the effect of catholic schooling on math and reading ......joseph p. robinson . stanford university....

54
The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading development in kindergarten through fifth grade Sean F. Reardon Stanford University Jacob E. Cheadle University of Nebraska-Lincoln Joseph P. Robinson Stanford University March, 2008 Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, 650.736.8517, ([email protected] ). This draft was prepared for the Annual Meeting of the Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness, March 2-4, 2008. Data for this paper were collected by the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) and provided to Reardon under an NCES Restricted Data License (license #04012902). Because some of the data we use are only available to researchers with an NCES restricted data license, the data are not available from the authors, but may be obtained from NCES. The research and opinions here are ours, and do not necessarily reflect those of the NCES. Any errors are ours as well.

Upload: others

Post on 12-Sep-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading development in kindergarten through fifth grade

Sean F. Reardon Stanford University

Jacob E. Cheadle

University of Nebraska-Lincoln

Joseph P. Robinson Stanford University

March, 2008

Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, 650.736.8517, ([email protected]). This draft was prepared for the Annual Meeting of the Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness, March 2-4, 2008. Data for this paper were collected by the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES) and provided to Reardon under an NCES Restricted Data License (license #04012902). Because some of the data we use are only available to researchers with an NCES restricted data license, the data are not available from the authors, but may be obtained from NCES. The research and opinions here are ours, and do not necessarily reflect those of the NCES. Any errors are ours as well.

Page 2: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading development in kindergarten through fifth grade

Abstract

Prior research estimating the effect of Catholic schooling has focused on high school, where

evidence suggests a positive effect of Catholic versus public schooling. In this paper, we estimate

the effect of attending a Catholic elementary school rather than a public school on the math and

reading skills of children in kindergarten through fifth grade. We use nationally representative data

and a set of matching estimators to estimate the average effect of Catholic schooling and the extent

to which the effect varies across educational markets. When we use public school students

nationwide to provide a counterfactual estimate of how Catholic school students would have

performed in public schools, we find that strong evidence indicating that Catholic elementary

schools are less successful at teaching math skills than public schools (Catholic school students are

3-4 months behind public school students by third and fifth grade), but no more or less successful at

teaching reading skills. When we compare Catholic students to matched public school students

attending schools in the same county, however, we obtain estimated math effects that are generally

somewhere between the (negative) national estimates and zero (but statistically indistinguishable

from either). We again find no evidence of a positive or negative Catholic schooling effect on

reading skills.

Page 3: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading development

in kindergarten through fifth grade

In this paper, we estimate the effect of attending a Catholic school rather than a public

school on the math and reading skills of children in kindergarten through fifth grade. While there

has been considerable prior research on the effects of attending a Catholic high school, there is very

little existing research on the effects of attending a Catholic elementary school.

The paper proceeds as follows. In section 1, we discuss the importance of understanding

the effects of Catholic schooling. In section 2, we review prior research on Catholic schooling. In

section 3, we describe the data and methods used for our analyses. Section 4 describes our results.

In section 5 we discuss the results at length, attending in particular to the reasons why the estimates

from different models differ. Section 6 concludes.

1. Introduction

The question of whether Catholic schools provide better education than public schools is of

interest for several reasons. First, the question has a rich history in the sociology of education

literature. In 1982, Coleman and colleagues reported that Catholic schooling provided substantial

positive effects on high school students’ math and reading skills (Coleman, Hoffer, and Kilgore

1982a, 1982b, 1982c). This research prompted considerable debate and subsequent research on the

effects of Catholic versus public schooling, and the reasons for such effects (see, for example,

Altonji, Elder, and Taber 2002; Bryk, Lee, and Holland 1993; Grogger and Neal 2000; Hoffer,

Greeley, and Coleman 1985; Morgan 2001; Neal 1997).

Second, understanding the effects of Catholic schools on student achievement is important

1

Page 4: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

on the basis of numbers alone. Catholic schools enroll a larger number of students than any other

type of private school. Of the 5.1 million K-12 students (10% of all U.S. K-12 students) enrolled in

private school, almost half (2.3 million in 2003-04) attend Catholic schools (Broughman and Swaim

2006; Reardon and Yun 2002). If Catholic schools produce better average student outcomes than

public schools for this large number of students (as suggested in some of the Catholic high school

effects literature), then it would be useful to understand the mechanisms through which Catholic

schools produce such effects. And if Catholic schools produce worse average outcomes, then the

large number of students affected is a cause for concern.

Third, policy proposals to provide private school vouchers to families hinge on claims that

students would learn more if they attended private schools than if they attended their local public

schools. Because Catholic schools make up by far the largest share of the private school sector in

the U.S., and because Catholic school tuition is much lower, on average, than tuition in other private

schools,1 voucher recipients (and low-income voucher recipients in particular) are more likely to

attend Catholic schools than other types of private schools. Thus, unless voucher programs were to

create large changes in the structure of the private schooling sector (which they may do, but not in

the short term), most students who take advantage of any proposed private school voucher program

will likely attend Catholic schools. Estimating the causal effects of Catholic schooling on

achievement thus provides information relevant to evaluating the likely effects of voucher programs.

Finally, information about the effects of Catholic schooling may shed light on the potential

of competition among schools to lead to improvements in school quality. Catholic school

enrollment, after reaching a peak in 1965 (when 5.6 million students were enrolled in 13,500

Catholic schools), has declined steadily over the last 40 years (to 2.3 million students in 7,500

1 Average elementary school annual tuition in 2003-04 was $3,533 in Catholic schools; $5,398 in other religious private schools; and $12,169 in non-sectarian private schools (Digest of Educational Statistics, 2006, Table 56; at Hhttp://nces.ed.gov/programs/digest/d06/tables/dt06_056.aspH).

2

Page 5: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

schools in 2006-7).2 This decline has occurred largely as a result of rising tuitions caused by increasing

financial pressure on the church and the movement of many Catholics to the suburbs, where the

Catholic Church has traditionally operated few schools (Baker 1999; Baker and Riordan 1998; Bryk, Lee,

and Holland 1993). A market model of private schooling would predict competition among Catholic

schools to attract and retain remaining Catholic students and to attract non-Catholics away from

public schools. This competition should (according to a market competition theory) lead to

improvements in Catholic schooling and to the closing of the weakest Catholic schools. As a result,

we might expect Catholic schools to be better than public schools because the same declining

enrollment pressures have not operated on public schools. Of course, a variety of other pressures

operate on both Catholic and public schooling, so any differences we find in the effects of Catholic

and public schooling should not be read as unambiguous evidence regarding the effects of

competition.

2. Prior research on the effect of Catholic schooling

We have been able to find only one study using a large-scale data set to estimate the effects

of Catholic elementary schooling on achievement outcomes in the elementary grades. Nonetheless,

there are two bodies of research which are relevant to consider. First, there is a considerable body

of research on the effects of attending a Catholic high school. Second, there is some research on the

effects of attending a private (not necessarily Catholic) school in the elementary grades. Much of this

latter research stems from research estimating the effect of school vouchers. We review each of

these research strands in turn, attending in particular to the methods used to identify the causal

effect in each case. 2 Note that this decline continues through the present. In 2006-7, 212 Catholic schools closed or consolidated, while 36 new Catholic schools opened. Since 2001-02, the total number of Catholic schools has declined from 8,146 to 7,498 and enrollment declined from 2.6 million to 2.3 million (Digest of Educational Statistics 2003, Table 62). See Hhttp://www.ncea.org/news/AnnualDataReport.aspH (accessed February 19, 2008) and Digest of Educational Statistics 2003, Table 62.

3

Page 6: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Prior research on the effect of attending Catholic high school

Most studies on the effects of Catholic high schools conclude that attending a Catholic high

school is has a positive effect on achievement for at least some group of students (Coleman, Hoffer,

and Kilgore 1982a, 1982b, 1982c; Morgan 2001; Murnane, Newstead, and Olsen 1985; Neal 1997).

None of these studies are based on experimental designs, of course (it would be hard to randomly

assign some students to attend Catholic schools and others to attend public schools); rather, each

relies on observing the test scores and graduation rates of students in both Catholic and public

schools and inferring causation using a statistical model. As a result, the validity of their conclusions

rests on the extent to which their assumptions of their models are plausible. Researchers have relied

on three types of models to address the issue of selection bias: 1) covariate adjustment (regression)

models; 2) instrumental variables models; and 3) matching estimators.

Covariate adjustment (regression) models. The seminal research on the effect of Catholic high

schools was done by Coleman, Hoffer, and Kilgore (1982b; 1982c). Using the High School and

Beyond (HSB) dataset and regression models that control for race and socioeconomic background,

they found a substantial positive effect of Catholic schooling on vocabulary and mathematics scores.

However, some have criticized the Coleman, Hoffer, and Kilgore results on the grounds that the

regression models do not adequately control for initial achievement and unobserved selection

mechanisms, resulting in biased estimates on Catholic schooling (Goldberger and Cain 1982).

Principal among the criticisms of the Coleman et al. paper is that proper steps were not taken to

ensure the comparability of the Catholic and public school students at entry into high school

because no assessments were given prior to the sophomore assessments. Moreover, Murnane,

Newstead, & Olsen (1985) illustrate how modeling the school-choice selection process—and, in

particular, doing so separately for whites, blacks, and Hispanics—can lead to a very different

outcome: Blacks and Hispanics experience significant and substantial benefits in both reading and

4

Page 7: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

mathematics from attending Catholic schools, whereas whites exhibit no significant evidence of an

effect. Similar results are found in other studies using covariate adjustment (Grogger and Neal

2000).

Instrumental variable models. Rather than attempting to eliminate selection bias through

modeling, another solution for removing selection bias is to use an instrumental variable or

variables—variables that predict Catholic school enrollment but are otherwise uncorrelated with

student outcomes. Neal (1997), for example, considers several potential instrumental variables: 1)

whether the student is Catholic; 2) the availability of Catholic secondary schools in the county; and

3) the proportion of the county that is Catholic. Using these three instruments and a bivariate

probit model estimated on data from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth (NLSY-79), Neal

finds positive effects of Catholic schooling, with the effects being small for suburban students and

larger for urban minority students. For urban students, Neal’s results show a positive Catholic

school effect on academic achievement, graduation rates, college attendance, and future wages. Neal

suggests that urban minority students benefit more than suburban students and white urban

students because of the poor quality of the public schools serving urban minority children. Thus,

Catholic schooling is a greater contrast to public schooling for urban minorities, which contributes

to the large estimated effect for this group. Evans and Schwab (1995) also use a variable indicating

whether a student is Catholic as an instrument, and find a strong positive effect of Catholic

schooling on graduation and college enrollment.

Of course, instrumental variables models rely on a set of assumptions as well. Altonji, Elder,

and Taber (2002) evaluate the extent to which the instruments used by Neal and Evans and Schwab

satisfy the assumptions needed to produce unbiased estimates. Using data from national several

national surveys and several tests of the instrumental variables assumptions, Altonji, Elder, and

Taber conclude that none of the three potential instruments are valid, calling into question the

5

Page 8: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

results that rely on them to estimate Catholic school effects. Instead of an instrumental variables

approach, Altonji, Elder, and Taber (2005) propose a method for estimating lower and upper

bounds on the Catholic school effects by using the observable selection processes as a basis for

bounding the possible bias due to unobservable selection mechanisms. Using the NELS dataset,

they obtain lower and upper bounds on the Catholic school effect, which suggest Catholic high

schools have a positive effect on graduation rates but not necessarily on achievement.

Matching estimators. The use of matching estimators is another approach to addressing

selection bias. Matching estimators, including propensity score matching, rely on the assumption

that Catholic and public school students who are similar on a set of observed characteristics can

stand as valid counterfactuals for one another. Matching estimators depend on the same assumption

that there are no omitted confounder variables as does covariate adjustment, but have several

advantages. First, matching estimators do not rely on assumptions about the functional form of the

model. Second, they estimate average effects only for the subpopulation of students who have

matches in the opposite treatment condition. And third, they facilitate the investigation of treatment

effect heterogeneity.

Morgan (Morgan 2001) uses a propensity score matching estimator to estimate the effect of

attending a Catholic school on math and reading achievement. Using NELS data, he finds a positive

effect of Catholic schooling on high school math and reading scores.

Heterogeneity of treatment effects

There are a number of reasons to expect that Catholic schooling effects may vary among

subsets of the population. There are three types of variation of particular interest. First, a number

of studies have found Catholic schooling to have larger effects for minority (typically Black) students

and urban students (Altonji, Elder, and Taber 2005; Evans and Schwab 1995; Neal 1997). Second,

6

Page 9: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Neal argues that the effect of Catholic schooling may vary among locations—where public schools

are lower in quality, Catholic schools may be better by comparison (Neal 1997). Third, the

likelihood of attending a Catholic school may be associated with the expected effect of Catholic

schooling. We would expect this to occur because of positive selection—those most likely to

benefit from Catholic schooling are likely to be those most likely to attend Catholic schools.

Contrary to this expectation, however, research has suggested students who are least likely to attend

Catholic school, including minority students, have the greatest benefits (Altonji, Elder, and Taber

2005; Evans and Schwab 1995; Morgan 2001; Neal 1997). Morgan (2001) suggests that this might

occur for a variety of reasons—Catholic schools may in fact be better for such students; such

students may work hard in Catholic schools because the financial sacrifice of their parents to pay for

private schooling is more salient to them than to higher-income students; Catholic schools may be

better for such students only relative to the lower-quality public schools available to them; or

because the enrollment of low income students in Catholic schools is more strongly conditioned on

their parents’ expectation of the effectiveness of Catholic schooling for their child than for higher-

income students. Morgan is unable to distinguish among these competing hypotheses in his

analysis, however.

Effects of attending a private elementary school

As described above, most of the studies of high school Catholic school effects report

positive effects of Catholic schooling. One exception is a recent study that uses a multilevel

regression model and NAEP data to estimate the effect of private schools (including Catholic

schools) on 4th and 8th grade math achievement (Lubienski and Lubienski 2006). Notably, this is also

the only study to specifically estimate Catholic elementary school effects as opposed to high school

effects. Lubienski and Lubienski find that Catholic school students score roughly .50 standard

7

Page 10: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

deviations lower than observationally similar public school students in 4th grade and .20 standard

deviations lower in 8th grade. Nonetheless, although their regression model controls for basic

student demographic characteristics (e.g., race, gender, free-lunch status), it does not include an

extensive set of control variables, relies on functional form assumptions, and assumes the Catholic

school effect is constant across locations.

Studies of voucher effects may provide information on the effects of Catholic schools.

Because Catholics are more likely to apply for vouchers and use those vouchers for Catholic school

attendance (Campbell, West, and Peterson 2005), voucher studies—at least those with substantial

Catholic school samples—may shed light on Catholic school effects.3 For example, voucher

experiments in New York City, Dayton, OH, and Washington, DC, provide estimates of the effects

of vouchers (thus private schools, including Catholic schools) on students in low-income (all three

cities) and moderate-income families (Dayton, OH, and Washington, DC). Each of the cities used a

randomized controlled experimental design, implemented through a lottery system. The dollar

amount of the vouchers varied across the programs, but the voucher covered the majority of the

tuition for students attending religious schools, which tend to charge lower tuition than secular

private schools. Of the voucher participants, 68% in NYC, 59% in Dayton, and 49% in DC, began

the experiment by using the voucher to attend Catholic schools; however, the percentages attending

Catholic schools dropped monotonically throughout the programs, with voucher participants

returning to public schools (Howell and Peterson 2006). Howell & Peterson’s analyses of these

programs found no consistent evidence of a positive effect of moving from a public to private

school. The only exceptions to this pattern of null effects were the finding (based on post-hoc race-

specific analyses) that black students benefitted significantly more in the second year of the program

3 Although research on the publicly-funded Milwaukee voucher program, for example, finds a small benefit of private schools on math achievement, the public funds from this voucher program could not be spent on religious-school tuition, and thus these results cannot be generalized to Catholic schools (Rouse 1998).

8

Page 11: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

(but not first or third years) in Washington, DC, and that vouchers had a positive effect on

achievement by fifth grade in the NYC program. Of course, although a large proportion of voucher

users attended Catholic schools, these experimental results should only be used as suggestive of

what we might find as a Catholic school effect.

One additional voucher study provides estimates of Catholic school effects, albeit not in the

U.S. context. McEwan and Carnoy’s (2000) study of Chilean schools found Catholic schools to be

slightly more effective than public schools in Spanish and math achievement levels for fourth-grade

students, after controlling for family socioeconomic status. However, they note that Chilean

Catholic schools spend more than public schools, and thus Catholic schools are no more efficient at

producing high achievement.

In sum, the research on Catholic high school effects generally has found positive effects of

attending a Catholic school, particularly for minority students and those least likely to attend

Catholic schools. None of this research is without potential bias, but the consistency of the results

suggests there may be a positive effect of Catholic high school enrollment. Evidence of Catholic

elementary schooling, however, is much sparser, and does not suggest any positive effect. In this

paper, we estimate Catholic elementary schooling effects in order to determine whether the pattern

of positive Catholic high school effects is found also in elementary school.

3. Data and Methods

The data for this study come from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study–Kindergarten

Class of 1998-1999 (ECLS-K), sponsored by the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES).

The ECLS-K contains data on a nationally-representative sample of 21,260 students from the

kindergarten class of 1998-99 (thus, representing a cohort born in roughly 1992-93). Students in the

sample were assessed in reading and mathematics skills at six time points during the years 1998-2004

9

Page 12: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

(fall 1998, spring 1999, fall 1999, spring 2000, spring 2002, and spring 2004). In addition to these

cognitive developmental measures, the ECLS-K data include information gathered from surveys of

parents, teachers, and school administrators regarding family, school, community, and student

characteristics.

We use math and reading scores on the ECLS-K direct cognitive assessments as measures of

children’s math and reading skills. The ECLS-K direct cognitive assessments are individually-

administered, oral, untimed, adaptive tests of math and reading skills. The content areas of the tests

are based on the NAEP 4th grade content areas, adapted to be age appropriate at each assessment.

The assessments were administered by trained ECLS-K assessors, and were scored using a 3-

parameter Item Response Theory (IRT) model. Details of the assessments are provided in the

ECLS-K psychometric reports (Pollack, Narajian, Rock, Atkins-Burnett, and Hausken 2005; Pollack,

Rock, Weiss, Atkins-Burnett, Tourangeau et al. 2005; Rock and Pollack 2002). We use the T-scores

(a version of the test score that is standardized within each assessment wave to a mean of 50 and a

standard deviation of 10) reported by NCES for all analyses in this paper. These scores are suitable

for repeated cross-sectional analyses (as we use here), though not for longitudinal growth models.

At each wave, students were only administered the ECLS-K math assessment if they were

proficient in oral English or oral Spanish, and were only administered the ECLS-K reading

assessment if they were proficient in oral English.4 In the early waves of the ECLS-K data

collection, many Hispanic and Asian students (29% of Hispanic students; 22% of Asian students)

were not fluent enough in oral English to be assessed in reading (in English). The 22% of non-

English proficient Asian students were also not assessed in math. Because the proportion of oral 4 Students were administered the math and reading assessments orally in English if either the school reported the student was from a home where English was the primary language or if the student passed the English Oral Language Development Scale (OLDS) assessment given by ECLS-K assessors. If the student was not proficient in English, but passed the Spanish OLDS, then he or she was administered the math test orally in Spanish, but was not administered the reading test. Students not proficient in oral English or Spanish (most of whom were Asian-origin students) at a given wave were not given the math or reading tests. These students were readministered the English OLDS assessment at subsequent waves, and given the math and reading tests once they passed the English OLDS.

10

Page 13: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

English-proficient Hispanic and Asian students grows over time (to 99% by the spring of third

grade), and because students not proficient in English certainly have lower average reading skills in

English than students proficient in oral English, trends in the mean reading and math scores of

those students with reading scores at a given wave are confounded by changes in the population of

students represented in the sample of students with test scores at that wave. The same bias may be

present in the patterns of math scores, but it will likely be much smaller than for reading, because

the number of students excluded from the math test for language reasons is much smaller. In order

to avoid bias in our estimates because of changes in the sample of students with test scores at each

wave, we estimate the effects of Catholic schooling on math and reading achievement at each wave

only for the subpopulations of students who had valid math and reading test scores, respectively, at

the start of kindergarten. Thus, our estimates of the average effect of Catholic schooling on math

skills may not generalize to children who are not proficient in English or Spanish at the start of

kindergarten. Likewise, our estimates of the average effect of Catholic schooling on reading skills

may not generalize to children who are not proficient in English at the start of kindergarten.

Data restrictions

In addition to restricting our sample to those students with valid math and reading scores in

the fall of kindergarten, we restrict our analyses to a subsample of students who meet several other

criteria. First, we restrict the sample to first-time kindergarten students attending either a Catholic

or public kindergarten in the fall of 1998 (second-time kindergarteners (n=2,641) and students

attending non-Catholic private schools (n=2,318) are excluded). These restrictions yield a sample of

16,301 students (1,979 Catholic and 14,322 public school students). Third, as noted above, we

restrict the sample to students present in the sample in each of waves 1, 2, 4, 5, and 6 and with valid

reading and math scores at wave 1. These restrictions ensure that our estimates at each wave are

11

Page 14: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

based on the same sample of students, and so are not biased by sample attrition or the missing test

scores of non-English or-Spanish proficient students in the early waves of the study. Moreover, the

restriction to students present in each wave of the study is necessary so that we can use the ECLS-K

panel weight (variable c1_6fc0).5 This reduced the sample to 7,614 (1,090 Catholic and 6,524 public

school students). Finally, we excluded 136 students missing data on race/ethnicity (8 students) or

key family variables (128 students) needed for the propensity score models. The final analytic

sample size is N=7,480 (1,078 students in 97 Catholic schools, and 6,402 students in 692 public

schools). Although this is much smaller than the full ECLS-K study sample, most of the sample

restrictions are by design rather than missing data. Of the sample of 7,614 students who were

English- or Spanish- proficient first-time kindergarten students in the Fall of 1998 attending

Catholic or public schools, only 2% are excluded from the sample due to missing data.

The ECLS-K sample was drawn by first selecting a random sample of counties or groups of

counties in the U.S., sampling schools within these counties, and then sampling students within

these schools. As a result, there are many Catholic schools in the sample in counties in which there

is also at least one public school in the sample. This feature of the sample design of ECLS-K allows

us, in some of our analyses, to compare the outcomes of Catholic and public school students

attending schools within the same educational market—schools which might meaningfully be

considered as viable educational alternatives for the same families. For these analyses, we further

restrict the main analytic sample to include only students who attended school in one of 60 counties

in the U.S. in which there was at least one Catholic and one public school in the ECLS-K study.

This sample, which we refer to as the “60 market sample” includes 1,001 Catholic and 2,554 public

school students (in 89 Catholic and 285 public schools).

5 By design, not all students who transferred out of their original school were followed in the ECLS-K study. The panel sample weights are used to reweight the sample remaining the study to be representative of the fall kindergarten population.

12

Page 15: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Sample description

On average, students attending Catholic school in kindergarten differ substantially from

those attending public school (see Tables 1 and A1). Catholic school students, on average, come

from more advantaged families—they have higher incomes ($23,000 higher, on average), higher

parental education levels, older mothers, were less likely to receive food stamps or be below the

poverty line, and were more likely to attend center-based child care before kindergarten (among

other differences—see Appendix Table A1 for detail). Not surprisingly, Catholic school students

also have much higher math and reading scores (.49 standard deviations higher in math and .42

standard deviations higher in reading)6 in the fall of kindergarten than public school students.

Averaging over the full set of covariates included in Table A1, Catholic school students

differ from public school students, on average, by roughly three-tenths of a standard deviation. The

patterns in the 60 market sample are very close to those in the full analytic sample, suggesting that

the 60 market sample is roughly representative of the larger population of interest.

Analytic strategy

Given that Catholic and public school students differ in many ways likely to affect their

cognitive skills, a simple comparison of their mean test scores at subsequent waves is likely to yield

substantially biased estimates of the effect of Catholic schooling. In this paper, we rely primarily on

a set of propensity score matching estimators to purge bias from our estimates of the effect of

Catholic schooling.

To estimate the effect of Catholic versus public schooling, we use several different

6 The ECLS-K T-scores are designed to have mean 50 and standard deviation 10 in the population. The values of .49 and .42 are the fall kindergarten Catholic-public differences in T-scores in reading and math, respectively, divided by the standard deviation 10. The ‘standardized differences’ reported in Tables 1 and 3 are computed by dividing the Catholic-public differences in T-scores by the pooled standard deviation, which is slightly smaller than the population standard deviation. As a result the standardized differences in Tables 1 and 3 are slightly larger than the population differences.

13

Page 16: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

estimators, including linear regression, linear regression including market fixed effects, a standard

propensity score matching estimator, a propensity score model that includes market fixed effects,

and two within-market matching estimators. For each estimator, we estimate a series of cross-

sectional regression models, estimating the Catholic-public student difference in test scores,

conditional on the model, at each wave. Although it would also be desirable to fit growth models,

taking advantage of the longitudinal nature of the ECLS-K design, we do not do so. Growth

models rely on the assumption that test scores are measured in an interval-scaled metric, meaning

that a difference of one point has the same substantive meaning at any point on the test score scale.

Reardon (2007), however, shows that the scale scores provide by ECLS-K are not measured in a

metric that is meaningfully interval-scaled (a one-point difference on the scale corresponds to one

additional item correct on the test, but because there are many more ‘difficult’ items on the ECLS-K

tests than ‘easy’ items, equal gains in skill at different initial skill levels will yield larger score gains for

students within higher initial skills than those with lower initial skills). As a result, growth models

using ECLS-K data may yield biased estimates of Catholic school effects.

WLS regression estimates

Initially, we estimate the effect of Catholic schooling using simple weighted least squares

regression. We begin by estimating a simple model (Model 1) with no controls, weighting

observations by their ECLS-K panel weight c1_6fc0. These estimates provide a description of the

observed difference in average test scores between Catholic and public school students at each wave.

In model 2, we add to Model 1 a long list of covariates (those included in Table A1, plus a set of

interaction and higher order terms—see footnote 8) as control variables. Because these models may

not fully capture differences in prior cognitive skill between Catholic and public school students, we

add the wave 1 test score as a control variable in Model 3 (thus, we cannot estimate model 3 for

14

Page 17: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

wave 1 outcomes). While it is possible that this model may overcontrol for prior cognitive skill,

because the wave 1 score was measured 1-2 months into the school year (and so may be affected by

Catholic schooling), this would have the effect of biasing the Catholic school estimates toward zero.

WLS market fixed effects estimates

Although the WLS regression models control for differences in student characteristics, the

estimates from these models may be biased by the non-random distribution of Catholic school

attendance in relation to public school quality. If, for example, the likelihood of attending Catholic

school and achievement levels, conditional on the type of school attended) are correlated, then

models that do not account for these between-market differences will yield biased estimates. For

example, if students are more likely to attend Catholic schools in educational markets with low-

quality public schools, conditional on the observed covariates in the regression models, the

regression estimates will be biased toward zero. To eliminate this potential bias, we include county

fixed effects (as a proxy for educational market fixed effects) in Models 4 and 5. These models have

the same form as Models 2 and 3, save for the inclusion of the fixed effects and the necessary

restriction of the sample to those students in the 60 counties with at least one Catholic and one

public school in the ECLS-K sample. The estimates from these models can be interpreted as the

average adjusted difference in test scores between students attending Catholic and public schools in

the same county.

National propensity score estimates

Models 1-5 use linear regression to estimate the effect of Catholic schooling. The linear

regression model depends not only on the assumption that there are no omitted confounding

variables that affect both Catholic school attendance and subsequent academic performance, but

15

Page 18: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

also on the assumption that the linear model is correctly specified. One alternative to regression is

propensity score matching (Rosenbaum 1983), an approach used by Morgan in the estimation of the

effect of Catholic schooling (Morgan 2001). While propensity score matching estimation relies on

the same assumption that there are no omitted confounders as does the regression model, it relaxes

the modeling assumption, and enables us to estimate the effect of Catholic schooling without relying

on the linearity assumption or the extrapolation of the model to regions where the density of public

or Catholic school students is low. Moreover, propensity score matching allows us to explicitly

identify the region of common support—the region of data in which there are both public and

Catholic school students who can be matched and used to estimate the effect of Catholic versus

public schooling.

In practice, there are many methods of matching, including stratification on the propensity

score, 1-1 (or M-1) propensity score matching, caliper matching on the propensity score, nearest

neighbor matching, exact matching on some covariates, and matching with and without replacement

(Abadie and Imbens 2002; Imbens 2004). The choice among these methods typically involves some

tradeoff between precision and potential bias, though it is beyond the scope of this paper to

investigate or employ each of these methods. Matching each Catholic school student to many

public school students (as we would, for example, with stratification on the propensity score, M-1

matching, or caliper matching with a wide caliper) would yield a large matched public school sample,

leading to greater precision in our treatment effect estimates, but it would do so at the risk of

introducing (additional) potential bias into the estimates, since the more matches we use, the less

similar, on average, they may be to their Catholic school counterparts. We opt here for a fairly

narrow caliper and match with replacement in order to ensure that each Catholic school student is

either matched to one or more very similar public school students or is dropped from the

estimation.

16

Page 19: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Specifically, we implement the propensity score matching estimator as follows. First, using

the full analytic sample and the full set of student covariates described in Table A1, we fit a logit

model predicting enrollment in Catholic versus public school in the fall of kindergarten. From this

model, we obtain the predicted probability ̂ of each student’s enrollment in Catholic school, given

his or her vector of observed covariates; this is the estimated propensity score. Second, for each

Catholic school student, we select as matches all public school students with estimated propensity

scores within one percentage point of that of the Catholic school student (if there are more than 10

public school students within one percentage point of a given Catholic school student, we select the

closest 10 as matches). We match with replacement, so that a given public school student may be

used as a match for more than one Catholic school student. Third, each matched public school

student is given a weight wi, defined as

∑∑ [1]

where c indexes Catholic school students and p indexes public school students, vj is the ECLS-K

longitudinal sample weight (ECLS-K variable c1_6fc0) for student j, and mjc=1 if public school

student j is used as a match for Catholic school student c (i.e., if | ̂ ̂ | .01) and mjc=0

otherwise. Each Catholic school student is assigned a weight wi=vi. The sums of these weights are

equal for the matched samples of Catholic and public school students. Importantly, the

construction of weights in this fashion reweights the matched public school sample to be similar to

the distribution of Catholic school students, meaning that our estimates should be interpreted as

estimates of the average effect of Catholic schooling on (the type of) students enrolled in Catholic

schools. We return to this point later. Fourth, we assess the balance of the matched samples—the

extent to which the matching has eliminated or reduced the correlation between Catholic/public

school status and the observed covariates—by using the matched samples and fitting, for each

17

Page 20: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

covariate X included in the propensity score model, a regression model (weighted by ) of the

form

ε , [2]

where is an indicator for whether a student attends Catholic or public school. The estimated

coefficient indicates the average difference in the covariate X between the weighted matched

public and Catholic student samples; a coefficient of zero indicates there is no average difference

between the two samples. We assess the overall quality of the matching by computing the average

of the absolute values of the ’s across all the covariates. To optimize the matching, we fit

variations of the propensity score model using higher-order terms and polynomial terms and use the

model that minimizes this average absolute standardized difference between the Catholic and public

school samples.7

Finally, we estimate the effect of Catholic versus public school on math and reading test

scores at each wave t by fitting regression models of the form

ε . [3]

The estimated coefficient will be an unbiased estimate of the effect of Catholic schooling on test

score Y at wave t under the assumption that matching has removed all potential bias—that, on

average, for students with the same estimated propensity score, the selection of a Catholic or public

school is uncorrelated a with student’s potential test scores. As above, we fit two versions of these

models. The first (Model 6) includes no covariates; the second (Model 7) includes the propensity

7 Here we follow the suggestions made by Imai, King, and Stuart and by Morgan and Todd for assessing balance rather than using the more conventional approach of assessing balance on the basis of whether we fail to reject the null hypotheses that each 0. They remind readers of the well-known (but sometimes ignored) facts that tests of null hypothesis are dependent on sample size, and that interpretation of a failure to reject the null hypothesis does not necessarily affirm the null is true. In small samples, in particular, we may fail to reject the null hypothesis even when there is a large difference in the covariates between the matched samples. (Imai, King, and Stuart forthcoming; Morgan and Todd 2007). Based on this approach, we use a final model that includes the full set of covariates listed in table A1 as well quadratic and cubic terms for household income, a quadratic term for the number of months the child has lived in the current home, interaction terms of race with household income, and interaction terms of mother’s and father’s occupation with a variable indicating if the student was black.

18

Page 21: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

score as a covariate in the model to adjust for any residual bias due to the observed covariates that

remains because of imperfect matching (Ho, Imai, King, and Stuart 2007; Imai, King, and Stuart

forthcoming). In addition, Model 7 includes the wave 1 test score as a covariate in the wave 2-6

models both because the matching and propensity score adjustment may not capture all differences

in pre-kindergarten cognitive skill, and because including the wave 1 score will likely increase the

precision of our estimates.

Propensity score estimates with market fixed effects

Although the propensity score matching estimator removes bias associated with the

observed covariates included in the matching model, the estimates from these models may still be

biased by the non-random distribution of Catholic school attendance in relation to public school

quality, as we noted above. To assess this, we restrict the sample to the 60 market sample, and

construct a matched sample of public school students to the Catholic school students in these

markets, using the propensity scores model estimated above. As above, we use 10-1 matching with

a .01 caliper and replacement. We then fit another pair of models (Models 8 and 9) that include

county fixed effects, using the matched sample within the 60 markets. The estimates from these

models are purged of bias associated with the covariates included in the matching model, as well as

any residual bias associated with the correlation of Catholic school enrollment with characteristics of

counties.

Within-market propensity score matching

We include one further refinement to the propensity score model. The matching algorithm

used in Model 6-9 matches students on observed student characteristics, but allows matched

students to be drawn from anywhere in the country. This means, for example, that a low-income

19

Page 22: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Catholic school student in Boston may be matched to a low-income public school student in

Birmingham. Moreover, because Catholic schooling is more common in certain parts of the country

and in urban rather than suburban areas, it is likely that the strategy of matching students without

regard for their location may yield samples that are matched on observable student characteristics

but not matched on factors that vary across locations. Recent research on the conditions under

which matching estimators are most likely to reduce bias, however, suggests that matching performs

best when matches are local—that is, when matched samples come from the same local market.8 In

order to ensure that our matched samples are matched on individual as well as contextual factors, we

use a within-market matching estimator, similar to that described by Reardon (2006).

The within-market matching estimator follows a similar algorithm to the standard matching

procedure described above, with a few key changes. We use the sample of students in the 60 market

sample, and fit a multilevel logit model (Raudenbush and Bryk 2002; Raudenbush, Bryk, Cheong,

and Congdon 2005) to estimate students’ propensity scores, including within-market centered values

of each of the covariates, as well as the same set of interaction and higher-order terms as we use in

the national propensity score matching model (see footnote 8). We include a market-level random

effect on the intercept. Initially, we constrain each of the coefficients on the covariates (which

indicate the average within-market association between the covariate and enrollment in Catholic

school) to be constant across markets. This may be an inappropriate constraint for some covariates,

however. For example, the association between family income and the probability of Catholic

school enrollment may vary across sites because of tuition differences across sites. We test the

hypotheses that the coefficients on key covariates are constant across sites, and relax these

8 Much of the work on the performance of matching estimators comes from the job training and employment literature. Given that employment outcomes depend on local labor market conditions, the need to match samples within local markets has some intuitive justification. The same logic likely applies in education, where schooling outcomes may be affected by local conditions (teacher quality may be affected by local labor market conditions; school resources may be affected by local socioeconomic conditions and school finance laws, and so on).

20

Page 23: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

assumptions in cases where the hypothesis is rejected and where allowing the coefficient to vary

across sites improves the quality of our matches.9 In other words, we fit a model to estimate the

within-market propensity score, where the association between some covariates and Catholic school

enrollment is allowed to vary across markets, while others are fixed (allowing the model to ‘borrow

strength” from observations in multiple markets). From this model, we obtain an estimated

propensity score for each student.

For each Catholic school student, we select as matches all public school students who are

enrolled in school within the same market and who have estimated propensity scores within five

percentage points of the target as the target Catholic school student. We use a caliper of .05 here

rather than .01 as above in order to obtain a larger sample of matched students. Even so, in 16 of

the 60 markets, there are 5 or fewer public school students with propensity scores within .05 of any

Catholic school student (despite the fact that there are an average of 42 public school students per

market in the 60 markets). We drop these 16 markets from the sample because they can provide

very little information on within-market Catholic school effects given their small regions of common

support. Within each of the remaining 44 markets, we drop from the sample public and Catholic

students who have no matches (no students in the opposite type of school and with propensity

score within .05 of theirs). We construct weights for the matched students using the same

procedure as described above. To assess the balance of the matched samples, we estimate the

average within-market difference between the matched samples for each covariate X (where we first

standardize each X) using a random-coefficient model of the form 9 We tested models allowing the within-market association between Catholic school enrollment and each of a set of family covariates to vary across markets. These covariates were family income, mother’s age at the birth of her first child, child’s date of birth, birth weight, mother’s occupation score, father’s occupational score, and number of places child has lived for more than 4 months. Some of these were chosen because local features of the educational system may affect their association (e.g., tuition differences across markets may affect the effect of income on Catholic school enrollment; differences in enrollment age cutoff policies in both Catholic and public kindergartens across markets may affect the association between age and Catholic school enrollment). Others were chosen because balance checks suggested that within-market balance might be improved by allowing the associations to vary across markets. Our final model includes random effects on three of these variables: family income, mother’s age at first birth, and date of birth.

21

Page 24: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

ε , [4]

where m indexes markets, and where the market-level error terms and are assumed

multivariate normal with means 0 and an unconstrained variance-covariance matrix. For categorical

covariates, we use a similar model with the appropriate non-linear link function. If the treatment

and control samples are matched on X within each site, we will have 0 and

0. We seek a matching model that minimizes the (or produces a very small) average of

the absolute ’s and average of the ’s. As above, there is no analytic solution to this

minimization; rather, we fit a number of versions of the multilevel propensity score model—adding

higher-order terms, interactions, and/or additional random effects—and match the sample based on

each set of estimated propensity scores, seeking the matching model specification that yields the

minimum imbalance.

To estimate the average within-market effect of Catholic versus public schooling, we fit two

types of models. First, we fit fixed effects models similar to those in models 8 and 9, except that

they are based on the within-county matched sample. These models provide estimates of the

average within-county effect of Catholic schooling. Unlike models 8 and 9, however, these models

do not rely on the additivity assumption of the linear fixed effects model, since they do not rely on

between-county information to estimate average within-county effects.

Second, we fit a pair of three-level random-coefficient models of e formth

ε . [5]

Here is an indicator of whether school j in market m is a Catholic school, and is the

precision-weighed mean value of in market m.10 The coefficient indicates the average within-

10 For these models, we construct the weights slightly differently than in the prior models. Specifically, we construct weights as described in Equation 1, save that we do not include the ECLS-K sample weights here (essentially, we constrain each vi=1), since these weights are not appropriate for use in multi-level models. The ECLS-K panel weights are constructed using county, school, and student level sampling information, and so are not appropriate for use in

22

Page 25: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

market difference in test scores at time t between students in Catholic and public schools in the

within-market matched samples: it is the average Catholic effect at wave t. The error term is

assumed normally distributed with mean 0 and variance to be estimated; it denotes the difference in

the Catholic school effect between market m and the overall average within-market Catholic school

effect. The remaining three error terms , , and ε are the market-, school-, and

student-level error terms, respectively, each assumed independent of the others and normally

distributed with mean 0.

The key parameters of the model are and . We test the null hypothesis that

the Catholic school effect is constant across markets by testing H : 0. We test the

null hypothesis that the average Catholic school effect is 0 by testing H : 0. As above, we fit

two versions of the within-market models, one as described in [5] (Model 12), and one that includes

the propensity score and the wave-1 test score as covariates (Model 13)11 to improve precision and

purge the estimates of bias due to imperfect matching.

Heterogeneity of Catholic school effects

Prior research has suggested that Catholic schooling has more positive effects for black and

Hispanic students and for poor students than for white and non-poor students. Although we

wanted to assess these hypotheses using the ECLS-K data, we were unable to, given the small

multilevel models, which require separate sample weights at each level of the model. Given that the multilevel models include county and school information implicitly, as well as a range of information on student characteristics, the only bias that may remain in the estimation is bias due to differential attrition from the ECLS-K sample. Catholic school students remaining in the sample through fifth grade may be different, on average, than those who transferred out of their school. If students who leave Catholic schools have lower average gains than students who stay in Catholic schools (a plausible scenario, because families may remove their children from Catholic schools if they think they are ineffective), then our omission of weights to adjust for sample attrition may lead to overestimation of the effect of Catholic schooling. However, when we fit Models 1-11 with and without the panel weights included, we obtain very similar results in each case, suggesting that the sample attrition is unrelated to potential outcomes and so does not lead to substantial bias. 11 Specifically, we include the propensity score and wave 1 test score centered around their school means and we include the school-mean propensity score and wave 1 score centered around their precision-weighted market means. In each case, the school means are constructed using the matching weights.

23

Page 26: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

sample sizes of some groups. Table 2 describes the sample sizes, by school type, race/ethnicity, and

poverty status, in the 60 market matched sample. Given the small number of poor and black

students, in particular, we are unable to obtain precise estimates of Catholic school effects by

race/ethnicity and poverty status.

Table 2 here

Comparison of analytic strategies

Each of the analytic strategies we use relies on a different set of assumptions and estimates

the effect of Catholic schooling over different populations. A brief summary of these differences

follows.

First, it is important to note that regression models (e.g., models 1-5) and propensity score

models do not typically estimate the average treatment effect over the same population. The WLS

and fixed effects models produces average treatment effect (ATE) estimates—estimates of the

average Catholic school effect over the total population represented by the ECLS-K analytic sample

(students who were first-time kindergarteners in Catholic or public schools in the Fall of 1998 and

who English proficient at the start of kindergarten). The fixed effects models (models 4 and 5) limit

the generalizability of the estimates to a population represented in the 60 market sample, but this

sample is very similar to that in the full analytic sample (see Table 1), suggesting that these estimates

likely generalize to the full analytic population. The propensity score models (models 6-13), in

contrast, produce estimates of the average Catholic school effect over the population of Catholic

school students for whom public school matches can be found in the data. In other words, these

are average-treatment-effect-on-treated (ATT) estimates—estimates of the average Catholic school

effect among the type of students who attend Catholic schools (they tell us the extent to which the

students in Catholic schools benefit or suffer as a result of attending Catholic school; they do not tell

us the extent of benefit/harm that public school students would have experienced had they attended

24

Page 27: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Catholic schools). If families for whom Catholic schooling would be likely to have a larger effect are

families that are most likely to select Catholic schooling (i.e., if the propensity score is related to the

expected treatment effect), then we would expect that the average of Catholic schooling will be

larger among those with higher propensity to attend Catholic school, meaning that the ATT

estimates will likely be more positive than the ATE estimates (assuming the estimates from each

model are unbiased).

Second, it is important to note that each model contains different potential sources of bias.

As noted above, the regression models may contain bias resulting from both omitted covariates and

from incorrect functional form specification. The propensity score models may contain bias

resulting from omitted covariates or from imperfect matching (though the models containing the

propensity score and wave 1 score as covariates should reduce much of the latter bias). The

difference between the within-market matching and the national matching with county fixed effects

in the outcome model is that the former matches on student level characteristics, on average, and

matches exactly on market characteristics, while the latter matches on student-level covariates, on

average, and adjusts for market characteristics using market fixed effects. Because we use a wider

caliper (.05 rather than .01) in the within-market matching, the within-market matched samples may

differ more on observed covariates than the national matched samples. In contrast, the within-

market samples will match exactly on market characteristics, while the national matched samples

likely will not, given the unequal distribution of Catholic schooling around the country. The

tradeoff in potential bias between models 9 and both of 11 and 13, then, is that model 9 includes

better matching on observable student covariates, but relies on regression adjustment to control for

market differences, while models 11 and 13 rely on somewhat weaker matching on observable

covariates, but ensure exact matching on market/county factors.

Third, it is important to note that the different sample sizes across models means that each

25

Page 28: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

of our estimators will have different precision. While we may prefer the models based on within-

market matching (models 10-13) from the perspective of bias reduction, these models will have

relatively low statistical power—they will not provide very precise estimates of average Catholic

schooling effects—because they rely on much smaller samples than the other models. For this

reason, it is important to attend to the confidence intervals for each estimate and not just their point

estimates.

4. Results

Matching

We begin by describing the balance achieved by the propensity score matching models.

Table 3 describes the average values of selected covariates in the Catholic and public matched

samples. In general the differences between the Catholic school students and their matched public

school counterparts are much smaller than in the full data (the average absolute differences .045 and

.077 standard deviations in the national and within-market matched samples, respectively).

Importantly, the differences in wave 1 math and reading scores are small and non-significant

(approximately .01 standard deviations in math and .10 standard deviations in reading) in the

matched samples, despite the fact that we did not use the wave 1 scores in the matching algorithm.

This suggests that the matching models are successful at capturing differences between public and

Catholic school students in factors that may affect their cognitive development when they enter

school.

Table 3 here

Effects on reading scores

Table 4 presents the estimated effects of Catholic schooling on students’ reading scores

26

Page 29: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

obtained from models 1-13. Model 1 reports unadjusted differences in reading scores between

Catholic and public school students, and shows that Catholic school students score better than

public school students at each wave in kindergarten through fifth grade. Nonetheless, all but one of

the other models indicates that these differences are fully accounted for by observable differences

between Catholic and public school students. In fact, the most obvious result from Table 4 is that

we find no evidence of even a modest Catholic school effect (positive or negative) on students’

reading scores at any wave. Moreover, the largest estimates are from the models predicting wave 1

Catholic school effects. The consistently positive sign on these (and the significance of the wave 1

effect in Model 4) suggest that the covariates and/or matching may not have fully accounted for

selection, since it is unlikely that Catholic schooling would have had a large effect so soon after

students started kindergarten. Because of this pattern, we focus on the results from models that

include the wave 1 score as a covariate (Models 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, and 13).

Table 4 here

Given the size of the estimated standard errors in these models, the models have 80% power

to detect reading effects on the order of 1.00 to 1.75 points (.100-.175 standard deviations) in

magnitude.12 Most of the point estimates are negative, but none approach statistical significance.

In addition to providing estimates of the average within-market effect of Catholic schooling,

Model 12 and 13 allow us to estimate the variance of the effect across markets. In both models 12

and 13, we cannot reject the null hypothesis that the effect of Catholic schooling is constant across

markets (all p-values are >.50). We therefore report results for Models 12 and 13 in Table 4 from

versions of these models that constrain the effect of Catholic schooling to be constant across

markets.

12 We compute the minimum detectable effect size as 2.80 times the standard error (Bloom 1995).

27

Page 30: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Effects on math scores

Table 5 presents the estimated effects of Catholic schooling on students’ math scores. Here

the pattern of estimates is quite different than in the reading models. First, note that covariate

adjustment and/or matching generally does a much better job of accounting for the observed

Catholic-public school difference in fall kindergarten scores (see Models 2, 4, 6, 8, 10, 12), with the

exception of model 4, where the difference is still moderately large. Because of this, we do not

observe large differences in the estimated effects at later waves between the models that include the

wave 1 score and those that do not. Nonetheless, we focus on the models that include the wave 1

score in each case, since these models have increased precision (smaller standard errors) due to the

inclusion of the wave 1 score.

Table 5 here

The regression models (model 3) suggest that Catholic schooling has a negative effect on

math scores, with the effect beginning as early as the Spring of kindergarten ( =-.75) and growing to

-2.0 by the spring of third grade. None of the other models find a significant effect this early,

however. Nonetheless, each of models 5, 7, and 9 estimates that indicate Catholic schooling has had

a negative effect on students’ math skills by the spring of third grade (and persisting through fifth

grade). These effects are moderate in size, ranging from one-tenth to more than one-fifth of a

standard deviation of math scores. Unlike the regression and national propensity score models, the

models based on the within-market matching yield estimates that are less negative (closer to zero, or

positive) than the other models, and that are not statistically distinguishable from zero.

As above, Models 12 and 13 allow us to test the hypothesis that the effect of Catholic

schooling on math scores varies across markets. Again, however, in both models 12 and 13, we

cannot reject the null hypothesis that the effect of Catholic schooling is constant across markets

(p>.5 in all models). We therefore report results for Models 12 and 13 in Table 5 from versions of

28

Page 31: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

these models that constrain the effect of Catholic schooling to be constant across markets.

5. Discussion

Main findings

Perhaps the most striking findings from our analyses are that 1) there is no evidence in any

of our models that Catholic schooling has a positive effect on reading or math skills in kindergarten

through fifth grade; and 2) Catholic schooling appears, in some of our models, to have a relatively

sizeable negative effect on Catholic school students’ math skills during the period from kindergarten

through fifth grade. This negative effect emerges during the period from first to third grade in each

of models 3, 5, 7, and 9. This result is consistent with the only other study of the effects of Catholic

schooling in elementary school, which found that Catholic school students score, on average, one-

half a standard deviation below their observationally similar public school counterparts on NAEP 4th

grade math tests (Lubienski and Lubienski 2006). Our estimates are smaller than this (.22 standard

deviations in third grade in model 7), but in the same direction. Given the richness of the ECLS-K

dataset, our estimates are conditioned on a much more extensive set of covariates (via matching in

our case) than the Lubienski and Lubienski NAEP estimates. Lubienski and Lubienski did not

estimate Catholic school effects on reading scores, so we cannot compare our (null) findings

regarding reading effects with theirs.

Figures 1 & 2 here

Interpreting the estimates from different models

Not all of our models find significant negative effects of Catholic schooling in third and fifth

grade, however. Models 7 and 9 yield negative and statistically significant estimates of Catholic

school effects on math skills by third grade. Models 11 and 13, however, yield estimates that are

29

Page 32: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

likewise negative, but whose standard errors are sufficiently large that we cannot reject the null

hypothesis that the effect of Catholic schooling is zero. Given the difference in estimates across

models (or at least the differences in whether we can reject the null hypothesis of no effect of

Catholic schooling), what can we conclude regarding the effect of Catholic schooling on math skills?

First, consider the different interpretations of the estimates in models 7, 9, 11, and 13. We

focus on these models because the matching provides arguably better (or at least no worse)

adjustment for confounding that does the covariate adjustment in models 3 and 5. In addition,

models 7, 9, 11, and 13 each provide estimates of the same estimand—the average Catholic

schooling effect on students enrolled in Catholic schools (ATT estimates)—while models 3 and 5

estimate average Catholic schooling effects over the population (ATE estimates). Model 7 estimates

the average effect of attending the Catholic school attended by the average Catholic school student

in the nation relative to attending the average public school attended by public school students

nationwide who are comparable to Catholic school students. The propensity score estimates

simulate an experiment where we assigned Catholic school students to attend public schools all over

the country. This is, of course, not a practical experiment, though the estimates may be nonetheless

informative. As a national average, they indicate that students enrolled in Catholic schools learn

substantially less (.21-.22 standard deviations less in math by 3rd and 5th grade, s.e.=0.05, p<.001)

than similar students enrolled in public schools. This translates into a difference of roughly 3

months of schooling by third grade and 4 or more months of schooling by fifth grade.13

From a policy or parental decision perspective, however, these estimates are not exactly what

13 A back-of-the envelope calculation provides this translation. Using the IRT theta scores for the ECLS-K tests, we compute that the average student gains roughly .75 on the theta math metric from spring first grade through spring third grade, and gains roughly .42 in the theta math metric from spring third grade through spring fifth grade. The standard deviation of theta scores in third and fifth grade is roughly .40. This means that students gain, on average, roughly one standard deviation per year from first to third grade, and roughly one-half a standard deviation per year from third to fifth. Thus, the fifth of a standard deviation effect size we estimate in third grade translates to either a fifth of a year (2 academic months) difference in first-third grade learning or two-fifths of a year (4 academic months) difference in third-fifth grade learning. Assuming a linear decline in learning rates, this suggests Catholic school students are roughly 3 months behind their public school counterparts in third grade, and 4 or more months behind in fifth grade.

30

Page 33: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

we want. Given the choice to enroll in a local public or Catholic school, what should a parent

choose? Models 9, 11, and 13 each attempt to estimate the Catholic school effect relative to local

public schooling. This is a more relevant estimand from the point of view of a parent. The

estimates from each of these models are uniformly less negative than those from model 7,

suggesting that the average Catholic school student lives in a county where the public schools are of

lower quality (in terms of teaching math skills) than the average public school nationally. In other

words, given their observed characteristics, students appear more likely to be enrolled in Catholic

schools in counties where the public schools attended by similar students are of lower quality. This

makes sense, as it suggests that the choice of Catholic versus public schooling is based in part on

some knowledge of the quality of local public schooling.

Nonetheless, model 9 indicates that Catholic schools are less successful at teaching math

than are local public schools (-0.16 standard deviations by fifth grade, s.e.=0.06, p<.05), while

models 11 and 13 are inconclusive on this point. How are we to resolve the differences between

model 9 and models 11 and 13? Although each of models 9, 11, and 13 estimate the within-market

Catholic school effect, they do so on the basis of different assumptions and with different statistical

power. One the one hand, it may be that the Catholic school effect estimates from model 9 are

biased by the lack of within-market matching. The matched samples in model 9 include very few

matched students within the same county (only 2% of the Catholic students have matches in model

9 who are in the same county), meaning that the adjustment for county differences using fixed

effects relies heavily on the additivity assumption of the linear fixed effects model.

From the perspective of potential bias reduction, our preferred models are 11 and 13. These

models eliminate bias due to differences between Catholic and public school students in factors that

affect math skill development prior to kindergarten (this is evident in the wave 1 difference in math

scores, which is less than .01 standard deviations in the within-market matched sample). They also

31

Page 34: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

eliminate bias due to between-county differences between Catholic and public school students

without relying on the additivity assumption of the fixed effects model (this is accomplished by the

fact that they rely on within-market matching). However, the potential bias reduction of models 11

and 13 comes at the cost of a substantial loss of precision. The estimates from models 11 and 13

have much larger standard errors than our other models, a result of the fact that they rely on a much

smaller sample size—the sample of Catholic and public school students who have matches within

their same county in the ECLS-K sample. As a result, minimum detectable effect sizes in models 11

and 13 are roughly .25 standard deviations, and the confidence intervals generally span values from

-0.25 to +0.10 in third and fifth grade. These estimates are consistent with the possibility that

Catholic schools have no effect or even a small effect, but they are also consistent with the

possibility that Catholic schools have a moderately-sized negative effect.

Between models 11 and 13, the key difference is the fact that model 11 estimates the student-

level effect of attending a Catholic school. We can think of it as indicating the average difference in

math skills we would observe if students were randomly assigned to attend either Catholic or public

schooling, but we let their parents choose which school of their assigned type they enrolled in.

Model 13, in contrast, estimates the school-level difference between Catholic and public schools. We

can think of it as indicating the average difference in math skills we would observe if students were

randomly assigned to attend randomly chosen local Catholic schools or a randomly chosen local

public school. If parents are good at choosing the better Catholic school among available options,

but have no choice regarding public school, then we would expect model 11 to yield more positive

comparisons among Catholic schools and local public schools. In math, this is what we observe—

the point estimates of Catholic school effects in model 11 are generally more positive (by .05-.10

standard deviations) than the corresponding estimates in model 13. Given the size of the standard

errors in these models, however, these differences between models are not statistically significant.

32

Page 35: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

In reading, however, the model 11 estimates are generally more negative than those in model 13, so

we caution against making too much of these differences.

If, for example, Catholic schools differ in quality, and our sample sizes are smaller in low-

quality public schools, then the between-school differences will differ from the between-student

differences, since in the latter each student gets equal weight, while in the former each school gets equal

weight. The fact that the estimates are more positive in the student-level model (11) suggests that

our weighted samples are smaller in lower-quality Catholic schools than in higher-quality Catholic

schools and/or smaller in higher-quality public schools than in lower-quality public schools. The

former would be expected if students are more likely to enroll and stay in higher-quality Catholic

schools than lower-quality schools.

In sum, students in Catholic elementary schools, on average, lose ground in math skills and

stay even in reading skills by third and fifth grade relative to similar students in public schools

nationwide. However, given that Catholic school enrollment is more likely in places where the

public schools are less successful at teaching math, our estimates suggest that Catholic students lose

somewhat less ground (and maybe lose no ground) in math relative to what they would have learned

had they attended their local public schools instead. Our estimates of these local effects are

relatively imprecise, however. We cannot rule out the possibility that Catholic schools are equally

good as their local public schools; nor can we rule out the possibility that they are significantly

worse.

How well do our estimates do at eliminating selection bias?

An issue to consider is the extent to which our models are successful at eliminating selection

bias. There are several reasons to believe we that we have been successful at eliminating most, if

not, all selection bias through our matching strategies (and less successful in the fixed effects

33

Page 36: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

models). First, although we did not match on students’ fall kindergarten test scores, the matching

models generally show relatively small differences between Catholic and public school students’ test

scores in the fall of kindergarten. Had we failed to match on some factors that were related to

Catholic school enrollment and to students’ cognitive development, we would expect to see

differences in scores between matched Catholic and public school students as early as fall

kindergarten. Because this is too early for Catholic schooling to have had a substantial effect on

student scores, evidence of Catholic school “effects” in wave 1 would suggest a failure to match on

all covariates relevant to Catholic schooling and cognitive skill. Our matching at wave 1 is

particularly good in the math outcome matching models (in models 6, 8, 10, and 12, the fall

kindergarten “effects” are all within .01 standard deviations of zero). In the reading models, the

Catholic-public differences between matched students are positive and larger (as large as .10

standard deviations) at wave 1, though not significant. Had there been any evidence of reading

effects, we would have been skeptical, given the less than perfect matching.

Second, we expect that any remaining selection bias in our within-market estimates would

likely tend to bias our estimates upward, so these estimates might be seen as upper bounds on the

within-county effects of Catholic schooling (Altonji, Elder, and Taber 2005). Setting aside the

uncertainty of the estimates for the moment, the negative coefficients on math scores are difficult to

explain away as a result of selection bias—we would have to postulate some unobserved factor(s)

that, within a given county and net of the long list of covariates including in the matching, are

positively correlated with enrollment in Catholic school, uncorrelated with fall kindergarten test

scores, but negatively correlated with math scores in third and fifth grade. In other words, we would

have to believe that the students who attend Catholic schools would have had similar math skills in

kindergarten but lower math skills by third and fifth grade, had they enrolled in their local public

school, than their observationally similar counterparts who were enrolled in public schools in the

34

Page 37: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

same county. We think this implausible.

Heterogeneity of Catholic school effects

Given existing research suggesting that Catholic high schools and voucher programs may

have larger benefits for minority students than white students, we wanted to investigate whether

there was any evidence of hetereogeneity of effects across subgroups. However, small sample sizes

limited our ability to use the propensity score matching estimator to estimate effects by subgroups

with any useful degree of statistical power. We did, however, test for variation in effects across

schooling markets (counties). We found no evidence of variation in Catholic school effects across

counties. Finally, because some prior research has found that Catholic high school effects are largest

for students with the lowest propensity to attend Catholic schools (Morgan 2001), we conducted

some additional analyses (not shown) to investigate whether a similar pattern holds in our data. We

found no evidence that the effects vary by propensity score within markets. In all, our results

suggest that Catholic schooling exerts a negative effect on math skill development, and this effect is

stable (within our power to detect) across the population and across locations.

Limitations and suggestions

Although the within-market matching estimator was successful at eliminating mean

differences between Catholic school students and their local public school counterparts, our ability

to obtain precise within-market estimates here was limited by the relatively small samples within

each county. Although the stratified random sampling of ECLS-K results in a number of counties

with sampled Catholic and public schools, a prospective design to estimate such effects would be

much stronger if we had larger within-county samples. Better still, we would have liked within-

district (rather than within-county) samples, so that we could be sure the Catholic and public school

35

Page 38: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

students are actually in the same public school market. Likewise, our ability to estimate effects by

subgroup would be improved by oversampling minority and poor students in Catholic schools.

6. Conclusion

We have focused in this paper on obtaining unbiased estimates of the effects of Catholic

schooling relative to public schooling. When we use public school students nationwide to provide a

counterfactual estimate of how Catholic school students would have performed in public schools,

we find that strong evidence indicating that Catholic elementary schools are less successful at

teaching math skills than public schools, but no more or less successful at teaching reading skills.

However, this conclusion is unambiguous only when we compare Catholic schooling to the national

average of public schooling. When we compare Catholic schooling to local public schooling, we

obtain estimated math effects that are generally somewhere between the (negative) national

estimates and zero (but statistically indistinguishable from either).

These results are somewhat surprising, we think. Certainly nothing in the Catholic high

school effects literature hints at negative effects of Catholic school enrollment, even though all prior

estimates are like our national estimates—the debate in that literature is between positive effects or

no effects. Nonetheless, NAEP data do suggest a negative effect of Catholic schooling on math

skills in 4th and 8th grades (Lubienski and Lubienski 2006; Lubienski, Lubienski, and Crane 2007), so

our results are not completely without precedent.

With regard to the national estimates, the obvious question is “why?” What features of

Catholic elementary schooling differ from public schooling that might lead to lower math skills (but

no differences in reading skills) among students enrolled in Catholic schools relative to their similar

peers in public schools nationwide? Perhaps the most likely explanations are differences in teachers,

instruction, and/or curricula. Lubienski, Lubienski, and Crane (2007), for example, find that

36

Page 39: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Catholic 4th grade schools have fewer certified teachers, spend less time on teacher professional

development, and focus their math curricula less on NCTM-recommended topics such geometry,

measurement, probability, and algebra than do public 4th grade schools. Moreover, they find that

these differences together account for roughly 20% of the adjusted difference in 4th grade NAEP

math scores between Catholic and public schools. It is, however, beyond the score of this paper to

investigate the mechanism by which Catholic schooling appears to produce lower math skills for

students enrolled in Catholic schooling.

Another possible explanation for some of the patterns we observe is that Catholic schools

operate in a market where they compete with the public schools for students. In some ways, our

estimates are consistent with the idea that parents choose Catholic schooling in part as a response to

low-quality public schools, though they are far from conclusive on this point. They may simply

choose for reasons that are correlated with public school quality (safety, discipline, etc.). However,

we find no evidence here that the Catholic schools parents choose are systematically better than

their local public schools, so it is clear that a well-informed comparison of school quality does not

solely determine their decisions. It may be that parents have better information about the quality of

public schools than of Catholic schools; it may be that parents choose for reasons other than school

quality in regard to math teaching (e.g., religious preference, discipline, school safety, etc.). Beyond

speculation, however, we can say no more on the basis of the evidence presented here.

37

Page 40: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

References

Abadie, Alberto and Guido W. Imbens. 2002. "Simple and bias-corrected matching estimators for

average treatment effects." National Bureau of Economic Research.

Altonji, Joseph G., Todd E. Elder, and Christopher R. Taber. 2002. "An Evaluation of Instrumental

Variable Strategies for Estimating the Effects of Catholic Schools." National Bureau of

Economic Research, Cambridge, Massachusetts.

—. 2005. "Selection on observed and unobserved variables: Assessing the effectiveness of Catholic

schools." Journal of Political Economy 113:151-184.

Baker, David P. 1999. "Schooling all the masses: Reconsidering the origins of American schooling in

the postbellum era." Sociology of Education 72:197-215.

Baker, David P. and Cornelius Riordan. 1998. "The 'eliting' of the common American Catholic

school and the national education crisis." Phi Delta Kappan 80:16-23.

Bloom, Howard S. 1995. "Minimum Detectable Effects: A Simple Way to Report the Power of

Experimental Designs." Evaluation Review 19:547-556.

Broughman, Stephen P. and Nancy L. Swaim. 2006. "Characteristics of Private Schools in the

United States: Results From the 2003–2004 Private School Universe Survey." U.S.

Department of Education. National Center for Education Statistics., Washington, DC.

Bryk, Anthony S., Valerie E. Lee, and Peter B. Holland. 1993. Catholic Schools and the Common Good.

New York: Basic Books.

Campbell, David E., Martin R. West, and Paul E. Peterson. 2005. "Participation in a national,

means-tested school voucher program." Journal of Policy Analysis and Management 24:523-541.

Coleman, James S., Thomas Hoffer, and Sally Kilgore. 1982a. "Achievement and segregation in

secondary schools: A further look at public and private school differences." Sociology of

Education 55:162-182.

38

Page 41: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

—. 1982b. "Cognitive outcomes in public and private schools." Sociology of Education 55:65-76.

—. 1982c. High School Achievement: Public, Catholic, and Private Schools Compared. New York: Basic

Books.

Evans, William N. and Robert M. Schwab. 1995. "Finishing High School and Starting College: Do

Catholic Schools Make a Difference?" The Quarterly Journal of Economics 110:941-974.

Goldberger, Arthur S. and Glen G. Cain. 1982. "The Causal Analysis of Cognitive Outcomes in the

Coleman, Hoffer and Kilgore Report." Sociology of Education 55:103-122.

Grogger, Jeffrey and Derek Neal. 2000. "Further Evidence on the Effects of Catholic Secondary

Schooling." Brookings-Wharton Papers on Urban Affairs:151-201.

Ho, Daniel E., Kosuke Imai, Gary King, and Elizabeth A. Stuart. 2007. "Matching as

Nonparametric Preprocessing for Reducing Model Dependence in Parametric Causal

Inference." Political Analysis 15:199-236.

Hoffer, Thomas, Andrew M. Greeley, and James S. Coleman. 1985. "Achievement Growth in Public

and Catholic Schools." Sociology of Education 58:74-97.

Howell, William G. and Paul E. Peterson. 2006. The education gap: Vouchers and urban schools.

Washington, DC: Brookings Instutution.

Imai, Kosuke, Gary King, and Elizabeth A. Stuart. forthcoming. "Misunderstandings among

experimentalists and observationalists about causal inference." Journal of the Royal Statistical

Society, Series A.

Imbens, Guido W. 2004. "Nonparametric estimation of average treatment effects under exogeneity:

A review." The Review of Economics and Statistics 86:4-29.

Lubienski, Christopher and Sarah Thule Lubienski. 2006. "Charter, Private, Public Schools and

Academic Achievement: New Evidence From NAEP Mathematics Data." National Center

39

Page 42: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

for the Study of Privatization in Education, Teachers College, Columbia University, New

York.

Lubienski, Sarah Thule, Christopher Lubienski, and Corrinna Crawford Crane. 2007.

"Understanding Achievement Differences in Public and Private Schools: The Roles of

School Climate, Teacher Certification, and Teaching Practices." in Annual Meeting of the

American Education Research Association. Chicago, IL.

McEwan, Patrick J. and Martin Carnoy. 2000. "The effectiveness and efficiency of private schools in

Chile’s voucher system." Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis 22:213-239.

Morgan, Stephen L. 2001. "Counterfactuals, causal effect heterogeneity, and the Catholic school

effect on learning." Sociology of Education 74:341-373.

Morgan, Stephen L. and Jennifer J. Todd. 2007. "A Diagnostic Routine for the Detection of

Consequential Heterogeneity of Causal Effects: With a Demonstration from School Effects

Research." Cornell University.

Murnane, Richard J., S. Newstead, and R. J. Olsen. 1985. "Comparing public and private schools:

The puzzling role of selection bias." Journal of Business and Economic Statistics 3:23-35.

Neal, Derek A. 1997. "The Effects of Catholic Secondary Schooling on Educational Achievement."

Journal of Labor Economics 15:98-123.

Pollack, Judith M., Michelle Narajian, Donald A. Rock, Sally Atkins-Burnett, and Elvira Germino

Hausken. 2005. "Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten Class of 1998-99

(ECLS-K), Psychometric Report for the Fifth Grade." U.S. Department of Education,

National Center for Education Statistics, Washington, DC.

Pollack, Judith M., Donald A. Rock, Michael J. Weiss, Sally Atkins-Burnett, Karen Tourangeau, Jerry

West, and Elvira Germino Hausken. 2005. "Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-

40

Page 43: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

41

Kindergarten Class of 1998-99 (ECLS-K), Psychometric Report for the Third Grade." U.S.

Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Washington, DC.

Raudenbush, Stephen W. and Anthony S. Bryk. 2002. Hierarchical Linear Models: Applications and Data

Analysis Methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Raudenbush, Stephen W., Anthony S. Bryk, Yuk Fai Cheong, and Richard Congdon. 2005.

Hierarchical Linear and Non-linear Modeling, Version 6: Users’ Guide and Software Program. Chicago:

Scientific Software International.

Reardon, Sean F. 2006. "Estimating Effects of Educational Practice and Policy Across Multiple Sites

Using a Multisite Matching Estimator." in Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research

Association. San Francisco, CA.

—. 2007. "Thirteen ways of looking at the black-white test score gap."

Reardon, Sean F. and John T. Yun. 2002. "Private school racial enrollments and segregation." Civil

Rights Project, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.

Rock, Donald A. and Judith M. Pollack. 2002. "Early Childhood Longitudinal Study-Kindergarten

Class of 1998-99 (ECLS-K), Psychometric Report for Kindergarten Through First Grade."

U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, Washington, DC.

Rosenbaum, Paul R., and Rubin, Donald B. 1983. "The central role of the propensity score in

observational studies for causal effects." Biometrika 70:41-55.

Rouse, Cecilia E. 1998. "Private school vouchers and student achievement: An evaluation of the

Milwaukee parental choice program." The Quarterly Journal of Economics 113:553-602.

Page 44: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

TABLE 1: Selected Characteristics of Catholic and Public School Students in Analytic Samples Full Analytic Sample 60 Market Sample

Variable description Catholic

Mean

Public-Catholic

DifferenceStandardized

Difference Catholic

Mean

Public-Catholic

DifferenceStandardized

DifferenceWave-1 math t-score 54.505 -4.195 ** -0.448 55.258 -4.718 ** -0.504Wave-1 reading t-score 54.466 -4.949 ** -0.518 55.106 -5.494 ** -0.575Family income (in $10,000) 7.074 -2.340 ** -0.533 7.114 -2.055 ** -0.468Mother's occupational score 33.958 -5.805 ** -0.263 34.731 -6.142 ** -0.278Mother's age at child's birth 31.564 -7.320 ** -0.332 32.303 -5.703 ** -0.259Child ever attended center care 0.845 -0.183 ** -0.401 0.855 -0.173 ** -0.379Ever receive food stamps 0.033 0.081 ** 0.234 0.029 0.079 ** 0.227Mother's age at first child birth 25.840 -3.237 ** -0.548 26.140 -3.210 ** -0.554Family at or below poverty line 0.028 0.132 ** 0.357 0.023 0.123 ** 0.332 Average Absolute Standardized Difference 0.308 0.306Sample Size 1,078 6,402 1,001 2,554 School Count 97 692 89 285 Notes: Sample means are weighted by ECLS-K panel weight c1_6fc0. **p<.01. Standardized differences are computed by dividing the public-Catholic difference by the pooled standard deviations of the Catholic and public school student samples in the full analytic sample. For Catholic-public sample means on the full list of covariates included in the analyses, see Appendix Table A1. Average absolute standardized difference is the average of the absolute value of the standardized public-Catholic difference in each of the variables listed in the top panel of Table A1 (which includes more covariates than those listed here).

42

Page 45: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

TABLE 2: Available Analytic Sample Sizes, by Poverty, Race/Ethnicity, and School Sector Public Catholic Total Poverty status

Not in poverty 1,838 968 2,806In poverty 149 27 176

Race

White 1,382 744 2,126Black 80 38 118Hispanic 323 121 444Asian 134 55 189Other 68 37 105

Total 1,987 995 2,982

Sample based on model 8 and 9 sample (nationally matched sample restricted to 60 markets).

43

Page 46: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

TABLE 3: Selected Characteristics of Catholic and Public School Students in Matched Samples National Matched Sample Within-Market Matched Sample

Variable description Catholic

Mean

Public-Catholic

DifferenceStandardized

Difference Catholic

Mean

Public-Catholic

DifferenceStandardized

DifferenceWave-1 math t-score 54.515 0.109 0.012 54.492 -0.059 -0.006Wave-1 reading t-score 54.480 -0.631 -0.066 54.373 -1.183 -0.124Family income (in $10,000) 7.066 0.151 0.034 6.849 0.556 0.127Mother's occupational score 33.919 -0.548 -0.025 33.890 1.474 0.067Mother's age at child's birth 31.570 0.214 0.010 31.556 1.814 0.082Child ever attended center care 0.870 -0.037 -0.062 0.879 -0.034 -0.074Ever receive food stamps 0.048 -0.008 -0.024 0.051 -0.042 ** -0.121Mother's age at first child birth 25.829 -0.004 -0.001 25.573 0.661 0.112Family at or below poverty line 0.035 -0.008 -0.021 0.037 -0.021 -0.056 Average Absolute Standardized Difference 0.045

0.077

Sample Size 1,073 3,744 744 478 School Count 96 359 61 50 Notes: Catholic school student samples are weighted by ECLS-K panel weight c1_6fc0. Public school student samples are weighted their matching weights (see text for details). Within-market differences are estimated by a model like model 10 (see text). Except as noted (** p<.10), all public-Catholic differences are statistically insignificant (p>.05). Standardized differences are computed by dividing the public-Catholic difference by the pooled standard deviations of the Catholic and public school student samples in the full analytic sample. For Catholic-public sample means on the full list of covariates included in the analyses, see Appendix Table A2. Average absolute standardized difference is the average of the absolute value of the standardized public-Catholic difference in each of the variables listed in the top panel of Table A2 (which includes more covariates than those listed here), excluding the propensity score.

44

Page 47: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

TABLE 4: Estimated Effects of Catholic Schooling on Reading Scores, by Wave and Model

WLS WLS with market

fixed effects National propensity score matchingWithin-market propensity score

matchingWave Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Model 7 Model 8 Model 9 Model 10 Model 11 Model 12 Model 13 Fall Kindergarten 4.949** 0.919 1.589* 0.631 0.570 1.183 0.719 (0.726) (0.701) (0.753) (0.809) (0.850) (0.911) (0.902) Spring Kindergarten 3.663** 0.277 -0.392 1.173 -0.009 0.124 -0.320 0.526 0.057 0.543 -0.293 0.480 0.400 (0.711) (0.657) (0.395) (0.804) (0.496) (0.766) (0.450) (0.770) (0.493) (0.982) (0.559) (0.783) (0.742) Spring 1st Grade 3.156** 0.214 -0.311 0.879 -0.052 -0.096 -0.430 0.317 0.096 -0.168 -0.805 -0.242 -0.382 (0.644) (0.573) (0.415) (0.739) (0.553) (0.632) (0.470) (0.735) (0.580) (0.844) (0.583) (0.706) (0.687) Spring 3rd Grade 3.573** 0.162 -0.300 0.878 0.060 -0.200 -0.495 0.151 -0.231 0.226 -0.225 -0.016 -0.006 (0.632) (0.502) (0.375) (0.626) (0.500) (0.619) (0.482) (0.606) (0.477) (0.829) (0.686) (0.682) (0.666) Spring 5th Grade 3.845** 0.052 -0.390 0.778 0.028 -0.262 -0.556 0.324 -0.104 0.872 0.459 0.551 0.556 (0.616) (0.485) (0.361) (0.606) (0.483) (0.621) (0.487) (0.631) (0.537) (0.995) (0.883) (0.732) (0.727)Included covariates Covariates included Yes Yes Yes Yes Propensity score Yes Yes Yes YesWave-1 test score Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes YesMarket fixed effects Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Sample Matched sample Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes60 market sample Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes YesWithin-market match Yes Yes Yes YesN(catholic) 1,078 1,078 1,078 1,001 1,001 1,073 1,073 995 995 744 744 744 744N(public) 6,402 6,402 6,402 2,554 2,554 3,744 3,744 1,987 1,987 478 478 478 478Models 1-5 include ECLS-K sampling weights (c1_6fc0). Models 6-11 include matching weights based on Equation 1. Models 12-13 include matching weights constructed without ECLS-K sampling weights. Models 12 and 13 do not include a random effect on Catholic school. Models 10-13 are based on within-market matched sample within 44 counties with at least 5 matched public school students. Standard errors (in parentheses) are corrected for clustering at the county level. See text for details on samples, models, and construction of matching weights.

45

Page 48: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

46

TABLE 5. Estimated Effects of Catholic Schooling on Math Scores, by Wave and Model

WLS WLS with market

fixed effects National propensity score matchingWithin-market propensity score

matchingWave Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Model 7 Model 8 Model 9 Model 10 Model 11 Model 12 Model 13 Fall Kindergarten 4.195** 0.360 0.936 -0.109 0.097 0.059 -0.018 (0.550) (0.479) (0.612) (0.536) (0.637) (0.607) (0.847) Spring Kindergarten 3.290** -0.474 -0.748* 0.341 -0.394 -0.671 -0.590 -0.139 -0.303 0.122 0.106 0.313 0.503 (0.631) (0.570) (0.352) (0.695) (0.395) (0.662) (0.444) (0.753) (0.492) (0.803) (0.617) (0.807) (0.771) Spring 1st Grade 2.618** -0.681 -0.891** 0.449 -0.090 -0.916~ -0.853* -0.324 -0.511 0.690 0.720 -0.357 -0.295 (0.552) (0.461) (0.340) (0.636) (0.465) (0.545) (0.388) (0.671) (0.481) (0.973) (0.832) (0.804) (0.780) Spring 3rd Grade 1.784** -1.780** -2.025** -0.604 -1.243** -2.230** -2.168** -1.412* -1.567** -0.869 -0.883 -1.209 -1.324 (0.556) (0.502) (0.403) (0.610) (0.477) (0.588) (0.475) (0.689) (0.545) (1.095) (0.947) (0.920) (0.924) Spring 5th Grade 2.118** -1.710** -1.935** -0.390 -0.970* -2.152** -2.088** -1.381~ -1.565* -0.359 -0.347 -0.814 -0.895 (0.570) (0.499) (0.399) (0.629) (0.492) (0.647) (0.526) (0.757) (0.609) (0.998) (0.880) (0.872) (0.890)Included covariates Covariates included Yes Yes Yes Yes Propensity score Yes Yes Yes YesWave-1 test score Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes YesMarket fixed effects Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Sample Matched sample Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes60 market sample Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes YesWithin-market match Yes Yes Yes YesN(catholic) 1,078 1,078 1,078 1,001 1,001 1,073 1,073 995 995 744 744 744 744N(public) 6,402 6,402 6,402 2,554 2,554 3,744 3,744 1,987 1,987 478 478 478 478Models 1-5 include ECLS-K sampling weights (c1_6fc0). Models 6-11 include matching weights based on Equation 1. Models 12-13 include matching weights constructed without ECLS-K sampling weights. Models 12 and 13 do not include a random effect on Catholic school. Models 10-13 are based on within-market matched sample within 44 counties with at least 5 matched public school students. Standard errors (in parentheses) are corrected for clustering at the county level. See text for details on samples, models, and construction of matching weights.

Page 49: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Figure 1

-.50

-.25

.00

.25

.50

Effe

ct siz

e

Model 1 Model 3 Model 5 Model 7 Model 9 Model 11 Model 13

SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5

estimated Catholic effect

95% CI bounds

Catholic school effects on reading score, by model and wave

47

Page 50: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

48

-.50

-.25

.00

.25

.50

Effe

ct siz

e

Model 1 Model 3 Model 5 Model 7 Model 9 Model 11 Model 13

SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5 SKS1 S3 S5

estimated Catholic effect

95% CI bounds

Catholic school effects on math score, by model and wave

Figure 2

Page 51: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Appendix Table A1 Full sample Sample restricted to 60 markets

Catholic Public-Catholic

p-value

Standardized difference Catholic

Public-Catholic

p-value

Standardized difference

Variable description

Propensity score 0.223 -0.114 ** -1.084 0.231 -0.114 ** -1.080Wave-1 math t-score 54.505 -4.195 ** -0.448 55.258 -4.718 ** -0.504Wave-1 reading t-score 54.466 -4.949 ** -0.518 55.106 -5.494 ** -0.575 Date of birth (measured in days) 80.642 -3.422 -0.028 71.204 -0.981 -0.008Father's occupational score 39.817 -9.748 ** -0.475 40.437 -9.661 ** -0.471Family income (in $10,000) 7.074 -2.340 ** -0.533 7.114 -2.055 ** -0.468Entered kindergarten late (0/1) 0.082 -0.017 -0.070 0.090 -0.024 * -0.099Mother's occupational score 33.958 -5.805 ** -0.263 34.731 -6.142 ** -0.278Number of places child has lived for at least 4 months 1.754 0.481 ** 0.359 1.733 0.498 ** 0.371Mother's age at child's birth (in years) 31.564 -7.320 ** -0.332 32.303 -5.703 ** -0.259Child ever attended center care (0/1) 0.845 -0.183 ** -0.401 0.855 -0.173 ** -0.379Child ever attended pre-k care (0/1) 0.789 -0.214 ** -0.442 0.804 -0.201 ** -0.415Child in center care now (0/1) 0.248 -0.070 * -0.175 0.250 -0.079 ** -0.197Ever receive food stamps (0/1) 0.033 0.081 ** 0.234 0.029 0.079 ** 0.227Father's current age (in years) 36.788 -4.146 ** -0.388 37.095 -4.055 ** -0.380Mother's age at first child birth (in years) 25.840 -3.237 ** -0.548 26.140 -3.210 ** -0.544Mother's current age (in years) 34.757 -2.236 ** -0.330 34.916 -1.867 ** -0.275Child was in non-relative care on a regular basis the year before entering kindergarten (0/1) 0.181 -0.017 -0.047 0.187 -0.016 -0.045Child's age at first non-relative care 7.313 0.293 0.018 6.400 1.440 0.089Child ever received non-relative care on a regular basis (0/1) 0.383 -0.045 * -0.094 0.389 -0.043 * -0.090Child now in non-relative care (not center) (0/1) 0.117 0.019 0.056 0.118 0.026 * 0.077Number of non-relative care arrangements year before kindergarten 0.159 0.032 0.068 0.161 0.044 0.093Number of months of serious financial problems since child's birth 0.448 0.430 ** 0.236 0.425 0.475 ** 0.260Number of months received TANF/AFDC 0.023 0.382 ** 0.360 -0.008 0.376 ** 0.355Number of months received food stamps 0.055 0.634 ** 0.478 -0.022 0.689 ** 0.520Received WIC benefits for child (0/1) 0.119 0.301 ** 0.626 0.105 0.266 ** 0.553Received WIC benefits when pregnant (0/1) 0.103 0.264 ** 0.554 0.096 0.228 ** 0.479Family at or below poverty line (0/1) 0.028 0.132 ** 0.357 0.023 0.123 ** 0.332Number of months child lived in current home 49.247 -10.494 ** -0.425 49.247 -10.305 ** -0.418Child birthweight (in ounces) 119.788 -1.797 -0.085 120.557 -2.223 -0.105Average standardized difference 0.308 0.306

49

Page 52: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Primary type of center care ** ** day care 0.107 0.000 0.099 0.004 pre-school 0.499 -0.238 0.508 -0.213 pre-kindergarten 0.184 -0.026 0.187 -0.037 Father's education ** ** less than high school 0.034 0.070 0.036 0.049 high school diploma or vo/tech 0.254 0.075 0.260 0.077 at least some college 0.615 -0.257 0.630 -0.263 Mother's education ** ** less than high school 0.008 0.115 -0.004 0.146 high school diploma 0.177 0.150 0.168 0.134 vo/tech 0.047 0.000 0.048 -0.005 some college 0.329 -0.044 0.336 -0.067 bachelor's degree or higher 0.427 -0.224 0.438 -0.208 Parent type ** ** biological mother & other father 0.056 0.030 0.056 0.040 biological mother only 0.098 0.136 0.077 0.154 biological father, but not biological mother 0.014 0.012 0.018 0.008 other 0.021 0.016 0.021 0.003 Race ** ** Black, not Hispanic 0.058 0.125 0.039 0.093 Hispanic 0.108 0.037 0.146 0.023 Asian 0.024 0.002 0.026 0.003 Other, not Hispanic 0.052 -0.016 0.048 -0.010

50

Page 53: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

Appendix Table A2 National Matched Sample Within-Market Matched Sample

Catholic Public-Catholic

p-value

Standardized difference Catholic

Public-Catholic

p-value

Standardized difference

Variable description

Propensity score 0.223 0.000 0.000 0.211 0.006 0.059Wave-1 math t-score 54.515 0.109 0.012 54.492 -0.059 -0.006Wave-1 reading t-score 54.480 -0.631 -0.066 54.373 -1.183 -0.124 Date of birth (measured in days) 80.281 -10.851 -0.090 78.613 -11.400 -0.094Father's occupational score 39.820 0.042 0.002 39.809 -0.418 -0.020Family income (in $10,000) 7.066 0.151 0.034 6.849 0.556 0.127Entered kindergarten late (0/1) 0.070 0.005 0.020 0.073 0.023 0.096Mother's occupational score 33.919 -0.548 -0.025 33.890 1.474 0.067Number of places child has lived for at least 4 months 1.756 -0.031 -0.023 1.744 -0.114 -0.085Mother's age at child's birth (in years) 31.570 0.214 0.010 33.849 -4.568 * -0.207Child ever attended center care (0/1) 0.870 -0.037 -0.080 0.879 -0.034 -0.074Child ever attended pre-k care (0/1) 0.804 -0.018 -0.038 0.821 -0.030 -0.061Child in center care now (0/1) 0.258 -0.027 -0.068 0.247 0.055 0.137Ever receive food stamps (0/1) 0.048 -0.008 -0.024 0.051 -0.042 ** -0.121Father's current age (in years) 36.777 0.318 0.030 36.643 0.355 0.033Mother's age at first child birth (in years) 25.829 -0.004 -0.001 25.573 0.661 0.112Mother's current age (in years) 34.751 0.196 0.029 34.636 0.164 0.024Child was in non-relative care on a regular basis the year before entering kindergarten (0/1) 0.139 0.049 * 0.136 0.153 0.029 0.079Child's age at first non-relative care 7.330 -1.056 -0.065 8.062 -2.582 -0.159Child ever received non-relative care on a regular basis (0/1) 0.408 -0.017 -0.035 0.433 -0.028 -0.059Child now in non-relative care (not center) (0/1) 0.109 0.018 0.054 0.126 0.002 0.007Number of non-relative care arrangements year before kindergarten 0.159 0.074 ** 0.157 0.176 0.046 0.096Number of months of serious financial problems since child's birth 0.449 0.023 0.012 0.496 -0.157 -0.086Number of months received TANF/AFDC 0.023 0.131 0.124 0.015 0.030 0.028Number of months received food stamps 0.055 0.129 0.097 0.035 0.120 * 0.091Received WIC benefits for child (0/1) 0.141 -0.009 -0.018 0.133 -0.022 -0.045Received WIC benefits when pregnant (0/1) 0.117 -0.013 -0.028 0.112 -0.020 -0.043Family at or below poverty line (0/1) 0.035 -0.008 -0.022 0.037 -0.021 -0.056Number of months child lived in current home 49.222 -0.170 -0.007 49.310 2.494 0.101Child birthweight (in ounces) 119.757 -0.105 -0.005 119.672 -0.157 -0.007Average standardized difference 0.045 0.077

51

Page 54: The effect of Catholic schooling on math and reading ......Joseph P. Robinson . Stanford University. March, 2008 . Direct correspondence to Sean F. Reardon, 520 Galvez Mall, #526,

52

Primary type of center care day care 0.105 -0.023 0.095 0.055 pre-school 0.498 0.008 0.542 -0.063 pre-kindergarten 0.185 -0.013 0.165 -0.019 Father's education less than high school 0.035 -0.014 0.021 -0.009 high school diploma or vo/tech 0.252 0.004 0.287 0.031 at least some college 0.619 0.027 0.601 -0.022 Mother's education less than high school 0.008 0.001 0.007 -0.003 high school diploma 0.175 0.022 0.166 0.102 vo/tech 0.049 -0.009 0.057 0.001 some college 0.331 -0.078 0.355 -0.109 bachelor's degree or higher 0.424 0.073 0.399 0.020 Parent type biological mother & other father 0.059 -0.022 0.051 -0.027 biological mother only 0.096 -0.017 0.093 -0.003 biological father, but not biological mother 0.015 -0.010 0.019 -0.011 other 0.022 -0.008 0.024 -0.014 Race Black, not Hispanic 0.058 -0.028 0.048 0.004 Hispanic 0.107 0.049 0.106 0.001 Asian 0.024 0.030 0.028 0.037 Other, not Hispanic 0.051 -0.014 0.046 -0.014