the biased mirror: a functional approach to intergroup motor … · the biased mirror: a functional...

119
The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. by Jennifer Nadine Gutsell A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Psychology Psychology University of Toronto © Copyright by Jennifer Gutsell 2012

Upload: others

Post on 09-Jul-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance.

by

Jennifer Nadine Gutsell

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Psychology

Psychology University of Toronto

© Copyright by Jennifer Gutsell 2012

Page 2: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

ii

The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance.

Jennifer Gutsell

Doctor of Philosophy

Psychology

University of Toronto

2012

Abstract

Similar neural circuits are activated during action and during the observation of action, and such

motor resonance is said to support action understanding and could potentially facilitate affect

sharing, communication, cooperation and interpersonal coordination. Here I describe a line of

research that uses suppression of electroencephalographic mu-oscillations as an indicator of

motor resonance to look at motor resonance in an intergroup context. In study 1 I find that group

categorization and intergroup biases can restrict motor resonance to the ethnic ingroup,

potentially making it unavailable during cross-group interactions. In study 2 I investigate a

reduction in affect sharing as a potential consequence of group biases in motor resonance. Using

EEG prefrontal asymmetry patterns as an indicator of negative affect and avoidance tendencies, I

show that participants do not share the sadness of outgroup members. Finally, I present two

studies that test the boundary conditions of the ingroup bias in motor resonance. Study 3 shows

that when motivational relevance of the outgroup is increased, because participants are in an

inclusive, perspective taking mindset, group biases in motor resonance disappear. Similarly,

study 4 shows that when the situation is highly motivationally relevant, such as when the

behavior poses a potential threat; people will show motor resonance in response to anyone

independent of group membership.

Page 3: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

iii

Acknowledgments

The past four years as a graduate student and working on this dissertation were truly an

invaluable experience for me that I will always treasure. I’d like to express my deepest gratitude

to the people who have guided and accompanied me on this journey.

First, I’d like to thank Michael Inzlicht. I am so very grateful for his constant positivity,

support and advice. His guidance, encouragement and criticism instilled in me the knowledge

and confidence that I needed to become the researcher I am today. At the same time, he gave me

the freedom and trust I needed to grow, to discover my own interests, and to ask the questions I

needed to answer. I have been incredibly fortunate to have an advisor who inspired me to strive

to be a better researcher and person. I hope that one day I will have as much of a profound

influence on the lives of my own students. These are some pretty big footprints to follow.

I also would like to thank Elizabeth Page-Gould, because she has brought me joy and

encouragement even when times were difficult, and she has been a bottomless well of knowledge

and inspiration. Moreover, I thank Jason Plaks, for his support and for his thought-provoking

questions that sparked new perspectives and ideas.

Also, invaluable for the success of this thesis were my lab mates Alexa Tullett, Shona

Tritt, Rimma Teper, Sonia Kang, and Jacob Hirsh. In the lab and during lab meetings, they

provided support, company and fueled my interest and motivation for my research, and outside

of the lab they offered their friendship and reminded me that there is more to life than the

dissertation.

I’ll be forever grateful for the opportunities, support and training I received from the SPA

community. The faculty and graduate students that constitute this incredible group provided a

Page 4: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

iv

true sense of home and family in the department. Special thanks go to my cohort Stephanie

Spielmann, Jessica Remedios, Judith Gere and Jeff Yen for setting the bar so high, and for

laughing with me about the struggles we shared.

I’d also like to thank my friends and colleagues Ruth Ditlmann and Teresa Tavassoli.

Together we took on the challenge to pursue graduate education in different countries far away

from home and without them it wouldn’t have been possible.

I’m also grateful for the support and friendship of Gertraud Stadler. Working with her

solidified my interest in becoming an academic, and her enthusiasm and personality continue to

inspire me.

I can’t express my gratitude to Peter Aulich, for his love, his unwavering belief in me,

and for all the joy and happy moments we shared. He has been my solid foundation, always

being there, cheering me up and giving me the strength I needed.

Finally I’d like to thank my mother, Barbara Gutsell for being such a strong, caring, and

inspiring woman. Her love and encouragement has given me the courage to follow my dreams.

Page 5: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

v

Table of Contents

Acknowledgments .......................................................................................................................... iii

Table of Contents ............................................................................................................................ v

List of Tables ............................................................................................................................... viii

List of Figures ................................................................................................................................ ix

1 General Introduction .................................................................................................................. 1

1.1 Biases in Social Perception ................................................................................................. 2

1.2 The Functional Approach to Person Perception ................................................................. 4

1.2.1 The human brain as a cognitive miser. ................................................................... 4

1.2.2 Social categorization. .............................................................................................. 6

1.2.3 Cognitive disregard for social outgroups. ............................................................... 7

1.3 Motor Resonance ................................................................................................................ 9

1.3.1 Motor resonance and action understanding. ......................................................... 11

1.4 Mu-suppression as a Measure of Motor Resonance ......................................................... 17

1.5 Overview ........................................................................................................................... 18

2 Group biases in motor resonance ............................................................................................. 20

2.1 Bounded Interpersonal Sensitivity .................................................................................... 20

2.2 Overview ........................................................................................................................... 21

2.3 Methods ............................................................................................................................. 22

2.3.1 Subjects and Design .............................................................................................. 22

2.3.2 Procedure .............................................................................................................. 22

2.3.3 Material ................................................................................................................. 23

2.3.4 Electrophysiological Recording and Processing ................................................... 25

2.4 Results ............................................................................................................................... 26

2.5 Discussion ......................................................................................................................... 30

Page 6: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

vi

3 Group biases in the sharing of emotive states .......................................................................... 33

3.1 The Nature of Empathy ..................................................................................................... 33

3.2 Empathy and its Limits ..................................................................................................... 35

3.3 Overview ........................................................................................................................... 37

3.4 Method .............................................................................................................................. 39

3.4.1 Participants ............................................................................................................ 39

3.4.2 Procedure .............................................................................................................. 39

3.4.3 Materials ............................................................................................................... 40

3.4.4 EEG Data Acquisition and Processing ................................................................. 43

3.5 Results ............................................................................................................................... 44

3.6 Discussion ......................................................................................................................... 48

4 Perspective taking reduces group biases in motor resonance .................................................. 51

4.1 Limiting and Facilitating Factors for Motor Resonance ................................................... 52

4.2 Cognitive Perspective Taking ........................................................................................... 53

4.3 Overview ........................................................................................................................... 54

4.4 Methods ............................................................................................................................. 55

4.4.1 Participants ............................................................................................................ 55

4.4.2 Procedure .............................................................................................................. 55

4.4.3 Materials ............................................................................................................... 56

4.4.4 Electrophysiological Recording and Processing ................................................... 57

4.5 Results ............................................................................................................................... 57

4.6 Discussion ......................................................................................................................... 60

4.6.1 The Objective Mindset .......................................................................................... 61

4.6.2 Perspective Taking ................................................................................................ 62

4.7 Conclusions ....................................................................................................................... 63

5 Threatening outgroup behavior is resonated with ................................................................... 65

Page 7: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

vii

5.1 Motor resonance – a Prosocial Process? ........................................................................... 66

5.2 A bias towards threat and negativity ................................................................................. 67

5.3 Overview ........................................................................................................................... 69

5.4 Method .............................................................................................................................. 70

5.4.1 Participants ............................................................................................................ 70

5.4.2 Procedure .............................................................................................................. 70

5.4.3 Materials ............................................................................................................... 71

5.4.4 Electrophysiological recording and processing .................................................... 72

5.5 Results ............................................................................................................................... 72

5.6 Discussion ......................................................................................................................... 76

5.6.1 Threatening Outgroup Behavior and Stereotypes ................................................. 78

5.6.2 Limitations and Future Directions ........................................................................ 79

5.7 Conclusions ....................................................................................................................... 79

6 General Discussion and Conclusions ....................................................................................... 81

6.1 Implications of a lack in motor resonance: ....................................................................... 82

6.2 Malleable motor resonance ............................................................................................... 86

6.3 Limitations and future directions ...................................................................................... 87

6.4 Conclusions ....................................................................................................................... 90

References ..................................................................................................................................... 92

Page 8: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

viii

List of Tables

Table 1: Correlations among asymmetry scores and individual difference measurses ................ 47

Page 9: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

ix

List of Figures

Figure 1: Outline of experiment design of study 1 ....................................................................... 24

Figure 2: EEG mu suppression during self action and the observation of ingroup and outgroup

members. ....................................................................................................................................... 27

Figure 3: Scalp topography of the correlations between mu suppression for outgroups and

prejudice.. ...................................................................................................................................... 30

Figure 4: Outline of experimental design of study 2 .................................................................... 41

Figure 5: Alpha asymmetry during the experience and observation of sadness as a function of

group membership. ....................................................................................................................... 45

Figure 6: Symbolic racism scores as a function of prefrontal alpha asymmetry scores in the

outgroup condition. ....................................................................................................................... 47

Figure 7: EEG mu-suppression during the observation of ingroup and outgroup members for

participants in a perspective taking mindset and for participants in an objective mindset. .......... 59

Figure 8: EEG mu-suppression during the observation of ingroup and outgroup members

performing a neutral, positive and threatening action.. ................................................................ 74

Page 10: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

1

1 General Introduction

“The opposite of love is not hate, it is indifference.” ~ Elie Wiesel

Humans have an incredible capacity to empathize with others, to understand others’ actions and

intentions, to anticipate their next move, their needs, and to use this information to coordinate

their own behavior with the behavior of others. When it comes to our loved ones, we use the full

extent of our empathic abilities. We automatically catch their moods, emotions, and the meaning

and intentions of their actions, which provide us with an intuitive understanding of their inner

states. Thereby, we help them when they are in need, we bond with them, and we can act in

unison and close cooperation. With those we hate it is different. We still have a need to

understand their emotions, actions and intentions, but for more selfish reasons. Here we use our

empathic abilities to anticipate our enemies’ next move and to predict their intentions in order to

be prepared. Hence, despite our selfish reasons, we still are receptive to the inner states of our

enemies. But, what about those towards whom we feel nothing but indifference? Could it be that

we do not intuitively catch their intentions, needs and emotions?

Throughout human evolution and historically, people did not have much contact with

others outside of their tight social ingroups, and for most people this is still true today (Fiske,

1992). Outgroup members are not the ones we have our most important and meaningful social

exchanges with — in a way, most of the time, we can afford to ignore them and to meet them

with indifference. Group categorization and biases, thus, could potentially limit the extent to

which people are receptive to other’s inner states.

Page 11: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

2

Findings from neuroscience suggest that, at least in part, people understand others’

actions, intentions and emotions through neural resonance: the mere perception of others’ actions

(Oberman et al., 2005), facial expressions (Carr, Iacoboni, Dubeau, Mazziotta, & Lenzi, 2003),

and pain (Singer et al., 2006) is matched with neural activity similar to when people perform the

action, express their emotions, or feel pain themselves. Neural resonance is associated with better

performances in tasks that require emotional empathy (Pineda & Hecht, 2009) and potentially

facilitates an intuitive understanding of the other’s actions and emotions (Iacoboni, 2009). In

this dissertation I will focus on motor resonance a basic form of neural resonance in the sensory

motor system. One way to investigate motor resonance is to use electroencephalography (EEG)

to measure neural activity in sensory motor areas during the observation of actions. Here, I

present a series of 4 studies that use this method to investigate potential group biases in motor

resonance. I propose that, as a default, people do not resonate with ethnic outgroup members; but

once outgroup members’ behavior becomes motivationally relevant, either because group

boundaries are blurred or because the outgroup member poses a threat, people become sensitive

to outgroup members’ inner states. In the following sections I will develop the rationale for this

hypothesis.

1.1 Biases in Social Perception

Research in social cognition and social neuroscience has revealed group biases when it

comes to processing characteristics and the inner states of others. For example, when research

participants believe that a target is an ingroup member, they are better at recognizing the target’s

face than when they believe the same target is an outgroup member (the “cross-race effect”;

Bernstein, Young, & Hugenberg, 2007; MacLin, Otto & Malpass, 2001). These effects could

potentially be explained by the fact that ingroup faces are processed more holistically (their

Page 12: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

3

features are more strongly integrated global representations) than outgroup faces (Hugenberg &

Corneille, 2009; Michel, Rossion, Han, Chung, & Caldara, 2006). Not only do people have

trouble telling outgroup members apart, they also have difficulty interpreting their facial

expressions (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002a). For example, when Adams and colleagues (2006)

examined the neural correlates of emotion recognition, they found that both Japanese and White

American participants perform better when judging an ingroup member’s emotional state than

when judging an outgroup member’s emotional state, and this ingroup advantage was also

reflected in enhanced activity in brain areas involved in emotion decoding. One consequence of

these biases in interpreting emotional expressions seems to be that people react less strongly to

emotions such as happiness and fear when displayed by outgroup members (Weisbuch &

Ambady, 2008). Specifically, pictures of positive and negative facial expressions displayed by

outgroup members do not elicit as strong of an affective prime for the evaluation of subsequently

presented positive and negative pictures than do pictures of facial expressions displayed by

ingroup members. Thus, both the perception of outgroup members’ facial features and their

emotional expressions seem to be impaired.

Individuated social perception is not entirely reserved for the social ingroup, however.

When an outgroup becomes motivationally relevant, people will be as responsive to outgroup

members as they are to ingroup members. For example, Ackerman and colleagues (2006)

showed that, although people have problems differentiating outgroup faces, when the faces are

angry, people are even better at recognizing outgroup faces than ingroup faces. Similarly, people

normally classify multiracial individuals as outgroup members and consequently show decreased

performance in recognizing them. When people, however, are motivated to include multiracial

individuals in the ingroup they remember them as well as non-ambiguous ingroup members

Page 13: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

4

(Pauker et al., 2009). Taken together, these findings suggest that people are relatively insensitive

to social outgroup members, unless they become motivationally relevant.

Recent findings in the neurosciences are changing our view of person perception. There

is growing evidence that person perception involves more than passively collecting visual

information and then interpreting it. The mere perception of another’s expressions, mannerisms,

and actions inherently brings with it the opportunity for an intuitive understanding of the other

that involves not only our visual system, but our motor systems as well. That is, we resonate with

the other’s experiences using our own body to gain an active understanding of their actions,

intentions and emotions. My goal here is to investigate whether this motor resonance based

social perception is also limited by our social categories and group boundaries. I will take a

functional approach to motor resonance and explore whether cross-group motor resonance is

influenced by motivational relevance of the target.

1.2 The Functional Approach to Person Perception

1.2.1 The human brain as a cognitive miser.

The human environment is highly complex, multifaceted, and rich. People are perhaps

the most complex stimuli we encounter and the sheer amount of people we interact with and the

diversity of social situations in which these interactions take place, could potentially be

overwhelming for a brain with limited cognitive capacity. In order to adapt to the complexity of

our world, social and physical, the human mind has developed “tools” to save cognitive

resources whenever possible (Macrae, Milne, & Bodenhausen, 1994). Therefore, at each level of

cognitive processing, we are always engaged in a selection of what is worth noticing and what is

not (VanRullen & Koch, 2003). This selective attention is the process that allows people to focus

Page 14: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

5

on just a subset of all the possible sensory inputs and thoughts (Luck & Vecera, 2002). Where

we put the focus of our attention is thus central to any subsequent thought processes; we can only

process information further if we actually have been paying attention to it in the first place.

Where we focus our attention, however, seems to be strongly influenced by whether the stimulus

is motivationally relevant or not (Moskowitz & Kirk, 2004), and this categorization between

relevant and irrelevant is made automatically and in a fraction of a second (Vogt, De Houwer, &

Moors, 2011). Therefore, selective attention to motivationally relevant social targets could

potentially influence whether people resonate with outgroup members or not.

Whether a stimulus is motivationally relevant can either originate from the stimulus

itself, such that some stimuli (e.g., food) are inherently or intrinsically motivationally relevant, or

from higher order constructs such as goals. Social stimuli are one kind of stimuli that particularly

draws people’s attention, and our brain seems to have developed in a way that makes humans

processing experts of various stimuli related to the human body or mind. For example, people

show a high degree of visual sensitivity to human movement: When research participants see

videos of point lights attached to the major joints of human models, they readily identify

different specific human movements (Jacobs, Pinto, & Shiffrar, 2004; Johansson, 1976).

Moreover, new born infants prefer looking at faces rather than at other, equally complex stimuli

(Johnson, Dziurawiec, Ellis, & Morton, 1991; Turati, Bulf, & Simion, 2008), faces capture

attention and distract people from a primary task (Lavie, Ro, & Russell, 2003) and there is a

specific area in the brain, the fusiform face area, for processing faces (Kanwisher, Mcdermott, &

Chun, 1997). Similarly, people selectively attend to the human body, whether depicted as a true

image, as mere silhouettes, or stick figures (Downing, Bray, Rogers, & Childs, 2004). Hence,

other human beings seem to be intrinsically salient and important for us and, therefore, capture

attention and receive priority in processing over other stimuli. However, not all humans are the

Page 15: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

6

same – people tend to categorize the different people they encounter into subgroups of people

and these subgroups might influence just how motivationally relevant another person is to an

observer.

1.2.2 Social categorization.

On its quest to save cognitive resources and processing capacity, the brain not only

selectively attends to certain stimuli, it also seeks to simplify the environment. Instead of

evaluating every situation, or every single person (Fiske & Taylor, 1984) independently, people

save cognitive resources by organizing them into category based knowledge structures (Neil

Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2001). This means that people do not perceive others entirely in terms

of their unique attributes and features. Instead they are strongly inclined to organize them into

social categories like race, gender, or age. These categories reduce cognitive effort, because

social targets can be evaluated based on their category without having to consider the complexity

of the individual. Once these categorical labels are attached to a person, all subsequent cognitive

processing is profoundly influenced by them. Consequently, categorical thinking biases what we

remember about the other person (Hamilton & Sherman, 1996), how we evaluate the other

person, and what impressions we derive of them, leading to stereotypes and prejudice (Fiske &

Neuberg 1990). Ultimately, and most relevant for the current purpose, social categorization

influences any target-related information that is encountered, even basic attention and perception

(Bodenhausen, 1988), potentially even motor resonance.

As noted, the principal function of categorization is to streamline social cognition and to

facilitate efficient social interactions by providing useful expectancies that can guide the

processing of subsequently encountered information (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). One

consequence of this streamlining is cognitive disregard. When people enter a social situation,

Page 16: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

7

they first quickly categorize any new social target into an existing or new social category. These

categories can then be used as selection criteria to separate those social targets that are worth

further exploring from those who can be disregarded (Rodin, 1987). Disregard, does not mean

that the disregarded person is necessarily disliked. Instead, ‘disregard’ criteria define those

people who are deemed unsuitable to our currently-active social purpose. For example, the same

older aged man could be disregarded by a young woman when her primary goal is to find a

partner, but would not be when her primary goal is to make business connections. Thus, our

motivations determine which social categories can be disregarded and which we should attend to.

This becomes problematic, however, when certain categories are inherently prone to disregard so

that people in these categories are treated with indifference in most situations – they no longer

are intrinsically motivationally relevant. One such category seems to be the category “social

outgroup member”.

1.2.3 Cognitive disregard for social outgroups.

According to social identity theory (Tajfel, 1982; Turner, 1975), individuals are generally

motivated to achieve a positive self-esteem and because personal self-esteem is tightly related to

one’s group identity, self-esteem can be enhanced by a positive evaluation of one’s own social

group. One’s own group, however, can only be evaluated in comparison with an outgroup. Thus,

people are strongly motivated to establish a positively discrepant comparison between the

ingroup and a relevant outgroup. In order to achieve this, they readily categorize their social

environment into “us” vs. “them” or ingroup vs. outgroup. The ingroup and outgroup status of

people, thus, exerts a powerful influence on social cognition (Allport, 1954).

In order to have a clear ingroup vs. outgoup distinction people tend to exaggerate the

differences between the ingroup and the outgroup (Tajfel, 1978), and importantly, they tend to

Page 17: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

8

see the members of an outgroup as more homogeneous and similar to one another than members

of the group perceive themselves (Park, Judd, & Box, 1990). Therefore, while people think that

ingroup members are very distinct and thus require careful consideration, outgroup members are

seen as all the same and thus it is deemed safe to save resources by assuming their traits, goals,

and intentions are stereotypical. People might be particularly susceptible to make these kinds of

assumptions, because they often mistakenly think that social categories are essence-based

groupings, in the sense that some underlying essence that all category members have in common

distinguishes them from members of other categories. That is, people conceive of many social

categories as if they were more like biological categories such as different kinds of species, and

not the social and cultural constructions they really are (Rothbart, & Taylor, 1992). Along with

such an essentialist view of social categories, then, comes the assumption that the category has

inductive potential (Haslam, Rothschild, & Ernst, 2000).

Taken together, people are strongly motivated to make the ingroup/ outgroup distinction

and to categorize new interaction partners into either the ingroup or the outgroup. Once others

are categorized into an outgroup, they are no longer considered as individuals, but simply as part

of a category. Consequently, when the outgroup as a whole is not considered motivationally

relevant, each individual will automatically be the target of cognitive disregard. Moreover, the

outgroup category itself is likely to be the target of cognitive disregard, because throughout

human evolutionary history and even nowadays the ingroup and not the outgroup was most

important for goal attainment.

From an evolutionary perspective, ingroup members are more beneficial for one’s

reproductive fitness. According to the concept of inclusive fitness (Hamilton, 1994), individuals

can promote their own evolutionary success by helping others who share some of their genes

Page 18: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

9

(Sime, 1983). The degree of genetic overlap, however, is impossible to detect, and thus people

have to use proxies of genetic overlap such as the degree of kinship, similarity, familiarity, and

affiliation. Moreover, people need other people for survival. From the perspective of a hunter-

gatherer, being excluded from the group was life threatening. In a hunter-gatherer society,

survival alone was nearly impossible, and even today, lasting, meaningful relationships are

essential for well-being (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Ingroup members are usually the ones with

whom we most often interact; when we interact with outgroup members it is mostly at a group

level rather than in one-on-one intimate interactions (Fiske, 1992). Consequently, shared social

understanding and effective social interactions with other group members are essential

(Caporael, 1997). If one depends on another person, one needs to understand him or her as an

individual. It would, thus, make sense that humans are particularly attuned to ingroup members,

that we devote much cognitive effort into perceiving and understanding ingroup members and

their intentions, actions and motives. The other side of the coin would be that, since most of the

time we can afford to ignore the outgroup and to treat outgroup members with indifference; we

would be less sensitive to outgroup member’s inner states and less attuned with their actions. I

propose that even motor resonance, the most basic mechanism underlying the understanding of

other’s inner states, is reduced when it comes to processing outgroup members. In the following

sections, I will explain motor resonance and how it can be measured in more detail.

1.3 Motor Resonance

A now classic effect in social psychology is that perceiving someone else’s behavior

increases a tendency in the perceiver to behave similarly (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). This

happens because actions and the mere perception or imagination of the same action share

common neural representations (Preston & de Waal, 2002; Prinz, 1987). In the brain, this action-

Page 19: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

10

perception link is said to be implemented by the so called mirror neurons; a subset of sensory

motor neurons that are active during motor activity, but also when the perceiver remains

completely still and sees someone else engaging in motor activity (Carr et al., 2003; Rizzolatti &

Craighero, 2004). Mirror neurons were discovered using single cell recordings in monkeys, but

single cell recordings in humans are hard to get due to ethical considerations, and thus far only

one study has shown mirror neurons in humans using single cell recording (Mukamel, Ekstrom,

Kaplan, Iacoboni, & Fried, 2010). Because of the ethical considerations, mirror neuron activity

in humans is usually investigated using corresponding neural activation of sensory motor areas

within an observer in response to the mere perception of motor activity in the target as a proxy.

The precise location of the mirror neuron system in the human brain is still a matter of

debate. Traditionally, researchers restricted the mirror neuron system to sensory motor areas,

specifically to a network consisting of ventral and dorsal premortor cortex, anterior inferior

parietal lobule and adjacent somatosensory area BA2, and the middle temporal gyrus (Keysers,

Kaas, & Gazzola, 2010). These brain areas are normally associated with planning, preparation,

execution, and proprioception of our own actions (Schippers & Keysers, 2011). However,

indirect measures of neural activity and single cell recording support the existence of neurons

with mirroring properties also in other motor regions, and also in non motor regions. For

example, Mukamel and colleagues (2010) recorded activity from 1177 cells in the human medial

frontal and temporal cortices while patients executed or observed hand grasping actions and

facial emotional expressions. They found a significant proportion of neurons with mirroring

capacity in the supplementary motor area, the parahippocampal gyrus, and the endorhinal cortex

– areas usually involved in movement initiation and sequencing and memory, and not

traditionally categorized as part of the mirror neuron system. Nonetheless, these areas might

contribute to the vicarious experience of others’ action and emotional states.

Page 20: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

11

Because for single cell studies in humans, researchers are limited to certain brain areas

based on clinical need, most areas of the brain have never been directly investigated for the

existence of neurons with mirroring capacity. Hence, future research might discover that the

mirror neuron system is a much wider network than previously thought of, and might not at all

be limited to the brain’s motor regions. Research using functional magnetic resonance imaging

(fMRI) gives some indication on to which brain functions might involve mirror neurons. Thus

far, shared neural networks have been inferred for the perception and experience of disgust

(Wicker et al., 2003), pain (Singer et al., 2004), touch (Keysers & Perrett, 2004), and facial

expressions (Carr et al., 2003). Neural resonance, thus, might be essential for a number of forms

of interpersonal sensitivity, including emotional contagion and empathy (Singer et al., 2006). For

the current purpose, however, I will focus on the traditional mirror neuron system that has been

extensively researched in monkeys and more recently in humans using motor resonance as a

proxy of mirror neuron activity. Although still under some debate (Gallese, Gernsbacher, Heyes,

Hickok, & Iacoboni, 2011) the motor-mirror neuron system is thought to be involved in the

understanding of others actions and intentions (e.g. Iacoboni, 2005; Oberman, Pineda, &

Ramachandran, 2007).

1.3.1 Motor resonance and action understanding.

Mirror neurons were discovered only relatively recently (Umilta et al., 2001), and

consequently our knowledge about them and their specific functions is still limited. Although it

is now widely accepted that mirror neurons exist in the macaque monkey and also in the human

brain, the field is still divided when it comes to the specific function of these neurons (Glenberg,

2011). From the very start mirror neurons have been linked to many different social phenomena

including action understanding, language comprehension, imitation, and empathy (Decety,

Page 21: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

12

2010; Gallese et al., 2011; Hickok, 2009), but action understanding is often seen as the core

function of mirror neurons (Hickok, 2009). Moreover action understanding is probably the

function most addressed, both by empirical research and in theoretical discussions (Hickok,

2009), and the field seems to have reached at least some consensus on whether mirror neurons

are involved in action understanding (Glenberg, 2011). For these reasons, I will limit discussion

on the function of mirror neurons to action understanding.

Summarizing a discussion between proponents and opponents of the mirror neuron

theory of action understanding (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), Celcilia Heyes commented: “So

what is the dispute really about? My guess is that it concerns, not what causes action

understanding, but what constitutes action understanding,’’(Gallese et al., 2011). Heyes stresses

the importance of a clear definition of motor resonance, and alludes to the fact that much of the

debate is caused by a lack of agreement on what action understanding actually is. Many mirror

neuron researchers use a very restricted definition of action understanding. For example, Gallese

and colleagues (1996) define action understanding as:” The capacity to recognize that an

individual is performing and action, to differentiate this action from others analogous to it, and to

use this information in order to act appropriately” Similarly, Rizzolatti (2001) defines action

understanding as follows: “The capacity to achieve the internal description of an action and to

use it to organize appropriate future behavior”. Such a basic form of action understanding is

stripped of most higher order cognitive processes, such as inferences about intention or the

capacity to name or to make logical inferences about the action. Instead, action understanding,

defined in this basic sense, is more of a hunch and intuitive sense of the semantics of the action,

i.e. what the action is about, what its goal is, and how it is related to other actions, than true

understanding in the usual sense. Different perspectives on what action understanding is have the

potential to cause conflict. For example, a common objection to the mirror neuron theory of

Page 22: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

13

action understanding is that we can understand actions that we cannot perform (Hickok, 2009). If

mirror neurons were indeed involved in action understanding, why is action understanding not

impaired by a lack of previous experience with the action; obviously someone who never played

tennis can still understand that the player is hitting the ball. However, this objection reflects the

belief that action understanding is a much more complex and cognitive process than assumed by

many supporters of the mirror neuron theory of action understanding. The more basic form of

action understanding comes from personal experience and this more personal understanding

would be missing when people observe actions they never performed themselves. The

inexperienced observer might be able to grasp that the player is hitting the ball, but he will not be

able to feel what it is like to hit the ball, and thus will have a harder time to predict the direction,

speed, and placement of the ball (Gallese et al., 2011). Such an intuitive understanding based on

experience could be independent from other, higher order and more cognitive forms of

understanding (Gallese et al., 2011).

There is empirical evidence for the involvement of motor resonance in a basic

understanding of actions and intentions. For example, research shows that motor resonance goes

beyond the mere mechanics of a motion, but seems to map the function and goal of the action;

Mirror neurons fire, even when the “observer” can only partially (Umilta et al., 2001), or not at

all (Kohler et al., 2002) see the action. Moreover, motor resonance is more pronounced for goal

directed action (Cattaneo, Caruana, Jezzini, & Rizzolatti, 2009) and specific groups of mirror

neurons respond to specific and unique goals of actions (Gallese et al., 2011), such as grasping

to put something into a container vs. grasping to bring something to one’s mouth (Bonini et al.,

2010). Another indication that mirror neurons code more than the visual features of the action is

that actions, absent from the person’s motor repertoire, are not mirrored. Research participants in

an fMRI experiment showed activation in equivalent motor regions during the observation of

Page 23: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

14

biting and lip smacking, but not of barking – a behavior that is not in the human motor repertoire

(Buccino et al., 2004). Finally, mirror neurons fire in response to sounds of actions, suggesting

that they code the meaning and not only the visual features of the action (Kohler et al., 2002).

In addition Keyser and Perrett’s hebbian model of mirror neurons (Keysers & Perrett,

2004) provides an explanation for how such an intuitive understanding of the action semantics,

could be acquired through motor resonance. According to this model, mirror neurons acquire

their mirroring properties through Hebbian learning (Keysers & Perrett, 2004). That is, as infants

move their own limbs they observe themselves. Throughout the repeated coupling of action

execution and the visual aspect of the action, neurons that fire in response to the observation and

motor neurons that fire during execution form Hebbian associations. Soon these associations

become so strong that previously motor neurons will also respond during observation, making

them visiomotor neurons. Similarly mirroring of facial expressions could have evolved though

the recurrent pairing of the child expressing emotions and the imitation of the child’s facial

expressions by the parent. Once the connections have been established the information flow can

go forward and back – visual information can affect motor areas and motor activity can affect

perception. This bidirectional information flow could play a key role in the transformation from

observing an action to simulating this action internally and predicting its sensory consequences

(Schippers & Keysers, 2011). For our own actions, this mechanism could be essential for action

control: During execution, the system sends an efferent copy of the action to sensory areas to

predict sensory and proprioceptive consequences of that action. Then, if the actual sensory

information and the predicted sensory consequences differ, the action can be adjusted. When we

observe others, this system could achieve an interpretation of the observed action by first

simulating the action in the premotor cortex and then predicting the sensory consequences of the

internally simulated action (Schippers & Keysers, 2011). The critical point here is that the

Page 24: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

15

simulation of the observed action not only allow us to predict others’ actions and action

consequences, it could potentially also bring about knowledge of the internal state of the other

that is associated with the action.

We only have a limited understanding of the nature of these vicarious experiences, but

there are some clues from studies using electrical brain stimulation in awake surgical patients

(Keysers et al., 2010). For example, stimulating the premotor cortex elicits overt movement, but

the patients do not realize that they actually move (Desmurget et al., 2009). Stimulating the

inferior parietal lobule creates a strong intention to move, but the patient does not actually move

(Desmurget et al., 2009). Finally, stimulation of the somatosensory cortex elicit the sensation of

movement and other somatosensory qualia (Desmurget et al., 2009). Theoretically, when we

observe others’ actions, the different areas of the sensory motor system could each contribute a

different aspect to the understanding of the action, such as sensing the intentions of others,

experiencing what it might feel to move, predicting their next move and also preparing our own

motor response (Keysers et al., 2010).

The notion that action understanding could be the result of joint activity in different areas

within the sensory motor system including areas that contain mirror neurons and those that do

not, is interesting. That’s because the existence of non-mirror regions that are involved in action

understanding has been perceived as a threat to the notion that mirror neurons themselves code

the semantic properties of the action (Hickok, 2009). The reason is that such non-mirror regions

could potentially process the meaning of the action and, at the same time, activate motor

neurons, creating the impression of mirroring. For example, neurons in the superior temporal

sulcus (STS) seem to code much more complex aspect of actions then mirror neurons. Moreover,

while these cells do not have motor properties, they provide input into the motor regions that

Page 25: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

16

contain mirror neurons. Because of these connections it could look like mirror neurons code

action semantics, when in fact they would not (Hickok, 2009). Once we allow for non-mirroring

and mirroring regions to jointly contribute to action understanding, we can focus less on what

individual mirror neurons code for and instead regard the entire network. In addition, such a

divers system could also be involved in a variety of motor related functions. One such process

seems to be action preparation. Based on the theoretical arguments and empirical findings

discussed above, there are good reasons to believe that motor resonance is involved in a basic

form of action understanding, but there are also findings suggesting an involvement in action

preparation (Fadiga, Fogassi, Pavesi, & Rizzolatti, 1995; Tkach, Reimer, & Hatsopoulos, 2007).

In any case, both are not mutually exclusive processes, and they could, therefore, both be related

to motor resonance, each contributing to important social functions such as smooth interactions,

cooperation and action coordination.

In sum, even though other higher order cognitive processes are involved in action

understanding, motor resonance could add an implicit and deeper understanding that comes from

personal experience. Without this first person perspective, visual information alone would not

provide us with the information critical to understanding what it means to undertake the action,

how it relates to other actions, and to predict the others next move. If we would resonate less

with outgroup members, we could potentially have a harder time coordinating our actions with

theirs, predicting their actions, and understanding their intentions. In what follows, I will

describe a series of studies that investigate how group categories, ingroup biases, and

motivational relevance influence the amount of motor resonance in response to others. In most

studies reported here I used suppression of electroencephalographic mu oscillations during the

observation of actions as an index of motor resonance.

Page 26: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

17

1.4 Mu-suppression as a Measure of Motor Resonance

The mu-rhythm is a well established measure of neural activity in the sensory motor

cortex (Kuhlman, 1978; Pfurtscheller, 1979). The alpha component of the mu-rhythm (8-12 Hz)

seems to originate from primary somatosensory cortex (Hari, 2006) and shows a synchronized

activity at rest, which is desynchronized, both during movement and during passive observation

of movement, leading to a suppression of mu (Pineda, 2005). During execution of movement,

neurons in the premotor, motor, and sensorimotor cortices are activated (Pineda, 2005).

Therefore, mu-suppression while a person is moving is probably the result of motor neurons and

mirror neurons. In the absence of overt movement, theoretically motor neurons should no longer

be active, leaving only mirror neurons to contribute to mu-suppression (Pineda, 2005). Mu-

suppression during action observation should, therefore, be exclusively attributed to the

discharge of mirror neurons. Anatomically mirror neurons, located in the ventral premotor cortex

could affect neurons in the primary sensorimotor cortex through existing strong connections

between these two regions, resulting in mu-suppression (Pineda, 2005).

In sum, the phenomenology of the mu rhythm is very similar to the phenomenology of

mirror neuron activity in that both are sensitive to movement and the mere observation of

movement. Moreover, the anatomical configuration of the motor system and the mirror neuron

system specifically are consistent with the possibility of a downstream modulation of sensory

motor neurons by mirror neurons, which could be reflected in the the mu-rhythm originating in

sensory motor cortex. Mu-suppression, thus, seems to be a good indicator of motor resonance

and mirror neuron activity (Puzzo, Cooper, Cantarella, & Russo, 2011).

Page 27: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

18

1.5 Overview

In what follows, I will describe four studies that use EEG mu-suppression to investigate

motor resonance in an intergroup context. My strategy throughout was to use videos of ingroup

and outgroup members as target stimuli, and ethnicity as the defining factor for group

membership.

In chapter two I will describe a study that was aimed to test the general effect of an

ingroup bias in motor resonance. I presented participants with videos of ingroup and outgroup

members and measured participants’ levels of motor resonance during action observation.

Taking a functional approach to motor resonance, I expected that by default people would only

show motor resonance in response to seeing members of their ingroup performing an action, and

not in response to the actions of outgroup members. Moreover, I expected that such an ingroup

bias in motor resonance would be aggravated by prejudice and for disliked outgroups.

Once the first study established that there indeed is a group bias in motor resonance, the

study described in chapter three tested one potential consequence of such bias – reduced affect

sharing with outgroup members. I used asymmetries in the suppression of prefrontal alpha-

rhythms as an indicator for negative, avoidance related affect in response to displays of sadness.

I predicted that participants would show alpha asymmetry patterns related to negativity when

looking at sad ingroup members, but not when looking at sad outgroup members.

In chapters four and five I then describe two studies that aimed to increase motor

resonance for outgroup members. In the first of the two studies, participants took the perspective

of an outgroup member, or adopted and objective mindset before they saw ingroup and outgroup

members performing actions. I expected that perspective taking would alleviate the ingroup bias

Page 28: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

19

in motor resonance, leading to resonance for outgroup members. In the final study, I looked at

the effects of action intent on motor resonance for outgroup members. Participants watched

videos of ingroup and outgroup members performing neutral, positive, and neutral actions. I

expected only threatening outgroup behavior to elicit motor resonance in the observer, because

of its high level of motivational relevance.

Page 29: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

20

2 Group biases in motor resonance

People are sensitive to others who fall within a closed circle defined by their social

relations; and members of outgroups are excluded from this circle (Turner et al., 1979). When

people connect with others, they resonate with them by adopting their postures, intonations, and

facial expressions. I suggest, however, that motor resonance is linked to the ingroup, thus

making it comparatively difficult to respond to the feelings, intentions, and needs of those

categorized as members of an outgroup. Measuring the neural processes involved in action

understanding, this study directly investigates how the basic building blocks of interpersonal

sensitivity are affected by group membership and social biases. Specifically, I propose that

people are less likely to resonate with the simple actions of others when those others belong to a

disliked outgroup. I also propose that this reduced mental simulation will be exacerbated for

those who are high in prejudice.

2.1 Bounded Interpersonal Sensitivity

Neural resonance in a broader sense seems to be subject to social factors. When

observing others in pain, people show activity in brain areas associated with the experience of

pain only for others they like and not for those they dislike (Singer et al., 2006). Similarly,

participants show diminished neural activation of pain circuits when observing ethnic outgroup

members in pain relative to ingroup members (Xu, Zuo, Wang, & Han, 2009).

Behavioral research also supports the idea of motor resonance being bound to the

ingroup. Although people mimic others’ expressions, gestures, and body postures, this occurs

with less frequency for outgroup members (Likowski, Mühlberger, Seibt, Pauli, & Weyers,

Page 30: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

21

2008). These findings suggest that the link between perception and action is weaker when it

comes to outgroups, but falls short of showing how exactly this occurs. I aim to fill this gap by

taking a direct look at motor resonance online, while people passively observe the actions of

ingroup and outgroup members. By measuring neural activity during observation and action, I

aim to measure potential group biases in motor resonance.

Research on prejudice has shown that our attitudes bias how we perceive and process

others’ behaviour. Recall that we have a harder time to recognize outgroup members’ faces

(Sporer, 2001) and to interpret their facial expressions (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002b); outgroups

are also less likely to activate neural areas for social cognition (Harris & Fiske, 2006) and social

perception (Van Bavel, Packer, & Cunningham, 2008). Given these biases in social perception

and processing it follows that we would be less sensitive to the actions, intentions, and thoughts

of outgroup members—something that should be reflected in the neural networks that implement

motor resonance. It also follows that the more we dislike outgroups—the more prejudiced we

are—the less we will show neural evidence of simulation.

2.2 Overview

We used EEG recordings to compare brain activity during the experience and observation

of actions, and used suppression of EEG oscillations in the 8-13 Hz mu frequency at scalp

locations over primary motor cortex as my main dependent measure. I predicted that participants

would show less mu suppression in response to outgroup members than to ingroup members and

that this difference in vicarious mu suppression would be stronger with increasing prejudice.

Sociological research (Kalin & Berry, 1996) indicates that Canadians of South Asian descent

belong to the least valued ethnic groups in Canada. Thus, while I expected less motor resonance

Page 31: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

22

for the average of all outgroups, I predicted the least motor resonance when observing South

Asians, followed by Blacks, and then by East Asians, who are generally not disliked in a

Canadian context.

2.3 Methods

2.3.1 Subjects and Design

We originally sampled 37 White participants, but excluded seven because some were left

handed (n = 3), self identified as non-White (n = 2), because of system malfunction (n = 1), or

incomplete data (n = 1). My final sample thus included 30 White, right-handed University of

Toronto Scarborough students (13 female; Mage= 18.46, SD= 3.81), who participated for course

credit. Participants completed three conditions in a one-way within subject design: ingroup,

outgroup and self condition. I defined ingroup as people who share the participant’s ethnic

identity (Caucasian), and outgroup as people who did not (African-Canadian, East-Asian, South-

Asian).

2.3.2 Procedure

Upon arrival in the lab, participants provided informed consent and were then fitted with

and electrode cap for EEG recording. I recorded 120 seconds of baseline EEG activity over

motor areas while participants were sitting still looking at a white screen. EEG was then recorded

while participants passively watched a set of videos. The videos showed a variety of ingroup and

outgroup members performing a simple action. After watching the videos, participants were

asked to perform the action themselves.

Page 32: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

23

2.3.3 Material

2.3.3.1 Symbolic racism scale.

During a mass testing session in an introductory psychology course, participants

completed the Symbolic Racism Scale (Henry & Sears, 2002) — a measure of modern racism,

indicative of a subtle form of racism that obscures racist feelings with abstract values, such as

justice and order.

2.3.3.2 Video-set manipulation.

Participants saw videos depicting members of different ethnic groups (Caucasian, East-

Asian, South-Asian, and African-Canadian) performing an action. The videos showed university-

aged male actors sitting at a table in front of a white wall with a glass of water in front of them.

Displaying a neutral face, the actors repeatedly reached for the glass, picked it up, took a small

sip of water, and then put the glass back in its place. In the ingroup condition, participants saw

three different Caucasian actors, each performing the action for 10 seconds. In the outgroup

condition, the participants saw three different actors of each ethnic outgroup perform the action

for 10 seconds.

To control for other differences in the videos, I presented screenshots of each of our

videos to a different sample of 33 introductory psychology students (25 female) and asked them

about their impressions. Specifically, I asked participants to rate the screen-shots in terms of

video quality (ingroup: M = 2.52, SD =.61; outgroup: M = 2.49, SD = 0.54), attractiveness

(ingroup: M = 1.68, SD = .57; outgroup: M = 1.70, SD = 0.65), likability (ingroup: M = 2.11, SD

= .68; outgroup: M = 2.20, SD = .68), and racial group typicality (ingroup: M = 2.52, SD = .61;

Page 33: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

24

outgroup: M = 2.49, SD = .54) on a 5 point Likert scale. Results revealed no significant

differences across any of these variables, all ps > .21, ns.

Figure 1 shows a graphic depiction of the experimental design. In total, I obtained 30

seconds of EEG data during the ingroup condition and 90 seconds during the outgroup condition

with 30 second blocks for each of the three different ethnic outgroups. The blocks of the four

different ethnic groups where presented in random order and the order of individual actors within

each block was also randomized. Thus each ethnic group and each actor within each ethnic group

was presented in a random order. Near the end of the session, participants performed the same

action themselves for 30 seconds.

Figure 1: Outline of experiment design: first participants completed a 120 s baseline

measure. Following, participants completed three conditions: outgroup, ingroup and self

condition. In the ingroup condition, participants saw three 10-s videos of people who

shared the participant’s ethnic identity (Caucasian). In the outgroup condition they saw

three 10-s videos of each ethnic outgroup (African–Canadian, East Asian and South-

Page 34: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

25

Asian). The ingroup and outgroup condition appeared in randomized order and where

followed by the self condition during which participants performed a simple action for 30

s.

2.3.4 Electrophysiological Recording and Processing

To measure mu rhythm, I recorded the EEG from 32 Ag/AgCl sintered electrodes

embedded in a stretch-lycra cap. Recordings were collected according to the 10-20 system with a

band pass filter at 0.1 - 100Hz and a notch filter at 60 Hz. The EEG was digitized at 512 Hz

using ASA acquisition hardware (Advanced Neuro Technology, Enschede, the Netherlands) with

a digital average earlobe reference. Vertical eye movements (VEOG) were monitored using a

supra-to sub-orbital bipolar montage and the continuous EEG recordings were corrected off-line

for eye-blinks using the VEOG channel and the Second Order Blind Identification (SOBI)

procedure which is a signal processing method for isolating and removing ocular artifacts (Tang,

Liu, & Sutherland, 2005). During recording, the impedances were kept below 5 Kohm.

Continuous artefact-free epochs of 2.0s from each interval were extracted through a Hamming

window and overlapped by 75% to minimize data loss. I performed fast Fourier transformation

and calculated power in the 8-13 Hz band for 10 seconds for each ethnic group and 10 seconds

for self-action. I averaged the spectral data over the three outgroups and then created mu

suppression index scores for each condition by calculating the log ratio of mu power during that

condition over mu power during baseline. A log-ratio score was used to normalize the data and

to control for individual differences in absolute mu power due to differences in impedance, and

scalp thickness. I did this for all electrodes. However, because all participants performed the

action with their dominant right hand I performed statistical analysis only for electrode C3

Page 35: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

26

located over the contra lateral, left primary motor area. More negative scores indicate a stronger

suppression of the mu rhythm and, hence, more cortical activity.

2.4 Results

We created log-ratio scores (log mu power during action / log mu power during baseline)

for all conditions. As expected, the self-action log-ratio scores (M = -.33, SD = .40) were

significantly different from zero, t (29) = - 4.44, p < .01, d = 0.83 indicating that participants

showed mu suppression and suggesting an increase in motor cortex activity during motor action

compared to the motionless baseline condition. To compare mu-suppression during the different

conditions I conducted a within subject ANOVA over the three conditions (self, ingroup and

outgroup) which resulted in a significant main effect of group F (2, 28) = 9.20, p < .01.

Page 36: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

27

Figure 2: EEG mu suppression at left central scalp location (C3) during action and

during the observation of ingroup and outgroup members. Error bars represent ± 1

standard error. Negative numbers denote more mu-suppression (i.e. more neural activity)

at C3

Figure 2 illustrates that the data confirmed my hypotheses. Participants showed increased

mu suppression when passively observing ingroup members (M = -.06, SD = .13). Although

these log-ratio scores were significantly different from self-action, t (29) = -3.97, p <.01, d =

0.76, critically, the increase was significantly different from zero, t (29) = -2.40, p < .03, d =

0.46. This suggests that there was motor cortex activity when participants passively observed

ingroup members. Importantly, participants showed no activity over motor areas when they

observed outgroup members (M = -.01, SD = .13), t (29) = -.03, p = .77, a level of activity that

was significantly lower than while observing ingroup members, t (29) = 2.12, p < .05., d = 0.38.

These findings suggest that participants did not resonate with the actions of outgroup members.

Page 37: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

28

The analysis also revealed a significant linear trend, F (1, 29) = 9.47, p < .01, indicating the most

mu suppression during the self condition, followed by the ingroup, and finally the outgroup

condition. This linear trend, however, was subsumed under a significant quadratic trend, F (1,

29) = 17.91, p < .01, reflecting much stronger mu suppression during action than during

observation of action.

Subsequent analysis revealed a marginal difference between the three different

outgroups, F (2, 28) = 3.05, p = .06, such that participants showed more mu suppression for East

Asians (M = -.04, SD= .15), followed by Blacks (M = -.03, SD = .15), and then South Asians (M

= .02, SD = .16). Importantly, participants did not show significant mu suppression when

observing any of the outgroups (all ps > .14), thus justifying my a-priori decision to average

across groups. However, the marginal difference between them suggests different levels of mu

suppression for each group. Interestingly the degree of mirroring neatly reflects the distinct

Canadian context, where South Asians’ tend to have lower status than Blacks, and where East

Asians are seen as a model minority (Kalin & Berry, 1996).

Additional analyses confirmed that prejudice was inversely correlated with activity at

scalp locations over motor cortex in response to outgroup members. The more prejudiced

participants were, the less mu suppression they showed in response to outgroups1, r (28) =.52, p

1 1. Note that mu suppression is coded such that negative values indicate more

suppression (i.e. more motor cortex activity). A negative association between mu suppression

and prejudice, therefore, appears like a positively valued correlation.

Page 38: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

29

< .01 (see Figure 3). In contrast, I found no such correlation for the ingroup condition r (28) =

.18, p =.36, two correlations that are significantly different from one another, Z = 1.83, p < .035

(one tailed). When I breakdown the omnibus outgroup correlation into the specific racial

outgroups, I find results that are consistent with a Canadian context: The correlation was

strongest for South-Asians, r (28) = .56, p < .01, and followed by Blacks, r (28) = .36, p = .05;

the correlation for East-Asians, however, fell below traditional levels of significance, r (28) =

.30, p = .11. Since mu activity is inversely related to motor cortex activity, these findings suggest

that the more participants are prejudiced, the less their motor cortex fires in response to the

passive viewing of outgroup members’ actions — an effect that is magnified for disliked

outgroups (South Asians, then Blacks, followed by East Asians).

Page 39: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

30

Figure 3: Scalp topography of the correlations between mu suppression for outgroups

and prejudice. Darker grey indicates scalp locations for which mu suppression is

positively correlated with symbolic racism scores. Due to the inverse relationship of mu

suppression and motor activity, this depiction indicates that less motor activity in left

parietal areas is associated with higher levels of prejudice.

2.5 Discussion

These data suggest that people resonate less with the actions of outgroups. That is, those

neural networks involved in the simulation of actions and intentions — most likely part of the

“mirror-neuron-system” — are less responsive to outgroup members than to ingroup members. I

take this as evidence that people experience less vicarious action and their associated somatic

and autonomic states when confronted with outgroups than with ingroups. Especially, prejudiced

people failed to resonate with outgroup members’ actions and this failure to resonate was

Page 40: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

31

particularly pronounced for disliked outgroups. This suggests that people — but especially

people high in prejudice — do not intuitively grasp outgroup members’ intentions and actions. I

interpret this correlation with caution, however. While a natural deficit in outgroup-action

simulation can contribute to prejudice, it is also likely that prejudiced people do not resonate

with outgroup members because of their biases and dislikes. Specifically, prejudice might bias

attention against outgroups thereby impairing motor resonance.

A deficit in the spontaneous “catching” of outgroup members’ actions and intentions

could have serious consequences for intergroup interactions. Motor resonance could potentially

facilitate social understanding and social coordination and thereby might improve interactions.

Thus, due to the lack in motor resonance, people might not be as responsive to outgroup

member’s inner states and less likely to understand their actions and effectively communicate

with them.

In addition to the consequences of lacking an intuitive action understanding intergroup

interactions might also suffer from a lack of affective sharing. The perception action model of

empathy (Preston & de Waal, 2002), connects action understanding with affect sharing by

proposing a common mechanism – the perception action link. According to this model social

animals and especially primates, who rely heavily on their conspecifics for survival, use the

action perception link in various ways to navigate through their complicated social systems

(Preston & de Waal, 2002). When someone observes another, the observer has access to the

target’s inner states, because the mere observation activates the same neural and bodily

representations in the target. This is not only true for the observation of actions, which brings

about representations of the experience of performing the action, but also for the observation of

facial expressions which brings about representations of the other’s affective and motivational

Page 41: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

32

states. As proposed in the facial feedback hypothesis (Izard, 1990; Laird, 1984), once the

observer overtly adopts the object’s expressions and posture, these elicit bottom-up processes

that elicit the correspondent emotions. In the same way, and independent of overt mimicry,

neural motor resonance with the objects facial expressions would activate the associated neural

networks for the experience of these emotions enabling the observer to share the affective and

motivational states of the target. This emotional contagion is the most basic process in empathy

(Atkinson, 2002). Hence biases in motor resonance could potentially also translate into biases in

the sharing of somatic, autonomic, and ultimately emotional and motivational states during

intergroup encounters. I aimed to test this possible implication of biases in motor resonance in a

study looking at neural signals associated with the experience of negative affective states and

avoidance motivation.

Page 42: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

33

3 Group biases in the sharing of emotive states

Prejudice is an ongoing problem in human societies, with humans committing some of

the most hateful acts because of their prejudices, including acts of discrimination, segregation,

and even wars and genocides. At the same time, humans are truly empathic creatures: One-day

old babies cry when they hear other babies cry (Sagi & Hoffman, 1970), and the recent discovery

of mirror neurons and neural resonance suggests that we intuitively “catch” and “match” other’s

emotions and actions by mere observation. Humans, it appears, have a natural instinct to connect

with others and to share their emotions.

Does this natural tendency to share others’ emotions occur for our social outgroups? Here

I propose that although people can easily pick up others’ emotions (possibly a precursor to

empathy), they are less likely to do so when that other belongs to an outgroup. By looking at

prefrontal alpha asymmetry in response to ingroup and outgroup members’ negative emotions,

this study investigates whether people resonate less with members of social outgroups than

ingroups and whether this bias in empathy is intensified by increasing levels of prejudice. First, I

define what I mean by empathy.

3.1 The Nature of Empathy

Empathy is the ability to share others’ emotions (Preston & de Waal, 2002), or more

specifically, an affective response more appropriate to another’s situation than one’s own

(Hoffman, 2000). Note though, that emotional sharing seems to be only the first step of the full

blown empathic response, which requires at least minimal realization that the emotions one is

Page 43: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

34

feeling are caused by the other (Jean Decety & Jackson, 2004). Affect sharing without self-other

distinction is considered less empathic because it is not other-oriented. For example in a lab

experiment, rats failed to press a button to save another rat that suffered from electric shock and

instead crunched fearfully in the furthest corner expressing high levels of distress (Rice, 1962).

The next evolutionary step after affect sharing and the next step in child development is

empathic concern. Here, while the subject feels the emotions of the object, the subject is also

aware that the object is the source of its emotions and can appreciate and comprehend the other’s

situation (Preston & de Waal, 2002). This new distinction between internally and externally

generated emotions opens the door for more sophisticated empathic emotions – emotions such as

guilt, compassion, sorrow, but also spitefulness, resentment and envy. Often, empathic concern

evokes an other-oriented, altruistic motivation to alleviate the other’s distress (Batson & Shaw,

1991). The last stage of empathy is empathic perspective taking. The observer takes the target’s

perspective, takes their subjective situation and mental state into account, and uses this

information to interpret the emotions they intuitively sensed through affect sharing.

To illustrate, picture a mother who hears her baby cry. Initially feeling distressed herself;

she quickly shifts perspectives and consequently feels compassion with the baby and a strong

motivation to alleviate the baby’s distress. She, then, assesses the baby’s situation, taking into

account what she knows about its preferences, goals, and behavioral patterns, to infer what

caused the distress and what can reduce it. Without the initial vicarious distress, however, the

mother would lack the basis for all subsequent steps in her progress to helping - she would lack

the emotions necessary to fuel the motivation to help. Similarly, research on psychopaths shows

that, even with their higher order empathic perspective taking functions intact, without the ability

to feel with other’s pain, through affect sharing, these individuals are able to severely harm and

Page 44: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

35

sometimes kill others (Blair, 2008) – affect sharing through motor resonance is important for all

subsequent stages of empathy.

Moreover, recent evidence indicates that motor resonance is linked to empathy—the

more people show evidence of neural simulation to others’ actions and emotions, the higher their

levels of trait empathy (Hooker, Verosky, Germine, Knight, & D’Esposito, 2010) and the better

they perform on tasks that are contingent on emotional empathy (Pineda & Hecht, 2009).

3.2 Empathy and its Limits

Although motor resonance and affect sharing may be unconscious, there is growing

evidence that they are subject to top-down influences. One such influence is group-affiliation,

with research suggesting an “empathy-gap” whereby people are less likely to catch and match

the emotions of the outgroup than of the ingroup. For example, people are less likely to help

outgroup than ingroup members in need (Kunstman & Plant, 2008; Saucier, Miller, & Doucet,

2005), and less likely to value the lives of outgroup members as much as ingroup members

(Pratto & Glasford, 2008).

Evidence from the social neurosciences suggests that group affiliation similarly exerts a

top-down influence on neural resonance of other’s suffering. A recent fMRI study, for example,

found that people show more neural activation in pain circuits when observing the painful

penetration of the face of ethnic ingroup members than of outgroup members (Xu et al., 2009)

and when they see people in distressing and emotionally painful situations (Mathur, Harada,

Lipke, & Chiao, 2010). Moreover, such differences in empathy-related neural activation predict

costly helping behavior (Hein, Silani, Preuschoff, Batson, & Singer, 2010) and using transcranial

magnetic simulation (TMS), Avenanti, Sirigu, & Aglioti (2010) found less corticospinal muscle

Page 45: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

36

inhibition when participants observed ethnic outgroup members receiving painful stimuli than

when observing the same thing happening to ingroup members.

Finally, as described in the previous chapter, I have provided evidence that motor

resonance for actions is limited to the ingroup and is not available for the actions of outgroup

members (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010). Rather than reflecting innate ingroup preferences, however,

such ingroup biases may occur as a function of culturally-learned prejudice (Chiao & Mathur,

2010). For example, people high in social dominance orientation (Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth, &

Malle, 1994; Sidanius & Pratto, 2003), a personality trait that is strongly related to higher levels

of prejudice, show less activation in neural areas for pain when they see people in painful

situations (Chiao et al., 2009). In an intergroup context, high-prejudice people are especially

likely to show diminished motor resonance with the outgroup, be that diminished motor cortex

activity (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010) or reduced corticospinal inhibition (Avenanti et al., 2010).

Furthermore, the reduction in motor resonance in these two studies was particularly acute for

culturally-disliked outgroups, e.g., South Asians in Canada (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010) and Blacks

in Italy (Avenanti et al., 2010). Together, these studies suggest that these brain effects are more a

function of culture than innate preference.

The empathy gap may be part of a broader bias in perceptual processes whereby we are

more generally less receptive to outgroup members, including their facial expression and

emotions. People have greater difficulty recognizing outgroup members’ faces and interpreting

their facial expressions and voices relative to the ingroup (Elfenbein & Ambady, 2002b; Sporer,

2001). In addition, there seem to be differences in not only how sensitive people are to the

emotional expressions of outgroup members, but also in how they react to these emotions. For

example, fear and happiness expressed by outgroup faces elicit the opposite emotional reaction

Page 46: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

37

in the observer (Weisbuch & Ambady, 2008). Moreover, people show greater amygdala response

in response to fearful ingroup compared to outgroup faces (Chiao et al., 2009) – possibly because

the amygdala is sensitive to motivational relevance (Cunningham, Bavel, & Johnsen, 2008) and

fear in ingroup members is more likely to signal danger to one’s self than fear in outgroup

members. Noticing the fear of someone from one’s own ingroup, therefore, becomes relevant to

one’s own safety. In short, the emotions expressed by ingroups and outgroups may be perceived

and reacted to differently.

3.3 Overview

All told, social biases act as top-down influences that interfere with motor resonance

when it comes to outgroup targets, I suggest that people are less able to catch and match the

emotions of other people when those other people come from an outgroup. To my knowledge no

one has directly examined the sharing of emotions with the ingroup vs. outgroup on a neural

level. Previous studies have focused on basic motor and sensory processes such as motor activity

or the sensation of pain (Avenanti et al., 2010; Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010; Mathur et al., 2010; Xu

et al., 2009), but empathy also involves the sharing of basic emotional and motivational states

(Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1994; Levenson & Ruef, 1992). It is unclear from past research,

therefore, if there is an empathy gap in the actual sharing of emotions. To further investigate this

specific issue, researchers need to measure emotional processes more directly. In the current

study, I measure prefrontal alpha asymmetry as an index of emotional and motivational states

(Davidson & Tomarken, 1989; Harmon-Jones, 2003) to reveal possible ingroup biases in the

extent to which people intuitively catch the emotions of other people.

Page 47: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

38

We used EEG to measure the relative activation of the right and left prefrontal cortices;

prefrontal cortical areas being essential for processing of reward and punishment related

information (Davidson, 2004). Studies have consistently associated activity in the left prefrontal

cortex with the expression and experience of approach-related motivational and affective states

such as happiness, but also anger. In contrast, activation of the right prefrontal cortex has been

related to the expression and experience of withdrawal-related motivational and affective states

such as fear or sadness (Davidson, 2004; Harmon-Jones, Gable, & Peterson, 2010). These

differences in functionality have often been measured using relative EEG activity in the alpha

band (8-13 Hz) in electrodes over left and right prefrontal cortex (for a review, see Davidson,

1995). For instance, higher activation of right prefrontal cortex, as indicated by relatively lower

alpha at right prefrontal electrode sites, is associated with, depression (Allen, Iacono, Depue, &

Arbisi, 1993), a general trait-like negative emotional style (Wheeler, Davidson, & Tomarken,

2007), and with negative emotional responses to films (Davidson, Ekman, Saron, Senulis, &

Friesen, 1990). In contrast, higher activation of left prefrontal cortex is associated with approach

related positive affect (Gable & Harmon-Jones, 2008) and approach related anger (Harmon-

Jones & Sigelman, 2001). Importantly, the prefrontal cortex is involved in the experience and

expression of emotions, but not in the perception of emotions (Davidson, 2004). Hence,

measures of prefrontal alpha asymmetry during observation of an object-other provide a measure

of vicarious emotions free from confounds due to the mere perceptions of emotions.

I predicted that participants would show stronger right prefrontal alpha asymmetry,

indicating negative, withdrawal related emotions while experiencing sadness and that they would

show similar right prefrontal alpha asymmetry when passively observing ingroup members

nonverbally expressing sadness. Reflecting an ingroup bias, I expected no such vicarious right

prefrontal alpha asymmetry during the observation of outgroup members experiencing sadness. I

Page 48: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

39

further predicted that this lack of vicarious emotion when passively observing the outgroup

would be associated with participants’ level of prejudice: the more prejudiced people are, they

less they will catch the outgroup’s emotions.

3.4 Method

3.4.1 Participants

Our original sample consisted of 30 right handed university students. Participants were

recruited from an introductory psychology course at the University of Toronto Scarborough, and

participated in the study for course credit. Two participants were excluded from my original

sample due to a technical malfunction of the EEG system and another two were excluded

because they self-identified as Black and mixed-race. Consequently, my final sample consisted

of 26 participants, 10 males and 16 females, from various non-Black ethnic backgrounds (8

White, 6 East Asian, and 12 South Asian). The participants ranged in age from 18 to 23 Years

(M= 18.46, SD= 3.81).

3.4.2 Procedure

Participants were told that the study’s purpose was to investigate the neural

underpinnings of emotions. Participants read and signed an informed consent sheet and were

then fitted with and electrode cap for EEG recording. EEG was recorded while participants

watched a set of videos. The videos showed a variety of ingroup and outgroup members

expressing sadness. At the end of each video set, participants completed an emotion induction

task during which they were led to feel sadness by vividly remembering past sad events in their

lives. To assess how individual differences in prejudice and in the ability to empathize with

Page 49: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

40

others are related to differences in the vicarious experience of emotional states, I then had

participants complete several trait measures of prejudice and empathy.

3.4.3 Materials

3.4.3.1 Symbolic racism scale.

Participants completed the symbolic racism scale (Henry & Sears, 2002) during a mass

testing session in an introductory psychology course at the beginning of the term. The symbolic

racism scale is a measure of modern racism – a subtle form of racism that obscures racist

feelings with abstract values, such as justice and order.

3.4.3.2 Video manipulation.

The study had a one way within subject design with three conditions (self condition,

ingroup condition, and outgroup condition). Figure 4 graphically illustrates the design. For the

purpose of my study I defined ingroup as people who shared the participant’s ethnic identity, and

outgroup as people who did not. Videos of ingroup and outgroup members (White, Black, South

Asian, and East Asian) expressing sadness served as the independent variable for the randomly

presented outgroup and ingroup conditions, and this was followed by the self condition. The

videos depicted the actors sitting at a table in front of a white wall expressing sadness. In the

outgroup condition the participant saw four different actors from each ethnic outgroup. For

example, a white participant would see four different black actors for 20 seconds each, followed

by 4 different East Asian actors, and 4 different South Asian actors. In the ingroup condition

participants saw four different ingroup members, each for 20 seconds, expressing sadness.

Page 50: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

41

Figure 4: Outline of experimental design

To control for differences besides the ethnicity of the individual actors, I asked a separate

pilot sample of 9 people to rate the videos. Specifically, after viewing each video, the raters used

a 5-point scale to indicate the extent to which they thought the videoed individual (1) felt sad, (2)

felt positive, (3) felt negative, (4) experienced a strong emotion, (5) experienced an arousing

emotion, (6) was likable, and (7) was attractive. Raters further indicated (8) the overall technical

quality of the video, and (9) the amount of empathy they felt for the actor. Results revealed no

significant differences between ethnic groups on any of these variables, all ps > .16, ns,

suggesting that the actors from each ethnic group were viewed similarly, at least at an explicit

level.

To ensure that participants attended the videos throughout the whole session, they

performed a control task during the ingroup and outgroup conditions. For this task, the videos

stopped between 2 and 5 times and the screen turned black for 1 second during each condition.

Participants were asked to count how many times the black screen appeared and to indicate the

Page 51: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

42

number of such pauses at the end of the condition. Participants reported the correct number of

black screens in 83% of all cases indicating that they attended to the videos.

All ingroup and outgroup videos were presented in a random order, thus the ingroup

condition could appear in any position in the set of videos. The actors in the videos were male

students of the University of Toronto and therefore were approximately the same age, but not

always the same sex as the participants. Each actor was displayed for twenty seconds.

Consequently I obtained 80 seconds of EEG data during the ingroup conditions and 240 seconds

during the outgroup conditions - 80 seconds during each of the three different ethnic outgroups.

At the end of the video set, the participants were asked to experience the emotion of

sadness themselves (self condition). To facilitate the experience of sadness, participants

completed an emotion induction exercise, which consisted of a series of instructions aimed to

elicit vivid memories of past sad events. Participants saw the following instruction: “Please think

about a situation in your past, which made you feel very sad. Imagine the situation as vividly as

you can.” After completion of the exercise participants continued experiencing the emotion for

80 seconds while they looked at a blank computer screen.

3.4.3.3 Empathy Quotient.

In order to assess the trait like ability to empathize with others, I had participants

complete the empathy quotient (Baron-Cohen & Wheelwright, 2004). The empathy quotient is a

60 item forced choice self report scale that taps into both cognitive and emotional aspects of

empathy.

Page 52: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

43

3.4.3.4 Reading the mind in the eyes test.

I administered a second measure related to empathy – reading the mind in the eyes test

(Baron-cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste, & Plumb, 2001) – a measure of mindreading and social

sensitivity. Specifically, in this test participants are presented with a series of 25 photographs of

the eye-region of different faces. For each photograph they have to select one of four words that

best describes what the person in the photograph is thinking or feeling.

3.4.4 EEG Data Acquisition and Processing

To measure prefrontal alpha asymmetries, I recorded the EEG from 32 Ag/AgCl sintered

electrodes embedded in a stretch-lycra cap. Recordings were collected from the 32 electrode sites

according to the 10-20 system with a band pass filter at 0.1 - 100Hz and a notch filter at 60 Hz.

The EEG was digitized at 512 Hz using ASA acquisition hardware (Advanced Neuro

Technology, Enschede, the Netherlands) with an average earlobe reference. Vertical eye

movements (VEOG) were monitored using a supra-to sub-orbital bipolar montage and the

continuous EEG recordings were corrected off-line for eye-blinks using the VEOG channel and

the Second Order Blind Identification (SOBI) procedure, which is a signal processing method for

isolating and removing ocular artifacts (Tang et al., 2005). During recording, the impedances

were kept below 5 Kohm to ensure a clear and strong EEG signal. Continuous artefact-free

epochs of 2.0s from each interval were extracted through a Hamming window and overlapped by

75% to minimize data loss. As said above, I recorded 80 seconds of data for the observation of

each ethnic group. To avoid habituation, however, I calculated power only for the first 10

seconds of each actor, resulting in 40 seconds of data for each ethnic group. For the same reason,

I only included the first 10 seconds of EEG data acquired during the self condition. Power was

calculated via fast Fourier transform. Power values (in µV2) were averaged across epochs within

Page 53: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

44

each interval. Total power within the alpha band (8-13Hz), an inverse indication of cortical

activity, was logarithmically transformed and asymmetry scores were calculated as right-site

minus homologous left-site alpha power for lateral prefrontal sites (F8-F7), which is consistent

with how prefrontal asymmetry is typically calculated (Harmon-Jones, 2006). Higher scores

indicate relatively greater left-than-right cortical activation indicating more approach motivation

and less sadness/withdrawal.

3.5 Results

Results confirmed my hypotheses suggesting an ingroup bias in emotional sharing (see

Fig. 5). I created index scores of relative alpha activity for each condition (log right alpha

activity at F8– log left alpha activity at F7); such that more negative scores reflect greater

withdrawal and negative emotionality. A within subject ANOVA over the three conditions (self,

ingroup, and outgroup) did not reveal a significant main effect for condition F (2, 23) = 3.90; p =

ns. However, although the difference in alpha asymmetry scores for the ingroup and the outgroup

condition was not significant t (24) = -.99; p = ns, a significant linear trend indicated that alpha

asymmetry was highest during personal experience of sadness, followed by the observation of

the ingroup, and then by the observation of the outgroup, F (1, 23) = 4.37, p < .05. Participants’

alpha asymmetry scores when personally experiencing sadness (M = -.08, SD = .17) and when

observing ingroup members (ideographically chosen) experiencing sadness (M = -.05, SD = .15)

did not differ from one another, t (24) = 1.28, p > .20. This suggests a similarity in

emotional/motivational states when personally experiencing/remembering sadness and when

observing the ingroup experiencing sadness. In contrast, participants did not show such

prefrontal alpha asymmetry when passively observing outgroup members: Participants had

significantly more alpha asymmetry when personally experiencing sadness than when observing

Page 54: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

45

an outgroup member experiencing sadness, (M = -.03, SD = .14), t (24) = 2.09, p < .05. These

effects were specific to frontal sites such that alpha asymmetry scores at posterior lateral sites for

all the conditions were positive (all Ms > .05), did not differ from one another (all ps > .43), and

did not show a significant linear trend F (1, 23) = .64; p = ns. These data suggest that observing a

sad person from one’s own group elicits about the same amount of sadness as feeling sad

oneself; outgroup members, in contrast, elicit significantly less sadness. These findings suggest

an ingroup bias in emotional sharing.

Figure 5: Alpha asymmetry scores (log right alpha activity – log left alpha activity)

during the experience and observation of sadness as a function of group membership.

More negative scores indicate avoidant, negative affect.

Page 55: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

46

When I break down the omnibus outgroup to specific ethnicities, I did not find any

differences between subgroups. For example, White participants tended to have similar levels of

brain activity when passively viewing Blacks, South Asians, or East Asians. Once in the

outgroup, in other words, participants stopped differentiating between ethnicities (cf. Gutsell &

Inzlicht, 2010).

Our second prediction regarding the role of prejudice gained limited support. The results

are depicted in figures 6 and Table 1. Prejudice was only marginally associated with prefrontal

asymmetry scores in response to outgroup members: The higher participants scored on the

modern racism scale, the higher their asymmetry score, that is the less relative right prefrontal

activation they showed, r (24) = .37, p = .07. This suggests that the more prejudiced people

were, the less they might have vicariously felt an avoidant motivational state when observing

outgroup member in distress. The more prejudiced, the less they caught outgroup members’

emotions. Another study with more power is needed to confirm these marginally significant

results. In contrast, the correlation between prejudice and prefrontal asymmetry scores in

response to ingroup members was not significant, r (24) = .32, p = .12. However, it is important

to note that these two correlations are not significantly different from one another. So while

prejudiced people do not seem to vicariously share emotive states with outgroups, they may

show a (non-significant) tendency to not share it with ingroup members either. This may suggest

that biased attitudes toward outgroups may reflect egocentric biases more generally. For

example, research on the prejudiced personality suggests that people who score high on prejudice

are less likely to be agreeable (Graziano, Bruce, Sheese, & Tobin, 2007). And agreeableness is

associated with prosocial behavior (Graziano et al., 2007) and empathy (Nettle, 2007). Someone

who is prejudiced, therefore, may be less agreeable in general and show a lack of empathy

towards others in general.

Page 56: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

47

Figure 6: Symbolic racism scores as a function of prefrontal alpha asymmetry scores in the

outgroup condition.

Table

1

Interestingly, and inconsistent with my predictions, people’s general tendency and ability

to empathize with others and to read their emotions did not significantly predict alpha asymmetry

Page 57: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

48

in response to ingroup or outgroup members. That being said, although the effects were not

significant nor do we observe a trend, the pattern of the results suggest that future studies with

more power might find a relation to empathy such that the higher people are in empathy, as

assessed by the empathy quotient (Lawrence, Shaw, Baker, Baron-Cohen, & David, 2004), the

higher their asymmetry scores to both ingroup and outgroup. Please see Table 1 for a more

detailed depiction of these results.

3.6 Discussion

Taken together, my research provides direct evidence that people are less likely to show

prefrontal right alpha asymmetry when observing sad outgroup others. I take this as evidence

that people generally do not vicariously feel the emotional and motivational states of those they

categorize as outgroup members. Moreover, the extent of vicarious right alpha asymmetry in

response to sad outgroup members is associated with level of prejudice. In other words, the more

prejudiced people are, the less likely they will intuitively catch the emotive states of outgroup

members. This bias in affective sharing could contribute to an empathy-gap, impairing the

experience of empathy for outgroups, which is a capacity that underlies and facilitates social

understanding and cooperation and fosters helping, morality, altruism, and justice (Batson et al.,

1997; Cialdini, Brown, Lewis, Luce, & Neuberg, 1997). Thus, when people fail to share the

emotional and motivational states of outgroup members they might not be as responsive to

outgroup member’s needs and might be less likely to help or even to understand that support is

needed. This could be particularly true for people high in prejudice.

It is important to note that my findings do not indicate any causal direction between the

lack of empathy towards outgroups and prejudice. While it is possible that a lack of empathy can

Page 58: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

49

contribute to prejudice, it is just as likely that prejudice contributes to a lack of empathy. It is

possible that both mechanisms are at work, creating a cycle in which people initially empathize

less with outgroups, which makes them a likely target for prejudice, which then further restricts

empathy towards the disliked group. The exact nature of the relationship is difficult to determine

and should be addressed by future research.

Finally it is important to stress that, although I suspect that the ease with which people

can empathize with the ingroup may have been biologically hardwired by evolutionary

processes, I am not suggesting that people are more or less likely to include or exclude specific

ethnic groups. Indeed, the gap in motor resonance may be specific to culturally disliked groups

that change from one society to the next (e.g. Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010). Instead, the specific

coalitional alliances that populate our ingroup category are likely arbitrary, and can in fact be

changed very easily (Turner et al., 1979). Recent research shows that despite a lifetime's

experience of race as a predictor of social alliance, minimal exposure to alternate coalitional

possibilities is enough to deflate the tendency to categorize by race (Cosmides, Tooby, &

Kurzban, 2003; Van Bavel & Cunningham, 2009).

Hence, even if we are less likely to resonate with the emotions and actions of the

outgroup, these effects may be temporary and can be erased when we foster empathy toward the

outgroup, or better yet, include a greater number of people into our ingroup and thereby

increasing their motivational relevance. One means to achieve this goal might be cognitive

perspective taking. Taking the perspective of an outgroup member has been shown to reduce

prejudice towards the outgroup (Batson et al., 1997) and this reduction is mediated by an

increase in perceived self-other overlap (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000). By fostering a feeling of

connectedness, perspective taking offers a way to overcome biases in motor resonance and

Page 59: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

50

thereby allows an intuitive understanding between people whether they share group membership

or not. In the following chapter I describe a study that looks at the potential of perspective-taking

in reducing group biases in motor resonance.

Page 60: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

51

4 Perspective taking reduces group biases in motor

resonance

People say that to gain a true understanding of another, one needs to put oneself into

another’s shoes and try to see the world through their eyes. The assumption here is that

understanding comes from using one’s own references and one’s own body to simulate the

experiences of the other. In recent years, neuroscience has supported these folk psychology

notions of how we understand each other: the mere perception of other’s actions (Oberman,

Ramachandran, & Pineda, 2008), facial expressions (Carr et al., 2003), and pain (Singer et al.,

2004) produces the same type of neural activity that would occur if one were to actually engage

in action, express emotions, or feel pain. Such neural resonance might be associated with more

emotional empathy (Pineda & Hecht, 2009) and potentially facilitates an intuitive understanding

of the other’s actions, intentions and emotions (Fadiga et al., 1995)

Neural resonance, however, can be more or less effective depending on the target. For

example, people only resonate with the pain of those they like (Singer et al., 2006). Neural

resonance can also be biased in systematic ways, excluding whole groups of social targets. For

instance research participants show activation in neural pain networks only when they see

members of their own ethnic group in pain, but not ethnic outgroups (Xu et al., 2009). Similarly,

participants show activation in sensory motor areas only when observing people of the same, but

not different ethnicities (Avenanti et al., 2010; Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010).

Page 61: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

52

Thus, the kind of social target matters. Whether we resonate with others, however, might

also depend on our state of mind. Specific mindsets might override prepotent tendencies. Here, I

ask how the perceiver’s mindset affects motor resonance and whether being in an empathic

mindset could increase motor resonance even for targets, such as ethnic outgroup members, who

we would otherwise not resonate with.

4.1 Limiting and Facilitating Factors for Motor Resonance

Motor resonance is not as automatic and inevitable as one might think; we do not

resonate with just any action, with just any target, at just any time. Whether someone resonates

with somebody else depends on many variables, some related to the kind of action, some related

to the target, and some related to the observer. For example, research participants show less

motor resonance in non-interactive situations than interactive situations (Oberman et al., 2008),

in situations that require complementary responses rather than non-complementary ones (Sartori,

Cavallo, Bucchioni, & Castiello, 2011), and those that involve unfamiliar rather than familiar

actions (Calvo-Merino, Glaser, Grèzes, Passingham, & Haggard, 2005).

Characteristics of the target, such as cultural background and ethnicity also modulate

motor resonance. As described in chapter two I found less motor resonance in response to ethnic

outgroup members than ethnic ingroup members performing a simple action (Gutsell & Inzlicht,

2010). Moreover, Molnar-Szakacs, Wu, Robles and Iacoboni (2007) found that whether

participants showed corticospinal muscle inhibition in response to culturally specific gestures

depended on the cultural background of the target person.

Prejudice, a perceiver variable, also influences motor resonance. People high in

prejudice are especially likely to show diminished motor resonance with the outgroup, be that

Page 62: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

53

diminished motor cortex activity (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010) or reduced corticospinal inhibition

(Avenanti et al., 2010). Thus, the perceivers’ chronic biases and assumptions shape how much

and with whom they resonate. Hence, changing the perceiver’s mindset might be a powerful tool

to influence the amount of motor resonance. In this study, I examine how experimental changes

to the perceiver’s mindset can influence motor resonance. Specifically, I ask if having people

take the perspective of someone else could enhance motor resonance.

4.2 Cognitive Perspective Taking

Cognitive perspective taking refers to the active contemplation of others’ psychological

experiences. It has long been considered to predict greater empathy. For example, perspective

taking facilitates altruistic behavior (Batson et al., 1997) and cooperation (Batson & Moran,

1999). In addition to these more general benefits of perspective taking, it can be helpful

specifically in an intergroup context (Batson et al., 1997; Dovidio et al., 2004; Galinsky & Ku,

2004). Perspective taking improves evaluations of specific members of the outgroup and of the

outgroup more generally (Dovidio et al., 2004; Galinsky & Ku, 2004). For example, nurses who

take the perspective of Black chronic pain patients show less racial bias in decisions for pain

treatment than those who do not take the patient’s perspective (Drwecki, Moore, Ward, &

Prkachin, 2011).

Taken together, cognitive perspective taking can improve interpersonal rapport and

facilitate altruistic behavior towards individuals who would otherwise be unlikely targets of

empathy. Moreover, the positive effects of perspective taking can generalize beyond the

interpersonal domain to encompass intergroup situations as well—taking the perspective of one

outgroup individual can affect how one perceives and treats other outgroup members in

Page 63: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

54

situations outside of the perspective taking context and at a later time. Given all the benefits of

perspective taking, a perspective taking mindset could be a powerful tool to decrease intergroup

biases, including biases in motor resonance.

4.3 Overview

Some research suggests that perspective taking could potentially have an effect on motor

resonance. People who tend to take the perspective of others also have a tendency to imitate

others (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Perspective taking also increases cognitive co-representation

of others’ behavior. In one study, participants, after having taken the perspective of an outgroup

member, showed less interference in a joint simon task – a measure of co-representation of

another’s actions (Müller et al., 2011). Moreover, directly measuring brain activity over sensory

motor areas using EEG, Woodruff and Bilyk (2011) showed that people who score high on a

self-report trait measure of perspective taking, show stronger neural activity in the motor cortex

while observing others performing actions.

Here, I directly investigate the effects of a perspective-taking mindset on motor

resonance in an intergroup context. I hypothesize that people in a perspective taking mindset will

resonate even with members of a social outgroup. In contrast, I hypothesize that putting people

in an objective mindset (a detached, uninvolved approach when thinking about others’

experiences) will lead them to resonate less with others, even others belonging to the ingroup.

Thus, I expected perspective taking to reduce group biases in motor resonance

To test this hypothesis I measured EEG oscillations over sensory motor areas in the mu-

frequency range while participants watched videos of ethnic ingroup and ethnic outgroup

members performing a simple action.

Page 64: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

55

4.4 Methods

4.4.1 Participants

36 self-identified White undergraduate students enrolled in an introductory psychology

course at the University of Toronto Scarborough participated in the study for course credit. Two

participants were excluded because they did not follow the instructions and four were excluded

because of missing data due to experimenter error or equipment failure. This resulted in a total of

30 participants (16 female; average age = 20.10, SD = 2.22) included in my final sample. I used a

2 (Mindset: perspective taking vs. objective) x 2 (Group: ingroup vs. outgroup) mixed design

with the last factor repeated. The ingroup was defined as participant’s ethnic ingroup (White)

and the outgroup as people of a different and often marginalized ethnic group (Black).

4.4.2 Procedure

Participants arrived in the laboratory one at a time and provided informed consent. After

fitting each participant with an electrode cap, I recorded 30 seconds of baseline EEG activity.

Next, participants were randomly assigned to one of the two mindset conditions. All participants

saw the same picture of a Black male university student and were asked to write a short story

describing a day in life of this student. Those in the perspective taking condition were asked to

take the perspective of the student while writing. Those in the control condition were asked to

remain as objective as possible depending while writing the story. After completion of the story,

participants saw a series of videos showing a variety of ingroup and outgroup members

performing a simple action while I recorded their EEG.

Page 65: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

56

4.4.3 Materials

4.4.3.1 Mindset manipulation.

To manipulate a perspective taking mindset versus an objective mindset I showed

participants a picture of a young Black university student. The student was shown sitting on a

couch, holding an open book, while gazing into the camera with a smile. Participants were asked

to write a short story about a day in his life either taking his perspective, or remaining objective.

Specifically, in the perspective taking condition participants were asked to: “Take the

perspective of the individual in the photograph and imagine a day in the life of this individual as

if you were that person, looking at the world through his eyes and walking through the world in

his shoes.” and to write the story in first person. In contrast, participants in the objective mindset

condition read the following instructions:” Try to be as objective as possible when imagining

what is happening to this individual and what his day is like. Don’t get caught up in imagining

what this individual might think or how he might feel.” and they were asked to write the story in

third person.

4.4.3.2 Video stimuli.

While I recorded their EEG, participants saw a series of videos showing young

university-aged men of African-Canadian and European-Canadian origin repeatedly drinking a

sip of water from a glass and putting it back down. The videos were taken from a set of videos

previously used in a study described here in chapter 2 and in Gutsell and Inzlicht (2010), though

participants in the current study did only see a subset of the videos presented in the previous

study. Specifically, participants saw two blocks of videos each containing three 10second video-

clips of 3 different White actors or 3 different Black actors, respectively. Both the order of the

video blocks and the order of the videos within the blocks were randomized. Following the

Page 66: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

57

videos, I asked participants to perform the same action themselves. Participants received a glass

of water and, repeatedly drank a small sip and put the glass back in place for 30 seconds.

4.4.4 Electrophysiological Recording and Processing

To measure mu-rhythm, I recorded the EEG from 32 Ag/AgCl sintered electrodes

embedded in a stretch-lycra cap. Recording procedure and data processing were identical to the

procedures used in Gutsell and Inzlicht (2010) described in chapter 2 of the current dissertation. I

performed fast Fourier transformation and calculated power in the 8–13 Hz band for 30 seconds

for each ethnic group and 30 seconds for self-action and for baseline. I then averaged the spectral

data over the three outgroups.

4.5 Results

To control for differences in baseline mu power, I calculated log-ratio scores of mu

power during the observation of action and during action (log mu oscillations during action

observation/ log mu oscillations during baseline) for all conditions. More negative scores

indicate a stronger suppression of the mu-rhythm and, hence, more cortical activity. Since the

resulting mu-suppression scores serve as an indicator of activation of neural sensory motor areas,

I expected strong mu-suppression during the self-action condition in the left hemisphere

contralateral to movement of the right dominant hand. Thus, although I calculated mu-

suppression scores for all electrodes I performed statistical analysis only for electrode C3 located

over the contralateral, left primary motor area.

As predicted, mu-suppression scores during the self-action condition (M=-.40, SD=.67)

were significantly different from zero t (29) = - 3.40; p = < .05, d = .60, indicating that sensory

Page 67: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

58

motor areas were active while participants performed the action themselves. Moreover, reflecting

that neural activation during actual actions is not influenced by the kind of cognitive mindset, I

found no significant difference in mu-suppression scores t (28) = .36 , ns, during the self-action

between participants in the objective (M = -44, SD = .85) and the perspective mindset condition

(M = -.36, SD = .41).

In contrast, I expected mindset to influence mu-suppression during the observation

conditions. Previous research has shown that by default, participants show mu-suppression for

ethnic ingroup members, but not for the ethnic outgroup (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010). To test

whether adopting a perspective taking mindset could alleviate these group biases in mu-

suppression, I first conducted a one sample t-test for the objective and the perspective taking

mindset separately. Figure 7 illustrates that that adopting a perspective taking mindset facilitates

mu-suppression: participants in a perspective taking mindset showed mu-suppression

significantly different from zero both when observing ingroup members (M = -.11, SD =.20; t

(15) = -2.16; p < .05, d = .55) and when observing ethnic outgroup members (M = -.13, SD = .23;

t (15) = -2.20; p < .05, d = .57). These findings suggest that participants in the perspective taking

mindset showed mu-suppression not only for the ingroup, but also for the outgroup. In contrast,

mu-suppression scores for participants who had adopted an objective mindset were not

significantly different from zero, neither for the ingroup (M = -.11, SD = .61; t (13) = -.68, ns, d

= .18) nor for the outgroup (M = 0.07, SD =.50; t (13) = .49, ns, d = .14) suggesting that an

objective mindset hampers mu-suppression for both the ingroup and the outgroup.

Taken together, these findings suggest that a perspective taking mindset affects people’s

general tendency to resonate with others: When in a perspective taking mindset, people show

Page 68: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

59

mu-suppression indicative of motor resonance, and when in an objective taking mindset, they do

not2.

Figure 7: EEG mu-suppression at left central scalp location (C3) during the observation

of ingroup and outgroup members for participants in a perspective taking mindset and for

participants in an objective mindset. Error bars represent ±1 standard error. Negative

numbers denote more mu-suppression (i.e. more neural activity) at C3.

To test whether the ingroup and the outgroup condition where differentially affected by

the mindset manipulation, I conducted a 2 (mindset: perspective taking vs. objective) X 2

2 Please note, however, that we have to be careful when interpreting the findings for the objective mindset condition.

Because we had to exclude several participants from analysis our sample size is relatively small (n = 15). Based on

the effect size obtained for mu-suppression for the ingroup in the objective mindset condition, with this sample size

we only have a power of .48 to find the effect. In order to have reasonable power (.80 or higher) we’d need at least

28 participants in the objective mindset condition. Nonetheless, given that we consistently do not find mu-

suppression for the outgroup in neutral conditions (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010; Gutsell & Inzlicht, in preparation – also

see chapter 5 in this document for these findings), we are relatively confident that we would find the obtained null

effect would also hold in a higher power design.

Page 69: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

60

(Group: ingroup vs. outgroup) mixed model ANOVA with the last factor repeated. This analysis

revealed no significant main effect for group F (1, 28) = 2.36, p = ns, but a marginally significant

interaction, F (1, 28) =3.84, p=.06. Additional analyses confirmed our hypothesis that having

people adopt an empathic mindset reduces group biases in motor resonance. Participants in the

objective mindset condition showed significantly more mu-suppression for the ingroup (M=-.11,

SD=.61) than for the outgroup (M=.07, SD=.50), F (1, 28) = 5.73; p <.05, d = .62. In contrast,

participants in a perspective taking mindset do not show such group distinction: Mu-suppression

for the ingroup (M = -.11, SD = .20) and the outgroup (M = -.13, SD = .23) did not differ, F(1,

28) =.10, p = ns, d =.08. These findings suggest that when in a perspective taking mindset,

people show equal amounts of mu-suppression indicative of motor resonance in response to

others regardless of the target’s group membership and, thus, no longer show an ingroup bias in

motor resonance. At the same time, adopting an objective mindset makes it unlikely that people

will resonate with others in general, and even less so when the other is a member of an ethnic

outgroup.

4.6 Discussion

Previous research indicates that people do not resonate with others if they are not part of

their own ethnic group (Avenanti et al., 2010; Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010). Using an

electrophysiological indicator of motor resonance, I investigated how a perspective taking

mindset influences the amount of motor resonance with others in general and with the outgroup

in particular. I found that after participants took the perspective of an outgroup member, they

showed a reduction in rhythmic mu-oscillations over sensory motor cortex when later observing

other outgroup members performing actions. Thus, when people are in a perspective taking

mindset, they resonate with the actions of outgroup members – something they do not usually do.

Page 70: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

61

Moreover, these same participants did not show group biases in motor resonance; instead they

displayed equal levels of mu-suppression in response to ingroup and outgroup members. In

contrast, participants, who adopted an objective mindset failed to show motor resonance in

response to outgroup members. Instead, they showed a group bias in motor resonance; such that

they resonated significantly less with outgroup members than with ingroup members.

Therefore, putting people in a perspective taking mindset is an effective mean to reduce

group biases in motor resonance. Motor resonance most likely helps people to gain an intuitive

understanding of others’ actions and intentions (Fogassi et al., 2005) and might even be involved

in basic empathic processes such as emotional contagion (Pineda & Hecht, 2009). Hence, group

biases in motor resonance might ultimately translate to misunderstandings and impaired social

rapport, empathy, and social coordination. Perspective taking has been shown to successfully

reduce prejudice (Batson et al., 1997; Galinsky & Ku, 2004), to build social bonds, and to

increase empathy and helping (Batson et al., 1997). Alleviating group biases in motor resonance

might yet be another important benefit.

4.6.1 The Objective Mindset

In past research, the objective mindset manipulation I used here was assumed to be a

neutral state and thereby served as a control condition (e.g. Galinsky, Wang, & Ku, 2008; Todd,

Bodenhausen, Richeson, & Galinsky, 2011). I wonder, however, if this is a fair assumption given

that in the present study, being in an objective mindset does not seem to be a neutral state at all.

In a neutral state, research participants tend to resonate with social ingroup members (Avenanti

et al., 2010; Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010) and neutral targets (Oberman et al., 2008), but participants

in the objective mindset of the current study failed to show significant mu-suppression to

Page 71: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

62

anyone. My findings, therefore, suggest that adopting an objective mindset decreases motor

resonance in general, not only for the outgroup.

We often stress the importance of staying objective, especially for certain professions.

Judges, doctors, scientists all strive for objectivity, but sometimes the drawbacks of an objective

mindset might offset the benefits. If a judge is blind to the subtle nuances of the witness’ facial

expressions and body language, she might miss important information. If the doctor fails to

intuitively catch and match his patient’s inner states he might have a harder time connecting with

and diagnosing them. Indeed, more empathic medical students perform better on standard

measures of medical competence, and their patients are more satisfied with their treatment (Blatt,

LeLacheur, Galinsky, Simmens, & Greenberg, 2010). At the same time, being objective can be

self-protective. For example, empathy and emotional contagion have been related to depression

and fatigue in health care service personnel (Miller, Stiff, & Ellis, 1988). Thus, the

consequences of adopting an objective mindset, both pro and con, should be further explored in

future research

4.6.2 Perspective Taking

Being in a perspective taking mindset has been shown to improve social interactions

(Todd et al., 2011). Research participants who had previously completed the perspective taking

mindset manipulation utilized in the current study, later liked an outgroup interaction partner

better, displayed more friendly nonverbal behavior, and were judged as being more pleasant

interaction partners than those who did not take a specific mindset or an objective mindset (Todd

et al., 2011). The increase in motor resonance could potentially contribute to these positive

intergroup interaction experiences. Mu-suppression is a very basic process that does not require

conscious awareness or effort (Rizzolatti & Craighero, 2004), and thus does not draw cognitive

Page 72: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

63

resources away from other important processes such as impression management and

conversational skills. By facilitating social understanding, coordination, and rapport in an

unobtrusive way, the increase of motor resonance could potentially facilitate smooth intergroup

encounters. Future research should investigate such potential contributions by looking at motor

resonance before and during intergroup interactions.

4.7 Conclusions

As a default, people tend not to resonate with the actions of outgroup members and

consequently they might not intuitively grasp outgroup member’s actions, intentions and

potentially even other inner states. The current research provides evidence for the utility of

perspective taking as a strategy for combating this subtle consequence of racial bias. Adopting a

perspective taking mindset has been shown to be conducive to smooth and pleasant intergroup

interaction experiences. The findings reported here suggest a potential mechanism: When in a

perspective taking mindset, people are more likely to resonate even with outgroup members,

which potentially could provide them with a richer, more intuitive understanding of the other’s

actions and intentions possibly leading to smoother and more positive intergroup interactions.

Perspective taking might be so efficient in reducing group biases in general and biases in

motor resonance in particular because it reduces social categorization, thereby annulling the

negative effect that categorizing someone as an outgroup member can have on motivational

relevance for the observer. Increasing closeness to the target, however, is not the only possible

approach to make a target motivationally relevant. If we truly take a functional approach to

motor resonance, it does not seem sensible to think that people only resonate with those who

they feel close to. Altruism and reciprocity are not the only reasons for why we need to

Page 73: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

64

understand others’ goals and intentions, for why others’ behavior is motivationally relevant.

Another reason for why a behavior might be relevant is that it poses a potential threat. Thus far,

I only investigated motor resonance in response to neutral outgroup actions. The next chapter

describes a study that investigated motor resonance for positive and threatening actions

performed by ingroup versus outgroup members.

Page 74: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

65

5 Threatening outgroup behavior is resonated with

When interacting with others, humans quickly and easily grasp the intentions, action

tendencies, and inner states of their interaction partners. One mechanism through which this

possibly could be achieved is through motor resonance, the activation of the observers own

neural motor networks at the sight of the actions of another ( e.g. Cheng, Tzeng, Decety, lmada,

& Hsieh, 2006; Pineda & Hecht, 2009; Schippers & Keysers, 2011; Ulloa & Pineda, 2007;

Woodruff, Martin, & Bilyk, 2011). By mapping the other’s actions and associated inner states

with their own neural system, observers could potentially gain a more intuitive and personal

understanding into the other’s experiences, which in turn could facilitate social coordination and

smooth interactions. Motor resonance, however, is biased by prejudice and social group

affiliations. For example, although people show neurophysiological EEG activity over sensory

motor areas when observing ingroup members performing an action, they do not show motor

resonance when looking at ethnic outgroup members performing an action (Gutsell & Inzlicht,

2010). Yet, from an evolutionary perspective, as well as in light of the challenges of modern,

multicultural societies, it seems unlikely that people would never resonate with outgroup

members. Depending on the situational context, the actions, intentions and experiences of an

outgroup member could potentially be of equal, if not even more interest, than those of an

ingroup member. Here I investigate motor resonance in just such a context: I ask whether motor

resonance is increased for actions performed by a threatening outgroup members.

Page 75: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

66

5.1 Motor resonance – a Prosocial Process?

Motor resonance is considered to underlie the understanding of actions and intentions

(Erlhagen, Mukovskiy, & Bicho, 2006), which is in turn essential for interpersonal and social

processes such as learning through observation, interpersonal coordination, communication, and

empathy (Knoblich & Sebanz, 2006) As such, mirror neurons and motor resonance can appear as

being somehow intrinsically prosocial. However, humans are not only empathic creatures. They

also the capacity to be Machiavellian, that is they use their ability to read others minds to predict

their behavior and ultimately outsmart them (Byrne & Whiten, 1988; Carruthers, 2009; Dunbar,

2000). In essence, motor resonance is just a tool that gives us access to other’s inner states. What

we do with this information can depend on many factors such as the specific situation, the

perceivers personality and his or her relationship to the target (Hein & Singer, 2008). For

example, we can understand the meaning and the nature of other’s sadness through resonating

with their facial expression and by catching their distress through emotional contagion. Whether

we will react with sympathy or schadenfreude or indifference depends on our relationship with

them. Similarly, we might be interested in understanding others’ intentions because we could

potentially learn from them or because they might aggress against us. In both cases motor

resonance will help us to achieve our goal. Therefore, whether we resonate with other people

should be determined by the motivational relevance of the target person. For example, a friend,

or an ingroup member might be more relevant than a stranger or an outgroup member. Motor

resonance should, however, also depend on how intrinsically relevant the actions are. A

communicative gesture or a threatening action should be more relevant than a meaningless

action.

Page 76: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

67

As outlined before, the actions and expressions of social ingroup members seem to be

intrinsically motivationally relevant. People tend to allocate their limited cognitive and

perceptional resources towards others who, for one reason or another, are motivationally relevant

to them (Ackerman et al., 2006). Moreover, these relevant others are most often members of the

social ingroup. Consequently, intra group interactions are most important to us, and processing

ingroup members’ inner states, goals and actions, most often takes priority (Ackerman et al.,

2006), and, thus, by default motor resonance might be limited to the ingroup. Indeed, as

described in the previous chapters, we showed that people do not show motor resonance in

response to outgroup members actions (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010) and they do not tend to catch

outgroup member’s affective and emotional states (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2012).

In these previous studies, I only investigated motor resonance in response to neutral

outgroup actions. Would people still show an ingroup bias in motor resonance, when the actions

of outgroup members were more relevant to their own motivations, such as when the behavior in

question would signal an attempt to affiliate or a threat? Threat in particular is highly

motivationally relevant and could, thus be an effective mean to increase motor resonance.

5.2 A bias towards threat and negativity

A common finding is that humans have an attentional bias toward negative information in

general, which likely is a consequence of our evolutionary history. According to this perspective,

threat information is more important for survival than non-threat information, a bias towards

negative and uncertain information is adaptive (e.g., Hirsh & Inzlicht, 2008; Rozin & Royzman,

2001). Individuals who are more vigilant for negative stimuli are more likely to avoid bad things

Page 77: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

68

and would, thus, be more likely to pass along their genes (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Finkenauer,

& Vohs, 2001).

An example for the negativity bias is the fact that people focus more on and are

motivated by potential losses than potential gains (Tversky & Kahneman, 1991). Similarly,

negative stimuli have a greater impact on information processing than do positive stimuli

(Taylor, 1991); they are also evaluated as being more extreme compared to equally intensive

positive stimuli (Ito, Larsen, Smith, & Cacioppo, 1998). Moreover, people pay more attention

towards negative information and are automatically drawn to it, and this happens within

milliseconds as reflected in a negativity bias in early attentional event related potentials (Smith,

Cacioppo, Larsen, & Chartrand, 2003).

The negativity bias also includes social stimuli. For example, Fiske (1980) found that

people looked longer at photographs depicting negative than positive behaviors, presumably

paying more attention to negative behavior when forming impressions. In addition to holding

attention, negative social stimuli can also draw attention. For example, research participants were

faster to pick a lone angry face out of a grit of happy faces than vice versa (Hansen & Hansen,

1988; Ohman, Lundqvist, & Esteves, 2001). Anger and threat might be particularly effective in

an intergroup context, such that angry out-group members might be motivationally relevant

because they pose a potential threat. In support of this hypothesis, Ackerman and colleagues

(2006) found that people were able to remember outgroup faces better when those faces

displayed an angry expression rather than a neutral one. Moreover, when participants cognitive

capacities were limited, participants actually remembered angry outgroup faces better than angry

ingroup faces, thus reversing the outgroup homogeneity effect that usually leads to worse

memory of outgroup members (Ostrom & Sedikides, 1992).

Page 78: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

69

5.3 Overview

Given the prevalence of the negativity bias and Ackerman and colleagues’ findings

showing the effectiveness of anger and threat to increase the motivational relevance of outgroup

members and to override ingroup biases in person perception, I aimed to test the effects that

threat can have on motor resonance for outgroup members.

Here, I use suppression of EEG mu-oscillations as an index of motor resonance, in order

to directly investigate whether threatening outgroup behavior is resonated with or not. I

measured motor resonance, while participants watched videos of ethnic ingroup and ethnic

outgroup members performing neutral, positive and threatening actions. I hypothesized that

people would show mu-suppression in response to ingroup members independent of intent of the

action, since ingroup members already motivationally relevant. Second, I hypothesize that people

will not show mu-suppression in response to neutral outgroup behavior, replicating our previous

findings (Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010). Critically, I expect that, because of the negativity bias and

the motivational relevance of threatening behavior, people will show mu-suppression in response

to threatening outgroup behavior but not positive or neutral outgroup behavior. I do not have

specific predictions about positive outgroup behavior. Positive behavior should be more

motivationally relevant than neutral behavior, potentially increasing show mu-suppression in

response to outgroup members. However, positive stimuli do not attract attention as much as

negative stimuli do (See Peeters & Czapinski, 1990, for a review). Hence, it is also possible that

positive behavior might not be motivationally relevant enough to affect show mu-suppression in

response to outgroup members.

Page 79: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

70

5.4 Method

5.4.1 Participants

33 self-identified European Canadian and East-Asian Canadian undergraduate students

enrolled in an introductory psychology course at the University of Toronto Scarborough

participated in the study for course credit. I had to exclude 3 participants due to missing data and

experimenter error during EEG recording. Moreover, I excluded 7 participants because they did

not show mu rhythm suppression during baseline recording3. The final sample consisted of 23

participants (12 female; 5 European Canadian; average age = 18.95, SD = 2.80). I used a 3

(Action intent: neutral vs. affiliative vs. threatening) x 3 (Group: ingroup vs. outgroup vs. self)

within subject design. The ingroup was defined as participant’s ethnic ingroup (European

Canadian or East-Asian Canadian respectively) and the outgroup as people of two different and

often marginalized ethnic groups (African Canadian and South-Asian Canadian).

5.4.2 Procedure

Participants arrived in the laboratory one at a time and provided informed consent. They

were then fitted with an electrode cap and I recorded 30 seconds of baseline EEG activity.

Following baseline recording participants were asked to watch a series of videos while trying to

sit as still as possible, not moving and simply passively observing, to ensure good EEG

3 Since we define motor resonance as the decrease of mu-oscillations over sensory motor areas during baseline, it is

necessary to have a proper baseline for each participants. Several participants did only display very weak mu-rhythm

during baseline which resulted in non-significant overall mu-suppression during the self neutral action condition,

creating the impression that participants’ motor cortex was inactive during actual movement. To rectify this

problem, we excluded all participants with baseline mu lower than 2.00. We chose this threshold by increasing it in

.2 increments starting from 0 and choosing the point at which mu-suppression during self-action became significant.

Page 80: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

71

recording. The videos depicted different examples of ethnic ingroup and outgroup members

performing a neutral, a positive and a threatening action. After the Video task, participants were

asked to perform the same actions they just saw themselves. Finally, participants filled out a

series of questionnaires and received a detailed debriefing.

5.4.3 Materials

5.4.3.1 Manipulation of action intent.

To manipulate the intent of the depicted actions, I used videos of ingroup and outgroup

members repeatedly performing a neutral, a positive and a threatening action. The actors in the

videos were of European Canadian, East-Asian Canadian, African Canadian, or South-Asian

Canadian ethnicity. However, participants only saw either European Canadian or East- Asian

Canadian actors depending on which of the two ethnicities constituted their own ethnic ingroup.

In addition to actors from their own ethnic group, each participants saw the same set of African

Canadian and South-Asian Canadian actors who constituted their ethnic outgroup. To ensure that

participants did not react differently to African Canadian and South-Asian Canadian actors I

conducted a 2(group:African Canadian vs. South-Asian Canadian) X 3(intent: neutral vs.

positive vs. threat) on mu-suppression scores. This analysis resulted in no significant main

effects of group or intent and no significant interaction (all p-values > .17). Moreover,

participants did not show differences in mu-suppression in response to African Canadian and

South-Asian Canadian actors in any of the intent conditions (all p-values > .35). To simplify

subsequent analysis, I calculated compound mu-suppression scores for the outgroup by

averaging African Canadian and South-Asian Canadian mu-suppression scores.

Page 81: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

72

The videos showed three different male, university aged actors for each of the three ethnic

groups (ingroup, African Canadian, and South-Asian Canadian), each standing in front of a white

wall and performing all three actions.

For the neutral action, actors displayed a neutral facial expression and repeatedly lifted

their right arm up and down. For the positive action, actors displayed a positive facial expression,

and repeatedly made the “thumbs up” hand gesture with their right hand. Finally, for the

threatening action, actors displayed an angry facial expression, repeatedly making the “giving

someone the (middle) finger” hand gesture. Each actor was shown three times performing each

action for 10 seconds at a time. The videos were presented in randomized order, randomized for

both actor’s ethnicity and kind of action. Near the end of the session, participants performed the

same three actions themselves for 30 seconds.

5.4.4 Electrophysiological recording and processing

To measure mu-suppression, I recorded the EEG from 32 Ag/AgCl sintered electrodes

embedded in a stretch-lycra cap. Recording procedure and data processing were identical to the

procedures used in Gutsell and Inzlicht(2010) and Gutsell and Inzlicht (2012) described here in

chapters 2 and 3. I performed fast Fourier transformation and calculated power in the 8–13 Hz

band for 30 seconds for each ethnic group and 30 seconds for self-action and for baseline. I then

averaged the spectral data over the three outgroups.

5.5 Results

We calculated log-ratio scores of mu power during the observation of action and during

baseline (log mu-oscillations during action observation/ log mu-oscillations during baseline) for

Page 82: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

73

all conditions to control for differences in baseline mu-power. Consequently, more negative

scores indicate a decrease in mu-rhythm compared to baseline, and thus more cortical activity.

Since the resulting mu-suppression scores serve as an indicator of activation of neural sensory

motor areas, I expected to find mu-suppression during movement and observation of movement

only in the left hemisphere contralateral to movement of the right dominant hand. Thus, I

performed statistical analysis only for electrode C3 located over the contralateral, left primary

motor area.

Consistent with my hypotheses, and replicating the findings described in chapter 2

(Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010), I found mu-suppression scores significantly different from zero t (22)

= -2.10; p < .05 when participants looked at ingroup members performing neutral actions (M = -

.18, SD = .40), but not when participants looked at outgroup members performing neutral actions

(M = -.11, SD = .40) t (22) = -1.31; p = 20. Please see figure 8 for a depiction of the results.

Looking at the effects of intent I found significant mu-suppression in response to

threatening ingroup members (M = -.18; SD = .38) t (22) = -2.29, p <.05 and marginally

significant mu-suppression in response to ingroup members displaying the positive action (M = -

.16; SD = .40) t (22) = -1.91, p =.07. Thus, participants showed mu-suppression all three intent

conditions. Interestingly, I found a different pattern for the outgroup: Similar to the neutral

condition, I did not find significant mu-suppression in response to positive actions (M = -.13; SD

= .43) t (22) = -1.49, p =.15, however, I did find significant mu-suppression, t (22) = -2.18, p <

.05, in response to threatening outgroup actions (M = -18, SD = .40). Taken together these

findings suggest, that participants always showed mu-suppression indicative of motor resonance

with ingroup members, but they only resonated with the actions of outgroup members when

these actions where threatening.

Page 83: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

74

Figure 8: EEG mu-suppression at left central scalp location (C3) during the observation

of ingroup and outgroup members performing a neutral, positive and threatening action.

Error bars represent ±1 standard error. Negative numbers denote more mu-suppression

(i.e. more neural activity) at C3.

To further investigate the interplay of group membership and action intent, I conducted a 3

(action intent: neutral, positive, threatening) X 2 (Group: ingroup vs. outgroup) within subject

ANOVA. I found no significant main effects and no significant interactions (All p-values >.12).

Since, in the absence of any significant main effects and interactions, planned comparisons are

interpretable unambiguously (Tybout et al., 2001), I looked at the simple effects of action intent.

I found that only in the neutral condition did mu-suppression scores for the ingroup and the

outgroup significantly differ from each other F (1, 22) = 4.86, p < .05. Whereas, both, the

Page 84: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

75

positive F (1, 22) = .53, p = ns and the threat condition F (1, 22) = .01 p = ns did not show any

difference for ingroup and outgroup actions. These findings draw a picture that is a little different

from what we saw earlier. Again, the neutral condition replicates the findings from (Gutsell &

Inzlicht, 2010), showing an ingroup bias in mu-suppression, such that participant show mu-

suppression significantly less than they do with outgroup members. When the actions were

intentional and had functional significance (either positive or negative), people showed mu-

suppression in response to ingroup and outgroup members equally. These findings suggest that

increasing motivational relevance does seem to override group biases in motor resonance.

Further supporting the hypothesis that when it comes to ingroup actions, the positive or

negative intent of the action does not make a difference, I did not find significant differences

between any of the action intentions (all p-values > .44) – participants resonated equally with

neutral, positive and threatening ingroup actions. In contrast, for the outgroup condition I found a

marginally significant p = .07 difference in mu-suppression between the neutral and the threat

condition, suggesting that participants process threatening outgroup actions differently than

neutral outgroup actions4. Although we have to be careful when interpreting this marginally

significant effect, these findings are consistent with the pattern of the other findings in this study.

In sum the data on threatening outgroup behavior suggests that the ingroup bias in motor

resonance that is usually found, is no longer present when the outgroup’s actions are threatening.

4 As outlined in chapter 3, left alpha asymmetry is related to approach motivation and has been shown to increase

with anger (Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009; Harmon-Jones & Sigelman, 2001). Therefore, looking at angry outgroup

members could possibly have resulted to an increase in left-frontal alpha asymmetry. In theory such activation of the

left-prefrontal cortex could have spread to more parietal areas, potentially affecting the mu-rhythm. To investigate

alpha asymmetry as a possible confound, we calculated alpha asymmetry scores (log right alpha at F4 – log left

alpha at F3) for all conditions. A 2(group: ingroup vs. outgroup) X 2(intent: neutral vs. positive vs. threat) on alpha

asymmetry did not result in any main effects or interactions (all p-values > .16). In addition, controlling for threat

related alpha asymmetry did not change the pattern of results for mu-suppression, and alpha-asymmetry did not

correlate with the mu-suppression scores (r = -.14; p = .52).

Page 85: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

76

Threatening outgroup behavior elicits significant mu-suppression, participants’ mu-suppression

for threatening outgroup behavior was equally strong as for threatening ingroup behavior, and

finally it seems as if participants show stronger mu-suppression in response to threatening

outgroup actions than to neutral outgroup behavior.

My findings on positive outgroup behavior, however, are less conclusive. Positive

outgroup behavior does not elicit significant mu-suppression, and mu-suppression in response to

positive outgroup actions did not differ in strengths from mu-suppression in response to neutral

outgroup behavior, suggesting that participants did not resonate with outgroup behavior, being it

neutral or positive. However, I do find that participants display similar levels of mu-suppression

for positive ingroup and outgroup actions. This could suggest that positive intent can overcome

group biases in motor resonance. However, since mu-suppression in response to positive ingroup

behavior was only marginally different than zero, it could also mean that participants did not

resonate with the positive actions of either of the two groups. The results of this study, hence,

suggest that motivational relevance does override group biases in motor resonance and that threat

is more effective in doing so than is positivity.

5.6 Discussion

Research suggests that as a default, people do not resonate with the actions of outgroups

(Gutsell & Inzlicht, 2010). Motivational relevance, however, could potentially override these

group biases in motor resonance. Here I find that threatening actions performed by outgroup

members are resonated with, suggesting that threat motivates people to resonate with others,

even with those they would usually ignore. These findings are consistent with a functional

perspective on person perception and motor resonance. According to this perspective, people

Page 86: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

77

allocate their limited resources to those social targets who are relevant to their own goals, and

who might provide functionally significant information (Ackerman et al., 2006; Ohman et al.,

2001). Because, ingroup members are the ones we care about and who we interact most with

(Fiske, 1992), outgroup members are often ignored. This reasoning is supported by findings

showing that taking the perspective of an outgroup member can alleviate the group bias in motor

resonance – when perspective taking brings the outgroup closer to the self (Galinsky, Ku, &

Wang, 2005), perceivers will resonate with the actions of outgroup members (Chapter 3). It

might, then, seem paradoxical that threatening behavior can reduce group biases in motor

resonance since aggression and hostility should usually aggravate group biases and increase the

distance between groups. Shouldn’t seeing threatening behavior in outgroup members increase

the group bias in motor resonance, rather than reduce it?

A functional approach to motor resonance can account for this seeming contradiction:

When someone who we do not feel close to and who we dislike, aggresses against us, it is

particularly important for us to pay close attention and to resonate with him in order to make

sense of his actions. Hence, high levels of prejudice and aversive emotions towards the particular

outgroup member would, if anything, make their actions even more motivationally relevant.

Demonstrating this dissociation between evaluation and allocation of processing resources,

research shows that people dislike and stereotype outgroup members more when they are in

competition to them (Judd & Park, 1988) and when they display aggression (Ackerman et al.,

2006). At the same time, they show greater recognition accuracy for outgroup faces. Therefore

when one is in competition to the outgroup, or when the outgroup poses a potential threat, people

no longer show the Cross-Category effect (Hugenberg & Sacco, 2008) – the tendency for

recognition accuracy to be better for ingroup than for outgroup faces – consistently shown under

neutral conditions. Hence, my findings provide support for a functional approach to motor

Page 87: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

78

resonance in an intergroup context, suggesting that motivational relevance is the driving factor

behind group biases in motor resonance.

5.6.1 Threatening Outgroup Behavior and Stereotypes

As outlined above, humans are generally prone to pay attention to negative and

threatening information (Ito et al., 1998; Rozin & Royzman, 2001; Smith et al., 2003). In

addition to this general bias, social categorization and stereotypes might uniquely contribute to

the effects of threat in an intergroup context. People categorize others into groups in order to use

these group labels as source of information (Macrae & Bodenhausen, 2000). Once a person

categorizes someone as belonging to the outgroup, their perceptions of that person will be

strongly influenced by the stereotypes of that social category (Fiske & Neuberg, 1990). In the

present study, half of the actors in the outgroup category, were young Black men, while the other

half were young South-Asian Canadian men. At least for young Black men there is a wealth of

evidence suggesting that this category is associated with threat both implicitly (Hugenberg &

Bodenhausen, 2003; Maner et al., 2005; Trawalter, Todd, Baird, & Richeson, 2008) and

explicitly (Cottrell & Neuberg, 2005). This association between Black men and threat can also be

seen on the neural level. For example, the amygdala – a brain region responsive to threat (Davis,

2006) but also motivational significance (Cunningham et al., 2004), is selectively activated by

the sight of Black male targets compared to White male targets (Cunningham et al., 2004;

Wheeler & Fiske, 2005). Given how deeply ingrained these threat associations seem to be, they

might also have contributed to my current findings. Since, in addition to the more general

negativity bias, outgroup members, and Black’s in particular, are associated with threat,

threatening outgroup actions might be exceedingly motivationally relevant compared to positive

actions performed by outgroup members or threatening actions performed by ingroup members.

Page 88: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

79

5.6.2 Limitations and Future Directions

My findings suggest that threatening outgroup behavior is motivationally relevant enough

to be resonated with, while positive outgroup behavior is not. Theoretically it makes sense that

threat would be a powerful tool to increase motivational relevance of outgroup behavior because

of the negativity bias, and stereotypes associating the outgroup to threat. Consequently simply

matching the intensity of the negative and positive stimuli without also considering the effects of

group membership on stimulus perception might be insufficient. For my design I chose

approximately equally intense actions for the positive and threat conditions that is I matched

them independent of group membership, but the complexities of emotion perception in

intergroup settings might call for a different approach. In a series of studies, Weisbuch and

Ambady (2008) showed that participants react with a negative emotional response such as fear to

joy expressions of outgroup members, and with positive emotions to fear expressions of

outgroup members, potentially because happiness of outgroup members may signal a

disadvantage for the ingroup, while fear signals an advantage. These findings suggest that group

membership alters the signal value of emotional displays (Weisbuch & Ambady, 2008). Hence, I

cannot be sure how participants perceived the positive actions of outgroup members, which

makes conclusions about the effects that positive outgroup actions can potentially have on motor

resonance difficult. More research is needed on motor resonance for positive outgroup actions.

5.7 Conclusions

I presented research showing that motor resonance is limited to the ingroup, but it is

available for outgroup behavior when it has motivational significance for the observer. Who we

resonate with, thus, is not fixed but rather, can change with the context. Depending on the

Page 89: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

80

situation the actions and experiences of outgroup members might be equally or even more

motivationally relevant than those of ingroup members. In such cases, people should resonate

with outgroup members who they would otherwise ignore.

Page 90: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

81

6 General Discussion and Conclusions

Understanding other’s action intentions and intuitively picking up on other’s inner states,

is a central human capacity. One important underlying mechanism is motor resonance – the

activation of the observer’s motor system during observation. Essentially, by mapping the other’s

actions and facial expressions onto their own motor system, observers gain an intuitive and

richer representation of what it is like to perform the action and to express the emotion, because

this representation includes not only the action pattern, but also information about the

proprioceptive states, the action tendencies, and the associated emotional and motivational states

(Iacoboni, 2005; Keysers et al., 2010).

The four studies described here investigate the dynamics of motor resonance in an

intergroup context. They provide insight into how motivational relevance can limit the amount of

motor resonance in response to social outgroup members and under which circumstances

motivational relevance, and with it motor resonance for outgroup members, can be increased.

Specifically, study one shows that people resonate with observed ingroup actions, but not with

outgroup actions, and this group bias in motor resonance is aggravated with increasing levels of

prejudice. Study 2 examined a potential consequence of such a lack in motor resonance for the

outgroup: reduced affect sharing. Participants showed EEG frontal asymmetry patterns

associated with aversive motivational states in response to videos of ingroup members displaying

the experience of sadness, but not when seeing sad outgroup members.

Page 91: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

82

After having established an ingroup bias in motor resonance and affective sharing in

study one and two respectively, I set out to investigate what variables might facilitate motor

resonance. In study 3 I found that having participants take the perspective of an outgroup

member can increase motor resonance in response to other outgroup members’ actions. At the

same time, study four suggested that when outgroup behavior is threatening, people do resonate

with outgroup members. Together, the findings of these latter two studies suggest that

motivational relevance plays an important role in the ingroup biases in motor resonance: When I

increase the motivational relevance of outgroup members by bringing the outgroup closer to the

self through cognitive perspective taking, participants resonate with outgroup members.

Demonstrating the same underlying mechanism, I also find that when I increase the motivational

relevance of outgroup members’ actions by making the action threatening, people again start to

resonate with outgroup members.

In sum, people tend to resonate with actions that have functional significance. Most

commonly, these are the actions of ingroup members. Consequently, people often ignore the

actions of outgroup members, depriving intergroup interactions of the potential many benefits of

motor resonance, and possibly hampering smooth communication and cooperation between

members of different groups. The good news is that there are a multitude of potential reasons for

why outgroup actions can become motivationally relevant leading to motor resonance across

group boundaries.

6.1 Implications of a lack in motor resonance:

Since by default people do not seem to resonate with outgroup members, it is important

to understand the potential implications. Thus far, to my knowledge, no research has directly

Page 92: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

83

tested the consequences of group biases in motor resonance, and only a few studies exist that

look at the consequences of a lack in motor resonance in general. However, there are some

findings and theoretical accounts that can provide clues.

Basic research on the function of mirror neurons can give some indication. Above I

described in detail evidence that suggests mirror neurons’ involvement in action understanding.

To summarize, mirror neurons are sensory motor neurons with the acquired property to respond

to the sight of motor acts in addition to the actual performance of the motor act the specific

mirror neuron is coding (Keysers & Perrett, 2004). Because mirror neurons are essentially motor

neurons fully integrated in the sensory motor system and with connections to visual areas,

activation of mirror neurons during observation could potentially provide insight into the internal

states that are associated with the action (Schippers & Keysers, 2011), including action

tendencies, basic intentions, and goals, as well as the sense of movement, the feeling of touch,

and object properties (Keysers et al., 2010). For example, people can usually estimate the weight

of an object simply by observing someone else lifting it, presumably by using the other’s

movement patterns as indicators for how heavy the object is. Interestingly, when research

participants’ somatosensory cortex is stimulated with disruptive TMS, leading to decreased

sensitivity of sensory motor neurons, participants weight estimation ability decreases (Keysers,

2012). This finding suggests that mapping the other’s actions on one’s own motor system

facilitates an intuitive understanding of what it means to perform the action.

In addition to action understanding mirror neurons might be involved in synchrony,

mimicry, and joint actions, three related but different processes. Synchrony is the coupling of

interacting individuals’ rhythmic movements, such as when the clapping of audiences slowly

turns into a synchronized rhythm, or when research participants subconsciously start

Page 93: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

84

synchronizing their rocking in rocking chairs (Richardson, Marsh, Isenhower, Goodman, &

Schmidt, 2007). Synchrony has been associated with feelings of rapport (Bernieri, Davis, Robert,

& Knee, 1994) and affiliation (Hove & Risen, 2009), and has been shown to promote

cooperation (Valdesolo, Ouyang, & DeSteno, 2010). Mimicry, on the other hand, is the

subconscious and automatic imitation of another’s motor movements, postures and mannerisms

(Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Hence mimicry differs from synchrony in that it involves more

complex behavior, not being limited to rhythmic movement, and that it is a one sided process

during which one individual imitates another individual, rather than a bidirectional synchronizing

between two individuals. Mimicry has been associated with increased liking (Chartrand &

Bargh, 1999) and feelings of closeness (van Baaren, Holland, Kawakami, & van Knippenberg,

2004).

Finally, several processes have to come together for joint action. Joint action requires

people to coordinate their actions with others to reach a common goal (Sebanz, Bekkering, &

Knoblich, 2006). To this end, people first have to direct their attention to the same events. Then,

they must form representations of the other’s actions, goals, and intentions in order to predict the

outcomes of those actions, as well as what the other person is going to do next. Finally, people

have to integrate the other’s actions into their own action planning (Knoblich & Sebanz, 2006).

It easily becomes apparent, that synchrony, mimicry and joint action all have one

important aspect in common: They require the co-representation of the other’s action. It does, of

course, make intuitive sense that motor resonance is the common underlying process. Several

researchers have suggested just that (Iacoboni, 2009; Knoblich & Sebanz, 2006; Obhi & Sebanz,

2011), and there is also initial empirical evidence in support for a causal contribution of motor

resonance to action co-representation and imitation. Specifically, two studies found that when

Page 94: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

85

disruptive rTMS is administered to mirror neuron areas, people show reduced accuracy in an

intentional imitation task (Catmur, Walsh, & Heyes, 2009; Heiser, Iacoboni, Maeda, Marcus, &

Mazziotta, 2003).

One final consequence of a lack in motor resonance could be reduced empathy. People do

not only map others’ body movements onto their own motor cortex, but also others’ facial

expressions, which in turn should lead to a co-representation of others’ facial expression along

with the associated internal states including the respective emotion. By showing that participants

do not display matching prefrontal – asymmetry patterns when observing outgroup members

experiencing sadness, my findings support the notion that biases in motor resonance could

indeed lead to biases in affect sharing. Moreover, recent findings suggest that the somatosensory

cortices are also involved in the mirroring process. These findings show that when people

witness the sensations, actions and somatic pain of others, they consistently experience matching

activation in their own somatosensory cortices (Keysers et al., 2010). In addition, single cell

recordings in monkeys have found mirror neurons in the somatosensory cortex (Ishida,

Nakajima, Inase, & Murata, 2010). Thus, a lack in motor resonance could be associated with a

lack in somatosensory resonance, leading to reduced sharing of the other’s sensations and pain.

Indeed, Xu and colleagues (2009) found that participants did show matching activation in the

neural areas usually active during the experience of pain when looking at videos of outgroup

members’ hands getting pricked with a needle, but they did not show indication of such vicarious

experience of pain when looking at outgroup members receiving painful needle pricks.

The degree to which synchrony, mimicry, joint action and empathy are actually

dependent on motor resonance and whether motor resonance is necessary for these phenomena

remains an open question (for a detailed discussion see Gallese et al., 2011). However, if the

Page 95: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

86

answer to this question is even only partially “yes”, then the group biases in motor resonance

observed here could have profound consequences to how we interact and cooperate with, and to

how we affiliate with and feel about others who are not part of the same social group.

6.2 Malleable motor resonance

The current work not only has implications for intergroup interactions and perception, but

also contributes to our knowledge of motor resonance. Motor resonance is a very basic

mechanism and has often be shown to happen automatically, without much influence from top-

down processes (e.g. Chong, & Mattingley, 2009; Fadiga et al., 1995; Iacoboni, 2005; van Schie

et al., 2008). However, relatively few processes are completely independent from top-down

information. Nonetheless, only a few studies have investigated the nature of top-down

influences on motor resonance. For instance, motor expertise and familiarity with the action

seem to influence motor resonance. Compared to novices, professional basketball players show

more motor resonance when watching other players play, and this increase predicts accuracy of

their evaluation of free shots (Aglioti, Cesari, Romani, & Urgesi, 2008). Similarly, research

participants show more motor resonance in tasks that involve unfamiliar rather than familiar

actions (Calvo-Merino et al., 2005). Further, context seems to be another factor that can

influence motor resonance. For example Iacoboni and colleagues (2005) found that when

participants were instructed to infer the intention of actions they showed more motor resonance,

than when they were instructed to observe the same actions passively. Likewise, research

participants show less motor resonance in non-interactive situations than interactive situations

(Oberman et al., 2008) and in situations that require complementary responses rather than non-

complementary ones (Sartori et al., 2011).

Page 96: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

87

In sum, we have evidence that mirror neuron activity and motor resonance are not

entirely driven by bottom up processes. Instead, they are susceptible to relatively basic, top-

down influence such as experience and context. Yet, the extent to which motor resonance can be

influenced by more abstract and complex factors such as attitudes or mindsets is still relatively

unexplored. To my knowledge modulation of the neural mirroring has, thus far, only been

investigated in the domain of empathy for pain (e.g. Hein et al., 2010; Singer et al., 2006; Xu et

al., 2009), and by looking at behavioral phenomena related to motor resonance, such as mental

co-representation of actions (Müller et al., 2011) and mimicry (Leighton, Bird, Orsini, & Heyes,

2010; Likowski et al., 2008). To my knowledge only one study has directly investigated motor

resonance using neuroimaging and shown that more complex mental states, in this case self-

construal, can influence motor resonance (Hogeveen & Obhi, 2011). Hence, the research

presented here can make an important contribution to the understanding of motor resonance. I

show that not only is motor resonance influenced by social categorization, but it can also be

influenced by culturally learned prejudice and by temporarily induced mindsets. After all, motor

resonance, like many other basic perception processes before, has turned out not to be as

automatic as previously thought to be, but instead turned out to be susceptible to the multifaceted

and rich world of higher order cognitive processes that constitute human social cognition.

6.3 Limitations and future directions

There are still several open questions regarding motor resonance and the influences that

group categorization, prejudice and motivational relevance exert on it. One important extension

of my research would be to replicate the effects with different groups. I chose ethnicity as the

defining factor of group membership, but of course there are multitudes of potential other group

memberships. In fact, any minor difference between people can be enough to elicit social

Page 97: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

88

categorization and the division into ingroup and outgroup (Diehl, 1990). Ethnicity is often tightly

connected with prejudice, and I showed that prejudice can affected motor resonance (Gutsell &

Inzlicht, 2010). Specifically, I showed that prejudice is negatively associated with motor

resonance for the outgroup, such that more prejudiced people resonate even less with outgroup

members, and the amount of motor resonance is reduced for outgroups that are disliked in

society. It would be important to see how motor resonance changes in respect to group

membership when the group in question is not a marginalized group, such as when the minimal

group paradigm is used. Moreover, the specific nature of the stereotypes associated with the

outgroup might also influence motor resonance and the effects of perspective taking and threat

on motor resonance. According to the stereotype content model (Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, & Xu,

2002), stereotypes can be described by two dimensions – warmth and competence. These

dimensions are crucial, because when people meet others, two concerns are of primary

importance: whether the other has well-meaning or hostile goals related to the self or the ingroup

(warmth), and whether they have the capability to pursue those goals (competence). Only the

ingroup and close allies score high on both, warmth and competence. Outgroups can either be

high in competence and low in warmth such as Black professionals, businesswomen, Jews and

Asians, they could be low in competence and high in warmth, such as housewives, and the

elderly, or they could be low on both, such as homeless people or welfare recipients. It seems

likely that stereotype content could influence motor resonance. For example, people show less

neural activity in brain areas usually involved in processing of social targets, when they look at

outgroup members who are stereotyped as low in warmth and low in competence (Harris &

Fiske, 2006). Since people low in competence, are less likely be motivationally relevant, motor

resonance for outgroups might depend on whether they are perceived as high in competence or

not. Similarly, the effects of threat might work differently for different outgroups, such that it

Page 98: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

89

would only effectively increase motor resonance when the aggressor is also seen as competent to

actually follow through with the threat. People’s social world is complex and future studies on

intergroup motor resonance should take this complexity into account.

An important question that requires more investigation is the effect that motivational

relevance has on motor resonance. Thus far we’ve investigated two very distinct factors, one

related to prosocial behavior and affiliation, the other one related to aggression and threat. I

chose these two manipulations specifically because they differed on these two dimensions, but

because they are so different, each might have unique effects on motor resonance. For example,

threat might be an especially powerful motivator because of the negativity bias, but it might also

operate differently depending on the specific group membership of the target and depending on

context. Similarly, we know that perspective taking exerts its beneficial effects through an

increase in self-other overlap (Galinsky et al., 2005), but we do not yet fully understand how

exactly closeness and affiliation are related to motivational relevance. Hence, it would be

important to look at motivational relevance in its pure form without possible confounds.

Incentives or variations in task relevance of other’s actions might, for instance, be a way to

subtly and in small increments vary motivational relevance directly.

Finally, another extension of my work would be to look at motor resonance during real

interactions. All four studies presented here used videos. Although the use of videos ensured a

controlled environment, making it easy to exactly match changes in motor resonance with

specific events during stimulus presentation, they remain only a simulation of real interpersonal

contact. One cannot interact with a person in a video, but motor resonance is a social process,

involved in action preparation and potentially joint action (Knoblich & Sebanz, 2006). Hence, it

would be important to conduct studies that measure motor resonance during observation of real

Page 99: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

90

interaction partners, giving participants the opportunity to react to the target’s actions, or to

coordinate their actions with them. Such research might potentially lead to results very different

from those that can be obtained using videos, both in terms of motor resonance in response to

outgroups, but also on the effects of perspective taking and threat. Real interactions would also

enable us to look at both the observer and the target. What is the experience of not being

resonated with like? Although the opportunities for meaningful and complex interactions in the

laboratory, especially when measuring brain activity, are limited by the requirements of the

measures, moving towards a more natural social experience holds many promises for future

research.

6.4 Conclusions

We presented research suggesting that, as a default, people do not resonate with ethnic

outgroup members, nor do they share their affective and motivational states. However, once an

outgroup member becomes motivational relevant, either because the observer is in a perspective

taking mindset, or because the outgroup member’s behavior poses a potential threat, people start

resonating, thereby becoming more sensitive to the inner states of outgroup members.

My findings advance current theoretical understanding of cross-group interactions

showing that a basic process underling action perception and emotional contagion might be

limited to the ingroup and will only be available for the outgroup in certain circumstances in

which motivational relevance is salient. In addition, my findings contribute to the understanding

of motor resonance by shedding light on the factors that facilitate and hamper motor resonance.

Additionally, these findings have practical implications: with the rise in cultural and

ethnic diversity in societies all around the world, cross-group encounters will become

Page 100: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

91

increasingly common, and will be part of most people’s everyday life. Better understanding the

dynamic processes underlying and affecting an intuitive understanding of others, can gain

important knowledge that might help to develop strategies to improve cross-group encounters.

Page 101: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

92

References

Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Griskevicius, V.,

Maner, J. K., et al. (2006). They All Look the Same to Me (Unless They’re Angry): From

Out-Group Homogeneity to Out-Group Heterogeneity. Psychological Science, 17(10), 836–

840. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01790.x

Aglioti, S. M., Cesari, P., Romani, M., & Urgesi, C. (2008). Action anticipation and motor

resonance in elite basketball players. Nature neuroscience, 11(9), 1109–1116.

doi:10.1038/nn.2182

Allen, J. J., Iacono, W. G., Depue, R. A., & Arbisi, P. (1993). Regional electroencephalographic

asymmetries in bipolar seasonal affective disorder before and after exposure to bright light.

Biological Psychiatry, 33(8-9), 642–646. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(93)90104-L

Atkinson, A. P. (2002). Emotion-specific clues to the neural substrate of empathy. Behavioral

and Brain Sciences, 25(01), 22–23. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X02240017

Avenanti, A., Sirigu, A., & Aglioti, S. M. (2010). Racial bias reduces empathic sensorimotor

resonance with other-race pain. Current biology, 20(11), 1018–22.

doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.071

Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The Empathy Quotient: An Investigation of Adults

with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism, and Normal Sex Differences.

Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 163–175.

doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00

Baron-cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “‘ Reading the

Mind in the Eyes ’” Test Revised Version: A Study with Normal Adults , and Adults with

Asperger Syndrome or High-functioning Autism. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 42(2), 241–251. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00715

Batson, C. D., & Moran, T. (1999). Empathy-induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma.

European Journal of Social Psychology, 29(7), 909–924. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-

0992(199911)29:7<909::AID-EJSP965>3.0.CO;2-L

Page 102: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

93

Batson, C. D., Sager, K., Garst, E., Kang, M., Rubchinsky, K., & Dawson, K. (1997). Is

empathy-induced helping due to self–other merging? Journal of personality and social

psychology, 73(3), 495–509. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.495

Batson, C. D., & Shaw, L. L. (1991). Evidence for Altruism, Toward a Pluralism of Prosocial

Motives. Psychological inquiry, 2(2), 107–122.

Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than

good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323–370. doi:10.1037//1089-2680.5.4.323

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The Need to Belong: Desire for Interpersonal

Attachments as a Fundamental Human Motivation. Psychological bulletin, 117(3), 497–

529.

Bernieri, F. J., Davis, J. M., Robert, R., & Knee, C. R. (1994). Interactional Synchrony and

Rapport: Measuring Synchrony in Displays Devoid of Sound and Facial Affect. Personality

and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(3), 303–311.

Bernstein, M. J., Young, S. G., & Hugenberg, K. (2007). The Cross-Category Effect, 18(8), 706–

712.

Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Fine cuts of empathy and the amygdala: dissociable deficits in

psychopathy and autism. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006), 61(1), 157–

70. doi:10.1080/17470210701508855

Blatt, B., LeLacheur, S. F., Galinsky, A. D., Simmens, S. J., & Greenberg, L. (2010). Does

perspective-taking increase patient satisfaction in medical encounters? Academic Medicine,

85(9), 1445–1452. doi:10.1097/ACM.0b013e3181eae5ec

Bodenhausen, G. V. (1988). Stereotypic biases in social decision making and memory: Testing

process models of stereotype use. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(5),

726–737. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.55.5.726

Bonini, L., Rozzi, S., Serventi, F. U., Simone, L., Ferrari, P. F., & Fogassi, L. (2010). Ventral

premotor and inferior parietal cortices make distinct contribution to action organization and

intention understanding. Cerebral Cortex, 20(6), 1372–1385. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhp200

Page 103: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

94

Buccino, G., Lui, F., Canessa, N., Patteri, I., Lagravinese, G., Benuzzi, F., Porro, C. a, et al.

(2004). Neural circuits involved in the recognition of actions performed by nonconspecifics:

an FMRI study. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 16(1), 114–26.

doi:10.1162/089892904322755601

Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action

Observation and Acquired Motor Skills: An fMRI Study with Expert Dancers. Cerebral

Cortex, 15(8), 1243–1249. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi007

Caporael, L. R. (1997). The Evolution of Truly Social Cognition : The Core Configurations

Model, 1(4), 276–298.

Carr, L., Iacoboni, M., Dubeau, M.-C., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. L. (2003). Neural

mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic

areas. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,

100(9), 5497–502. doi:10.1073/pnas.0935845100

Carruthers, P. (2009). How we know our own minds: the relationship between mindreading and

metacognition. The Behavioral and brain sciences, 32(2), 121–38; discussion 138–82.

doi:10.1017/S0140525X09000545

Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and

implications. Psychological bulletin, 135(2), 183–204. doi:10.1037/a0013965

Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2009). Associative sequence learning: the role of

experience in the development of imitation and the mirror system. Philosophical

transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences, 364(1528),

2369–80. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0048

Cattaneo, L., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., & Rizzolatti, G. (2009). Representation of goal and

movements without overt motor behavior in the human motor cortex: A transcranial

magnetic stimulation study. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(36), 11134–11138.

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2605-09.2009

Page 104: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

95

Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: the perception-behavior link and

social interaction. Journal of personality and social psychology, 76(6), 893–910.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893

Cheng, Y.-W., Tzeng, O. J. L., Decety, J., lmada, T., & Hsieh, J.-C. (2006). Gender differences

in the human mirror system: A magnetoencephalography study. NeuroReport: For Rapid

Communication of Neuroscience Research, 17(11), 1115–1119.

doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000223393.59328.21

Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Oby, E. R., Li, Z., Parrish, T., & Bridge, D. J. (2009). Neural

representations of social status hierarchy in human inferior parietal cortex.

Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 354–63. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.023

Chiao, J. Y., & Mathur, V. a. (2010). Intergroup empathy: how does race affect empathic neural

responses? Current biology, 20(11), R478–80. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.001

Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the

empathy–altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. Journal of personality

and social psychology, 73(3), 481–494. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.481

Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Kurzban, R. (2003). Perceptions of race. Trends in Cognitive

Sciences, 7(4), 173–179. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00057-3

Cottrell, C. a, & Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Different emotional reactions to different groups: a

sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice”. Journal of personality and social

psychology, 88(5), 770–89. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.770

Cunningham, W. A., Bavel, J. J. V., & Johnsen, I. R. (2008). Affective Flexibility Evaluative

Processing Goals Shape Amygdala Activity, 19(2), 152–160.

Cunningham, W. A, Johnson, M. K., Raye, C. L., Chris Gatenby, J., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R.

(2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces.

Psychological science, 15(12), 806–13. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00760.x

Page 105: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

96

Davidson, R. J. (2004). What does the prefrontal cortex “do” in affect: perspectives on frontal

EEG asymmetry research. Biological psychology, 67(1-2), 219–33.

doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.03.008

Davidson, R. J., Ekman, P., Saron, C. D., Senulis, J. a., & Friesen, W. V. (1990). Approach-

withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology: I. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(2), 330–341. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.2.330

Davidson, R. J., & Tomarken, A. J. (1989). Laterality and emotion: an electrophysiolocial

approach. In B. F & G. J (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology (pp. 419–441). Amsterdam:

Elsevier.

Davis, M. (2006). Neural systems involved in fear and anxiety measured with fear-potentiated

startle. The American psychologist, 61(8), 741–56. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.8.741

Decety, J. (2010). To What Extent is the Experience of Empathy Mediated by Shared Neural

Circuits? Emotion Review, 2(3), 204–207. doi:10.1177/1754073910361981

Decety, Jean, & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy.

Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews, 3(2), 71–100.

doi:10.1177/1534582304267187

Desmurget, M., Reilly, K. T., Richard, N., Szathmari, A., Mottolese, C., & Sirigu, A. (2009).

Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science (New York, N.Y.),

324(5928), 811–3. doi:10.1126/science.1169896

Diehl, M. (1990). The Minimal Group Paradigm : Theoretical Explanations and Empirical

Findings. European Review of Social Psychology, 1(1), 37–41.

doi:10.1080/14792779108401864

Dovidio, J. F., ten Vergert, M., Stewart, T. L., Gaertner, S. L., Johnson, J. D., Esses, V. M., Riek,

B. M., et al. (2004). Perspective and Prejudice: Antecedents and Mediating Mechanisms.

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(12), 1537–1549.

doi:10.1177/0146167204271177

Page 106: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

97

Downing, P. E., Bray, D., Rogers, J., & Childs, C. (2004). Bodies capture attention when nothing

is expected. Cognition, 93(1), B27–38. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.010

Drwecki, B. B., Moore, C. F., Ward, S. E., & Prkachin, K. M. (2011). Reducing racial disparities

in pain treatment: The role of empathy and perspective-taking. Pain, 152(5), 1001–1006.

doi:10.1016/j.pain.2010.12.005

Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002a). On the universality and cultural specificity of emotion

recognition: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 203–235. doi:10.1037//0033-

2909.128.2.203

Elfenbein, H. A., & Ambady, N. (2002b). Is there an in-group advantage in emotion recognition?

Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 243–249. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.128.2.243

Erlhagen, W., Mukovskiy, A., & Bicho, E. (2006). A dynamic model for action understanding

and goal-directed imitation. Brain research, 1083(1), 174–88.

doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.114

Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Pavesi, G., & Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Motor facilitation during action

observation: A magnetic stimulation study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 73(6), 2608–2611.

Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of

social relations. Psychological review, 99(4), 689–723. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.689

Fiske, S. T. (1980). Attention and weight in person perception: The impact of negative and

extreme behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(6), 889–906.

doi:10.1037//0022-3514.38.6.889

Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype

content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and

competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878–902.

doi:10.1037//0022-3514.82.6.878

Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal

Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding. Science, 308(5722), 662–667.

doi:10.1126/science.1106138

Page 107: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

98

Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of

attention. Psychological Science, 19(5), 476–482. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02112.x

Galinsky, A. D., & Ku, G. (2004). The Effects of Perspective-Taking on Prejudice: The

Moderating Role of Self-Evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(5),

594–604. doi:10.1177/0146167203262802

Galinsky, A. D., Ku, G., & Wang, C. S. (2005). Perspective-Taking and Self-Other Overlap:

Fostering Social Bonds and Facilitating Social Coordination. Group Processes &

Intergroup Relations, 8(2), 109–124. doi:10.1177/1368430205051060

Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Perspective-taking: decreasing stereotype

expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. Journal of personality and

social psychology, 78(4), 708–24. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.708

Galinsky, A. D., Wang, C. S., & Ku, G. (2008). Perspective-takers behave more stereotypically.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(2), 404–419. doi:10.1037/0022-

3514.95.2.404

Gallese, V., Gernsbacher, M. a., Heyes, C., Hickok, G., & Iacoboni, M. (2011). Mirror Neuron

Forum. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(4), 369–407.

doi:10.1177/1745691611413392

Glenberg, a. M. (2011). Positions in the Mirror Are Closer Than They Appear. Perspectives on

Psychological Science, 6(4), 408–410. doi:10.1177/1745691611413393

Graziano, W. G., Bruce, J., Sheese, B. E., & Tobin, R. M. (2007). Attraction, personality, and

prejudice: liking none of the people most of the time. Journal of personality and social

psychology, 93(4), 565–82. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.4.565

Gutsell, J. N., & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental

simulation of actions during observation of outgroups. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 46(5), 841–845. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.011

Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, S. J. (1996). Pereceiving Persons and Groups. Psychological

review, 103(2), 336–355.

Page 108: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

99

Hamilton, W. D. (1994). The Genetical Evolution of Social Behavior. II. Journal of Theoretical

Biology, 7, 17–52.

Hansen, C., & Hansen, R. (1988). Finding the Face in the Crowd: An Anger Superiority Effect.

Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(6), 917–924.

Hari, R. (2006). Action-perception connection and the cortical mu rhythm. Progress in brain

research, 159(06), 253–60. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(06)59017-X

Harmon-Jones, E. (2003). Clarifying the emotive functions of asymmetrical frontal cortical

activity. Psychophysiology, 40(6), 838–848. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.00121

Harmon-Jones, E. (2006). Unilateral right-hand contractions cause contralateral alpha power

suppression and approach motivational affective experience. Psychophysiology, 43(6), 598–

603. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00465.x

Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. a, & Peterson, C. K. (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical

activity in emotion-related phenomena: a review and update. Biological psychology, 84(3),

451–62. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.08.010

Harmon-Jones, E., & Sigelman, J. (2001). State anger and prefrontal brain activity: Evidence that

insult-related relative left-prefrontal activation is associated with experienced anger and

aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(5), 797–803. doi:

10.1037/0022-3514.80.5.797

Harris, L. T., & Fiske, S. T. (2006). Dehumanizing the Lowest of the Low: Neuroimaging

Responses to Extreme Out-Groups. Psychological Science, 17(10), 847–853.

doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01793.x

Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories.

British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(1), 113–127. doi:10.1348/014466600164363

Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge

University Press.

Page 109: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

100

Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C. D., & Singer, T. (2010). Neural responses to

ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping.

Neuron, 68(1), 149–60. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003

Hein, G., & Singer, T. (2008). I feel how you feel but not always: the empathic brain and its

modulation. Current opinion in neurobiology, 18(2), 153–8.

doi:10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.012

Heiser, M., Iacoboni, M., Maeda, F., Marcus, J., & Mazziotta, J. C. (2003). The essential role of

Broca’s area in imitation. European Journal of Neuroscience, 17(5), 1123–1128.

doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02530.x

Henry, P. J., & Sears, D. O. (2002). The Symbolic Racism 2000 Scale. Political Psychology,

23(2), 253–283. doi:10.1111/0162-895X.00281

Hickok, G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in

monkeys and humans. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 21(7), 1229–43.

doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21189

Hirsh, J. B., & Inzlicht, M. (2008). The devil you know: Neuroticism predicts neural response to

uncertainty. Psychological Science, 19(10), 962–967. doi:10.1111/j.1467-

9280.2008.02183.x

Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for caring and justice.

New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Hogeveen, J., & Obhi, S. S. (2011). Altogether now: Activating interdependent self-construal

induces hypermotor resonance. Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(2), 74–82.

doi:10.1080/17588928.2010.533164

Hooker, C. I., Verosky, S. C., Germine, L. T., Knight, R. T., & D’Esposito, M. (2010). Neural

activity during social signal perception correlates with self-reported empathy. Brain

research, 1308, 100–13. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.10.006

Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases

affiliation. Social Cognition, 27(6), 949–961. doi:10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

Page 110: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

101

Hugenberg, K., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2003). Facing prejudice: implicit prejudice and the

perception of facial threat. Psychological science, 14(6), 640–3.

Hugenberg, K., & Corneille, O. (2009). Holistic processing is tuned for in-group faces. Cognitive

science, 33(6), 1173–81. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2009.01048.x

Hugenberg, K., & Sacco, D. F. (2008). Social Categorization and Stereotyping: How Social

Categorization Biases Person Perception and Face Memory. Social and Personality

Psychology Compass, 2(2), 1052–1072. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00090.x

Hurley, S., Shared, T., Model, C., Control, H., & Enable, S. C. (2008). Editorial Note: The

shared circuits model ( SCM ): How control , mirroring , and simulation can enable

imitation , deliberation , and mindreading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–58.

doi:10.1017/S0140525X07003123

Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding Others: Imitation, Language, and Empathy. In S. Hurley &

N. Chater (Eds.), (pp. 77–99). Cambridge, MA, US: MIT Press. Iacoboni, M. (2009).

Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(0066-4308,

0066-4308), 653–670. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604

Ito, T. A., Larsen, J. T., Smith, N. K., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1998). Negative Information Weighs

More Heavily on the Brain : The Negativity Bias in Evaluative Categorizations, 75, 887–

900.

Izard, C. E. (1990). Facial Expressions and the Regulation of Emotions. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 58(3), 487–498. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.3.487

Jacobs, A., Pinto, J., & Shiffrar, M. (2004). Experience, context, and the visual perception of

human movement. Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and

performance, 30(5), 822–35. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.30.5.822

Johansson, G. (1976). Spatio-Temporal Differentiation and Integration in Visual Motion

Perception. Psychological Research, 1976(38), 379–393.

Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns ’ preferential tracking

of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19.

Page 111: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

102

Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1988). Out-Group Homogeneity: Judgments of Variability at the

Individual and Group Levels. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54(5), 778–

788.

Kalin, R., & Berry, J. W. (1996). Interethnic attitudes in Canada: Ethnocentrism, consensual

hierarchy and reciprocity. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 28(4), 253–261.

doi:doi: 10.1037/0008-400X.28.4.253

Kanwisher, N., Mcdermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The Fusiform Face Area: A Module in

Human Extrastriate Cortex Specialized for Face Perception, 17(11), 4302–4311.

Keysers, C., Kaas, J. H., & Gazzola, V. (2010). Somatosensation in social perception. Nature

reviews. Neuroscience, 11(6), 417–28. doi:10.1038/nrn2833

Keysers, C., & Perrett, D. I. (2004). Demystifying social cognition: A Hebbian perspective.

Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(11), 501–507. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.09.005

Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2006). The Social Nature of Perception and Action. Current

Directions in Psychological Science, 15(3), 99–104. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2006.00415.x

Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing

sounds, understanding actions: action representation in mirror neurons. Science (New York,

N.Y.), 297(5582), 846–8. doi:10.1126/science.1070311

Kuhlman, W. N. (1978). Functional topography of the human mu rhythm.

Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology, 44(1), 83–93. doi:10.1016/0013-

4694(78)90107-4

Kunstman, J. W., & Plant, E. A. (2008). Racing to help: Racial bias in high emergency helping

situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 1499–1510.

doi:10.1037/a0012822

Laird, J. D. (1984). The real role of facial response in the experience of emotion: A reply to

Tourangeau and Ellsworth, and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

47(4), 909–917. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.47.4.909

Page 112: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

103

Lavie, N., Ro, T., & Russell, C. (2003). The role of perceptual load in processing distractor

faces. Psychological science, 14(5), 510–515.

Lawrence, E. J., Shaw, P., Baker, D., Baron-Cohen, S., & David, A. S. (2004). Measuring

empathy: reliability and validity of the Empathy Quotient. Psychological Medicine, 34,

911–924. doi:10.1017/S0033291703001624

Leighton, J., Bird, G., Orsini, C., & Heyes, C. (2010). Social attitudes modulate automatic

imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(6), 905–910.

doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.07.001

Levenson, R. W., & Ruef, A. M. (1992). Empathy: A Physiological Substrate. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 63(2), 234–246. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.234

Likowski, K. U., Mühlberger, A., Seibt, B., Pauli, P., & Weyers, P. (2008). Modulation of facial

mimicry by attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(4), 1065–1072.

doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.10.007

MacLin, Otto, H., & Malpass, R. S. (2001). Racial Categorization of Faces The Ambiguous Race

Face effect. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 98–118.

Macrae, C N, & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2000). Social cognition: thinking categorically about

others. Annual review of psychology, 51, 93–120. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.93

Macrae, C. N. & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2001). Social cognition : Categorical person perception.

British Journal of Psychology, 92, 239–255.

Macrae, C. N., Milne, A. B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (1994). Stereotypes as energy-saving

devices: A peek inside the cognitive toolbox. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,

66(1), 37–47. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.66.1.37

Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Robertson, T. E., Hofer, B., Neuberg, S. L., Delton,

A. W., et al. (2005). Functional projection: how fundamental social motives can bias

interpersonal perception. Journal of personality and social psychology, 88(1), 63–78.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.1.63

Page 113: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

104

Mathur, V. A., Harada, T., Lipke, T., & Chiao, J. Y. (2010). Neural basis of extraordinary

empathy and altruistic motivation. NeuroImage, 51(4), 1468–1475.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.025

Michel, C., Rossion, B., Han, J., Chung, C.-S., & Caldara, R. (2006). Holistic Processing Is

Finely Tuned for Faces of One ’ s Own Race. Psychological science, 17(7), 608–616.

Miller, K. I., Stiff, J. B., & Ellis, B. H. (1988). Communication and empathy as precursors to

burnout among human service workers. Communication Monographs, 55(3), 250–265.

doi:10.1080/03637758809376171

Molnar-Szakacs, I., Wu, A. D., Robles, F. J., & Iacoboni, M. (2007). Do you see what I mean?

Corticospinal excitability during observation of culture-specific gestures. PloS one, 2(7),

e626. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000626

Moskowitz, G. B., & Kirk, E. R. (2004). The implicit volition model: On the preconscious

regulation of temporarily adopted goals. Advances, 36, 317–413.

Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, J., Iacoboni, M., & Fried, I. (2010). Single-neuron

responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology, 20(8),

750–6. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045

Müller, B. C. N., Kühn, S., van Baaren, R. B., Dotsch, R., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011).

Perspective taking eliminates differences in co-representation of out-group members’

actions. Experimental Brain Research, 211(3-4), 423–428. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2654-7

Nettle, D. (2007). Empathizing and systemizing: what are they, and what do they contribute to

our understanding of psychological sex differences? British journal of psychology (London,

England: 1953), 98(Pt 2), 237–55. doi:10.1348/000712606X117612

Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V., &

Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum

disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198.

doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014

Page 114: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

105

Oberman, L. M., Pineda, J. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The human mirror neuron

system: A link between action observation and social skills. Social Cognitive and Affective

Neuroscience, 2(1), 62–66. doi:10.1093/scan/nsl022

Oberman, L. M., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2008). Modulation of mu suppression in

children with autism spectrum disorders in response to familiar or unfamiliar stimuli: The

mirror neuron hypothesis. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1558–1565.

doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.01.010

Obhi, S. S., & Sebanz, N. (2011). Moving together: toward understanding the mechanisms of

joint action. Experimental brain research. Experimentelle Hirnforschung. Expérimentation

cérébrale, 211(3-4), 329–36. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2721-0

Ohman, a, Lundqvist, D., & Esteves, F. (2001). The face in the crowd revisited: a threat

advantage with schematic stimuli. Journal of personality and social psychology, 80(3),

381–96. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11300573

Ostrom, T., & Sedikides, C. (1992). Ostrom, Sedikides - 1992 - out-group homogeneity effects

in natural and minimal groups.pdf. Psychological bulletin, 112(3), 536 – 552.

Park, B., Judd, C. M., & Box, C. (1990). Measures and Models of Perceived Group Variability,

59(2), 173–191.

Pauker, K., Weisbuch, M., Ambady, N., Sommers, S. R., Adams, R. B., & Ivcevic, Z. (2009).

Not so black and white: memory for ambiguous group members. Journal of personality and

social psychology, 96(4), 795–810. doi:10.1037/a0013265

Pfurtscheller, G. (1979). Evaluation of Event-Related Desynchronization (ERD) Preceding and

Following Voluntary Self-Paced Movement. Electroencephalography and Clinical

Neurobiology, 46, 138–146.

Pineda, J A, & Hecht, E. (2009). Mirroring and mu rhythm involvement in social cognition: Are

there dissociable subcomponents of theory of mind? Biological Psychology, 80(3), 306–

314. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.11.003

Page 115: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

106

Pineda, Jaime A. (2005). The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and

“hearing” into “doing.” Brain Research Reviews, 50(1), 57–68.

doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2005.04.005

Pratto, F., & Glasford, D. E. (2008). Ethnocentrism and the value of a human life. Journal of

personality and social psychology, 95(6), 1411–28. doi:10.1037/a0012636

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: A

personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 67(4), 741–763. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.741

Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases.

Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25(1), 1–20. doi:10.1017/S0140525X02000018

Prinz, W. (1987). Ideo-motor action. In H. Heuer & A. F. Sanders (Eds.), (pp. 47–76). Hillsdale,

NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Puzzo, I., Cooper, N. R., Cantarella, S., & Russo, R. (2011). Measuring the effects of

manipulating stimulus presentation time on sensorimotor alpha and low beta reactivity

during hand movement observation. NeuroImage, 57(4), 1358–1363.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.05.071

Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007).

Rocking together: dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination.

Human movement science, 26(6), 867–91. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.002

Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual review of

neuroscience, 27(0147-006X, 0147-006X), 169–92.

doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230

Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the

understanding and imitation of action. Perspectives, 2(September), 1–10.

Rodin, M. J. (1987). WHO is memorable to whom: A study of cognitive disregard. Social

Cognition, 5(2), 144–165.

Page 116: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

107

Rozin, P., & Royzman, E. B. (2001). Negativity Bias, Negativity Dominance, and Contagion.

Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 296–320.

doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0504_2

Sartori, L., Cavallo, A., Bucchioni, G., & Castiello, U. (2011). Corticospinal excitability is

specifically modulated by the social dimension of observed actions. Experimental Brain

Research, 211(3-4), 557–568. doi:10.1007/s00221-011-2650-y

Saucier, D. A., Miller, C. T., & Doucet, N. (2005). Personality and Social Psychology Review,

1–16. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0901

Schippers, M. B., & Keysers, C. (2011). Mapping the flow of information within the putative

mirror neuron system during gesture observation. NeuroImage, 57(1), 37–44.

doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.018

Sebanz, N., Bekkering, H., & Knoblich, G. (2006). Joint action: Bodies and minds moving

together. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(2), 71–76. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009

Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (2003). Social dominance theory and the dynamics of inequality: A

reply to Schmitt, Branscombe, & Kappen and Wilson & Liu. British Journal of Social

Psychology, 42(2), 207–213. doi:10.1348/014466603322127193

Sime, J. D. (1983). Affiliative behaviour during escape to building exits. Journal of

Environmental Psychology, 3(1), 21–41. doi:10.1016/S0272-4944(83)80019-X

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy

for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303(5661),

1157–1162. doi:10.1126/science.1093535

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006).

Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature,

439(7075), 466–469. doi:10.1038/nature04271

Smith, N. K., Cacioppo, J. T., Larsen, J. T., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). May I have your

attention, please: electrocortical responses to positive and negative stimuli.

Page 117: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

108

Neuropsychologia, 41(2), 171–83. Retrieved from

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12459215

Sporer, S. L. (2001). The cross-race effect: Beyond recognition of faces in the laboratory.

Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 170–200. doi:10.1037/1076-8971.7.1.170

Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33,

1–39.

Tang, A. C., Liu, J.-Y., & Sutherland, M. T. (2005). Recovery of correlated neuronal sources

from EEG: the good and bad ways of using SOBI. NeuroImage, 28(2), 507–519.

doi:doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.062

Taylor, S. E. (1991). Asymmetrical effects of positive and negative events: the mobilization-

minimization hypothesis. Psychological bulletin, 110(1), 67–85. Retrieved from

http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1891519

Tkach, D., Reimer, J., & Hatsopoulos, N. G. (2007). Congruent activity during action and action

observation in motor cortex. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society

for Neuroscience, 27(48), 13241–50. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2895-07.2007

Todd, A. R., Bodenhausen, G. V., Richeson, J. a, & Galinsky, A. D. (2011). Perspective taking

combats automatic expressions of racial bias. Journal of personality and social psychology,

100(6), 1027–1042. doi:10.1037/a0022308

Trawalter, S., Todd, A. R., Baird, A. A, & Richeson, J. A. (2008). Attending to Threat: Race-

based Patterns of Selective Attention. Journal of experimental social psychology, 44(5),

1322–1327. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.03.006

Turati, C., Bulf, H., & Simion, F. (2008). Newborns’ face recognition over changes in viewpoint.

Cognition, 106(3), 1300–21. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.06.005

Turner, J C, Brown, R. J., & Tajfel, H. (1979). Social comparison and group interest in ingroup

favouritism. European Journal of Social Psychology, 9(2), 187–204.

doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420090207

Page 118: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

109

Turner, John C. (1975). Social comparison and social identity: Some prospects for intergroup

behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 5(1), 1–34.

doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420050102

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1991). Loss aversion in riskless choice: a reference-dependent

model. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 106(89), 1039–1061.

Tybout, A. M., Sternthal, Keppel, Verducci, Meyers-Levy, Barnes, Maxwell, et al. (2001).

Analysis of Variance. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 10, 5–35.

Ulloa, E. R., & Pineda, J. A. (2007). Recognition of point-light biological motion: Mu rhythms

and mirror neuron activity. Behavioural brain research, 183(2), 188–194.

doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.06.007

Umilta, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., & Rizzolatti, G.

(2001). I Know What You Are Doing: A Neurophysiological Study. Neuron, 31, 155–165.

Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). The rhythm of joint action: Synchrony

promotes cooperative ability. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(4), 693–695.

doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004

Van Bavel, J. J., & Cunningham, W. a. (2009). Self-categorization with a novel mixed-race

group moderates automatic social and racial biases. Personality & social psychology

bulletin, 35(3), 321–35. doi:10.1177/0146167208327743

Van Bavel, J. J., Packer, D. J., & Cunningham, W. A. (2008). The neural substrates of in-group

bias: A functional magnetic resonance imaging investigation. Psychological Science,

19(11), 1131–1139. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02214.x

VanRullen, R., & Koch, C. (2003). Competition and selection during visual processing of natural

scenes and objects. Journal of vision, 3(1), 75–85. doi:10:1167/3.1.8

Vogt, J., De Houwer, J., & Moors, A. (2011). Unintended Allocation of Spatial Attention to

Goal-Relevant but Not to Goal-Related Events. Social Psychology, 42(1), 48–55.

doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000042

Page 119: The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor … · The Biased Mirror: A Functional Approach to Intergroup Motor Resonance. Jennifer Gutsell Doctor of Philosophy Psychology

110

Weisbuch, M., & Ambady, N. (2008). Affective divergence: Automatic responses to others’

emotions depend on group membership. Journal of personality and social psychology,

95(5), 1063–1079. doi:10.1037/a0011993

Wheeler, M. E., & Fiske, S. T. (2005). Controlling Racial Prejudice Social-Cognitive Goals

Affect Amygdala and Stereotype Activation. Psychological Science, 16(1), 56–63.

Wheeler, R. E., Davidson, R. J., & Tomarken, A. J. (2007). Frontal brain asymmetry and

emotional reactivity: A biological substrate of affective style. Psychophysiology, 30(1), 82–

89. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03207.x

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J., Gallese, V., Rizzolatti, G., Deusinglaan, A., et al.

(2003). Both of Us Disgusted in My Insula: The Common Neural Basis of Seeing and

Feeling Disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.

Woodruff, C. C., Martin, T., & Bilyk, N. (2011). Differences in self- and other-induced Mu

suppression are correlated with empathic abilities. Brain research, 1405(0006-8993, 0006-

8993), 69–76. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.046

Xu, X., Zuo, X., Wang, X., & Han, S. (2009). Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership

modulates empathic neural responses. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(26), 8525–8529.

doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2418-09.2009

van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., & van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and

Prosocial Behavior. Psychological Science, 15(1), 71–74. doi:10.1111/j.0963-

7214.2004.01501012.x

van Schie, H. T., Koelewijn, T., Jensen, O., Oostenveld, R., Maris, E., & Bekkering, H. (2008).

Evidence for fast, low-level motor resonance to action observation: An MEG study. Social

Neuroscience, 3(3-4), 213–228. doi:10.1080/17470910701414364