the evolution of geographic variation in birdsong...evolution in z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of...

56
The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong Jeffrey Podos* and Paige S. Warren { *department of biology, graduate program in organismic and evolutionary biology, university of massachusetts amherst, massachusetts 01003 { department of natural resources conservation, graduate program in organismic and evolutionary biology, university of massachusetts amherst, massachusetts 01003 I. INTRODUCTION Evolutionary biologists have a longstanding interest in how organisms vary geographically. This interest is motivated in part by recognition of a relationship between geographic variation and the process of speciation. Traits that vary over a given species’ range may serve as neutral, noncon- tributing indicators of the early stages of divergence, for example as differ- ent populations adapt to distinct environments and undergo corresponding genotypic and phenotypic divergence (Schluter, 2000). In other cases, traits that vary geographically might contribute to the speciation process. Primary examples of such traits are mating ornaments and displays which, in many animals, are centrally involved in mate recognition and mate selection (Andersson, 1994; Boughman, 2001; Foster, 1999; Panhuis et al., 2001; Wells and Henry, 1998; WestEberhard, 1983). Geographic divergence of mating signals can, under particular circumstances, facilitate assortative mating, reproductive isolation, and thus continued divergence among popu- lations (Irwin et al., 2001; Lachlan and Servedio, 2004; Liou and Price, 1994; Payne et al., 2000; Ptacek, 2000; Slabbekoorn and Smith, 2002a). Diver- gence of mating signals and mate recognition systems is increasingly recog- nized as an important factor in speciation (Ryan, 1986). Studies of vocal signals in birds offer potentially useful opportunities for empirical tests of the relationships among geographic signal divergence, reproductive isolation, and speciation (reviewed by Edwards et al., 2005). Many bird vocalizations express significant geographic variation, a phe- nomenon that can be attributed largely to the tendency for many birds to 403 0065-3454/07 $35.00 Copyright 2007, Elsevier Inc. DOI: 10.1016/S0065-3454(07)37009-5 All rights reserved. ADVANCES IN THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR, VOL. 37

Upload: others

Post on 12-Jul-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

ADVANCES IN THE STUDY OF BEHAVIOR, VOL. 37

The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong

Jeffrey Podos* and Paige S. Warren{

*department of biology, graduate program in organismic andevolutionary biology, university of massachusetts

amherst, massachusetts 01003{department of natural resources conservation, graduate program inorganismic and evolutionary biology, university of massachusetts

amherst, massachusetts 01003

I. INTRODUCTION

Evolutionary biologists have a long‐standing interest in how organismsvary geographically. This interest is motivated in part by recognition of arelationship between geographic variation and the process of speciation.Traits that vary over a given species’ range may serve as neutral, noncon-tributing indicators of the early stages of divergence, for example as differ-ent populations adapt to distinct environments and undergo correspondinggenotypic and phenotypic divergence (Schluter, 2000). In other cases, traitsthat vary geographically might contribute to the speciation process. Primaryexamples of such traits are mating ornaments and displays which, in manyanimals, are centrally involved in mate recognition and mate selection(Andersson, 1994; Boughman, 2001; Foster, 1999; Panhuis et al., 2001;Wells and Henry, 1998; West‐Eberhard, 1983). Geographic divergence ofmating signals can, under particular circumstances, facilitate assortativemating, reproductive isolation, and thus continued divergence among popu-lations (Irwin et al., 2001; Lachlan and Servedio, 2004; Liou and Price, 1994;Pa yne et al. , 2000; Ptacek, 2000 ; Slabbekoo rn and Smith, 2002a). Diver-gence of mating signals and mate recognition systems is increasingly recog-nized as an important factor in speciation (Ryan, 1986).

Studies of vocal signals in birds offer potentially useful opportunities forempirical tests of the relationships among geographic signal divergence,reproductive isolation, and speciation (reviewed by Edwards et al., 2005).Many bird vocalizations express significant geographic variation, a phe-nomenon that can be attributed largely to the tendency for many birds to

4030065-3454/07 $35.00 Copyright 2007, Elsevier Inc.DOI: 10.1016/S0065-3454(07)37009-5 All rights reserved.

Page 2: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

404 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

learn to vocalize through imitation. Imitative vocal learning enables theready generation and rapid transmission of novel patterns of vocal structure(Slabbekoorn and Smith, 2002a; Slater, 1989). Indeed, it has been arguedthat consequent plasticity of the vocal phenotype has been instrumental ingenerating the high species diversity that characterizes some avian taxa(Fitzpatrick, 1988; Vermeij, 1988; cf. Baptista and Trail, 1992; see alsoIrwin and Price, 1999; ten Cate, 2000). A Science Citation Index query(Fig. 1A) attests to rapidly expanding activity, over the past 15 years, in thestudy of the causes and consequences of geographic variation in birdsong.

Prospects for this field of inquiry, however, did not look very promisingjust a few decades ago. Mounting unease centered especially on questionsabout the evolution of ‘‘dialects,’’ a particular form of vocal geographicvariation. This unease was well illustrated in Baker and Cunningham’sinfluential review of the phenomenon of dialects, a main goal of which wasto articulate a ‘‘synthetic theory’’ on dialect origins andmaintenance (Bakerand Cunningham, 1985). Responses to this review, provided by a panel of

90

60

30

0

5-year span

Num

ber

of li

tera

ture

cita

tions

1965–1969

1970–1974

1975–1979

1980– 1984

1985– 1989

1990– 1994

1995– 1999

2000– 2004

FIG. 1. Number of primary literature citations, binned over 5‐year spans, for two searches on

Thompson’s ISI Web of Science. (A) Filled bars, search query as follows: topic! [bird* AND

song AND (geographic* OR dialect*)]. At the time of the search, Web of Science queries did

not include abstract text for literature pre‐1990; thus only data post‐1990 are presented.

A marked increase in citations is evident, pointing to increased activity in the field. (B) Open

bars, number of publications that Marler and Tamura (1964), a classic paper on birdsong

dialects. The post‐1990 trend mirrors that of the broader query (closed bars). Additionally,

this search reveals a temporary reduction in activity in the field in the early 1990s,

corresponding to the timing of the Baker and Cunningham (1985) exchange.

Page 3: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 405

peer experts, were forceful and included laments about difficulties in quan-tifying patterns of vocal geographic variation, in comparing data across taxa,in extrapolating laboratory data to field situations, and, most critically forpresent purposes, in identifying evolutionary factors that facilitate dialectformation and maintenance (Baptista, 1985; Brenowitz, 1985; Kroodsma,1985a; Lemon, 1985; McGregor, 1985). With regard to this latter issue,Baker and Cunningham argued that dialects are necessarily maintainedthrough adaptive processes, and thus, in order to understand their evolutionwe must examine their present function. A broad alternative hypothesis,that dialects emerge as epiphenomena of other evolutionary processes(Andrew, 1962), was summarily dismissed as being ‘‘pointless’’ (Baker andCunningham, 1985, p. 86). The peer expert panel generally found Baker andCunningham to be overly supportive of local adaptation hypotheses ofdialect evolution and overly dismissive of alternative hypotheses (Baptista,1985; McGregor, 1985; Waser, 1985). Given the wide range of stated unre-solved issues, along with limited evidence at the time that could support anyparticular hypothesis of dialect evolution, it is perhaps not surprising thatpublication of the Baker and Cunningham exchange was followed by aperiod of dampened enthusiasm for the field (Fig. 1B).

Our goal in this chapter is to assess the present state of the field, from bothempirical and conceptual perspectives. Prior reviews on the topic of geo-graphic variation in bird vocalizations have been numerous (Baker andCunningham, 1985; Krebs and Kroodsma, 1980; Mundinger, 1982;Slabbekoorn and Smith, 2002a), and readers may question the value of yetanother contribution. Yet new information continues to accrue. Moreover,recent general advances in the study of birdsong—a field that has remainedactive in realms ranging from mechanisms to ecology and evolution—haverenewed theway that we can study geographic variation, in at least twoways.First, we have gained numerous insights into the range of possible functionsof song learning, particularly in the arenas of social and sexual selection(Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005; Kroodsma and Byers, 1991; Nowicki et al.,2002). Our increasingly detailed understanding of the myriad functions ofsong learning suggests that patterns of vocal geographic variation mayemerge as secondary by‐products of selection on other functions, ratherthan through direct selection for geographic patterns themselves (Slater,1989). Second, advances in our understanding of mechanisms of vocalproduction suggest additional ‘‘non‐functional’’ scenarios by which geo-graphic vocal variation may emerge. Whereas earlier models of song evolu-tion focused on vocal imitation and cultural evolution, geographic variationin song may also emerge through evolution of the morphological and physi-ological underpinnings of song production.

Page 4: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

406 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

We begin with a brief overview of the study of geographic variationin birdsong, focusing in particular on song dialects and hypothesis thathave traditionally been put forward to explain their evolution (Section II).To evaluate the status of these hypotheses, we next conduct a comparativesurvey based on data gleaned from published literature (Section III). Ourfocus on dialects in these two sections is not motivated by an opinion thatdialects are inherently more interesting than other patterns of geographicvariation, or by an opinion that dialects are a unique phenomenon requiringspecial explanation. Rather it is simply because dialects have been the focusof the majority of published studies in this area. We then discuss how recentadvances in the study of birdsong have enriched our ability to addressquestions about vocal geographic variation, arguing in particular that mod-ern studies on mechanisms of song learning and production support ‘‘by‐product’’ models of vocal geographic evolution (Section IV). As we arguebelow, the by‐product hypothesis of vocal geographic evolution can beregarded as a broader version of the ‘‘epiphenomenon’’ hypothesis ofvocal dialect evolution. In Section V, we summarize the factors that maycontribute together to the evolution of geographic variation in birdvocalizations.

II. EVOLUTION OF GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN SONG: LITERATURE OVERVIEW

A. THE IMPORTANCE OF LEARNING MECHANISMS AND DISPERSAL PATTERNS

Perhaps the most appropriate place to begin an overview of the literatureon geographic variation in birdsong is with Marler and Tamura’s classicwork on white‐crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli (Marlerand Tamura, 1962, 1964). The phenomenon of geographic variation inbirdsong had been observed and reported on previously, but only a handfulof published studies (Borror, 1961) had made use of sound spectrograms,which provide an invaluable visual aid for assessing patterns of vocalgeographic variation. Marler and Tamura described, for Z. leucophrysnuttalli of Northern California, the now‐classic ‘‘dialect’’ pattern of geo-graphic variation, in which songs within particular populations ‘‘all sharecertain salient characteristics . . . which differ in certain consistent respectsfrom the patterns found in neighboring populations’’ (Marler and Tamura,1964, pp. 1483–1484). A Berkeley population of birds, for instance, wasfound to sing trills with a specific structure (fewer notes, of descendingfrequencies) distinct from trills sung by other nearby populations (Marlerand Tamura, 1962). As suggested by Marler and Tamura and as generallycorroborated by later studies (reviewed by Kroodsma et al., 1985), twomain

Page 5: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 407

factors appear to facilitate dialect evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli. Thefirst is that songs are culturally transmitted across generations, via vocallearning. A central role for vocal learning in song development in thesebirds was first demonstrated in a series of laboratory studies in which youngmales were exposed to various training regimes (Marler and Tamura, 1964).Training models presented over loudspeakers, during the sensitive phase ofsong acquisition, were shown to be copied precisely by these birds, evenwhen training models had been recorded from nonnatal localities (Marlerand Tamura, 1964). By contrast, in the absence of training models, birdswere found to develop songs with atypical, degraded acoustic structure. Itwas thus argued that song learning enables the ready transmission of songpatterns across generations, not just from fathers to sons but also potential-ly to other young males in a population. Song learning facilitates dialectformation by providing a mechanism for generating vocal novelties, whichmay emerge through copying ‘‘errors’’ (Marler and Tamura, 1964; see alsoBaptista, 1977; Lemon, 1975; Marler and Peters, 1987, 1988; Slater, 1986,1989).

The second main factor that appears to contribute to song dialectevolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal.Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested two possibilities:that dialects may emerge if male birds remain on the grounds where theylearned to sing, or that dialects may emerge if males do disperse but thensettle preferentially in locations where they hear songs similar to theirown. Either explanation would be consistent with the observation of vocal‘‘neighborhoods.’’ Banding/recapture studies (Baker and Mewaldt, 1978;Petrinovich et al., 1981) confirmed that dispersal distances in this subspeciesare indeed moderate, most commonly within a few hundred meters andrarely exceeding 1 km. By contrast, data in support of biased dispersal inthis subspecies have been equivocal, and their interpretation controversial( Baker and Mewaldt, 1981 ; Baker et al. , 1985; Hafn er and Peters on, 1985;Petrinovich et al., 1981).

The Z. leucophrys nuttalli system turns out to be comparatively, althoughby no means absolutely, straightforward; other systems examined to datefeature their own complications and deviations from the nuttalli scenario.This point is well illustrated by variation found even within this one species.Two additional white‐crowned sparrow subspecies,Z. leucophrys pugetensisand Z. leucophrys oriantha, also exhibit song dialects. In the former sub-species, dialects occur over larger neighborhood areas (Baptista, 1977;Chilton and Lein, 1996a; DeWolfe and Baptista, 1995; Heinemann, 1981;Nelson and Soha, 2004), and in the latter subspecies, songs vary systemati-cally among subalpine meadow populations (Chilton et al., 1995; Harbisonet al., 1999; Orejuela and Morton, 1975). Birds of both subspecies turn out

Page 6: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

408 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

to be less philopatric than their nuttalli counterparts, especially Z. leu-cophrys oriantha, which is fully migratory. Recent field and laboratorystudies by Nelson and colleagues indicate that birds of both subspeciesmemorize multiple models at their natal grounds, and then, postdispersal,‘‘select’’ among memorized models to best match song types present attheir breeding grounds (‘‘overproduction’’ and ‘‘selective attrition’’:Nelson, 2000; Nelson et al., 1995, 1996a). An alternative model of songlearning, postdispersal acquisition and memorization of song models, asposited by Heinemann (1981), appears not to apply in these birds. A fourthsubspecies, Z. leucophrys gambelli, a long‐distance migrant that breeds inthe sub‐Arctic, shows no evidence of dialects (Austen and Handford, 1991;DeWolfe et al., 1974; Nelson, 1998). Nelson (1999) argues that the post-dispersal, prebreeding time frame in this subspecies is too brief to allow thekinds of extended interactions that are required for song matching viaselective attrition.

Studies of white‐crowned sparrows thus demonstrate that learning stra-tegies, dispersal patterns, and resulting geographic song patterns can behighly variable even within a single species. This variation of course repre-sents just a tiny sample of the diversity present in the songbirds as a group(Krebs and Kroodsma, 1980; Kroodsma, 1996; Slater, 1989). Indeed, aprimary message of prior reviews of geographic song variation has beenof caution in extrapolating results across species (Kroodsma, 1996). Onewidespread phenomenon that does not apply in white‐crowned sparrows,for instance, is postdispersal acquisition and learning of song models, as hasbeen shown, for example, in the brood‐parasitic brown‐headed and bronzedcowbirds, Molothrus ater and M. aeneus (Rothstein and Fleischer, 1987;Warren, 2002). The white‐crowned sparrow system also does not addressthe potential influence of improvisation on geographic song patterns(Kroodsma and Verner, 1978; Kroodsma et al., 1997; Marler et al., 1972).Moreover, unlike many other species, adult white‐crowned sparrows tendto produce only a single song type. Patterns of geographic variation, and theecological and learning‐based causes of these patterns, tend to be morechallenging although sometimes still possible to document in species withsong repertoires (Searcy et al., 2002; Slater et al., 1984; Williams andSlater, 1990).

Explaining the formation of dialects might be relatively straightforward ifpatterns of geographic variation in song evolved solely as an incidentalby‐product of particular learning mechanisms and patterns of dispersal(Fig. 2A). However, since the earliest studies of avian vocal geographicvariation, authors have also spent considerable energy contemplating thepotential fitness benefits of particular geographic song patterns. Implicit inthis exercise is the hypothesis that selection for particular geographic patterns

Page 7: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

FIG. 2. Traditional framework to explain the evolution of vocal geographic variation in

songbirds. (A) Solid lines: Geographic variation in song emerges, in a proximate sense, as a by‐product of specific learning mechanisms and dispersal patterns. The evolution of learning

mechanisms that foster early, accurate imitation, combined with the evolution of limited

dispersal distances, for example, will result in the evolution of sharp dialects. (B) Dashed

lines: Selection for particular patterns of geographic variation may alter the evolution of

learning mechanisms and patterns of dispersal, in a feedback loop. For instance, selection

favoring strong assortative mating (local adaptation hypothesis) may conceivably favor the

evolution of early song imitation and limited dispersal, and thus the evolution of sharp dialects.

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 409

alters, in a feedback loop, the evolution of the learning mechanisms anddispersal patterns that shape vocal geographic divergence (Fig. 2B; Jenkins,1985). To explore this scenario further we turn again to the phenomenon ofdialects and hypotheses that have been put forward to explain their evolution.We provide brief commentary on definitions of song dialects, and then exam-ine three broad categories of hypotheses that have traditionally been for-warded to explain their evolution.

B. DEFINITION OF SONG DIALECTS

As noted by Slat er (1989 , p. 33), ‘‘geograph ic vari ation [in song] is seldomthe simple matter that the word ‘dialect’ might imply.’’ Indeed, ‘‘dialects’’have been described across a range of geographic scales (e.g., compareLea der et al ., 2000 with Warre n, 2002; see also Mund inger, 1982 ) and fora variety of vocal parameters. Schematically, we can represent geographicstructure in vocal parameters as taking a range of spatial patterns, includingrandom variation (Fig. 3A), gradual and shallow clines (Fig. 3B), or steepclines with stepped variation (Fig. 3C). This latter form is consistent withclassic definitions of dialects (Marler and Tamura, 1962; Mundinger, 1982).Numerous researchers have noted that strict dialects (Fig. 3C) may be acomparatively rare phenomenon (Slater, 1986, 1989). The functional hypoth-eses that we review in Section II.C have drawn on the presence of sharpboundaries between dialect neighborhoods as justification for regardingdialects as being actively maintained by selection.

Page 8: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

Random

Clinal

Dialect

Geographic locality

Voc

al p

aram

eter

(s)

FIG. 3. Schematic representation of three patterns of geographic variation in song. Geograph-

ic variation can be manifested at varying scales. Dialect variation features sharp transitions in

vocal parameters between localities, and consistency in vocal parameters within localities.

410 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

C. HYPOTHESES TO EXPLAIN THE EVOLUTION OF SONG DIALECTS

1. Local Adaptation Hypothesis

The local adaptation hypothesis, alluded to by Marler and Tamura (1962)and then formalized by Nottebohm (1969), posits that females gain fitnessadvantages when they are able tomate withmales from their natal regions, inpreference to males from other regions. According to this hypothesis, birdsthat select mates from their natal regions will gain fitness advantages becausetheir offspring will more likely express adaptations to local ecological condi-tions. Because song is a key mating signal in many species of birds, songstructure is thus posited to diverge by locality, under selection for accuratelymarking birds’ natal localities. Baker and colleagues arguedmore specificallythat dialects serve as markers for ‘‘coadapted gene complexes,’’ and thatdialect boundaries represent secondary contact zones between partially

Page 9: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 411

isolated populations (Baker, 1982; Baker and Thompson, 1985; Baker et al.,1982). The local adaptation hypothesis makes four predictions, each of whichhas been subject to considerable scrutiny.

The first prediction is that birds should learn their vocalizations early,before dispersing from their natal regions (MacDougall‐Shackleton andMacDougall‐Shackleton, 2001; Payne, 1981; Rothstein and Fleischer,1987). Decades of study have now shown wide diversity in the timing ofsong acquisition, and so this predictionmay hold for some species but clearlydoes not hold for others. Brown‐headed cowbirds, for instance, learn songspostdispersal, and thus dialects in this species can best be attributed to otherhypotheses (Rothstein and Fleischer, 1987). The timing of learning hastraditionally been studied in laboratory conditions, and a general point ofdiscussion concerns the applicability of laboratory results to field contexts.In particular, it has been noted that species that only learn early in thelaboratory, from taped tutor songs, may still retain the ability to learnsongs later in life, if trained by live tutors (Baptista and Petrinovich, 1984).The relative impact of social influences on song learning continues as an areaof active study (Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005; Johannessen et al., 2006).

Second, the local adaptation hypothesis predicts that dispersing birds willtend to settle, more than would be expected by chance, in localities wherebirds sing their natal dialects, as opposed to localities in which birds sing‘‘foreign’’ dialects. Tests of this prediction require mark/recapture studies,and results so far have been inconclusive (Baker and Mewaldt, 1978; BakerandMewaldt, 1981; Baker et al., 1985; Hafner and Peterson, 1985; Petrinovichet al., 1981). A call by Kroodsma (1985a) for additional empirical work in thisarea still rings true.

Third, the local adaptation hypothesis predicts that dialect groups shouldcome to be genetically differentiated, as a result of recent histories ofassortative mating. The main challenge in tests of this prediction has beento identify genetic parameters in which dialect neighborhoods differ. Againthe evidence has been inconclusive and open to interpretation (Baker et al.,1982; Lougheed and Handford, 1992; Payne and Westneat, 1988; Zink andBarrowclough, 1984). In the most comprehensive study on this topic todate, MacDougall‐Shackleton and MacDougall‐Shackleton (2001) ana-lyzed variation in microsatellite allele frequencies among eight dialectregions of Z. leucophyrs oriantha, and concluded that ‘‘dialect borders areassociated with some reduction in gene flow, but they appear to be very lowwalls rather than barriers in any strict sense’’ (MacDougall‐Shackleton andMacDougall‐Shackleton, 2001, p. 2574).

The fourth prediction of the local adaptation hypothesis is that femalesshould evolve mating preferences for males from their natal dialects.A number of assays have been developed to measure the impact of vocal

Page 10: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

412 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

variation on female preferences (Searcy, 1992). In tests of the influence ofdialect variation on female preferences, researchers have generally turnedto the copulation solicitation display assay. In this assay, females are cap-tured, acclimated to laboratory conditions, and then treated with exoge-nous estradiol, normally via silastic implants, to increase sexual receptivity.Songs of different dialects are then presented over loudspeakers, and thestrength and vigor of resulting solicitation displays documented. Females ofa number of species have been found to respond more often and morevigorously to local songs than to foreign songs (Baker et al., 1981, 1987a;Searcy et al., 1997, 2002). Differential preferences for local songs presum-ably emerge because of greater familiarity with local dialects (Baker et al.,1981), or through learned associations between local songs and socialfeedback experienced early in life (Riebel, 2003). Learned preferences formales that sing local songs could foster, through coevolution of dialect andpreference, the divergence of dialect forms (Riebel, 2003).

It is useful to note that of these four predictions, only the second is uniqueto the local adaptation hypothesis. The others predictions are also consistentwith other hypotheses of dialect evolution (see below).

Early studies of vocal geographic variation in birds focused on the localadaptation hypothesis (Marler and Tamura, 1962; Nottebohm, 1969), whichis perhaps not surprising given that birdsong studies of that era weregenerally geared toward questions about species recognition. Many birdsproduce species‐specific vocalizations, and ornithologists have longhypothesized that vocalizations thus aid conspecific recognition (Marler,1957, 1960). With the advent of portable tape recorders and loudspeakers,this hypothesis was tested and supported in a host of playback studies, whichshowed time and again that birds respond more strongly to playback ofconspecific than heterospecific song (Falls, 1963; Gill and Murray, 1972;Martin andMartin, 2001; Milligan, 1966; reviewed by Becker, 1982). Similarpatterns of elevated responses to playback of conspecific versus heterospe-cific song have also been demonstrated in females from numerous species(Searcy, 1992). From an evolutionary perspective, interspecific divergencein vocal structure is thought to be driven by two related factors, namelyselection for avoiding interspecific acoustic competition (Nelson andMarler, 1990) and selection against cross‐species mating and hybrid produc-tion (Butlin, 1995; de Kort and ten Cate, 2001; Haavie et al., 2004; Seddon,2005 ; see Sætre et al., 1997, for a paral lel argumen t for the evo lution ofplumage divergence). According to these hypotheses, individuals within apopulation that best transmit the correct species identity, through produc-tion of the most species‐typical songs, experience lower probabilities offitness‐reducing interspecific hybridization (Noor, 1999). It was a smalllogical step from the species recognition hypothesis to the local adaptation

Page 11: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 413

hypothesis. Both hypotheses focus on the same evolutionary factors, notablyselection against mating ‘‘errors.’’ The only substantive difference betweenthe hypotheses is thatmating errors occur at inter‐ versus intraspecific scales.As with tests of conspecific recognition, tests of local recognition abilitieshave relied heavily on playback designs (Baker et al., 1987a; Harris andLemon, 1974; Lemon, 1967; McGregor, 1983; Milligan and Verner, 1971;Petrinovich and Patterson, 1981; Ratcliffe and Grant, 1985; Searcy et al.,1997, 2002).

2. Social Adaptation Hypotheses

A second class of hypotheses for dialect evolution also focuses on therole of song in recognition, but with regard to social groups rather thanlocality. These ‘‘social adaptation hypotheses’’ suggest that males gainfitness advantages by singing songs similar to those of other males in theirregion, whereas males that sing nonlocal songs are subject to social penal-ties. One version of this hypothesis, the deceptive mimicry hypothesis(Payne, 1981), posits that subordinate males that successfully mimic thevocalizations of dominant males are able to improve their access to mates,and also to reduce probabilities of aggressive interactions with dominantmales. Under this scenario, dialects should be temporally unstable anddepend on which individuals are dominant at a given time (Payne, 1981;Rothstein and Fleischer, 1987). A related hypothesis is that of ‘‘honestconvergence,’’ which posits that dialects serve as honest signals of long‐term residence (Rothstein and Fleischer, 1987). Similarly, the colony pass-word hypothesis (Feekes, 1977) proposes that dialects can serve as markersof group membership in colonial species, and thus facilitate the identifica-tion of intruders into a colony.

Social adaptation hypotheses predict that individuals will learn newvocalizations on dispersal to a new dialect area, in order to match (mimic)vocalizations at the new locality (Payne, 1981). If individuals are con-strained to acquire and crystallize new vocalizations early, prior to juveniledispersal, then the social dynamics of adults cannot play a role in themaintenance of dialects. Postdispersal learning has been implicated in anumber of species, including the cowbird systems mentioned earlier(Rothstein and Fleischer, 1987; Warren, 2002).

Not surprisingly, male–male interactions have been a primary focus intests of social adaptation hypotheses. The deceptive mimicry hypothesispredicts that accurate mimics should incite less aggressive responses fromdominant males, and moreover that males should be more aggressivetoward other males from neighboring dialects as opposed to from theirhome dialects (Rothstein and Fleischer, 1987). The colony passwordhypothesis similarly predicts that members of a colony should be more

Page 12: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

414 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

aggressive toward males from foreign rather than local dialects, becauseforeign dialects would indicate potential intruders from other colonies(Feekes, 1977).

Dialect size is also an important component of social adaptation hypoth-eses. The colony password and deceptive mimicry hypotheses predict thatdialect areas should be small, corresponding to ‘‘socially cohesive units’’(Payne, 1981). The honest convergence hypothesis does not require thatdialects be small, but does, however, require them to be not substantiallylarger than average adult dispersal distance (Warren, 2002). If a dialect areais too large, dialect identity can no longer serve as an honest signal of localresidence because newly arrived males would produce vocal signals indis-tinguishable from those of local residents (Warren, 2002). In general, it isdifficult to conceive of a scenario in which social adaptation maintainsboundaries between larger regional dialects; if dialects were too large,most individuals within a dialect would be unlikely to ever encounterother dialects.

3. Epiphenomenon Hypothesis

The epiphenomenon hypothesis, first articulated (although only briefly)by Andrew (1962), posits that dialects emerge as incidental by‐products ofparticular patterns of learning and dispersal, both of which evolve underselection pressures unrelated to dialect formation. In other words, selectionon dialect patterns per se need not produce dialects or maintain the discreteboundaries that characterize dialects. This has been considered by someto be the null hypothesis for dialect evolution, and it indeed makesfewer assum ptions than do the other classes of hypothese s ( Fig. 2A vs B;Lemon, 1975; Wiens, 1982). As we understand it, the epiphenomenonhypothesis differs from functional hypotheses of dialect evolution in thatit does not require a history of continuous contact between birds fromdifferent dialect groups or a history of negative selection against birdsthat sing foreign songs. Continuous contact is necessary in the functionalhypotheses, for example as intruders that sing foreign songs are rejected, inorder to generate the negative selection pressures implied therein.

Most simply, nonfunctional divergence among dialect groups can resultfrom differential trajectories of selection among isolated populations.To illustrate, numerous lines of evidence now suggest that songs undergoselection for optimal transmission through the acoustic environment(Nottebohm, 1969, 1985;Wiley andRichards, 1978; reviewedbySlabbekoorn,2004). Such selection pressures may drive intraspecific vocal divergence, andthus the emergence of dialect patterns, when different populations come tooccupy distinct habitats (Doutrelant et al., 1999; Handford and Lougheed,1991; Hunter and Krebs, 1979; Patten et al., 2004; Slabbekoorn and

Page 13: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 415

Smith, 2002b). Limited dispersal, and thus limited contact among localities,may facilitate the nonfunctional evolution of dialects, as vocal noveltiespersevere only in limited regions (Lemon, 1975; Slater, 1989). (A note onterminology: vocal divergence through acoustic adaptation to differenthabitats itself clearly has functional bases. However, any resulting geo-graphic patterns would be considered nonfunctional, given that the locusof acoustic adaptation was site specific and thus did not favor geographicdiversification per se).

Slater (1985, 1986, 1989) offered a compelling defense of the epiphenom-en on hypo thesis for dialect evolution. As noted by Slat er (1986 , p. 96), ‘‘It ispossible that dialects have no functional significance, but that the differ-ences they represent are simply spurious byproducts of vocal learning.If song is learnt and dispersal after learning is restricted, some sort ofvariation in both time and space seems inevitable. . .’’ Slater defened theepiphenomenon hypothesis by raising three points. First, song learning mayevolve for a range of functions besides dialect recognition. Such functionsmay include helping birds to better match the local acoustic environment orto enable accurate transmission of complex songs (Slater, 1986). Second,selection on bird vocalizations generally acts at the level of individuals,whereas dialects are population‐level phenomena. Third, variations indispersal patterns and the accuracy of song learning, as studied in computersimulations, illustrate that the epiphenomenon mechanism can indeed gen-erate discrete dialect patterns, particularly in species with small repertoiresizes (Goodfellow and Slater, 1986; Lachlan et al., 2004; Slater, 1989).

We regard the epiphenomenon hypothesis as a type of ‘‘by‐product’’hypothesis of vocal evolution. By‐product mechanisms of interpopulationdivergence refer generally to situations in which selection on one trait orsuite of traits drives incidental changes in, or limits on the expression of,other traits or suites of traits. Correlated evolution of multiple traits mayarise through shared genetic or phenotypic mechanisms, such as whenmultiple functions make use of the same anatomical or physiological com-ponents (Patek et al., 2006; Podos and Hendry, 2006). Correlated evolutionof multiple traits may also arise as result of life history constraints, asmetabolic or energetic resources allocated to certain traits limit the develop-ment or expression of other traits (Roff, 1992). By‐product mechanismshave been of particular interest in studies of ‘‘ecological speciation,’’ whichposits that incidentally modified trait(s) can impact patterns of mating andreproductive isolation (Boughman, 2001; Dobzhansky, 1951; Mayr, 1942;Orr and Smith, 1998; Podos, 2001; Podos and Hendry, 2006; Rice andHostert, 1993; Ruegg et al., 2006; Schluter, 2000, 2001). Rundle et al.(2000), for instance, provided evidence that adaptive divergence of bodysize in benthic versus limnetic stickleback fishes has facilitated assortative

Page 14: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

416 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

mating by morph, presumably because body size is used as a cue in mateassessment and selection. According to by‐product models of trait diver-gence, the emergence of reproductive isolation among populations is notnecessarily a product of selection for that isolation, but potentially a sec-ondary consequence of ecological adaptation in other traits (Podos andHendry, 2006).

III. ASSESSING HYPOTHESES OF DIALECT EVOLUTION

The potential relevance of the three hypothesis classes outlined abovehas now been addressed in a moderate number of species. Our goal in thissection is to survey the primary literature, in order to identify potentiallycommon conditions that underlie dialect formation and maintenance. Ofparticular interest is the timing of song acquisition, given that a key predic-tion distinguishing the two functional hypotheses is whether acquisitionoccurs pre‐ or postdispersal. Other potential correlates of dialect patternsexamined here include social systems, degree of seasonal mobility, andvocal repertoire size. Examination of these parameters cannot providedefinitive support for or against the three classes of dialect hypotheses,especially given difficulties noted by many other researchers in comparingdialect data across species. Rather, our intent is to gain a sense of trends orconditions that may favor dialect evolution as a general phenomenon.

A. OUR APPROACH

We surveyed the literature for species reported to exhibit dialects(Table I). Our survey included not just birds but also primates, cetaceans,anurans, and insects. To meet our definition of dialects, we required evi-dence of sharp geographic boundaries between signal forms, of discretedifferences among signal forms, and of uniformity of signal forms withingiven localities (Fig. 3C). Thus, a species exhibiting geographic variation ina continuous character such as pulse rate or dominant frequency was notincluded unless this variation was shown to be partitioned discretely amongsites (e.g., as shown in Leader et al., 2000). We excluded studies for whichdata were ambiguous about one or more of the above criteria (Naguib et al.,2001; Podos, 2007). Moreover, a species exhibiting many discretely differ-ent signal forms was not included unless these signal types were geographi-cally partitioned such that narrow boundaries between types could beclearly mapped. We thus excluded from our analysis ‘‘island’’ dialects(Ratcliffe, 1981), in which dialect localities are geographically isolated

Page 15: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

TABLE I

SPECIES REPORTED TO EXHIBIT VOCAL DIALECTS, INCLUDED IN OUR ANALYSES

# Species Common name Family Signal References

1 Amazona auropalliata Yellow‐naped Amazon Psittacidae group calls Wright, 1996; Wright and Wilkinson, 2001;

Wright et al., 2005

2 Phaethornis

longuemareus

Little Hermit Trochilidae song Wiley, 1971

3 Miliaria calandra Corn bunting Fringillidae song Holland et al., 1996; McGregor, 1980, 1983;

McGregor and Krebs, 1984; McGregor

and Thompson, 1988; McGregor et al.,

1988

4 Emberiza citrinella Yellowhammer Fringillidae song Baker et al., 1987a; Møller, 1982; Hansen,

1985, 1999; Glaubrecht, 1989, 1991;

Rutkowska‐Guz and Osiejuk, 2004

5 Emberiza hortulana Ortolan Fringillidae song Conrads, 1976; Conrads and Conrads, 1971;

Thielcke, 1969

6 Zonotrichia leucrophrys

nuttalli

Nuttall’s white‐crownedSparrow

Fringillidae song Baker, 1974, 1975, 1983; Baker and

Mewaldt, 1978; Baker and Thompson,

1985; Baker et al., 1981, 1982, 1984a,b,

1987b; Baptista, 1975; Baptista et al.,

1997; Cunningham et al., 1987; Marler

and Tamura, 1962, 1964; Milligan and

Verner, 1971; Petrinovich and Patterson,

1981; Trainer, 1983; Zink and

Barrowclough, 1984

7 Zonotrichia leucophrys

oriantha

Montane White‐crownedSparrow

Fringillidae song Baptista, 1977; Chilton and Lein, 1996a,b;

Nelson, 2000; Nelson et al., 1996b, 2004;

Soha et al., 2004

(Continued)

417

Page 16: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

TABLE I (Continued)

# Species Common name Family Signal References

8 Zonotrichia leucophrys

pugetensis

Puget Sound White‐crownedSparrow

Fringillidae song Baptista and King, 1980; Baptista and

Morton, 1982, 1988; Harbison et al., 1999;

MacDougall‐Shackleton and

MacDougall‐Shackleton, 2001;MacDougall‐Shackleton et al., 2002;

Nelson et al., 1996a

9 Zonotrichia capensis Rufous‐crownedSparrow

Fringillidae song King, 1972; Lougheed and Handford, 1992;

Nottebohm, 1969, 1975; Tubaro and

Segura, 1994; Zink et al., 1991

10 Pooecetes gramineus Vesper Sparrow Fringillidae song Kroodsma, 1972

11 Amphispiza belli Sage Sparrow Fringillidae song Rich, 1981

12 Pipilo maculatus Spotted Towhees Fringillidae song Borror, 1975

13 Passerina cyanea Indigo Bunting Fringillidae song Payne, 1981, 1982, 1983; Payne and

Westneat, 1988

14 Cacicus cela Yellow‐rumped Cacique

(Surinam)

Fringillidae song Feekes, 1977

15 Cacicus cela Yellow‐rumped Cacique

(Panama)

Fringillidae song Trainer, 1988, 1989; Trainer and Parsons,

2002

16 Molothrus ater Brown‐headed Cowbird Fringillidae song Alderson et al., 1999; Dolbeer, 1982;

Fleischer and Rothstein, 1988;

O’Loghlen, 1995; O’Loghlen and

Rothstein, 1993, 1995; Rothstein and

Fleischer, 1987; Rothstein et al., 1999;

Teather and Robertson, 1986;

Yokel, 1986

17 Molothrus aeneus Bronzed Cowbird (Winter) Fringillidae song Dolbeer, 1982; Rothstein, 1980; Warren,

2000, 2003

418

Page 17: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

18 Molothrus aeneus Bronzed Cowbird

(Breeding)

Fringillidae song Carter, 1984; Clotfelter, 1995; Dolbeer,

1982; Rothstein, 1980; Warren, 2000,

2002, 2003

19 Dolichonyx oryzivorus Bobolink Fringillidae song Avery and Oring, 1977; Trainer and

Peltz, 1995

20 Fringilla coelebs Chaffinch Fringillidae call (‘‘rain

call’’)

Baptista, 1990; Sick, 1939; Sorjonen, 2001;

Thielcke, 1969, 1988a,b, 1989

21 Chloris chloris Greenfinch Fringillidae song Guttinger, 1974, 1976

22 Carpodacus mexicanus House Finch Fringillidae song Mundinger, 1975, 1982

23 Calcarius pictus Smith’s longspur Fringillidae song Briskie, 1999

24 Himatione sanguinea Apapane Fringillidae song Ward, 1964

25 Thryomanes bewickii Bewicks Wren Certhiidae song Kroodsma, 1974, 1985b

26 Troglodytes troglodytes European Wren (UK) Certhiidae song Catchpole and Rowell, 1993

27 Troglodytes troglodytes European Wren (France) Certhiidae song Kreutzer, 1974

28 Certhia brachydactyla Short‐toed Treecreeper Certhiidae song Seitz et al., 1994; Thielcke, 1969, 1984, 1986,

1987; Thielcke and Wuestenberg, 1985

29 Turdus iliacus Redwing Passeridae song Bjerke, 1974, 1980, 1982, 1984; Bjerke and

Bjerke, 1981; Espmark, 1981, 1982;

Fonstad et al., 1984; Mork, 1974

30 Vidua chalybeata Village Indigobird Passeridae song Payne, 1973, 1981, 1987; Payne and Payne,

1977

31 Vidua purpurascens Dusky Indigobird Passeridae song Payne, 1973, 1981, 1987

32 Poecile atricapillus Black‐capped Chickadee Paridae call

(‘‘gargle’’)

Ficken et al., 1978, 1985

33 Poecile atricapillus Black‐capped Chickadee Paridae song (‘‘fee

bee’’)

Kroodsma et al., 1999

34 Poecile carolinensis Carolina Chickadee Paridae song (‘‘fee

bee’’)

Ward, 1966

(Continued)

419

Page 18: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

TABLE I (Continued)

# Species Common name Family Signal References

35 Pachycephala olivacea Olive Whistler Corvidae song White, 1985, 1986, 1987

36 Cyanocitta cristata Blue Jay Corvidae group calls

(‘‘bell’’

call)

Kramer and Thompson, 1979

37 Creadion carunculatus Saddleback Callaeatidae song Jenkins, 1978

38 Menura

novaehollandiae

Superb Lyrebird Menuridae song Powys, 1995; Robinson and Curtis, 1996

39 Nectarinia osea Orange‐tufted Sunbirds Nectariniidae song Leader et al., 2000, 2002, 2005

40 Orcinus orca Orca, Killer Whale Cetacea group calls Ford, 1991

41 Pyseter macrocephalus Sperm Whale Certacea group calls Weilgart and Whitehead, 1997

42 Saguinus labiatus

labiatus

Red‐chested Moustached

Tamarin

Callithricidae long calls Masataka, 1988

420

Page 19: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 421

from each other. This is not to say that vocal vari ation in speci es distri butedacross islands cannot be consid ered dial ectal; our criteria here were set forpur poses of analys is only.

B. LITE RATURE S URVEY

Our searc h for published studies of dialect s was con ducted using bothdigi tal da tabases (ISI Web of Scienc e) and search es of citati on sect ions ofpubl ished works on dial ects. We ident ified abou t 200 studi es of 52 species inwhi ch the authors suggest ed that the vocal system could be descri bed asdial ectal. Of these, 42 cases met our present definition of diale cts ( Fig. 3C).We include data from 141 studies of these 42 cases in our analysis ( Table I ).We compile d data for them ( Table II ), as follow s.

1. Dialect Chara cteristics

We coded three descripti ve charac terist ics of dialects: spatial extent(dial ect scale), temporal stabi lity of dialect bounda ries , and presenc e orab sence of bilingual ism at bounda ries be tween dialect s. Di alect scale wascod ed according to four categ ories: microgeo graphic, small , medi um, andlarg e, coded, respec tively, as 0, 1, 2, or 3. Microgeogr aph ic dialect s feature10 indi viduals or less in each dialect area, an d span less than 2 km in anygiven direction . Small dial ects con sisted of dial ects areas that spa n 2–10 km.Med ium dialects were consi dered to span 10–100 km, and larg e dial ectsgreat er than 100 km. In some cases , dialect s were only described in terms ofnum bers of individual s. Smal l, medi um, and large dialects were then classi-fie d as containing less than 100 indi viduals, less than 1000 indi viduals, andmore than 1000 indi viduals, respec tively. Most dial ect areas do not greatlyexceed 1000 km.

The temp oral stabi lity of dialect bounda ries was described explicitl y bymany authors . Some taxa retain similar bounda ries and acousticcharac terist ics of son g types over long pe riods of time ( Hansen , 1999;Thielcke, 1987) while others change rapidly, even within a season(Trainer, 1989). We classified dialect stability as short‐term, moderate‐term, and long‐term, coded as 1, 2, or 3, respectively. Short‐term dialectsmaintained 2–6 years of stability. In the case of most passerine species,this probably corresponds to the life span of individuals of the species(Weat herhead and Forbes, 1994). Mod erate ‐ term dialect s were defined asbeing stable for 6–20 years, and long‐term dialects as being stable for greaterthan 20 years. It is probable that some taxa coded with moderate‐termdialects actually are stable over the long‐term, and that long‐term stabilityhas not yet been demonstrated.

Page 20: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

TABLE II

VOCAL AND ECOLOGICAL PARAMETERS OF DIALECT S PECIES a

# Species

Spatial

scale

Temporal

stability Bilingual Territoriality Mobility

Repertoire

size max

Timing of

learning

1 Amazona auropalliata 2 1 0 0 1 post

2 Phaethornis longuemareus 0 2 1 0 1

3 Miliaria calandra 1 2 1 1 1 3 post

4 Emberiza citrinella 3 3 1 1 1 5

5 Emberiza hortulana 2 1 1 1 1 2

6 Zonotrichia leucrophrys nuttalli 1 3 0 1 0 1 pre

7 Zonotrichia leucophrys oriantha 3 3 1 1 1 2 pre

8 Zonotrichia leucophrys pugetensis 3 2 1 1 1 2 pre

9 Zonotrichia capensis 2 3 1 1 1 1 pre

10 Pooecetes gramineus 1 1 1 43

11 Amphispiza belli 1 0 1 1 1

12 Pipilo maculatus 1 1 1 2

13 Passerina cyanea 0 1 1 1 1 post

14 Cacicus cela 2 0 0 1 4 post

15 Cacicus cela 2 0 0 0 1 8 post

16 Molothrus ater 2 2 1 1 1 3 post

17 Molothrus aeneus 2 1 1 0 1 1

18 Molothrus aeneus 3 1 1 1 1 1

19 Dolichonyx oryzivorus 1 1 0 1 43 post

20 Fringilla coelebs 2 3 1 1 1 1

21 Chloris chloris 2 0 0 1 0 35 post

422

Page 21: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

22 Carpodacus mexicanus 1 1 1 0 1 4 pre

23 Calcarius pictus 0 2 0 1 1 post

24 Himatione sanguinea 1 1 0 1

25 Thryomanes bewickii 1 1 1 0 16 post

26 Troglodytes troglodytes 0 0 1 0 6

27 Troglodytes troglodytes 1 1 1 1 0 1 pre

28 Certhia brachydactyla 3 3 1 1 5 pre

29 Turdus iliacus 1 2 1 1 1 2 post

30 Vidua chalybeata 1 0 1 1 0 12 post

31 Vidua purpurascens 1 0 1 1 0 12 post

32 Poecile atricapillus 1 1 1 1

33 Poecile atricapillus 1 0 1 0 9 post

34 Poecile carolinensis 2 1 0

35 Pachycephala olivacea 2 0 1 0 10

36 Cyanocitta cristata 2 1 0 2

37 Creadion carunculatus 0 1 1 1 0 1 post

38 Menura novaehollandiae 1 2 1 0 3

39 Nectarinia osea 0 1 1 1 0 1

40 Orcinus orca 2 3 0 0 0 17 pre

41 Pyseter macrocephalus 1 1 0 0 1 30 pre

42 Saguinus labiatus labiatus 2 1 0 1 post

aSee text for an explanation of coding; blank cells indicate a lack of sufficient available data.

423

Page 22: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

424 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

Bilingualism refers to the propensity of individuals to acquire more thanone dialect, particularly in areas near a dialect boundary. Following ourdefinition of dialects, the proportion of a population that is bilingual shouldbe low in most cases. But bilingualism was reported regularly among thespecies we reviewed. We classified bilingualism as either absent or present(coded as 0 or 1, Table II) based on the authors’ reporting. We classifiedbilingualism as absent only when an author either stated it to be so or gavesufficiently exhaustive detail on vocal behavior of the focal populations toallow us to make this inference.

2. Ecological Correlates

We compiled data for two ecological and life history characteristics com-monly cited in the literature: social system and degree of seasonal mobility.First, we assessed, based on the literature, whether taxa with dialects werepredominantly territorial or, alternatively, social (e.g., colonial) on theirbreeding grounds. With regard to seasonal mobility, taxa were classified aseither sedentary or migratory, again on the basis of published descriptions.

3. Vocal Correlates

Data on one vocal parameter, repertoire size, was compiled for eachspecies or subspecies in our analysis. Rather than analyzing the ranges ofrepertoire sizes within each species, we included in our analyses onlymaximum repertoire size. We focus on maximum repertoire size becausethis provides a good indication of the potential for individuals to acquiresongs of more than one dialect. Coding each cell with a single number aidedour multivariate analyses below.

4. Timing of Song Acquisition

While the song acquisition phase of song learning has not been charac-terized in detail for many species, it can sometimes be inferred fromobservational studies of song acquisition in the field. This is particularlyso in demonstrations of postdispersal learning, in which individuals areshown to match their songs to the local dialect after dispersing from theirnatal grounds. Thus, we classified any taxa in which individuals regularlyalter their songs after dispersal as a ‘‘postdispersal learner’’ for the purposesof this analysis. This includes species that delete song types learned earlierthat do not match neighbors. We classified taxa as predispersal learnerswhen the predominant pattern appears to be early song acquisition, with noknown examples of modification of song after natal dispersal. These cate-gories parallel the open‐ended and close‐ended song learning categories as

Page 23: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 425

defined by Beecher and Brenowitz (2005) and others. Notably, songlearning in some species likely bridges pre‐ and postdispersal periods.Z. leucophrys oriantha and Z. leucophrys pugetensis, for instance, are be-lieved to memorize song models predispersal but then crystallize only asubset of memorized models postdispersal, based on vocal interactions withneighbors (Nelson, 2000). For such species, we coded song learning as pri-marily predispersal, based on the supposed period of song model acquisition.

One notable shortcoming in our analysis is that we did not includedispersal distances, in spite of the fact that these are considered a key factorin shaping vocal geographic variation. Unfortunately, dispersal is difficult tomeasure and therefore not available in the literature for many species.Moreover, our system of classifying taxa as pre‐ versus postdispersal lear-ners incorporates dispersal distance, at least to some extent. Taxa identifiedas having postdispersal learning have most likely been so identified becauseindividuals have been observed dispersing across dialect boundaries. Thus,these taxa will have longer dispersal distances than will predispersal lear-ners, almost by definition.

5. Analytical Approaches

While our survey included a wide range of taxa, we decided to limit ourquantitative analysis to birds, which constituted the large majority of vocaldialect cases identified (Table I, taxa #1–39). We first tested whether dialectcharacteristics in birds differed systematically between pre‐ and postdispersallearners. The lack of information on vocal ontogeny for many species greatlylimited our sample size, and therefore the number of parameters we couldinclude in a multivariate analysis. Because dialect characteristics such asspatial scale are potentially linked to ecological variables such as territoriality,we conducted two separate discriminant function analyses with song ontoge-ny as the grouping variable in both cases. In the first, we asked whether ourthree ecological and vocal parameters classified pre‐ and postdispersal lear-ners as distinct groups. In the second, we asked whether the three dialectparameters classified pre‐ and postdispersal learners as distinct groups.

We explored potential correlates of dialect variation using nonparametricmethods, comparing, using univariate analyses, each of the dialect para-meters to the ecological and vocal parameters shown in Table II. It wouldhave been preferable to apply the comparative method, that is, takingphylogenetic relationships into account in our analysis. This was not practi-cal, however, given the limited information currently available about therelationships of the taxa examined. It seems unlikely that phylogeneticfactors would have strongly influenced our findings, given that dialectcharacteristics such as spatial scale can vary widely even among closelyrelated taxa (see, e.g., Zonotrichia species and subspecies, #6–9, Table II).

Page 24: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

426 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

C. RESULTS

The majority of research on vocal geographic variation has been con-ducted on songbirds from temperate regions. Considerably, less is knownabout vocal geographic variation in other bird groups in other regions,notably tropical suboscine passerines. Thus, the sample analyzed heredoes not represent an unbiased snapshot of natural vocal variation. Ourfirst impression from our survey is that the absolute number of speciesexhibiting sharp dialects is lower than the abundant literature on dialectsmight lead one to suspect. Most entries are for dialects in song (n¼ 36), but5 are for group calls. In five cases for which intraspecific variation could noteasily be summarized, we use multiple entries for a single species. One ofthese is the black‐capped chickadee, for which dialects are reported in twodifferent vocal signals, the ‘‘gargle’’ and the ‘‘fee bee’’ (Table II). In theother cases, populations or subspecies are so distinct in their reporteddialect or other characteristics as to warrant separate entries. For example,three white‐crowned sparrow subspecies (Z. leucophrys nuttalli, Z. leu-cophrys oriantha, and Z. leucophrys pugetensis, #6–7) differ both in thecharacteristics of the dialects they exhibit as well as in reported patterns ofmigratory behavior (Table II). In the bronzed cowbird (M. aeneus), dialectsoccur in distinct geographic patterns in breeding versus wintering popula-tions (Warren, 2002).

The majority of the cases of vocal dialects we identified occur in passerinebirds (n ¼ 37). These were distributed among just eight families (sensuSibley and Monroe, 1990), with most examples from four subfamilies of theFringillidae (n ¼ 18 species, Fig. 4). The Fringillids represent roughly 40%of the species that were found to exhibit some degree of song sharing,according to a survey of the Fringillids by Handley and Nelson (2005).Slater (1989) and others have argued that dialects are a rare phenomenon.According to our chapter, they certainly seem to be uncommon in passer-ines, occurring in less than a quarter of passerine families, at least as basedon available data. Yet, dialects may actually be a common form of geo-graphic variation in the Fringillids, a group with a high propensity for songsharing (45 of the 65 taxa reviewed by Handley and Nelson, 2005).

1. Variation in Dialect Characteristics

The three dialect characteristics, spatial scale, temporal stability, and bilin-gualism, were not correlated with one another (all Spearman’s r < 0.25,p > 0.2, N ¼ 27, except N ¼ 20 for temporal stability‐bilingualism compari-son). Spatial scale and temporal stability were each normally distributedacross our sample. It was more difficult to assess bilingualism than the other

Page 25: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

2018

18

16

14

12

10

8

6

4

2

0

3 32 2

1 1 1 1 1

Fringil

lidae

Certh

iidae

Passe

ridae

Corvid

aePar

idae

Callae

atida

eM

enur

idae

Necta

riniid

aePsit

tacid

aeTro

chilid

ae

Num

ber

of s

peci

es

FIG. 4. Number of species in which vocal dialects were identified, according to family, in

birds. All but two families, the Psittacidae and Trochilidae, are passerines.

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 427

two variables, with many cases lacking sufficient detail. Of the 27 cases inwhich this could be assessed with confidence, the majority, 74%, exhibitedsome level of bilingualism.

2. Ecological Correlates of Dialect Characteristics

The 42 dialect systems we found exhibit a wide variety of ecologicalcharacteristics, with representatives of both territorial and social speciesand of both sedentary and migratory species (Fig. 5, Table II). Given thedominance of passerine birds in the sample, we were not surprised to findthat a majority of the birds with dialects are territorial (79%, Fig. 5). It wasmore surprising to find that sedentary species were the minority in thesample (Fig. 5), since it is thought that dialects should evolve more readilyin sedentary species. Across our dialect sample, territorial species tended tobe sedentary more frequently than did social species (w2 ¼ 3.83, p ¼ 0.05).This relative dearth of sedentary species with dialects might be due to thedominance in our sample of temperate and arctic species, many of whichare seasonally migratory.

Page 26: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

100%

80%

60%

40%

20%

0%Territoriality Seasonal mobility

Mig

Sed

FIG. 5. Distribution of ecological correlates among species with vocal dialects. Bars indicate

the percentage of species that are territorial (black) versus nonterritorial (white) and sedentary

(‘‘sed’’) versus migratory (‘‘mig’’).

428 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

We found few significant relationships between ecological variablesand dialect variables, but the trends uncovered suggest potential mechan-isms underlying variation among dialect systems. Migratory species tend tohave larger dialect regions (Kruskal‐Wallis, Z ¼ �1.85, p ¼ 0.06), and weresomewhat more likely than sedentary species to show bilingualism atdialect boundaries (w2 ¼ 2.82, p ¼ 0.09). But we found no relationshipbetween seasonal mobility and the stability of dialect regions over time.By contrast, territorial species tend to have more temporally stable dialectsthan do social species (Kruskal‐Wallis, Z ¼ �1.9, p ¼ 0.05). But we find noeffect of territoriality on spatial scale of dialects or the propensity forbilingualism.

3. Vocal Correlates of Dialect Characteristics

We find that 32% (12 of 37) of songbird species with dialects in ourreview have repertoires of only a single song type, and that 68% (25 species)have repertoires of fewer than five song types. According to Beecher andBrenowitz (2005), the corresponding percentages for all passerines are 30%of single song type and 50% repertoires of less than five song types. The twodata sets thus correspond closely in this regard.

As with the ecological variables, there are no statistically significantcorrelations between repertoire size and dialect characteristics. However,species with smaller repertoires tend to have more stable dialects over time(Spearman’s r ¼ �0.38, p ¼ 0.05) and a lower propensity for bilingualism(Spearman’s r ¼ �0.36, p ¼ 0.12). Repertoire size is not associated withdialect scale.

Page 27: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 429

4. Song Ontogeny

Dialect species were somewhat more likely to show postdispersal ratherthan predis persal learn ing (Fig. 6). While this lends greate r suppo rt for thesocial adaptation than local adaptation hypothesis, neither is strongly sup-ported. The social adaptation hypothesis can be rejected in the nine dialectsystems in which individuals rarely disperse across dialect boundaries andapparently do not acquire the local song type when they do cross bound-aries. While some of the cases of predispersal learning have been disputed(Baptista and Petrinovich, 1984) or are based on single studies (Kreutzer,1974), many others are well established (Lachlan and Slater, 2003). Thus,each of the functional hypotheses can be rejected in at least some cases.Furthermore, there are many missing cells in the table. The timing of songacquisition and memorization relative to natal dispersal is not known formore than a third of the cases in our table.

Multivariate approaches identified few good predictors of the timing ofsong acquisition. First, we conducted a discriminant function analysis usingthe ecological and vocal parameters, territoriality, seasonal mobility andrepertoire size, and song ontogeny as the grouping variable. This discrimi-nant function was not significant (Exact F ¼ 1.20, df ¼ 3, N ¼ 18, p ¼ 0.33),with a canonical correlation of 0.45 for the first canonical axis. We con-ducted a second discriminant function analysis using three factors specifi-cally describing the dialect systems, the spatial scale of dialects, their

20

18

16

14

12

1010

16 16

8

6

4

2

0Predispersal Postdispersal Unknown

Num

ber

of ta

xa

FIG. 6. Numbers of all taxa with dialects that exhibit either predispersal learning or post-

dispersal learning, or for whom timing of learning has not been described. The two functional

hypotheses, local adaptation and social adaptation, make mutually exclusive predictions

regarding timing of song memorization and acquisition.

Page 28: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

430 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

temporal stability, and presence of bilingualism. This was also not signifi-cant (Exact F ¼ 1.89, df ¼ 3, N ¼ 10, p ¼ 0.20) with a canonical correlationof 0.60 for the first canonical axis.

Eliminating bilingualism from the analysis, a variable with many missingdata points, improves the classification considerably, as shown in Fig. 7.This classification is statistically significant (Exact F ¼ 6.01, df ¼ 3, N ¼ 15,p ¼ 0.01), with the first canonical axis (Canon1) accounting for 67% of thevariation among the cases of dialects. Temporal stability shows the highestcorrelation with Canon1 (r ¼ 0.88, p < 0.0001), but dialect size is alsostrongly correlated with Canon1 (r ¼ 0.59, p ¼ 0.002). Thus, the temporaland spatial features of dialects appear to be moderately successful predic-tors of learning ‘‘strategy’’ (sensu Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005).

Temporal stability of dialects remained a significant predictor of learningstrategy in univariate tests. This and repertoire size were the only para-meters to show a tendency to differ between pre‐ and postdispersal lear-ners. Postdispersal learners were slightly more likely to have largermaximum repertoire sizes than predispersal learners (Kruskal‐Wallis,

3.5

3.0

2.5

2.0

1.5

1.0

0.5

−0.51.0 1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0

Canonical 1

Can

onic

al 2

3.5 4.0 4.5 5.0 5.5

0.0

14, 15, 21

5

18

8

4, 7, 28

9

16

3, 29, 38

6

2, 23

37, 39

19, 22,24, 27

30, 31

Pos

t

Pre

Spatial scale

+ +

Temporal stability

FIG. 7. Results of discriminant function analysis, using the spatial scale and temporal

stability of dialect regions as independent variables and the timing of song acquisition as a

grouping variable. Circles indicate the 50% confidence interval for predispersal (dark circle)

and postdispersal learners (light circle). All bird species are plotted and numbered according to

their entries in Table II.

Page 29: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 431

Z ¼ �1.54, p ¼ 0.12). This finding is supported by the observation that allseven predispersal cases have repertoires with five or fewer song types.Dialects in predispersal learners are also significantly more stable over time(Kruskal‐Wallis, Z ¼ 2.43, p ¼ 0.015), though there is clearly considerablevariation in both groups. Some dialect systems in postdispersal learners arequite stable over time, for example in brown‐headed cowbirds (M. ater)(Fleischer and Rothstein, 1988). Dialects in predispersal learners thus occurin species with smaller repertoires, and when they occur, they tend to berelatively stable in time. By contrast, dialects in postdispersal learners tendto cover smaller regions and be less stable over time, though there isconsiderable variation among dialects exhibited by postdispersal learners.

D. DISCUSSION

Our survey illustrates both the rarity and diversity of dialect systems innature. No single functional hypothesis can account for all or even amajority of published examples of dialects, and a substantial portion ofthe cases reject at least one of these hypotheses. The local adaptationhypothesis is rejected for 16 cases of dialects in which individuals arecapable of modifying songs after dispersal to new dialect regions (Fig. 6).Likewise, the social adaptation hypothesis is rejected for 10 cases in whichindividuals appear not to disperse across dialect boundaries or to modifytheir signals after natal dispersal (Fig. 6). Although timing of song acquisi-tion remains unknown for more than a third of all dialect systems (Table II),it seems unlikely that further work will support one of these functionalhypotheses absolutely over the other. We suggest that the diversity ofdialect systems, occurring as it does across a range of social systems, scales,and ecological conditions, argues against any given functional hypothesis ofdialect evolution.

The assembled data provide additional insights into the selective forcesunderlying the evolution of these diverse patterns of dialect variation.First, we note some common features across the species in our chapter.Although we searched intensively for cases of dialects in such well‐studiedacoustic performers as insects and anurans, we only found dialects inspecies with evidence of vocal learning. In birds, these were the passer-ines, psitticines, and trochilids. Among mammals, the cetaceans and pri-mates were the only groups represented. The majority of the cases (37 of42) were in passerine birds. The predominance of passerines in the sampleno doubt reflects, to some extent, historical and taxonomic biases inresearch on geographic variation in acoustic signaling. However, itremains noteworthy that no examples of dialects to date have been

Page 30: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

432 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

found in species lacking imitative learning. We conclude that imitativelearning, as predicted by Kroodsma (1996) among others, is a necessarycondition for dialect formation.

Our survey also reveals a strong incidence of dialects within the Fringillids,including species that share songs with neighbors (compare our data withHandley and Nelson, 2005). At the surface, this relationship suggests poten-tial support for social adaptation hypotheses such as the deceptive mimicryhypothesis. However, as we argue in Section IV, and as was argued by Slater(1989), evidence of relationships between song sharing and vocal dialectevolution provides more direct support for by‐product hypotheses of dialectevolution.

1. Ecological and Vocal Correlates of Dialect Parameters

Our comparisons of vocal and ecological parameters in dialect speciessuggest several issues worth pursuing in future work. First, the spatialcharacteristics of dialects appeared to be associated with seasonal mobility,with migratory species tending to evolve larger dialect regions. Theseinterspecific comparisons appear to corroborate patterns described for thethree subspecies of the white‐crowned sparrow with dialects, which rangefrom the sedentaryZ. leucophrys nuttalliwith its small dialect regions to themigratory Z. leucophrys oriantha and Z. leucophrys pugetensis with theirlarger dialect region (Nelson, 1999). Further refinement of comparisons toinclude distances traveled by migratory populations may reveal an evenstronger relationship with dialect size.

Second, territorial species tend to maintain the same dialects for longerperiods of time than do social species such as the colonial yellow‐rumpedcacique (Trainer, 1989). Perhaps more revealing is the lack of a relationshipbetween migratory behavior and stability of dialects. This suggests thatseasonal mobility alone does not disrupt dialect patterns. Other life historycharacteristics that could be addressed include the degree of site fidelity andthe rate of population turnover (Handley and Nelson, 2005; Kroodsma,1996; Wiens, 1982).

Third, larger song repertoires are associated with more stable dialectregions, but not with other dialect characteristics. This may reflect under-lying selection on either repertoire size or accuracy of vocal imitation(Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005). Species in which accurate imitation isadvantageous typically have lower repertoire sizes and are expected tohave more stable song neighborhoods (Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005). Wenote, however, that predispersal learners also tend to have smaller reper-toires and more stable dialects. The significant correlation between reper-toire size and dialect stability disappears when we treat pre‐ andpostdispersal learners separately. Thus, an unresolved question is whether

Page 31: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 433

the stability of geographic variation in song is a consequence of timing ofsong acquisition, or of selection on traits such as repertoire size or imitativelearning. The timing of song acquisition itself may be a consequence ofselection on other traits such as dispersal distances or length of breedingseasons (Nelson, 1999).

2. Role of Song Ontogeny

The evolutionary consequences of geographic variation in song arethought to be determined in large part by song ontogeny, in particular bythe timing of song acquisition and memorization (Slabbekoorn and Smith,2002a). Our survey of birds with dialects found examples of species withboth pre‐ and postdispersal learning. These groups differ somewhat in thecharacteristics of their dialects, particularly in the stability of dialects overtime (Fig. 7). But these differences are not overwhelming, and lack ofinformation on song ontogeny in our sample significantly hampers ourability to draw conclusions about its role in dialect evolution. Nevertheless,it seems clear that song ontogeny is only one of many characteristics ofspecies that influence how geographic variation in song evolves.

IV. RECENT STUDIES OF AVIAN VOCAL EVOLUTION, AND HOW THEY SUPPORT

BY‐PRODUCT MODELS OF VOCAL GEOGRAPHIC DIVERGENCE

As of two decades ago, researchers had mustered little direct empiricalsupport for functional hypotheses of dialect evolution (Kroodsma, 1985a).Our survey in the preceding section suggests that little has changed on thisfront. There is and probably will never be a simple, universal explanationfor the phenomenon of dialects. Perhaps more importantly, we suggest thatthe traditional focus on dialects has eclipsed investigation into the broaderphenomenon of vocal geographic evolution, of which dialects are but oneform expressed. In this section, we argue that by‐product hypotheses ofvocal geographic evolution—the only one of the three sets of dialecthypotheses that seems to have broader applicability—have been receivingnew sources of support, through recent general advances in the study ofbirdsong.

A. PHYLOGENETIC SIGNAL IN VOCAL EVOLUTION

Over decades, the study of behavioral evolution has been transformedby increased attention to phylogenetic factors (Brooks and McLennan,1991; Martins, 1996). This has been the case for the study of birdsongs, inspite of the prior presumption that these signals are too plastic to permit

Page 32: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

434 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

historical analyses (Irwin, 1996; Payne, 1986; Price and Lanyon, 2002; tenCate, 2004). The principal approach used so far to account for historicalfactors in bird vocal evolution has involved comparative analyses(Catchpole, 1980; Irwin, 1990; Kroodsma, 1977; Podos, 1997; Read andWeary, 1992; Slabbekoorn et al., 1999; Wiley, 1991). Toward this end,reference to hypotheses of phylogenetic relationships has proven particu-larly helpful. Phylogenetic hypotheses enable researchers to estimate vocalancestral character states, as has now been done in a number of bird groups(de Kort and ten Cate, 2004; Irwin, 1988; Payne, 1986; Price and Lanyon,2002, 2004). In their studies of oropendolas and caciques, to illustrate, Priceand Lanyon (2002, 2004) used ancestral state reconstruction to infer thatsome vocal parameters (e.g., note structure, peak frequencies) are highlyvariable, whereas other vocal parameters (e.g., the presence or absence of atrill or a click within songs, and note and song duration) have remainedstable over evolutionary time. Phylogenetic hypotheses have also allowedfor independent contrast and similar statistical analyses, to assess correlatedevolution in other taxonomic groups between song traits and neural, mor-phological, or ecological parameters (Podos, 2001; Seddon, 2005; Szekelyet al., 1996; Van Buskirk, 1997). Returning to the example of oropendolasand caciques, phylogenetic reconstruction enabled analysis of correlationsbetween rates of vocal evolution and the intensity of sexual selection indifferent lineages (Price and Lanyon, 2002, 2004).

B. MULTIPLE FUNCTIONS AND TRADE‐OFFS IN VOCAL EVOLUTION

Evidence of historical signal in vocal evolution suggests alternativesto functional hypotheses of evolution, which assume that vocal traitsare sufficiently plastic to be easily molded to whatever selective pressuresare presently in play. Instead, vocal features may be evolutionarily con-served when they are subject to multiple evolutionary pressures (Beecherand Brenowitz, 2005; Gil and Gahr, 2002; Nowicki and Podos, 1993).A rigorous empirical example of how song may evolve under multipleselection pressures was provided by Seddon (2005), who documented con-current impacts of morphological adaptation, interspecific competition, andacoustic adaptation on the divergence of vocal structure in Neotropical ant-birds (Passeriformes: Thamnophilidae). When traits are subject to multipleselection pressures, they may face trade‐offs in their evolution, that is, byresponding only partially to some pressures (Roff, 1992). Evolutionaryresponses to specific functions, through their impacts on morphology, phys-iology, and behavior, may also alter how other traits are expressed, or evenprovide opportunities for the evolution of new functions (Gould and Vrba,1982; Patek et al., 2006). The relevance of these issues for questions about

Page 33: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 435

bird vocal evolution has been highlighted by recent advances in two areas:on mechanisms of vocal learning and on mechanisms of vocal production.Advances in both areas, we argue below, support by‐product models ofvocal geographic divergence.

1. Mechanisms of Vocal Learning

Researchers have long wondered why some animals have evolved imita-tive learning as a mechanism in vocal signal ontogeny (Nottebohm, 1972).After all, many other animals develop effective vocal signals withoutrecourse to imitation. Two traditional explanations for the evolution ofvocal imitation are that it enables transmission of particularly complexpatterns of vocal structure across generations, and that it helps animals toadapt their vocal signals to local acoustic environments (Slater, 1986).Recent work in songbirds has expanded this list in two significant directions.

a. Vocal imitation and the development of song sharing among territorialneighbors One of the main documented functions of birdsong is to mediateinteractions among neighboring territorial males (Catchpole and Slater, 1995;Hyman, 2002; Searcy andAndersson, 1986; Todt andNaguib, 2000).Researchhas focused on the use of ‘‘shared’’ songs by interacting neighbors and thepotential fitness benefits of song sharing (Brown and Farabaugh, 1997;Handley and Nelson, 2005; Lachlan et al., 2004; Molles and Vehrencamp,2001; Payne and Payne, 1997; Todt and Naguib, 2000). Two hypotheses toexplain song sharing are that it provides a reliable means by which males candiscriminate neighbors from strangers, and that it enables increased precisionin communicating aggressive intent among territorial neighbors. Birds dowellto distinguish neighbors from strangers because, unlike neighbors, strangersare ‘‘inherently expansionist’’ (Beecher andBrenowitz, 2005, p. 147), and thusrequire closer monitoring. Research by Beecher and colleagues on songsparrows (Melospiza melodia) illustrates how neighboring males may employshared songs to escalate or de‐escalate aggressive interactions. To escalate aninteraction, males may first respond to a singing neighbor with a nonsharedsong, then by singing a ‘‘repertoire match’’ (a shared song, although not theone being sung by the neighbor at the moment), and then by singing a precisetype match (Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005).

Selection for song sharingmay influence the evolution of vocal geographicpatterns—beyond obvious effects on the finest‐scale geographic patterns,that is, among neighbors—because of how it shapes the evolution of songlearning strategies in populations (Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005; Handleyand Nelson, 2005; Slater, 1989). First of all, selection for song sharing maypresumably influence the number of song types that birds will evolve tolear n, that is song rep ertoire size. As stated by Beec her and Brenowitz (2005 ,

Page 34: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

436 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

p. 147), ‘‘selection for song sharing and selection for large song repertoiresare at least partially contrary . . . as a logical consequence of the fact that asong learning strategy cannot optimize both goals.’’ This is because selectionfor song sharing may favor, Beecher and Brenowitz (2005) argue, the evolu-tion of smaller repertoires with correspondingly greater percentages ofsongs shared among neighbors. Expanding to a broader geographic scale,species that evolve smaller repertoires may be more likely to express theclassic dialect pattern (Williams and Slater, 1990). A second potential im-pact of selection for song sharing is on the timing of song learning. Songsharing may be promoted in species that retain sufficient flexibility to matchsongs produced by neighbors on their breeding grounds, after dispersal fromnatal grounds (Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005; Martens and Kessler, 2000;Trainer, 1989). For species or subspecies with significant postnatal dispersal,‘‘open‐ended’’ song learning programs should be favored because suchlearning programs increase the likelihood that birds would be able tomatch the songs of neighboring males (Rothstein and Fleischer, 1987).Evolution of the classic dialect pattern may thus be facilitated indirectly,via selection for song sharing. The observation that many Fringillids expressboth song sharing and dialects (Handley and Nelson, 2005; Section III)supports this hypothesis.

b. Vocal imitation and male quality Another proposed function of vocalimitation is that it enables song structure to be used as an accurate indicatorof male quality (Buchanan et al., 1999, 2003; Nowicki et al., 1998, 2002). Thishypothesis suggests that learned songs serve as honest indicators of malequality because their accurate reproduction requires successful brain devel-opment in the face of potentially severe nutritional and developmentalstress. Males who accurately reproduce vocal features of song tutors, orwho are able to develop complex vocal features, in effect advertise high‐quality genes, developmental histories, and learning abilities. Such maleswould presumably offer higher quality genetic input and rearing environ-ments for females that choose them as mates.

Studies suggest that nutritional or developmental stress may indeedimpair the normal de velopmen t of vocal brain nuclei (Buc hananet al., 2004; MacD onald et al. , 2006; Now icki et al ., 2002 ; but see Gil et al .,2006). Empirical research on the potential consequences of developmentalstress for song development has focused especially on three vocal structuralparameters: syllable or song repertoire size, the accuracy of vocal tutormatching, and rates or durations of vocal output (Buchanan et al., 2003;Nowicki et al., 2002; Spencer et al., 2003, 2004, 2005). Presumably, males ofhigher quality will be able to develop larger vocal repertoires, given thecorrelations between early stress, song nuclei volume, and vocal repertoire

Page 35: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 437

size (Nowicki et al., 2002). An honest indicator mechanism such as thismight help to explain female preferences for large vocal repertoires, asshown in some species (Searcy and Yasukawa, 1996). Accuracy in imitationmay similarly provide an honest indicator of a male’s brain developmentalstatus, given the complexity of the auditory and neural systems that aredevoted to the process, and given the numerous environmental challengesthat may impede song nucleus development (Nowicki et al., 2002). Rates ordurations of vocal output may provide an indicator of the quality of a male’soverall health and developmental history (Spencer et al., 2005). Nowickiet al. (1998) noted a possible trade‐off in the evolution of copying accuracyand repertoire size development, in which a premium on imitation accuracy(quality of copying) might impede the development of large repertoires(quantity of copying) and vice versa. In terms of neural mechanisms,selection for high imitation accuracy may perhaps constrain the develop-ment of larger repertoires if the brain space and developmental resourcesrequired for accurate imitation secondarily limit the quantity of vocalmaterial that can be imitated in the first place.

Our point with this example is that selective pressures on repertoiresize and the accuracy of vocal tutor matching may impose, either indepen-dently or jointly, secondary effects on the evolution of vocal geographicpatterns. Species with larger repertoires are generally thought not to evolvestringent dialects (reviewed by Searcy et al., 2002), a supposition that is partlysupported by our analysis in Section III. Beyond the question of whetherdialects occur or not, species that have evolved a premium on copying accu-racy may evolve greater stability and within‐neighborhood similarity in thestructure of their songs (Slater, 1986; see also our analyses in Section III).

Vocal geographic structure may thus arise indirectly through selectionfavoring males with learning programs that render them more adeptat ‘‘managing’’ interactions for selfish gain (Kroodsma, 1996), or throughselection on song structure as an honest indicator of male genetic and devel-opmental quality (Nowicki et al., 2002). These arguments echo Slater’s sug-gestion that selection at the level of individuals drives geographic patternsthat appear at the level of populations (Slater, 1989).

2. Mechanisms of Vocal Production

The ontogeny and evolution of vocalizations are impacted not just bylearning but also by the mechanisms that underlie vocal production(Elemans et al., 2004; Fee et al., 1998; Nowicki et al., 1992; Podos andNowicki, 2004a; Suthers and Goller, 1997). Some recent studies have empha-sized the fact that vocal production in birds requires the input and activity ofnot just the sound source (the syrinx) but also other motor components,including the respiratory system and vocal tract (Beckers et al., 2003; Hoese

Page 36: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

438 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

et al., 2000; Nowicki, 1987; Nowicki and Marler, 1988; Riede et al., 2006;Suthers, 2004; Suthers et al., 1999). These additional motor components ofvocal production can impose performance limits on the expression and evo-lution of song features (reviewed by Podos and Nowicki, 2004a). Movementsof respiratory muscles, to illustrate, are coordinated precisely with syrinxactivity, and appear to be essential in controlling the timing of vocal output(Suthers et al., 1999). Maximal rates of breathing cycles may thus limit theevolution of temporal modulations in song. Components of the vocal tract,including the trachea, larynx, and beak, modify the spectral structure of song,and in particular serve to dampen harmonic overtones and thus enable theproduction of pure‐tonal songs (Beckers et al., 2003; Hoese et al., 2000;Nowicki and Marler, 1988; Riede et al., 2006; Westneat et al., 1993). Maximalrates of vocal tract reconfiguration, such as those achieved through changesin beak gape, can limit trill rates and frequency bandwidth within trilledvocalizations (Nowicki et al., 1992; Podos, 1997).

Recent empirical studies suggest two related effects of production con-straints on vocal evolution. First, production constraints can bias the evolu-tion of individual vocal features. The evolution of trill rate in swampsparrows (Melospiza georgiana), to illustrate, appears to be limited byindividual birds’ vocal performance abilities. This was revealed in a studyin which young male swamp sparrows were trained with tutor songs inwhich trill rates had been artificially elevated (Podos, 1996; see also Podoset al., 1999). Birds proved able to memorize the rapid tutor songs, butunable to produce accurate copies of these songs, in manners consistentwith a hypothesis of motor constraints on song production (Podos, 1996).Second, production constraints on vocal evolution may be manifest not onlyin individual features but also as trade‐offs among multiple vocal features.Songs of birds of the sparrow family Emberizidae, to illustrate, exhibit atrade‐off between trill rates and frequency bandwidth (Podos, 1997). Simi-lar patterns have now been described in additional taxa (Ballentine et al.,2004; Draganoiu et al., 2002; Illes et al., 2006), and evidence also suggeststhat songs produced at higher performance levels—that is, with greater trillrates and/or frequency bandwidths—are more effective with regard to bothinter‐ and intraspecific function (Ballentine et al., 2004; Illes et al., 2006).An acoustic trade‐off between trill rate and frequency bandwidth is consis-tent with a hypothesis of physical constraint: in order to achieve particularlyrapid trill rates, the requirement for pure‐tonal quality (and thus rapid vocaltract reconfigurations) sets performance limits on the ranges of frequenciesthat can be produced over a given time interval (Podos and Nowicki,2004a). Physical limits or trade‐offs in vocal evolution are worth attentionbecause they may counter selection for particular functions. Thus, to

Page 37: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 439

illustrate, selection for songs with particularly rapid trills, for exampleunder a local adaptation scenario, would presumably be impeded by physi-cal constraints on trill production.

Selection on components of the vocal apparatus for nonvocal functionsmay also invoke secondary effects on vocal evolution (Podos and Hendry,2006 ; Podos and Now icki, 2004b). Body size, to illustrat e, evolve s in manyanimals in response to a range of selective factors such as thermoregulation,fecundity, reproductive rate, and dispersal (Blanckenhorn, 2000; Roff,1992). Resulting changes in body size may impose secondary impacts onthe fundamental frequencies of bird vocalizations, given tight correlationsbetween body size and syrinx size, and the functional relationship of syrinxsize and vocal frequency production (Bertelli and Tubaro, 2002; Cutler,1970; Ryan and Brenowitz, 1985). A second scenario, involving beak andsong evolution, has been illustrated recently for Darwin’s finches of theGalapagos Islands, Ecuador. In these birds, beak form and function hasbeen shown to evolve in precise correspondence with varying ecologicalparameters, namely food availability and interspecific competition (Grantand Grant, 1995, 2002, 2006). Analyses of songs of birds with knownmorphologies have now revealed that the same two vocal parametersmentioned above, trill rate and frequency bandwidth, correlate with varia-tion in beak morphology (Huber and Podos, 2006; Podos, 2001). Thiscorrelation seems likely to be the result of proximate constraints on beakgape changes and thus vocal tract configurations during vocal production.Consistent with this hypothesis, birds with larger beaks, predicted to suffergreater constraints on vocal performance (Nowicki et al., 1992), haveevolved songs with slower trill rates and narrower frequency bandwidths(Huber and Podos, 2006; Podos, 2001; Podos and Nowicki, 2004b; Podoset al., 2004a). Thus, within given lineages of Darwin’s finches, morphologicaladaptation under selection for feeding opportunities is predicted to impactvocal performance, and thus the evolution of some vocal features, that isthose vocal features that require precise tract reconfigurations for theirproduction.

These scenarios of vocal evolution suggest that adaptation to divergentenvironments may produce, on its own account, structured patterns of vocalgeographic variation. To continue with the example of Darwin’s finches,populations of some species have diverged in genetics and morphology ondifferent islands, presumably as a result of the different selective environ-ments on those islands, and a result of limited gene flow between islands(Lack, 1947; Petren et al., 2005). Given the role of the beak in vocal produc-tion, adaptive divergence in beakmorphologymay thus have driven intraspe-cific, between‐island vocal divergence, with, for example, the largest‐beaked

Page 38: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

440 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

populations of a given species experiencing themost severe constraints on trillevolution (Podos and Nowicki, 2004b). The main point of this example, forpresent purposes, is that geographic patterns of song variationmay emerge asincidental by‐products of selection for nonvocal functions, without need forselection for the patterns themselves, as is implied by functional adaptationhypotheses of vocal geographic evolution.

V. EVOLUTION OF GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN AVIAN VOCAL

SIGNALS: PROSPECTUS

As we argued above, advances on both empirical and conceptual frontsprovide increasing support for a role of by‐product models of vocal geo-graphic variation. We do not, however, intend to suggest that all facets ofsong evolution are explained through by‐product mechanisms. Rather,there are myriad factors that may impact geographic divergence of thevocal phenotype. This final section, which follows closely from Podoset al. (2004b), is devoted to surveying the range of scenarios by whichsong features may diverge among different populations of a species.

A. INTERPLAY OF MEMES AND MECHANISMS IN VOCAL EVOLUTION

To better address the range of factors involved in vocal evolution we find ituseful to distinguish two distinct ‘‘substrates’’ of vocal evolution: memes andmechanisms. Memes refer to song parameters that are transmitted acrossgenerations via learning, whereas mechanisms refer to phenotypic bases ofvocal expression (development and production) that are transmitted acrossgenerations via genetic inheritance (Podos et al., 2004b). Traditional explana-tions for patterns of vocal geographic evolution have, in our viewpoint, beenhampered by a nearly exclusive focus on meme evolution. Part of the reasonfor the relative neglect of mechanisms, we believe, is that their effect isnormally manifest over comparatively broad timescales (Podos, 1997; Ryanand Brenowitz, 1985) and are thus more difficult to identify and study.

In the evolution of learned vocalizations, vocal memes and vocalmechanisms may evolve on nonintersecting trajectories. Thus, for instance,selection for increased trill rates may augment trill rates in a population, inthe event that the mechanisms responsible for trill production in thatlineage are able to accommodate such increases. Similarly, evolutionarychanges in body size may have no impact on the vocal frequenciesexpressed in a population, in the event that vocal frequencies were initiallynot produced near their limits of possibility. But memes and mechanismsmay also interact in vocal evolution. Evolutionary changes in mechanisms

Page 39: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 441

of vocal production and learning may adjust potential routes of memeevolution, and evolutionary stability in vocal mechanisms may limit theresponse of memes to directional selection (Podos et al., 2004b). Recogni-tion of the interplay of memes and mechanisms in vocal evolution allows usto identify five categories of potential causes for the evolution of vocalgeographic variation (Podos et al., 2004b).

B. POTENTIAL CAUSES OF VOCAL GEOGRAPHIC EVOLUTION

Songs, like any other phenotype, evolve through the combined effects ofdrift and selection. We identify two scenarios that involve drift and threethat involve selection. We do not claim the scenarios to be mutually exclu-sive or collectively exhaustive. Rather, song divergence likely involves all ofthese processes, emphasized to varying degrees and at different times in anylineage’s evolutionary history. We do not attempt to integrate details aboutdispersal patterns or the timing of learning, which must play a central role invocal geographic evolution (Ellers and Slabbekoorn, 2003; Krebs andKroodsma, 1980). Nor do we attempt to evaluate how long‐term ecologicalprocesses, such as changes in land use or impacts of fire on habitat, mayinfluence bird distributions and thus dialect formation (Laiolo and Tella,2005).

1. Cultural Drift

Song features may evolve as a result of inaccurate transmission of songmemes across generations because of ‘‘errors’’ in learning (Grant and Grant,1996; Payne, 1996). Distinct trajectories of cultural evolution via copy errorsmay explain vocal differences among diverging populations, especially duringthe initial stages of divergence (Lemon, 1975; Slabbekoorn and Smith, 2002a).To illustrate, evolutionary divergence in the phonology (fine structure) ofnotes, resulting from inaccurate imitation,may explain interisland differencesin note structure in some species ofDarwin’s finches (Grant andGrant, 1996).Lineages that readily express cultural errors, along with some isolation ofdescendent populations, seem likely to generate vocal geographic variation.

2. Genetic Drift

Song features may also evolve via random changes in the anatomical,physiological, and neural mechanisms that underlie vocal ontogeny and pro-duction—and, more specifically, in the genetic loci that underpin thesemechanisms. Genetic drift may presumably impact vocal geographic evolu-tion when song memes in a lineage are produced at or near some anatomical,developmental, or performance limit. Consider, for instance, drift in thegenetic loci that underlie syrinx mass. Syrinx mass appears to set lower limits

Page 40: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

442 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

on vocal frequencies, such that only larger syringes can produce lower fre-quency sounds. If genetic drift leads to reduced syrinx mass within a givenpopulations,wewould expect the potential for frequencyproduction to followsuit. Thus, vocal frequencies, if initially produced near maximal performancecapacities (comparatively low frequencies), may accordingly be ‘‘bumped’’ tohigher levels in the offshoot population (Podos et al., 2004b). Genetic driftmay also alter the structure and function of the brain nuclei involved in songlearning, for instance, through randomalterations in the timing of interactionsbe twe en s on g n uc le i ( Li vi ng st on et al., 2000). Such random changes may haveconsequences for the timing and content of song acquisition.

3. Cultural Selection

Cultural selection occurs when certain vocal memes are favored overothers, as a result of the differential effectiveness of those memes in theprocess of communication. A primary example concerns selection for optimalsound transmission. Songs are known to vary in how well they transmit indifferent environments, and cultural selection is thought to thus shape certainvocal parameters (Slabbekoorn, 2004; Wiley and Richards, 1978). Songs withslow repetition rates and low frequencies, to illustrate, have been shown toevolve more often in forested habitats than in other habitats, presumablybecause slow, low‐frequency songs suffer relatively less degradation in forest-ed habitats than elsewhere. Songs withmore effective transmission propertiesmay be favored by selection not only in the context of interactions amongadults but also in song model imitation by juveniles (Hansen, 1979). Culturalselection for optimal sound transmission has been implicated in the diver-gence of song among populations of a number of species (Doutrelant et al.,1999; Handford and Lougheed, 1991; Hunter and Krebs, 1979; Ruegg et al.,2006; Slabbekoorn and Smith, 2002b; Wiley, 1991).

4. Natural Selection

Natural selection may drive vocal geographic divergence through its influ-ence on either memes or mechanisms. With respect to memes, natural selec-tion may facilitate vocal divergence via ‘‘reinforcement,’’ in which selectionagainst hybrid production favors those birds that produce the most species,p o pu l a ti on , o r l oc al it y d i st i n c ti ve s o n g s ( Butlin and Ritchie, 1994; Marler,1957, 1960; Nelson and Marler, 1990; Ptacek, 2000). This is the broadercontext in which we would place the local adaptation hypothesis of dialectevolution. The example of the multifunctional role of the beak in singing andfeeding, discussed in the previous section, illustrates how natural selection onmechanisms can cause incidental vocal evolution (Nowicki et al., 1992; Podosand Nowicki, 2004a,b). To reiterate, natural selection in the context of selec-tion for food availability, food type, and interspecific competition is known to

Page 41: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 443

drive precise changes in beak formand function (Grant andGrant, 1995, 2002,2006).Given the role of the beak in vocal production, such evolution can drivevocal changes as a secondary consequence (Podos and Nowicki, 2004b).

5. Sexual Selection

Sexual selection is traditionally regarded as favoring elaborate or complexforms of vocal signals, especially as a result of female choice (Andersson,1994; Catchpole and McGregor, 1985; Searcy and Andersson, 1986; Searcyand Yasukawa, 1996). Sexual selection may also favor vocal features thatchallenge males’ developmental and performance capacities (Nowicki et al.,2002) or that enable increased precision in communication in male–maleinteractions (Beecher and Brenowitz, 2005; Todt and Naguib, 2000). As ageneral observation, the course of sexual selection is often haphazard, withdifferent signal parameters favored or exaggerated in different lineages(Boughman, 2001; Panhuis et al., 2001). Divergent pathways of sexual selec-tion on songmay similarly result in signal divergence in offshoot populations,at least to the extent that populations remain in genetic and cultural isolation.To illustrate we turn again to potential trade‐offs involving repertoire size. Insome lineages, female preferences for complex signals may favor the evolu-tion of large repertoires, whereas in other lineages female preferences foraccurate imitation may favor small repertoires. Moreover, selection for songsharing amongmales in other lineages may favor moderate‐sized repertoires.Divergence of sexual selection pressures among populations may thuspresumably lead to geographic divergence in repertoire size.

VI. SUMMARY

Our goal in this chapter has been to evaluate, from both empirical andconceptual perspectives, the factors that facilitate the evolution of geo-graphic variation in bird vocalizations. Studies on this topic have tradition-ally focused on the evolution of song ‘‘dialects,’’ and have emphasizedfunctional hypotheses to explain their evolution. Two such hypotheses,‘‘local adaptation’’ and ‘‘social adaptation’’ hypotheses, focus on the poten-tial role of song in aiding recognition of males, either by locality or by socialgroup. A quantitative survey of results from papers published on dialects,between 1962 and 2006, however, suggests limited direct support for func-tional hypotheses. An alternative set of hypotheses suggests that songfeatures may diverge through ‘‘by‐product’’ scenarios, in which selectionfor nonrecognition functions drives incidental changes in song structure,and geographic variation therein. Examples of such functions involve theevolution of song learning in neighbor–neighbor song sharing and the

Page 42: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

444 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

evolution of song learning in the context of sexual selection for malequality. We also describe scenarios by which songs may diverge indirectlythrough selection on components of the vocal apparatus such as body sizeand beak form and function. To conclude, we outline scenarios by whichsongs may diverge geographically; via cultural drift, genetic drift, culturalselection, natural selection, and sexual selection. Empirical study of thesescenarios, together with countinued descriptions of vocal learning strategiesand patterns of dispersal, may provide insights into vocal geographic evolu-tion and thus propensities for speciation by reproductive isolation.

Acknowledgments

J.P. gratefully acknowledges financial support from the National Science Foundation (NSF

IOB‐0347291). P.W. gratefully acknowledges financial support from the National Science

Foundation (NSF IBN‐98‐01490) and the Zoology Scholarship Fund for Excellence (Dorothea

Stengl) at University of Texas. Helpful comments on previous versions of this chapter were

provided by M. Naguib, P. Slater, L. Higgins, D. Hillis, M. Kirkpatrick, C. Sexton, M. Ryan,

and W. Wilczynski.

References

Alderson, G. W., Gibbs, H. L., and Sealy, S. G. (1999). Determining the reproductive beha-

viour of individual brown‐headed cowbirds using microsatellite DNA markers. Anim.

Behav. 58, 895–905.

Andersson, M. (1994). ‘‘Sexual Selection.’’ Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

Andrew, R. J. (1962). Evolution of intelligence and vocal mimicking. Science 137, 585–589.Austen, M. J. W., and Handford, P. (1991). Variation in the songs of breeding Gambel’s white‐

crowned sparrows near Churchill, Manitoba. Condor 93, 147–152.

Avery, M., and Oring, L. W. (1977). Song dialects in the bobolink (Dolichonyx oryzivorus).

Condor 79, 113–118.

Baker, M. C. (1974). Genetic structure of two populations of white‐crowned sparrows with

different song dialects. Condor 76, 351–356.

Baker, M. C. (1975). Song dialects and genetic differences in white‐crowned sparrow (Zono-

trichia leucophrys). Evolution 29, 226–241.

Baker, M. C. (1982). Vocal dialect recognition and population genetic consequences. Am.

Zool. 22, 561–569.

Baker, M. C. (1983). The behavioral response of female Nuttall’s white‐crowned sparrows to

male song of natal and alien dialects. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 12, 309–315.

Baker, M. C., and Cunningham, M. A. (1985). The biology of bird‐song dialects. Behav. Brain

Sci. 8, 85–133.Baker, M. C., and Mewaldt, L. R. (1978). Song dialects as barriers to dispersal in white‐

crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli. Evolution 32, 712–722.

Baker, M. C., and Mewaldt, L. R. (1981). Response to ‘‘Song dialects as barriers to dispersal:

A re‐evaluation.’’ Evolution 35, 189–190.Baker, M. C., and Thompson, D. B. (1985). Song dialects of sparrows: Historical processes

inferred from patterns of geographic variation. Condor 87, 127–141.

Page 43: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 445

Baker, M. C., Spitler‐Nabors, K. J., and Bradley, D. C. (1981). Early experience determines

song dialect responsiveness of female sparrows. Science 214, 819–821.

Baker, M. C., Thompson, D. B., Sherman, G. L., Cunningham, M. A., and Tomback, D. F.

(1982). Allozyme frequencies in a linear series of song dialect populations. Evolution 36,

1020–1029.

Baker, M. C., Baker, A. E. M., Cunningham, M. A., Thompson, D. B., and Tomback, D. F.

(1984a). Reply to ‘‘Allozymes and song dialects: A reassessment.’’ Evolution 38, 449–451.Baker, M. C., Tomback, D. F., Thompson, D. B., Theimer, T. C., and Bradley, D. C. (1984b).

Behavioral consequences of song learning: Discrimination of song types by male white‐crowned sparrows. Learn. Motiv. 15, 428–440.

Baker, M. C., Tomback, D. F., Thompson, D. B., and Cunningham, M. A. (1985). Reply to

Hafner and Petersen. Evolution 39, 1177–1179.

Baker, M. C., Bjerke, T. K., Lampe, H. U., and Espmark, Y. O. (1987a). Sexual response of

female yellowhammers to differences in regional song dialects and repertoire sizes. Anim.

Behav. 35, 395–401.

Baker, M. C., Spitler‐Nabors, K. J., Thompson, A. D. Jr., and Cunningham, M. A. (1987b).

Reproductive behaviour of female white‐crowned sparrows: Effect of dialects and syn-

thetic hybrid songs. Anim. Behav. 35, 1766–1774.Ballentine, B., Hyman, J., and Nowicki, S. (2004). Vocal performance influences female

response to male bird song: An experimental test. Behav. Ecol. 15, 163–168.

Baptista, L. F. (1975). Song dialects and demes in sedentary populations of the white‐crownedsparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli). Univ. Calif. Pub. Zool. 105, 1–52.

Baptista, L. F. (1977). Geographic variation in songs and dialects of the Puget Sound white‐crowned sparrow. Condor 79, 356–370.

Baptista, L. F. (1985). Bird‐song dialects: Social adaptation or assortativemating?Behav. Brain

Sci. 8, 100–101.

Baptista, L. F. (1990). Dialectal variation in the raincall of the chaffinch (Fringilla coelebs).Die

Vogelwarte 35, 249–256.

Baptista, L. F., and King, J. R. (1980). Geographical variation in song and song dialects of

montane white‐crowned sparrows. Condor 82, 267–284.

Baptista, L. F., and Morton, M. L. (1982). Song dialects and mate selection in montane white‐crowned sparrows. Auk 99, 537–547.

Baptista, L. F., and Morton, M. L. (1988). Song learning in montane white‐crowned sparrows:

From whom and when. Anim. Behav. 36, 1753–1764.

Baptista, L. F., and Petrinovich, L. (1984). Social interaction, sensitive phases and the song

template hypothesis in the white‐crowned sparrow. Anim. Behav. 32, 172–181.Baptista, L. F., and Trail, P. W. (1992). The role of song in the evolution of passerine diversity.

Syst. Biol. 41, 242–247.

Baptista, L. F., Jesse, A., Bell, D. A., and Cebrian, C. (1997). Acquisition and recall of

Gambel’s sparrow dialects by Nuttall’s white‐crowned sparrows in the wild. Wilson Bull.

109, 516–521.

Becker, P. H. (1982). The coding of species‐specific characteristics in bird sounds. In ‘‘Acoustic

Communication in Birds’’ (D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, Eds.), Vol. 1, pp. 213–252.

Academic Press, New York.

Beckers, G. J. L., Suthers, R. A., and ten Cate, C. (2003). Pure‐tone birdsong by resonance

filtering of harmonic overtones. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 100, 7372–7376.

Beecher, M. D., and Brenowitz, E. A. (2005). Functional aspects of song learning in songbirds.

Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 143–149.Bertelli, S., and Tubaro, P. L. (2002). Body mass and habitat correlates of song structure in a

primitive group of birds. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 77, 423–430.

Page 44: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

446 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

Bjerke, T. (1974). Geographic variation in the song of the redwing, Turdus iliacus. Sterna 13,

65–76.

Bjerke, T. (1982). Origin of song traditions in the redwing Turdus iliacus: Evidence from field

studies. Fauna Norvegica, Series C., Cinclus 5, 84–92.

Bjerke, T. K. (1980). Continuity and change in dialect structure of the redwing Turdus iliacus.

Fauna Norvegica Series C, Cinclus 3, 73–79.

Bjerke, T. K. (1984). The response of male redwings Turdus iliacus to playback of different

dialects. Fauna Norvegica, Series C, Cinclus 7, 24–27.

Bjerke, T. K., and Bjerke, T. H. (1981). Song dialects in the redwing Turdus iliacus. Ornis

Scandin. 12, 40–50.Blanckenhorn, W. U. (2000). The evolution of body size: What keeps organisms small?Q. Rev.

Biol. 75, 385–407.

Borror, D. J. (1961). Intraspecific variation in passerine bird songs. Wilson Bull. 73, 57–78.

Borror, D. J. (1975). Songs of the rufous‐sided towhee. Condor 77, 183–1985.Boughman, J. W. (2001). Divergent sexual selection enhances reproductive isolation in stickle-

backs. Nature 411, 944–948.

Brenowitz, E. A. (1985). Bird‐song dialects: Filling in the gaps. Behav. Brain Sci. 8, 101–102.

Briskie, J. V. (1999). Song variation and the structure of local song dialects in the polygynan-

drous Smith’s longspur. Can. J. Zool. 77, 1587–1594.

Brooks, D. R., and McLennan, D. H. (1991). ‘‘Phylogeny, Ecology, and Behavior.’’ University

of Chicago Press, Chicago.

Brown, E. D., and Farabaugh, S. M. (1997). What birds with complex social relationships can

tell us about vocal learning: Vocal sharing in avian groups. In ‘‘Social Influences on Vocal

Development’’ (C. T. Snowden and M. Hausberger, Eds.), pp. 98–127. Cambridge

University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Buchanan, K. L., Catchpole, C. K., Lewis, J. W., and Lodge, A. (1999). Song as an indicator of

parasitism in the sedge warbler. Anim. Behav. 57, 307–314.

Buchanan, K. L., Spencer, K. A., Goldsmith, A. R., and Catchpole, C. K. (2003). Song as an

honest signal of past developmental stress in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris).

Proc. Roy. Soc. B 270, 1149–1156.

Buchanan, K. L., Leitner, S., Spencer, K. A., Goldsmith, A. R., and Catchpole, C. K. (2004).

Developmental stress selectively affects the song control nucleus HVC in the zebra finch

brain. Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond B. 271, 2381–2386.Butlin, R. K. (1995). Reinforcement: An idea evolving. Trends Ecol. Evol. 10, 432–434.

Butlin, R. K., and Ritchie, M. G. (1994). ‘‘Behavior and speciation.’’ In Behavior and Evolu-

tion, (P. J. B. Slater and T. R. Halliday, Eds.), pp. 43–79. Cambridge University Press,

Cambridge, UK.

Carter, M. D. (1984). The Social organization and parasitic behavior of the bronzed cowbird in

South Texas. PhD thesis. University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN.

Catchpole, C. K. (1980). Sexual selection and the evolution of complex songs among European

warblers of the genus Acrocephalus. Behaviour 74, 149–166.

Catchpole, C. K., and McGregor, P. K. (1985). Sexual selection, song complexity and plumage

dimorphism in European buntings of the genus Emberiza. Anim. Behav. 33, 1378–1380.

Catchpole, C. K., and Rowell, A. (1993). Song sharing and local dialects in a population of the

European Wren Troglodytes troglodytes. Behaviour 125, 67–78.

Catchpole, C. K., and Slater, P. J. B. (1995). ‘‘Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations.’’

Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Chilton, G., and Lein, M. R. (1996a). Long‐term changes in songs and song dialect boundaries

of Puget Sound white‐crowned sparrows. Condor 98, 567–580.

Page 45: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 447

Chilton, G., and Lein, M. R. (1996b). Song repertoires of Puget Sound white‐crowned sparrows

Zonotrichia leucophrys pugetensis. J. Avian Biol. 27, 31–40.

Chilton, G., Baker, M. C., Barrentine, C. D., and Cunningham, M. C. (1995). White‐crownedsparrow (Zonotrichia leucophrys). In ‘‘The Birds of North America’’ (A. Poole and

F. B. Gill, Eds.), pp. 1–28. Academy of Natural Sciences, Philadelphia, PA.

Clotfelter, E. D. (1995). Courtship displaying and intrasexual competition in the bronzed

cowbird. Condor 97, 816–818.Conrads, K. (1976). Studien an fremddialekt‐sangern und dialekt‐mischsangern des ortolans

(Emberiza hortulana). J. Ornithol. 117, 438–450.

Conrads, K., and Conrads, W. (1971). Regionaldialekte des ortolans (Emberiza hortulana) in

Deutschland. Die Vogelwelt 92, 81–100.

Cunningham, M. A., Baker, M. C., and Boardman, T. J. (1987). Microgeographic song varia-

tion in the Nuttall’s white‐crowned sparrow. Condor 89, 261–275.

Cutler, B. (1970). ‘‘Anatomical studies of the syrinx of Darwin’s finches.’’ Master’s thesis. San

Francisco State University, San Francisco.

de Kort, S. R., and ten Cate, C. (2001). Response to interspecific vocalizations is affected by

degree of phylogenetic relatedness in Streptopelia doves. Anim. Behav. 61, 239–247.

de Kort, S. R., and ten Cate, C. (2004). Repeated decrease in vocal repertoire size in Strepto-

pelia doves. Anim. Behav. 67, 549–557.

DeWolfe, B. B., and Baptista, L. F. (1995). Singing behavior, song types on their wintering

grounds and the question of leap‐frog migration in Puget Sound white‐crowned sparrows.

Condor 97, 376–389.DeWolfe, B. B., Kaska, D. D., and Peyton, L. J. (1974). Prominent variations in the songs of

Gambel’s white‐crowned sparrows. Bird Banding 45, 224–252.

Dobzhansky, T. (1951). ‘‘Genetics and the Origin of Species.’’ Columbia University Press,

New York.

Dolbeer, R. A. (1982). Migration patterns for age and sex classes of blackbirds (Agelaius

phoeniceus) and starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). J. Field Ornithol. 53, 28–46.

Doutrelant, C., Leitao, A., Giorgo, M., and Lambrechts, M. (1999). Geographical variation in

blue tit song, the result of an adjustment to vegetation type? Behaviour 136, 481–493.

Draganoiu, T. I., Nagle, L., and Kreutzer, M. (2002). Directional female preference for an

exaggerated male trait in canary (Serinus canaria) song. Proc. Roy. Soc. B 269, 2525–2531.

Edwards, S. V., Kingan, S. B., Calkins, J. D., Balakrishnan, C. N., Jennings, W. B.,

Swanson, W. J., and Sorenson, M. D. (2005). Speciation in birds: Genes, geography, and

sexual selection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 102, 6550–6557.

Elemans, C. P. H., Spierts, I. L. Y., Muller, U. K., van Leeuwen, J. L., and Goller, F. (2004).

Birdsong: Superfast muscles control dove’s trill. Nature 431, 146.

Ellers, J., and Slabbekoorn, H. (2003). Song divergence and male dispersal among bird

populations: A spatially explicit model testing the role of vocal learning. Anim. Behav.

65, 671–681.Espmark, Y. (1981). Song dialects in redwings Turdus iliacus. Var Fagelvarld 40, 81–90.

Espmark, Y. (1982). Microgeographical and individual variation in the morphology of song of

the redwing Turdus iliacus. Fauna Norvegica, Series C., Cinclus 5, 73–83.

Falls, J. B. (1963). Properties of bird song eliciting responses from territorial males. Proc.

Internat. Ornithol. Cong. 13, 259–271.

Fee, M. S., Shraiman, B., Pesaran, B., andMitra, P. P. (1998). The role of nonlinear dynamics of

the syrinx in the vocalizations of a songbird. Nature 395, 67–71.

Feekes, F. (1977). Colony‐specific song in Cacicus cela (Icteridae, Aves): The password

hypothesis. Ardea 65, 197–202.

Page 46: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

448 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Witkin, S. R. (1978). Vocal repertoire of the black‐cappedchickadee. Auk 95, 34–48.

Ficken, M. S., Ficken, R. W., and Apel, K. M. (1985). Dialects in a call associated with pair

interactions in the black‐capped chickadee Parus atricapillus. Auk 102, 145–151.

Fitzpatrick, J. W. (1988). Why so many passerine birds? A response to Raikow. Syst. Zool. 37,

71–76.

Fleischer, R. C., and Rothstein, S. I. (1988). Known secondary contact and rapid gene flow

among subspecies and dialects in the brown‐Headed cowbird. Evolution 42, 1146–1158.

Fonstad, T., Espmark, Y., Lampe, H., and Bjerke, T. (1984). Breeding biology of the redwing

Turdus iliacus in central Norway. Fauna Norvegica, Series C., Cinclus 7, 75–82.Ford, J. K. B. (1991). Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal

waters of British Columbia [Canada]. Can. J. Zool. 69, 1454–1483.

Foster, S. A. (1999). The geography of behaviour: An evolutionary perspective. Trends Ecol.

Evol. 14, 190–195.Gil, D., and Gahr, M. (2002). The honesty of bird song: Multiple constraints for multiple traits.

Trends Ecol. Evol. 17, 133–141.

Gil, D., Naguib, M., Riebel, K., Rutstein, A., and Gahr, M. (2006). Early condition, song

learning, and the volume of song brain nuclei in the zebra finch. J. Neurobiol. 66,1602–1612.

Gill, F. B., and Murray, B. G. (1972). Discrimination behavior and hybridization of the blue‐winged and golden‐winged warblers. Evolution 26, 282–293.

Glaubrecht, M. (1989). Zur geographischen variabilitat des gesangs der goldammer, Emberiza

citrinella, im norddeutschen Dialekt‐Grenzgebiet. J. Ornithol. 130, 277–292.

Glaubrecht, M. (1991). Gesangsvariation der goldammer (Emberiza citrinella) in Nord-

deutschland und auf den Danischen Inseln. J. Ornithol. 132, 441–445.Goodfellow, D. J., and Slater, P. J. B. (1986). A model of bird song dialects. Anim. Behav. 34,

1579–1580.

Gould, S. J., and Vrba, E. S. (1982). Exaptation: A missing term in the science of form.

Paleobiology 8, 4–15.Grant, P. R., and Grant, B. R. (1995). Predicting microevolutionary responses to directional

selection on heritable variation. Evolution 49, 241–251.

Grant, P. R., and Grant, B. R. (1996). Cultural inheritance of song and its role in the evolution

of Darwin’s finches. Evolution 50, 2471–2487.Grant, P. R., and Grant, B. R. (2002). Unpredictable evolution in a 30‐year study of Darwin’s

finches. Science 296, 707–711.

Grant, P. R., and Grant, B. R. (2006). Evolution of character displacement in Darwin’s finches.

Science 313, 224–226.

Guttinger, H. R. (1974). Gesang des Grunlings (Chloris chloris). Lokale Unterscheide und

Entwicklung bei Schallisolation. J. Ornithol. 115, 321–337.

Guttinger, H. R. (1976). Variable and constant structures in greenfinch songs (Chloris chloris)

in different locations. Behaviour 60, 304–318.

Haavie, J., Borge, T., Bures, S., Garamszegi, L. Z., Lampe, H. M., Moreno, J., Qvarnstrom, A.,

Torok, J., and Sætre, G. P. (2004). Flycatcher song in allopatry and sympatry—conver-

gence, divergence, and reinforcement. J. Evol. Biol. 17, 227–237.Hafner, D. J., and Peterson, K. E. (1985). Song dialects and gene flow in the white‐crowned

sparrow, Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli. Evolution 39, 687–694.

Handford, P., and Lougheed, S. C. (1991). Variation in duration and frequency characteristics

in the song of the rufous‐collared sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis, with respect to habitat,

trill dialect and body size. Condor 93, 644–658.

Page 47: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 449

Handley, H. G., and Nelson, D. A. (2005). Ecological and phylogenetic effects of song sharing

in songbirds. Ethology 111, 221–238.

Hansen, P. (1979). Vocal learning: Its role in adapting sound structures to long‐distancepropagation and a hypothesis on its evolution. Anim. Behav. 27, 1270–1271.

Hansen, P. (1985). Geographic song variation in the yellowhammer (Emberiza citrinella).

Natura Jutlandica 21, 209–219.

Hansen, P. (1999). Long‐term stability of song elements in the yellowhammer Emberiza

citrinella. Bioacoustics 9, 281–295.

Harbison, H., Nelson, D. A., and Hahn, T. P. (1999). Long‐term persistence of song dialects in

the mountain white‐crowned sparrow. Condor 101, 133–148.Harris, M. A., and Lemon, R. E. (1974). Songs of song sparrows: Reactions of males to songs of

different localities. Condor 76, 33–44.

Heinemann, D. (1981). Song dialects, migration, and population structure of Puget Sound

white‐crowned sparrows. Auk 98, 512–521.Hoese, W. J., Podos, J., Boetticher, N. C., and Nowicki, S. (2000). Vocal tract function in

birdsong production: Experimental manipulation of beak movements. J. Exp. Biol. 203,

1845–1855.

Holland, J., McGregor Peter, K., and Rowe Candida, L. (1996). Changes in microgeographic

song variation of the corn bunting Miliaria calandra. J. Avian Biol. 27, 47–55.

Huber, S. K., and Podos, J. (2006). Beak morphology and song production covary in a

population of Darwin’s finches. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 88, 489–498.

Hunter, L. M., and Krebs, J. R. (1979). Geographical variation in the song of the great tit

(Parus major) in relation to ecological factors. J. Anim. Ecol. 48, 759–785.

Hyman, J. (2002). Conditional strategies in territorial defense: Do carolina wrens play tit‐for‐tat? Behav. Ecol. 13, 664–669.

Illes, A. E., Hall, M. L., and Vehrencamp, S. L. (2006). Vocal performance influences male

receiver response in the banded wren. Proc. Roy. Soc. B 273, 1907–1912.

Irwin, D. E., and Price, T. (1999). Sexual imprinting, learning and speciation. Heredity 82,

347–354.

Irwin, D. E., Bensch, S., and Price, T. D. (2001). Speciation in a ring. Nature 409, 333–337.

Irwin, R. E. (1988). The evolutionary significance of behavioral development: The ontogeny

and phylogeny of bird song. Anim. Behav. 36, 814–824.

Irwin, R. E. (1990). Directional sexual selection cannot explain variation in song repertoire size

in the New World blackbirds (Icterinae). Ethology 85, 212–224.

Irwin, R. E. (1996). The phylogenetic content of avian courtship display and song evolution.

In ‘‘Phylogenies and the Comparative Method in Animal Behavior’’ (E. P. Martins, Ed.),

pp. 234–252. Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Jenkins, P. F. (1978). Cultural transmission of song patterns and dialect development in a free‐living bird population. Anim. Behav. 26, 50–78.

Jenkins, P. F. (1985). Song learning, competition, and dialects. Behav. Brain Sci. 8, 108.Johannessen, L. E., Slagsvold, T., and Hansen, B. T. (2006). Effects of social rearing condition

on song structure and repertoire size: Experimental evidence from the field.Anim. Behav.

72, 83–95.

King, J. R. (1972). Variation in the song of the Rufous‐collared Sparrow, Zonotrichia capensis,

in Northwestern Argentina. Z. Tierpsych. 30, 340–373.

Kramer, H. G., and Thompson, N. S. (1979). Geographic variation in the bell calls of the blue

jay (Cyanocitta cristata). Auk 96, 423–425.

Krebs, J. R., and Kroodsma, D. E. (1980). Repertoires and geographical variation in birdsong.

Adv. Study Behav. 11, 143–177.

Page 48: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

450 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

Kreutzer, M. (1974). Stereotypie et variations dans les chants de proclamation territoriale chez

le troglodyte (Troglodytes troglodytes). Revue du Comportement Animal 8, 270–286.

Kroodsma, D. E. (1972). Variations in songs of Vesper sparrows in Oregon.Wilson Bulletin 84,173–178.

Kroodsma, D. E. (1974). Song learning, dialects, and dispersal in the Bewick’s wren. Z.

Tierpsychol. 35, 352–380.

Kroodsma, D. E. (1977). Correlates of song organization among North American wrens. Am.

Nat. 111, 995–1008.

Kroodsma, D. E. (1985a). Limited dispersal between dialects? Hypotheses testable in the field.

Behav. Brain Sci. 8, 108–109.Kroodsma, D. E. (1985b). Geographic variation in songs of the Bewick’s wren: A search for

correlations with avifaunal complexity. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 16, 143–150.

Kroodsma, D. E. (1996). Ecology of passerine song development. In ‘‘Ecology and Evolution

of Acoustic Communication in Birds’’ (D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, Eds.), pp. 3–19.

Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.

Kroodsma, D. E., and Byers, B. E. (1991). The function(s) of bird song.Am. Zool. 31, 318–328.

Kroodsma, D. E., and Verner, J. (1978). Complex singing behaviours amongCistothoruswrens.

Auk 95, 703–716.Kroodsma, D. E., Baker, M. C., Baptista, L. F., and Petrinovich, L. (1985). Vocal ‘‘dialects’’ in

Nuttall’s white‐crowned sparrow. Curr. Ornithol. 2, 103–133.

Kroodsma, D. E., Houlihan, P. W., Fallon, P. A., andWells, J. A. (1997). Song development by

grey catbirds. Anim. Behav. 54, 457–464.Kroodsma, D. E., Byers, B. E., Halkin, S. L., Hill, C., Minis, D., Bolsinger, J. R., Dawson, J.‐A.,

Donelan, E., Farrington, J., Gill, F. B., Houlihan, P., Innes, D., et al. (1999). Geographic

variation in black‐capped chickadee songs and singing behavior. Auk 116, 387–402.Lachlan, R. F., and Servedio, M. R. (2004). Song learning accelerates allopatric speciation.

Evolution 58, 2049–2063.

Lachlan, R. F., and Slater, P. J. B. (2003). Song learning by chaffinches: How accurate, and

from where? Anim. Behav. 65, 957–969.Lachlan, R. F., Janik, V., and Slater, P. J. B. (2004). The evolution of conformity‐enforcing

behaviour in cultural communication systems. Anim. Behav. 68, 561–570.

Lack, D. (1947). ‘‘Darwin’s Finches.’’ Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Laiolo, P., and Tella, J. L. (2005). Habitat fragmentation affects culture transmission: Patterns

of song matching in Dupont’s lark. J. Appl. Ecol. 42, 1183–1193.

Leader, N., Wright, J., and Yom‐Tov, Y. (2000). Microgeographic song dialects in the orange‐tufted sunbird (Nectarinia osea). Behaviour 137, 1613–1627.

Leader, N., Wright, J., and Yom‐Tov, Y. (2002). Dialect discrimination by male orange‐tuftedsunbirds (Nectarinia osea): Reactions to own vs. neighbor dialects. Ethology 108, 367–376.

Leader, N., Wright, J. W., and Yom‐Tov, Y. (2005). Acoustic properties of two urban song

dialects in the orange‐tufted sunbird (Nectarinia osea). Auk 122, 231–245.Lemon, R. E. (1967). The response of cardinals to songs of different dialects.Anim. Behav. 15,

538–545.

Lemon, R. E. (1975). How birds develop song dialects. Condor 77, 385–406.

Lemon, R. E. (1985). Functional studies in bird song. Behav. Brain Sci. 8, 109–110.Liou, L. W., and Price, T. D. (1994). Speciation by reinforcement of premating isolation.

Evolution 48, 1451–1459.

Livingston, F. S., White, S. A., andMooney, R. (2000). SlowNMDA‐EPSCs at synapses criticalfor song development are not required for song learning in zebra finches. Nat. Neuros-

ciences 3, 482–488.

Page 49: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 451

Lougheed, S. C., and Handford, P. (1992). Vocal dialects and the structure of geograph-

ic variation in morphological and allozymic characters in the rufous‐collared sparrow

Zonotrichia capensis. Evolution 46, 1443–1456.MacDonald, I. F., Kempster, B., Zanette, L., and MacDougall‐Shackleton, S. A. (2006). Early

nutritional stress impairs development of a song‐control brain region in both male and

female juvenile song sparrows (Melospiza melodia) at the onset of song learning. Proc.

Roy. Soc. B 273, 2559–2564.MacDougall‐Shackleton, E.A., andMacDougall‐Shackleton, S. A. (2001). Cultural and genetic

evolution in mountain white‐crowned sparrows: Song dialects are associated with popula-

tion structure. Evolution 55, 2568–2575.MacDougall‐Shackleton, E. A., Derryberry, E. P., and Hahn, T. P. (2002). Nonlocal male

mountain white‐crowned sparrows have lower paternity and higher parasite loads than

males singing local dialect. Behav. Ecol. 13, 682–689.

Marler, P. (1957). Specific distinctiveness in the communication signals of birds. Behaviour 11,13–19.

Marler, P. (1960). Bird song and mate selection. In ‘‘Animal Sounds and Communication’’

(W. E. Lanyon and W. N. Tavolga, Eds.), pp. 348–367. American Institute of Biological

Science, Washington, DC.

Marler, P., and Peters, S. (1987). A sensitive period for song acquisition in the song sparrow,

Melospiza melodia: A case of age‐limited learning. Ethology 76, 89–100.

Marler, P., and Peters, S. (1988). The role of song phonology and syntax in vocal learning

preferences in the song sparrow, Melospiza melodia. Ethology 77, 125–149.Marler, P., and Tamura, M. (1962). Song ‘‘dialects’’ in three populations of white‐crowned

sparrows. Condor 64, 368–377.

Marler, P., and Tamura, M. (1964). Culturally transmitted patterns of vocal behavior in

sparrows. Science 146, 1483–1486.

Marler, P., Mundinger, P., Waser, M. S., and Lutjen, A. (1972). Effects of acoustical stimulation

and deprivation on song development in red‐winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus).

Anim. Behav. 20, 586–606.Martens, J., and Kessler, P. (2000). Territorial song and song neighbourhoods in scarlet

rosefinch Carpodacus erythrinus. J. Avian Biol. 31, 399–411.

Martin, P. R., and Martin, T. E. (2001). Behavioral interactions between coexisting species:

Song playback experiments with wood warblers. Ecology 82, 207–218.Martins, E. P. (1996). ‘‘Phylogenies and the Comparative Method in Animal Behavior.’’

Oxford University Press, New York.

Masataka, N. (1988). The response of red‐chested moustached tamarins to long calls from their

natal and alien populations. Anim. Behav. 36, 55–61.

Mayr, E. (1942). ‘‘Systematics and the Origin of Species.’’ Columbia University Press,

New York.

McGregor, P. K. (1980). Song dialects in the corn bunting (Emberiza calandra).Z. Tierpsychol.

54, 285–297.

McGregor, P. K. (1983). The response of corn buntings to playback of dialects. Z. Tierpsychol.

62, 256–260.

McGregor, P. K. (1985). Song dialects: What has to be explained, and with what? Behav. Brain

Sci. 8, 110.

McGregor, P. K., and Krebs, J. R. (1984). Song learning and deceptive mimicry. Anim. Behav.

32, 280–287.

McGregor, P. K., and Thompson, D. B. A. (1988). Constancy and change in local dialects of the

corn bunting. Ornis Scand. 19, 153–159.

Page 50: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

452 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

McGregor, P. K., Walford, V. R., and Harper, D. G. C. (1988). Song inheritance and mating in

a songbird with local dialects. Bioacoustics 1, 129–130.

Milligan, M. M. (1966). Vocal responses of white‐crowned sparrows to recordings of their own

and another species. Anim. Behav. 14, 356–361.

Milligan, M. M., and Verner, J. (1971). Inter‐population song dialect discrimination in the

white‐crowned sparrow. Condor 73, 208–213.

Møller, A. P. (1982). Song dialects in a population of yellowhammers Emberiza citrinella in

Denmark. Ornis Scand. 13, 239–246.

Molles, L. E., and Vehrencamp, S. L. (2001). Songbird cheaters pay a retaliation cost: Evidence

for auditory conventional signals. Proc. Roy. Soc. B 268, 2013–2019.Mork, K. (1974). Ringing results for redwing Turdus iliacus in Norway. Sterna 13, 77–107.

Mundinger, P. C. (1975). Song dialects and colonization in the house finch, Carpodacus

mexicanus, on the East Coast. Condor 77, 407–422.

Mundinger, P. C. (1982). Microgeographic and macrogeographic variation in the acquired

vocalizations of birds. In ‘‘Acoustic Communication in Birds’’ (D. E. Kroodsma and

E. H. Miller, Eds.), Vol. 2, pp. 147–208. Academic Press, New York.

Naguib,M., Hammerschmidt, K., andWirth, J. (2001).Microgeographic variation, habitat effects

and individual signature cues in calls of chiffchaffs Phylloscopus collybita canarensis.

Ethology 107, 341–355.

Nelson, D. A. (1998). Geographic variation in song of Gambel’s white‐crowned sprrow.

Behaviour 135, 321–342.

Nelson, D. A. (1999). Ecological influences on vocal development in the white‐crownedsparrow. Anim. Behav. 58, 21–36.

Nelson, D. A. (2000). Song overproduction, selective attrition and song dialects in the white‐crowned sparrow. Anim. Behav. 60, 887–898.

Nelson, D. A., and Marler, P. (1990). The perception of birdsong and an ecological concept of

signal space. In ‘‘Comparative Perception, Complex Signals’’ (W. C. Stebbins and

M. A. Berkley, Eds.), Vol. 2, pp. 443–478. John Wiley and Sons, New York.

Nelson, D. A., and Soha, J. A. (2004). Perception of geographical variation in song by male

Puget Sound white‐crowned sparrows, Zonotrichia leucophrys pugetensis. Anim. Behav.

68, 395–405.

Nelson, D. A., Marler, P., and Palleroni, A. (1995). A comparative approach to vocal learning:

Intraspecific variation in the learning process. Anim. Behav. 50, 83–97.Nelson, D. A., Whaling, C., and Marler, P. (1996a). The capacity for song memorization varies

in populations of the same species. Anim. Behav. 52, 379–387.

Nelson, D. A., Marler, P., and Morton, M. L. (1996b). Overproduction in song development:

An evolutionary correlate with migration. Anim. Behav. 51, 1127–1140.

Nelson, D. A., Hallberg, K. I., and Soha, J. A. (2004). Cultural evolution of Puget sound white‐crowned sparrow song dialects. Ethology 110, 879–908.

Noor, M. A. F. (1999). Reinforcement and other consequences of sympatry. Heredity 83,503–508.

Nottebohm, F. (1969). The song of the chingolo, Zonotrichia capensis, in Argentina: Descrip-

tion and evaluation of a system of dialects. Condor 71, 299–315.

Nottebohm, F. (1972). The origins of vocal learning. Am. Nat. 106, 116–140.Nottebohm, F. (1975). Continental patterns of song variability in Zonotrichia capensis: Some

possible ecological correlates. Am. Nat. 109, 605–624.

Nottebohm, F. (1985). Sound transmission, signal salience, and song dialects. Behav. Brain Sci.

8, 112–113.Nowicki, S. (1987). Vocal‐tract resonances in oscine bird sound production: Evidence from

birdsongs in a helium atmosphere. Nature 325, 53–55.

Page 51: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 453

Nowicki, S., and Marler, P. (1988). How do birds sing? Music Percept. 5, 391–426.

Nowicki, S., and Podos, J. (1993). Complexity, coupling, and contingency in the production of

birdsong. In ‘‘Perspectives in Ethology, Vol. 10: Behavior and Evolution’’ (P. P. G. Bateson,

P. Klopfer, and N. Thompson, Eds.), pp. 159–186. Plenum Press, New York.

Nowicki, S., Westneat, M. W., and Hoese, W. J. (1992). Birdsong: Motor function and the

evolution of communication. Sem. Neurosci. 4, 385–390.

Nowicki, S., Peters, S., and Podos, J. (1998). Song learning, early nutrition, and sexual selection

in songbirds. Am. Zool. 38, 179–190.

Nowicki, S., Searcy, W. A., and Peters, S. (2002). Brain development, song learning and mate

choice in birds: A review and experimental test of the ‘‘nutritional stress hypothesis.’’

J. Comp. Physiol. A. 188, 1003–1014.

O’Loghlen, A. L. (1995). Delayed access to local songs prolongs vocal development in dialect

populations of Brown‐headed Cowbirds. Condor 97, 402–414.

O’Loghlen,A.L., andRothstein, S. I. (1993). An extreme example of delayed vocal development:

Song learning in a population of wild brown‐headed cowbirds. Anim. Behav. 46, 293–304.

O’Loghlen, A. L., and Rothstein, S. I. (1995). Culturally correct song dialects are correlated

with male age and female song preferences in wild populations of brown‐headed cowbirds.

Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 36, 251–259.Orejuela, J. E., and Morton, M. L. (1975). Song dialects in several populations of mountain

white‐crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys oriantha) in the Sierra Nevada. Condor

77, 145–153.

Orr, M. R., and Smith, T. B. (1998). Ecology and speciation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 13, 502–506.Panhuis, T. M., Butlin, R., Zuk, M., and Tregenza, T. (2001). Sexual selection and speciation.

Trends Ecol. Evol. 16, 364–371.

Patek, S. N., Baio, J. E., Fisher, B. L., and Suarez, A. V. (2006). Multifunctionality and

mechanical origins: Ballistic jaw propulsion in trap‐jaw ants. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

103, 12787–12792.

Patten, M. A., Rotenberry, J. T., and Zuk, M. (2004). Habitat selection, acoustic adaptation,

and the evolution of reproductive isolation. Evolution 58, 2114–2155.Payne, R. B. (1973). ‘‘Behavior, Mimetic Songs and Song Dialects and Relationships of the

Parasitic Indigobirds (Vidua) of Africa.’’ American Ornithologists’ Union (Allen Press,

Inc.), Lawrence Kansas.

Payne, R. B. (1981). Population structure and social behavior: Models for testing the ecological

significance of song dialects in birds. In ‘‘Natural Selection and Social Behavior: Recent

Research and New Theory’’ (R. D. Alexander and D. W. Tinkle, Eds.), pp. 108–120.

Chiron Press, New York.

Payne, R. B. (1982). Ecological consequences of song matching: Breeding success and intra-

specific song mimicry in indigo buntings. Ecology 63, 401–411.

Payne, R. B. (1983). The social context of song mimicry: Song matching dialects in indigo

buntings (Passerina cyanea). Anim. Behav. 31, 788–805.Payne, R. B. (1986). Bird songs and avian systematics. In ‘‘Current Ornithology’’

(R. J. Johnston, Ed.), pp. 87–126. Plenum Press, New York.

Payne, R. B. (1987). Song dialects and neighborhood habitats in the indigobirdsVidua chalybeata

andVidua purpurascens at Lochinvar National Park, Zambia. J. Field Ornithol. 58, 152–170.Payne, R. B. (1996). Song traditions in indigo buntings: Origin, improvisatin, dispersal, and

extinction in cultural evolution. In ‘‘Ecology and Evoluton of Acoustic Communication in

Birds’’ (D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, Eds.), pp. 198–220. Cornell University Press,

Ithaca, NY.

Payne, R. B., and Payne, K. (1977). Social organization and mating success in local song

populations of Village Indigobirds, Vidua chalybeata. Z. Tierpsychol. 45, 113–173.

Page 52: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

454 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

Payne, R. B., and Payne, L. L. (1997). Field observations, experimental design, and the time and

place of learning bird songs. In ‘‘Social Influences onVocal Development’’ (C. T. Snowden

and M. Hausberger, Eds.), pp. 57–84. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Payne, R. B., andWestneat, D. F. (1988). A genetic and behavioral analysis of mate choice and

song neighborhoods in indigo buntings. Evolution 42, 935–947.

Payne, R. B., Payne, L. L., Woods, J. L., and Sorenson, M. D. (2000). Imprinting and the origin

of parasite‐host species associations in brood‐parasitic indigobirds, Vidua chalybeata.

Anim. Behav. 59, 69–81.

Petren, K., Grant, P. R., Grant, B. R., and Keller, L. F. (2005). Comparative landscape genetics

and the adaptive radiation of Darwin’s finches: The role of peripheral isolation.Mol. Ecol.

14, 2943–2957.

Petrinovich, L., and Patterson, T. (1981). The responses of white‐crowned sparrows to songs of

different dialects and subspecies. Z. Tierpsychol. 57, 1–14.

Petrinovich, L., Patterson, T., and Baptista, L. F. (1981). Song dialects as barriers to dispersal:

A re‐evaluation. Evolution 35, 180–188.

Podos, J. (1996). Motor constraints on vocal development in a songbird. Anim. Behav. 51,

1061–1070.

Podos, J. (1997). A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a

songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution 51, 537–551.

Podos, J. (2001). Correlated evolution of morphology and vocal signal structure in Darwin’s

finches. Nature 409, 185–188.

Podos, J. (2007). Discrimination of geographical song variants by Darwin’s finches. Anim.

Behav. 73, 833–844.

Podos, J., and Hendry, A. P. (2006). The biomechanics of ecological speciation. In ‘‘Ecology

and Biomechanics: A Mechanical Approach to the Ecology of Animals and Plants’’

(A. Herrel, T. Speck, and N. Rowe, Eds.), pp. 301–321. CRC Press, Boca Raton.

Podos, J., and Nowicki, S. (2004a). Performance limits on birdsong. In ‘‘Nature’s Music: The

Science of Birdsong’’ (P. Marler and H. Slabbekoorn, Eds.), pp. 318–342. Elsevier

Academic Press, San Diego, CA.

Podos, J., and Nowicki, S. (2004b). Beaks, adaptation, and vocal evolution in Darwin’s finches.

Bioscience 54, 501–510.

Podos, J., Nowicki, S., and Peters, S. (1999). Permissiveness in the learning and development of

song syntax in swamp sparrows. Anim. Behav. 58, 93–103.Podos, J., Southall, J. A., and Rossi‐Santos, M. R. (2004a). Vocal mechanics in Darwin’s

finches: Correlation of beak gape and song frequency. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 607–619.

Podos, J., Huber, S. K., and Taft, B. (2004b). Bird song: The interface of evolution and

mechanism. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 35, 55–87.

Powys, V. (1995). Regional variation in the territorial songs of superb lyrebirds in the central

tablelands of New South Wales. Emu 95, 280–289.

Price, J. J., and Lanyon, S. M. (2002). Reconstructing the evolution of complex bird song in the

oropendolas. Evolution 56, 1514–1529.

Price, J. J., and Lanyon, S. M. (2004). Patterns of song evolution and sexual selection in the

oropendolas and caciques. Behav. Ecol. 15, 485–497.

Ptacek, M. B. (2000). The role of mating preferences in shaping interspecific divergence in

mating signals in vertebrates. Behav. Processes 51, 111–134.

Ratcliffe, L. M. (1981). Species recognition in Darwin’s ground finches (GeospizaGould) PhD

thesis. McGill University, Montreal Canada.

Ratcliffe, L. M., and Grant, P. R. (1985). Species recognition in Darwin’s finches (Geospiza

Gould). III. Male responses to playback of different song types, dialects, and heterospe-

cific songs. Anim. Behav. 33, 290–307.

Page 53: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 455

Read, A. F., and Weary, D. M. (1992). The evolution of bird song: Comparative analyses. Phil.

Trans. R. Soc. London B. 338, 165–187.

Rice, W. R., and Hostert, E. E. (1993). Laboratory experiments on speciation: What have we

learned in 40 years? Evolution 47, 1637–1653.

Rich, T. (1981). Microgeographic variation in the song of the sage sparrow. Condor 83,

113–119.

Riebel, K. (2003). The ‘‘mute’’ sex revisited: Vocal production and perception learning in

female songbirds. Adv. Study Behav. 33, 49–86.

Riede, T., Suthers, R. A., Fletcher, N. H., and Blevins, W. E. (2006). Songbirds tune their vocal

tract to the fundamental frequency of their song. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 103,5543–5548.

Robinson, F. N., and Curtis, H. S. (1996). The vocal displays of the lyrebirds (Menuridae).Emu

96, 258–275.

Roff, D. A. (1992). ‘‘The Evolution of Life Histories: Theory and Analysis.’’ Chapman and

Hall, New York.

Rothstein, S. I. (1980). The preening invitation or head‐down display of parasitic cowbirds: 2.

Experimental analysis and evidence for behavioral mimicry. Behaviour 75, 148–184.

Rothstein, S. I., and Fleischer, R. C. (1987). Vocal dialects and their possible relation to honest

status signaling in the brown‐headed cowbird. Condor 89, 1–23.

Rothstein, S. I., Verner, J., and Farmer, C. (1999). Cowbird vocalizations: An overview and the

use of playbacks to enhance cowbird detestability. In ‘‘Ecology and Management of

Cowbirds and Their Hosts’’ (J. N. M. Smith, T. L. Cook, S. K. Robinson, S. I. Rothstein,

and S. G. Sealy, Eds.), pp. 69–81. University of Texas Press, Austin Texas.

Ruegg, K., Slabbekoorn, H., Clegg, S., and Smith, T. B. (2006). Divergence in mating sig-

nals correlates with ecological variation in the migratory songbird, Swainson’s thrush

(Catharus ustulatus). Mol. Ecol. 15, 3147–3156.

Rundle, H. D., Nagel, L., Boughman, J. W., and Schluter, D. (2000). Natural selection and

parallel speciation in sympatric sticklebacks. Science 287, 306–308.

Rutkowska‐Guz, J. M., and Osiejuk, T. S. (2004). Song structure and variation in yellow-

hammers Emberiza citrinella from western Poland. Polish J. Ecol. 52, 333–345.

Ryan, M. J. (1986). Neuroanatomy influences speciation rates among anurans. Proc. Natl.

Acad. Sci. USA 83, 1379–1382.

Ryan, M. J., and Brenowitz, E. A. (1985). The role of body size, phylogeny, and ambient noise

in the evolution of bird song. Am. Nat. 126, 87–100.

Saetre, G. P., Moum, T., Bures, S., Kral, M., Adamjan, M., and Moreno, J. (1997). A sexually

selected character displacement in flycatchers reinforces premating isolation. Nature 387,589–592.

Schluter, D. (2000). ‘‘ The Ecology of Adaptive Radiation.’’ Oxford University Press, Oxford.

Schluter, D. (2001). Ecology and the origin of species. Trends Ecol. Evol. 16, 372–380.

Searcy,W.A. (1992).Measuring responses of female birds tomale song. In ‘‘Playback and Studies

of Animal Communication’’ (P. K. McGregor, Ed.), pp. 175–189. Plenum, New York.

Searcy, W. A., and Andersson, M. (1986). Sexual selection and the evolution of song. Annu.

Rev. Ecol. Syst. 17, 507–533.

Searcy, W. A., and Yasukawa, K. (1996). Song and female choice. In ‘‘Ecology and Evolution

of Acoustic Communication in Birds’’ (D. E. Kroodsma and E. H. Miller, Eds.),

pp. 454–473. Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.

Searcy, W. A., Nowicki, S., and Hughes, M. (1997). The response of male and female song

sparrows to geographic variation in song. Condor 99, 651–657.Searcy, W. A., Nowicki, S., Hughes, M., and Peters, S. (2002). Geographic song discrimination

in relation to dispersal distances in song sparrows. Am. Nat. 159, 221–230.

Page 54: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

456 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

Seddon, N. (2005). Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives vocal evolution in

neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution 59, 200–215.

Seitz, A., Reh, W., Veith, M., and Wolfes, R. (1994). Gene flow between and within natural

vertebrate populations. Anim. Biotech. 5, 155–168.

Sibley, C. G., and Monroe, B. L. J. (1990). ‘‘Distribution and Taxonomy of the Birds of the

World.’’ Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.

Sick, H. (1939). Uber die Dialektbildung beim ‘‘Regenruf’’ des Buchfinken. J. Ornithol. 87,568–592.

Slabbekoorn, H. (2004). Singing in the natural environment: The ecology of birdsong.

In ‘‘Nature’s Music: The Science of Birdsong’’ (P. Marler and H. Slabbekoorn, Eds.).

Elsevier Academic Press, San Diego, CA.

Slabbekoorn, H., and Smith, T. B. (2002a). Bird song, ecology and speciation. Phil. Trans. R.

Soc. London B 357, 493–503.

Slabbekoorn, H., and Smith, T. B. (2002b). Habitat‐dependent song divergence in the little

greenbul: An analysis of environmental selection pressures on acoustic signals. Evolution

56, 1849–1858.

Slabbekoorn, H., de Kort, S., and ten Cate, C. (1999). Comparative analysis of perch coo

vocalizations in Streptopelia doves. Auk 116, 737–748.Slater, P. J. B. (1985). White rats and general theories. Behav. Brain Sci. 8, 115–116.

Slater, P. J. B. (1986). The cultural transmission of bird song. Trends Ecol. Evol. 1, 94–97.

Slater, P. J. B. (1989). Bird song learning: Causes and consequences.Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 1, 19–46.

Slater, P. J. B., Clements, F. A., and Goodfellow, D. J. (1984). Local and regional variations in

chaffinch song and the question of dialects. Behaviour 88, 76–97.

Soha, J. A., Nelson, D. A., and Parker, P. G. (2004). Genetic analysis of song dialect popula-

tions in Puget Sound white‐crowned sparrows. Behav. Ecol. 15, 636–646.Sorjonen, J. (2001). Long‐term constancy of two rain‐call dialects of the Chaffinch Fringilla

coelebs in Finnish and Russian Karelia: A consequence of site‐fidelity? Ornis Fennica 78,

73–82.

Spencer, K. A., Buchanan, K. L., Goldsmith, A. R., and Catchpole, C. K. (2003). Song as an

honest signal of developmental stress in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata). Horm.

Behav. 44, 132–139.

Spencer, K. A., Buchanan, K. L., Goldsmith, A. R., and Catchpole, C. K. (2004). Developmen-

tal stress, social rank and song complexity in the European starling (Sturnus vulgaris).

Proc. Roy. Soc. B 271, S121–S123.

Spencer, K. A., Buchanan, K. L., Leitner, S., Goldsmith, A. R., and Catchpole, C. K. (2005).

Parasites affect song complexity and neural development in a songbird. Proc. Roy. Soc. B

272, 2037–2043.

Suthers, R. A. (2004). How birds sing and why it matters. In ‘‘Nature’s Music: The Science of

Birdsong’’ (P. Marler and H. Slabbekoorn, Eds.), pp. 272–295. Elsevier Academic Press,

San Diego, CA.

Suthers, R. A., and Goller, F. (1997). Motor correlates of vocal diversity in songbirds.

In ‘‘Current Ornithology’’ (V. Nolan Jr., E. D. Ketterson, and C. F. Thompson, Eds.),

pp. 235–288. Plenum Press, New York.

Suthers, R. A., Goller, F., and Pytte, C. (1999). The neuromuscular control of birdsong. Phil.

Trans. R. Soc. London B 354, 927–939.

Szekely, T., Catchpole, C. K., DeVoogd, A., Marchl, Z., and DeVoogd, T. J. (1996). Evolu-

tionary changes in a song control area of the brain (HVC) are associated with evolutionary

changes in song repertoire among European warblers (Sylviidae). Proc. Roy. Soc. London

B 263, 607–610.

Page 55: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

GEOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN BIRDSONG 457

Teather, K. L., and Robertson, R. J. (1986). Pair bonds and factors influencing the diversity of

mating systems in brown‐headed cowbirds [Molothrus ater]. Condor 88, 63–69.

ten Cate, C. (2000). How learning mechanisms might affect evolutionary processes. Trends

Ecol. Evol. 15, 179–181.

ten Cate, C. (2004). Birdsong and Evolution. In ‘‘Nature’s Music: The Science of Birdsong’’

(P. Marler and H. Slabbekoorn, Eds.), pp. 296–317. Elsevier Academic Press,

San Diego, CA.

Thielcke, G. (1969). Geographic variation in bird vocalizations. In ‘‘Bird Vocalizations: Their

Relations to Current Problems in Biology and Psychology’’ (R. A. Hinde, Ed.),

pp. 311–339. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Thielcke, G. (1986). Constant proportions of mixed singers in tree creeper populations (Certhia

familiaris). Ethology 72, 154–164.

Thielcke, G. (1987). Long‐term consistency of short‐toed tree creeper dialect Certhia brachy-

dactyla. J. Ornithol. 128, 171–180.Thielcke, G. (1988a). Buchfinken (Fringilla coelebs) eliminieren erlernte gesange vom baum-

pieper (Anthus trivialis). Vogelwarte 34, 319–336.

Thielcke, G. (1988b). Neue befunde bestaen Baron PERNAUs (1660–1731) angaben uber

lautau ßerungen des buchfinken (Figilla coelebs). J. Ornithol. 129, 55–70.Thielcke, G. (1989). Experiments on sensitive phases and song variation in the chaffinch

Fringilla coelebs. J. Ornithol. 130, 435–454.

Thielcke, G., and Wusternberg, K. (1985). Experiments on the origin of dialects in the short‐toed tree creeper Certhia brachydactyla. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 16, 195–201.

Todt, D., and Naguib, M. (2000). Vocal interactions in birds: The use of song as a model in

communication. Adv. Study Behav. 29, 247–296.

Trainer, J. M. (1983). Changes in song dialect distributions and microgeographic variation in

song of white‐crowned sparrows Zonotrichia leucophrys nuttalli. Auk 100, 568–582.

Trainer, J. M. (1988). Singing organization during aggressive interactions among male yellow‐rumped caciques. Condor 90, 681–688.

Trainer, J. M. (1989). Cultural evolution in song dialects of yellow‐rumped caciques in Panama.

Ethology 80, 190–204.

Trainer, J. M., and Parsons, R. J. (2002). Delayed vocal maturation in polygynous Yellow‐rumped Caciques. Wilson Bull. 114, 249–254.

Trainer, J. M., and Peltz, B. S. (1995). Song repertoire of the bobolink: A reassessment.

Ethology 102, 50–62.

Tubaro, P. L., and Segura, E. T. (1994). Dialect differences in the song of Zonotrichia capensis

in the southern Pampas: A test of the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Condor 96,1084–1088.

Van Buskirk, J. (1997). Independent evolution of song structure and note structure in Ameri-

can wood warblers. Proc. Roy. Soc. London B 264, 755–761.

Vermeij, G. J. (1988). The evolutionary success of passerines: A question of semantics? Syst.

Zool. 37, 69–71.

Ward, R. (1966). Regional variation in the song of the carolina chickadee. Living Bird 5,

127–150.

Ward, W. V. (1964). The songs of the Apapane. Living Bird 3, 97–117.Warren, P. S. (2000). Song dialects in the bronzed cowbird: A model system for testing

evolutionary hypotheses. In ‘‘Integrative Biology,’’ p. 267. University of Texas, Austin,

TX.

Warren, P. S. (2002). Geographic variation and dialects in songs of the bronzed cowbird

(Molothrus aeneus). Auk 119, 349–361.

Page 56: The Evolution of Geographic Variation in Birdsong...evolution in Z. leucophrys nuttalli is that of limited or biased dispersal. Toward this end, Marler and Tamura (1962) suggested

458 JEFFREY PODOS AND PAIGE S. WARREN

Warren, P. S. (2003). Winter dialects in the bronzed cowbird and their relationship to breeding‐season dialects. Anim. Behav. 65, 1169–1178.

Waser, P. M. (1985). Are dialects epiphenomena? Behav. Brain Sci. 8, 117.Weatherhead, P. J., and Forbes, M. R. L. (1994). ‘‘Natal philopatry in passerine birds: Genetic

or ecological influences?’’ Behavioral Ecology 5, 426–433.

Weilgart, L., and Whitehead, H. (1997). Group‐specific dialects and geographical variation in

coda repertoire in South Pacific sperm whales. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 40, 277–285.Wells, M. M., and Henry, C. S. (1998). Songs, reproductive isolation, and speciation in cryptic

species of insects: A case study using green lacewings. In ‘‘Endless Forms: Species and

Speciation’’ (D. J. Howard and S. B. Berlocher, Eds.), pp. 217–233. Oxford University

Press, New York, NY.

West‐Eberhard, M. J. (1983). Sexual selection, social competition, and speciation. Q. Rev.

Biology 58, 155–183.

Westneat, M. W., Long, J. H. Jr., Hoese, W., and Nowicki, S. (1993). Kinematics of birdsong:

Functional correlation of cranial movements and acoustic features in sparrows. J. Exp.

Biol. 182, 147–171.

White, F. W. G. (1985). Microgeographic variation in the songs of the olive whistler in

Kosciusko national park. Emu 85, 181–187.White, F. W. G. (1986). Summer‐winter movements of olive whistlers Pachycephala olivacea in

the Snowy mountains, New South Wales, Australia. Corella 10, 125–126.

White, F. W. G. (1987). Macrogeographic variation in the olive whistler in Australia. Emu 87,

14–25.

Wiens, J. A. (1982). Song pattern variation in the sage sparrow (Amphispiza belli): Dialects or

epiphenomena? Auk 99, 208–229.

Wiley, R. H. (1971). Song groups in a singing assembly of little hermits. Condor 73, 28–35.Wiley, R. H. (1991). Associations of song properties with habitats for territorial oscine birds of

eastern North America. Am. Nat. 138, 973–993.

Wiley, R. H., and Richards, D. G. (1978). Physical constraints on acoustic communication in

the atmosphere: Implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behav. Ecol.

Sociobiol. 3, 69–94.

Williams, J. M., and Slater, P. J. B. (1990). Modeling bird song dialects: The influence of

repertoire size and numbers of neighbours. J. Theor. Biol. 145, 487–496.

Wright, T. F. (1996). Regional dialects in the contact call of a parrot. Proc. Roy. Soc. London B

263, 867–872.

Wright, T. F., andWilkinson, G. S. (2001). Population genetic structure and vocal dialects in an

amazon parrot. Proc. Roy. Soc. London B 268, 609–616.Wright, T. F., Rodriguez, A. M., and Fleischer, R. C. (2005). Vocal dialects, sex‐biased

dispersal, and microsatellite population structure in the parrot Amazona auropalliata.

Mol. Ecol. 14, 1197–1205.

Yokel, D. A. (1986). Monogamy and brood parasitism: An unlikely pair. Anim. Behav. 34,1348–1358.

Zink, R. M., and Barrowclough, G. F. (1984). Allozymes and song dialects: A reassessment.

Evolution 38, 444–448.

Zink, R. M., Dittmann, D. L., and Rootes, W. L. (1991). Mitochondrial DNA variation and the

phylogeny of Zonotrichia. Auk 108, 578–584.