study 2 docu…  · web viewindeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has...

69
Cultural Narratives Running head: CULTURAL NARRATIVES The Cultural Narratives of Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers: Cultural Identity Clarity, Collective Esteem and Relative Deprivation Evelyne Bougie Esther Usborne Roxane de la Sablonnière Donald M. Taylor Word count: 9 963 Authors’ Notes: Evelyne Bougie, Statistics Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; Esther Usborne and Donald M. Taylor, Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada; Roxane de la Sablonnière, Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Québec, Canada. 1

Upload: others

Post on 25-Mar-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Running head: CULTURAL NARRATIVES

The Cultural Narratives of Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers: Cultural Identity

Clarity, Collective Esteem and Relative Deprivation

Evelyne Bougie

Esther Usborne

Roxane de la Sablonnière

Donald M. Taylor

Word count: 9 963

Authors’ Notes:

Evelyne Bougie, Statistics Canada, Ottawa, Ontario, Canada; Esther Usborne and Donald M.

Taylor, Department of Psychology, McGill University, Montréal, Québec, Canada; Roxane de

la Sablonnière, Department of Psychology, Université de Montréal, Montréal, Québec,

Canada.

1

Page 2: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Abstract

Research has consistently demonstrated a positive relationship between personal identity

clarity and personal esteem (Campbell, 1990). The present research employed a novel

methodology in order to investigate the association between identity clarity and esteem at a

collective level of self-definition. In Study 1, the cultural narratives of Francophone and

Anglophone Quebecers were coded for clarity. Cultural identity clarity was marginally

significantly associated with collective esteem for Francophones, but not for Anglophones. In

Study 2, the relationship between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem was further

explored using information derived from participants’ cultural narratives. Feelings of relative

deprivation were incorporated into the analysis. For both Francophones and Anglophones,

cultural identity clarity mediated the positive relationship between relative deprivation and

collective esteem at important, threatening, and thus defining events in the group’s history.

Keywords: Narrative, Culture, Identity, Esteem, Relative Deprivation

2

Page 3: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

The Cultural Narratives of Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers: Cultural Identity

Clarity, Collective Esteem and Relative Deprivation

For many years, social psychologists have sought to understand the human quest for

self-esteem and psychological well-being. Although an array of psychological factors have

been associated with well-being, some classic and current social psychological theorists posit

that a key factor is the clarity of one’s personal identity (e.g., Baumgardner, 1990; Campbell,

1990; Erikson, 1968; Maslow, 1954; Rogers, 1951). Erikson (1968) theorized that knowing

oneself and experiencing oneself as possessing continuity and sameness were essential for

personal well-being. Similarly, Maslow (1954) and Rogers (1951) asserted that the

development and maintenance of a consistent personal identity acted as the foundation of an

individual’s well-being. More recently, researchers have empirically tested the association

between personal identity clarity and well-being and found that people who have a well-

articulated sense of who they are have more positive self-esteem and experience more positive

affect about the self (Baumgardner, 1990; Campbell, 1990).

Clarity of personal identity has therefore been theoretically and empirically linked to

self-esteem and psychological well-being, but this association has only been tested at a

personal level. Identity however, extends beyond the personal level and encompasses social,

group, or collective components (Tajfel, 1978; Tafjel & Turner, 1979). This has led to the

suggestion that the collective component of one’s self-concept may also be related to

psychological well-being and esteem (Taylor, 1997, 2002). Taylor proposes that the clarity of

one’s collective or cultural identity, the identity that comes from the groups to which one

belongs, is central to self and collective esteem. Without a clear cultural identity the

individual has no normative template to rely on when engaging in the process of elaborating a

global identity, thereby making it difficult for this individual to achieve a positive evaluation

of the self and the group.

3

Page 4: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Seminal research at the personal level demonstrated a consistent relationship between

personal identity clarity and personal self-esteem (Campbell, 1990). The first step then

towards an understanding of identity clarity that goes beyond a personal level requires

evaluating the relationship between identity clarity and esteem at the level of the group. Study

1 will test this relationship using a novel methodology, the cultural narrative. We argue that

this methodology is essential for an in depth understanding of a construct as complex as

cultural identity. In Study 2, the relationship between cultural identity clarity and collective

esteem will be tested using a more traditional methodology, but one that is derived directly

from participants’ cultural narratives. The rich identity information contained in the cultural

narratives allows for the incorporation of participants’ experiences of group-level threat

(relative deprivation) into our analyses. We first present the theoretical association between

collective (cultural) identity clarity and collective esteem before proceeding to the cultural

narrative methodology.

Collective Identity Clarity

Social Identity Theory (Tajfel, 1978; Tafjel & Turner, 1979; SIT) suggests that the

identity arising from the group or groups to which an individual belongs is a fundamental

component of the self-concept. Social or collective identity, be it gender identity, cultural

identity or any other group-based identity, represents self-knowledge derived from an

individual’s group membership together with the value and emotional significance attached to

it. According to social identity theorists, identification with a group or collective involves

both a cognitive and an evaluative component (Tajfel & Turner, 1979; Taylor, 1997, 2002).

The cognitive component addresses the question “who or what is my group?” and includes

characteristics that an individual shares with other members of his or her group and that are

group-defining in some respect. These defining characteristics comprise the traits, ideological

positions, shared behavior, experiences and history that are associated with the group. For

4

Page 5: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

example a person may consider that, like their group, they value relationships at the expense

of individual success.

The evaluative component of social or collective identity, collective esteem, refers to

an individual’s overall evaluation of the worth of his/her collective identity. Collective esteem

includes the perceived value placed on the social group by the self and by others, as well as

the affective commitment and closeness the individual feels to other members of the social

group (Ashmore, Deaux & Mclaughlin-Volpe, 2004). To the extent that one’s social group is

valued and compares favorably with relevant comparison outgroups, one’s collective esteem

is positive. For example, if an individual feels proud to be a member of a group because

valuing relationships at the expense of individual success is something this group values

positively and is something that is respected by other groups, this person would have high

collective esteem.

The present research takes into account both the cognitive and evaluative components

of collective identity, but argues that it is the clarity of the cognitive component that is a

necessary precondition for the presence of the evaluative component. Taylor (1997, 2002)

proposes that a clear and certain knowledge of a group’s shared history, behaviours, values,

norms, and characteristics is essential for an individual to form an evaluation of that group.

That is, in order to feel pride in one’s group, one first has to clearly know what one’s group

actually is. If an individual has an unclear knowledge of the values and characteristics that

define his or her collective identity, if, for example, he or she is unsure if the group values

relationships at the expense of individual success, it will be very difficult for this person to

compare their group with other groups on this dimension. Without a clear and confident

definition of one’s collective or cultural identity, it would be impossible for an individual to

even engage in meaningful social comparison processes and to form a confident evaluation of

their group. Feelings of group pride or collective esteem then come only after having a clear

5

Page 6: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

and certain knowledge of one’s collective or cultural identity. The present research seeks, for

the first time, to empirically investigate this theoretical link between cultural identity clarity

and collective esteem.

The Cultural Narrative

It is difficult to assess a construct as rich and complex as the identity arising from

one’s collective or cultural group. Traditional measures of identity can be grouped into two

methodological approaches, the reactive or “traditional” and the spontaneous or “narrative”

(McGuire, 1984; McGuire & McGuire, 1981). The first emphasizes the individual’s traits and

is evaluated with questionnaires and scales, and the second emphasizes the nature of the

individual’s experience as they tell the “story of their group”. Traditional methods have been

used most often to study identity and typically require individuals to position themselves on a

dimension chosen by the experimenter, such as how they perceive their own, or their group’s

particular abilities or traits. A major weakness of the traditional method is that participants are

simply reacting to dimensions chosen by the researcher. No information is obtained about

whether the individual is genuinely concerned about this aspect of his or her self-concept

(McGuire, 1984). Furthermore, self-descriptions measured through the traditional approach

are largely de-contextualized and do not make full use of the more detailed, nuanced, and

intricately contextualized information involved in one’s self-concept. We therefore argue that

a more spontaneous, but nevertheless rigorous approach to measuring the content of people’s

self-concepts is necessary to fully account for the richness of individuals’ identities,

particularly their cultural identities.

At the individual level, McAdams’ (1996, 2001) seminal narrative approach to

personal identity assumes that a unified description of one’s identity can be construed through

a story, a spontaneous measure of identity. Indeed, according to McAdams, individuals confer

unity and meaning to their sense of self by constructing a coherent story that provides the

6

Page 7: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

person with a purposeful self-history. One’s personal identity is thus revealed in the capacity

to form and keep a particular narrative, or life story, going. McAdams (1996, 2001) has

developed, and extensively used the Life Story Interview as a method of accessing people’s

representations of their personal identity. The Life Story Interview is a structured sequence of

open-ended questions in which participants are first asked to divide their life into chapters and

to then briefly describe the content of each chapter. Participants are also required to describe

specific critical events, such as a nadir experience, an event representing a particularly low

point in one’s personal story.

In the present research, a Cultural Narrative Interview, modeled on the Life Story

Interview, was developed and used to investigate cultural identity clarity. In order to fully

account for the complexities of participants’ identities, this spontaneous and contextualized

method was employed. One of the foundations of a collective identity is the knowledge of a

shared history (Ashmore et al., 2004; Taylor, 1997, 2002). Accordingly, each individual in the

present study was asked to “tell” a group story, an internally represented narrative of the

particular cultural group of which he/she is a member (Ashmore et al., 2004). Rather than

historical accuracy, the cultural narratives could be subsequentally coded for clarity (Baerger

& McAdams, 1999). The link between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem could

thus be explored.

Research Context

The present research focused on two natural groups with a long, well-established

history of intergroup relations: Francophones and Anglophones in Quebec, Canada. Both

Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers possess interesting and rich backgrounds on which

to draw a cultural narrative. Both groups can be perceived as advantaged or disadvantaged,

depending on the social and temporal focus of the comparison. Anglophones have long

enjoyed the privilege of being an elite minority in Quebec despite living in a predominantly

7

Page 8: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

French province where approximately 80% of the residents are Francophone (Statistics

Canada, 2006). Francophones can be viewed as the more historically disenfranchised group,

for they have faced economic disadvantages and threats to their language and culture due to

their minority status in English-dominated North America. The growth of Francophone

nationalism in the 1960s, however, has to some extent reversed the intergroup power

distribution in Quebec, leaving Anglophone Quebecers feeling increasingly threatened

(Bourhis, 1994; Caldwell, 1984; Lepicq & Bourhis, 1995). There have been two referendums

on Quebec sovereignty where Quebecers voted on whether or not they wanted to secede from

the rest of Canada. In addition, there has been legislation designed to protect the French

language (Bill 101 and Bill 178). We hypothesize that the clarity of the cultural narratives,

depicting the competing histories and statuses of both Anglophone and Francophone

Quebecers, will be associated with collective esteem.

Study 1

The goal of Study 1 was twofold. The first goal was to test the main hypothesis that

cultural identity clarity as measured by the cultural narrative would be positively related to

collective esteem. The second goal was to provide a more complete understanding of

participants’ cultural identities using, for the first time, a new methodology—the cultural

narrative. Although used in the context of personal life stories, the narrative methodology has

not been used to explore cultural identity clarity, and may thus provide us with a more

comprehensive and contextualized understanding of Francophone and Anglophone cultural

identities. We hoped to pinpoint the significant narrative chapters for both Anglophones and

Francophones. Because the narratives do not represent an objective history, but rather a

history interpreted through the lens of the participant, we expected that this methodology

would allow participants to put emphasis on chapters that they deemed to be the most

important.

8

Page 9: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Method

Participants

Twenty Francophone (ten men and ten women) and twenty Anglophone (ten men and

ten women) Quebecers participated in Study 1. This relatively small sample size was

necessary given the labor-intensive nature of the procedure used to assess and code cultural

identity clarity through the Cultural Narrative Interview. The narratives of two Anglophone

participants were eliminated because one chose to tell a cultural narrative related to his Asian-

Canadian background and one focused on her personal narrative. A total of eighteen

Anglophone Quebecers (nine women and nine men) were retained for analysis. The mean age

for Anglophone participants was 20.2 years, ranging from 19-23 years old. The mean age of

Francophone participants was 21.5 years, ranging from 18-25. All Anglophone participants

reported speaking English as their maternal language and sixteen reported having lived in

Quebec since they were born. Two Anglophone participants were born in another Canadian

province and had been living in Quebec since the age of five and six years old. All twenty

Francophone participants reported speaking French as their maternal language, as well as

having lived in Quebec since they were born.

Materials and Procedure

All participants took part in the Cultural Narrative Interview first and then completed a

questionnaire assessing collective esteem. The Cultural Narrative Interview was a structured

sequence of open-ended questions that asked participants to construct and narrate their

group’s collective story. One male and one female who were native speakers of English each

interviewed five male and five female Anglophone participants. Similarly, one male and one

female who were native speakers of French each interviewed five male and five female

Francophone participants. Participants took between 40-60 minutes to complete the Cultural

Narrative Interview. All interviews were tape-recorded and later transcribed.

9

Page 10: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Following McAdams’ (1995) protocol, the Cultural Narrative required participants to

outline their group’s story organized into chapters, and briefly describe the overall content,

key themes, or events of each chapter. In the second part of the interview, participants were

asked to concentrate on a few key events that according to them, stood out in the story of their

group as particularly important. A key event was described as a specific happening, a critical

incident, or a significant episode in their people’s past. One specific request was for

participants to report a “nadir” experience, an experience that they considered to be a low

point in their group’s history, characterized by extremely negative emotions, such as despair,

disillusionment, terror, or guilt.

The construct of cultural identity clarity was operationalized by means of a coding

system based on Baerger and McAdam’s (1999) Life Story Coding Criteria for personal life

stories. The coding system that we used consisted of three indices: orientation, structure, and

integration. The orientation index refers to whether or not the individual provides the

background information necessary to understand the story. The structure index refers to the

extent to which the narrative displays some linear, chronological, and/or causal structure in

the transmission of factual information. Finally, the integration index refers to whether or not

the narrative imparts information in an integrated manner, ultimately communicating the

meaning of the experiences described within the context of the larger narrative.

Each narrative clarity index was coded according to a 7-point Likert scale ranging

from 1 (very low) to 7 (very high). Participants were given an overall clarity score that

averaged their narrative clarity indices. One native Quebecer, fluent in both English and

French listened to and coded all 38 narratives. Another native Francophone Quebecer, blind

to the purpose of the study coded the 20 French interviews, and a native Anglophone, also

blind to the purpose of the study coded the 18 English interviews. Intercorrelations among the

three cultural identity clarity indices are presented in Table 1.

10

Page 11: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

The questionnaire completed after the Cultural Narrative Interview was comprised of a

self-report scale that assessed participants’ collective esteem (Luhanten and Crocker, 1992).

This scale was translated into French using a back-translation procedure (Brislin, 1970). The

scale assessed global, relatively stable levels of collective esteem and was comprised of four

subscales. The focus of the present study was on the evaluative component of this scale,

including the two subscales representing membership and private collective esteem.

Membership esteem involves individuals’ judgments of how good or worthy they are as a

member of their group (e.g., “I am a worthy member of the English (French) Quebecer

community”). Private collective esteem assesses one’s personal judgment of how good one’s

group is (e.g., “I often regret that I belong to the English (French) Quebecer community

(reverse-coded)”). The obtained Cronbach’s alpha for the membership and private scales

combined was .71.

Results and Discussion

Preliminary Analysis

All the scores were normally distributed. All skewness and kurtosis values fell within

an acceptable range of -1.10 to - .03 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were identified

in the sample based on the criterion that no one deviated by more than 3 standard deviations

from the variable mean, and also showed a Mahanalobis distance greater than the exclusion

criterion set at p < .001. All 38 participants were thus retained for the analyses. No variable

involved more than 5% missing data.

Cultural Identity Clarity and Collective Esteem

In order to address the first goal of Study 1 and test our hypothesis that cultural

identity clarity would be positively related to collective esteem, we conducted a stepwise

regression analysis. In step 1, language group (Francophones vs. Anglophones) and cultural

identity clarity were entered as predictors of collective esteem. In step 2, the interaction

11

Page 12: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

between language group and clarity was added to the model in order to test if the relationship

between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem differed according to language group.

The results of the analysis yielded no main effect of language group on collective esteem, and

no main effect of clarity on collective esteem. However, the interaction between language

group and clarity was marginally significant, β = 29, p = .09, R2 = .10, implying that the

relationship between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem differed depending on a

participant’s language group. Although this relationship was only marginally significant, we

took this finding seriously as our sample size was relatively small, with only twenty and

eighteen participants in each of the language groups. The effect size of the interaction was

between small and medium (Cohen, 1988).

Given that the relationship between clarity and collective esteem appeared to be

different depending on participants’ membership as either an Anglophone or a Francophone,

we conducted separate analyses for each of the groups. For Francophones, the correlation

between clarity and collective esteem was found to be marginally, positively significant, r

= .41, N = 20, p = .08. Here, a co-efficient of .41 represents a medium effect size (Cohen,

1992). Although only marginally significant, it is noteworthy given the small number of

participants, and thus provides modest support for the hypothesized positive relationship

between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem. For Anglophones, however, the

relationship between narrative clarity and collective esteem was non-significant, r = -.18, N =

18, n.s. Based on the obtained relationships between narrative clarity and collective esteem,

our hypothesis was only, at best, partially supported. Cultural identity clarity and collective

esteem were positively related for Francophones, but not for Anglophones.

Cultural Narrative Content

The second goal of Study 1 was to provide a more complete understanding of

participants’ cultural identities, using, for the first time, the cultural narrative methodology.

12

Page 13: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Here we include a brief description of the cultural narratives of Francophone and Anglophone

participants, followed by a discussion of the significant chapters for each group. Both groups

were able to successfully generate a coherent story of their group and to pinpoint important

events for their people. Francophone participants spontaneously generated an average of 5.6

chapters as constituting their people’s story. One-hundred percent of Francophone

participants described the arrival of French colonizers in North America as constituting their

first chapter. All of the Francophone participants clearly perceived their cultural narrative

beginning as early as the 1500’s, with the discovery of the new world. Next, 95% of

Francophone participants described one or more chapters having to do with early conflicts

between French and English colonizers in new France, namely the 1754-1760 Conquest War,

which resulted in Great Britain taking over New France, and or the Patriot’s Rebellions, also

resulting in a defeat for the French at the hands of the English. Finally, 80% of Francophone

participants described events related to the theme of Francophone Quebecer nationalism as

constituting one or more chapters in their people’s story. The era when Maurice Duplessis

was Quebec’s Prime Minister (1936-1939, 1944-1959), the Quiet Revolution in the 1960s, the

creation of the separatist party “Parti Québébecois” in 1968, the election of René Levesque as

Quebec’s Premier, and/or the general theme of political independence of Quebec from Canada

appear to be important narrative building blocks for Francophone participants. Events that

Francophone Quebecers reported as being particularly important were most often the 1837-

1838 Patriots rebellion and the 1754-1760 British Conquest.

Compared to Francophone participants, Anglophone participants spontaneously

generated a smaller number of chapters, describing an average of 3.8 chapters as constituting

their people’s story. For eighty-one percent of Anglophone participants, Chapter 1 involved

the description of European colonizers’ arrival in North America, and/or the early interactions

and conflicts between French and English colonizers in the New World. The next important

13

Page 14: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

narrative building block for Anglophones was events related to the theme of growing

Francophone nationalism from the 1960s on (the period of the Quiet Revolution), and the

consequent feelings of threat within the Anglophone Quebecer community. Seventy-eight

percent of Anglophone participants described the Francophone Quebecer separatist movement

generally, the 1980 and 1995 referendums, and the introduction of education and language

laws in the 1970s as constituting one or more chapters in their people’s history. Events that

Anglophone Quebecers reported as being particularly important were events related to

Francophone nationalism including the two referendums for Quebec sovereignty and the

introduction of Bill 101.

The Significant Chapters for Francophones and Anglophones

Most Francophones and Anglophones reported the same historical events in the story

of their respective groups. For example, a large number of participants, regardless of the

group they belong to, reported events associated with the five following historical periods:

The New World era, The Conquest era, the Duplessis era, The Quiet Revolution era, and

finally the present time. Thus, both groups expressed noticeable areas of consensus regarding

the basic outlines of their respective groups’ stories.

However, the importance given to the reported key historical events differed

dramatically between the two cultural groups. Francophone participants spent more time than

Anglophones on the early conflicts between Anglophones and Francophones, showing the

importance of this historical period (the Conquest era). In contrast, Anglophones accorded

relatively greater importance to more contemporary historical events. This is evidenced by, as

we reported in the previous section, the fact that Francophones provided an outline of their

people’s story that contained significantly more chapters than did Anglophones. Anglophone

participants generated an average of 3.8 chapters when telling their people’s story,

significantly fewer than their Francophone counterparts (M = 5.6; t(34) = -4.60, p < .001).

14

Page 15: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

This difference is linked to the fact that Francophones provided a more detailed account of the

colonial historical period. Indeed, for more than half (53%) of our Anglophone participants,

Chapter 2 was situated in the second half of the 1900s. However, Chapter 2 focused on events

that took place before 1838 for all of our Francophone participants (100%), namely, the

settling of the French people in Nouvelle-France and the inter-colonial conflicts. In short, the

Anglophone Quebecers’ narrative mostly took place after the 1960s-1970s (the Quiet

Revolution era); whereas, several important narrative building blocks for Francophones refer

to events that go back to the period ranging from the early 1500s to 1867 (The Conquest era).

Asking participants of both groups about their “nadir” experience provided another

example of the striking differences in the importance given to historical events. A “nadir”

experience represents a low point in the participant’s cultural narrative. Such a narrative nadir

occurred between the years 1754 and 1838 for almost half of Francophones (45%), compared

to six percent of the Anglophones. In fact, for Anglophones, their main narrative nadir

occurred much more recently: in the 1970s for a third (33%) of them (compared with fifteen

percent of Francophones), and between the years 1990 to 2002 for another third (33%) of

them (compared with 10% of Francophones). Overall, we found that the early years of the

Conquest Era during which the French fought and were defeated by the English played a key

role in shaping the cultural narrative of Francophone Quebecer participants. The Anglophone

Quebecer participants described a cultural narrative that was primarily shaped by the growing

threat from Francophone Quebecers from the era of the Quiet Revolution on.

Interpreting the Results

The hypothesized positive relationship between cultural identity clarity and collective

esteem, measured using the cultural narratives of Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers,

was only partially supported. This relationship was marginally, positively significant for

Francophones, and non-significant for Anglophones. These results are disappointing in that

15

Page 16: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

they provide only weak support for our hypothesis, and are not consistent with the strong

relationship obtained between clarity and esteem at a personal level (Campbell, 1990). At first

glance, our findings appear to indicate that clarity may not be as central for collective esteem

as we had anticipated. However, a comprehensive analysis of participants’ cultural narratives

is of great value when attempting to interpret these results. The narratives provided us with a

wealth of information pertaining to Francophones’ and Anglophones’ overall perceptions of

the history of their group and pointed to certain important, and especially threatening, periods

for each group. The fact that very different historical periods, characterized by the experience

of group-level threat, appear to shape and heavily influence the cultural narratives of each

group signals that our evaluation of clarity based on the participants’ overall group narratives

may have diluted or glossed over the crucial impact of certain key events, events that actually

had the most impact in shaping a group’s identity.

In Study 2, we thus examine perceptions of cultural identity clarity at different key

periods of a group’s history, rather than in an overall, gestalt fashion. In addition, the

information gleaned from the narratives points to another important factor often involved in

predicting collective well-being, namely the centrality of the experience of group-level threat

for both groups, and the impact that it had on shaping the group’s overall narrative. Feelings

of group-level threat, also known as group-based relative deprivation, at key points in the

group’s history are also assessed in Study 2 and related to participants’ experiences of

collective esteem.

According to the cultural narratives, Francophones found the early years of the

Conquest era to be particularly important and threatening for their group. Anglophones, on

the other hand, found the later period from the Quiet Revolution onwards to be the most

important and threatening. These differences in the importance of key events for each group

are reminiscent of recent advances in the field of relative deprivation (de la Sablonnière,

16

Page 17: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

2008; de la Sablonnière, Taylor, Perozzo & Sadykova, in press). Research has long

investigated feelings of group-based or social relative deprivation, which refers to an overall,

subjective feeling of threat that arises when group members engage in social comparison, and

perceive their group to be receiving less than what they feel they are entitled to (Crosby,

1982). However, the results of recent studies suggest that: 1) it is important to use more than

one past or future point of social comparison when predicting collective esteem using relative

deprivation, and 2) to have an impact on collective esteem, the selected point of social

comparison has to be an important historical event for group members (de la Sablonnière,

2008; de la Sablonnière, et al., in press). Specifically, instead of an overall impression of a

group’s history, it is certain important events in the past that are the group’s primary reference

points against which their experience of well-being is assessed. In Study 2 then, instead of an

overall impression of a group’s history, we propose to use specific events that are perceived to

be particularly important for the group.

Study 2

In Study 2, we continue to examine the centrality of cultural identity clarity in

determining collective esteem; however, we do so while taking feelings of relative deprivation

into account. We hypothesize that the clarity of a group’s cultural identity, assessed at a

specific point in time that is important and threatening for the group would mediate the

relationship between relative deprivation and collective esteem. A period that is considered by

group members as a major threat is particularly influential in defining a group’s identity,

which in turn is associated with increased collective esteem. At key points in a group’s

history, certainty about one’s group identity caused by a threatening evaluation of a specific

period will drive the positive relationship between group level threat and collective esteem.

Specifically, for the context of the present study, we propose that this mediational relationship

will be obtained for both Francophones and Anglophones if the historical period in question

17

Page 18: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

represents an important and threatening period for the members of each group respectively.

From Study 1, we predict that the most important and threatening period for Francophone

Quebecers will be the time of the Struggle between Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers

(The Conquest era). For Anglophones, the most important and threatening period will be the

time of the Quiet Revolution.

Based on the narratives of both Anglophones and Francophones obtained in Study 1,

we created a measure that allowed a new group of Anglophone and Francophone participants

in Study 2 to rate their group’s experience of relative deprivation and cultural identity clarity

at a selection of times in the group’s history. We thus measured both social relative

deprivation and cultural identity clarity at selected key points in time, rather than evaluating

cultural identity clarity based on the groups’ entire histories.

Method

Participants

Native French- and English-speaking students who had been living in Quebec since

they were born participated in Study 2. Participants were recruited by verbal announcements

made in classrooms, and by means of posters placed in one major Anglophone university as

well as two major Francophone universities in Montreal. Francophone and Anglophone

Quebecers, between 18 and 27 years of age and of European descent, interested in completing

a “History and Well-Being” questionnaire in exchange for monetary compensation were

asked to contact the principal investigator to schedule an appointment.

A total of 61 Anglophone Quebecers (24 males and 37 females) and 61 Francophones

Quebecers (18 males and 43 females) completed the questionnaire. All Anglophone

participants reported English as their maternal and dominant language, and all Francophone

reported French as their maternal and dominant language. The mean age for Anglophones was

18

Page 19: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

20.9 years old (ranging from 18 to 27), and the mean age for Francophones was 21.5 years old

(ranging from 18 to 27).

Measures

Study 2 was devised in a more structured manner than Study 1. In Study 1,

participants spontaneously narrated their own perception of their ingroup’s story. In Study 2,

the important elements of the Anglophone and the Francophone Quebecer cultural narrative

that were generated in Study 1 were integrated into a single summary of the narratives’ shared

points and then presented to both Anglophone and Francophone participants. Moreover, while

the methodology in Study 1 precluded the use of a large sample size, Study 2 used a

questionnaire format, which allowed testing a greater number of Anglophone and

Francophone Quebecers. Finally, Study 2 utilized self-report measures in order to assess the

constructs of cultural identity clarity, relative deprivation, and collective esteem.

In the first part of the questionnaire, participants were asked to read the summary of

Quebec’s history. Following the cultural narrative interviews that were conducted with

Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers in Study 1, the history of Quebec was divided into

five chapters: The New World era (Chapter 1), The Conquest era (Chapter 2), the Duplessis

era (Chapter 3), and The Quiet Revolution era (Chapter 4) and a chapter about the present

(Chapter 5). Each chapter was described as objectively and neutrally as possible by respecting

facts and refraining from editorial comments. Following is a brief description of each

chapter.

Chapter 1, ‘The New World Era’ ranged from the late 1400s through to 1754 and

described the various European people who reached “The New World”. Chapter 2, ‘The

Conquest Era’ which ranged from 1754 to 1867, described the struggles between French and

English colonies in the New World, which resulted in the domination of Britain over the

French colonies in Quebec. Chapter 3, ‘The Duplessis Era’ ranged from the Canadian

19

Page 20: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Confederation in 1867 through to the death of Premier Maurice Duplessis in1959. Chapter

4,‘The Quiet Revolution Era’ which ranged from 1960 to 1995, described the period of

Quebec modernization and empowerment and discussed the arrival of the separatist party

“Parti Québécois” onto Quebec’s political scene, the introduction of language laws aimed at

making French the predominant language in Quebec, and the two referendums on Quebec

sovereignty. Finally, Chapter 5, ‘The Present’ alluded to the main issues faced by Quebecers

today including Quebec’s growing ethnic diversity; and the globalization phenomenon.

Participants were asked to read each of these chapters, and then answer questions

pertaining to how they perceived what happened to their own ingroup during each chapter of

Quebec history. These questions assessed group-based relative deprivation. Participants were

also asked to indicate how confident or sure they felt about their overall ratings for each

chapter. This question assessed cultural identity clarity for each chapter. Following is a

detailed description of these two measures, along with our measure of collective esteem.

Relative deprivation. Measures of relative deprivation used for chapter 1 to 5 were

adapted from previous studies that assessed both the cognitive and the evaluative components

of social relative deprivation (Dambrun et al., 2006; de la Sablonnière & Tougas, 2008). The

cognitive component of social relative deprivation was assessed by asking participants to

indicate “the extent to which English/French Quebecers were advantaged or disadvantaged

compared to French/English Quebecers in terms of… a) social rights, b) political leverage, c)

economic prosperity and d) language and culture”. The scale for these questions ranged from -

5 (definitely disadvantaged), to 0 (Equal), to +5 (definitely advantaged). The following two

questions evaluated the affective component of relative deprivation: “To what extent are you

satisfied with regards to the general situation for English/French Quebecers during this

chapter.”(recoded); “To what extent are you frustrated/angry with regards to the general

situation for English/French Quebecers during this chapter.” The scale for these questions

20

Page 21: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

ranged from 0 (totally satisfied/not at all frustrated), to 5 (moderately), to 10 (not at all

satisfied/totally frustrated). For these two last items, responses were recoded such that the

scale ranged from -5 to 5 to be consistent with the cognitive component of social relative

deprivation. Responses from the 6 items of social relative deprivation were added and

averaged to form a composite score so that 5 indicated high relative deprivation. The

Cronbach’s alpha values for Anglophones’ and Francophones’ perceived social relative

deprivation were .79 for Chapter 1, .95 for Chapter 2, .89 for Chapter 3, .94 for Chapter 4

and .84 for Chapter 5.

Cultural identity clarity. Cultural identity clarity was assessed by asking participants

to rate how confident or sure they felt about their overall ratings of what happened to their

ingroup during each chapter of Quebec history. The scale for these questions ranged from 0

(not at all sure) to 10 (totally sure). This format for evaluating cultural identity clarity has

been used successfully by de la Sablonnière and colleagues (in press). In that study, cultural

identity clarity at the “present time” was found to be moderately related to collective esteem

(r = .25, p< .001), suggesting that although cultural identity clarity and collective esteem

constitute two distinct concepts, they are indeed related.

Collective esteem. This concept was assessed using 6 items from the Luhtanen and

Crocker’s Collective Self-Esteem Scale (CSES; 1992). The membership component and the

private component of collective self-esteem were assessed because both refer to the evaluative

dimension of collective esteem and subsequently to well-being. Membership esteem involves

individuals’ judgments of how good or worthy they are as members of their social ingroup.

Private collective self-esteem assesses one’s personal judgments of how good one’s social

ingroup is (for a discussion see also Roccas et al., 2008). Three statements assessed the

membership component of collective self-esteem: “I am a worthy member of the English (or

French) Quebecer community”; “I feel I don’t have much to offer to the English (or French)

21

Page 22: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Quebecer community” (recoded) and “I often feel that I am useless member of the English (or

French) Quebecer community” (recoded). Three questions evaluated the private component of

collective self-esteem: “I often regret that I belong to the English (or French) Quebecer

community” (recoded); “In general, I am glad to be a member of the English (or French)

Quebecer community”; “I feel good about the English (or French) Quebecer community”. All

ratings were made using 11-point Likert scales, where 0 = strongly disagree, 5 = neither

agree nor disagree, and 10 = strongly agree. Responses from the 6 items of collective esteem

were added and averaged to form a composite score. Appropriate items were reverse-scored

such that a high score on this variable indicated positive collective esteem. The Cronbach’s

alpha for the collective esteem scale was .79.

Results and Discussion

Preliminary analysis

All the scores were normally distributed. All skewness and kurtosis values fell within

an acceptable range of -1.59 to + 1.31 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No outliers were

identified in the sample based on the criterion that no one deviated by more than 3 standard

deviations from the variable mean, and also showed a Mahanalobis distance greater than the

exclusion criterion set at p < .001. One-hundred-and-twenty-two participants were thus

retained for the analyses. Considering that no variable possessed more than 5% of missing

data (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001), no variable was excluded.

Descriptive Analysis

In this section, we first present descriptive analyses for all variables. Second, using

group-based trajectory modeling (Jones & Nagin, 2007; Jones, Nagin & Roeder, 2001; Nagin,

1999), we explore each cultural group’s trajectory of relative deprivation over time. This was

done in order to determine if the group members’ experiences of group-based relative

deprivation was consistent with what we would predict based on our analysis of participants’

22

Page 23: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

cultural narratives in Study 1. This analysis will help us determine if the Conquest Era for

Francophone Quebecers and the Quiet Revolution Era for Anglophone Quebecers, are indeed

perceived by members of these groups as important periods in which their group experienced

high levels of group-based relative deprivation.

Descriptive Statistics. Descriptive analyses, as depicted in Table 2, of cultural identity

clarity, revealed two main findings. First, the means indicated that both Anglophones and

Francophones reported moderate levels of cultural identity clarity for Chapter 1, but relatively

high levels of cultural identity clarity for Chapters 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6, as scores were situated

above the scale’s midpoint. Additionally, the means for collective esteem indicated relatively

high levels of collective well-being among both Anglophones and Francophones, as scores for

both groups were situated above the scale’s midpoint (MA = 8.2 and MF = 7.4). An ANOVA

comparing Anglophones and Francophones revealed a significant difference on collective

self-esteem (F (1, 120) = 13.7, p < .001), such that Francophone Quebecers had a higher level

of overall collective esteem (M = 8.2) than did Anglophone Quebecers (M = 7.4).

Anglophones’ and Francophones’ perceptions of relative deprivation over time. In

order to identify Anglophones’ and Francophones’ perceptions of group-based relative

deprivation over time, group-based trajectory modeling (Jones & Nagin, 2007; Jones, Nagin

& Roeder, 2001; Nagin, 1999) was conducted. This analysis, which has been successfully

used in the domain of social psychology (see de la Sablonnière, Taylor, Perozzo, & Sadykova,

in press) allowed us to test whether Francophones and Anglophones follow a different pattern

of group-based relative deprivation across time, and furthermore which historical period was

judged to be the most threatening for Francophones and Anglophones.

Group-based trajectory modeling provided a flexible method for identifying distinctive

clusters of individual trajectories within a population. The statistical model underlying group-

based trajectories uses finite mixtures of specified probability distributions to determine, by

23

Page 24: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

maximum likelihood, the parameter estimates describing the model that best fits the data

(Jones & Nagin, 2007; Nagin, 1999, 2005). In order to estimate the optimal model, the

number and shape (linear or U-shaped for example) of trajectories is a key step in model

selection. Consistent with the recommendations of D’Unger, Land, McCall, and Nagin (1998)

and Nagin (2005), the Bayesian Information criterion (BIC) was used to select the optimal

model. The BIC provides an index of how well the model selected fits the data, in addition to

favouring parsimony. In general, the BIC closest to zero denotes the most appropriate model.

As all the variables explored in the present study were measured using Likert-type scales, the

censored normal distribution (CNORM) was used to estimate trajectories and group

memberships (Jones & Nagin, 2007; Jones et al., 2001; Nagin, 1999, 2005). The program

used to perform group-based trajectory modeling is a customized SAS-based procedure

(PROC TRAJ; Jones et al., 2001). An important output of the model is the posterior

probabilities of group membership. These probabilities estimate the probabilities of belonging

to each group for each individual. Consequently, each participant in the sample is assigned to

the group with the largest posterior probabilities of group membership.

Table 3 reports BIC scores for different models tested. A model with two trajectories,

model 4, was found to be the best fitting model as revealed by the BIC. Figure 1 shows the

estimated trajectories associated with relative deprivation perceived by Anglophones and

Francophones regarding the pattern of their group’s general condition in terms of social

rights, political leverage, economic prosperity, language and culture.

Inspection of Figure 1 shows that the optimal model includes two different trajectories

that follow the “typical” expected trajectory of Relative Deprivation for Francophones and

Anglophones. Indeed, each trajectory is directly linked to the particular history of each

language group in question. Specifically, all Francophones, that is 100 % or 61 persons,

follow the typical trajectory of Francophones. Ninety-eight point 4 percent or 60

24

Page 25: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Anglophones followed the Anglophone’s typical pattern; whereas, 1.6 % of the Anglophones,

that is one Anglophone, followed the Francophone’s typical pattern. This case was removed

from further analysis because it differed significantly from other members of the group and

thus, was considered an outlier.

The typical expected trajectory of Francophone Quebecers revealed that almost all

Francophones who participated in the study had first seen their level of Social relative

deprivation increase from the New World (Chapter 1) to the struggle between Francophone

and Anglophone Quebecers (Chapter 2), to then decrease in the transition between the

Struggle and the Quiet Revolution periods (Chapter 4). Finally, the level of relative

deprivation increases at a slightly slower pace until the time of our study (Chapter 5). Thus,

the Struggle between Anglophone and Francophone Quebecers (Chapter 2) appears to

represent a major threat for Francophones.

Anglophones that followed the “typical” trajectory perceived quite a different pattern of

group-based relative deprivation, such that their level of relative deprivation greatly decreased

from the New World (Chapter 1) to the struggle between Francophone and Anglophone

Quebecers (Chapter 2), to then increase until the Quiet revolution (Chapter 4). However,

Anglophones felt less relative deprivation in the present (Chapter 5) than during the Quiet

Revolution. Accordingly, the Quiet Revolution (Chapter 4) constituted the most major threat

for Anglophone Quebecers.

Mediation Analysis

Once we were certain that these two periods (the Conquest era for Francophones and

the Quiet Revolution era for Anglophones) constituted the most threatening periods for the

two groups involved, we tested our hypothesis that cultural identity clarity acts as a

psychological mechanism that mediates the relationship between group-based relative

deprivation and collective esteem for these periods. However, we first examined the

25

Page 26: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

intercorrelations between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem across all periods in

order to confirm that during a period representing the greatest threat to the group, the

association between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem would be the strongest. For

Francophones we then tested the hypothesized mediational relationship for the Conquest Era,

and for Anglophones, we tested the mediational relationship for the era of the Quiet

Revolution.

First, the intercorrelations between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem were

examined across all chapters for Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers (See Table 4),

providing preliminary evidence in support of our hypothesis. As expected, for Anglophones,

the link between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem was significant and positive at

the time of the Quiet Revolution (Chapter 4) (r = .30, n = 60, p <. 05) while the correlation

proved to be non-significant for the other chapters (r value range from .00 to .25). For

Francophones, cultural identity clarity during the Conquest Era (Chapter 2) was found to be

positive and to have the highest correlation with collective esteem (r = .44, n = 61, p <. 01).

The link between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem was also positive for chapters

three to five (r value range from .26 to .36) and non-significant for chapter one (r = .09, n =

61, p = .49). Together, these results indicated that during a time of threat, an historical period

that enhances the sense of cohesiveness of the group, the link between identity clarity and

collective esteem was positive.

In order to further test the hypothesis that cultural identity clarity mediates the

relationship between social relative deprivation and collective esteem, the ratings provided by

Francophones for the Conquest Era were subjected to multiple regressions (Barron & Kenny,

1986; see Figure 2). Standardized coefficients (βs) are reported. In support of our hypothesis,

we found that group-based relative deprivation significantly predicted cultural identity clarity

(a = .48, p <.001) and collective esteem (d = .47, p < .001) when it was the only predictor.

26

Page 27: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

When cultural identity clarity was introduced to the model, the direct effect of relative

deprivation on collective esteem dropped (c = .33, p < .05), and cultural identity clarity

significantly predicted collective esteem, b = .28, p < .05. The Sobel test indicated that the

indirect effect of relative deprivation on collective esteem via cultural identity clarity was

significant, z = 1.97, p = .05, providing evidence for a partial mediation. Cultural identity

clarity partially mediated the relationship between relative deprivation and collective esteem

for Francophone Quebecers during the Conquest Era.

The ratings provided by Anglophones for the era of the Quiet Revolution were also

analyzed using multiple regressions (see Figure 3). We found that group-based relative

deprivation significantly predicted cultural identity clarity (a = .35, p < .05) and collective

esteem (d = .27, p < .05) when it was the only predictor. When cultural identity clarity was

introduced to the model, the direct effect of relative deprivation on collective esteem

completely disappeared (b = . 19, n.s.), and cultural identity clarity was a marginally

significant predictor of collective esteem, c = .24, p = .08. The Sobel test indicated that the

indirect effect of relative deprivation on collective esteem via cultural identity clarity

approached significance, z = 1.53, p = .13. Cultural identity clarity mediated the relationship

between relative deprivation and collective esteem at a level that approached significance for

Anglophone Quebecers during the era of the Quiet Revolution.

At a time of threat to the group, be it the Conquest era for Francophones, or the Quiet

Revolution era for Anglophones, the relationship between group-based relative deprivation

and collective esteem was positive. This relationship was at least partially mediated by

cultural identity clarity in both cases, pointing to the importance that clarity plays during these

particular historical events. A threatening event defines and clarifies a group’s identity, which

in turn leads to greater collective esteem. This result is particularly interesting given its

counter-intuitive nature. Indeed, it is not the most positive, nor is it even the most recent event

27

Page 28: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

that defines a group’s identity and is associated with collective esteem. Instead it is a

historical period of group-level threat that appears to be driving a group’s experience of

clarity and well-being.

General Discussion

For the first time, cultural identity clarity and its relationship with collective esteem

was examined in two studies conducted with Francophone and Anglophone Quebecers. Study

1 was an initial investigation into the relationship between cultural identity clarity and

collective esteem using a novel methodology, the cultural narrative. The results of Study 1

were somewhat disappointing, in that they only partially supported our theoretical predictions.

The relationship between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem was marginally,

positively significant for Francophones, and non-significant for Anglophones. Thus, Study 2

was conducted in order to better predict collective esteem based on participants’ perception of

group-based relative deprivation and cultural identity clarity, measured at separate periods in

a group’s history. Rather than evaluating cultural identity clarity based on a gestalt impression

of a group’s history, key events, deemed to be especially threatening, and thus defining, for

the group were explored. We therefore combined past research from the field of relative

deprivation with our novel hypothesis pertaining to cultural identity clarity. In doing so, we

predicted that cultural identity clarity would mediate the positive relationship between group-

based relative deprivation and collective esteem at important, threatening, and thus defining

events in a group’s history. Our predictions were supported when Francophone Quebecers

were asked about the Conquest era, and when Anglophone Quebecers were asked about the

era of the Quiet Revolution.

Theoretical Contributions and Future Directions

The present research was successful in highlighting the importance of cultural identity

clarity for collective esteem, providing support for, and extending Taylor’s (1997, 2002)

28

Page 29: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

theory of cultural identity clarity. Specifically, knowing one’s group with certainty and clarity

predicted collective well-being when it was examined at key historical periods that

represented a time of group-level threat. Indeed, it was this experience of group-level threat

that appeared to drive the hypothesized positive relationship between cultural identity clarity

and well-being. In future research then, cultural identity clarity cannot be examined in a

decontextualized manner, solely at the present time, or as an overall feeling of a group’s

history. From the present research, and consistent with other research examining relative

deprivation (de la Sablonnière et al., in press), we understand that it is essential to explore

specific historical periods that are important for the group. Interestingly, it was not the most

positive or the most recent events that were the most influential here. Instead, it was those that

were the most threatening.

Our research reinforces current theorizing in relative deprivation research in that it

emphasizes the importance of taking key historical events into consideration when evaluating

relative deprivation and its relationship with collective esteem. However, the results of our

study also suggest that further research needs to be conducted in this domain. In Study 2, our

findings showed a positive relationship between group-level threat and collective esteem.

However, the more intuitively obvious relationship, where group-based relative deprivation is

negatively related to collective esteem, has also been found (de la Sablonnière et al., in press).

Future research needs to more closely examine this relationship perhaps by exploring the

nature of the key historical period in question. The extent to which the period represents an

eventual gain or loss for the group might influence the relationship between relative

deprivation and esteem. Again, we point to the importance of contextualizing one’s

examination of group level well-being in this group’s rich and varied history.

Methodological Contributions

29

Page 30: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Both of the present studies employed an underused, but promising methodology—the

Cultural Narrative. Hammack (2008) along with Ashmore et al. (2004) argue for the use of

such a methodology when studying identity issues. Hammack (2008) posits that the use of

narratives is an interdisciplinary and comprehensive approach to studying identity, while

Ashmore et al. (2004), argue that a group’s shared story is an important component of their

collective identity and even suggest devising a semi-structured measure of identity based on

McAdams Life Story Interview (1996, 2001). This was precisely our strategy for the present

research. Our use of the narrative methodology had unexpected advantages. It provided us

with a nuanced understanding of each group’s history, an understanding that was more

complete than what we would have gleaned from a more traditional approach to the study of

cultural identity. We were able to understand the differential impact of particular historical

periods on participants’ experience of identity and collective well-being. Future research

examining cultural identity might benefit from such a comprehensive methodology.

Applied Contributions

For groups faced with cultural identity challenges, for example, immigrants to a new

culture, or groups struggling with a history of colonization, a focus on clarifying cultural

identity might result in increased feelings of collective esteem. In particular, articulating and

defining an especially threatening historical event, perhaps one that the group has collectively

overcome, might result in a clarification of their cultural identity, which in turn would lead to

feelings of collective esteem. Indeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has

recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine the

well-being effects of experimentally increasing feelings of certainty about group members’

collective histories, or about times of historical threat. An intervention constructed around a

group’s history might be more effective than simply boosting self-esteem or psychological

30

Page 31: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

well-being on a case-by-case basis, and would instead be a contextualized method of building

the foundation of cultural identity, and by extension, collective well-being.

Conclusion

The present research successfully explored cultural identity clarity and examined its

relationship with collective esteem. This relationship, which was previously tested only at an

individual level, was thus expanded to a group level of analysis. More importantly, the

present research emphasized the significance of contextualizing group-level psychological

variables. Our results indicate that key events in a group’s history have a crucial impact on the

construction of its members’ current experiences of collective well-being. Therefore,

examining the psychological experiences of group members means going deeper than

obtaining a gestalt, briefly examined impression of current feelings. For social psychologists

engaged in understanding the human quest for psychological well-being, a historically-

embedded approach might generate a more comprehensive appreciation and awareness of its

diverse antecedents.

31

Page 32: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

References

Ashmore, R. D., Deaux, K., & McLaughlin-Volpe, T. (2004). An organizational

framework for collective identity: Articulation and significance of

multidimensionality. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 80-114.

Baerger, D. R. & McAdams, D. P. (1999). Life story coherence and its relation to

psychological well-being. Narrative Inquiry,9, 69-96.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in

social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Baumgardner, A. H. (1990). To know oneself is to like oneself: Self-certainty and self- affect.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1062-1072.

Bouris, R. Y. (1994). Ethnic and language attitudes in Quebec. In J. W. Berry & J. A.

Laponce (Eds.), Ethnicity and culture in Canada: The research landscape (pp. 322-

360). Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-Cultural

Psychology, 1, 185-216.

Caldwell, G. (1984). Anglo-Quebec: Demographic realities and options for the future. In R.

Y. Bourhis (Ed.). Conflict and language planning in Quebec (pp. 205-221). Clevedon:

Multilingual Matters.

Campbell, J. D. (1990). Self-esteem and clarity of the self-concept. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 59, 538-549.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale,

NJ: Erlbaum

Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155-159.

Crosby, F. (1982). Relative deprivation and working women. New York: Oxford University

32

Page 33: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Press

Dambrun, M., Taylor, D. M., McDonald, D. A., Crush, J., & Méot, A. (2006). The relative

deprivation- gratification continuum and the attitudes of South Africans towards

immigrants: A test of the V-curve hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 91, 1032-1044.

de la Sablonnière, R. (2008). Le bien-être psychologique des francophones et des

anglophones : le rôle des points tournants de l’histoire du Québec. Diversité Urbaine,

automne, 131-144.

de la Sablonnière, R., Taylor, D. M., Perozzo, C., & Sadykova, N. (in press).

Reconceptualizing relative deprivation in the context of dramatic social change: The

challenge confronting the people of Kyrgyzstan. European Journal of Social

Psychology.

de la Sablonnière, R., & Tougas, F. (2008). Relative deprivation and social identity in times

of dramatic social change: The case of nurses. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,

38, 2293-2314.

D’Unger, A. V., Land, K. C., McCall, P. L., & Nagin, D. S. (1998). How many latent classes

of delinquent/criminal careers? Results from mixed Poisson regression analyses.

Journal of Sociology, 103, 1593-1630.

Erikson, E. H. (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis. New York: Norton.

Hammack, P. L. (2008). Narrative and the cultural psychology of identity. Personality and

Social Psychology Review, 12, 222-247.

Jones, B. L., & Nagin, D. S. (2007). Advances in group-based trajectory modeling and an

SAS procedure for estimating them. Sociological Methods and Research, 35, 542-571.

Jones, B. L., Nagin, D. S., & Roeder, K. (2001). A SAS procedure based on mixture models

for estimating developmental trajectories. Sociological Methods and Research, 29,

33

Page 34: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

374-393.

Lepicq, d., & Bourhis, R. Y. (1995). Aménagement linguistique et norme langagière au

Québec. Situations du français, 33, 109-128.

Luhtanen, R., & Crocker, J. (1992). A collective self-esteem scale: Self-evaluation of one's

social identity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 302-318.

Maslow, A. H. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York: Harper & Brothers.

McAdams, D. P. (1995). What do we know when we know a person? Journal of Personality,

63, 365-396.

McAdams, D. P. (1996). Personality, modernity, and the storied self : A contemporary

framework for studying persons. Psychological Inquiry, 7, 295-321.

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5,

100-122.

McGuire, W. J. (1984). Search for the self : Going beyond self-esteem and the reactive self.

In R. A. Zucker, J. Arnoff, & A. I. Rabin (Eds.), Personality and the prediction of

behavior (pp. 73-120). Montreal : Academic Press, Inc.

McGuire, W. J., & McGuire, C. V. (1981). The spontaneous self-concept as affected by

personal distinctiveness. In M. D. Lynch, A. A. Norem-Hebeisen, & K. J. Gergen

(Eds.), Self-concept : Advances in theory and research (pp. 147-171). Cambridge:

Ballinger Publishing Company.

Nagin, D. S. (1999). Analyzing developmental trajectories: A semiparametric, group-based

approach. Psychological Methods, 4, 139-157.

Nagin, D. S. (2005). Group-based modeling of development. Cambridge, MA : Harvard

34

Page 35: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

University Press.

Roccas, S., & Brewer, M. B. (2002). Social identity complexity. Personality and Social

Psychology Review, 6, 88-106.

Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy. Boston:

Houghton Mifflin.

Statistics Canada (2006). Detailed Mother Tongue (148), Single and Multiple Language

Responses (3) and Sex (3) for the Population of Canada, Provinces, Territories,

Census Metropolitan Areas and Census Agglomerations, 2006 Census - 20% Sample

Data, Canadian Census 2006.

Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Needham

Heights: Allyn and Bacon.

Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of

intergroup relations. London: Academic Press.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W. G.

Austin & S. Worchel. (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-

47). Monterey:Brooks/Cole.

Taylor, D.M. (1997). The quest for collective identity: The plight of disadvantaged ethnic

minorities. Canadian Psychology, 38, 174-189.

Taylor, D. M., (2002) The quest for identity: From minority groups to Generation Xers.

Westport, CT: Praeger Publications.

35

Page 36: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Table 1.

Intercorrelations Among the Narrative Clarity Indices.

1 2 3

1. Orientation 1 .84*** .66***

2. Structure 1 .68***

3. Integration 1

Note. N = 38, *** p ≤ .001 (2-tailed).

36

Page 37: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Table 2.

Means and Standard Deviations for Independent and Dependent Variables

Anglophones (n=61) Francophones (n=61)

Mean Std. Dev. Mean Std. Dev.

Relative deprivation

Chapter 1: New World -0.7 1.0 -0.8 1.7

Chapter 2 : Conquest -2.5 1.3 3.0 1.6

Chapter 3 : Duplessis -1.6 1.3 1.6 1.5

Chapter 4: Quiet Rev. 1.9 1.7 -1.8 1.6

Chapter 5: Now 0.8 1.7 -0.8 1.3

Cultural identity clarity

Chapter 1: New World 4.8 2.4 5.1 2.2

Chapter 2 : Conquest 6.5 2.1 6.9 1.8

Chapter 3 : Duplessis 6.3 2.1 6.3 1.8

Chapter 4: Quiet Rev. 7.5 1.9 7.4 1.5

Chapter 5: Now 7.5 1.7 7.3 1.8

Collective esteem 7.4 1.4 8.2 1.1

37

Page 38: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Table 3.

Bayesian information criterion (BIC) by model type

Model K Ordre BIC

1 1 2 -1368.93

2 2 2, 2 -1358.50

3 3 2, 2, 2 -1368.11

4 2 3, 3 -1213.37

Note. K, the number of groups. The order indicates

whether the trajectory was fit using a constant (0), a

linear (1), a quadratic (2), or a cubic (3) function.

38

Page 39: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Table 4.

Correlations between cultural identity clarity and collective esteem per chapter

Collective identity clarity: Anglophones (n = 61) Francophones (n = 61)

Chapter 1: New World .00 t .09 t

Chapter 2 : Conquest .01 t .44**

Chapter 3 : Duplessis .13 t .33**

Chapter 4: Quiet Rev. .30* .26*

Chapter 5: Now .25 t .36**

Note. t p ≤ .10, * p ≤ .05, ** p ≤ .01 (2-tailed).

39

Page 40: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Figure 1. Trajectories of social relative deprivation

-5

-4

-3

-2

-1

0

1

2

3

4

5

The New World The Struggle Maurice Duplessis'Era

The Quiet Revolution Now

Historical period

Soc

ial r

elat

ive

depr

ivat

ion

(-5=

Low

; +5

= hi

gh)

Anglophone's typical pattern Francophone's typical pattern

40

Page 41: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Figure 2. Relationship among relative deprivation, cultural identity clarity and collective

esteem for Francophone Quebecers during the Conquest era (The struggle).

Note. ** p < .001, * p < .05. a,b,c,d = standardized coefficients.

Figure 3. Relationship among relative deprivation, cultural identity clarity and collective

esteem for Anglophone Quebecers during the Quiet Revolution era.

41

Relative Deprivation

Collective Esteemd = .47**

Collective Esteem

Cultural Identity Clarity

Relative Deprivation

a = .48** c = .28*

b = .33*

Relative Deprivation

Collective Esteemd = .27*

Collective Esteem

Cultural Identity Clarity

Relative Deprivation

a = .35* c = .24†

b = .19 n.s.

Page 42: Study 2 docu…  · Web viewIndeed, the contribution of a group’s history to its identity has recently been emphasized (see Hammack, 2008). In future research, one could examine

Cultural Narratives

Note. * p < .05, † p < .10 . a,b,c,d = standardized coefficients.

42