the effects of symmetrical and asymmetrical social interaction on children’s logical inferences -...

7
British Joiirnal of Developmental Psychology (1998), 16, 175-181 0 1998 The British Psychological Society Printed in Greai Britain 175 The effects of symmetrical and asymmetrical social interaction on children’s logical inferences Antonio Roazzi Universia’nde Federal de Pmambuco, Mesirado em Psicologia, CFCH 8 ana’nr, Cia’nde Universitliria, Riia Acadhico Helio Ramos, sln, 50670-901, Recife-PE, Brazil. Peter Bryant Dtpariment of Experimental Psychology, O x f r d University, UK We report an intervention experiment on the effects of social interaction on 4- and 5-year-old children’s performance in a simple logical, inferential task. We found that children performing badly in a logical inferential task who then had the experience of cooperating with others more advanced than themselves did better in this task in a post-test given three days later and in a further test given after a three-week interval. We also found that children who had the experience of cooperating with others who had also performed badly (at the same level) in the pre-test did not get better at making logical inferences. We conclude that social interaction between less and more advanced children enhances the less advanced children’s ability to make logical inferences, and we argue that this is probably due to the effects of discussion and of agreement, rather than to social conflict, during the intervention period. There is considerable evidence that young children’s performance in two central Piagetian tasks, conservation and perspective taking, improves as a result of social interaction with more advanced peers (e.g. Ames & Murray, 1982; Carugati, 1984; Carugati, De Paolis & Mugny, 1979; Carugati & Mugny, 1985; Doise & Mugny, 1981, Light, Foot, Colbourn & McClelland, 1987; Miller & Brownell, 1975; Mugny, Giroud & Doise, 1979, Perret- Clermont, 1980; Perret-Clermont & Brossard, 1988; Silverman & Geiringer, 1973). The effect is often used to support the idea that logical development is heavily influenced by social experiences (Moro, 1986,1987,1991; Mugny & Doise, 1978a,b; Taal & Oppenhei- mer, 1989; Weinstein & Bearison, 1985). However, the exact nature of the social experiences which cause the effect is still not clear. One unsettled issue is whether social interactions between children at the same intellectual level also have a beneficial effect. The issue is important because if ‘symmet- rical’ interactions, as interactions between children at the same level are usually called, are also effective, we could conclude that social interaction and possible social conflict per se are the crucial factor, as indeed Doise & Mugny (1981) and Perret-Clermont (1980) suggest they are. However, if children’s performance only improves after interactions with * Requests for reprints

Upload: antonio-roazzi

Post on 25-Sep-2015

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

We report an intervention experiment on the effects of social interaction on 4- and 5-year-old children’s performance in a simple logical, inferential task. We found thatchildren performing badly in a logical inferential task who then had the experience of cooperating with others more advanced than themselves did better in this task in a post-test given three days later and in a further test given after a three-week interval.We also found that children who had the experience of cooperating with others who had also performed badly (at the same level) in the pre-test did not get better at making logical inferences. We conclude that social interaction between less and moreadvanced children enhances the less advanced children’s ability to make logical inferences, and we argue that this is probably due to the effects of discussion and of agreement, rather than to social conflict, during the intervention period.

TRANSCRIPT

  • British Joiirnal of Developmental Psychology (1998), 16, 175-181 0 1998 The British Psychological Society

    Printed in Greai Britain 175

    The effects of symmetrical and asymmetrical social interaction on childrens logical inferences

    Antonio Roazzi Universiande Federal de Pmambuco, Mesirado em Psicologia, CFCH 8 ananr, Ciande Universitliria, Riia

    Acadhico Helio Ramos, sln, 50670-901, Recife-PE, Brazil.

    Peter Bryant Dtpariment of Experimental Psychology, Oxfrd University, UK

    We report an intervention experiment on the effects of social interaction on 4- and 5-year-old childrens performance in a simple logical, inferential task. We found that children performing badly in a logical inferential task who then had the experience of cooperating with others more advanced than themselves did better in this task in a post-test given three days later and in a further test given after a three-week interval. We also found that children who had the experience of cooperating with others who had also performed badly (at the same level) in the pre-test did not get better at making logical inferences. We conclude that social interaction between less and more advanced children enhances the less advanced childrens ability to make logical inferences, and we argue that this is probably due to the effects of discussion and of agreement, rather than to social conflict, during the intervention period.

    There is considerable evidence that young childrens performance in two central Piagetian tasks, conservation and perspective taking, improves as a result of social interaction with more advanced peers (e.g. Ames & Murray, 1982; Carugati, 1984; Carugati, De Paolis & Mugny, 1979; Carugati & Mugny, 1985; Doise & Mugny, 1981, Light, Foot, Colbourn & McClelland, 1987; Miller & Brownell, 1975; Mugny, Giroud & Doise, 1979, Perret- Clermont, 1980; Perret-Clermont & Brossard, 1988; Silverman & Geiringer, 1973). The effect is often used to support the idea that logical development is heavily influenced by social experiences (Moro, 1986,1987,1991; Mugny & Doise, 1978a,b; Taal & Oppenhei- mer, 1989; Weinstein & Bearison, 1985).

    However, the exact nature of the social experiences which cause the effect is still not clear. One unsettled issue is whether social interactions between children at the same intellectual level also have a beneficial effect. The issue is important because if symmet- rical interactions, as interactions between children at the same level are usually called, are also effective, we could conclude that social interaction and possible social conflict per se are the crucial factor, as indeed Doise & Mugny (1981) and Perret-Clermont (1980) suggest they are. However, if childrens performance only improves after interactions with

    * Requests for reprints

    WolverineNotaBritish Journal of Develop. Psychology Vol 16(2), 175-181 - 1998 [doi 10.1111%2Fj.2044-835x.1998.tb00917.x] Roazzi, A & Bryant, P. - The effects of symmetrical and asymmetrical social interaction on children's logical inferences

  • 176 Antonio Roazzi and Peter Bryant

    children who are at a higher level than them, a more plausible conclusion would be that young children are prepared to accept opinions about physical and spatial laws as true when these are stated by another child whom they see as more advanced than them- selves.

    The evidence on this point is inconsistent. Some studies apparently show that symmetrical social interaction does reliably improve childrens perspective taking (Doise & Mugny, 1979; Emler & Valliant, 1982), but Russell (1981a) found no such effect. Perret-Clermont (1980) and Ames & Murray (1982) report that symmetrical interaction between non-conservers does improve their performance in conservation tasks later on. On the other hand, Russell, Mills & Reiff-Musgrove (1990) did not find this with conservation of liquid, and Russell (1982a) also failed to find the effect with length conservation. Russell also reported that symmetrical interaction does not help children in transitive inference (19824, in appearance-reality (Russell & Haworth, 1988) or in class inclusion (198lb) tasks either. It is surprising that such an important question has been left to hang fire. Until we have a satisfactory answer, we can draw no firm conclusion about the claim that social construction plays an important part in childrens cognitive development. More evidence is needed and with a wider range of tasks.

    We now report an experiment in which we looked at the effect of symmetrical and asymmetrical social interaction on childrens performance in a newly devised inference task. In this task children had to make a deductive inference about number. They were given a balance scale and boxes with different numbers of identical objects, and in some boxes the number of objects was identified, while in others it was not. The child was shown that a box A, in which the number was not identified, was heavier for instance than one with three objects and lighter than one with five objects. The childs task was to infer the number in box A. This was a simple inferential task and our aim was to see whether performance in it improved as a result of social interaction between children at equal intellectual levels as well as between less and more advanced children.

    Method

    Participants We saw 90 children with an age range of4:2 to 5:5 years (mean 4:72, SD 3.43), ofwhom 45 were male and 45 female. There were four groups: one control and three experimental (called equal, close and distant). The number of participants in each group, and their mean ages, with standard deviations in months, were: control (N = 24, mean 4:8, SD 3.66), equal (N = 18, mean 4:11, SD 3.05), close (N = 24, mean 4 9 , SD 2.27) and distant (N = 24, mean 4:7, SD 3.46).

    Material W e used a balance scale with the following characteristics: beam length 16 cm, diameter of trays 6.5 cm, distance from the beam to the bottom of the plate 18 cm, and from the bottom of the plate to the base 4 cm. We also used several boxes: (a) four boxes with two, three, four and five sweets, painted; (6) two boxes with four to five sweets, painted, which could be opened; (c) six boxes, with transparent tops, allowing the number of sweets inside to be seen. The distribution of the sweets inside these boxes was one, two, three, four, five and six.

  • Social interaction a n d infwences 177

    Procedure The experiment consisted of a pre-test, an intervention period and two post-tests. In all four phases we used an inferential balance-scale task. The aim of this balance-scale task was to discover the childs capacity to work out how many sweets were inside a box by means of comparisons using a balance scale. The task was to judge the number of sweets inside an opaque box [target box (T)] by comparing its weight with that of two other boxes (comparative boxes 1 and 2: Cbl and Cb2) (see also Roazzi, 1988).

    The examiner began by suggesting that the child take part in a game in which helshe play the part of a buyer, and the experimenter that of a salesman.

    Having established that the child understands how the balance scale works, the experimenter began the inferential problems. In each trial the experimenter first took an opaque box, hereafter named target (T). He put the target box on the left-hand tray, then took two boxes of sweets, hereafter named comparisons 1 and 2 (Cbl and Cb2). The child knew how many sweets were in the comparison boxes, because the tops were transparent so that the sweets could be seen.

    The experimenter said: Look what happens when I put this box (Cbl) on this tray (the right-hand tray). The experimenter then took the first comparison box off and put it besides the balance scale. He put the second box (Cb2) on the right-hand tray and said: Now look what happens when I put this other box on the balance-scale. How many sweets do you think are in this box (T)? Why?

    Phases of the experiment (a) Pre-test. The aim of this phase was to identify the childrens initial level of logical inferential skills. Children were tested in eight trials. In each trial there was only one possible answer, as for example, when the target box was heavier than a comparison box with three sweets and lighter than one with five sweets (Cbl 3-Cb2 YTarget 4).

    Those children who scored badly (minimum score 1 and maximum score 2) in these pre-test trials were selected as the experimental children (E-Ch) and were randomly assigned to one of four interaction groups-equal peer interaction (equal group), close peer interaction (close group) and distant peer interaction (distant group) and no interaction (control group). The mean scores for the four groups in the pre-test were 1.72, 1.87, 1.75 and 1.79 respectively for equal, close, distant and control group. The bases for selecting the childrens partners in the equal, close and distant groups are given in the following section.

    (b) hterurtionphuse. In a second stage, a week after the pre-test, the children in the equal, close and distant groups were given an extra session in groups of three. In each group E-ch worked with two confederates (Ct).

    Distant group: The two confederates (Cfl and Cf2) had performed well in the balance-scale pre-test (minimum correct 5-maximum 8), while the third child (E-ch) had performed badly. The E-ch remained seated at the head of a table with the other two children at his side, opposite each other.

    The session consisted of 12 trials. In each trial the experimenter told the children that he was going to start the game with the boxes of sweets and the balance scale that they had used the previous week. In each trial only one of the children had to provide the answer, but in this session the other two children were allowed to help himlher. The other children could provide clues, but were not allowed to give the answer directly to the child who had to produce the solution.

    In eight of the trials, it was the E-ch who had to produce the answer: each of the Cf children had to do so on two trials.

    There was an incentive for the three children to ensure that as many as possible of the answers given in this session were correct. The children were told that, if they got the number of sweets in at least 10 boxes right, they would all get two bags of sweets; but if they failed to guess more than three boxes correctly, nobody would get anything. The experimenter took note of the childrens answers without saying whether each answer was correct or not. Only at the end of the 12 trials did the experimenter give the number of correct answers. The procedure with the target boxes and the comparison boxes was identical to that used in the pre-test.

    Closegroup: The procedure for this group was identical to that of the distant one; the only difference was

  • 178 Antonio Roazzi and Peter Bryant

    that the pre-test scores of the Cf children were closer to those of the E-ch. The minimum pre-test score of the Cf children was 3 and maximum 4.

    Equalgroup: The procedure for this group was identical to that of the distant and close groups except that the Cf childrens pre-test scores were the same as those of the E-ch (minimum 1-maximum 2).

    Controlgroup: The children in the control group were given the same 12 trials, but on their own, and they were given the same incentive to do well.

    (c) Post-test 1. test (identical to the pre-test) was carried out three days after the experimental phase.

    (d) Phase ofpost-test 2.

    The procedure in the post-tests was identical to the procedure in the pre-test. A first post-

    A second post-test (identical to the first) took place three weeks after the first.

    Results

    Table 1. Mean correct and standard deviation of each group (three experimental and one control) in each experimental phase (pre-test, post-test 1 and post-test 2)

    Experimental Control Equal Close Distant Total

    phases Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD Mean SD

    Pre-test 1.79 .42 1.72 .46 1.87 .34 1.75 .44 1.79 .41 Post-test 1 2.25 .44 2.89 .76 3.67 .70 4.04 1.16 3.23 1.07 Post-test 2 2.33 .48 3.17 .98 4.25 .99 4.37 1.34 3.56 1.31 Total 2.12 .44 2.59 .73 3.26 .68 3.38 .98 2.86 .93

    Key. Control = no interaction group; Equal = equal peer interaction group; Close = close peer interaction group; Distant = distant peer interaction group.

    Table 1 gives the results for the pre-test and the two post-tests. It shows that the three groups of children were at similar levels in the pre-test but diverged sharply in the post- tests. All three experimental groups produced better scores in the post-tests than in the pre-test, and the improvement in the scores of these three interaction groups were much greater than in the control group.

    There were also sizeable differences in the improvement shown by the three experi- mental groups. In the first post-test the children in the equal group did better than the control group but worse than the other two intervention groups in which the children had interacted with children with slightly and far higher pre-test scores than their own. This pattern of differences persisted in the second post-test. This suggests that interaction with other children with higher inferential capacities has a positive effect on the development of logical inferential skills and that this effect is a lasting one.

    The number of correct answers was analysed in a 4 (groups: equal, close, distant, control) X 3 (tests: pre-test, post-test 1, post-test 2) mixed analysis of variance. This produced a main effect of the group (F(3,86) = 23.09, p < .001). A Scheffi post hoc test showed that the control (mean 2.12) and the equal (mean 2.59) peer interaction groups achieved significantly lower performance levels than the distant interaction group (mean 3.39,p < . O l ) and than the close interaction group (mean 3.26,p < .Ol) . No difference was found between the distant and close interaction groups.

    A significant main effect of test was also found (F(2,172) = 204.22,p < .OOl) . A Scheff6

  • Social interaction and inferences 179

    post hoc tests showed that there was significantly lower performance in the pre-test (mean 1.79) than in post-test 1 (mean 3.23, p < . O l ) and post-test 2 (mean 3.56, p < .01). No difference was found between the two post-tests.

    There was also a significant group X test interaction (F(6,172) = 16.63, p < .001). A Scheffi post hor test showed no significant difference among the three groups in the pre- test. In post-test 1 significant differences were found between the control (mean 2.25) and equal (mean 2.89) groups and the two other experimental groups distant (mean, 4.04, p < .01) and close (mean 3.67,p < . O l ) ; the difference between control and equal as well as between distant and close group was not significant. The same significant differences were found in post-test 2: significant differences were found between the control (mean 2.33) and equal (mean 3.17) groups and the two other experimental groups distant (mean, 4.37, p < . O l ) and close (mean 4.25,p < . O l ) ; the difference between control and equal as well as between distant and close group were not significant.

    A comparison between the three different tests for each group showed an improvement in performance from the pre-test to the post-test 1 for the three experimental groups (p < . O l ) but not for the control group. A significant improvement from post-test 1 to post-test 2 was found only with the close group (p < .O l ) .

    Discussion

    The study showed that social interaction between less and more advanced children does improve performance of the less advanced ones on a simple logical task. These children make inferences more successfully after having done the task cooperatively with more advanced children, and the effects of this cooperation seem to last for some time. In other words this result indicates that while children are in a phase of cognitive development, the suggestions and remarks of other children (a little cleverer and also much cleverer) help to integrate the thought in a developing stage into a more coherent whole. This result is in line with those of many other studies on a variety of logical tasks such as Mugny et al. (1979) regarding length conservation, Weinstein & Bearison (1985) regarding two- dimensional space, substance, weight, continuous and discontinuous quantity, Perret-Clermont ( 1 980) regarding childrens ability to conserve number and liquid and Mugny & Doise (1978a) and Doise, Mugny & Perret-Clermont (1975) regarding the coordination of spatial perspectives.

    However our study also showed a large difference in the effects of asymmetrical and symmetrical interaction. There were slight improvements in the children who had interacted with others no more advanced than they were, but these were not significant and they were always less than in children who had interacted with their more advanced peers. This second result agrees with previous data for liquid (Russell et al., 1990) and length (Russell, 19826) conservation, for class inclusion (Russell, 198 l a ) and appearance- reality problems (Russell & Haworth, 1988), but it is inconsistent with Ames & Murrays data (1982) on conservation. Nevertheless the bulk of evidence now suggests that symmetrical interaction usually fails where asymmetrical interaction succeeds. We can thus rule out the possibility that social interaction per se is effective, and we can draw the same negative conclusion about having to be more explicit about the task in the interaction session. Both these factors applied to the symmetrical groups experiences in the interaction session, and yet their performance did not improve.

  • 180 Antonio Roazzi and Peter Biyant

    This negative result should not detract from the importance of the success of the other two interaction groups. Our study shows that social interaction can improve childrens ability to make simple logical inferences. It is unlikely that mere observation of more advanced children caused this effect. Weinstein & Bearison (1985) showed that children learned more from interacting with others than from observing them. This suggests that the children in the asymmetrical groups benefitted from the interaction through the experience of discussing the task with children who knew better than they did and being persuaded by them.

    One important difference between our methods and those used in previous investiga- tions was our emphasis on cooperation rather than on conflict. We encouraged a common interest-if at the end the children got 10 choices right, everyone would get bags of sweets. This raises doubts about the ideas of Piaget on the importance of conflict, since Piaget gives conflict, and not cooperation, a basic and crucial role in producing development. For Piaget, the child is able to develop cognitive abilities as a result mainly of internal intellectual conflicts and of the subsequent disequilibrium which originates from these conflicts. This approach was also adopted by Perret-Clermont (1980) and by Doise & Mugny (1981) to explain the effects of social interaction: both sets of authors adopted the idea that social conflict was the important factor in their intervention studies.

    The Piagetian theory of conflict as a mechanism able to cause development has been criticized (e.g. Bryant, 1982, 1985, 1986) on the grounds that conflict is an effective signal of the existence of a problem, but provides no means of solving it. As an alternative, Bryant (1986) proposed the agreement hypothesis which is that children adopt a new intellectual strategy when they see that i t leads to the same conclusion as another strategy which they use already. So they begin to measure in order to compare two sizes when they see that such measurement produces results which agree with direct comparisons of the two quantities when this is possible. In this context the child develops as a result of the comprehension of the connection between different intellectual strategies. This idea about agreement could easily be extended to explain the positive results of social interaction. Children could adopt new strategies when they see that these agree with the conclusions reached by other children, and particularly by children whom they recognize as more advanced than them.

    In conclusion, this is the first study to show that social interaction can improve childrens ability to make simple logical inferences. It also showed that this improvement takes place as a result of interaction with more advanced peers and not with peers at the same intellectual level.

    References

    Ames, G. & Murray, F. (1982). When two wrongs make a right: Promoting cognitive change by social

    Bryant, P. (1982). The role of conflict and agreement between intellectual strategies in childrens ideas

    Bryant, P. (1985). The distinction between knowing when to do a sum and knowing how to do it.

    Bryant, P. (1986). Theories about the causes of cognitive development. In P.L.C. van Geert (Ed.), Theory

    conflict. Developmental Psychology, 18, 8 9 4 8 9 7 .

    about measurement. British Joiirnal OfPsychoIogy, 73, 242-25 1.

    Educational Psychology, 5 , 207-2 15.

    Bziilding it1 Developmetltal Psychology, pp. 167-187. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

  • Social interaction and infwences 181

    Carugati, F. (1984). La nozione di conflitto in psicologia dello sviluppo. Eta Evolutiva, 19, 76-84. Carugati, F., De Paolis, P. & Mugny, G. (1979). A paradigm from the study of social interactions in

    Carugati, F. & Mugny, G. (1985). La thiorie du conflit socio-cognitif. In G. Mugny (Ed.), PsychologieSociale

    Doise, W. & Mugny, G. (1979). Individual and collective conflicts of centrations in cognitive develop-

    Doise, W. & Mugny, G. (1981). Le Dheloppment Social de Llntelligence. Paris: Inter-Editions. Doise, W., Mugny, G. & Perret-Clermont, A.N. (1975). Social interaction and the development of

    Emler, N. & Valiant, G.L. (1982). Social interaction and cognitive conflict in the development of spatial

    Light, P., Foot, T. Colbourn, C & McClelland, I. (1987). Collaborative interactions at the microcomputer

    Miller, S.A. & Brownell, C.A. (1975). Peers, persuasion, and Piaget: Dyadic interaction between conservers

    Moro, M.L.F. (1986). A construglo da inteliggncia e a aprendizagem escolar de criangas de familias de baixa

    Moro, M.L.F. (1 987). Aprendizagem Operatdria: A lnterapio Social d? Crianfa. Slo PaulolCuritiba: Cortezl

    Moro, M.L.F. (1991). Interag6es sociais e construgiio do conhecimento. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 7,

    Mugny, G. & Doise, W. (1978a). Socio-cognitive conflict and structuration of individual and collective

    Mugny, G. & Doise, W. (17786). Factores sociologicos y psicologicos del desarrollo cognitivo. Anuario de

    Mugny, G., Giroud, J.C. & Doise, W. (1979). Conflit de centrations et progrss cognitif, 11: Nouvelle

    Perret-Clermont, A.N. (1980). Social Interaction and Cognitive Development in Children. London: Academic

    Perret-Clermont, A.N. & Brossard, A. (1988). Llnteraction des Processus Cognit.$ et Sociaux h n s les

    Roazzi, A. (1988). Childrens Cognitive Skills: A Social Class Comparison. DPhil thesis, University of

    Russell, J. (1918a). Why socio-cognitive conflict may be impossible: The status of egocentric errors in the

    Russell, J. (19816). Dyadic interaction in a logical problem requiring inclusion ability. Child Development.

    Russell, J. (1982a). Cognitive conflict, transmission, and justification: Conservation attainment through

    Russell, J. (19826). Propositional attitudes. In M. Beveridge (Ed.), Children Thinking through Language, pp.

    Russell, J. & Haworth, H.M. (1988). Appearance versus reality in dyadic interaction: Evidence for a

    Russell, J., Mills, I. & Reiff-Musgrove, P. (1990). The role of symmetrical and asymmetrical social conflict

    Silverman, I.W. & Geiringer, E. (1973). Dyadic interaction and conservation induction: A test of Piagets

    Taal, M. & Oppenheimer, L. (1989). Socio-cognitive conflict and peer interaction: Development of

    Weinstein, B.D. & Bearison, D.J. (1985). Social interaction, social observation, and cognitive development

    cognitive development. Italian Journal of Psychology, 6 , 147-1 5 5.

    du Developpetnetit Cognitif. Bern: Peter Lang.

    ment. Eziropean Journal ofSocial Psychology, 7 , 105-108.

    cognitive operations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5 , 367- 383.

    coordination skills. British Journal ofPsychology, 73, 295- 303.

    keyboard. Educational Psychology, 7, 13-2 1.

    and nonconservers. Child Development, 46, 992- 997.

    renda: Continuando um debate. Cadernos de Pesquisa, 56, 66-72.

    Scientia et Labor.

    215-227.

    performances. European Journal ofsocial Psychology, 8, 181- 192.

    Psicologia, 18, 22-40.

    illustrations expirimentales. Bulletin de Psychologies, 32, 979-985.

    Press.

    Interactions. Relations Interpersonnelles et Dheloppernetit des Savoirs. Neuchatel: Delval.

    Oxford.

    dyadic performance of a spatial task. Educational Psychology. 1, 159-169.

    52,1322-1325.

    dyadic interaction. Jozirnal of Genetic Psychology, 140, 283-297.

    75-98. London: Edward Arnold.

    lingering phenomenism. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 11, 155-170.

    in cognitive change. Journal OfExpwimental Child Psychology, 49, 58-78.

    equilibrium model. Child Development, 44, 81 5-820.

    compensation. European Journal ofsocial Psychology, 19, 77-83.

    in young children. European Journal of Social Psychology, 15, 333-343.

    Received 25 January 1995; revised version received 25 April I997