t -d h support for the republican party · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with...

75
T HE ANTI -D EMOCRAT DIPLOMA : H OW HIGH SCHOOL EDUCATION INCREASES SUPPORT FOR THE R EPUBLICAN PARTY * J OHN MARSHALL D ECEMBER 2016 Attending high school can alter student life trajectories by affecting labor market prospects and exposure to ideas and networks. However, schooling’s influence com- petes with powerful early socialization forces, and may be confounded by well-established selection biases. Consequently, little is known about whether or how high school edu- cation shapes downstream political preferences and voting behavior. Using a difference- in-differences design exploiting variation in U.S. state dropout laws across cohorts, I find that raising the school dropout age substantially increases Republican partisan identification and voting later in life. Instrumental variables estimates, which reflect dropout laws principally impacting minorities, show that each completed grade of high school increases Republican support by around 10 percentage points. High school’s effects operate by increasing income, which increases support for conservative eco- nomic policies and ultimately the Republican party. These effects influence future state legislature composition, suggesting that recent Democrat attempts to raise state dropout ages are strategically misguided. * I thank Jim Alt, Charlotte Cavaille, Dan Carpenter, Jon Fiva, Anthony Fowler, Andy Hall, Shigeo Hirano, Torben Iversen, Larry Katz, Horacio Larreguy, Rakeen Mabud, Jonathan Phillips, Jim Snyder, and Harvard University work- shop participants for many insightful comments on earlier drafts. I wish to thank Paul Bolton, John Bullock, Philip Oreopoulos and Jim Snyder for kindly sharing data. Department of Political Science, Columbia University. [email protected]. 1

Upload: others

Post on 19-Jul-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

THE ANTI-DEMOCRAT DIPLOMA:HOW HIGH SCHOOL EDUCATION INCREASES

SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY∗

JOHN MARSHALL†

DECEMBER 2016

Attending high school can alter student life trajectories by affecting labor marketprospects and exposure to ideas and networks. However, schooling’s influence com-petes with powerful early socialization forces, and may be confounded by well-establishedselection biases. Consequently, little is known about whether or how high school edu-cation shapes downstream political preferences and voting behavior. Using a difference-in-differences design exploiting variation in U.S. state dropout laws across cohorts, Ifind that raising the school dropout age substantially increases Republican partisanidentification and voting later in life. Instrumental variables estimates, which reflectdropout laws principally impacting minorities, show that each completed grade of highschool increases Republican support by around 10 percentage points. High school’seffects operate by increasing income, which increases support for conservative eco-nomic policies and ultimately the Republican party. These effects influence futurestate legislature composition, suggesting that recent Democrat attempts to raise statedropout ages are strategically misguided.

∗I thank Jim Alt, Charlotte Cavaille, Dan Carpenter, Jon Fiva, Anthony Fowler, Andy Hall, Shigeo Hirano, TorbenIversen, Larry Katz, Horacio Larreguy, Rakeen Mabud, Jonathan Phillips, Jim Snyder, and Harvard University work-shop participants for many insightful comments on earlier drafts. I wish to thank Paul Bolton, John Bullock, PhilipOreopoulos and Jim Snyder for kindly sharing data.†Department of Political Science, Columbia University. [email protected].

1

Page 2: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

1 Introduction

Education is a fundamental component of U.S. economic and social policy, and continues to be a

salient political issue.1 However, as recently as 2010, nearly 20% of students per cohort still fail to

graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-

nority populations (Murnane 2013). Particularly for the Democratic party, raising the high school

dropout age is a key tool helping to ensure that children from disadvantaged backgrounds share

in the economic returns to education (see Oreopoulos 2009). President Obama underscored the

importance of this issue in his 2012 State of the Union address, stating that “When students are

not allowed to drop out, they do better. So ... I am proposing that every state ... requires that all

students stay in high school until they graduate or turn 18.” Dropout ages have generally increased

over the 20th century, and many states have recently considered raising, or successful raised, their

minimum leaving age to 17 or 18.2

While high school education undoubtedly shapes labor market prospects and social interac-

tions, its capacity to impact voters’ political preferences and voting behavior is not theoretically

obvious. First, it is not clear that schooling is able to overcome competing influences on partisan

identification and voting behavior. In particular, a prominent literature argues that partisan iden-

tities and vote choices are highly durable over the course of an individual’s lifetime (e.g. Bartels

2010; Campbell et al. 1960; Converse and Markus 1979; Jennings, Stoker and Bowers 2009). Such

behaviors are believed to develop primarily at home from a young age (e.g. Jennings, Stoker and

Bowers 2009; Kroh and Selb 2009) or as a young adult (e.g. Meredith 2009; Stoker and Bass

2011). From this perspective, schooling—and especially its labor market consequences only expe-

1Across federal, state and local levels, spending on education represents 5% of GDP in 2015. It is thethird largest budget item behind pensions and health care.

2In 2013, Kentucky increased the dropout age to 18 (or receiving a high school diploma). Similarly,Rhode Island raised its leaving age to 18, effective of 2011. Maryland also legislated in 2012 to raise itsdropout age to 17 for 16-year-old students for the 2015-2016 school year, and to 18 for the 2017-2018school year. Similar bills have been introduced in Alaska, Delaware, Illinois, Massachusetts, Montana,South Carolina and Wyoming since 2011.

2

Page 3: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

rienced later in life, once preferences are well-established—could have limited political impact in

the face of powerful habits and early socialization.

Second, even if high school education is able to influence partisan preferences, it is not clear

whether this predisposes voters toward the Democratic or Republican party. One one hand, edu-

cation increases downstream income (e.g. Acemoglu and Angrist 2000), which may cause voters

to support lower taxation (e.g. Meltzer and Richard 1981; Romer 1975). At least once the returns

to schooling are realized later in life, remaining in high school could thus increase Republican

support. Conversely, high school education may increase exposure to liberal values, either in the

classroom (e.g. Dee 2004; Hillygus 2005; Niemi and Junn 1998) or through relatively liberal so-

cial networks (e.g. Alwin, Cohen and Newcomb 1991; Green, Palmquist and Schickler 2002; Nie,

Junn and Stehlik-Barry 1996). This could instead increase support for the Democratic party. Both

sets of arguments suggest that schooling has the potential to transform students’ life trajectories,

causing students to change their policy stances and ultimately support different political parties

later in life.

The extant evidence investigating high school education’s partisan effects is correlational and

often conflicting. Classic survey studies document a positive association between greater education

and voting Republican (e.g. Campbell et al. 1960; Verba, Schlozman and Brady 1995). However,

such correlations may simply reflect the well-documented differences in the types of people that

become better educated (e.g. Kam and Palmer 2008; Sondheimer and Green 2010), or the fact that

education is itself often a cause of the variables researchers control for. Furthermore, by failing

to disentangle either the direction of this relationship or its mechanisms, scholars have struggled

to square the widely-documented correlation between income (which education increases) and

support for conservative economic policies (e.g. Erikson and Tedin 2015; Fisher 2014; Gelman

et al. 2010) with the correlation between education and socially liberal attitudes (e.g. Dee 2004;

Gerber et al. 2010).

This article identifies how raising the high school dropout age, and each additional completed

3

Page 4: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

grade of late high school education that this imparts, affects voter policy preferences and voting

behavior in later life. To estimate these partisan effects of schooling, I employ a difference-in-

differences design leveraging cross-state differences in the dropout age across different cohorts

(see also Acemoglu and Angrist 2000; Angrist and Krueger 1991). The high school dropout age

remains an important policy instrument; despite substantial progress, in 2010 16.3% of students

per cohort failed to graduate from high school by age 20-24 (Murnane 2013). My estimates show

that raising the dropout age by an additional year significantly increases the average student’s

completed grades of schooling. In addition to estimating the effects of raising the dropout age, I

also use school leaving laws to instrument for the number of grades of schooling that an individual

completes. Two-sample instrumental variables (IV) methods address the difficulty that the number

of grades completed is rarely measured in large-scale political surveys (Angrist and Krueger 1992;

Inoue and Solon 2010).3 Given that the dropout age does not affect post-high school education,

the IV estimates identify the effect of completing an additional grade of high school for students

that only remained in school due to a higher state dropout age.

Examining large nationwide surveys conducted around the 2000, 2004 and 2008 Presidential

campaigns, I find that remaining in high school causes voters to become substantially more con-

servative in later life. I first demonstrate that raising the dropout age has a large political effect in

its own right. Increasing the minimum school leaving age by a year causes a 1-3 percentage point

shift, per birth-year cohort, toward identifying as a Republican partisan or voting for the Repub-

lican party. The decline in Democrat support is commensurate. The IV estimates further indicate

that each additional completed grade of high school increases the probability that a student will

subsequently identify as a Republican partisan and vote for a Republican Presidential candidate

by around 10 percentage points. Among compliers, who I demonstrate to be disproportionately

black and Hispanic, the dropout age thus dramatically affects downstream political behavior, caus-

3As discussed below, instrumenting for an indicator of completing high school could considerably up-wardly bias IV estimates (Marshall 2016).

4

Page 5: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

ing voters to become considerably more conservative. Tentative estimates indicate that these shifts

have accumulated across cohorts to increase the Republican seat share in state House and Senate

chambers.

Furthermore, I show that high school’s partisan effects operate primarily via an income-based

channel. Previous evidence from the U.S. indicates that an additional year of schooling increases

wages by around 10% (e.g. Acemoglu and Angrist 2000; Angrist and Krueger 1991; Ashenfelter

and Rouse 1998; Oreopoulos 2006). Consistent with education’s economic returns inducing voters

to support low redistribution (Meltzer and Richard 1981; Romer 1975), the political effects of edu-

cation are greatest at an individual’s mid-life earnings peak, while each additional completed grade

causes voters to favor Republican positions on economic issues. Although this could simply reflect

voters becoming Republican partisans and adopting the party’s policy positions (Lenz 2012; Zaller

1992), education does not increase support for Republican positions on non-economic issues such

as abortion, gun control, health care spending and military spending. Furthermore, contrary to the

predictions of civic education (Dee 2004; Hillygus 2005) and social network (Green, Palmquist

and Schickler 2002; Mendelberg, McCabe and Thal forthcoming; Nie, Junn and Stehlik-Barry

1996) socialization theories, I find no evidence to suggest that high school education affects polit-

ical engagement or socially liberal values. Together, these results indicate that, by increasing an

individual’s subsequent income, high school education causes voters to support economic policies

that, ultimately, lead them to identify as and vote Republican.

These findings raise a “catch 22” for the Democratic party: while increasing access to education

by raising the school dropout age is an important plank of their policy agenda, it comes at the cost

of the beneficiaries of additional schooling electing Republican politicians in the future. Moreover,

given that the beneficiaries are principally black and Hispanic voters, the results further suggest

that income motivations can in part explain why some minority voters support a Republican party

that has generally opposed minority interests and immigration over the period my analysis covers.

The results also recast our understanding of the life-cycle development of political behavior.

5

Page 6: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Previous studies have emphasized both the “primary principle,” that what is learned first—typically

from parents (e.g. Jennings, Stoker and Bowers 2009)—is learned best (see Erikson and Tedin

2015), and particularly that early adulthood is central to political socialization (Mendelberg, Mc-

Cabe and Thal forthcoming; Stoker and Bass 2011). In contrast, although there is some evidence

that high school increases political participation (Sondheimer and Green 2010), adolescent civics

classes and peer interactions at school have produced limited effect on civic and partisan attitudes

(Erikson and Tedin 2015; Green et al. 2011). However, my findings show that high school edu-

cation has important consequences for partisan identification and vote choice later in life, which

can overcome other influences before and after high school, and are driven by downstream income

opportunities rather than socialized values. Given that education’s pro-Republican effects coincide

with voters’ earnings peak, and thus vary across a voter’s lifetime, the results imply that parti-

san preferences are significantly more responsive to changes in circumstances over a lifetime than

previous studies highlighting the stickiness of partisan identity suggest.

By identifying how education affects which party an individual supports, this article departs

from the large literature debating the relationship between education and political participation

(e.g. Berinsky and Lenz (2011); Henderson and Chatfield (2011); Kam and Palmer (2008); Sond-

heimer and Green (2010)). This article instead emphasizes high school’s partisan effects. In this

respect, it complements recent evidence that attending an affluent college with a norm of finan-

cial gain induces students to support conservative economic policies (Mendelberg, McCabe and

Thal forthcoming). This article demonstrates that a similar income-oriented mechanism applies

to the realization of income gains accruing to high school education for disadvantaged popula-

tions, such that schooling shapes voters to oppose redistribution and vote Republican party later in

life. Given that high school’s effects are most pronounced at a voter’s earnings peak, my findings

suggest that Mendelberg, McCabe and Thal’s (forthcoming) study of students at college—before

financial gains are realized—could understate the long-term consequences of conservative college

campuses. Combined, these findings provide a causal interpretation to the increasing likelihood of

6

Page 7: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

voting Republican as a voter becomes more educated, at least until the postgraduate level where

the correlation reverses (Fisher 2014).

2 Theoretical perspectives

Education plays a central role in the lives of American voters. However, although much has been

written about how education affects income, socioeconomic outcomes and civic participation, ed-

ucation’s effect on partisan behavior has received surprisingly limited attention. To the extent that

it is able to overcome other powerful early life forces, I build upon contrasting economic and soci-

ological mechanisms to consider how schooling could affect which political party a voter identifies

with and ultimately votes for.

2.1 Education increases income, decreasing support for taxation?

Perhaps the most obvious, but potentially most politically relevant, effect of education may be to al-

ter an individual’s earnings trajectory. A large body of research now shows that education increases

an individual’s wage income later in life. Twin studies in the U.S. suggest that an additional year of

schooling increases annual wages by 11-16% (e.g. Ashenfelter and Rouse 1998), while studies us-

ing compulsory schooling laws to instrument for schooling estimate this rate of return to be 8-18%

(Acemoglu and Angrist 2000; Angrist and Krueger 1991; Oreopoulos 2006, 2009). The human

capital interpretation of this relationship claims that education imparts productive skills, which are

rewarded in competitive labor markets (e.g. Becker 1964).

Linking schooling’s effect on income to political behavior, Romer (1975) and Meltzer and

Richard (1981) (henceforth RMR) argue that individuals with higher wages will prefer lower in-

come tax rates and less income redistribution. To the extent that redistributive policy preferences

influence vote choice, preferring lower income taxation entails supporting candidates advocating

low tax rates. Particularly since Ronald Reagan’s Presidency, the Republican party has consistently

7

Page 8: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

supported lower income taxation. Furthermore, as ideological and policy differences between the

Democratic and Republican parties have grown (McCarty, Poole and Rosenthal 2006), the differ-

ence between the Democratic and Republican parties on this issue has become easy for voters to

discern. According to American National Election Surveys conducted since 2000, 71% of voters

identify the Republican party as more right-wing than the Democrat party. Moreover, since the

1980s, survey correlations have consistently shown that higher-income voters are more likely to

identify with and vote for the Republican party (Erikson and Tedin 2015; Fisher 2014; Gelman

et al. 2010).

Together, the human capital and RMR models imply that increased schooling should make

voters more favorable toward Republican economic policies, and particularly their proposals for

lower income tax rates, by increasing their income. Furthermore, given that voters can relatively

accurately predict their future income, redistributive preferences may also reflect expected earnings

(Alesina and La Ferrara 2005). Although policy preferences do not necessarily translate into vote

choices, differences in vote choices are likely to increase with realized returns to education.

Critics of human capital theory have instead argued that education is a costly and unproductive

signal of a worker’s underlying productivity (Spence 1973). By this logic, education does not itself

impart skills that improve a worker’s productivity, but instead represents a costly signal enabling

workers that are productive along other dimensions to distinguish themselves from less produc-

tive workers, for whom the costs of becoming educated are greater. Accordingly, an increase in

education should not affect the national income distribution or a voter’s support for the economic

policies proposed by different political parties. While such a signaling model, in conjunction with

the RMR logic, could still be consistent with a correlation between education and vote choices,

there should remain no empirical relationship when using a research design that controls for the

underlying differences in productivity that drive educational choices in the signaling model.

8

Page 9: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

2.2 Education increases exposure to liberal values?

From the perspective of political socialization, education’s effects on political behavior could be

very different. A considerable literature shows that education is correlated with socially and polit-

ically liberal attitudes. For example, more educated voters display greater support for freedom of

expression (Dee 2004; Gerber et al. 2010), ethnic diversity (e.g. Bobo and Licari 1989; Campbell

et al. 1960) and immigration (e.g. Hainmueller and Hiscox 2010). At least in the recent decades

examined in this article, these values have become strongly associated with the Democratic party

(Levendusky 2009). Some studies find that these correlations also extend to economic policy pref-

erences (e.g. Gerber et al. 2010), although others do not (e.g. Weakliem 2002). Thus, while

education may bestow economically valuable knowledge and skills, it could also instill liberal

values.

Two main mechanisms have been proposed to explain these associations: direct effects of

curricula; and changes in social network composition. First, civic education and social science

classes generally encourage political engagement and trust in the political system (Niemi and Junn

1998), and seek to instill liberal values of tolerance (Dee 2004; Hillygus 2005). Such classes

typically start in late high school, and can intensify if students choose to concentrate in such areas

at college. Furthermore, formative experiences appear to affect political beliefs decades later in

life (Jennings, Stoker and Bowers 2009; Meredith 2009). However, recent experimental evidence

finds that while enhanced high school civics classes increase political knowledge, this knowledge

quickly decays and does not affect support for civil liberties (Green et al. 2011). Given that other

empirical evidence has emphasized the importance of college education (see Galston 2001), civic

education’s importance may be confined to higher education.

Second, education could more indirectly affect a voter’s political beliefs by altering the com-

position of their social network. More educated individuals sort into more prestigious, ethnically

diverse and politically-connected networks (Green, Palmquist and Schickler 2002; Nie, Junn and

9

Page 10: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Stehlik-Barry 1996), or at least become exposed to such groups (Alwin, Cohen and Newcomb

1991). Such networks are often characterized by liberal social values and high levels of political

interest, knowledge and discussion, and might thus increase support for the Democratic party. At

lower levels of education, individuals may enter social networks like labor unions with a clear

left-wing political agenda (Abrams, Iversen and Soskice 2010).

While both mechanisms could induce socially liberal attitudes, the observed correlations may

nevertheless be spurious. Family background, values, cognitive abilities and life experiences are

likely to be correlated with both education and socially liberal attitudes (Jencks et al. 1972; Kam

and Palmer 2008). Excepting Green et al.’s (2011) randomized control trial, which provides little

evidence of durable value change, this concern is particularly salient because these studies do not

exploit plausibly random variation in education, and often control for variables like income that

are themselves downstream consequences of education.

3 Empirical design

The difficulty of identifying the partisan effects of education is widely acknowledged. As noted

above, there are many confounding explanations for any correlation between schooling and policy

preferences and voting behavior (see e.g. Kam and Palmer 2008; Sondheimer and Green 2010).

Beyond the omitted variable concern that has besieged the extant literature, another concern is in-

terpreting potential heterogeneity in schooling’s political effects. First, the effect of schooling is

likely to be heterogeneous across types of individual (see Card 1999). Second, schooling’s effects

may depend upon the grade in question, e.g. because middle school, high school and college cover

different content. As suggested above, different levels of schooling may thus differentially stimu-

late income-based and socialization-based mechanisms. Accordingly, a failure to separate levels of

schooling would return a misleading null finding if, for example, high school’s conservative effects

were counterbalanced by liberal effects of attending college.

10

Page 11: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

To address these concerns, I exploit cross-state variation in state minimum dropout ages—both

as an instrument for schooling, but also as a topical policy variable of interest in its own right—

across cohorts using a difference-in-differences design. Since I show that dropout ages do not

induce students to attend college, my estimates identify the effect of completing an additional grade

of high school for individuals that would otherwise have dropped out. This study thus offers a rare

opportunity to cleanly identify the causal effects of a specific level of schooling on partisanship

and voting behavior.

3.1 State school dropout age laws

Compulsory schooling laws are legislated by state governments and define the minimum age at

which a student must enter and may drop out of schooling. The first such law was adopted in

Massachusetts in 1852 and 41 states had adopted similar laws by 1910, principally to meet de-

mand for educated workers and promote assimilation (Goldin and Katz 2008; Kotin and Aikman

1980). The laws have since predominantly focused on the dropout age, which gradually increased

throughout the twentieth century. The minimum leaving age is now 18 for almost half the states

in the country. While the enforcement of dropout laws is relatively weak, states can and do punish

habitual truancy (see Oreopoulos 2009). Labor economists have thus frequently used compulsory

schooling laws as instruments to estimate the economic effects of schooling in the U.S. (e.g. Ace-

moglu and Angrist 2000; Angrist and Krueger 1991; Lochner and Moretti 2004; Milligan, Moretti

and Oreopoulos 2004).

Figure 1 plots the minimum age at which a student is permitted to drop out of school across

the 48 contiguous states and Washington, DC between 1920 and 2004.4 Unsurprisingly, there has

4Alaska and Hawaii are omitted throughout due to lack of data. Although more complex measures ofcompulsory schooling have previously been adopted (e.g. Acemoglu and Angrist 2000; Angrist and Krueger1991), the minimum dropout age captures the relevant legislation for the empirical analyses conducted here.In particular, I omit child labor laws because they offer limited variation and are mostly irrelevant for theperiod I study, and thus produce a weak first stage. Their inclusion as additional instruments does not affectthe results.

11

Page 12: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

1214

1618

1214

1618

1214

1618

1214

1618

1214

1618

1214

1618

1214

1618

1920 1940 1960 1980 2000 1920 1940 1960 1980 2000 1920 1940 1960 1980 2000 1920 1940 1960 1980 2000 1920 1940 1960 1980 2000 1920 1940 1960 1980 2000 1920 1940 1960 1980 2000

AL AR AZ CA CO CT DC

DE FL GA IA ID IL IN

KS KY LA MA MD ME MI

MN MO MS MT NC ND NE

NH NJ NM NV NY OH OK

OR PA RI SC SD TN TX

UT VA VT WA WI WV WY

Min

imum

sch

ool l

eavi

ng a

ge

Year

Figure 1: U.S. state minimum school leaving age laws, 1920-2004

Note: Dropout law data are from Oreopoulos (2009), and based on the National Center for EducationStatistic’s Education Digest.

been a general upward trend in minimum leaving ages. However, there remain numerous instances

of reversal as in Maine, Mississippi, and Oregon. Given that high school dropout rates remain non-

trivial—consistently registered at around 20% for all students since the 1970s, with considerably

higher rates among ethnic minorities (Murnane 2013)—many states are actively seeking to raise

the legal dropout age. Since 1914, the majority (74.6%) of state-years specified a dropout age of

16; 8.8% are below 16; 7.8% use 17; and 8.7% use 18. Since the leaving age reached in 16 in all

states in 1993, when Mississippi increased its dropout age to 16, today’s policy debate centers on

raising the leaving age to 17 or 18.

12

Page 13: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

I use two indicators—1(dropout age = 16) and 1(dropout age ≥ 17)—to examine the effect

of these leaving ages on a given cohort, relative to the baseline of a dropout age below 16.5 The

former indicator primarily captures changes in dropout ages in the early and mid twentieth century,

while the latter captures more recent reforms that affected today’s younger generations. The results

below—which use recent surveys—separate between these thresholds, and show similar effects

when focusing only on dropout ages of 17 or above.

3.2 Identification strategy

To identify the effects of schooling on which party voters identify with and vote for later in life,

I leverage state-level reforms in the dropout age using a difference-in-differences design. I first

identify the political effects of raising the dropout age in its own right, before further applying an

IV strategy to identify the effect of completing an additional grade of schooling for individuals that

only remained in school because of changes in the dropout age affecting their cohort.

3.2.1 Reduced form: identifying the effect of increasing the school dropout age

To identify the political effects of state dropout laws, I use cohorts from states that did not change

their dropout age as controls for the same cohorts in states where a reform of the minimum leav-

ing age occurred. This difference-in-differences design thus separates out common trends across

cohorts in political support from the impact of dropout laws on cohorts in states where a reform

occurred. The design entails estimating equations of the form:

Yicst = δ11(dropout agecs = 16)+ δ21(dropout agecs ≥ 17)+

αc +θs +φsbirth yearc +ηt +Witγ + εicst , (1)

5The leaving ages of 17 and 18 are combined because forcing students only months from completinghigh school to remain in school until 18 has little impact on schooling outcomes (see also Lochner andMoretti 2004). Using 1(dropout age = 18) as an additional instrument, or using indicators for each mini-mum leaving age, provide similar results.

13

Page 14: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

where Yicst is an outcome (e.g. Republican identifier/voter) at survey year t for an individual i from

cohort c in state s. To exploit only variation across different cohorts within a state, the difference-

in-differences strategy includes fixed effects αc for birth-year (cohort) and fixed effects θs for the

state in which a respondent state grew up. Cohort fixed effects control for all differences across

cohorts, including generation effects reflecting political events that may have occurred at more im-

pressionable ages—such as early adulthood—for some cohorts (see Stoker and Bass 2011). State

fixed effects capture all time-invariant characteristics of states, such as varying levels of school-

ing and political support. In addition, state-specific linear cohort trends, φsbirth yearc, allows for

underlying trends in Yicst across cohorts to vary by state.6 Pre-treatment individual characteris-

tics Wit—gender and race indicators, and quartic age polynomial terms—are included to enhance

estimation efficiency, while survey year fixed effects ηt capture common shocks across electoral

campaign periods. Standard errors are clustered by state-cohort.

This estimation strategy thus compares changes in support for the Republican party across

cohorts within states that changed their dropout age to changes across cohorts within states that did

not. The key identifying assumption is that in the absence of changes in dropout laws, individuals

from treated states would experience parallel trends in Republican support to individuals from

control states. There are two main types of challenge to this assumption: individual selection into

particular dropout laws; and school dropout age reforms reflecting unobserved trends which also

affect Republican support.

One concern is that individuals likely to be impacted by the reform may move in response to

dropout age reforms. However, among poor women of childbearing age—whose children are more

likely to be remain in school only because they are required to—cross-state migration is especially

low (Molloy, Smith and Wozniak 2011), and is concentrated well before their children typically

reach high school age (Gelbach 2004). Furthermore, the principal reasons to move across states—

for college, marriage, family reasons, and natural disaster (Molloy, Smith and Wozniak 2011)—are

6Table A5 in the Appendix shows similar results excluding state-specific cohort trends.

14

Page 15: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

unlikely to be linked to dropout age reforms. Moreover, it is likely that if a parent is willing to

move to improve their child’s education, they would also persuade their child to remain in school

past the dropout age anyway. Robustness checks in the Appendix show that the selective migration

required to nullify the results is not plausible.

Since legislation is not randomly enacted, another concern is the context of dropout age re-

forms. Perhaps the most plausible violation of the exogeneity of minimum school leave ages with

respect to political outcomes is if reforms correlate with state-level political changes. For example,

state legislatures dominated by Republicans may disproportionately raise the minimum leaving

age. However, Table A2 in the Appendix shows that changes in the school leaving age are not

predicted by partisan political forces. Specifically, leaving ages are uncorrelated with Republican

control of upper, lower and both state houses, Republican state legislative seat shares, and Repub-

lican governors. The lack of such correlations suggest that dropout age reforms were unlikely to

have been associated with waves in local political sentiment.

Although political trends are uncorrelated with changes in state school leaving ages, reforms

could still reflect socioeconomic changes. However, Lochner and Moretti (2004) show that state

high school graduation trends are uncorrelated with school age reforms. More specific potential

concerns are addressed below by controlling for state-level political, educational, economic and

demographic conditions at the time when a voter attended high school. Most importantly, the state-

specific cohort trends in the baseline specification provide a demanding general check against the

possibility that leaving age reforms could reflect long-run processes that may differ across states,

such as changes in school quality (Stephens and Yang 2014), and that could also affect political

behavior.

3.2.2 Instrumental variables: identifying the effect of an additional grade of late high school

To estimate the effect of an additional year of schooling—as opposed to increasing the state’s

school dropout age—on identifying as and voting Republican, I build upon the reduced form

15

Page 16: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

specification by using the dropout age to instrument for schooling. I use the same difference-

in-differences design to estimate the following structural equation:

Yicst = βSicst +αc +θs +φsbirth yearc +ηt +Witγ + εicst , (2)

where Sicst is a measure of schooling received by individual i. I instrument for schooling by

estimating the following first stage:

Sicst = π11(dropout agecs = 16)+π21(dropout agecs ≥ 17)+

αc +θs +φsbirth yearc +ηt +Witγ +υicst . (3)

Using the predicted values from equation (3), IV estimates of equation (2) identify the local av-

erage causal response for dropout law compliers (Angrist and Imbens 1995), i.e. the effect of an

additional year of schooling on voters that only completed an additional year of schooling due to a

change in their state’s dropout age.7

In addition to the parallel trends assumption needed to estimate the effect of the minimum

school leaving age, identification further requires a strong first stage, that the instruments satisfy

monotonicity, and an exclusion restriction requiring that the dropout age only affects political

outcomes through increased schooling. First, the results in Table 3 below confirm that increasing

the dropout age significantly increases the level of schooling obtained. Second, it is unlikely that a

higher leaving age would cause an individual to choose less schooling, although Figure A3 in the

Appendix more formally supports the monotonicity assumption by showing that the cumulative

distribution of years of schooling for higher dropout ages lies strictly to the right of the distribution

for lower dropout ages. Third, additional education is temporally proximate to the point at which

the dropout age binds. Consequently, most downstream behavior is likely to be a function of a

7This estimand weights the reduced form effects of completing each grade of schooling by the proportioninduced by the respective instruments to achieve that grade.

16

Page 17: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

respondent’s level of schooling. There is thus limited scope for changes in the minimum school

leaving age to affect political preferences through channels other than additional education, and

thereby violate the exclusion restriction. I reinforce this claim empirically below, showing that

there is little evidence to support plausible violations.

3.3 Data

I use data from the National Annenberg Election Survey (NAES) and the American Community

Survey (ACS). The NAES collates rolling surveys conducted throughout the 2000, 2004 and 2008

Presidential election campaigns. More than 50,000 randomly-sampled adults were interviewed by

telephone over the period of each campaign, and together these yield a maximum pooled sample of

176,796 respondents.8 In addition to its large sample size, a key advantage of the NAES over other

widely used political surveys is the wide-ranging set of questions.9 The diverse range of politi-

cal outcomes is essential for assessing the mechanisms underpinning education’s political effects.

To demonstrate the robustness of the results, I also examine the American National Election Sur-

vey (ANES); this serves as an important out-of-sample robustness check because it uses different

survey protocols and covers all Congressional elections since 1952.

The ACS, which has conducted interviews with U.S. citizens at monthly intervals since 2000,

supplements the NAES’s political questions by measuring the number of completed grades of

schooling. To match the years when NAES respondents were surveyed, I use ACS data from the

publicly-available microdata from the 2000, 2001, 2003, 2004, 2007 and 2008 samples (Ruggles

et al. 2010).

I now briefly describe how the main variables are measured. Summary statistics are provided

in Table 1. The Appendix provides detailed variable definitions.8Interviews were conducted for around a year over the following months: 12/1999-1/2001, 10/2003-

11/2004, 12/2007-11/2008.9The General Social Survey collects the number of years of schooling, but only registers a respondent’s

Census division at age 16 (there are only 9 divisions). Furthermore, questions regarding the political mech-anisms are asked irregularly or not at all.

17

Page 18: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

3.3.1 Dependent variable: Republican support

I use three measures of support for the Republican party in the NAES. The first, which does not

rely on voter turnout, is partisan self-identification. In the sample, 31% of respondents identify as

Republicans, while 35% identify as Democrats; the residual are independents or “don’t know.”10

Throughout, I use an indicator to capture Republican partisanship. Secondly, I measure vote in-

tention at the forthcoming Presidential election, coding an indicator for intending to vote for the

Republican Presidential candidate (either George W. Bush or John McCain). Finally, I code an

indicator for whether a respondent voted Republican at the last Presidential election (for Bob Dole

or George W. Bush).11 In the sample, the Republican candidates received 47% of intended Pres-

idential votes while 45% of respondents actually voted for the Republican Presidential candidate

at the previous election.12 I show that the results are mirrored if Democrat indicators are used

instead.

3.3.2 Assigning state dropout ages

Survey respondents are mapped to the dropout age affecting their cohort based on their year of

birth and the state in which they resided when the minimum dropout age binds. Birth year cohort

is inferred from a respondent’s age when surveyed by the NAES, and given as year of birth in the

ACS.13 Survey respondents are then mapped to the dropout age in their state at the age at which

the law binds their decision to leave school. Since state of residence as a teenager is not available,

this is approximated by current state of residence in the NAES and state of birth in the ACS.14

10Considering only strong partisans and removing “don’t know” responses provides similar results.11Abstention is coded as 0, so result do not reflect turnout shifts.12This corresponds to 53% of voters that turned out for the Republican candidate. The Republican Presi-

dential candidate in the 1996, 2000, 2004 and 2008 elections ultimately received 40.7%, 47.9%, 50.7% and45.7% of the votes cast. Weighting by the NAES sample sizes, the vote share corresponding to the intendedvote measure is 48.7%, while 47.9% of voters reported voting Republican in the previous election.

13This approach is common and yields similar first stage results to studies using month of birth (Ace-moglu and Angrist 2000).

14Assigning dropout ages by current state of residence yields a similar first stage.

18

Page 19: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Tabl

e1:

Sum

mar

yst

atis

tics

Nat

iona

lAnn

enbe

rgE

lect

oral

Stud

yA

mer

ican

Com

mun

ities

Surv

eyO

bs.

Mea

nSt

d.de

v.M

in.

Max

.O

bs.

Mea

nSt

d.de

v.M

in.

Max

.

Dep

ende

ntva

riab

les

Rep

ublic

anpa

rtis

an17

6,79

60.

300.

460

1In

tend

tovo

teR

epub

lican

forP

resi

dent

144,

742

0.46

0.50

01

Vote

dR

epub

lican

forP

resi

dent

atla

stel

ectio

n12

5,19

60.

450.

500

1

Edu

catio

n(e

ndog

enou

sva

riab

les)

Com

plet

edgr

ades

443,

539

11.6

41.

330

12B

eyon

d12

thgr

ade

443,

539

0.52

0.50

01

Exc

lude

din

stru

men

tsD

ropo

utag

e=16

177,

653

0.72

0.45

01

443,

539

0.73

0.44

01

Dro

pout

age≥

1717

7,65

30.

260.

440

144

3,53

90.

250.

430

1

Pre

-tre

atm

entc

ontr

olva

riab

les

Age

177,

653

49.2

816

.67

1897

443,

539

49.3

016

.51

1895

Mal

e17

7,65

30.

440.

500

144

3,53

90.

440.

500

1W

hite

177,

653

0.87

0.34

01

443,

539

0.87

0.34

01

Bla

ck17

7,65

30.

080.

280

144

3,53

90.

080.

270

1A

sian

177,

653

0.01

0.08

01

443,

539

0.01

0.08

01

Coh

ort(

year

aged

14)

177,

653

1968

.59

16.4

019

2020

0444

3,53

919

68.6

916

.44

1920

2004

Surv

eyye

ar17

7,65

320

03.8

73.

0720

0020

0844

3,53

920

03.9

93.

0420

0020

08

Mec

hani

sms

Red

uce

tax

136,

870

0.35

0.48

01

Ban

abor

tion

131,

690

0.21

0.41

01

Low

gun

cont

rols

81,6

330.

380.

490

1L

owhe

alth

spen

ding

69,5

500.

310.

460

1hi

ghm

ilita

rysp

endi

ng78

,741

0.48

0.50

01

Rep

ublic

anno

n-ec

onom

icis

sues

163,

365

0.00

1.00

-1.1

52.

42Po

litic

alin

tere

stsc

ale

177,

624

0.00

1.00

-2.2

32.

33Po

litic

alkn

owle

dge

scal

e12

3,43

60.

001.

00-1

.19

1.36

Dis

cuss

polit

ics

168,

961

0.00

1.00

-1.1

81.

56Tu

rnou

t(pr

es.e

lect

ion)

150,

306

0.83

0.28

01

Trus

tfed

eral

gove

rnm

ent

54,6

310.

250.

430

1Pr

otec

tenv

iron

men

tmor

e12

6,52

20.

140.

350

1B

anga

ym

arri

age

72,3

350.

320.

470

1

19

Page 20: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

The absence of state of residence at high school age could induce bias if compliers supporting

particular political parties systematically migrate to states with different dropout ages. However,

there is little evidence to justify this concern. First, relatively few respondents move across states:

61% of ACS respondents live in their state of birth, while 72% of ANES respondents live in

the state they lived in at age 14. Moreover, I show below that increasing the dropout age does

not increase the likelihood of attending college—a key source of cross-state migration. Third,

a bounding exercise in the Appendix demonstrates that, even under generous assumptions, the

selective migration required to account for the results is not plausible. Finally, the Appendix shows

that the ANES dataset—which measures state of residence at age 14—yields similar estimates.

3.3.3 Completed grades of high school

To translate the reduced form effect into an IV estimate for completing an additional grade of

schooling, a measure of the number of grades of completed schooling is required. The NAES

only measures completing high school, but does not distinguish between finer levels of partial

high school completion that different dropout ages could impact. While previous studies have

used an indicator for completing high school (e.g. Lochner and Moretti 2004; Milligan, Moretti

and Oreopoulos 2004), Marshall (2016) shows that this is likely to significantly upwardly bias

the magnitude of IV estimates. Intuitively, this is because coarsening years of schooling into a

dichotomous variable for completing high school causes the first stage to only capture the effect of

the instrument on completing high school, neglecting the effect of the dropout age on increasing

levels of schooling without inducing the completion of high school. Consequently, the exclusion

restriction is violated if any other level of schooling (induced by changes in the dropout age) affects

identifying as or voting Republican. For example, Marshall (2016) shows in the United Kingdom

that the effects of education are not solely a function of graduating from high school, but rather are

monotonically increasing in the number of years of schooling, and thus upwardly bias estimates of

20

Page 21: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

completing high school by three times.15

Fortunately, an interval measure of schooling, like the number of completed grades of school-

ing, allows for consistent estimation of the local average causal response, provided—as in this

case—that multiple grades are affected by the instrument (Marshall 2016). This causal quantity

weights the local average treatment effect for each additional year by the extent to which leaving

age laws contribute to the first stage for each additional year of schooling.

Since years of schooling is not measured in the NAES (or the ANES), I combine the NAES data

with data from the ACS using two-sample IV methods (see below). The ACS provides fine-grained

data on each citizen’s education, and permits schooling to be measured by the number of grades

completed. To avoid complications with coding different types of post-high school education, the

variable is top-coded at completing 12th grade, and thus ranges from 0 to 12. This coding is

inconsequential because, as shown in Table 3 below, dropout ages do not affect schooling beyond

12th grade.

3.4 Two-sample IV estimation

For the IV estimates, I use two-sample 2SLS (TS2SLS) to estimate equation (2). This entails

estimating the first stage in equation (3) using the ACS dataset and the reduced form in equation

(1) using the NAES dataset, before efficiently combining the two as a consistent two-step estimator

of the effect of each additional grade of schooling (Inoue and Solon 2010).16 This approach was

first proposed by Angrist and Krueger (1992) and Franklin (1989), and has since been employed to

answer a variety of empirical questions (see Angrist and Pischke 2008). I derive the cluster-robust

covariance matrix for the TS2SLS estimator in the Appendix. The reduced form estimates require

only the NAES data, and thus do not rely on two-sample methods.

Beyond the standard IV assumptions discussed above, TS2SLS requires that the reduced form

15Table A7 in the Appendix provides IV estimates instrumenting for completing high school, and reportsimplausibly large estimates consistent with coarsening bias.

16TS2SLS is implemented in R; the program is available in the replication code.

21

Page 22: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

and first stage datasets independently sample from the same population (Inoue and Solon 2010).

This in turn implies that key sample moments—variable means, variances and cross-products—are

identical in expectation, and are thus “exchangeable.” Both the ACS and NAES sample voting age

citizens, once ineligible voters from the NAES sample and those aged below 18 and born outside

the U.S. are removed from the ACS sample. To address possible differences in response rates, I

guarantee that the sample moments match by stratifying by year of birth, male, race and survey

year to randomly draw ACS observations to replicate the distribution of NAES respondents over

these pre-treatment covariates.17 Full details of this procedure are provided in the Appendix. The

summary statistics in Table 1 demonstrate that this approach almost exactly replicates the first

two moments for each variable in the samples. Encouragingly, dropout ages—which were not

matched—are also produce almost identical sample moments across the two datasets. Table A4 in

the Appendix shows similar first stage estimates without using this matching procedure.

4 Effects of high school dropout age and completed grades of

high school on Republican support

I now present the downstream political effects of raising the high school dropout age. The main

finding is that both higher dropout ages and each additional grade of high school substantially

increase the probability that an individual will identify with, or vote for, the Republican party later

in life.

4.1 Reduced form estimates: raising the dropout age

I first estimate the effect of increasing the school dropout age on the propensity of an individual

to identify as or vote Republican. Columns (1)-(3) in panel A of Table 2 report the reduced form

17By correcting for any differences between samples, TS2SLS is also consistent when sampling ratesdiffer with any control variable (Inoue and Solon 2010).

22

Page 23: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

difference-in-differences estimates, where a positive coefficients signifies an increase in support

for the Republican party. Respectively, columns (1), (2) and (3) report the effects on identifying as

a Republican partisan, intending to vote for the Republican Presidential candidate, and voting for

the Republican Presidential candidate at the previous Presidential election. Columns (4)-(6) report

analogous measures for the Democratic party.

Table 2: The effect of school dropout age on identifying as and voting Republican

Republican support Democrat supportPartisan Intend Vote Partisan Intend Vote

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Dropout age=16 0.013 0.035*** 0.026* -0.017 -0.026** -0.010(0.011) (0.013) (0.014) (0.011) (0.012) (0.014)

Dropout age≥17 0.030** 0.050*** 0.043*** -0.029** -0.037*** -0.028*(0.012) (0.014) (0.016) (0.012) (0.013) (0.015)

Observations 176,796 144,742 125,196 176,796 144,742 125,196Outcome mean 0.30 0.46 0.45 0.33 0.42 0.36Test: Dropout age=16 = 0.002 0.010 0.006 0.019 0.050 0.004

Dropout age≥17 (p value)

Notes: Outcome variables are: identifying as a Republican/Democrat partisan (“partisan”), intendingto vote Republican/Democrat for President (“intend”), and voting Republican/Democrat in previousPresidential election (“vote”). All specifications include male, white, black and Asian dummies, quartic(demeaned) age polynomials, state-specific cohort trends, and state grew up, cohort and survey yearfixed effects, and are estimated using OLS. Differences in the number of observations reflect differencesin the NAES modules in which each outcome was included. The omitted dropout age category is dropoutage≤15. Standard errors clustered by state-cohort in parentheses. * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01.

The results show that raising the dropout age significantly increases Republican support. The

first coefficient estimate in each column indicates that, compared to cohorts in states requiring

students to remain in school until at most age 15, raising the dropout age to 16 increases the

proportion of Republican partisans by 1.3 percentage points, those intending to vote Republican

by 3.5 percentage points and those actually voting Republican by 2.6 percentage points. Relative

to the sample mean, these estimates imply around a 5% increase in Republican support among

cohorts affected by the reform, and thus indicate that changing the dropout age substantially alters

23

Page 24: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

partisan identification and voting behavior.

To examine the more topical question relating to the impact of further increasing the dropout

age to 17 or above, the first coefficient in each column can be subtracted from the second. These

comparisons shows that, relative to cohorts in states requiring students to remain in school until age

16, raising the dropout age to 17 or higher increases the proportion of Republican partisans by 1.7

percentage points, those intending to vote Republican by 1.5 percentage points and those actually

voting Republican by 1.7 percentage points. Coefficient tests at the foot of Table 2 demonstrate that

each of these differences is statistically significant. The results again imply substantial increases

in support, of around 3-5%, for the Republican party. The slightly larger effect at 16 most likely

reflects the comparison with a more varied omitted category containing respondents facing laws

mandating that students only remain in school until between 12 and 15. Most students in this group

face a leaving age of 14.

Columns (4)-(6) show broadly commensurate decreases in Democrat support, suggesting that

increasing the dropout age causes would-be Democrat supporters to become Republican support-

ers. For both dropout age indicators, comparing the increase in Republican support to the decrease

in Democrat support indicates that relatively few voters become non-partisans or independents,

vote for third-party candidates or abstain. At least among those affected by the reform, this sug-

gests that staying in high school is able to reverse the powerful early life forces that otherwise drive

voters to become Democrats.

4.2 Instrumental variables estimates: completing an additional grade of late

high school

The IV estimates identify the effect of completing an additional grade of high school for individuals

that only remained in school because of an increase in the dropout age affecting their state-cohort.

In contrast with the reduced form estimates, which aggregate across entire state-cohorts, these

24

Page 25: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

estimates apply only to those individuals induced to remain in education by dropout law reforms.

In Table 3, the first stage estimates from the ACS sample is presented in column (1), column (2)

examines the effect on post-high school education, and the IV estimates are presented in columns

(3)-(5).18

Table 3: The effect of an additional completed grade of high school on identifying as and votingRepublican

Completed Beyond Republican supportgrades 12th grade Partisan Intend VoteOLS OLS TS2SLS TS2SLS TS2SLS(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)

Dropout age=16 0.248*** -0.013*(0.037) (0.007)

Dropout age≥17 0.284*** -0.007(0.040) (0.008)

Completed grades 0.089* 0.165*** 0.140**(0.046) (0.055) (0.058)

First stage (ACS) observations 443,539 443,539 443,539 436,333 443,539Reduced form (NAES) observations 176,796 144,742 125,196Outcome mean 11.64 0.51 0.33 0.42 0.36First stage F statistic 25.4 2.8 25.4 25.4 25.4Test: Dropout age=16 = 0.004 0.107

Dropout age≥17 (p value)

Notes: See Table 2. First stage observations decline for vote intention because the ACS sample isreduced to match the variables in the NAES sample.

The first stage estimates in column (1) show that raising the dropout age significantly increases

schooling. Relative to state-cohorts facing a dropout age below 15, raising the leaving age to 16

increases the average number of grades of schooling completed by 0.25 grades. Further raising the

dropout age to at least 17 keeps students in school for an additional 0.04 grades. These positive

effects are broadly similar in magnitude to previous estimates pertaining to cohorts born somewhat

earlier in the twentieth century (Acemoglu and Angrist 2000; Dee 2004; Stephens and Yang 2014).

18The results for Democrat support are similar to Table 2, and henceforth omitted to save space.

25

Page 26: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Moreover, the F statistic at the foot of column (1) indicates that the dropout age instrument is

sufficiently strong to avoid weak instrument biases.

To understand which levels of schooling the IV estimates pertain to, it is important to examine

which levels of education are affected by the instruments. While column (1) demonstrated that

raising the dropout age increases in the number of completed grades of schooling up to high school

level, it is possible that the minimum school leaving age also influences post-secondary education.

However, column (2) shows that, on average, increasing the dropout age does not increase the

probability that an individual completes schooling beyond 12th grade (including any amount of

college).19 Importantly, the estimates in this article thus only apply to completing additional years

of high school, without also capturing potentially countervailing effects of attending college.

Reinforcing the reduced form findings, the TS2SLS estimates in panel C show that high school

has a large pro-Republican effect among those induced to remain in school by a higher dropout

age. Columns (3)-(5) show that an additional grade of late high school substantially increases

the probability that an individual identifies as a Republican or supports the Republican Presiden-

tial candidate. This effect is particularly large for vote choices, where an additional year of late

high school increases the probability of voting Republican by approximately 15 percentage points.

Nevertheless, the 9 percentage point increases in Republican partisan identification is also notable

and, combined with the reduced form estimates, suggests that high school education has major

downstream conservative effects on voters. These estimates likely reflect particularly large effects

of late high school among a group of compliers from relatively disadvantaged backgrounds where

education is uncommon, and thus the marginal effects of schooling on life opportunities may be

greatest.

19Acemoglu and Angrist (2000) and Lochner and Moretti (2004) also find that university attendance isunaffected in different samples.

26

Page 27: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table 4: Heterogeneity in the effect of school dropout age on schooling and identifying as andvoting Republican, by black and Hispanic voters

Completed Republican supportgrades Partisan Intend Vote

(1) (2) (3) (4)

Dropout age=16 0.047 0.004 0.020 0.019(0.042) (0.013) (0.014) (0.015)

Dropout age=16 × Black or Hispanic 0.917*** 0.042** 0.063*** 0.058**(0.139) (0.018) (0.020) (0.027)

Dropout age≥17 0.067 0.025* 0.037** 0.032*(0.045) (0.014) (0.016) (0.016)

Dropout age≥17× Black or Hispanic 1.003*** 0.027 0.059*** 0.086***(0.140) (0.019) (0.021) (0.028)

Observations 443,539 176,796 144,742 125,196

Note: See Table 2.

4.3 Which voters do dropout age reforms induce to become Republicans?

It is essential to recognize which types of voters fail to complete high school in order to understand

which voters were induced, by school leaving age reforms, to support the Republican party. As

Murnane (2013) shows, blacks and Hispanics are significantly less likely than whites to graduate

from high school by age 20-24. Around 80% of each cohort of white voters born since World War

II have consistently completed high school. In contrast, only 60% of black and Hispanic students

born just after World War II completed high school, although the difference relative to whites

dropped below 10% for those born in the 1980s.

Given that rates of high school completion among whites are relatively high, changes in the

dropout age are likely to disproportionately influence black and Hispanic voters.20 The heteroge-

neous effects in column (1) of Table 4 show that increases in the dropout age indeed principally

impact respondents that identify as either black or Hispanic. In fact, most of the increase in com-

pleted school grades is driven by such students: the results show that for each dropout age category,

20Higher female high school graduation rates are also reflected in unreported results.

27

Page 28: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

the increase in completed grades of school is primarily driven by black and Hispanic students—

especially in the case of raising the dropout age to 16.21

Consistent with the types of students induced to remain in school by higher school leaving

age laws, columns (2)-(4) confirm that increases in Republican support are also primarily driven

by blacks and Hispanics.22 In particular, the effect of raising the leaving age to 16 is entirely

concentrated among black or Hispanic students, while the effect of raising the leaving age to 17

or above is 2-4 times greater among such minorities. These results indicate that, because of their

lower baseline probability of remaining in high school, raising the school leaving age has induced

significant numbers of black and Hispanic voters to vote Republican.23 After demonstrating the

robustness of these estimates, I investigate the mechanisms underlying this effect.

4.4 Robustness checks

The main findings rest on two key identifying assumptions. The parallel trends assumption is

required for both the reduced form and IV estimates. The IV estimates additionally require an ex-

clusion restriction that the dropout age only affects political behavior through additional schooling.

I now show that the results are robust to potential violations of these assumptions.

4.4.1 Parallel trends

The state-specific cohort trends included in the baseline specifications provide a powerful check

against general parallel trend concerns. Another common test includes lags and leads of dropout

age reforms (see Angrist and Pischke 2008). If the estimates indeed pick up the effects of the

reform, rather than differential pre-trends across states that did and did not make reforms, leads

21The difference between the two lower-order dropout age coefficients is not quite statistically significant,but suggests that some white voters also remained in school longer in states raising the dropout age to 17 ormore.

22The IV estimates are omitted due to the weak first stage for non-black/Hispanic students.23Given that schooling and Republican support increase broadly commensurately, it is unlikely that dif-

ferences in how black and Hispanic voters respond to high school drive these heterogeneous effects.

28

Page 29: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

-.05

0.0

5.1

Mar

gina

l effe

ct

-5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4Cohort relative to the reform

Effect of dropout age=16 on Republican partisanship

-.02

0.0

2.0

4M

argi

nal e

ffect

-5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4Cohort relative to the reform

Differential effect of dropout age>=17 on Republican partisanship-.0

50

.05

.1.1

5M

argi

nal e

ffect

-5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4Cohort relative to the reform

Effect of dropout age=16 on Republican vote intention

-.02

0.0

2.0

4.0

6M

argi

nal e

ffect

-5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4Cohort relative to the reform

Differential effect of dropout age>=17 on Republican vote intention

-.1-.0

50.0

5.1

.15

Mar

gina

l effe

ct

-5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4Cohort relative to the reform

Effect of dropout age=16 on Republican vote

-.02

0.0

2.0

4.0

6M

argi

nal e

ffect

-5 -4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4Cohort relative to the reform

Differential effect of dropout age>=17 on Republican vote

Figure 2: Effect of raising the dropout age on cohorts either side of the reform date

Notes: All estimates from specifications including five pre- and post-reform variables. Bars represent95% confidence intervals.

should not affect Republican support. Figure 2 examines this expectation, comparing the five co-

horts before and after a reform. Consistent with the parallel trends assumption, there is limited

evidence of pre-trends and a relatively durable increase in Republican support among cohorts af-

fected by an increase in the minimum school leaving age. The increase in Republican support

among cohorts facing an increased dropout age of 16 is somewhat more volatile, while the addi-

tional effect of increasing the leaving age to 17 or above is precise and durable.

Nevertheless, these general checks—linear state-specific cohort trends, and lags and leads—

could still fail to capture certain more particular concerns. Accordingly, I also simultaneously

29

Page 30: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

control for various potential confounds that vary across states and cohorts around the time when

respondents attended high school. First, I include the Presidential vote share by state at the most

recent election to control for changes in state political context when respondents were in high

school.24 Second, statewide labor market opportunities are proxied for by state personal income

per capita at ages 16 and 18. Third, I include the state’s pupil-teacher ratio to control for changes

in the quality of education. Finally, the black proportion of the state population is included to

control for the possibility that white voters start to support the Republican party as the share of

black voters rises. Columns (1)-(3) of Table 5 show that controlling for these state-level variables

at age 14, 16 and 18 does not meaningfully alter the results.

It is also possible that the types of states that changed their dropout laws were also starting

to follow a different, and potentially non-linear, trajectory that differentially influenced different

cohorts. As Figure 1 shows, many of the reforms were concentrated in the south and midwest—

areas that have experienced substantial economic and demographic changes over the past 50 years

in which this sample attended school. To ensure that other experiences which could differ across

cohorts facing different dropout ages within these states, column (4) of Table 5 includes cohort-

region fixed effects.25 The results show that, even when only exploiting changes in the minimum

schooling leaving age across cohorts within similar parts of the U.S., the dropout age and com-

pleted grades of schooling continue to substantially increase Republican support.

A final issue is that the electoral context of the survey differentially impacts certain types of

voter that may also have faced higher school leaving ages. I address this by including cohort-

survey year and state-survey year fixed effects, and thus leveraging the difference-in-differences

variation only from within each year of a Presidential election campaign. The results in column

(5) reinforce the robustness of the findings.

24At the cost of sample size, similar results are obtained when controlling for state House and state SenateRepublican seat shares.

25Census regions are: midwest, northeast, south and west.

30

Page 31: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table 5: Further parallel trends checks

Cohort-Cohort- and state-region year

State-level controls at... fixed fixed...age 14 ...age 16 ...age 18 effects effects

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5)

Panel A: Reduced form effect on Republican partisanship

Dropout age=16 0.011 0.010 0.008 -0.000 0.013(0.012) (0.013) (0.012) (0.012) (0.011)

Dropout age≥16 0.028** 0.027* 0.026* 0.018 0.029**(0.013) (0.014) (0.013) (0.014) (0.013)

Observations 173,605 171,202 171,146 176,714 176,674Test: Dropout age=16 = Dropout age≥17 (p value) 0.003 0.003 0.001 0.001 0.003

Panel B: Reduced form effect on Republican vote intention

Dropout age=16 0.032** 0.026* 0.025* 0.024* 0.034***(0.014) (0.014) (0.014) (0.014) (0.013)

Dropout age≥16 0.045*** 0.040** 0.041*** 0.037** 0.049***(0.015) (0.016) (0.015) (0.015) (0.014)

Observations 142,265 140,365 140,235 144,677 144,742Test: Dropout age=16 = Dropout age≥17 (p value) 0.037 0.025 0.010 0.030 0.012

Panel C: Reduced form effect on Republican vote

Dropout age=16 0.018 0.016 0.021 0.017 0.025*(0.015) (0.016) (0.015) (0.015) (0.014)

Dropout age≥16 0.034** 0.031* 0.036** 0.032* 0.043***(0.017) (0.017) (0.017) (0.016) (0.015)

Observations 122,942 120,914 120,247 125,163 125,196Test: Dropout age=16 = Dropout age≥17 (p value) 0.027 0.032 0.042 0.022 0.008

Panel D: IV effect of schooling on Republican partisanship

Completed grades 0.167** 0.235*** 0.191** 0.244** 0.076*(0.073) (0.086) (0.089) (0.121) (0.048)

First stage F statistic 17.0 16.9 17.0 10.0 24.3

Panel E: IV effect of schooling on Republican vote intention

Completed grades 0.199** 0.243** 0.208** 0.297** 0.149***(0.085) (0.098) (0.101) (0.131) (0.053)

First stage F statistic 14.1 14.0 14.1 10.4 24.3

Panel F: IV effect of schooling on Republican vote

Completed grades 0.202** 0.268** 0.235** 0.304** 0.135**(0.088) (0.105) (0.109) (0.146) (0.059)

First stage F statistic 12.3 12.3 12.3 10.0 24.3

Notes: See Table 2. State-level controls include: the pupil-teacher ratio, the previous Republican Presidential vote share, state income per

capita, and the black proportion of the population. Differences in the number of observations in columns (1)-(3) reflects missingness on state-

level controls. Column (4) drops those born before 1909 due to the lack of observations for estimating cohort-region fixed effects. Column (5)

drops observations from cohort-survey year and state-survey year cells with 1 or fewer observations. The number of reduced form (NAES)

observations for IV specifications is identical to the corresponding reduced form outcome panel. Panels A-C are estimated using OLS and

panels D-F are estimated using TS2SLS.

31

Page 32: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

4.4.2 Exclusion restriction

The preceding robustness checks provide strong support for the parallel trends assumption under-

pinning the reduced form estimates, but also suggest that the main estimates are robust to con-

trolling for plausible exclusion restriction violations. Nevertheless, it still remains possible that

the dropout age could have directly increased Republican support through other channels; this

would upwardly bias the IV estimates, although the reduced form estimates would remain unbi-

ased. Given the temporal proximity of a binding dropout age to the decision to remain in school,

the most plausible exclusion restriction violations reflect short-term or concurrent changes induced

by school leaving age reforms.

First, by simply keeping a student out of the labor force (rather than through the direct effects

of an additional year of schooling), a greater minimum leaving age could affect early life choices

like marriage and fertility. If, for example, schooling reduced early marriage and fertility, then

students may be more likely to avoid interaction with the welfare state and ultimately support

the Republican party. However, Table A6 in the Appendix shows that the dropout age does not

systematically decrease the likelihood that a respondent has ever been married or lower the age of

either a respondent’s oldest or youngest child living at home.

Second, raising (or lowering) the minimum leaving age could create cross-cohort spillovers,

causing older (younger) cohorts to behave more like treated cohorts. However, this would re-

duce between-cohort differences within states, and thus downwardly bias estimates of schooling’s

effects.

Finally, I conduct a sensitivity analysis to calculate the extent of exclusion restriction viola-

tion required to nullify the results. Using Conley, Hansen and Rossi’s (2012) most conservative

sensitivity test—their union of confidence intervals method—I show in the Appendix that almost

one half of the reduced form effect of the dropout age on the Republican vote outcomes must

operate through avenues other than schooling for the TS2SLS estimates to become statistically

32

Page 33: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

insignificant at the 90% level. Given the preceding checks, such large violations are unlikely.

5 Mechanisms

Having shown that staying in high school increases support for the Republican party, I now ex-

amine the channels through which this substantial effect operates. I use the same difference-in-

differences and IV methods to test whether the dropout age and additional completed grades of

high school affect the mechanisms underpinning the income and socialization-based theories con-

sidered above.26 The results indicate that, by increasing income, schooling affects economic policy

preferences, which in turn increase the likelihood that such voters ultimately support the Republi-

can party.

5.1 Education increases income and opposition to taxation

I provide four important pieces of evidence consistent with the income-based RMR channel. First,

although potentially common to some alternative explanations, it is well-established that education

increases wages. As noted above, studies using school dropout ages as instruments consistently

show that each additional year of schooling increases wages at peak working age in the U.S. by

around 10% (see Acemoglu and Angrist 2000; Angrist and Krueger 1991; Oreopoulos 2006).

Second, although it is reasonable to expect voters to support the party that they expect will

benefit them in the future (Alesina and La Ferrara 2005), if education is driving opposition to taxa-

tion by significantly increasing an individual’s income, then this effect should be most pronounced

when the return to education is greatest—at an individual’s earnings peak. To examine this im-

plication, I compare the effects of the dropout age on the 40% of respondents aged between 45

and 65 at the date of the survey to all other respondents. Based on the ACS sample, wages are

26Although establishing the effects of potential mediators requires strong assumptions, examining a rangeof potential mediators and placebo-like tests can support some mechanisms and eliminate others.

33

Page 34: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

highest between these ages.27 The results in Table 6 show that the effect of the dropout age on

support for the Republican party is indeed substantially higher among 45-65 year-olds: the inter-

actions indicate that, at an individual’s earnings peak, the effect of raising both dropout age levels

on identifying as and voting Republican are around 15 percentage points higher, while intention to

vote Republican is also higher but is not statistically significant. This indicates that realizing the

returns to education is a key component of education’s political effect, although the estimates still

suggest a non-zero effect outside the peak earnings period. These results further imply that even

though voter policy preferences are likely to be sticky from early adulthood (Erikson and Tedin

2015; Stoker and Bass 2011), they can nevertheless respond to changing economic incentives over

their lifecycle.

The lack of differential effect on years of schooling in column (4) shows that these earnings

peak findings do not reflect the possibility that higher minimum schooling leaving ages produced

larger effects on education for a particular generation. Moreover, columns (5)-(8) show that the

results are robust to simultaneously controlling for the interaction of the earnings peak indicator

with all other fixed effects and covariates. Most importantly, such interactive controls ensure that

the results reflect variation across voters within cohorts around their earnings peak, and therefore

are not driven by certain cohorts generally behaving differently around their earnings peak.

Third, I use survey attitudes to examine whether additional grades of schooling increase op-

position to taxation—the key driver of the RMR model. The IV estimate in column (1) of Table

7 shows that educated respondents regard themselves as more conservative when asked to place

themselves on a (standardized) 100-point liberal-to-conservative scale; Table A8 in the Appendix

shows similar reduced form estimates. An additional year of late high school thus equates to almost

a quarter of a standard deviation increase in conservative preferences. Although economic policy

preferences are an important element determining how voters place themselves on such scales (e.g.

27I regressed (log) wage on dummies for age, controlling for state and cohort fixed effects. The coeffi-cients for ages 45-65 systematically produced the largest coefficients (ignoring the tiny fraction of workingrespondents aged 80 or higher).

34

Page 35: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table 6: Heterogeneous effects of dropout ages on Republican support by earnings peak

Partisan Intend Vote Schooling(1) (2) (3) (4)

Panel A: Baseline estimatesDropout age=16 0.012 0.034*** 0.018 0.216***

(0.011) (0.013) (0.015) (0.037)Dropout age=16 × Earnings peak 0.143*** 0.059 0.169*** -0.118

(0.034) (0.066) (0.042) (0.108)Dropout age≥17 0.029** 0.048*** 0.034** 0.238***

(0.012) (0.014) (0.017) (0.038)Dropout age≥17 × Earnings peak 0.142*** 0.063 0.168*** -0.141

(0.035) (0.066) (0.043) (0.108)

Observations 176,796 144,742 106,879 440,892

Panel B: Including earnings peak interactive controlsDropout age=16 0.001 0.018 0.008 0.126***

(0.012) (0.014) (0.017) (0.039)Dropout age=16 × Earnings peak 0.183*** 0.101 0.190*** -0.112

(0.047) (0.084) (0.055) (0.134)Dropout age≥17 0.012 0.027* 0.015 0.152***

(0.013) (0.016) (0.019) (0.040)Dropout age≥17 × Earnings peak 0.189*** 0.126 0.198*** -0.164

(0.049) (0.086) (0.057) (0.138)

Observations 176,796 144,742 106,879 440,892

Notes: See Table 2. Earnings peak is an indicator for respondents aged between 45 and 65. Thespecifications in panel B interact all covariates with the earnings peak indicator. Differences in samplesize reflect data availability.

Conover and Feldman 1981), this evidence does not necessarily pinpoint economic policies as the

source of the change. To better capture economic policy preferences, column (2) shows that an

additional grade of high school increases the belief that taxes should be reduced by 14 percentage

points. Furthermore, this belief is strongly correlated with identifying with or voting for the Re-

publican party,28 and thus consistent with the argument that high school causes voters to become

28While this link from potential mediator to outcome cannot be well-identified, specifications akin toequation (1) show that voters believing that taxes should be reduced are more than 10 percentage pointsmore likely to be Republican partisans or vote for the Republican Presidential candidate.

35

Page 36: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

more fiscally conservative, and ultimately support the Republican party.

Fourth, if voters adopt the policy positions of the political party or candidate they identify

with (e.g. Lenz 2012; Zaller 1992), changes in economic policy preferences could instead re-

flect changes in partisanship. If voters reflect their party’s positions, they should also shift toward

prominent Republican stances on other issues. Columns (3)-(6) of Table 7 provide no evidence

for such changes: an additional completed grade of high school does not significantly increase

the likelihood that respondents oppose abortion, gun control, school spending, health spending,

or cutting military spending.29 The effects of schooling on these issues are consistently smaller

in magnitude than schooling’s effect on economic policy preferences, while the coefficients for

military spending, gun controls and banning abortion point in the opposite direction to what the

correlation with Republican support would suggest. Combining all the non-economic policy indi-

cators as a scale, column (7) shows that schooling does not shift respondents toward Republican

positions on non-economic policy issues. These results are in line with Gerber, Huber and Wash-

ington (2010), who find that experimentally inducing a shift in the party a voter identifies with

does not affect their policy positions. In sum, these tests strongly suggest that it is economic policy

preferences that drive the political choices of those receiving more high school education.

5.2 No evidence of political socialization

The socialization theories reviewed above argue that schooling cultivates political engagement and

induces voters to adopt socially liberal values. Although such changes are typically associated with

supporting the Democratic Party, it remains possible that socialization mechanisms operate along-

side income channels or instead drive support for the Republicans. However, I find no evidence

for the basic mechanisms underpinning these theories.

The civic education and social network hypotheses emphasize the importance of changes in

political engagement in changing political attitudes. However, consistent with existing research

29These issues are all strongly correlated with identifying as and voting Republican in the NAES surveys.

36

Page 37: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Tabl

e7:

The

effe

ctof

anad

ditio

nalc

ompl

eted

grad

eof

high

scho

olon

pote

ntia

linc

ome

and

soci

aliz

atio

nm

echa

nism

s

Con

serv

ativ

eR

educ

eB

anL

owL

owH

igh

Rep

ublic

ansc

ale

taxe

s†ab

ortio

n†gu

nhe

alth

mili

tary

non-

econ

omic

cont

rols

†sp

endi

ng†

spen

ding

†is

sues

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

(7)

Com

plet

edgr

ades

0.21

9**

0.13

7***

-0.0

38-0

.078

0.06

4-0

.028

-0.1

23(0

.092

)(0

.053

)(0

.048

)(0

.065

)(0

.072

)(0

.066

)(0

.093

)

Red

uced

form

(NA

ES)

obse

rvat

ions

172,

400

136,

830

131,

662

81,0

2269

,513

78,7

0316

3,31

4Fi

rsts

tage

(AC

S)ob

serv

atio

ns44

3,53

944

1,12

044

1,12

030

6,63

630

9,41

730

9,41

744

1,12

0Fi

rsts

tage

Fst

atis

tic25

.425

.725

.716

.717

.817

.825

.7

(8)

(9)

(10)

(11)

(12)

(13)

(14)

Polit

ical

Polit

ical

Dis

cuss

Turn

out

Trus

tPr

otec

tB

ankn

owle

dge

inte

rest

polit

ics

(pre

s.fe

dera

len

viro

.ga

ysc

ale

scal

eel

ectio

n)go

vt.†

mor

e†m

arri

age†

Com

plet

edgr

ades

-0.0

93-0

.048

-0.1

100.

037

0.01

3-0

.063

0.19

3*(0

.104

)(0

.100

)(0

.083

)(0

.037

)(0

.072

)(0

.040

)(0

.111

)

Red

uced

form

obse

rvat

ions

123,

411

177,

624

168,

961

150,

266

54,6

3112

6,52

272

,313

Firs

tsta

ge(A

CS)

obse

rvat

ions

414,

146

443,

539

443,

539

441,

120

438,

752

311,

836

185,

840

Firs

tsta

geF

stat

istic

24.7

25.4

25.4

25.7

25.1

17.8

8.3

Not

es:

See

Tabl

e2,

and

the

App

endi

xfo

rde

taile

dva

riab

lede

finiti

ons.

All

outc

ome

vari

able

sar

est

anda

rdiz

ed,e

xcep

tbin

ary

outc

omes

deno

ted

by†.

All

spec

ifica

tions

are

estim

ated

usin

gT

S2SL

S.D

iffer

ence

sin

sam

ple

size

refle

ctda

taav

aila

bilit

y.

37

Page 38: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

(Galston 2001), columns (8) and (9) of Table 7 show that an additional completed grade of high

school does not significantly affect, and if anything reduces, multi-item scales of political knowl-

edge or political interest. Furthermore, columns (10)-(12) identify no link between schooling and

greater discussion of politics with friends or family, self-reported turnout in the last Presidential

election, or trust in the federal government. These findings suggest that voters induced to remain in

high school by higher dropout ages are not induced to engage and participate in politics more, and

thus that changes in partisanship and vote choice reflect party switching rather than mobilization.30

Moreover, policy and issue preferences do not change in accordance with socialization expla-

nations. As noted above, there is no evidence to suggest that greater schooling is associated with

socially liberal positions on gun control or abortion. Columns (13) and (14) further show that

completing an additional grade of high school is not significantly correlated with support for envi-

ronmental protection, and if anything increases support for constitutionally banning gay marriage.

In combination with not voting for the Democrats, I thus find no evidence to suggest that voters

become more socially liberal on civil liberties issues.

6 Implications of raising the dropout age for electoral outcomes

The preceding analysis has identified large political effects of implementing policies to increase

the dropout age. The increases in Republican support are particularly notable, given that many

students in any given cohort would remain in school regardless of the dropout age. This suggests

that, at least in politically competitive areas, the changes in voting behavior could have significantly

altered state-level electoral outcomes, at least once the number of affected cohorts accumulated to

reach a critical mass.31

Quantifying such effects on electoral outcomes is challenging for at least two reasons. First,

30Recent studies highlight college’s positive correlation with political engagement (e.g. Henderson andChatfield 2011; Mendelberg, McCabe and Thal forthcoming). The lack of change in engagement may thusreflect dropout laws not impacting college attendance.

31The NAES does not consistently ask about state elections.

38

Page 39: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

it is difficult to estimate the proportion of the electorate affected by dropout age reforms at any

given election. Second, as shown above, the effects of schooling become most pronounced in

later life when the returns to education are greatest. Both challenges imply that the effect of

raising the dropout age on electoral outcomes may only become noticeable many decades after the

reform, once those affected constitute a large proportion of the electorate and are most likely to

vote according to their higher midlife income. To tentatively estimate the electoral effects of raising

the minimum school leaving age, I use the following difference-in-differences specification:

Yst = δ11(dropout agest−45 = 16)+ δ21(dropout agest−45 ≥ 17)+θs +ηt +ϕsyeart + εst , (4)

where Yst is a measure of state-level Republican political representation in state s in election year

t between 1959 and 2010. The dropout age is lagged by 45 years—approximately the number of

years after which those first affected by the reform reach the end of their earnings peak—to allow

the number of affected cohorts to accumulate over time and reach the peak effect of high school’s

influence on political behavior. State-specific trends, ϕsyeart , are again included to address the

possibility that trends in Republican representation in those states which implemented a leaving

age reform 45 years earlier are not parallel to those that did not.

The results in Table 8 suggest that prior dropout age reforms have significantly influenced state

electoral outcomes. Columns (1) and (3) show that increasing the leaving age to 16 increased the

Republican seat share in the state House and Senate 45 years later by 3.8 and 5.1 points respec-

tively. These estimates are in line with the individual-level estimates in Table 2. Although raising

the leaving age to 17 or above similarly increased Republican representation in state chambers,

relative to dropout ages below 16, it does not increase the seat share beyond raising the leaving

age to 16. This could reflect the fact that such reforms became more frequent later in the twentieth

century, and thus the effects have yet to fully materialize. Columns (2) and (4) instead use a lin-

ear measure of the dropout age and produce similar results. However, it is essential to emphasize

39

Page 40: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table 8: Effect of dropout age reform on downstream Republican state-level seat shares

Republican state Republican SenateHouse seat share Senate seat share

(1) (2) (3) (4)

Dropout age (45 year lag)=16 0.038*** 0.051***(0.013) (0.016)

Dropout age (45 year lag)≥17 0.032 0.039(0.020) (0.025)

Dropout age (45 year lag) 0.014** 0.012(0.006) (0.009)

Obervations 1,121 1,121 1,043 1,043Outcome mean 0.45 0.45 0.45 0.45Test: Dropout age (45 year lag)=16 = 0.729 0.607

Dropout age (45 year lag)≥17 (p value)

Notes: See Table 2. All specifications include state and election fixed effects and state-specific trends,and are estimated using OLS. Differences in the number of observations reflect missing data.

that such estimates are tentative, given both the challenges noted above and the large time gap be-

tween treatment and outcome. Nevertheless, they suggest that the dropout age may have produced

important electoral consequences that demand further investigation.

7 Conclusion

Despite education’s key role in determining students’ life trajectories, this is the first study to

identify whether and how high school education impacts political preferences and voting behavior

later in life. While previous studies have linked education to civic and political participation,

this article gives schooling a clear partisan tint by showing that it causes voters to support more

conservative on redistributive issues, and vote on the basis of such policy preferences. The findings

sharply contrast with previous studies highlighting the stickiness of partisan preferences: I find

that raising the dropout age by a year increases Republican partisan identification and voting by

1-3 percentage points per cohort, while completing an additional grade of high school increases

40

Page 41: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

the probability that a voter will identify with or vote for the Republican party later in life by around

10 percentage points.

The large magnitude of high school’s political effects highlight the importance of school dropout

laws. Beyond the individual-level analysis, my tentative analysis suggests that the Republican state

seat share is significantly greater 45 years after the state dropout age was raised. The results thus

imply that increasing the dropout age may have significantly altered electoral outcomes, which

are likely to have in turn influenced policy outcomes and the welfare of the electorate (e.g. Lee,

Moretti and Butler 2004). In the context of significant efforts, particularly from Democrats, to

increase dropout ages, my findings pose an important strategic problem for Democrats aiming to

simultaneously help poorly-educated voters while winning elections. This problem is magnified

by the fact that it is the disadvantaged voters, such as minorities, which the Democrats seek to help

that ultimately turn to the Republican party after realizing the returns to schooling. Conversely,

the results suggest that Republican opposition—e.g. in South Carolina in early 2016—may be

misguided from a long-run strategic perspective.

Both income and socialization theories could explain high school education’s conservative ef-

fects. However, an explanation combining human capital theory (including evidence that there is a

significant economic return to schooling) with the RMR model of income-based policy preferences

receives significant empirical support. Consistent with this explanation, I show that additional high

school education causes voters to support low tax rates, without adopting Republican positions on

non-economic issues. Furthermore, the largest effects of education are registered around voters’

mid-life earnings peak. Conversely, there is no systematic evidence suggesting that additional

schooling affects political engagement or socially liberal values.

However, school dropout ages only significantly increase levels of high school education. Con-

sequently, the results in this article speak only to high school education. While high school remains

an important policy issue, given that nearly 20% of students—particularly black and Hispanic

students—still fail to graduate from high school (Murnane 2013), another key question is whether

41

Page 42: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

the effects of college education differ. There remains considerable debate over this issue. On

one hand, many scholars argue that college education is particularly effective at instilling socially

liberal values, whether through curricula or social interactions and networks. On the other, re-

cent research suggests that colleges with a culture emphasizing financial gain may cause students

to become economically conservative (Mendelberg, McCabe and Thal forthcoming). Although

Mendelberg, McCabe and Thal (forthcoming) focus on student attitudes, rather than downstream

voting behavior, their results nevertheless suggest that college can also produce powerful conser-

vative effects via a mechanism also linked to aspirations for financial gain. The extent to which

such findings generalize to all types of college or postgraduate education represent an important

question for future research extending my findings for high school.

42

Page 43: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

References

Abrams, Samuel, Torben Iversen and David Soskice. 2010. “Informal Social Networks and Ratio-

nal Voting.” British Journal of Political Science 41:229–257.

Acemoglu, Daron and Joshua D. Angrist. 2000. “How Large Are Human Capital Externalities?

Evidence from Compulsory Schooling Laws.” NBER Macroeconomics Annual 15:9–59.

Alesina, Alberto and Eliana La Ferrara. 2005. “Preferences for Redistribution in the Land of

Opportunities.” Journal of Public Economics 89(5):897–931.

Alwin, Duane Francis, Ronald Lee Cohen and Theodore Mead Newcomb. 1991. Political attitudes

over the life span: The Bennington women after fifty years. Madison: University of Wisconsin

Press.

Angrist, Joshua D. and Alan B. Krueger. 1991. “Does Compulsory School Attendance Affect

Schooling and Earnings?” Quarterly Journal of Economics 106(4):979–1014.

Angrist, Joshua D. and Alan B. Krueger. 1992. “The Effect of Age at School Entry on Educational

Attainment: An Application of Instrumental Variables with Moments from Two Samples.” Jour-

nal of the American Statistical Association 87(418):328–336.

Angrist, Joshua D. and Guido W. Imbens. 1995. “Two-Stage Least Squares Estimation of Average

Causal Effects in Models With Variable Treatment Intensity.” Journal of the American Statistical

Association 90(430):431–442.

Angrist, Joshua D. and Jorn-Steffan Pischke. 2008. Mostly Harmless Econometrics: An Empiri-

cist’s Companion. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Ashenfelter, Orley and Cecilia Rouse. 1998. “Income, schooling, and ability: Evidence from a

new sample of identical twins.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 113(1):253–284.

43

Page 44: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Bartels, Larry M. 2010. The Study of Electoral Behavior. In The Oxford Handbook of American

Elections and Political Behavior, ed. Jan E. Leighley. Oxford University Press pp. 239–261.

Becker, Gary S. 1964. Human Capital: A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis, with Special Refer-

ence to Education. University of Chicago Press.

Berinsky, A. J. and Gabriel S. Lenz. 2011. “Education and Political Participation: Exploring the

Causal Link.” Political Behavior 33(3):357–373.

Bobo, Lawrence and Frederick C. Licari. 1989. “Education and political tolerance testing the

effects of cognitive sophistication and target group affect.” Public Opinion Quarterly 53(3):285–

308.

Campbell, Angus, Philip E. Converse, Warren E. Miller and Donald E. Stokes. 1960. The American

Voter. New York: Wiley.

Card, David. 1999. The causal effect of education on earnings. In Handbook of Labor Economics

Volume 3, ed. Orley Ashenfelter and David Card. Elsevier pp. 1801–1863.

Conley, Timothy G., Christian B. Hansen and Peter E. Rossi. 2012. “Plausibly Exogenous.” Review

of Economics and Statistics 94(1):260–272.

Conover, Pamela Johnston and Stanley Feldman. 1981. “The origins and meaning of lib-

eral/conservative self-identifications.” American Journal of Political Science 25(4):617–645.

Converse, Philip E. and Gregory B. Markus. 1979. “Plus ca change: The new CPS election study

panel.” American Political Science Review 73(1):32–49.

Dee, Thomas S. 2004. “Are there civic returns to education?” Journal of Public Economics

88(9):1697–1720.

44

Page 45: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Erikson, Robert S. and Kent L. Tedin. 2015. American Public Opinion (9th Edition). New York:

Pearson.

Fisher, Patrick. 2014. Demographic Gaps in American Political Behavior. Westview Press.

Franklin, Charles H. 1989. “Estimation across data sets: two-stage auxiliary instrumental variables

estimation (2SAIV).” Political Analysis 1(1):1–23.

Galston, William A. 2001. “Political knowledge, political engagement, and civic education.” An-

nual Review of Political Science 4(1):217–234.

Gelbach, Jonah B. 2004. “Migration, the life cycle, and state benefits: How low is the bottom?”

Journal of Political Economy 112(5):1091–1130.

Gelman, Andrew, Park, Boris Shor, Joseph Bafumi and Jeronimo Cortina. 2010. Red State, Blue

State, Rich State, Poor State: Why Americans Vote the Way They Do. Princeton, NJ: Princeton

University Press.

Gerber, Alan S., Gregory A. Huber, David Doherty, Conor M. Dowling and Shang E. Ha. 2010.

“Personality and Political Attitudes: Relationships Across Issue Domains and Political Con-

texts.” American Political Science Review 104(1):111–133.

Gerber, Alan S., Gregory A. Huber and Ebonya Washington. 2010. “Party affiliation, partisanship,

and political beliefs: A field experiment.” American Political Science Review 104(4):720–744.

Goldin, Claudia D. and Lawrence F. Katz. 2008. The Race Between Education and Technology.

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Green, Donald P., Bradley Palmquist and Eric Schickler. 2002. Partisan Hearts and Minds: Polit-

ical Parties and the Social Identities of Voters. Yale University Press.

45

Page 46: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Green, Donald P., Peter M. Aronow, Daniel E. Bergan, Pamela Greene, Celia Paris and Beth I.

Weinberger. 2011. “Does knowledge of constitutional principles increase support for civil liber-

ties? Results from a randomized field experiment.” Journal of Politics 73(2):463–476.

Hainmueller, Jens and Michael J. Hiscox. 2010. “Attitudes toward highly skilled and low-

skilled immigration: Evidence from a survey experiment.” American Political Science Review

104(1):61–84.

Henderson, John and Sara Chatfield. 2011. “Who Matches? Propensity Scores and Bias in the

Causal Effects of Education on Participation.” Journal of Politics 73(3):646–658.

Hillygus, D. Sunshine. 2005. “The missing link: Exploring the relationship between higher edu-

cation and political engagement.” Political Behavior 27(1):25–47.

Inoue, Atsushi and Gary Solon. 2010. “Two-Sample Instrumental Variables Estimators.” Review

of Economics and Statistics 92(3):557–561.

Jencks, Christopher, Marshall Smith, Henry Acland and Mary Jo Bane. 1972. Inequality: A Re-

assessment of the Effect of Family and Schooling in America. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Jennings, M. Kent, Laura Stoker and Jake Bowers. 2009. “Politics across generations: Family

transmission reexamined.” Journal of Politics 71(3):782–799.

Kam, Cindy D. and Carl L. Palmer. 2008. “Reconsidering the Effects of Education on Political

Participation.” Journal of Politics 70(3):612–631.

Kotin, Lawrence and William F. Aikman. 1980. Legal Foundations of Compulsory Schooling. Port

Washington, NY: Kennikat Press.

Kroh, Martin and Peter Selb. 2009. “Inheritance and the dynamics of party identification.” Political

Behavior 31(4):559–574.

46

Page 47: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Lee, David S., Enrico Moretti and Matthew J. Butler. 2004. “Do voters affect or elect policies?

Evidence from the US House.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 119(3):807–859.

Lenz, Gabriel S. 2012. Follow the Leader? How Voters Respond to Politicians’ Policies and

Performance. University of Chicago Press.

Levendusky, Matthew. 2009. The partisan sort: How liberals became Democrats and conserva-

tives became Republicans. University of Chicago Press.

Lochner, Lance and Enrico Moretti. 2004. “The Effect of Education on Crime: Evidence from

Prison Inmates, Arrests, and Self-Reports.” American Economic Review 94(1):155–189.

Marshall, John. 2016. “Coarsening bias: How instrumenting for coarsened treatments upwardly

biases instrumental variable estimates.” Political Analysis 24(2):157–171.

McCarty, Nolan, Keith T. Poole and Howard Rosenthal. 2006. Polarized America: The Dance of

Ideology and Unequal Riches. MIT Press.

Meltzer, Allan H. and Scott F. Richard. 1981. “A rational theory of the size of government.”

Journal of Political Economy 89(5):914–927.

Mendelberg, Tali, Katherine T. McCabe and Adam Thal. forthcoming. “College Socialization and

the Economic Views of Affluent Americans.” American Journal of Political Science .

Meredith, Marc. 2009. “Persistence in Political Participation.” Quarterly Journal of Political Sci-

ence 4(3):187–209.

Milligan, Kevin, Enrico Moretti and Philip Oreopoulos. 2004. “Does education improve citizen-

ship? Evidence from the United States and the United Kingdom.” Journal of Public Economics

88(9):1667–1695.

47

Page 48: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Molloy, Raven, Christopher L. Smith and Abigail Wozniak. 2011. “Internal Migration in the

United States.” Journal of Economic Perspectives 25(3):173–196.

Murnane, Richard J. 2013. “U.S. High School Graduation Rates: Patterns and Explanations.”

Journal of Economic Literature 51(2):370–422.

Murphy, Kevin M. and Robert H. Topel. 1985. “Estimation and Inference in Two-Step Econometric

Models.” Journal of Business and Economic Statistics 20(1):88–97.

Nie, Norman H., Jane Junn and Kenneth Stehlik-Barry. 1996. Education and Democratic Citizen-

ship in America. University of Chicago Press.

Niemi, Richard G. and Jane Junn. 1998. Civic Education: What Makes Students Learn. Yale

University Press.

Oreopoulos, Philip. 2006. “Estimating Average and Local Average Treatment Effects of Education

when Compulsory Schooling Laws Really Matter.” American Economic Review 96(1):152–175.

Oreopoulos, Philip. 2009. Would More Compulsory Schooling Help Disadvantaged Youth? Evi-

dence from Recent Changes to School-Leaving Laws. In The Problems of Disadvantaged Youth:

An Economic Perspective, ed. Jonathan Gruber. University of Chicago Press pp. 85–112.

Romer, Thomas. 1975. “Individual welfare, majority voting, and the properties of a linear income

tax.” Journal of Public Economics 4(2):163–185.

Ruggles, Steven, J. Trent Alexander, Katie Genadek, Ronald Goeken, Matthew B. Schroeder and

Matthew Sobek. 2010. “Integrated Public Use Microdata Series: Version 5.0.”.

Sondheimer, Rachel M. and Donald P. Green. 2010. “Using Experiments to Estimate the Effects

of Education on Voter Turnout.” American Journal of Political Science 41(1):178–189.

Spence, Michael. 1973. “Job market signaling.” Quarterly Journal of Economics 87(3):355–374.

48

Page 49: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Stephens, Jr, Melvin and Dou-Yan Yang. 2014. “Compulsory Education and the Benefits of

Schooling.” American Economic Review 104(6):1777–1792.

Stoker, Laura and Jackie Bass. 2011. Political Socialization: Ongoing Questions and New Di-

rections. In The Oxford Handbook of American Public Opinion and the Media, ed. George C.

Edwards, III, Lawrence R. Jacobs and Robert Y. Shapiro. Oxford University Press pp. 453–465.

Verba, Sidney, Kay Lehman Schlozman and Henry E. Brady. 1995. Voice and Equality: Civic

Voluntarism in American Politics. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Weakliem, David L. 2002. “The effects of education on political opinions: An international study.”

International Journal of Public Opinion Research 14(2):141–157.

Zaller, John. 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge University Press.

49

Page 50: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

A Appendix

Contents

A.1 Detailed variable definitions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A1

A.2 ANES data for robustness checks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A6

A.3 No evidence of political determinants of school leaving age reforms . . . . . . . . A11

A.4 Technical details for IV estimation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A12

A.4.1 Support for the monotonicity assumption . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A12

A.4.2 Two-sample 2SLS estimation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A13

A.4.3 Procedure for matching ACS and NAES sample moments . . . . . . . . . A18

A.5 Robustness checks referenced in the main article . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A19

A.5.1 Sensitivity to cross-state migration . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A19

A.5.2 First stage without sample matching restrictions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A20

A.5.3 Excluding state-specific cohort trends . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A21

A.5.4 Exclusion restriction tests . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A21

A.5.5 Upwardly biased estimates of completing high school . . . . . . . . . . . . A24

A.5.6 Reduced form mechanism estimates . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . A25

A.1 Detailed variable definitions

The variables used are defined in detail below, including source variable names corresponding to

National Annenberg Election Survey (NAES) and American Communities Survey (ACS) variables.

All NAES variable codes correspond to their definition in the codebook for their respective waves,

where wave 1 is the 2000 Presidential election, wave 2 is the 2004 Presidential election, and wave

3 is the 2008 Presidential election. Table 1 in the main article provides summary statistics.

A1

Page 51: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

• (Republican) Partisan. Indicator coded 1 for the respondent identifying as a Republican.

Residual category is independents, Democrat partisans and “don’t know”. Non-answers

were deleted. This question was asked both before and after the Presidential election. NAES

variables “cV01” in wave 1, “cMA01” in wave 2, and “MA01” in wave 3.

• Intend (to vote Republican for President). Indicator coded 1 for respondents intending to

vote for the Republican Presidential candidate in the forthcoming election. The omitted

category contains any other vote, excluding non-voters and those who do not provide an

answer. This question is not asked in post-election surveys. NAES variable “cR23” in wave

1, “cRC03”, “cRC07” and “cRC10”-“cRC14” in wave 2, and “RCa03”, “RCa05”, “RCa07”

and “RCa08” in wave 3.

• Vote (Republican for President). Indicator coded 1 for respondents reporting that they voted

for the Republican Presidential candidate at the previous election (Bob Dole in 1996, George

W. Bush in 2000 and 2004, or John McCain in 2008). The omitted category contains any

other vote or not turning out. NAES variable “cR35” in wave 1, “cRD01” in wave 2, and

“RDc01” in wave 3.

• Male. Indicator variable for respondents identifying as male. Non-responses were deleted.

NAES variable “cW01” in wave 1, “cWA01” in wave 2, and “WA01” in wave 3; ACS vari-

able “sex”.

• Race. Three indicators for respondents identifying their race as white (including Hispanic,

because this is how the ACS is coded), black, or Asian (Chinese, Japanese and Other Asian

of Pacific Islanders in the ACS). The omitted category is all other races. Coded from NAES

variable “cW03” in wave 1, “cWA04” in wave 2, and “WC03” in wave 3; ACS variable

“race”.

• Black or Hispanic. Indicator coded 1 for respondents identifying their race as black or their

A2

Page 52: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

ethnicity as Hispanic. Coded from NAES variable “cW03” in wave 1, “cWA04” in wave 2,

and “WC03” in wave 3; ACS variables “race” and “hispan”.

• Age. Years of age at date at which survey was conducted. Quartic versions of age are

standardized. NAES variable “cW02” in wave 1, “cWA02” in wave 2, and “WA02” in wave

3; ACS variable “age”.

• Cohort (year aged 14). Calculated as the year of the survey, minus the respondent’s age at

the date of the survey, plus 14. NAES variables “cW02” and “cdate” in wave 1, “cWA02”

and “cDATE” in wave 2, and “WA01” and “DATE” in wave 3; ACS variables “age” and

“year”.

• State grew up. Respondent’s current state of residence in NAES and state of birth in ACS.

NAES variable “cST” in wave 1, “cST” in wave 2, and “WFc01” in wave 3; ACS variable

name “bpl”.

• Survey year. Set of indicators for the year—2000, 2001, 2003, 2004, 2007 or 2008—in

which the survey was conducted. Because month of survey is not available in the ACS, the

precise survey completion date must be coarsened to year. NAES variable “cdate” in wave

1, “cDATE” in wave 2, and “DATE” in wave 3; ACS variable name “year”.

• Dropout ages. See definition in the Appendix of the main article. State-year dropout age

data from Oreopoulos (2009) were kindly provided by Philip Oreopoulos, and are based on

the National Center for Education Statistics’ Education Digest.

• Schooling. Number of completed years of grade school (excluding Kindergarten) or above;

top coded at 12 years of education (i.e. completing high school). Those that did not graduate

high school with a diploma are coded as 11. ACS variable name “educd”.

• Beyond 12th grade. Indicator coded 1 for respondents registering any education beyond the

high school level. Coded from ACS variable name “educ”.

A3

Page 53: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

• Reduce taxes. Indicator coded 1 for respondents who strongly think taxes should be reduced;

“don’t know” was excluded. Coded from NAES variable “cBB01” in wave 1 (coded 1 for

respondents who see the amount Americans pay in taxes as an “extremely serious” problem),

“cCB13” in wave 2 (respond with “strongly” favoring tax reduction), and “CBb01” (respond

with “cut taxes”) in wave 3.

• Ban abortion. Indicator coded 1 for respondents who stated that abortion should never be

permitted. Coded from NAES variable “cBF03” in wave 1 (coded 1 for respondents who

agreed that the federal government should ban all abortions), “cCE01” in wave 2 (coded

1 for respondents who “strongly favor” or “somewhat favor” banning all abortions), and

“CEa01” in wave 3 (coded 1 for respondents who state that abortion is “not permitted under

any circumstances”).

• Low gun controls. Indicator coded 1 for respondents who answered that the federal govern-

ment should do “more” to restrict the kinds of guns that people can buy. Coded from NAES

variable “cBG06” in wave 1 and “cCE31” in wave 2; this was not asked in wave 3.

• Low health care spending. Indicator coded 1 for respondents who said that the federal gov-

ernment should spend more money on providing health care to those that do not already

have it. Coded from NAES variable “cBE02” in wave 1 and “cCC02” in wave 2; this was

not asked in wave 3.

• High military spending. Indicator coded 1 for respondents who said that the federal govern-

ment should spend more on military defense. Coded from NAES variable “cBJ07” in wave

1 and “cCD03” in wave 2; this was not asked in wave 3.

• Republican non-economic issues. Standardized summative rating combining the ban abor-

tion, do not increase gun controls, do not increase health care spending, and increase military

spending. Cronbach’s alpha inter-item reliability coefficient of 0.39, pooled across samples.

A4

Page 54: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

• Political knowledge scale. Standardized summative rating scale combining correct/incorrect

answers to up to six general political knowledge questions, including: identifying Republi-

cans as more conservative than Democrats; the Congressional majority required to overturn

a Presidential veto; which body determines whether a law is constitutional; identity of the

majority party in the House; who the Vice-President is; ability to name own Senator. Cron-

bach’s alpha inter-item reliability coefficient of 0.60 in 2003/2004 and 0.60 in 2007/2008;

underlying variable were unavailable for 2000/2001. NAES variables “MC01”-“MC04” in

wave 3, and “cMC01”, “cMC03”, “cMC05”, “cMC07”, “cMC09” and “cUA02” in wave 2.

• Political interest scale. Standardized summative rating scale combining the following five

measures of interest in politics: four-point scale measuring regularity with which the respon-

dent reports following government and public affairs (“cK01” in wave 1, and “cKA03” in

wave 2); four-point scale measuring regularity with which the respondent reports follows

the Presidential campaign (“cK02” in wave 1, “cKA01” in wave 2, and “KA01” in wave 3);

number of days in the last week that the respondent watched a public or cable news program

(“cE01” and “cE02” in wave 1, and “cEA01” and “cEA03” in wave 2); number of days in

the last week (0 to 7) that the respondent read a daily newspaper (“cE13” in wave 1, and

“cEA01” in wave 2); number of days in the last week (0 to 7) that the respondent discussed

politics with family or friends (“cK05” in wave 1, “cKB01” in wave 2, and “KB01” in wave

3). Cronbach’s alpha inter-item reliability coefficient pooled across all surveys is 0.61.

• Discuss politics. Standardized number of days in the last week (0 to 7) that the respondent

discussed politics with family or friends. NAES variable “cK05” in wave 1, “cKB01” in

wave 2, and “KB01” in wave 3.

• Turnout (pres. election). Self-reported turnout in the most recent Presidential election.

• Trust federal government. Indicator coded 1 for respondents that trust the federal government

to what is right either “always” or “most of the time”; residual category includes “some of

A5

Page 55: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

the time”, “never” and “don’t know”. NAES variable “cM01” in wave 1, “cMB01” and

“cMB02” in wave 2, and “MB01” in wave 3.

• Protect environment more. Indicator coded 1 for respondents who believe that the federal

government should do more to protect the environment; responses of the same, less or noth-

ing were coded as 0. NAES variable “cBS01” in wave 1 and “cCF08” in wave 2; this was

not asked in wave 3.

• Ban gay marriage. Indicator coded 1 for respondents that would favor a constitutional

amendment banning gay marriage.

• Age of eldest child. Age of oldest child in the respondent’s household. ACS variable name

“eldch”.

• Age of youngest child. Age of youngest child in the respondent’s household. ACS variable

name “yngch”.

• Single. Indicator coded 1 for respondents that have never been married. ACS variable name

“marst”.

A.2 ANES data for robustness checks

The American National Election Survey (ANES) collects many political variables which are pooled

across Presidential and mid-term elections 1952-2000 and 2008.32 Surveys typically interview

several thousand randomly-sampled voting-age citizens; pooling across elections produced a max-

imum sample of 35,873 respondents.33 The ANES thus has a far smaller sample size than the

NAES. Given the large number of fixed effects and trends included in the main specifications, the

greater sample size of the NAES is preferred. Nevertheless, the ANES has several advantages over

322002-2006 surveys were omitted because they did not ask where respondents grew up, a central com-ponent of the identification strategy.

33Observations suffering missingness were deleted.

A6

Page 56: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Figure A1: US years of schooling by cohort (Census data)

the NAES, and thus showing similar results is an important robustness check: the ANES covers a

wider range of election extending much further back in time; by extending further back in time,

the ANES also encompasses greater variation in schooling; and by asking respondents which state

they lived in at age 14, cross-state migration concerns are minimized.

Like the NAES, the number of years of schooling is not measured in the ANES. Accordingly,

this article complements the ANES with extracts from the 1950-2000 decennial Censuses. By

using decennial Census data instead of ACS surveys from the year of NAES survey, the assumption

that respondents are drawn from the same population is a little less likely to hold. Nevertheless, I

again ensure the Census sample of 636,713 individuals is approximately a random sample from the

same population the ANES is drawn from. To achieve this, I followed exactly the same stratified

procedure as for the ACS data, as detailed in the main article. The two datasets are again combined

using two-sample methods. Figure A1 shows average years of schooling by cohort, while Figure

A2 shows that the monotonicity check is satisfied.

The variables used in this analysis are defined in detail below, including source variable names

A7

Page 57: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

020

4060

8010

0

Cum

ulat

ive

% o

f obs

erva

tions

0 5 10 15

Years of completed schooling

CSL<16 CDF CSL=16 CDF CSL>=17 CDF

Figure A2: Monotonic first-stage (Census data)

corresponding to ANES and Census variables. All ANES variables codes correspond to the Time

Series Cumulative Data File. Table A1 provides summary statistics.

• Republican partisan. Indicator coded 1 for the respondent identifying as a Republican, in-

cluding those weak partisans who when pressed leaned toward the Republicans. Residual

category is independents and Democrat partisans; non-answers were deleted. ANES variable

VCF0303.

• Vote Republican. Vote Republican for President, Vote Republican for House and Vote Re-

publican for Senate are indicators for voting Republican in any of these elections, including

at mid-terms. The omitted category contains Democrat and other party/candidate voters;

non-voters and those who do not provide an answer are deleted. ANES variables VCF0704,

VCF0707 and VCF0708.

• Gender. Indicator variable for respondents identifying as male. Non-responses were deleted.

A8

Page 58: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Tabl

eA

1:Su

mm

ary

stat

istic

s:A

NE

San

dC

ensu

ssa

mpl

es

Am

eric

anN

atio

nalE

lect

ion

Stud

yC

ensu

sO

bs.

Mea

nSt

d.de

v.M

in.

Max

.O

bs.

Mea

nSt

d.de

v.M

in.

Max

.

Dep

ende

ntva

riab

les

Rep

ublic

anpa

rtis

an35

,873

0.35

0.48

01

Vote

Rep

ublic

anfo

rPre

side

nt14

,712

0.48

0.5

01

Vote

Rep

ublic

anfo

rHou

se19

,081

0.43

0.5

01

Vote

Rep

ublic

anfo

rSen

ate

13,3

390.

450.

50

1

End

ogen

ous

vari

able

Scho

olin

g63

6,71

310

.75

2.62

012

Exc

lude

din

stru

men

tsD

ropo

utag

e=16

35,9

970.

750.

430

163

6,71

30.

740.

440

1D

ropo

utag

e≥17

35,9

970.

170.

370

163

6,71

30.

180.

380

1

Pre

-tre

atm

entc

ovar

iate

sA

ge35

,997

43.7

216

.04

1797

636,

713

43.0

716

.67

1893

Mal

e35

,997

0.44

0.5

01

636,

713

0.42

0.49

01

Whi

te35

,997

0.85

0.35

01

636,

713

0.86

0.35

01

Bla

ck35

,997

0.12

0.32

01

636,

713

0.12

0.33

01

Asi

an35

,997

00.

060

163

6,71

30

0.06

01

Yea

rage

d14

35,9

971,

950.

6019

.41,

914

1,99

663

6,71

31,

951.

2018

.81,

914

1,99

6C

ensu

sye

ar(d

ecad

e)35

,997

1,98

0.05

13.3

41,

950

2,00

063

6,71

31,

980.

2713

.41,

950

2,00

0

A9

Page 59: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

ANES variable VCF0104; Census variable name “sex”.

• Race. Three indicators for respondents identifying their race as White or Hispanic, Black, or

Asian (Chinese, Japanese and Other Asian of Pacific Islanders in the Census). The omitted

category is all other races. Hispanics are grouped with whites under the Census. Coded from

ANES variable VCF0106a; Census variable “race”.

• Age. At time of survey. Quartic version of age is not standardized to ensure comparability

across ANES and Census datasets. ANES variable VCF0101; Census variable name “age”.

• Year aged 14. Calculated as the year of the survey, minus the respondent’s age at the date

of the survey, plus 14. Since no person in the Census could be 14 later than 1996, the

small number of ANES observations for respondents 14 after 1996 were dropped. Since no

person in the ANES was 14 before 1914, all such Census observations were dropped (see

above). 1914 is the omitted category when a full set of indicators is used. Coded from ANES

variables VCF0101 and VCF0104; Census variables “age” and “year”.

• State grew up. The ANES asks respondents what state they grew up in, taking the state

where more time between 6 and 18 was spent if respondents moved across states. The

Census asks which state respondents were born in, which requires a stronger no-migration

assumption when matching individuals to the state dropout ages. Foreign-born respondents

were excluded. ANES variable VCF0132 and VCF0133; Census variable “bpl”.

• Census decade. Indicator for each decade of the Census conducted, 1950-2000. For the

ANES, I count the nearest years as part of that Census decade; e.g. the ANES surveys

1986-1994 are coded for the 1990 Census indicator; the 2008 election is included with

the 2000 Census. Because TS2SLS estimation requires using exactly the same variables

across datasets, election-specific indicators (which would be possible if we were only using

the ANES) cannot be used. Coded from ANES variable VCF0004; Census variable name

A10

Page 60: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

“year”.

• Dropout age. Same as ACS.

• Schooling. Same as ACS. Census variable name “educd”.

A.3 No evidence of political determinants of school leaving age reforms

To investigate the concern that school leaving age reforms may reflect strategic behavior by par-

ticular parties or reflect waves of political sentiment, I test whether a state’s minimum schooling

leave age is correlated with partisan political forces in state s in year t by estimating the following

difference-in-differences equation:

dropout agest = Pstλ +ϑs +ωt + ξst , (A1)

where Pst is a vector of state-level political variables, and θs and ωt are respectively state and year

fixed effects.

The results in Table A2 provide no evidence for the political endogeneity concern. Column (1)

shows no statistical association between the minimum dropout age and indicators for whether the

Republicans controlled the upper, lower or both state houses. Using the Republican seat shares

instead, column (3) again finds that political support is uncorrelated with changes in dropout ages.

Finally, column (5) finds no significant correlation with Republican state Governors. The lack

of such correlations suggest that minimum schooling leaving age reforms were unlikely to have

been implemented strategically, or have been correlated with waves in local political sentiment.34

Columns (2), (4) and (6) show that the results are also robust to including state-specific time trends.

34Unreported checks using the three leaving age indicators and lagging the independent variables tocapture delayed implementation similarly show no statistically significant associations.

A11

Page 61: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table A2: Correlation between state political control and state minimum schooling leaving age

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Republican House majority -0.010 -0.079*(0.056) (0.041)

Republican Senate majority -0.047 -0.068(0.093) (0.061)

Unified Republican houses 0.007 0.044(0.075) (0.064)

Republican House seat share 0.194 -0.356(0.251) (0.251)

Republican Senate seat share 0.166 -0.093(0.271) (0.238)

Republican Governor 0.091* 0.031(0.054) (0.025)

Observations 3,890 3,890 3,890 3,890 3,725 3,725State-specific trends X X X

Notes: Outcome is the minimum dropout age in a given state. All specifications include state and yearfixed effects, and are estimated using OLS. Standard errors clustered by state in parentheses. * denotesp < 0.1, ** denotes p < 0.05 *** denotes p < 0.01.

A.4 Technical details for IV estimation

A.4.1 Support for the monotonicity assumption

Although monotonicity is fundamentally untestable, one implication of monotonicity with multi-

valued treatments is that the CDFs F(Si|dropout ageics = z) should not cross (Angrist and Imbens

1995). Figure A3 plots the CDFs for each dropout age instrument, and support the monotonicity

assumption as the CDF of higher dropout ages lie everywhere below the CDF of lower dropout

ages. Although important covariates such as state grew up in fixed effects are not included, these

figures provide an alternative graphical depiction of the first-stage relationship with the largest

changes in the relative CDFs occurring where the instruments apply. While this cannot prove

monotonicity holds, because the counterfactual is never observed and the controls used in the main

analysis are not used here, it is suggestive in that a possible violation receives no empirical support.

A12

Page 62: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

020

4060

8010

0

Cum

ulat

ive

% o

f obs

erva

tions

0 5 10 15

Years of completed schooling

CSL<16 CDF CSL=16 CDF CSL>=17 CDF

Figure A3: Monotonic first-stage (ACS data)

A.4.2 Two-sample 2SLS estimation

The goal is estimate the following system of IV equations:

Yi = TiβT +Wiβ−T + ui = Xiβ + ui (A2)

Ti = ZiΠ+ εi, (A3)

where Xi includes exogenous covariates Wi and the treatment variable(s) Ti, while Zi includes Wi

and q excluded instruments. Identification requires that only p ≤ q treatment variables can be

instrumented for.

Two methods have been proposed for IV estimation with two samples. Angrist and Krueger

(1992) propose a Wald-style estimator where the reduced form estimates are divided by their first

stage counterparts, which can be generalized to the overidentified case where the number of in-

struments outnumber the number of endogenous variables. Inoue and Solon (2010) show that this

A13

Page 63: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

estimator is less efficient than the 2SLS counterpart—first proposed by Franklin (1989)—that will

be used in the empirical application here. The advantage of this estimator is that it corrects for

finite-sample differences between the two samples.35 Furthermore, its extension to multiple in-

struments and multiple endogenous variables is straight-forward—both of which are important in

many empirical applications, including the analysis in this article.

In matrix form (stacking over i in each sample), the two-sample 2SLS (TS2SLS) estimator is:

βT S2SLS = (X ′1X1)

−1X ′1Y1, (A4)

where X1 = (T1,W1) is the matrix of predicted values in sample 1. The OLS regression coefficients

generating T1 are based on p first stage regressions estimated in sample 2:

X1 = Z1Π = Z1(Z′2Z2)−1Z′2X2. (A5)

The following assumptions are required to ensure the consistency of the TS2SLS estimator (see

Franklin 1989; Inoue and Solon 2010):

1. Random sampling from the same population: Y1i,Z1in1i=1 and T2i,Z2in2

i=1 are indepen-

dently and identically distributed draws of size n1 and n2 from the same population with

finite second moments.

2. Instrument exogeneity: E[Z′1iε1i] = E[Z′2iε2i] = 0.

3. Exclusion restriction: E[Z′1iu1i] = 0.

4. Rank conditions: (a) Z′1iZ1i and Z′2iZ2i have full rank, (b) X ′1iZ2i and X ′2iZ2i have full rank.

5. Interchangeable sample moments: (a) E[Z′1iX1i] = E[Z′2iX2i], (b) E[Z′1iZ1i] = E[Z′2iZ2i].

35Inoue and Solon (2010) show that the TS2SLS estimator remains consistent even when differences inthe sampling rates vary with some of the instrumental variables.

A14

Page 64: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Assumption 1 says that the samples must draw from the same population. Assumption 2 requires

that the instrument be exogenous in the first stage. Assumption 3 is implied by the exclusion re-

striction, but is written in terms of expectations. Assumption 4 is a standard rank condition required

for matrix invertibility. Assumption 5 requires that crucial samples moments can be interchanged,

thereby permitting substitution between samples. As n1 and n2 converge to the population size,

Assumption 5 necessarily holds.

Franklin (1989) proves the n1-consistency of the TS2SLS estimator. However, calculating the

TS2SLS standard errors is not obvious. Calculating the standard errors from a regression of Y1 on

X1 neglects the uncertainty in the first stage, in addition to distributional differences between the

first stage and reduced form samples.

The Murphy and Topel (1985) two-stage framework for understanding “generated regressors”—

accounting for the uncertainty introduced where a variable is estimated as a proxy to enter a

separate regression—incorporates such estimation uncertainty.36 Proposition 1 derives the ho-

moskedastic and cluster-robust variance (matrices), of which the robust variance is the particular

case of G1 = n1 and G2 = n2 clusters. (i is dropped to facilitate exposition.)

Proposition 1. The asymptotic variance of the TS2SLS estimator, V[β T S2SLS], is

2u +

n1

n2β

T S2SLS′S Ωβ

T S2SLSS

]E[X ′1X1]

−1, Ω = E[ε ′ε|X1] =

σ2

1 · · · σ1,p

... . . . ...

σp,1 . . . σ2p

(A6)

when the reduced form squared error σ2u = E[u2|X1] and the error covariances Ω of the p first

stage regressions are homoskedastic; when the reduced form and first stage errors are grouped

36Inoue and Solon (2010) acknowledge this approach but derive homoskedastic and heteroskedastic vari-ance matrices in an alternative way, but do not provide a cluster-robust variance estimate.

A15

Page 65: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

into G1 and G2 clusters respectively, the cluster-robust variance is

E[X ′1X1]−1[

V[β T S2SLS]+n1

n2E[X ′1(β

T S2SLS′S ⊗Z1)]V(Π)E[(β T S2SLS′

S ⊗Z1)′X1]

]E[X ′1X1]

−1,(A7)

where β T S2SLSS is the vector of coefficients on p endogenous variables, the uncorrected TS2SLS

variance is given by V[β T S2SLS] = G1G1−1 ∑

G1g=1 E[X ′1gu1gu′1gX1g] and the variances from m first-

stage regressions are V(Π) = G2G2−1 Φ⊗E[Z′2Z2]−1, where

Φ =

E[Z′2Z2]−1

∑G2g=1 E[Z′2gε2g1ε ′2g1Z2g] · · · E[Z′2Z2]−1

∑G2g=1 E[Z′2gε2g1ε ′2gpZ2g]

... . . . ...

E[Z′2Z2]−1∑

G2g=1 E[Z′2gε2gpε ′2g1Z2g] . . . E[Z′2Z2]−1

∑G2g=1 E[Z′2gε2gpε ′2gpZ2g]

. (A8)

Proof : Start by separating X into its endogenous and exogenous components,

Yi1 = Xi1β−S +Ti1βS + ui = Xi1β−S + Ti1βS +[Ti1− Ti1]+ ui, (A9)

where Ti1 = Zi1Π = Zi1(Z′2Z2)−1Z′2T2 is the predicted value of the treatment using the first stage

estimates, and Ti1 is the true and unobserved treatment in sample 1. An OLS regression would

yield:

√n1

β−T −β−S

βS−βS

=

(1n1

X ′1X1

)−1 1√

n1X ′1u1 +

(1n1

X ′1X1

)−1 1√

n1X ′1[Ti1− Ti1]βS, (A10)

where subscripts i and superscripts T S2SLS are omitted to save space. Using the expansion result

A16

Page 66: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

in Murphy and Topel (1985: 374) yields:

√n1(β −β ) ≡

√n1

β−T −β−S

βS−βS

a=

(1n1

X ′1X1

)−1 1√

n1X ′1u1

+

(1n1

X ′1X1

)−1(n1

n2

)1/2 1n1

X ′1(β′T ⊗Z1)

√n2(Π−Π),(A11)

where (β ′T ⊗Z1) is the matrix of defined in equation (12) of Murphy and Topel (1985).

Let Π be a consistent estimator of the first stage for the endogenous variables, such that√

n2(Π−Π)a∼ N(0,V(Π)). Using our consistent first stage estimate, the asymptotic variance

is therefore given by:

V(β −β ) = E[X ′1X1]−1[

V[β ]+n1

n2E[X ′1(β

′T ⊗Z1)]

−1V[Π]E[(β ′T ⊗Z1)′X1]

−1]

E[X ′1X1]−1,(A12)

where V[β ] is the variance of the naive TS2SLS estimator. (Note that E[X ′1u1] = 0, in conjunction

with a consistent first stage, implies the consistency of the estimator.)

This establishes the general asymptotic variance formula in Proposition 1. We now apply the

homoskedastic and cluster-robust error structures:

1) Homoskedastic errors. Under homoskedasticity, the naive variance from the TS2SLS re-

gression is simply σ2u (X

′1X1)−1. To correct for the first stage estimation, we have:

X ′1(β′T ⊗Z1)V(Π)(β ′T ⊗Z1)

′X1 = X ′1(β′T ⊗Z1)(Ω⊗ (Z′1Z1)

−1)(β ′T ⊗Z1)′X1 (A13)

= X ′1(β′T ΩβT ⊗Z1(Z′1Z1)

−1Z′1)X1 (A14)

= β′T ΩβT (X ′1X1), (A15)

where the first line uses the definitions of homoskedasticity given in the proposition, the sec-

ond line applies the mixed product property of Kronecker products, and the third line exploits

Z1(Z′1Z1)−1Z′1X1 = X1 (because all exogenous variables are contained in both X1 and Z1) and the

A17

Page 67: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

fact that β ′T Ωβ ′T is a scalar. Substituting into the general variance matrix yields the homoskedastic

variance formula in Proposition 1.

2) Clustered errors. In the clustered case, we simply let V(Π) = G2G2−1 Φ⊗E[Z′2Z2]−1.

Standard errors are given by the square roots of the diagonal elements of V[β T S2SLS]/n1. Using

the analogy principle, expectations can be replaced by sample moments.

In the case of a single endogenous regressor, V(Π) is simply the standard cluster-robust vari-

ance matrix for the first stage:

E[Z′2Z2]−1

[G2

G2−1

G2

∑g=1

E[Z′2gε2gε′2gZ2g]

]E[Z′2Z2]

−1. (A16)

When there are multiple endogenous variables, the first stage estimates may be correlated across

models. This requires the more complex formulation in Proposition (1).

A.4.3 Procedure for matching ACS and NAES sample moments

As noted in the main text, I use stratified random sampling to ensure that the sampling moments of

the ACS data used to estimate the first stage match the analagous sampling moments in the NAES

dataset used to estimate the reduced form. This enables me to correct for observable differences

across these samples, which draw from the same population (once ineligible voters are removed

from the NAES and those aged below 18 and born outside the U.S. are removed from the ACS).

To match the sample moments, I first removed respondents aged 14 before 1920 (since no

NAES respondent was 14 before 1920) and respondents who grew up in Alaska or Hawaii, were

born outside the U.S., or became naturalized citizens. I then stratified by cohort-gender to draw

a random sample of 2,000,000 ACS respondents to ensure that the subsample has the same by-

cohort-by-gender distribution as the NAES. Finally, I randomly dropped respondents to reduce

the sample to ensure that the proportion of respondents from each survey year reflects the NAES

dataset distribution, before in turn randomly dropping respondents to ensure that the ACS sample

A18

Page 68: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

matches the NAES distribution over race (white, black and Asian dummies). This produced a

maximum pooled sample of 443,539 voting age respondents.

A.5 Robustness checks referenced in the main article

A.5.1 Sensitivity to cross-state migration

Although the main article argues that cross-state migration is unlikely to be driving the results, I

nevertheless conduct two key robustness checks to show that this is unlikely to be a concern: a

bounding exercise that shows that the type of migration required to upwardly bias the estimates is

implausible; and analysis using the ANES, where state of residence at age 14 can be used to assign

the dropout age. These tests complement the lags and leads check in Figure 2, which is hard to

reconcile with migration unless most of those driving the results moved immediately as the reform

came into effect.

The bounding exercise evaluating the extent of biased migration required to explain the re-

duced form estimates. Consider the most extreme case where voters migrate from a state with a

low dropout age (baseline category of 15 or lower) to a state with a high dropout age (17 or higher).

In the NAES sample, the average proportion of Republican partisans in these states are, respec-

tively, 32.3% and 32.1%; the sample-weighted average population in high-dropout age states is 3.3

times larger. To account for the smallest reduced form estimate (a 0.03 percentage point increase

in Republican partisanship), if 48% of migrants from the low-dropout age state are Republican

partisans (which is 50% greater than the sample average), the net migration rate from low to high-

dropout age states would have to average almost 14% per cohort. For Republican vote intention

and reported vote choice, selective migration (50% above the sample average for each measure of

voting) would respectively require net per cohort migration rates of 15% and 12% to fully explain

the reduced form estimates. Beyond the fact migration is unlikely to be so politically biased, net

migration between such states is substantially lower than the rates required to account for the main

A19

Page 69: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

results. In the ACS data, respondents are in fact slightly more likely to migrate to a state with a

lower dropout age than the state in which they were born.37

Furthermore, I show that the results are robust to using the ANES dataset, which uses a substan-

tially smaller and less recent sample. However, the ANES has several advantages over the NAES,

and thus showing similar results is an important robustness check: the ANES covers a much wider

span of elections, and thus includes more voters from earlier cohorts when individuals left school

earlier (average schooling is 10.8 grades); and by asking respondents which state they lived in

at age 14, cross-state migration concerns are minimized because dropout ages can be mapped to

individuals by location at age 14. The second feature is particularly relevant for addressing the

migration concern because it ensures that the state dropout age is correctly assigned at the start of

high school.

The TS2SLS estimates in panel A of Table A3 again show that schooling significantly increases

Republican partisanship and self-reported Republican voting in prior House and Senate elections.

State-specific cohort trends are excluded, given the comparatively weak power of the analysis and

a weak first stage. To further address the migration concern, the results in panel B show that the

ANES estimates are robust to focusing only on respondents who live in the state where they resided

at age 14. More generally, these findings also indicate that the political effects of increasing the

dropout age do not simply reflect idiosyncratic features of the NAES sample. The ANES data and

procedures are described in detail below.

A.5.2 First stage without sample matching restrictions

Table A4 show that the first stage estimates, when no restrictions are applied to match sample

moments, are very similar to those reported in Table 3.

37Using the ACS data, I coded as 1 respondents who live in a state with a high dropout age than theirstate of birth, and and a lower-dropout age state as -1. Summing across the sample (including non-movers),net migration was 4.5 percentage points toward lower-dropout age states.

A20

Page 70: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table A3: The effect of schooling on identifying as and voting Republican—ANES dataset

Panel A: Full sample Partisan Vote House Senate(1) (2) (3) (4)

Schooling 0.103*** 0.050 0.163*** 0.071*(0.022) (0.035) (0.033) (0.038)

Reduced form observations 35,873 14,712 19,081 13,339First-stage observations 636,713 636,713 636,713 636,713First-stage F statistic 196.6 196.6 196.6 196.6

Panel B: Non-migrants Partisan Vote House Senate(5) (6) (7) (8)

Schooling 0.135*** 0.091** 0.160*** 0.095**(0.027) (0.042) (0.040) (0.043)

Reduced form observations 25,823 10,505 13,700 9,504First-stage observations 389,442 389,442 388,757 388,419First-stage F statistic 141.9 141.9 141.1 142.7

Notes: Outcomes are Republican partisanship (“partisan”), and voting Republican for President(“vote”), House and Senate. All specifications are estimated using TS2SLS, and include pre-treatmentscontrols (male, white, black and Asian dummies, and quartic demeaned age polynomials), state grew upin, survey decade, and cohort fixed effects. Differences in sample size reflect data availability. Standarderrors clustered by state-cohort in parentheses. * denotes p < 0.1, ** denotes p < 0.05, *** denotesp < 0.01.

A.5.3 Excluding state-specific cohort trends

Table A5 reports the main estimates where state-specific cohort trends—a key check for parallel

trends violations—are excluded. While the effect on partisanship increases somewhat, the effect

on vote choice decreases somewhat. However, all estimates remain statistically significant.

A.5.4 Exclusion restriction tests

Table A6 reports the exclusion restriction tests cited in the main article. The results indicate that

the dropout age does not meaningful effect fertility and martial choices—the age of a respondent’s

children or their probability of being married at the time of the survey. The age of children vari-

A21

Page 71: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table A4: The effect of school dropout age on completed grades of high school, without samplematching restrictions

Completedgrades

(1)

Dropout age=16 0.183***(0.021)

Dropout age≥17 0.211***(0.023)

Observations 3,084,049Outcome mean 11.62First stage F statistic 43.2Test: Dropout age=16 = Dropout age≥17 (p value) 0.001

Notes: The specification include male, white, black and Asian dummies, quartic (demeaned) age poly-nomials, state-specific cohort trends, and state grew up, cohort and survey fixed effects, and is estimatedusing OLS. The omitted dropout age category is dropout age≤15. Standard errors clustered by state-cohort in parentheses. * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01.

ables, in particular, suggest that adolescent life choices were not substantially affected.

I also conduct an exclusion restriction sensitivity analysis in line with the Conley, Hansen

and Rossi (2012) union of confidence intervals approach. Specifically, I replace Yicst with Yicst −

δ11(dropout agecs = 16)− δ21(dropout agecs ≥ 17) in equation (2). I then estimate this using

TS2SLS, and examine the sensitivity of the results for different δ1 and δ2 parameter specifications.

I restrict attention to violations such that the relationship between δ1 and δ2 reflects the ratio be-

tween the reduced form coefficients estimated in columns (1)-(3) of Table 2. For example, for

Republican partisanship I consider δ1 = 0.013 and δ2 = (0.030/0.013)δ . The TS2SLS estimates

cease to be statistically significant at the 90% level when δ reaches 0.002, 0.017 and 0.010 respec-

tively for the partisanship, vote intention and vote choice outcomes. These violations respectively

represent around 16%, 48% and 38% of the full reduced form estimate, and thus suggest that a

very large violation is required to nullify the vote choice outcomes.

A22

Page 72: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Tabl

eA

5:T

heef

fect

ofsc

hool

drop

outa

gean

dco

mpl

eted

grad

eson

iden

tifyi

ngas

and

votin

gR

epub

lican

,exc

ludi

ngst

ate-

spec

ific

coho

rttr

ends

Rep

ublic

ansu

ppor

tD

emoc

rats

uppo

rtPa

rtis

anIn

tend

Vote

Part

isan

Inte

ndVo

te(1

)(2

)(3

)(4

)(5

)(6

)

Pane

lA:R

educ

edfo

rmD

ropo

utag

e=16

0.03

6***

0.02

9***

0.01

3-0

.028

***

-0.0

20**

0.00

2(0

.010

)(0

.011

)(0

.013

)(0

.010

)(0

.010

)(0

.012

)D

ropo

utag

e≥17

0.05

1***

0.03

5***

0.02

3*-0

.038

***

-0.0

25**

-0.0

13(0

.011

)(0

.012

)(0

.013

)(0

.011

)(0

.011

)(0

.013

)

Obs

erva

tions

176,

796

144,

742

125,

196

176,

796

144,

742

125,

196

Out

com

em

ean

0.30

0.46

0.45

0.33

0.42

0.36

Test

:Dro

pout

age=

16=

Dro

pout

age≥

17(p

valu

e)0.

001

0.30

50.

063

0.02

50.

353

0.00

5

Pane

lB:I

nstr

umen

talv

aria

bles

Com

plet

edgr

ades

0.10

8***

0.07

3***

0.05

2*-0

.081

***

-0.0

54**

-0.0

33(0

.026

)(0

.027

)(0

.029

)(0

.024

)(0

.025

)(0

.027

)

Firs

tsta

ge(A

CS)

obse

rvat

ions

443,

539

436,

333

443,

539

443,

539

436,

333

443,

539

Red

uced

form

(NA

ES)

obse

rvat

ions

176,

796

144,

699

125,

196

176,

796

144,

699

125,

196

Out

com

em

ean

0.30

0.46

0.45

0.33

0.42

0.36

Firs

tsta

geF

stat

istic

51.4

51.0

51.4

51.4

51.0

51.4

Not

es:

See

abov

efo

rde

taile

dva

riab

lede

finiti

ons.

All

spec

ifica

tions

incl

ude

mal

e,w

hite

,bla

ckan

dA

sian

dum

mie

s,qu

artic

(dem

eane

d)ag

epo

lyno

mia

ls,a

ndst

ate

grew

up,c

ohor

tand

surv

eyfix

edef

fect

s,an

dar

ees

timat

edus

ing

OL

S.D

iffer

ence

sin

the

num

bero

fobs

erva

tions

refle

ctdi

ffer

ence

sin

the

NA

ES

mod

ules

inw

hich

each

outc

ome

was

incl

uded

.T

heom

itted

drop

outa

geca

tego

ryis

drop

outa

ge≤

15.

Stan

dard

erro

rscl

uste

red

byst

ate-

coho

rtin

pare

nthe

ses.

*p<

0.1,

**p<

0.05

,***

p<

0.01

.

A23

Page 73: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table A6: Exclusion restriction tests

Age of Age of Singleeldest youngestchild child(1) (2) (3)

Dropout age=16 0.100 0.349* 0.007*(0.182) (0.179) (0.004)

Dropout age≥17 -0.070 0.238 0.001(0.192) (0.186) (0.005)

Observations 168,525 168,525 443,539Outcome mean 16.18 13.44 0.17

Notes: See above for detailed variable definitions. All specifications include male, white, black andAsian dummies, quartic (demeaned) age polynomials, state-specific cohort trends, and state grew up,cohort and survey fixed effects, and are estimated using OLS. The omitted dropout age category isdropout age≤15. Standard errors clustered by state-cohort in parentheses. Standard errors clustered bystate-cohort in parentheses. * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01.

A.5.5 Upwardly biased estimates of completing high school

Table A7 provides the estimate when dropout ages are used to instrument for completing high

school. As noted in the main text, these estimates will be biased if the instruments induce additional

schooling beyond completing high school and such years of schooling affect the political outcomes

for such compliers.

Consistent with such an exclusion restriction violations inducing considerable bias, the esti-

mates in Table A7 for both the NAES and ANES samples are extremely large. Relative the effect

sizes for each additional year of schooling, the effect magnitudes increases substantially.38 Of the

specifications, four produce a coefficient that exceeds the theoretical maximum of one. In the other

three, the coefficient is again far larger than the comparable weighted per-year estimates obtained

in the main article. These estimates are thus consistent with a large bias due to dichotomizing an

endogenous variable with multiple intensities. Furthermore, since the first stage is relatively strong

38Equations including linear state-specific cohort trends are excluded because the first stage becomesvery weak.

A24

Page 74: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Table A7: IV estimates of the effect of completing high school on Republican partisanship andvoting

Panel A: NAES sample Partisan Intend Vote(1) (2) (3)

Complete high school 1.372*** 0.937*** 1.307(0.360) (0.338) (0.893)

Observations 176,796 144,742 106,879First stage F statistic 12.8 10.6 2.4

Panel B: ANES sample Partisan Vote House Senate(4) (5) (6) (7)

Complete high school 0.884*** 0.587** 1.518*** 0.847**(0.235) (0.244) (0.463) (0.402)

Observations 35,669 14,638 18,980 13,262First stage F statistic 11.5 10.8 6.9 5.2

Notes: See Table A7 for variable definitions. All specifications include gender, white, black and Asian dummies,quartic (demeaned) age polynomials, and state grew up, cohort and survey fixed effects, and are estimated using2SLS. The omitted dropout age category is dropout age≤15. First stage observations decline for vote intentionbecause the ACS sample is reduced to match the variables in the NAES sample. Standard errors clustered bystate-cohort in parentheses. * p < 0.1, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01.

in most specifications, the inflated coefficients cannot simply be attributed to weak instrument bias.

Nevertheless, while the bias is large in this application, it is important emphasize that the extent of

bias is application-specific (see Marshall 2016).

A.5.6 Reduced form mechanism estimates

Table A8 shows the reduced form estimates of the dropout age on the possible mediators examined

in Table 7. The results reinforce the IV estimates.

A25

Page 75: T -D H SUPPORT FOR THE REPUBLICAN PARTY · graduate from high school by their early 20s, with substantially lower graduation rates among mi-nority populations (Murnane2013). Particularly

Tabl

eA

8:T

heef

fect

ofdr

opou

tage

son

pote

ntia

linc

ome

and

soci

aliz

atio

nm

echa

nism

s

Con

serv

ativ

eR

educ

eB

anL

owL

owH

igh

Rep

ublic

ansc

ale

taxe

s†ab

ortio

n†gu

nhe

alth

mili

tary

non-

econ

omic

cont

rols

†sp

endi

ng†

spen

ding

†is

sues

(1)

(2)

(3)

(4)

(5)

(6)

(7)

Dro

pout

age=

160.

053*

*0.

032*

*-0

.010

0.02

50.

015

-0.0

03-0

.032

(0.0

22)

(0.0

13)

(0.0

12)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

18)

(0.0

16)

(0.0

24)

Dro

pout

age≥

170.

063*

*0.

040*

**-0

.011

0.01

40.

018

-0.0

11-0

.034

(0.0

25)

(0.0

14)

(0.0

14)

(0.0

17)

(0.0

19)

(0.0

18)

(0.0

26)

Obs

erva

tions

172,

400

136,

830

131,

662

81,0

2269

,513

78,7

0316

3,31

4

Polit

ical

Polit

ical

Dis

cuss

Turn

out

Trus

tPr

otec

tB

ankn

owle

dge

inte

rest

polit

ics

(pre

s.fe

dera

len

viro

.ga

ysc

ale

scal

eel

ectio

n)go

vt.†

mor

e†m

arri

age†

(8)

(9)

(10)

(11)

(12)

(13)

(14)

Dro

pout

age=

16-0

.026

-0.0

10-0

.026

0.00

90.

008

-0.0

160.

016

(0.0

28)

(0.0

21)

(0.0

23)

(0.0

09)

(0.0

19)

(0.0

10)

(0.0

17)

Dro

pout

age≥

17-0

.026

-0.0

14-0

.032

0.01

10.

001

-0.0

17*

0.03

4*(0

.030

)(0

.024

)(0

.025

)(0

.011

)(0

.021

)(0

.010

)(0

.019

)

Obs

erva

tions

123,

411

177,

624

168,

961

150,

266

54,6

3112

6,52

272

,313

Not

es:A

llou

tcom

eva

riab

les

are

stan

dard

ized

,exc

eptb

inar

you

tcom

esde

note

dby

†;va

riab

lede

finiti

ons

are

prov

ided

abov

e.A

llsp

ecifi

catio

nsin

clud

em

ale,

whi

te,b

lack

and

Asi

andu

mm

ies,

quar

tic(d

emea

ned)

age

poly

nom

ials

,sta

te-s

peci

ficco

hort

tren

ds,a

ndst

ate

grew

up,c

ohor

tand

surv

eyfix

edef

fect

s.A

llsp

ecifi

catio

nsar

ees

timat

edus

ing

OL

S.D

iffer

ence

sin

sam

ple

size

refle

ctda

taav

aila

bilit

y.St

anda

rder

rors

clus

tere

dby

stat

e-co

hort

inpa

rent

hese

s.*

p<

0.1,

**p<

0.05

,***

p<

0.01

.

A26