access are wrapped in larger persistent controversies of Western communities:

    concerns about federalism, the role of public lands, and the fear of outsiders, to name

    a few. Our argument is that losing groups strategically entangle policy issues in

    larger, emotionally charged debates in an effort to gain a competitive advantage by

    expanding the scope of the policy issue. In short, these policy surrogates are used toignite the larger controversies already simmering in the political culture and to

    mobilize opposition.

    5. Scientific Certainty and Disagreement. Nie (2003, p. 323) argues that scientific dis-

    agreement is also a major cause of intractable natural resource-based political con-

    flict. Furthermore, Nie (p. 323) notes that environmental policies have increasingly

    become disputes over science and concludes that political actors frame value and

    interest based political conflict as scientific ones and that they escape responsibil-

    ity for making the tough choices required of them. Thus, this driver, contradictory

    to the policy surrogate driver, suggests that policy actors intentionally reduce the

    scope of policy issues, ignoring the larger normative and cultural issues that invari-

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 91

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    6/22

    ably surround resource-based environmental conflict. We argue that groups that are

    winning in a policy issue are likely to define the issue in terms of scientific certainty,

    thus ignoring the larger normative issues involved in the controversy. Such a cer-

    tainty attempts to bring closure to debates surrounding policy issues, maintains the

    status quo and the minimum winning coalition, and simultaneously hopes to demo-bilize the opposition. Conversely, losing groups attack scientific results and present

    a scientific disagreement in an attempt to open up the issue for a continued

    deliberation.

    Research Questions

    This study addresses three research questions. First, we attempt to methodologi-

    cally demonstrate the useful intersection of NPA and policy change theory. Can such

    ontologically opposing theories be legitimately brought together in the study of

    policy change? Second, we operationalize NPA into measurable tools to examine how

    groups expand or contain policy issues, not just that they do. Do policy narrative

    strategies of interest groups explain how these groups expand or contain policy

    issues despite divergent core policy beliefs? Third, how does this new method of

    analysis add to the existing literature on policy change?

    The Case Study

    The systematic analysis of different interest groups narrative political strategies

    is conducted as a case study in the turbulent policy arena of the Greater YellowstoneArea (GYA). The 19 million acres of the GYA, with Yellowstone National Park (YNP)

    comprising 2.2 million acres of the region, are not only a world famous area for

    geysers, wildlife, and scenic wonders but a well documented hotbed of political

    conflict (e.g., Cawley & Freemuth, 1993; Tierney & Frasure, 1998; Wilson, 1997). In

    fact, environmental policymaking in the region is often intractable or wicked (Rittel

    & Webber, 1973). To use the terminology of Jenkins-Smith and Sabatier (1993, p. 49),

    the conflict is intense and highly political since core policy beliefs (e.g., federalism,

    the relationship between humans and nature, science) are disputed and competing

    sides ground their arguments in myth (Tierney & Frasure, 1998).

    Environmental groups and scientists have sought to redefine Yellowstonesimage from that of an isolated national park with definitive boundaries to that of the

    only intact ecosystem left in the continental United States (Clark & Minta, 1994). To

    use the theory of Baumgartner and Jones (1993), environmental groups have sought

    to redefine the image of the area from Yellowstone as zoo to Yellowstone as an

    open ecological system. Such an effort at image redefinition has intensified the

    political conflict in the past decade. Two interest groups have dominated efforts by

    competing advocacy coalitions to define the policy images of GYA. The Blue Ribbon

    Coalition (BRC) represents motorized recreation users (wise use coalition) and is

    based in Pocatello, Idaho.1 The Greater Yellowstone Coalition (GYC), based in

    Bozeman, Montana, represents the environmental coalition.2 These two groups are

    purposive interest groups, for those who join pursue ideological and issue-

    92 Policy Studies Journal, 35:1

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    7/22

    oriented goals without material rewards (Berry, 1989, p. 47). This is important given

    the Sabatier and Jenkins-Smith (1999, p. 134) hypothesis that purposive groups are

    more constrained in their willingness to compromise beliefs and policy positions.

    From 1997 through 2004, the politics of the GYA have been characterized by

    continuous changes in public policy, instability, and policy wickedness. Policymonopolies have collapsed for short periods of time only to find resurgence and an

    ability to regain political power. During the years of the Clinton administration,

    environmental groups pushed large policy initiatives into effect. The policy issues

    that demonstrated newfound environmental power in the GYA included wolf rein-

    troduction in 1995, regulations that ended snowmobiling inside YNP in 2000, and a

    national roadless initiative in national forests in 1999.

    There is one exception to the wave of Clinton GYA environmental policy success,

    where the wise use coalition retained their monopoly: the management of free-

    ranging bison. In the winter of 199697, over 1,100 bison were killed by the Montana

    State Livestock Department with assistance from the National Park Service because

    of concern over the potential role of bison in brucellosis transmission to cattle. Efforts

    by the Clinton administration and environmentalists to end the killing failed as a

    powerful subsystem of ranchers, federal and state elected officials, and the U.S.

    Department of Agriculture Animal, Plant, Health, Inspection Services retained its

    historic hegemonic stance.

    Yet again, with the exception of the bison controversy, the policy changes in the

    1990s were overwhelmingly in the direction of the GYA environmental advocacy

    coalition. The election of George W. Bush in 2000, however, led to a large-scale

    resurgence of the wise use coalition in at least two policy areas. Bushs first term sawthe dramatic reversal of the Clinton era snowmobile ban in YNP. The presidents

    second term saw the overturning of the roadless rule in favor of state control over the

    use of national forests. It is in the context of this turbulent policy arena from 1997

    through 2004 that the BRC and the GYC both generated strategic political narratives.

    Research Methodology

    A content analysis was performed on one hundred five documents produced by

    the GYC (52 documents) and the BRC (53 documents) over eight years (January 1,1997 through December 31, 2004). The documents address one of three policy issues

    in the GYA: (i) bison and brucellosis (14 documents); (ii) snowmobile access in YNP

    (70 documents); and (iii) the roadless initiative (21 documents). Our choice of content

    analysis was straightforward. Content analysis is unobtrusive, allows for a reliability

    analysis, permits a longitudinal analysis, and is efficient and inexpensive. The docu-

    ments analyzed were readily archived and complete, thereby avoiding some of the

    disadvantages of using content analysis (Johnson & Reynolds, 2005, pp. 23234).

    Based on NPA and policy change theory, we propose seven hypotheses predicting

    an association between use of a winning or losing narrative frame (independent

    variable, see Table 1) and seven different narrative political strategies (dependent

    variables, see Table 1).

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 93

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    8/22

    Hypotheses

    For the following seven alternative hypotheses, each has a null hypothesis that

    asserts no association between winning or losing policy narrative frames and the

    attending narrative political strategy. Because these are nominal-level variables, no

    direction is predicted.

    Hypothesis 1a: There is an association between winning policy narrative frames

    and identification of winners in the narrative.Hypothesis 1b: There is an association between losing policy narrative frames and

    identification of losers in the narrative.

    Hypothesis 2: There is an association between winning policy narrative frames and

    the diffusion of benefits in the narrative; similarly, there is an association between

    losing policy narrative frames and concentration of benefits in the narrative.3

    Hypothesis 3: There is an association between winning policy narrative frames and

    the concentration of costs in the narrative; similarly, there is an association between

    losing policy narrative frames and diffusion of costs in the narrative.

    Hypothesis 4: There is an association between losing policy narrative frames and

    use of condensation symbols.

    Table 1. Operationalization of Dependent, Independent, and Control Variables

    Variables Definition n

    Dependent VariablesIdentification of winner Identify a winner of policy objective

    0 =none identified (no); 1 = winner (yes)

    105

    Identification of loser Identify a loser of policy objective0 =none identified (no); 1 = loser (yes)

    105

    Benefits Concentrate or diffuse benefits of policy objective0 =concentrated; 1 = diffused

    61

    Costs Concentrate or diffuse costs of policy objective0 =concentrated; 1 = diffused

    85

    Condensation symbol Reduces issue into loaded, dichotomous symbol0 =no use; 1 = used condensation symbol

    105

    Policy surrogate Wraps a specific issue in larger normative issues0 =no use; 1 = used policy surrogate

    105

    Science Use of scientific certainty or disagreement0 =scientific certainty; 1 = scientific disagreement

    54

    Independent VariableWinninglosing The narrative frame regarding policy objective

    0 =losing; 1 =winning105

    Control VariablesPresidential administration President at the time the narrative was written

    0 =Clinton; 1 = Bush105

    Use of science Whether the narrative used science or not0 =no science used; 1 =used science

    105

    Source:Greater Yellowstone Coalition and Blue Ribbon Coalition documents, 19972004.

    94 Policy Studies Journal, 35:1

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    9/22

    Hypothesis 5: There is an association between losing policy narrative frames and

    use of policy surrogates.

    Hypothesis 6: There is an association between winning policy narrative frames and

    the use of scientific certainty in the narrative; similarly, there is an associationbetween losing policy narrative frames and the use of scientific uncertainty in the

    narrative.

    Dependent Variables

    Temporally, whether the policy narrative is winning or losing precedes the

    political strategies used; thus, the dependent variables in this study are the political

    strategies (see Table 1). Using content analysis, a series of questions was developed

    to operationalize the dependent variables for the seven hypotheses (see Appendix A,

    questions 18 on the codebook).

    Independent Variable

    Whether a policy narrative is winning or losing explains what political strategies

    are employed. The problem of how to operationalize whether an interest group was

    winning or losing invoked much discussion among research team members. At one

    point, an objective measure was going to be utilized. In other words, based on

    executive, judicial, and administrative decisions, the interest group would be deter-

    mined to be winning or not. Interestingly, this proved difficult because of the vola-tility of Yellowstone policy issues during the time period under study. No interest

    group could be said to enjoy a true policy monopoly throughout the period (the

    bison issue is the most likely exception) because governmental decisions on these

    issues rarely achieved a permanent or stable status. Thus, the team decided that what

    was important in narrative terms was not objective winning or losing, but rather the

    perceptions of the interest group on whether they were winning on an issue (the

    group supported the status quo) or losing (the group felt that they were under attack

    in the policy environment). Question 9 of the code book in Appendix A measures

    this perception of winning and losing.

    Control Variables

    ACF controls of coalitional resources (i.e., presidential administration) and coa-

    litional policy learning (i.e., whether or not scientific evidence was used) were used.

    The ACF theory asserts that over time, policy change is, in part, a function of

    changing governing coalitions (affecting coalitional resources) and coalitional tech-

    nical expertise (impacting policy learning). To better assess the relationship between

    political strategies and winninglosing narrative frames, each Chi-square test (in

    succeeding discussions) was subsequently controlled for presidential administration

    and whether or not the narrative used science.

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 95

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    10/22

    The content analysis was conducted by three coders. Ten documents were pre-

    tested using an initial codebook. The documents were coded independently by the

    coders who then met periodically after coding every 2535 documents. At their

    meetings, the coders discussed their results, redefined and narrowed rules, and

    ultimately reconciled their disagreements. The reliability of the three coders wasevaluated by comparing them in three pairs on their coding of all questions. The

    reliability ratings range from a low of 78 percent to a high of 93 percent, with an

    average of agreeing 84 percent of the time (see Appendix B), thus reasonably estab-

    lishing intercoder reliability.

    Given that the narrative strategies were operationalized as nominal-level vari-

    ables, a Chi-square test of significance was conducted for each hypothesis to in-

    vestigate the statistical difference between the occurrence of narrative frame

    (winninglosing) and that of political narrative strategy or if the strategies utilized

    are attributed to chance alone. A continuity correction was applied with the occur-

    rence of small cells (n 5); a Fishers Exact Test was used to determine the statis-

    tical significance (Ramsey & Schafer, 1997, pp. 54751). The magnitude of the

    Chi-square results was assessed with a Cramrs V, the preferred Chi-square

    measure of association (McClendon, 2004, p. 455). Odds ratios (ORs) were calcu-

    lated as a cross-product ratio (Knoke, Bohrnstedt, & Mee, 2002, p. 161) and were

    used to indicate the odds of a specific political strategy occurring with a winning

    or losing narrative.

    Research Results

    Table 2 provides descriptive statistics of the one hundred five narratives coded.

    Note that of the winning and losing narratives, 71 of the 105 documents (68 percent)

    were coded as losing narratives, whereas only 34 (32 percent) were coded as

    winning. There are at least two reasons for this. First, groups may well be more likely

    to articulate and distribute a policy narrative when they are losing as their goal is to

    change the status quo, and their narrative is a form of both political defense and

    attack. Second, as discussed earlier, there were no clear policy monopolies in this

    time period. Instead, both interest groups experienced back-and-forth short-term

    wins and losses characteristic of wicked problems. Thus, both interest groups felt

    under attack consistently from nonfriendly forces. This is evidenced by the fact thatboth groups produced more losing policy narratives than winning across all three

    policy issues regardless of presidential administration.

    Hypotheses 1a and b: Identification of Winners and Losers

    Table 3 indicates statistically significant associations between winning narrative

    frames and the identification of a specific winner (c2[d.f. =1] =13.049,p

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    11/22

    Table 2. Descriptive Statistics of Interest Group Narratives by Winning or Losing Frame, Policy Issue,and Presidential Administration

    InterestGroup

    TotalDocuments

    WinningNarratives

    LosingNarratives

    Policy Issue PresidentialAdministration

    GYC 52 (100%) 14 (27%) 38 (73%) Bison ClintonWinning 3 (30%) Winning 4 (31%)Losing 7 (70%) Losing 9 (69%)Total 10 (100%) Total 13 (100%)

    Snowmobiles BushWinning 7 (22%) Winning 10 (26%)Losing 25 (78%) Losing 29 (74%)Total 32 (100%) Total 39 (100%)

    RoadlessWinning 4 (40%)Losing 6 (60%)Total 10 (100%)

    BRC 53 (100%) 20 (38%) 33 (62%) Bison ClintonWinning 0 (0%) Winning 6 (26%)Losing 4 (100%) Losing 17 (74%)Total 4 (100%) Total 23 (100%)

    Snowmobiles BushWinning 18 (47%) Winning 14 (47%)Losing 20 (53%) Losing 16 (53%)Total 38 (100%) Total 30 (100%)

    RoadlessWinning 2 (18%)Losing 6 (82%)Total 8 (100%)

    Total 105 (100%) 34 (32%) 71 (68%)

    Source:GYC and BRC documents, 19972004.GYC, Greater Yellowstone Coalition; BRC, Blue Ribbon Coalition.

    Table 3. Chi-Square Results for Identification of Winners and Losers by Narrative Frame

    Losing Narrative Winning Narrative Total

    Identification of winner Yes 45.1%

    (32)

    82.4%

    (28)

    60

    No 54.9%(39)

    17.6%(6)

    45

    Total 100.0%(71)

    100.0%(34)

    105

    c2(d.f. =1) = 13.049,p < 0.001; CramrsV=0.353,p < 0.001; ORWW =5.69

    Identification of loser Yes 95.8%(68)

    58.8%(20)

    88

    No 4.2%(3)

    41.2%(14)

    17

    Total 100.0%(71)

    100.0%(34)

    105

    c2(d.f. =1) = 23.134,p < 0.001; CramrsV=0.469,p < 0.001; ORLL =15.87

    Source:Greater Yellowstone Coalition and Blue Ribbon Coalition documents, 19972004.OR, odds ratio; ORWW, odds ratio of a winning frame; ORLL, odds ratio of a losing narrative frame.

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 97

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    12/22

    frame identifying a specific winner is 5.69; thus, winning policy narratives are five

    times more likely than losing narrative frames to identify a winner. Losing narrative

    frames contained a specific loser 95.8 percent of the time (fo =68; fe =59.5), compared

    with winning narrative frames identifying a loser 58.8 percent of the time (fo =20;

    fe =28.5). The odds ratio of a losing narrative frame identifying a specific loser is15.87; thus, losing narrative frames are fifteen times more likely than winning frames

    to identify a loser. The magnitude of these relationships is strong, with Cramrs

    V=0.353 (p

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    13/22

    time (fo =6; fe =10.8), compared with winning narratives that do so 50 percent of the

    time (fo =14; fe =9.2). Losing narratives concentrate benefits 81.8% of the time

    (fo =27; fe =22.2), compared with winning narratives that do so 50 percent of the time

    (fo =14; fe =18.8). Losing narratives are 4.5 times more likely to concentrate benefits,

    whereas winning narratives are 4.5 times more likely to diffuse benefits (OR =4.5).We can accept hypothesis 2.

    The association between (i) winning narratives and diffusing benefits and (ii)

    losing narratives and concentrating benefits is a political strategy used by interest

    groups to influence policy outcome. For example, the GYC applauded the success of

    the Clinton era snowmobile reductions by citing the improved National Park Service

    employees health as well as that of all visitors (Scott, 2004); thus, they diffused the

    benefits of the ban to many people. Similarly, the BRC presented the diffuse distri-

    bution of the benefits of snowmobile use to local economies, residents, and snow-

    mobile riders (Collins, 1998). Examples of concentrating benefits when losing are

    found as a political strategy in both the BRC and the GYC narratives. In a time when

    snowmobiling was under attack in the courts, the BRC contended that the only

    beneficiary from snowmobile regulation was the environmental group Fund for

    Animals (Cook, 1997). Similarly, the GYC concentrated benefits by claiming that

    President Bush was ignoring larger national interests and instead was bowing to

    intense lobbying by the snowmobile industry and the parks border towns (GYC,

    2002). Concentrating or diffusing the benefits of a policy proposal is a political

    narrative strategy employed to influence policy outcome.

    Hypothesis 3: Concentration and Diffusion of Costs

    Table 4 also indicates a statistically significant association between the concen-

    tration and diffusion of costs of the narrative frame (c2[d.f. =1] =11.683, p

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    14/22

    Hypothesis 4: Use of Condensation Symbols

    There is a statistically significant association between the use of condensation

    symbols and narrative frames (c2[d.f. =1] =3.490,p

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    15/22

    narratives only 11.8 percent of the time (fo =4; fe =8.7). Losing narratives are more

    than three times more likely to use a policy surrogate than a winning narrative

    (ORLPS =3.59). We can accept hypothesis 5.

    In political narratives, losing groups are more likely to strategically wrap the

    issue in the larger contentious cultural context by using policy surrogates. This useof a policy surrogate is again consistent with Baumgartners and Jones (1993) theory

    of issue expansion when a group is losing and with the research of Nie (2003) on

    environmental policy conflict. The BRCs policy surrogates tend to focus on either

    federalism or environmental elitism, arguing, we cant rely on the federal govern-

    ment to represent the publics interest (Cook, 1997). Furthermore, the BRC argued

    that policy was needed to see our natural resources protected FOR the people

    instead of FROM the people (Eggers, 1999). The GYC almost exclusively used

    surrogates when they were losing, only using a surrogate once when they were

    winning on an issue. Their surrogates focused on corruption by special interests, as

    exemplified in this statement from one of their articles: National interest is being

    sacrificed to the special interest of the snowmobile industry in of all places, Ameri-

    cas first national park (Sieck, 2002).

    Hypothesis 6: Scientific Certainty or Uncertainty

    As revealed in Table 6, there is no statistical association between winninglosing

    narrative frames and how science is used, either to show certainty or uncertainty. We

    reject hypothesis 6. Approximately 50 percent of both winning and losing narratives

    use science in their narratives; of those, both narrative frames used scientific cer-tainty at high rates, 89.5 and 85.7 percent, respectively.

    When both interest groups used science regardless of whether they were

    winning or losing, they tended to use it in terms of scientific certainty to back up

    their policy preference. Nie (2003, p. 323) concludes that competing groups in envi-

    ronmental policy controversies use science to forward their preferred policy objec-

    tives. The focus of science used in the two groups narratives is different; the GYC

    uses a conservation biology approach whereas the BRC uses a more technological

    approach (McBeth et al., 2005, p. 422). In general, the conflict over science between

    competing interest groups is usually a battle over the stable policy core beliefs

    embedded in the science rather than part of a dynamic narrative political strategy.

    Table 6. Chi-Square Results for Science by Narrative Frame

    Losing Narrative Winning Narrative Total

    Science Certainty 85.7%(30)

    89.5%(17)

    47

    Uncertainty 14.3%(5)

    10.5%(2)

    7

    Total 100.0%(35)

    100.0%(19)

    54

    c2(d.f. =1) = 0.154,ns; CramrsV= 0.053,ns

    Source:Greater Yellowstone Coalition and Blue Ribbon Coalition documents, 19972004.

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 101

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    16/22

    Controlling for Presidential Administration and Use of Science

    The ACF theory asserts that changes in governing coalitions affect policy change

    in that coalitional resources expand or contract, depending on whether the admin-

    istration aligns itself with a groups core beliefs or not. For example, the Bushadministrations shared policy beliefs added resources (power) to the BRC. Not

    surprisingly, controlling for presidential administration resulted in additive relation-

    ships among all six statistically significant political strategies. The relationship

    between political strategies and narrative frame persisted in direction and varied

    only somewhat in each control table. Thus, in understanding policy change, changes

    in governing coalitions and political strategies are critical.

    Additionally, the ACF theory differentiates between policy learning within a

    belief system andacrossbelief systems (Jenkins-Smith & Sabatier, 1993, p. 48). In the

    former, science is used to bolster a groups core beliefs; in the latter, scientific

    evidence and coalitional technical expertise can alter core beliefs over time. Given the

    wicked-problem nature of the GYA, when groups use science, it is used within a

    belief system to reify a groups policy beliefs. In controlling for those narratives that

    used science, five of the six political strategies remained virtually unchanged; thus,

    use of science is not related to winning or losing strategies. However, controlling for

    use of science led to the evaporation of any relationship between condensation

    symbols and narrative frame, thus weakening the interpretation of the use of con-

    densation symbols as a narrative political strategy.

    Discussion

    In this study, we seek to present a new methodological approach to the under-

    standing of the policy change process by integrating NPA and policy change theory

    while upholding the standards of traditional social science research. Our first

    research questionwhether or not NPA can be used appropriately within the

    context of traditional policy change theoryis answered affirmatively. In this study,

    issue expansion and containment in the turbulent GYA policy arena is empirically

    tested through coding interest group narratives. We systematically test whether or

    not winning narrative frames attempt to contain the issue with predictable narrative

    strategies (identification of winners, diffusion of benefits and concentration of costsof policy success, and use of scientific certainty) and whether or not losing narrative

    frames attempt to expand the issue with predictable narrative strategies (identifica-

    tion of losers, concentration of benefits and diffusion of costs of policy failure, use of

    condensation symbols and policy surrogates, and use of scientific uncertainty).

    While advocacy coalitions embed stable policy core beliefs in narratives, they also

    use those narratives to further dynamic political strategies.

    Our second research questionwhether or not operationalized narrative strat-

    egies reflect how groups attempt to contain or expand the policy issueis also

    answered affirmatively. When using the ACF controls, five of the seven hypotheses

    are supported. The data provide evidence for the notion that interest group narra-

    tives are indicators of a groups political strategies and tactics and are tied to whether

    102 Policy Studies Journal, 35:1

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    17/22

    a group is winning (and trying to contain an issue) or losing (and trying to expand

    an issue). Importantly, these strategies are not tied to core beliefs, nor are they

    ideologically based or reflective of writing ability or style. These strategies cut across

    ideological lines, are used by both sides in the policy dispute, and are connected to

    how a group perceives its position in the policy battle. Thus, narratives as a source ofstudy are strategic, predictable, and testable and are an appropriate unit of analysis

    for scholars interested in studying policy change.

    Finally, our third research question explores the additions to the literature. This

    method of analysis integrates NPA with policy change theory and adds to the

    existing literature. The contribution here addresses Brown and Stewarts (1993,

    p. 101) criticism of the ACF. We argue that narratives as political strategies are a

    valuable source of study for researchers. The activity in the GYA occurred in periods

    of alternating victories and losses. Although several external subsystem events (e.g.,

    court opinions, well-publicized media events, changes in presidential administra-

    tions) could have swung the policy battles toward one group or another by produc-

    ing shifts in coalitional resources, the two interest groups consistently perceived

    themselves as losing 67.6 percent of the time. Losing narratives, as we have seen, are

    more confrontational and seek to expand conflict to additional parties. In wicked

    policy problems, interest group narratives only reinforce and exacerbate policy

    intractability. Short-term wins are quickly replaced by the perception of losing and

    the need to retaliate. The effect is that the narratives almost continually expand the

    scope of the conflict, thus drawing in more groups to the policy dispute. As seen in

    the eight-year course of this study, the result is long periods of protracted conflict.

    The GYA policymaking meets the conditions of what Sabatier and Jenkins-Smith(1999, p. 132) call the devil shift or the situation where opposing coalitions

    remember losses more than victories and inflate the evilness and power of oppos-

    ing groups. In addition, this research involved two purposive interest groups, and

    these groups, as hypothesized by Sabatier and Jenkins-Smith (1999, p. 134), maintain

    a tight script and thus resist alterations to their scripts that would move dialogues

    toward policy learning.

    In policy environments where there is both a clear policy monopoly and a clear

    out-of-power coalition, we would assume that the minimal coalition of a policy

    monopoly would rarely perceive that they are losing and that their narratives would

    consistently reflect the theory of issue containment. Research on narratives in stablepolicy environments might provide initial signs for policy researchers that the policy

    equilibrium had been punctuated.

    Conclusion

    This work has used a case study of environmental policy making in the GYA to

    examine the interest group use of narrative political strategies in defending existing

    policies or advocating new policies. Grounded in the theories of Sabatier, Jenkins-

    Smith, Baumgartner, Jones, Schattschneider, Stone, and others, the methodological

    model is generalizable to any policy subsystem in such policy areas as economic

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 103

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    18/22

    development, energy, crime, and foreign policy. The intersection of policy change

    theory and NPA prompts theory building.

    In determining the extent to which our work contributes to this theory building,

    we turn to Sabatier (1999, pp. 26670), who argues that there are seven guidelines for

    theory development. First, our analysis is empirical with testable hypotheses.Second, our method allows for testing of our hypotheses in a variety of policy

    settings. Third, we found a causal relationship between perception of winning and

    losing and policy narrative strategies and have accounted for some ACF controls.

    Fourth, our study suggests that individuals are political, seek to win, and intention-

    ally and strategically use narratives to either contain or diffuse a policy issue. Fifth,

    we have shown a consistency among five of our seven hypotheses. Sixth, our aim is

    to build a long-term research agenda and invite others to build upon our method-

    ology. Finally, our research uses principles from the ACF, punctuated equilibrium,

    and three streams of policy change and enhances these works with NPA. We con-

    clude that narrative political strategies are a vital source for analyzing policy change

    in a complex political environment.

    Mark K. McBethis a professor of political science at Idaho State University.Elizabeth A. Shanahanis an assistant professor of political science at Montana StateUniversity.Ruth J. Arnellis a doctoral student in political science at Idaho State University.Paul L. Hathawayis a doctoral student in political science at Idaho State University.

    Notes

    A different version of this paper was presented at the 2005 Western Political Science Conference inAlbuquerque, New Mexico. The authors wish to thank Teri Peterson for her statistical consultations.

    1. The BRC is part of a larger advocacy coalition (the wise use coalition) that includes ranchers, localbusiness elites, snowmobile, ATV, and motorcycle manufacturers, elected officials, and scientists.

    2. The GYC is part of a larger advocacy coalition (the environmental coalition) that includes nationalenvironmental groups, local business elites, elected officials, and scientists.

    3. The identification of benefits as diffuse or concentrated resulted in mutually exclusive coded responses;in other words, when benefits were coded, they were either concentrated or diffused. Hence, they areincluded in the same hypothesis. The same is true for concentrateddiffuse costs (hypothesis 3) anduncertaintycertainty in use of science (hypothesis 6).

    References

    Achter, Paul J. 2004. TV Technology, and Mccarthyism: Crafting the Democratic Renaissance in an Age ofFear.Quarterly Journal of Speech90: 30726.

    Baumgartner, Frank R. 1989. Conflict and Rhetoric in French Policy Making . Pittsburgh, PA: University ofPittsburgh Press.

    Baumgartner, Frank R., and Bryan D. Jones. 1993.Agendas and Instability in American Politics. Chicago, IL:University of Chicago Press.

    Berry, Jeffrey M. 1989. The Interest Group Society, 2nd ed. New York: Harper Collins Publishers.

    Brown, Anthony, and Joseph Stewart Jr. 1993. Competing Advocacy Coalitions, Policy Evolution, andAirline Deregulation. In Policy Change and Learning: An Advocacy Coalition Approach, ed. Paul A.Sabatier, and Hank C. Jenkins-Smith. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, 83103.

    104 Policy Studies Journal, 35:1

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    19/22

    Catton, John, and Betsy Buffington. 2002. Snowmobiling Abuses in Yellowstone Continue Despite CostlyNew Mitigation Efforts. http://www.greateryellowstone.org/snowmobiles_exhaust_nr.htmlAccessed August 20, 2002.

    Cawley, McGreggor R., and John Freemuth. 1993. Tree Farms, Mother Earth, and Other Dilemmas: ThePolitics of Ecosystem Management in Greater Yellowstone.Society and Natural Resources6: 4153.

    Clark, Timothy W., and Steven C. Minta. 1994. Greater Yellowstones Future. Moose, WY: HomesteadPublishing.

    Cobb, Roger W., and Charles D. Elder. 1983. Participation in American Politics: The Dynamics of AgendaBuilding. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Collins, Clark. 1998. Help Save Snowmobiling on Our Public Lands. Blue Ribbon Magazine (July).http://www.sharetrails.org/mag/jul98. Accessed August 17, 2002.

    Cook, Adena. 1997. Yellowstone Suit Settlement Denounced: Hanky Panky Uncovered. Blue RibbonMagazine(November). http://www.sharetrails.org/mag/nov97/hanky.Accessed August 17, 2002.

    Eggers, Viki. 1999. Earth Island Petition Debunked: Enviro Facts Are Fantasy. Blue Ribbon Magazine(March). http://www.sharetrails.org/mag/Mar99/Story4.htm.Accessed February 21, 2003.

    Fischer, Frank, and John Forrester. 1993.The Argumentative Turn in Policy Analysis and Planning. Durham,NC: Duke University Press.

    Greater Yellowstone Coalition (GYC). 2001a. A Nationwide Plea is Heard: Barring Political Interference,Americas Roadless Forests Will Be Protected. Greater Yellowstone Report Newsletter (Spring 2001).http://www.greateryellowstone.org/roadless_sp01nl.html. Accessed December 13, 2003.

    . 2001b. Yellowstones Road to Recovery. Greater Yellowstone Report(Early Summer): 911.

    . 2002. Park Plan Stinks, No Matter How You Cut It. http://www.greateryellowstone.org/snowmobiles_gftrib_ed02.html. Accessed December 13, 2003.

    Hajer, Maarten. 1993. Discourse Coalitions and the Institutionalization of Practice: The Case of Acid Rainin Great Britain. In The Argumentative Turn in Policy Analysis and Planning, ed. Frank Fischer, and

    John Forester. Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 4376.

    Jenkins-Smith, Hank C., and PaulA. Sabatier. 1993. The Dynamics of Policy-Oriented Learning. In PolicyChange and Learning: An Advocacy Coalition Approach, ed. Paul A. Sabatier, and Hank C. Jenkins-Smith.Boulder, CO: Westview Press, 4158.

    Johnson, Janet Buttolph, and H. T. Reynolds. 2005.Political Science Research Methods. 5th ed. Washington,DC: CQ Press.

    Kingdon, John W. 1995. Agendas, Alternatives, and Public Policies. New York: Longman.

    Knoke, David, George W. Bohrnstedt, and Alisa Potter Mee. 2002.Statistics for Social Science Data Analysis,4th ed. Itasca, IL. F.E. Peacock Publishers.

    McBeth, Mark K., Elizabeth A. Shanahan, and Michael D. Jones. 2005. The Science of Storytelling:Measuring Policy Beliefs in Greater Yellowstone.Society and Natural Resources18: 41329.

    McClendon, McKee J. 2004.Statistical Analysis in the Social Sciences. Belmont, CA: Thomson.

    Nie, Martin A. 2003. Drivers of Natural Resource-Based Political Conflict.Policy Sciences36: 30741.

    Radaelli, Claudio M. 1999. Harmful Tax Competition in the EU: Policy Narratives and Advocacy Coali-tions.Journal of Market Studies37: 66182.

    Ramsey, Fred L., and Daniel W. Schafer. 1997.The Statistical Sleuth: A Course in Methods of Data Analysis.Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.

    Riker, William H. 1962. A Theory of Winning Coalitions. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

    Rittel, Horst W. J., and Melvin M. Webber. 1973. Dilemmas in a General Theory of Planning. PolicySciences4: 15569.

    Roe, Emery. 1994. Narrative Policy Analysis. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

    Sabatier, Paul A. 1999. Fostering the Development of Policy Theory. InTheories of the Policy Process, chap.10, ed. Paul A. Sabatier. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, 26176.

    . 2000. Clear Enough to Be Wrong. Journal of European Public Policy 7: 13440.

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 105

    http://www.greateryellowstone.org/snowmobiles_exhaust_nr.htmlhttp://www.sharetrails.org/mag/jul98http://www.sharetrails.org/mag/nov97/hankyhttp://www.sharetrails.org/mag/Mar99/Story4.htmhttp://www.greateryellowstone.org/roadless_sp01nl.htmlhttp://www.greateryellowstone.org/http://www.greateryellowstone.org/http://www.greateryellowstone.org/roadless_sp01nl.htmlhttp://www.sharetrails.org/mag/Mar99/Story4.htmhttp://www.sharetrails.org/mag/nov97/hankyhttp://www.sharetrails.org/mag/jul98http://www.greateryellowstone.org/snowmobiles_exhaust_nr.html
  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    20/22

    Sabatier, Paul A., and Hank C. Jenkins-Smith, eds. 1993.Policy Change and Learning: An Advocacy CoalitionApproach. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

    . 1999. The Advocacy Coalition Framework: An Assessment. In Theories of the Policy Process, ed.Paul A. Sabatier. Boulder, CO: Westview Press, 11766.

    Schattschneider, E. E. 1960.The Semi-Sovereign People. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Schneider, Anne L., and Helen M. Ingram. 2005. Deserving and Entitled: Social Constructions and Public

    Policy. Albany: State University of New York Press.

    Schon, Donald, and Martin Rein. 1994. Frame Reflection: Toward the Resolution of Intractable Policy Contro-versies. New York: Basic Books.

    Scott, Michael. 2004. Rangers No Longer Getting Sick in Yellowstone. News Release(February).http://www.news.greateryellowstone.bridgeband.net/article. Accessed July 7, 2004.

    Sieck, Hope. 2002. Yellowstones Winter in Question. Greater Yellowstone Reports(Summer): 67.

    Stone, Deborah. 2002. Policy Paradox: The Art of Political Decision Making, revised ed. New York: W.W.Norton.

    Tierney, John, and William Frasure. 1998. Culture Wars on the Frontier: Interests, Values, and Policy

    Narratives in Public Lands Politics. In Interest Group Politics, ed. Allan J. Cigler, and Burdett Loomis.Washington, DC: CQ Press, 30326.

    Welch, Jack. 2000. Blue Ribbon Delivers 10,170 Comment Letters to National Park Service on Winter UsePlan for Yellowstone.Blue Ribbon Magazine (January). http://www.sharetrails.org/mag/Jan2000/story6.htm. Accessed August 17, 2002.

    Wilson, Matthew A. 1997. The Wolf in Yellowstone: Science, Symbol, or Politics? Deconstructing theConflict between Environmentalism and Wise Use. Society and Natural Resources10: 45368.

    Wood, B. Dan, and Alesha Doan. 2003. The Politics of Problem Definition: Applying and Testing Thresh-old Models.American Journal of Political Science 47 (4): 64053.

    Appendix A: Abbreviated Code Book

    1. Does the narrative identify a specific winner (entity that benefits) of a policy

    decision or potential decision? For example, anti-recreationists will rejoice over

    this policy decision or the snowmobile industry is clearly rooting for this

    lawsuit to be thrown out of court.

    A-Yes (go to question #2) B-No (skip to question #3)

    2. What best describes how the narrative constructs the benefits of the policy

    decision?

    A-The narrative is constructed as providing concentrated benefits (a few

    gain). For example, the wealthy environmentalists will have YNP astheir personal playground or this decision benefits the snowmobile

    industry.

    Paragraph number(s) and group:

    B-The narrative is constructed as providing diffused benefits (many gain).

    For example, the American people will benefit from the closing of YNP to

    snowmobiles or snowmobile enthusiasts from throughout the country

    applauded this decision.

    Paragraph number(s) and group:

    3. Does the narrative identify a specific loser (entity that pays the costs) of a policy

    decision? For example, the American people are the losers when industry

    controls government or local businesses are hurt by these actions of the NPS.

    106 Policy Studies Journal, 35:1

    http://www.news.greateryellowstone.bridgeband.net/articlehttp://www.news.greateryellowstone.bridgeband.net/articlehttp://www.sharetrails.org/mag/Jan2000http://www.sharetrails.org/mag/Jan2000http://www.news.greateryellowstone.bridgeband.net/articlehttp://www.news.greateryellowstone.bridgeband.net/article
  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    21/22

    A-Yes (go to question #4) B-No (skip to question #5)

    4. What best describes how the narrative constructs the costs of the policy decision?

    A-The narrative is constructed as providing concentrated costs (a few pay).

    For example, This regulation will harm only a small number of greedybusiness owners who fail to adapt to changing times or The throwing out

    of this policy will only harm the sensibilities of a few extremists.

    Paragraph number(s) and group:

    B-The narrative is constructed as providing diffused costs (the many pay).

    For example, this plan protects bison while projecting costs over many

    differing groups or this plan protects snowmobiling with only minor

    adjustments required of business owners who must now be licensed guides

    and use cleaner machines.

    Paragraph number(s) and group:

    5. Does the narrative contain at least one condensation symbol? The definition of a

    condensation symbol is a word or phrase that shrinks and reduces complicated

    concepts into simple, manageable, or memorable forms.

    A-Yes, list and identify paragraph(s) B-No

    6. Does this narrative use a policy surrogate? For example, policy surrogate =

    greedy snowmobile corporations exploit Yellowstone for their own purposes

    while the pollution gets worse and worse or this issue is all about people in

    Washington, DC telling people in our small towns about how to live their lives.

    A-Yes, list and identify paragraph(s) B-No

    7. Does the narrative use science to define a problem, counter a problem definition,

    or justify a policy approach?

    A-Yes. (go to question #8) B-No (go to question #9)

    8. Is the mention of science used in the context of:

    A-Disputing science B-Establishing scientific certainty

    9. What is the stance of the narrative towards the policy being discussed?

    A. Winning (supports the policy environment and actions discussed in the

    narrative)B. Losing (the group is under attack even if they are partially winning)

    McBeth et al: Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory Intersection 107

  • 8/10/2019 The Intersection of Narrative Policy Analysis and Policy Change Theory - Mark K. McBeth, Elizabeth A. Shanahan,

    22/22

    Appendix B: Intercoder Reliability

    Question Agreement (%) Disagreement (%) Total Codings

    1 243 (78%) 72 (22%) 3152 23 (93%) 9 (7%) 1323 275 (87%) 40 (13%) 3154 210 (89%) 25 (11%) 2355 268 (85%) 47 (85%) 3156 259 (82%) 56 (18%) 3157 156 (84%) 30 (16%) 1868 156 (96%) 6 (4%) 1629 251 (80%) 64 (20%) 315

    TOTAL 1,941 (85%) 349 (15%) 2,290 (100%)

    Note. Questions 1, 3, 5, 6, and 9 are paired codings comparing the three coders to each other. All coderscoded this screening questions. These questions all sum to 315 (105 documents 3 coders). Questions 2and 4 are also paired codings but have smaller numbers because of screenings. The first 75 documents for

    question #7 were coded by only two coders. Because there were only 2 coders there was only 1 pairedcoding instead of 3 on this question. Thus the total number of codings for question 7 equals only 186.

    108 Policy Studies Journal, 35:1