the effects of music in advertising on choice behavoiur

Upload: fcortes13

Post on 03-Jun-2018

222 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/11/2019 The Effects of Music in Advertising on Choice Behavoiur

    1/9

    The Effects of Music in Advertising on Choice Behavior: A Classical Conditioning ApproachAuthor(s): Gerald J. GornReviewed work(s):Source: Journal of Marketing, Vol. 46, No. 1 (Winter, 1982), pp. 94-101Published by: American Marketing AssociationStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/1251163.

    Accessed: 11/03/2013 11:04

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at.http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    .JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    .

    American Marketing Associationis collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to

    Journal of Marketing.

    http://www.jstor.org

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=amahttp://www.jstor.org/stable/1251163?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/1251163?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ama
  • 8/11/2019 The Effects of Music in Advertising on Choice Behavoiur

    2/9

    Gerald

    J. Gorn

    h e Effects

    o f

    M usic

    n

    Advertising

    n

    hoice

    Behavior

    Classical

    onditioning

    Approach

    OMMERCIALS

    typically

    contain both

    product

    specific

    information and

    background

    features

    such as

    pleasant

    music,

    attractive

    colors,

    and humor.

    This

    paper

    examines the

    impact

    of the

    background

    features on

    product preferences.

    One

    experiment

    was

    conducted

    to determine whether

    background

    features

    of

    a commercial

    affected

    product preferences

    when

    only

    minimal

    product

    information was

    presented.

    A

    second

    experiment

    examined the

    relative

    importance

    of

    background

    features

    and

    product

    information in

    different

    situations.

    The

    impact

    of

    product

    information in a

    commer-

    cial on

    beliefs and

    attitudes

    would

    typically

    be

    inter-

    preted

    within an

    information

    processing

    framework.

    It is suggested here that a classical conditioning

    framework

    could

    account for the

    potential

    impact

    of

    background

    features on

    product

    attitudes.

    Classical

    Gerald

    . Gorns a

    professor

    n

    he

    Faculty

    f Commercend

    Business

    Administration,

    niversity

    f British

    olumbia.he

    author

    would ike

    to

    thank he

    Institut

    'administration

    es

    Entreprises,

    niversity

    ix-

    Marseille,

    rance,

    or ts

    helpful

    ssistance

    n

    formulating

    nd

    pretest-

    ing

    he

    project.

    Do

    features like

    humor, sex,

    color,

    and

    music

    in

    a

    commercial

    merely

    increase

    our

    attention

    to

    product

    information in

    a

    message,

    or can

    they

    directly

    influence

    our

    attitudes?

    The

    results of

    an

    experiment

    using

    a

    classical

    conditioning ap-

    proach

    suggest

    that

    hearing

    liked or

    disliked mu-

    sic

    while

    being exposed

    to

    a

    product

    can

    directly

    affect

    product

    preferences.

    A

    second

    experiment

    differentiated

    communication

    situations

    where

    a

    classical

    conditioning approach

    or an

    information

    processing approach

    might

    be

    appropriate

    in

    ex-

    plaining product

    preference.

    conditioning

    suggests

    that

    positive

    attitudes

    towards

    an

    advertised

    product

    (conditioned stimulus)

    might

    develop through

    its association

    in a

    commercial

    with

    other

    stimuli that

    are reacted

    to

    positively

    (uncondi-

    tioned

    stimuli).

    Attractive

    colors,

    pleasant

    music,

    and

    humor are

    examples

    of

    potential

    unconditioned

    stim-

    uli

    in a

    commercial.

    Potentially,

    classical

    condition-

    ing

    could,

    in

    fact,

    explain

    the effect

    of

    many

    variables

    in

    communication-attitude

    change

    situations.

    For

    ex-

    ample,

    the communicator

    effect

    may

    to some

    extent

    be due to

    the association

    of the attitude

    object

    with

    positive

    affect

    attached to

    the

    communicator.

    In

    marketing,

    classical

    conditioning

    is

    often men-

    tioned

    and

    generally

    accepted

    as a

    process

    relevant

    to

    advertising (e.g., Engel, Blackwell, and Kollat 1978;

    Schiffman

    and

    Kanuk

    1978).

    There

    is,

    however,

    little

    empirical

    research

    on

    whether

    preferences

    for

    objects

    can

    actually

    be

    classically

    conditioned. In

    psychol-

    ogy,

    where classical

    conditioning

    has

    been

    investi-

    gated

    more

    extensively,

    there is

    little

    evidence

    that

    attitudes

    can

    be

    classically

    conditioned

    (Brewer

    1974,

    Fishbein

    and

    Ajzen

    1975).

    The

    limited

    popularity

    of

    classical

    conditioning

    may

    be due to

    several

    difficul-

    ties associated

    with

    typical

    conditioning

    experiments.

    Journal of

    Marketing

    Vol. 46 (Winter1982), 94-101.

    4 / Journalof Marketing,Winter1982

    This content downloaded on Mon, 11 Mar 2013 11:04:59 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/11/2019 The Effects of Music in Advertising on Choice Behavoiur

    3/9

    Difficulties

    With

    Classical

    Conditioning

    Demand

    Characteristics

    Fishbein

    and

    Ajzen

    (1975)

    suggest

    that

    attitude

    hange

    in

    conditioning experiments

    may

    be a

    function

    of

    the

    demand

    characteristicsof the

    experimental

    situation.

    They

    believe

    that

    the

    subject

    must

    be

    consciously

    aware of the presence of the unconditionedstimulus

    when the

    conditioned

    stimulus is

    present

    in order

    for

    conditioning

    to

    take

    place.

    This

    awareness

    may

    lead

    subjects

    to

    believe that

    the

    experimenter

    wants

    them

    to

    respond

    positively

    to a

    conditioned

    stimulus if

    it

    is

    paired

    with a

    positive

    unconditioned

    stimulus.

    But

    awareness of

    the conditioned

    stimulus/uncondi-

    tioned

    stimulus

    contingency

    should

    not

    automatically

    mean that

    attitude

    change

    is

    more

    likely

    the result

    of

    demand

    characteristics than

    conditioning.

    In

    fact,

    there is

    evidence of

    attitude

    conditioning

    even

    where

    demand

    ype

    responses

    were

    minimized

    through

    elab-

    orate cover

    stories

    later verified

    as

    believed

    by

    sub-

    jects (Zanna, Kiesler, and Pilkonis 1970).

    Lack of

    Awareness

    The

    consumer

    may

    not

    always

    be aware that

    the

    un-

    conditioned

    stimuli

    in a

    commercial

    may

    affect

    his/

    her

    product

    attitudes.

    Even

    more

    generally,

    the con-

    sumer

    may

    not

    be

    aware of the

    real forces

    impacting

    on

    both

    attitudes

    and

    behavior. For

    example, many

    in-store

    features

    may

    influence

    consumers

    although

    they

    may

    not

    be

    easily

    evoked

    in

    post-purchase

    ex-

    planations

    of

    buying

    (amount

    of

    shelf

    space

    devoted

    to

    a

    product,

    color

    of

    the

    package,

    and

    so

    on).

    Nisbett

    and

    Wilson

    (1977)

    reviewed

    empirical

    evi-

    dence

    casting

    doubt on

    people's

    ability

    to

    introspect

    accurately

    regarding

    heir

    cognitive

    processes.'

    They

    suggest

    that

    people

    often

    speculate

    about

    the

    potential

    causes

    of

    their

    own

    behavior

    and

    select

    stimuli

    which

    they

    think

    are

    probable

    reasons for

    their

    behavior.

    They

    may

    conclude

    that

    what

    they

    think

    should

    be

    the

    cause

    of

    their

    behavior

    is in

    fact

    the

    cause

    of

    their

    behavior

    (Nisbett

    and

    Wilson

    1977).

    In

    a

    typical

    communication

    situation,

    people

    may

    not

    realize or

    accept

    the

    impact

    of

    unconditioned

    tim-

    uli

    on

    their

    responses,

    since

    unconditioned

    stimuli

    should

    not

    logically

    be

    related to

    behavior.

    Possible

    classical conditioning effects might, therefore, be

    underestimatedand

    underreported

    n

    self-reports.

    In

    analyzing

    their

    behavior,

    people

    may

    attempt

    o

    con-

    vince

    both

    themselves

    and

    the

    researcher

    hat

    they

    are

    strictly

    rational.

    Cognitive

    bias

    can

    result

    from

    an

    at-

    'The

    Nisbett

    and

    Wilson

    (1977)

    article

    is

    controversial;

    see

    both

    Ericson

    and

    Simon

    (1980)

    and

    Weitz

    and

    Wright

    (1979)

    for

    criti-

    cisms.

    tempt

    to

    think

    well

    of

    oneself

    (Greenwald

    1980).

    A

    rational

    analysis

    of

    behavior

    in a

    communication

    sit-

    uation

    might

    be

    biased

    in

    favor

    of

    information

    and,

    therefore,

    of an

    information

    processing

    model.

    For

    example,

    it

    may

    be more

    logical

    to

    say

    that

    you

    like

    the

    product

    more

    after

    seeing

    a

    commercial,

    because

    you

    now

    believe it

    has

    X

    characteristic,

    rather

    than

    because

    you

    liked

    the

    music

    in the

    commercial.

    Thus

    evidence

    supporting

    classical

    conditioning

    s

    unlikely

    to emerge in

    self-reports.

    Arousal

    In

    testing

    for

    classical

    conditioning, product

    infor-

    mation in

    the

    commercial

    mustbe

    kept

    minimal,

    oth-

    erwise the

    unconditioned

    stimuli in

    the

    commercial

    might

    merely

    be

    arousing

    interest

    in

    product

    infor-

    mation.

    Background

    features

    such

    as

    sex, music,

    color,

    and

    humor

    have

    typically

    been

    treated as

    arousal

    stimuli

    or

    stimuli

    that

    reinforce

    the

    informa-

    tion in the

    commercial

    rather han

    unconditioned

    tim-

    uli (see Engel, Blackwell, andKollat 1978, for a dis-

    cussion

    of

    this

    material).

    n

    the

    print

    medium,

    Kroeber-

    Riel

    (1979)

    found

    that

    ads

    that

    were

    arousing

    pro-

    duced

    better

    recall

    of

    the

    information in

    the

    ad.

    A

    greater

    knowledge

    of the

    product

    information

    could

    presumably

    create

    more

    positive

    product

    attitudes.

    However,

    by

    minimizing

    product

    information

    in a

    commercial

    one

    can

    investigate

    the

    potential

    ability

    of

    unconditioned

    timulito

    change

    consumer

    attitudes

    directly.

    Mere

    Exposure

    Versus

    Classical

    Conditioning

    Mere exposure effects can be confoundedwith clas-

    sical

    conditioning

    effects.

    While

    people

    may

    develop

    favorable

    attitudes

    owards

    products

    advertised n

    the

    context

    of

    unconditioned

    stimuli,

    these

    attitude

    shifts

    may

    be

    simply

    a

    function

    of

    mere

    exposure

    to

    the

    advertised

    product.

    Zajonc

    (1968)

    has

    emphasized

    that

    exposure

    leads

    to

    liking.

    Krugman

    (1965)

    sug-

    gested

    that

    in low

    involvement

    situations,

    people

    may

    purchase

    a

    product

    simply

    because

    they

    have

    been

    exposed

    to

    it

    before

    through

    a

    commercial.

    It is in-

    teresting

    to

    note that

    with

    the

    Krugman

    model,

    ex-

    posure

    leads

    to

    behavior,

    with

    attitude

    not

    concep-

    tualized as an intervening variable. With classical

    conditioning,

    however,

    the

    person

    is

    not

    choosing

    an

    object

    simply

    because

    it's

    been

    seen

    before.

    His/her

    affect

    is also

    involved,

    so

    that

    if

    the

    product

    (condi-

    tioned

    stimulus)

    is

    paired

    with

    a

    negative

    uncondi-

    tioned

    stimulus t

    would

    be

    avoided

    despite

    exposure.

    The

    following

    experiment

    nvestigating

    he

    impact

    of the

    background

    eatures

    of

    a

    commercial

    on

    prod-

    uct

    preferences

    allowed

    for

    a

    test

    of

    mere

    exposure

    versus

    classical

    conditioning

    effects.

    Effects

    of

    Music

    n

    Advertising 95

    This content downloaded on Mon, 11 Mar 2013 11:04:59 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/11/2019 The Effects of Music in Advertising on Choice Behavoiur

    4/9

    Experiment

    One

    Basic

    Structure

    and

    Hypothesis

    In

    the

    present

    study,

    subjects

    were

    exposed

    to a neu-

    tral

    product

    (conditionedstimulus)

    in

    the context of

    a

    background

    eature

    (unconditionedstimulus),

    often

    found in

    commercials.

    To minimize the difficulties

    mentioned earlier

    in

    employing

    a

    conditioning para-

    digm, a numberof steps were taken. To lessen de-

    mand

    characteristics,

    an

    unobtrusive,

    behavioralmea-

    sure of

    productpreference

    was used instead

    of

    a

    paper

    and

    pencil

    measure.

    Information

    on

    the conditioned

    stimulus was

    kept

    as close

    to zero as

    possible

    to dem-

    onstrate hat the

    unconditioned timulus

    could

    directly

    affect

    product preference

    even

    where

    product

    infor-

    mation

    was minimal.

    A

    negative

    unconditioned

    stim-

    ulus condition

    was structured o test

    for mere

    expo-

    sure versus

    classical

    conditioning.

    Consistent

    with

    a

    conditioning

    interpretation,

    t was

    hypothesized

    that

    subjects

    would

    prefer

    an

    unexposed

    versus

    exposed

    product

    if

    the

    exposed

    product

    were

    paired

    with a

    negative unconditionedstimulus. In contrast, where

    the

    conditioned

    stimulus was

    paired

    with

    a

    positive

    unconditioned

    stimulus,

    subjects

    would choose the

    exposed

    rather han

    unexposed

    product.

    Sample

    244

    undergraduates

    andomly

    assigned

    during regis-

    tration to two sections of

    a first

    year management

    course at McGill

    University

    served as

    subjects.

    Pilot

    In a

    pilot,

    a

    group

    of 10

    subjects

    evaluated

    10

    dif-

    ferentpieces of music on a scale rangingfrom dislike

    very

    much

    (1)

    to like

    very

    much

    (5).

    The results

    of

    the

    pilot

    led

    to

    the choice

    of a

    one-minute extract of

    music from the movie "Grease" as the

    positive

    un-

    conditioned stimulus

    (x=4.3).

    One

    minute

    of

    classi-

    cal Indian music

    (x=

    1.5)

    served as the

    negative

    un-

    conditionedstimulus.2

    Two

    different colors of a

    pen

    were

    used as con-

    ditioned

    stimuli,

    light

    blue and

    beige,

    neutral

    enough

    so

    that

    associating

    them

    with liked

    or

    disliked

    music

    could

    change

    color

    preferences.

    A

    pilot

    on

    23

    subjects

    had

    revealedthat 80%

    felt neutralabout

    ight

    blue and

    beige pens,

    whereas,

    for

    example, yellow pens

    were

    generally disliked, with black pens generally liked.

    A

    second

    sample

    (N=41)

    of

    subjects

    were asked di-

    rectly

    whether

    hey

    preferred

    beige

    or

    light

    blue

    pens.

    It

    was

    expected

    that if

    subjects

    were

    neutralabout

    the

    two

    colors,

    then

    half would

    choose each.

    The

    results

    2Although

    musical

    preferences

    are

    affected

    by

    prior learning,

    music

    was

    treated as

    the

    unconditioned

    stimulus since it was

    not

    being

    con-

    ditioned

    in

    the

    present

    experiment.

    supported

    his

    notion,

    as 22 out of 41

    picked

    the

    beige

    pen,

    19 the

    light

    blue

    pen.

    Design

    and Procedure

    The

    experiment

    was carriedout

    during

    class time. The

    experimenter explained

    that an

    advertising agency

    was

    trying

    to select

    music

    (unconditionedstimulus)

    to use

    in

    a commercial

    for a

    pen

    (conditioned

    stim-

    ulus)

    producedby

    one of its clients.

    They

    would

    hear

    some music that was

    being

    considered while

    they

    watched the

    slide of the

    pen

    which the

    agency

    was

    planning

    to advertise.

    The

    pen

    was

    inexpensive

    look-

    ing

    and cost

    forty-nine

    cents.

    Very

    little information

    regarding

    he

    pen's

    attributeswas visible in the slide.

    The

    following

    conditions

    were structured:

    )

    con-

    dition

    LI-Liked

    music,

    light

    blue

    pen;

    2)

    condition

    L2-Liked music,

    beige pen;

    3)

    condition

    Dl-Disliked

    music,

    light

    blue

    pen;

    4)

    condition

    D--Disliked

    mu-

    sic,

    beige

    pen.3

    While

    subjects

    had been

    randomly

    assigned

    to the

    two sections

    of the

    course,

    a

    counter-

    balancing procedure

    was used to control further

    for

    any differences that might have existed between the

    two sections.

    To counterbalance he music and the

    color of the

    pen

    withineach

    class,

    one-halfof the

    first

    class was

    randomlyassigned

    to the liked

    music-light

    blue

    pen

    condition

    (LI),

    while the other

    half was as-

    signed

    to the

    disliked

    music-beige

    pen

    condition

    (D2).

    In

    the second

    class,

    one-half

    of the students

    were

    randomly

    assigned

    to the

    liked

    music-beige

    pen

    condition

    (L2),

    with the other half

    assigned

    to the

    disliked

    music-light

    blue

    pen

    condition

    (DI).

    The

    two conditions

    in

    each class were

    run

    sequentially

    with one-half

    of

    the class

    taking

    a break while the

    other half

    participated

    n

    the

    project.

    The

    subjects

    in each condition heard the music

    while

    they

    watched

    the

    slide.

    They

    then evaluated he

    music

    on

    a

    scale

    ranging

    from dislike

    very

    much

    (1)

    to

    like

    very

    much

    (5).

    They

    were told that

    they

    would

    receive either a

    light

    blue or

    beige pen

    for their

    help,

    donated

    by

    the

    company

    that

    manufactures he

    pen.

    The

    experimenter

    held

    up

    each

    pen

    briefly

    and com-

    mentedthat

    if

    they

    wanted

    a

    light

    blue

    one,

    to

    go

    over

    to the left side of the room to

    pick

    one

    up

    and

    drop

    off

    their

    question

    sheet in a box

    provided.

    If

    they

    wanted a

    beige

    one,

    they

    were told to

    go

    to

    the

    right

    side of

    the room to

    pick

    up

    the

    pen

    and

    drop

    off

    their

    question sheet. Using two different locations for the

    boxes

    (150

    pens per

    box)

    of

    beige

    and blue

    pens,

    with

    question

    sheet

    drop-off

    boxes

    next to

    them,

    allowed

    3No condition

    containing

    both

    positive

    and

    negative

    music was

    structured,

    since it was felt

    the

    demand

    characteristics

    would be too

    great

    under

    such

    circumstances;

    subjects

    would

    be more

    likely

    to

    think

    that

    we

    expected

    them to

    pick

    the

    color of the

    pen

    associated

    with the

    positive

    music if

    both

    positive

    and

    negative

    music were

    used

    in the

    same

    condition.

    96 / Journalf Marketing, inter 982

    This content downloaded on Mon, 11 Mar 2013 11:04:59 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/11/2019 The Effects of Music in Advertising on Choice Behavoiur

    5/9

    for a

    relatively

    unobtrusive

    recording

    of choices

    and

    linking

    of these choices

    to the evaluation

    of

    the

    mu-

    sic.4 As

    a result of the music evaluation

    ratings, ap-

    proximately

    10

    subjects

    were

    eliminatedfrom

    each

    of

    the four conditions.

    They

    were eliminated

    if in

    the

    liked music

    conditions,

    they

    did

    not evaluate

    the

    mu-

    sic from

    "Grease"

    as

    either

    like

    very

    much

    (5)

    or

    like

    somewhat

    (4),

    or in

    the

    disliked music

    conditions,

    if

    they

    did not evaluate the Indian

    music

    as either

    dislike

    very much (1) or dislike somewhat (2). For the re-

    maining subjects,

    it was

    predicted

    that

    they

    would

    pick

    the color

    of

    the

    pen they

    saw when

    they

    heard

    the liked

    music and the

    alternative

    color

    when

    they

    heard

    the disliked

    music.

    Once out

    of

    class,

    subjects

    were

    handed the

    fol-

    lowing

    free

    response question: "Why

    did

    you

    pick

    the

    color

    of the

    pen you

    picked

    rather than

    the

    other

    color? If

    you

    have

    any

    reasons

    please

    list

    them

    be-

    low."

    There

    was

    room for

    thiee

    answers.

    Finally,

    a

    discussion

    about the natureof the

    proj-

    ect

    was held with

    10 students at

    the end of class

    one

    and 10 studentsat the end of class two. Not one men-

    tioned that our real

    purpose

    was to influence

    their

    particular

    olor

    preferences.

    When told that this

    was

    in

    fact the

    purpose,

    some

    wonderedhow

    we were

    able

    to detect their

    preferences,

    since

    they

    did

    not

    put

    the

    color of the

    pen

    they picked

    or their names

    down

    on

    the

    question

    sheet.

    They

    also did not

    see

    anyone

    re-

    cording

    their

    choices

    when

    they

    picked

    the

    pens

    from

    the boxes.

    Results

    Comparing

    he

    effect of

    liked

    versus disliked

    music

    (Li

    and

    L2

    collapsed

    versus

    DI

    and

    D2

    collapsed),

    there was a very clearcutimpactof the music in the

    expected

    direction.

    As

    can be

    seen in

    Table

    1,

    74

    out

    of

    94

    subjects

    (79%)

    picked

    the

    color

    of the

    pen

    as-

    sociated with

    the liked

    music,

    while

    only

    30

    out

    of

    101

    subjects

    (30%)

    picked

    the

    color of

    the

    pen

    as-

    sociated with

    the disliked

    music.5

    When

    asked for

    possible

    reasons

    for

    their

    choice,

    126

    out of

    205

    subjects

    (62%)

    indicated

    that

    they

    did

    have

    a

    particular

    reason.

    Of

    these

    126

    people,

    114

    (91%)

    mentioned

    color

    preference

    as their

    reason.

    4A

    behavioral rather

    than

    verbal

    measure of

    preference

    was

    used,

    in

    part,

    because it could be recorded

    unobtrusively.

    In

    addition,

    be-

    havioral

    measures of

    attitudes

    are

    generally

    preferred

    to

    verbal

    mea-

    sures,

    although

    less

    prevalent

    in

    the

    literature

    (Carlsmith,

    Ellsworth,

    and

    Aronson

    1976).

    'The color of

    pen

    did

    not

    seem to

    matter.

    36

    out

    of

    43

    people

    (84%)

    picked

    the

    light

    blue

    pen

    when it

    was

    associated

    with

    the

    liked

    music

    (L,);

    38 out

    of 51

    (74%)

    picked

    the

    beige

    rather than

    the

    light

    blue

    pen

    when

    the

    beige pen

    was

    associated

    with the

    liked

    music

    (L2).

    Similarly,

    17

    out of 52

    (33%)

    chose

    the

    light

    blue

    pen

    when it

    was

    associated

    with

    disliked

    music

    (D,)

    while

    13

    out

    of

    49

    (26%)

    picked

    the

    beige

    pen

    when it

    was

    associated

    with

    the

    disliked music

    (D2).

    These

    differences

    were

    not

    statistically

    significant.

    TABLE1

    Liked

    Versus

    Disliked

    Music

    and Pen

    Choices

    Pen Choice

    "Advertised"

    "Non-advertised"

    pen

    pen

    Liked

    music

    74

    20

    94

    Disliked

    music 30

    71

    101

    104

    91

    195

    x2

    =

    47.01

    (p