testing the health belief model using prostate cancer ... · cancer screening methods and...

34
Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer Screening Intention: Comparing Four Statistical Approaches Applied to Data from the 2008-09 Nashville Men’s Preventive Health Survey By Marka M. Anderson Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Vanderbilt University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of MASTER OF ARTS in Sociology December, 2013 Nashville, Tennessee Approved: Tony N. Brown, Ph.D. C. André Christie-Mizell, Ph.D

Upload: others

Post on 07-Jul-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer Screening Intention:

Comparing Four Statistical Approaches Applied to Data from the 2008-09 Nashville Men’s

Preventive Health Survey

By

Marka M. Anderson

Thesis

Submitted to the Faculty of the

Graduate School of Vanderbilt University

in partial fulfillment of the requirements

for the degree of

MASTER OF ARTS

in

Sociology

December, 2013

Nashville, Tennessee

Approved:

Tony N. Brown, Ph.D.

C. André Christie-Mizell, Ph.D

Page 2: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Prostate cancer is the most frequently diagnosed cancer among men (CDC 2011). It

causes the highest number of cancer-related deaths among men, second only to lung cancer

(CDC 2011). These statistics indicate that prostate cancer screening, despite known problems

with screening sensitivity and specificity (see Moyer 2012), is essential for protecting men’s

health. The importance of screening is especially true for particular groups of men because risk is

correlated with age, race, and family history. Specifically, the greatest risk of prostate cancer

occurs among men who are 50 years of age or older, African American, or have an immediate

relative who has been diagnosed with prostate cancer (CDC 2010). Screening for prostate cancer

often includes a blood test for the prostate specific antigen (PSA) and/or a digital rectal exam

(DRE; CDC 2010).

Disagreement regarding guidelines for prostate cancer screening intensified recently

(Bazell 2012). Issues of disagreement include the age at which men should begin screening for

prostate cancer and the screening methods themselves. On May 21, 2012, the U.S. Preventive

Services Task Force, which was appointed to complete a federally funded study of prostate

cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should

no longer get regular PSA tests as part of screening for prostate cancer (Moyer 2012). The

taskforce advised that the risk of possible misdiagnosis and side effects from treatment

outweighed benefits of potential early detection and intervention (see Moyer 2012). Immediately

following the federal taskforce’s announcement, the American Urological Association (AUA)

released a statement, expressing its counterclaim—that men should not be discouraged from

obtaining PSA blood tests (see AUA 2012; Bazell 2012). However, almost a year later, at its

annual meeting on May 3, 2013, the AUA announced new guidelines that advised against

screening with a PSA blood test for men under the age of 55 with average-risk and for any man

2

Page 3: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

over the age of 70 (Pittman 2013). The AUA advocated for targeted screening and not universal

guidelines (Pittman 2013). Men’s perceptions of their risk and the benefits of screening change

when recommendations regarding prostate cancer screening change. It is therefore important

(and timely) to investigate men’s intentions to get screened for prostate cancer.

The health belief model (HBM) is a very popular theoretical framework developed to

explain preventive health behaviors such as prostate cancer screening. Despite numerous

methodological and conceptual criticisms of it since its inception in the 1950’s, the HBM guides

the study of various preventive health behaviors, including ones involving multiple types of

cancer, such as screenings for breast and cervical cancers (see Tanner-Smith and Brown 2010),

self-examination for testicular cancer (see McClenahan et al. 2007), and genetic testing for

colorectal cancer (see Cyr, Dunnagan, and Haynes 2010).

Invoking the HBM, the present study examines prostate cancer screening intention using

community survey data from the 2008-09 Nashville Men’s Preventive Health Survey (NMPHS).

The present study also compares results from four statistical approaches typically used to test the

HBM: (1) logistic regression, (2) logistic regression with interactions, (3) path analysis, and (4)

structural equation modeling (SEM) with latent variables. Overall, the HBM has limited

explanatory power when applied to prostate cancer screening intention. Based on results from the

four statistical approaches, the present study recommends using path analysis to test the HBM. It

also recommends two major revisions to the HBM—(1) adding a direct path from modifying

factors to preventive health behavior, and (2) adding a direct path from cues to action to

preventive health behavior. The next sections review what is known about the HBM and

examine statistical approaches typically used to test it. Thereafter, I present analyses from the

3

Page 4: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

2008-09 NMPHS before advocating for theoretical revision to the HBM in the Discussion

section.

LITERATURE REVIEW

The Health Belief Model

To explain why early screening programs for health conditions such as tuberculosis were

unsuccessful, social psychologists in the 1950’s constructed the Health Belief Model (HBM, see

Figure 1) (Rosenstock 1974a). The HBM is a theoretical framework that explains preventive

health behaviors. However, since its inception, the HBM also explained individuals’ seeking of

medical treatment/advice and compliance with actions related to symptoms and diagnosis (Janz

and Becker 1984; Strecher and Rosenstock 1997). The model asserts that an individual will take

preventive action if she believes that (a) she is at risk for developing a health condition, (b) the

health condition will have severe negative impact, (c) there exists a preventive health behavior

that could be beneficial in either reducing her risk or the seriousness of the health condition, and

(d) the benefits of this preventive health behavior outweigh the barriers to or negative

consequences of the preventive health behavior (Janz and Becker 1984; Rosenstock 1974a;

Rosenstock 1974b; Strecher and Rosenstock 1997). The model also accounts for the influence of

various sociodemographic variables, such as sex, race, socioeconomic status, and insurance

status, which work to regulate individuals’ assessments of health conditions and preventive

health behaviors (Rosenstock 1974a). The HBM includes four major constructs: (1) perceived

susceptibility and seriousness or threat of health condition, (2) perceived benefits of preventive

health behavior, (3) perceived barriers to preventive health behavior, and (4) cues to action. Over

4

Page 5: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Figure 1: Diagram of Health Belief Model for Prostate Cancer Screening Intention Adapted from Strecher and Rosenstock (1997, p. 115) and Tanner-Smith and Brown (2010, p.97)

Modifying Factors Age Race Income Education Insurance Status Prostate Cancer

Knowledge

Perceived Susceptibility/ Severity (Threat) of

Prostate Cancer

Cues to action Doctor Family Faith Community Men of Similar Age Education Program

Perceived Benefits of Prostate Cancer

Screening

Perceived Barriers to Prostate Cancer

Screening

Prostate Cancer Screening Intention

5

Page 6: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

the last decade, some studies also include self-efficacy as a correlate of preventive health

behaviors and find that it accounts for a large part of the variance (Garcia and Mann 2003).

Perceived susceptibility/severity (threat) of health condition. Perceived susceptibility

refers to the subjective perception of risk of developing the health condition. It ranges from an

individual’s belief that she is at no risk of acquiring the health condition to the belief that she is

extremely vulnerable to getting the health condition. Likert scale response of strongly agree to

strongly disagree to “It is extremely likely that I will develop [health condition] in my lifetime”

(Bynum et al. 2011), and “I feel that my chances of getting [health condition] in the future are

good if I fail to perform [preventive health behavior]” are examples of measures of this construct

(McClenahan et al. 2007). Perceived severity encompasses beliefs regarding troubles the health

condition could generate in the individual’s life and also emotional responses when an individual

thinks about developing the health condition. It can be measured in terms of whether the

individual believes the health condition could affect her financial security (McClenahan et al.

2007) and how anxious or stressed she would feel if she had the health condition (Werner 2003).

The perceived threat construct of the HBM is an incorporation of perceived susceptibility and

severity. It is an overall measure of how great an individual believes her risk is of developing a

specific health condition and how seriously she perceives developing that health condition will

impact her life.

Perceived benefits of preventive health behavior. The perceived benefits construct

includes aspects of how effective and beneficial the individual believes the preventive health

behavior to be in reducing her risk of developing the health condition. Items such as, “I have a

lot to gain by doing [preventive health behavior]” (McClenahan et al. 2007) or “Completing

[preventive health behavior] could save my life” (Bynum et al. 2011) capture the construct.

6

Page 7: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Perceived barriers to preventive health behavior. The perceived barriers construct of the

HBM encompasses the negative qualities or costs of the preventive health behavior. Aspects that

can be considered here are whether the preventive health behavior is expensive, embarrassing,

tedious, painful, or distressing. These features are characteristics which would trigger avoidance.

Another possible barrier to a preventive health behavior includes the availability of alternative

actions that have similar functions. To assess this construct, previous measures include responses

to “It is embarrassing for me to do [preventive health behavior]” or “[preventive health behavior]

can be painful” (McClenahan et al. 2007).

Cues to action. Cues to action are described as triggers or instigators. The inclusion of

cues to action as a construct in the HBM is to account for a prompting moment “to set the

process in motion” (Rosenstock 1974a:5). This construct encompasses various items, such as

mass media campaigns, newspaper articles, advice from others, family member’s or friend’s

illness (Rosenstock 1974a), media information (Strecher and Rosenstock 1997), and a reminder

from doctor/friend (Tanner-Smith and Brown 2010).

Critiques of the Health Belief Model

There are several conceptual critiques of the HBM. First, it explains only a small amount

of variation in preventive health behaviors (Harrison, Mullen, and Green 1992). Second, because

it focuses on predominantly psychosocial elements, the HBM only explains variance in

preventive health behaviors that is due to individuals’ beliefs and attitudes (Janz and Becker

1984). Third, there is great variation in the way HBM constructs are understood and measured

(see, for example, Abraham and Sheeran 2005; Janz and Becker 1984; Rosenstock 1974b).

Literature advises that standardized tools and measures should be created for the

operationalization of the various constructs of the model (see Harrison, Mullen, and Green 1992;

7

Page 8: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Janz and Becker 1984). Fourth, all constructs in the HBM are not always measured and modeled.

For example, the cues to action construct is often omitted in studies using the HBM (see reviews

by Harrison, Mullen, and Green 1992; Janz and Becker 1984).

In addition, there are several methodological criticisms. First, research using the HBM

has been criticized for treating each HBM construct as equally weighted variables that work

simultaneously in influencing preventive health behaviors (Strecher and Rosenstock 1997). One

recommendation is that the model should be tested with interaction terms based on levels of

perceived threat as each construct could work differently based on high or low one perceived

threat (Strecher and Rosenstock 1997). Second, Abraham and Sheeran (2005) point out that there

is inconsistent adherence to procedures or guidelines for how the constructs of the HBM should

be related. Third, researchers test the HBM using inconsistent statistical approaches. In fact, four

statistical approaches dominate: (1) logistic regression, (2) logistic regression with interactions,

(3) path analysis, and (4) structural equation modeling (SEM).

The present study addresses a number of the conceptual and methodological criticisms of

the HBM listed above by first, including all HBM constructs in the analyses and second, by

comparing results from the four dominant statistical approaches used to test the HBM. Results

reveal the consistent statistical significance of perceived threat in increasing prostate cancer

screening intention. This study also finds that other HBM constructs (i.e., perceived barriers and

perceived benefits) do not significantly predict prostate cancer screening intention. Notably,

results show that cues to action does significantly increase prostate cancer screening intention.

Also modifying factors, such as age and insurance status, have a significant effect in increasing

prostate cancer screening intention. Based on results from the four statistical approaches, going

forward I recommend using path analysis to test the HBM. I also recommend (1) adding a direct

8

Page 9: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

path from modifying factors to preventive health behavior and (2) adding a direct path from cues

to action to preventive health behavior.

METHODS

Data

Data come from the 2008-09 Nashville Men’s Preventive Health Survey (NMPHS). The

NMPHS includes a random sample (based on a multi-stage stratified and clustered method) of

392 men aged 40 to 70, living in Davidson County, TN, with no history of prostate cancer (see

Hull et al. 2013). Davidson County, TN was first divided into four distinct geographic areas,

creating four strata. Five census tracts were randomly selected from each stratum and each

designated tract was stratified into two substrata based on its proportion (greater than or less than

33%) of non-Hispanic black males ages 40 through 70. Ten census blocks were randomly chosen

from each substratum, resulting in a total of 80 clusters. 651 of the successfully contacted

households were confirmed to have men who met the eligibility criteria of the study (i.e., being a

white or black male between the ages of 40 and 70 with no history of prostate cancer). Of the

651 eligible men, 392 surveys were completed with a response rate of 60%. The survey included

an array of questions (i.e., demographics, prostate cancer knowledge, screening behavior and

intent to screen, reasons for getting screened, assessment of screening techniques, health

information sources, access to and experience with medical care, and psychosocial measures).

Excluding respondents with missing data on any variable used in the analyses, the

estimation sample size is 251. Table 1 shows descriptive statistics for the overall sample and the

9

Page 10: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics (Means or Percentages, Minimum and Maximum Values) of Variables Used in the Analysis from Nashville Men’s Preventive Health Survey, 2008-09

Mean/Percentage Standard Deviation Minimum Maximum

Outcome Intention to Screen for Prostate Cancer 70.12%

Health Belief Model Constructs Perceived Threata 3.120 1.881 .000 8.000

Cues to Action a 2.068 1.203 .000 5.000 Perceived Benefits a 2.283 .931 .000 3.000 Perceived Barriers a 2.462 1.686 .000 7.000 Modifying Factors/Controls

Self-Efficacy 23.180 4.126 9.000 30.000 Prostate Cancer Knowledge 12.920 2.349 5.000 18.000 Race

African-American 50.60% White 49.40% Age 55.510 8.521 41.000 72.000

Family Income <$10,000-24,999 31.87%

$25,000-44,999 15.94% $45,000-64,999 16.73% $65,000-84,999 12.75% $85,000-100,000+ 22.71% Highest Education

Less than High School 13.15% High School 24.70% Some College 29.08% Bachelor's Degree 21.51% Greater than Bachelor's 11.56% Insurance Status

Insured 78.09% Not Insured 21.91%

Total n 251 a Descriptive statistics are based on summated items and are not scaled.

10

Page 11: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

variables of interest, including HBM constructs. Considering the estimation sample, 49% of

respondents are white and 51% are black. Average age is 55.5 years old and ranges from 41 to

72. 13% have less than a high school education whereas 25% have obtained a high school

education, 29% have some college, and about 33% have a bachelor’s degree or higher. 32% of

respondents have an annual family income between <$10,000 and $24,999 while 16% have

between $25,000 and $44,999 and 17% have between $45,000 and $64,999. The last two annual

family income categories, $65,000-$84,999 and $85,000-$100,000+, represent 12.75% and

22.71% of the estimation sample, respectively. 78% of respondents have medical insurance

whereas 22% are uninsured.

Measures

Prostate cancer screening intention. The dependent variable is a composite variable

based on stated intention to be screened for prostate cancer in either the form of a PSA blood test

and/or a digital rectal exam (DRE) in the next twelve months. It is coded as either a zero if the

participant stated no to both questions of whether he planned to get a PSA blood test and a DRE

exam in the next twelve months or a one if the participant stated yes to either one or both

questions. 70.1% of respondents in the estimation sample express an intention to be screened for

prostate cancer in the next twelve months.

Health belief model constructs. Table 2 includes a detailed list of the questions that

correspond to each construct of the HBM. All items are coded as either zero or one so that

greater values indicate higher perceived threat, more cues to action, greater perceived benefits,

and more perceived barriers. Summated items for perceived threat range from 0 to 8 with a mean

of 3.120. Summated values for cues to action range from 0 to 5 with a mean of 2.068. Perceived

11

Page 12: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Table 2. Questionnaire Items Used to Measure Health Belief Model Constructs for Prostate Cancer Screening Intention Construct Item Intention Do you plan to get a PSA blood test in the next 12 months?

Do you plan to get a DRE exam in the next 12 months?

Perceived Susceptibility/Severity (Threat)

What do you think are your chances of getting prostate cancer at some time in your life?: Not at all (0) to Very likely (1)

What do you think your chances are compared to other men?: Lower than most (0) to Higher than most (1)

How worried are you about getting prostate cancer?: Not at all (0) to A lot (1)

You were concerned about it because you are getting older.a

Because a family member had prostate cancer.a

Because you worry that prostate cancer could cause problems with your sex life.a

Because you worry that prostate cancer could cause you problems with urinating.a

Because you don’t want to die from prostate cancer.a

Cues to Action If your doctor recommended that you get screened for prostate cancer.a

If your family encouraged you to get screened.a

If members of your faith community encouraged you to get screened.a

Because you feel it is common for men your age to get screened.a

Have you ever attended a prostate cancer education program?

Perceived Benefits You wanted to know if you have prostate cancer.a

You thought that you might live longer if prostate cancer was detected early.a

Getting prostate cancer screening was part of taking care of your health.a

Perceived Barriers Do you think that getting a DRE is embarrassing?

Getting a DRE feels uncomfortable.b

Getting a DRE violates your manhood.b

Getting a DRE violates your privacy.b

Getting a DRE makes you feel like less of a man.b

Getting a DRE hurts.b

Getting a DRE could stimulate homosexual (gay) tendencies.b a Please tell me which reasons caused you/would cause you to get screened, by answering Yes or No b Please tell me your opinion of whether each one is true or false

12

Page 13: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

benefits has a mean value of 2.283 and ranges from 0 to 3 while perceived barriers ranges from

zero to seven with a mean of 2.462.

Modifying factors/controls. Modifying factors and controls in these analyses are prostate

cancer knowledge, race, annual family income, highest education completed, and insurance

status, and self-efficacy. Prostate cancer knowledge is measured utilizing eighteen True-or-False

questions regarding the prostate and prostate cancer, including items about the location of the

prostate, prostate cancer risk, screening, and treatment. Knowledge items are coded so that a

correct answer is indicated with a 1 and a wrong answer is noted with a 0 and items are then

summated. Higher values indicate greater prostate cancer knowledge. For this sample, prostate

cancer knowledge values range from 5 to 18 with a mean of 12.92. Race is coded as a dummy

variable with black as the excluded group. Age is treated as a continuous variable. Due to small

cell counts and in order to prioritize parsimony, annual family income is collapsed into five

categories from the original 20 that were utilized in the study questionnaire: (1) <$10,000-

24,999 (the excluded group); (2) $25,000-44,999; (3) $45,000-64,999; (4) $65,000-84,999; (5)

$85,000-100,000+. Highest education is also treated as an ordered categorical variable with five

groups: (1) less than high school (excluded group); (2) high school; (3) some college; (4)

bachelor’s degree; (5) greater than bachelor’s degree. Insurance status is coded as a dummy

variable where 1 indicates that the participant is insured and 0 indicates that he is currently

uninsured. Descriptive statistics for all variables used in the analyses are shown in Table 1. Self-

efficacy is measured utilizing the 10 item General Self-Efficacy Scale (GSE) (Schwarzer and

Jerusalem 1995). Each item is coded on a three-point scale in which 0=not at all true, 1=hardly

true, 2=moderately true, and 3=exactly true. Items are summated so that higher values indicate

greater self-efficacy. For this sample, self-efficacy ranges from nine to 30 with a mean of 23.2.

13

Page 14: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

All analyses were conducted in the statistical environment R. The R script file is available upon

request.

RESULTS

Results are divided into four sections based on the corresponding four statistical

approaches used to test the HBM: (1) logistic regression, (2) logistic regression with interactions,

(3) path analysis, and (4) structural equation modeling (SEM) with latent variables. Adequacy of

the final models estimated by the four statistical approaches—logistic regression (Model 6),

logistic regression with interactions (Model 11), path analysis (Model 12), and SEM with latent

variables (Model 13)—can be assessed by examining model fit indices and explanatory power.

Also, models can be evaluated based on whether estimated coefficients conform to expectations

established by the HBM.

Section 1: Logistic Regression

For logistic regression HBM constructs are summated indices of their corresponding

items. These sums are standardized. As shown in Table 3, in Models 1-4 each HBM construct is

treated as an individual predictor of prostate cancer screening intention. This statistical approach

reveals five major results regarding the HBM of prostate cancer screening intention. First,

looking at Model 1, as a single predictor, an increase in perceived threat corresponds to a

significant increase in one’s odds of prostate cancer screening intention. Second, according to

Model 2, as a single predictor, more cues to actions also significantly increases one’s odds of

prostate cancer screening intention. Third, as separate predictors, perceived benefits of getting

screened in Model 3 and perceived barriers to getting screened in Model 4 do not have

significant effects on the odds of prostate cancer screening intention. Fourth, in Model 5 and

14

Page 15: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Table 3. Standardized Logistic Regression Coefficients of the Effects of Health Belief Model Constructs and Modifying Factors on the Odds of Prostate Cancer Screening Intention

Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Model 5 Model 6 Intercept 2.426 *** 2.411 *** 2.354 *** 2.354 *** 2.454 *** 1.405

(.142)

(.141)

(.138)

(.138)

(.144)

(.506) Health Belief Model Constructs

Perceived Threat 1.500 **

1.384 · 1.432 ·

(.148)

(.180)

(.196)

Cues to Action

1.443 *

1.282

1.261

(.146)

(.171)

(.189)

Perceived Benefits

1.133

.908

.942

(.135)

(.157)

(.177)

Perceived Barriers

1.131

1.027

1.058

(.141)

(.150)

(.167)

Modifying Factors/Controls Self-Efficacy

.948

(.158)

Prostate Cancer Knowledge

1.148

(.174)

Race (African-American=excluded) White

1.040

(.322)

Age

1.798 **

(.180)

Education (Less than HS=excluded) High School

1.329

(.528)

Some College

1.876

(.550)

Bachelor's Degree

1.671

(.620)

Greater than Bachelor's

2.262

(.763)

Income (<$10,000-24,999=excluded) $25,000-44,999

.244 **

(.486)

$45,000-64,999

.434

(.526)

$65,000-84,999

1.203

(.670)

$85,000-100,000+

.642

(.531)

Health Insurance (Not insured=excluded) Insured

2.429 *

(.387) Psuedo R² .026

.022

.003

.003

.034

.167

AIC 302.13

303.54

309.31

309.37

305.85

290.98 Log Likelihood (df) -149.07 (2) -149.77 (2) -152.65 (2) -152.69 (2) -147.93 (5) -127.49 (18)

n 251 251 251 251 251 251 Notes: Numbers in parentheses are standard errors of log odds. ·p<0.1 *p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001 (two-tailed tests)

15

Page 16: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Model 6, perceived threat is the only HBM construct that is a significant predictor of prostate

cancer screening intention. Fifth, in regards to modifying factors in Model 6, an increase in age

as well as having medical insurance corresponds to an increase in odds of prostate cancer

screening intention.

In concordance with previous critiques of the HBM, the final logistic regression model

with all constructs and modifying factors (Model 6) is only able to explain 16.7% of the variance

in prostate cancer screening intention based on Hosmer and Lemeshow's pseudo R². Another

model fit statistic of notable importance for comparison is the Akaike information criterion

(AIC), which for Model 6 is 290.98.

Section 2: Logistic Regression with Interactions

Strecher and Rosenstock (1997) suggest that, rather than assuming that every construct of

the model works separately, the effect of each construct on the odds of a particular preventive

health behavior could depend on one’s level of perceived threat. Therefore, in this statistical

approach I test if perceived threat has a significant moderating effect on the relationships

between other constructs of the HBM and the odds of prostate cancer screening intention.

For logistic regression with interactions HBM constructs are summated indices of their

corresponding items. These sums are standardized. Models 7-9 (included in Table 4) test

perceived threat, each construct, and their interaction separately. First, Model 7 reveals a

marginally significant interaction between cues to action and perceived threat, suggesting that

the effect of cues to action on odds of prostate cancer screening intention depends on ones

perception of how threatening prostate cancer is. Although growth in perceived threat

corresponds to a significant increase in odds of prostate cancer screening intention,

16

Page 17: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Table 4. Standardized Logistic Regression Coefficients (the Odds of Prostate Cancer Screening Intention) Testing Interactions between Perceived Threat and other Health Belief Model Constructs

Model 7 Model 8 Model 9 Model 10 Model 11 Intercept 2.805 *** 2.266 *** 2.470 *** 2.738 *** 1.794

(.168)

(.152)

(.147)

(.173)

(.537) Health Belief Model Constructs

Perceived Susceptibility/Severity (Threat) 1.410 * 1.516 * 1.487 ** 1.500 * 1.537 *

(.175)

(.163)

(.152)

(.194)

(.212)

Cues to Action 1.262

1.234

1.195

(.171)

(.185)

(.200)

Perceived Benefits

1.048

1.033

1.088

(.165)

(.178)

(.199)

Perceived Barriers

1.043

1.072

1.096

(.149)

(.154)

(.171)

Interactions Perceived Threat*Cues to Action .766 ·

.667 * .674 *

(.154)

(.175)

(.200)

Perceived Threat*Perceived Benefits

1.207

1.398 · 1.445 ·

(.156)

(.182)

(.201)

Perceived Threat*Perceived Barriers

.927

.905

0.941

(.141)

(.143)

(.170)

Modifying Factors/Controls Self-Efficacy

.934

(.163)

Prostate Cancer Knowledge

1.207

(.176)

Race (African-American=excluded) White

1.037

(.329)

Age

1.849 ***

(.183)

Education (Less than HS=excluded) High School

1.349

(.536)

Some College

1.676

(.560)

Bachelor's Degree

1.612

(.634)

Greater than Bachelor's

1.833

(.784)

Income (<$10,000-24,999=excluded) $25,000-44,999

.233 **

(.504)

$45,000-64,999

.410 ·

(.542)

$65,000-84,999

1.320

(.682)

$85,000-100,000+

.606

(.541)

Health Insurance (Not insured=excluded) Insured

2.180 · (.399) Psuedo R² .042

.031

.027

.057

.185

AIC 301.33

304.58

305.79

304.80

291.58 Log Likelihood (df) -146.67 (4) -148.29 (4) -148.90 (4) -144.4 (8) -124.789 (21)

n 251 251 251 251 251 Notes: Numbers in parentheses are standard errors of log odds. ·p<0.1 *p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001 (two-tailed tests)

17

Page 18: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

unexpectedly, the interaction term suggests that with this growth in threat the effect of cues to

action corresponds to a decrease in odds of prostate cancer screening intention. Second, in

Models 8 and 9, in which perceived threat’s interaction with perceived benefits and its

interaction with perceived barriers are tested respectively, only perceived threat’s direct effect is

significant and indicates an increase in odds of screening intention. These results suggests that,

when considered in separate regressions, an increase in ones perceived threat does not trigger

perceived benefits or barriers to have a significant effect on odds of prostate cancer screening

intention.

Model 10 includes all interactions between perceived threat and the other HBM

constructs. Model 11 adds modifying factors and controls. First, in Model 10 and Model 11 there

is still a significant and negative moderating effect of perceived threat on the relationship

between cues to actions and odds of prostate cancer screening intention. Second, Model 10 and

Model 11 reveal a significant interaction between perceived threat and perceived benefits. As an

individual perceives greater threat in getting prostate cancer, a growth in perceived benefits of

screening corresponds to an increase in an individual’s odds of prostate cancer screening

intention. Third, as seen in Model 6, in Model 11 being older and insured increases ones odds of

screening intention.

The final model (Model 11) for this statistical approach explains 18.5% of the variance in

odds of prostate cancer screening intention according to Hosmer and Lemeshow's pseudo R².

Also Model 11’s AIC is 291.58.

Comparison of Logistic Regressions With and Without Interactions. When comparing the

final logistic regression models (Models 6 and 11) the AIC captures model fit whereas Hosmer

and Lemeshow's pseudo R² evaluates explanatory power. First, in terms of the AIC, smaller

18

Page 19: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

values indicate better fit than models with larger AIC values (Bollen and Curran 2006). Based on

this assessment, Model 6 (logistic regression with summated constructs) has a smaller AIC than

Model 11, although the difference is only 0.6. Second, based on pseudo R², Model 11 (logistic

regression with interactions) explains greater variation in the outcome with 0.185 as compared to

0.167. Also Model 11 has significant interaction terms, indicating the role of perceived threat as

a moderator that triggers the influence of other HBM constructs on prostate cancer screening

intention.

However, in terms of estimated coefficients for the HBM constructs, both methods have

problematic results. Constructs that theory suggests are significant are not found to be so. In

Model 6 and Model 11 perceived threat is the only construct that has a significant direct effect on

prostate cancer screening intention. Even when the other constructs act as sole predictors

(Models 2-4) only cues to action has a significant direct effect on the outcome. These findings

could point to the inadequacy of the overall HBM in explaining prostate cancer screening

intention.

Section 4: Path Analysis

The diagram of the HBM (see Figure 1) depicts not only direct paths from constructs to

prostate cancer screening intention but also indirect effects. The diagram indicates that cues to

action indirectly effects screening intention through perceived threat rather than having a direct

influence. Also based on Figure 1, there is no indication of direct effects of modifying factors,

such as sociodemographics, on prostate cancer screening intention. Rather, the diagram shows

modifying factors to have indirect effects through their impacts on perceived threat, perceived

benefits of screening, and perceived barriers to screening. In this case path analysis can act as a

19

Page 20: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

better test of theory than logistic regressions as it can more accurately model relationships

between constructs (Duncan 1966).

Some sociological HBM analyses attempt estimation of a path model by accounting for a

relationship or direct effect between each pair of constructs (e.g., Jolly et al. 2009). Other studies

include an analysis with an estimation of direct paths from each construct to the outcome but

does not account for relationships between constructs (e.g., Cummings et al. 1979; McClenahan

et al. 2007). As compared to other attempts in the literature, the path analysis in this study is an

effort to most closely replicate the paths and effects indicated by the HBM diagram.

For the path analysis, the items for each HBM construct are summated before calculating

the correlation matrix upon which the path analysis is run. As seen in Table 5, results from the

path analysis suggest five significant relationships, two of which are only marginally significant.

First, the cues to action construct has a significant effect on perceived threat. As expected, with

an additional cue to action there is an increase in an individual’s perceived threat of prostate

cancer. Second, higher income corresponds to lower levels of perceived threat. Third, prostate

cancer knowledge has a marginally significant effect on perceived benefits, indicating that as an

individual learns more about the prostate and prostate cancer the perceived benefits of getting

screened increase. Fourth, the path analysis reveals that being white corresponds to fewer

perceived barriers to getting screened for prostate cancer. Fifth, when looking at the direct paths

to prostate cancer screening intention, results are consistent with the findings from the logistic

regression analyses—only perceived threat has a significant effect on screening intention while

the paths between screening intention and perceived benefits and barriers are not significant.

Only an increase in perceived threat correlates to an increase in odds of prostate cancer screening

intention.

20

Page 21: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Table 5. Model 12: Standardized Path Analysis Coefficients of the Health Belief Model for Prostate Cancer Screening Intention

Dependent Variables

Inde

pend

ent V

aria

bles

Perceived Threat Perceived Benefits Perceived Barriers Screening Intention

Health Belief Model Constructs Perceived Threat

.243 ***

(.061)

Perceived Benefits

-.026

(.061)

Perceived Barriers

.018

(.061)

Cues to Action .508 ***

(.055)

Modifying Factors/Controls Self-Efficacy

-.007

(.061)

White -.031

-.106

-.252 ***

(.056)

(.065)

(.064)

Age -.012

.022

.029

(.064)

(.073)

(.073)

Income -.129 · -.134

.048

(.072)

(.082)

(.082)

Education -.055

-.063

-.019

(.072)

(.083)

(.082)

Currently Insured .083

.036

-.045

(.072)

(.082)

(.081)

Prostate Cancer Knowledge .056

.122 · -.050 (.061) (.070) (.069)

Variances .715

.952

.933

.939 AIC 8113.506

RMSEA .192 *** χ² 154.240 ***

n 251

Notes: Numbers in parentheses are standard errors.

·p<0.1 *p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001 (two-tailed tests)

21

Page 22: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Considering overall model fit indices, the path analysis (Model 12) has an AIC of

8113.506. The χ² test is a test of overall model fit with a null hypothesis of perfect fit (Bollen and

Curran 2006). A significant χ² test statistic specifies that the null hypothesis of perfect fit must be

rejected. Model 12 has a significant χ² statistic, indicating that that model does not fit the data

well. According to the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) values greater than

0.1 represent a poor fit (Bollen and Curran 2006). Based on a RMSEA of 0.192 this model

appears to be a poor fit.

Section 5: SEM with Latent Variables

Some sociological HBM research use SEM but in vastly different ways (e.g., Jolly et al.

2009; McClenahan et al. 2007; Sapp and Weng 2007). For example, there is no consistency in

which relationships are estimated and whether constructs are included as latent or observed

variables. Sapp and Weng (2007) justify their treatment of the HBM constructs as observed

variables by stating that it reduces complexity (e.g., Sapp and Weng 2007). However, even

within each construct vastly different aspects of the overall concept are measured. For example,

in terms of perceived threat, worrying you might die could carry more weight in how threatening

you perceive a health problem to be than knowing a relative who has the health problem. The

present study tests whether the HBM constructs should be treated as latent factors as part of SEM

rather than as summated observed variables and whether items within each construct should be

weighted differently.

Table 6 includes the factor loadings and statistical significances of the items for each

HBM construct. First, according to the factor loadings, there is a great variability in the

weighting of some items, suggesting that an equal weighting when summated is not appropriate.

Second, two items do not have significant loadings. Comparison of one’s chances of getting

22

Page 23: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Table 6. Model 13: Structural Equation Factor Loading Estimates for the Health Belief Model Latent Constructs for Prostate Cancer Screening Intention

Construct Item Estimate Perceived Risk What do you think are your chances of getting prostate cancer at some time in your

life?: Not at all (0) to Very likely (1)

1.00a

What do you think your chances are compared to other men?: Lower than most (0) to Higher than most (1)

.454

How worried are you about getting prostate cancer?: Not at all (0) to A lot (1)

.727 *

You were concerned about it because you are getting older. 2.300 **

Because a family member had prostate cancer. 1.341 **

Because you worry that prostate cancer could cause problems with your sex life.

3.182 **

Because you worry that prostate cancer could cause you problems with urinating.

3.113 **

Because you don’t want to die from prostate cancer. 1.479 **

Cues to Action If your doctor recommended that you get screened for prostate cancer.

1.00a

If your family encouraged you to get screened. 2.410 ***

If members of your faith community encouraged you to get screened.

2.220 ***

Because you feel it is common for men your age to get screened. 1.269 ***

Have you ever attended a prostate cancer education program? .333

Perceived Benefits You wanted to know if you have prostate cancer. 1.00a

You thought that you might live longer if prostate cancer was detected early.

1.596 ***

Getting prostate cancer screening was part of taking care of your health.

.679 ***

Perceived Barriers Do you think that getting a DRE is embarrassing? 1.00a

Getting a DRE feels uncomfortable. .402 **

Getting a DRE violates your manhood. 1.456 ***

Getting a DRE violates your privacy. 1.314 ***

Getting a DRE makes you feel like less of a man. .931 ***

Getting a DRE hurts. .492 ***

Getting a DRE could stimulate homosexual (gay) tendencies. .484 *** aThe first path for each factor was set to 1; therefore, no significance level is designated.

·p<0.1 *p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001 (two-tailed tests)

23

Page 24: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

prostate cancer to those of other men does not significantly load onto the latent variable,

perceived threat. Also, attendance at a prostate cancer education program is not significant in the

cues to action construct.

Table 7 highlights the coefficients and their significance levels for the suggested paths

outlined in Figure 1. The SEM approach estimated somewhat similar results in term of path

coefficients as compared to path analysis. First, consistently, cues to action has a significant and

positive effect on perceived threat. Second, contradictory to path analysis results, insurance

status has a marginally significant effect on perceived threat. Having medical insurance

correlates with greater perceived threat associated with prostate cancer. None of the modifying

factors have a significant effect on perceived benefits. Third, as seen in path analysis results,

being white corresponds to fewer perceived barriers to prostate cancer screening. Fourth, also

consistent with our path analysis, greater perceived threat corresponds to an increase in odds of

prostate cancer screening intention. And fifth, an unexpected finding and also one that diverges

from the path analysis results, an increase in perceived benefits to screening corresponds to a

decrease in odds of prostate cancer screening intention.

Overall fit indices for the structural equation model with latent variables (Model 13)

include an AIC of 20988.343. The significant RMSEA of .085 suggests poor fit, although some

suggest guidelines that a RMSEA between 0.05 and 0.1 indicate a moderate fit (Browne and

Cudeck 1993). Also the significant chi-square statistic indicates that this model is not a perfect fit

for the data.

24

Page 25: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Table 7. Model 13: Structural Equation Model Path Coefficients of the Health Belief Model for Prostate Cancer Screening Intention

Dependent Variables

Inde

pend

ent V

aria

bles

Perceived Threat Perceived Benefits Perceived Barriers Screening Intention

Health Belief Model Constructs Perceived Threat

1.618 **

(.554)

Perceived Benefits

-.312 *

(.133)

Perceived Barriers

.079

(.118)

Cues to Action .534 **

(.201)

Modifying Factors/Controls Self-Efficacy

-.015

(.060)

White -.014

-.060

-.144 **

(.015)

(.041)

(.043)

Age .010

.037

.017

(.017)

(.045)

(.046)

Income -.029

-.032

.021

(.020)

(.051)

(.051)

Education -.007

-.051

.003

(.019)

(.051)

(.052)

Currently Insured .042 · -.083

-.009

(.022)

(.052)

(.051)

Prostate Cancer Knowledge .020

.067

-.025

(.017) (.045) (.044)

Variances .024

.266

.298

.861 AIC 20988.343

RMSEA .085 *** χ² 1124.343 ***

n 251

Notes: Numbers in parentheses are standard errors.

·p<0.1 *p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001 (two-tailed tests)

25

Page 26: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Comparison of Path Analysis and SEM. For path analysis (Model 12) and SEM with

latent factors (Model 13) there are corresponding model fit indices (chi-square and RMSEA) that

indicate the models are a poor fit (or possibly moderate fit in the case of Model 13’s RMSEA).

Also with path analysis (Model 12) constructs thought to be significant based on theory are not.

Paths from perceived benefits to outcome, from perceived barriers to outcome, and from self-

efficacy to outcome are not significant. However, certain modifying factors do have significant

effects on HBM constructs. Notably, growth in prostate cancer knowledge corresponds to greater

perceived benefits to prostate cancer screening, which suggests that with proper education men

can understand the advantages to getting screened.

With the SEM approach and the treatment of HBM constructs as latent variables there is

the additional test of the significance of the various items and whether they have differential

factor loadings. The present study’s findings in Model 13 show variability in the significant

factor loadings of the items for each construct. For example, worrying that prostate cancer could

affect his sex life matters more in terms of how an individual perceives the threat of prostate

cancer as compared to when a family member has prostate cancer. In regards to SEM there are

again problems with the estimated path coefficients. Perceived barriers does not have a

significant effect on prostate cancer screening intention. And in this final approach (Model 13)

perceived benefits to screening has an unexpected negative effect on prostate cancer screening

intention. This divergent finding may point to the inadequacy of the overall model and/or

indicate that the perceived benefits construct is not completely measured by the corresponding

items.

Comparison of All Four Statistical Approaches. Overall no statistical approach proves

fully superior in testing the HBM. All four approaches show problems and insignificant or

26

Page 27: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

inverse coefficients according to the HBM predictions. These problems point to the widespread

critique of the HBM, which is that constructs are not operationally defined in a sufficient and

consistent way. When constructs are assessed utilizing different measures then inconsistent

results, regarding significance and direction, can occur.

The more appropriate statistical approach for testing the HBM is indicated by how it is

diagramed. The diagram of the HBM implies relationships and paths between constructs,

outcome, and modifying factors. This diagram suggests that a statistical approach is required that

can model how these constructs relate. Also, arguably, although this study’s results show that

when the HBM constructs are treated as latent variables factor loadings for items are significant

and variable, it is problematic to think of these items as measuring latent factors. Arguably, these

items are counts for the corresponding constructs rather than overall measures of the same

common underlying factor. The need for an approach that can best model and test the

relationships between HBM constructs without the problematic treatment of items as measures of

underlying latent factors gives credence to path analysis as the more appropriate statistical

approach for testing the diagramed HBM.

When considering the results of all four approaches as a collective some interesting

patterns emerge, suggesting revisions to the HBM. A more precise understanding of how

constructs, modifying factors, and outcome are related will improve analytic methods that

appropriately estimate and test the HBM. In terms of revisions, based on this study’s results, I

suggest the following two additions to the model (see Figure 2 for revised HBM).

First, in the final logistic regression models (Models 6 and 11) age, insurance status, and

one income category had significant effects on prostate cancer screening intention. These results

27

Page 28: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

suggest that there should be a direct path from the modifying factors to the preventive health

behavior, which is not currently included in the diagram (see Figure 1).

Second, this study suggests the addition of a direct path from the cues to action construct

to preventive health behavior as it functions as a trigger or catalyst to the behavior. Support for

cues to action as more central HBM construct is found in four points in this study’s results. First,

in Model 2, when acting as a sole predictor of prostate cancer screening intention, cues to action

has a significant effect. Second, in Model 11 an interaction between perceived threat and cues to

action is significant. Third, the results of path analysis (Model 12) and SEM (Model 13)

consistently show cues to action to have a significant effect on perceived threat. The revised

diagram of the HBM with additional paths is included in Figure 2.

DISCUSSION

With the health belief model (HBM) as its framework, this study reveals four things

about prostate cancer screening intention. First, there is an overall significant relationship

between perceived threat of prostate cancer and prostate cancer screening intention. Second,

when included as a sole predictor of prostate cancer screening intention, an additional cue to

action corresponds to an increase in odds of prostate cancer screening intention. Third, path

analysis and structural equation modeling (SEM) also support an indirect effect between cues to

action and prostate cancer screening intention through perceived threat as more cues to action

correspond to an increase in perceived threat. Fourth, modifying factors, such as age and being

insured, correspond to an increase in odds of prostate cancer screening intention.

28

Page 29: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Notes: Revisions to the original Health Belief Model are in dotted lines. Pathways for future investigation are in dashed lines.

Figure 2: Revised Diagram of Health Belief Model

Self-Efficacy

Past Preventive Health Behavior

Modifying Factors Age Race Income Education Insurance Status Knowledge

Perceived Susceptibility/ Severity

(Threat) of Health Condition

Cues to action Doctor Family Faith Community People of Similar Age Education Program

Perceived Benefits of Preventive Health

Behavior

Perceived Barriers to Preventive Health

Behavior

PREVENTIVE HEALTH BEHAVIOR

29

Page 30: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Based on the comparison of results from four statistical approaches for testing the HBM

there are two major take-home points. First, this study argues that the emphasis should be on the

proper analysis of diagramed paths between HBM constructs with the appropriate treatment of

items as summed counts for the corresponding constructs. Thus, path analysis should be the

preferred approach for testing the HBM. The second take-home point of the present study is the

recommendation for two major revisions to the HBM in order to create a more accurate diagram

(see Figure 2). First, a direct pathway should be drawn from modifying factors, such as age and

insurance status, to preventive health behavior. The importance of these factors is especially true

for health behaviors similar to prostate cancer screening (i.e. actions that are recommended to

older individuals and require a medical professional for completion). Second, I recommend a

revision in which the cues to action construct is given a more central position and direct effect. In

discussing the origins of the HBM, Rosenstock (1974a) implies that, although with an adequate

degree of susceptibility and seriousness there is the force to act and with the awareness of

benefits and barriers to the preventive health behavior there is an optimal course of action, these

elements might not be enough to lead to a particular health behavior. The crucial element is the

stimulus or trigger to act.

In regards to implications for the larger field of HBM research, the present study

encourages three things. First, as previous critiques have stated (see, for example, Harrison,

Mullen, and Green 1992; Janz and Becker 1984), it is essential to have consistent measures of

the HBM constructs. The present study maintains that future analyses must work towards

measuring all constructs on the same metric. Consistent measurement will aid us in not only

comparing results across studies but also in judging the actual adequacy of the HBM in

predicting preventive health behaviors. Second, the present study also supports the critique that

30

Page 31: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

the HBM is unable to explain a significant amount of variation in preventive health behaviors.

Therefore, as an individual’s past state is often a good predictor of his present state, this study

suggests a future investigation into how a measure of past preventive health behavior could be

worked into the HBM diagram and through which pathways it could influence current or future

preventive health behavior. Theoretically, the experience of past preventive health behavior

could influence an individual’s current or future preventive health behavior through pathways to

his perception of how threatening the health condition is and how he perceives the benefits of

and barriers to the preventive health behavior. The addition of previous preventive health

behavior to the HBM could prove to explain greater variation in current or future preventive

health behavior (see Figure 2). And lastly, as debates arise between major entities, such as

medical professionals and health agencies, about what proper health guidelines should be

regarding various preventive health behaviors, it will be important to understand whose

recommendation matters. Therefore, future research should focus on cues to action as a

significant construct of the HBM. Who the cue to action is coming from and whether that has a

significant effect on its potency in triggering preventive health behaviors could be an important

inquiry for future research.

The present study is not without limitations that could inform future research. First, the

2008-09 Nashville Men’s Preventive Health Survey is cross-sectional data. Thus, there is no way

to address temporal priority, which is essential to path analysis because it implies relationships

located in time from distal to proximal. Second, we can say little about the impact of the new

prostate cancer screening guidelines because the data is from 2008 and 2009, before changes

went into effect. Some findings reported herein may be inconsistent with current beliefs

regarding prostate cancer screening. Third, only men who have no history of prostate cancer

31

Page 32: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

between the ages of 40 and 70 years old who are living in Davidson County are included in the

sample. Therefore, these data are not nationally representative. Fourth, items measuring barriers

to prostate cancer screening only refer to screening aspects that involve the DRE. Prostate cancer

screening can involve a DRE and/or a PSA blood test. However, items as part of the perceived

barriers construct do not assess barriers to the PSA blood test. This omission might explain why

perceived barriers is not a significant predictor of prostate cancer screening intention. Finally,

there was a significant amount of missing data.

CONCLUSION

The risk that prostate cancer poses for men cannot be ignored. If left untreated, this

condition can ravage the lives of the men most important to us and could eventually lead to the

deaths of our fathers, husbands, brothers, and uncles. It is essential that men are diagnosed in a

timely manner in order to ensure that prostate cancer can be treated most effectively. For this

vulnerable population prostate cancer screening is a procedure that was once described as routine

for aging men but is now under scrutiny over its utility. As men deal with the possibility of a

diagnosis without exhibiting symptoms, they must also grapple with an experience that can be

perceived as frightening and embarrassing. Therefore, it is important to understand the conflict

men experience in struggling with the threat prostate cancer poses to their lives and the pros and

cons of screening for this condition. And as men receive contradictory recommendations

regarding prostate cancer screening, it is even more important to understand the complexity of

their decision making process.

32

Page 33: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

REFERENCES

Abraham, Charles, and Paschal Sheeran. 2005. “The Health Belief Model.” Pp. 28-80 in Predicting Health Behaviour, edited by Mark Conner and Paul Norman. New York, NY: Open University Press.

American Urological Association (AUA). 2012. “Prostate Cancer: Screening and Management.” Retrieved (http://www.auanet.org/common/pdf/education/Prostate-Cancer-PSA.pdf).

Bazell, Robert. 2012. “Panel: PSA Tests Unnecessary.” NBC Nightly News.

Bollen, Kenneth, and Patrick Curran. 2006. Latent Curve Models: A Structural Equation Perspective. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Browne, Michael, and Robert Cudeck. 1993. “Alternative ways of assessing model fit.” Pp. 111-136 in Testing Structural Equation Models, edited by Kenneth Bollen and John Scott Long. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Bynum, Shalanda, Heather Brandt, Patricia Sharpe, Michelle Williams, and Jelani Kerr. 2011. “Working to Close the Gap: Identifying Predictors of HPV Vaccine Uptake among Young African American Women.” Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved 22(2):549-61.

Cummings, K M, Alan Jette, Bruce Brock, and Don Haefner. 1979. “Psychosocial Determinants of Immunization Behavior in a Swine Influenza Campaign.” Medical Care 17(6):639-649.

Cyr, Amanda, Tim Allen Dunnagan, and George Haynes. 2010. “Efficacy of the Health Belief Model for Predicting Intention to Pursue Genetic Testing for Colorectal Cancer.” Journal of Genetic Counseling 19(2):174-186.

Duncan, Otis Dudley. 1966. “Path Analysis: Sociological Examples.” The American Journal of Sociology 72(1):1-16.

Garcia, Kelli, and Traci Mann. 2003. “From ‘I Wish’ to ‘I Will’: Predictors of Behavioral Intentions.” Journal of Health Psychology 8(3):347-360.

Harrison, Joel, Patricia Mullen, and Lawrence Green. 1992. “A meta-analysis of studies of the Health Belief Model with adults.” Health Education Research 7(1):107-116.

Hayslip Jr., Bert, Daniel Weigand, Robert Weinberg, Peggy Richardson, and Allen Jackson. 1996. “The Development of New Scales for Assessing Health Belief Model Constructs in Adulthood.” Journal of Aging and Physical Activity 4:307-323.

Hull, Pamela. 2009. Psychosocial and Cultural Barriers to Prostate Cancer Screening: Racial Comparisons. Retrieved (http://www.dtic.mil/dtic/tr/fulltext/u2/a511620.pdf).

Janz, Nancy K., and Marshall H. Becker. 1984. “The Health Belief Model: A Decade Later.” Health Education & Behavior 11(1):1-47.

33

Page 34: Testing the Health Belief Model Using Prostate Cancer ... · cancer screening methods and guidelines, announced its final recommendation that men should ... cancer, such as screenings

Jolly, Curtis M., Budry Bayard, Richard T. Awuah, Simon C. Fialor, and Johnathan T. Williams. 2009. “Examining the Structure of Awareness and Perceptions of Groundnut Aflatoxin among Ghanaian Health and Agricultural Professionals and its Influence on their Actions.” The Journal of Socio-Economics 38(2):280-287.

McClenahan, Carol, Mark Shevlin, Gary Adamson, Cara Bennett, and Brenda O’Neill. 2007. “Testicular self-examination: a test of the health belief model and the theory of planned behaviour.” Health Education Research 22(2):272-84.

Moyer, Virginia. 2012. “Screening for Prostate Cancer: U.S. Preventive Services Task Force Recommendation Statement.” Annals of Internal Medicine 157(2):120-134.

National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Department of Cancer Prevention and Control (CDC). 2010. “Prostate Cancer: Should I get Screened.” Retrieved (http://www.cdc.gov/cancer/prostate/pdf/prostate_fs.pdf).

Pittman, Genevra. 2013. “Urologists: Many men under 55 can skip prostate screening.” NBC News. Thomson Reuters.

Rosenstock, Irwin. 1974a. “Historical Origins of the Health Belief Model.” Pp. 1-8 in The Health Belief Model and Personal Health Behavior, edited by Marshall H. Becker. Thorofare, NJ: Charles B. Slack, Inc.

Rosenstock, Irwin. 1974b. “The Health Belief Model and Preventative Health Behavior.” Pp. 27-59 in The Health Belief Model and Personal Health Behavior, edited by Marshall H. Becker. Thorofare, NJ: Charles B. Slack, Inc.

Sapp, Stephen G., and Chih-Yuan Weng. 2007. “Examination of the Health-Belief Model to Predict the Dietary Quality and Body Mass of Adults.” International Journal of Consumer Studies 31(3):189-194.

Schwarzer, Ralf, and Matthias Jerusalem. 1995. “Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale.” Pp. 35-57 in Measures in Health Psychology: A User’s Portfolio. Casual and Control Beliefs, edited by J. Weinman, S. Wright, and M. Johnston. Windsor, UK: Nfer-Nelson.

Strecher, Victor, and Irwin Rosenstock. 1997. “The health belief model.” Pp. 113-117 in Cambridge Handbook of Psychology, Health and Medicine, edited by Andrew Baum, Stanton Newman, John Weinman, Robert West, and Chris McManus. Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press.

Tanner-Smith, Emily E, and Tony N Brown. 2010. “Evaluating the Health Belief Model: A critical review of studies predicting mammographic and pap screening.” Social Theory & Health 8(1):95-125.

Werner, Perla. 2003. “Factors influencing intentions to seek a cognitive status examination: a study based on the Health Belief Model.” International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 18:787-94.

34