tasks – with and without corrective feedback

37
Tasks – with and without corrective feedback. Rosemary Erlam The University of Auckland [email protected] Shawn Loewen Michigan State University

Upload: rae

Post on 09-Jan-2016

42 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

DESCRIPTION

Tasks – with and without corrective feedback. Rosemary Erlam The University of Auckland [email protected] Shawn Loewen Michigan State University. acknowledging my co-researcher. Shawn Loewen Michigan State University. The night/date of your dreams. Who did you go out with? - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Tasks – with and without corrective feedback.

Rosemary ErlamThe University of [email protected]

Shawn LoewenMichigan State University

Page 2: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

acknowledging my co-researcher

• Shawn Loewen

• Michigan State University

Page 3: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

The night/date of your dreams

• Who did you go out with?

• Where did you go?

• What did you do?

• What time did you come home?

Etc etc

Page 4: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

feedback

• S: then we fall in love• T: we fell in love?• S: we fell in love• [implicit feedback]

• S: I go out with Clive Owen

• T: I go out? I went out

• S: I went out with Clive Owen

• [explicit feedback]

Page 5: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

vs no feedback

What do you predict?

Page 6: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

What does the research literature say?

• Three recent meta-analyses• 1. Russell & Spada (2006)• 15 studies investigating the effectiveness of oral and written

feedback• effect sizes large, although smaller for oral than written

feedback• 2/ Li (in press)• 33 studies examining the effectiveness of corrective

feedback (following errors in oral production) in second language learning

• medium effect for corrective feedback maintained over time• lab-based studies show a greater effect than classroom-

based ones• shorter treatments generated a greater effect than longer

Page 7: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

3. Mackey & Goo (2007)

• meta analysis of research on interaction

• 16 examined effectiveness of corrective feedback given to learners during oral interaction

• large effect sizes on all post tests

• need studies with delayed post tests

• interaction with feedback may not be more effective than interaction alone

Page 8: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

More research needed . . .

• effectiveness of feedback needs to be investigated in relation to different target structures (Ellis, 2007)

• range of measures of learning need to be used– measures of implicit as well as explicit knowledge (Ellis, 2007)

• studies that include delayed post tests (Mackey & Goo, 2007)

Page 9: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Research questions

• Do learners completing tasks make gains in implicit language knowledge when they are given feedback targeting specific language errors?

• Do they make greater gains than students who complete the same tasks but get no feedback?

Page 10: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Research questions

• Do learners completing tasks make gains in explicit language knowledge when they are given feedback targeting specific language errors?

• Do they make greater gains than students who complete the same tasks but get no feedback?

Page 11: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Participants

• 50 students of L2 French from an American university

• 32 in Year 2, 18 in Year 3• average age 20• 40 female, 10 male!!• all but one had English as L1• 60% of Year 2 & 80% of Year 3 had spent

time in a French speaking country – average 5 months

Page 12: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Research design

• Pre-test• Participants completed 8 tasks designed

to elicit the target structures• 2 sessions – half an hour targeting each

target structure – 2 hours in total• 40 students in feedback group, 10 in no

feedback• Posttest 1 – 1 day later• Posttest 2 – 3 weeks later

Page 13: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

noun adjective agreement

• Un arbre (masculine)• Un arbre vert

• Une voiture (feminine)• Une voiture verte

• low perceptual salience & low communicative value

• unacquired by classroom learners despite frequency in the input (Harley, 1989)

Page 14: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Use of être with intransitive verbs in the passé composé

• passé composé – auxiliary + verb

• for most verbs auxiliary is avoir

• for reflexive verbs & small no of intransitive verbs auxiliary is être– J’ai fait du cheval– Je suis monté sur l’échelle

• differs in non obvious ways from L1

• does not carry a heavy communicative load

Page 15: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Research design cont.

• Year 2 students (n = 22)• Worked at tasks eliciting

noun/adj agreement and use of être in passé composé (2 hours)

• Year 3 students ( n = 18)• Worked at tasks eliciting

noun/adj agreement (1 hour)

• Both received feedback

• No feedback – Yr 2 students (n = 10)

• Worked at tasks designed to elicit noun/adjective agreement and use of être in passé composé (2 hours)

• Received no feedback

Page 16: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Tasks . . .

• Les personnages de tele• Comment est-elle/il?• âge• taille• physique• caractère

• La soirée de vos rêves?• sortis avec qui?• ou allé?• fait quoi?• rentré à quelle heure etc?

Page 17: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Feedback• Implicit• S: je pense elle n’est pas

intelligent parce qu’elle• R: elle n’est pas intelligente?• S: elle n’est pas intelligente

• S: je ne sais à quelle heure nous avons rentrés

• R: nous sommes rentrés?• S: nous sommes rentrés parce

que Espagne est un autre continent

• Explicit• S: elle est heureux• R: elle est heureux? Elle

est heureuse• S: heureuse

• S: ils ont allé• R: ils ont allé? Ils sont

allés• S: ils sont allés, oui, ils

sont allés au café

Page 18: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Feedback . . .

• directed at individual students but tasks designed to optimize likelihood that all students attend to corrective episodes

• Groups received average of

19 instances of feedback for noun/adj agreement (range 8 – 32)

10 instances of feedback for être in passé composé (range 3 – 16)

Page 19: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

instruments

• Implicit language knowledge

• Elicited imitation test(Erlam, 2006; Ellis, 2005)Les petites filles rêvent de se marier

en robe blanche.

• Spontaneous production test

Décrivez la Princesse Diana et la Mère Thérèse. Vous avez la possibilité de passer une soirée avec l’une d’elles. Laquelle choisissez-vous? Pourquoi?

• Explicit language knowledge

• Untimed grammaticality judgment test

• Ungrammatical sentences only (Ellis, 2004; 2005)

C’est une idée faux.

Page 20: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Research questions

• Do learners completing tasks make gains in implicit language knowledge when they are given feedback targeting specific language errors?

• Do they make greater gains than students who complete the same tasks but get no feedback?

Page 21: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Elicited imitation test

noun adjective agreement

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1

1 2 3

feedback

no feedback

Page 22: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Elicited imitation testverb etre

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1

1 2 3

feedback

no feedback

Page 23: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Spontaneous production test

noun adjective accuracy

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

1 2 3

feedback

no feedback

Page 24: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Spontaneous production test

verb etre accuracy

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

100

1 2 3

feedback

no feedback

Page 25: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Research questions

• Do learners completing tasks make gains in explicit language knowledge when they are given feedback targeting specific language errors?

• Do they make greater gains than students who complete the same tasks but get no feedback?

Page 26: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Grammaticality judgment test

noun adjective agreement

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1

1 2 3

feedback

no feedback

Page 27: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Grammaticality judgment test

verb etre

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1

1 2 3

feedback

no feedback

Page 28: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Research questions

• Do learners completing tasks make gains in implicit language knowledge when they are given feedback targeting specific language errors? Yes/No

• Do they make greater gains than students who complete the same tasks but get no feedback? No

Page 29: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Research questions cont

• Do learners completing tasks make gains in explicit language knowledge when they are given feedback targeting specific language errors? Yes

• Do they make greater gains than students who complete the same tasks but get no feedback? Yes – for noun/adjective agreement, No for être

Page 30: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Conclusions . . .explanations

• feedback facilitated learning• But tasks (designed to elicit target structures)

also resulted in learningWhy?

• may have focused learner’s attention briefly on form whilst engaged in communication of meaning

• may have noticed gaps between their own interlanguage resources & language they needed

Page 31: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Vocabulary prompt

• sortir• aller • rentrer

• Etc

• [use of verb être in the passé composé is a rule that is easy to apply

• allows for item learning rather than system learning]

Page 32: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Reasons cont.

• No Feedback group reported high awareness of target structures

• opportunity to engage in a different type of instruction may have motivated them more to attend to the content of the activities (Lyster & Mori, 2006; Yang & Lyster, forthcoming)

Page 33: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Awareness of target structure . . .

Noun/adj être

n % n %

feedback 40 64 22 38

no feedback

10 44 10 67

Page 34: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

What is missing?

• A control group that completed the tests only

• [no tasks]

Page 35: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Control group – EI test

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1

1 2 3

noun adj agreement

verb etre

Page 36: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

Control group – GJT test

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

0.6

0.7

0.8

0.9

1

1 2 3

noun adjective agreement

verb etre

Page 37: Tasks – with and without corrective feedback

references• Erlam, R. (2006). Elicited imitation as a measure of L2 implicit knowledge: An

empirical validation study. Applied Linguistics, 27(3), 464-491.• Ellis, R. (2007). The differential effects of corrective feedback on two grammatical

structures. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition (pp. 339-361). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

• Harley, B. (1989). Functional grammar in French immersion: A classroom experiment. Applied Linguistics, 10, 331-359.

• Li, S. (in press). The effectiveness of corrective feedback in SLA: A meta-analysis. Language Learning.

• Lyster, R., & Mori, H. (2006). Interactional feedback and instructional counterbalance. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 269-300.

• Mackey, A., & Goo, J. (2007). Interaction research in SLA: A meta-analysis and research synthesis. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition (pp. 407-453). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

• Russell, J., & Spada, N. (2006). The effectiveness of corrective feedback for the acquisition of L2 grammar. In J. M. Norris & L. Ortega (Eds.), Synthesizingresearch on language learning and teaching (pp. 133-164 ). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.