ssrn-id1014424

Upload: andre-taudiry

Post on 02-Jun-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    1/42Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1014424

    Does Functional Experience Impair the Balance in Balanced Scorecards?

    M. Elizabeth HaywoodAssistant Professor

    College of Business AdministrationRider University2083 Lawrenceville RoadLawrenceville, NJ 08648-3099(609) [email protected]

    Nathan V. StuartAssistant ProfessorSchool of AccountancyCollege of Business Administration

    University of South Florida4202 East Fowler Avenue, BSN 3403Tampa, FL 33620-5500(813) [email protected]

    AbstractIn this paper, we examine the effect that individual characteristics, specifically functionalexperience and level of work experience, have on how managers acquire and use balancedscorecard information. Using Hambrick and Masons information processing theory, we predict

    that a managers functional experience will be associated with his or her information-acquisitionbehavior and final choice in an investment decision. We test our hypotheses using a laboratoryexperiment in which participants make a capital investment decision from a menu of availableopportunities. We do not detect an association between individual characteristics andinformation-acquisition behavior but we do detect an association between individualcharacteristics and investment choice. We discuss how our findings help resolve thediscrepancies in previous studies and suggest implications for companies that use the balancedscorecard.

    This version: August 9, 2007

    We are grateful to Maureen Butler, Uday Murthy, and the workshop participants at theUniversity of South Florida and the 2006 Mid-Atlantic Regional Meeting of the AmericanAccounting Association for helpful comments and suggestions.

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    2/42Electronic copy available at: http://ssrn.com/abstract=1014424

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    3/42

    Does Functional Experience Impair the Balance in Balanced Scorecards?

    INTRODUCTION

    The balanced scorecard is a managerial accounting tool designed to align managerial

    decisions with corporate strategy by focusing attention and effort on a balanced set of

    performance measures that are both financial and nonfinancial. Balanced scorecard information,

    if properly constructed, can enable managers to make decisions that fit best with firm strategy

    and it can encourage managers to exert effort to achieve strategic objectives. Most studies to date

    on the balanced scorecard address the latter property: how does the balanced scorecard affect

    performance evaluation judgments. In this paper, we examine the balanced scorecards effect on

    managerial information acquisition and investment choice behavior.

    Several experimental studies provide mixed evidence as to whether users (managers) fully

    process the available information when making decisions. Lipe and Salterios (2000) seminal

    study asks participants to provide performance evaluations for the managers of two divisions of

    the same clothing firm. They report results consistent with participants focusing on performance

    measures common to the two divisions and ignoring the measures that reflect each divisions

    unique strategy when making their evaluations. These results suggest that managers may not use

    balanced scorecard information in a truly balanced fashion. Several studies (e.g., Banker et al.

    2004; Roberts et al. 2004) modify the Lipe and Salterio (2000) experimental design in an attempt

    to mitigate the bias against unique measures, with varying degrees of success.

    In this paper, we examine the balanced scorecard in a strategic decision-making context. In

    particular, we investigate whether decision makers biases limit their information acquisition

    and/or processing in a balanced scorecard setting. Using the information acquisition and

    processing theory of Hambrick and Mason (1984), we predict that a managers functional

    - 1 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    4/42

    experience (i.e., background in a business area such as finance or marketing) affects his or her

    information-acquisition behavior and as well as his or her processing of the information into

    actual decisions. There is evidence that individuals emphasize information related to their

    functional experience (Dearborn and Simon 1958; Ettlie 1990; Hitt and Tyler 1991) as well as

    evidence that individuals emphasize information related to otherfunctional areas (Walsh 1988;

    Beyer et al. 1997). This study adds to our knowledge of this phenomenon.

    We also examine whether length of work experience, another individual characteristic,

    interacts with functional experience to affect information acquisition and processing in strategic

    decision-making. Hall (1987) suggests that as individuals continue to work in a certain field, they

    become more socialized to that particular area. Accounting researchers have examined job tenure

    in auditing contexts and have found that the judgments and choices of experienced personnel

    differ significantly from those of novices (Choo and Trotman 1991; Bedard and Mock 1992;

    Davis 1996; Wright 2001; Earley 2002; Lehmann and Norman 2006). Through work experience,

    individuals can either develop information acquisition and processing skills that enable unbiased

    decision-making or create inflexible and narrow cognitive routines that preclude unbiased

    decision-making. Our study also increases our understanding of this dimension of individual

    development.

    We conduct an experiment to study decision makers with varied functional background and

    length of work experience. MBA students read a case study of a firm that uses the balanced

    scorecard, use the computer to acquire information organized in balanced scorecard categories

    about options for a capital investment, and make a strategic investment decision based on that

    information. We analyze participants information-acquisition behavior and find no differences

    associated with functional background or length of work experience. We analyze participants

    - 2 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    5/42

    investment decisions and find significant differences in investment choice associated with

    functional background and length of work experience. Participants with more experience

    systematically emphasize specific functional characteristics in their investment choices.

    Interestingly, experienced participants do notnecessarily emphasize information related directly

    to their functional background.

    Our study makes several contributions to the managerial accounting literature on information

    acquisition and decision-making. First, our findings suggest that companies should be aware that

    the decision processes of more experienced employees may override the balanced nature of the

    balanced scorecard. These employees, although they seem to acquire information without bias,

    process it in such a way as to bias their actual decisions. Our finding that it is the cognitive

    interpretation process that creates the bias, and not the information acquisition process, is unique

    to the literature. Second, we find that the nature of this bias differs according to the functional

    background of the decision maker. In particular, individuals with financial experience emphasize

    the financial characteristics of the decision alternatives while individuals whose functional

    background aligns with the customer dimension of the balanced scorecard de-emphasize the

    customer-related characteristics of the decision alternatives. This result suggests that companies

    may not be able to address the bias we identify with a single approach.

    We organize the rest of this paper as follows. In the second section, we develop our theory

    and hypotheses regarding functional background, work experience, and information acquisition

    and processing. The third section describes our research design and we report our results in the

    fourth section. We discuss the results and address the limitations of our study in the fifth section,

    and we identify future research opportunities and conclude in the final section.

    - 3 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    6/42

    MOTIVATION AND HYPOTHESES

    The Balanced Scorecard

    The balanced scorecard is a management accounting tool that incorporates performance

    measures from different strategic functional areas. A recent Bain & Co. survey of global business

    (Rigby 2005) suggests that 57% of firms are using the balanced scorecard in one form or

    another. Kaplan and Norton (1992, 1996, 2001) initially emphasized, and continue to promote,

    the balanced scorecard as a decision support tool, claiming that the balanced scorecard would

    enable companies to incorporate nonfinancial and forward-looking information into strategic

    planning to make up for deficiencies in traditional financial data. Kaplan and Norton (1996,

    2001) have also acknowledged that companies evaluate the effectiveness of current employee

    effort by using multiple performance measures.

    Kaplan and Norton emphasize the importance of including multiple measures with different

    properties in the strategic planning process. An employee who fails to balance each of the

    balanced scorecard perspectives could be making poor strategic choices for his or her

    organization. Each [balanced scorecard perspective] represents only one component in a

    network of management activities and processes that must be performed to generate superior,

    sustainable performance (Kaplan and Norton 2001, 26). For instance, Kaplan and Norton

    (2001) note that companies who stressed quality in the 1980s and 1990s often won national

    quality awards but ended up in financial peril.

    We make the embedded assumption that firms include specific measures in the balanced

    scorecard because they believe decisions made with the total scorecard information set will

    provide additional benefits to the firm that are greater than the costs of providing and processing

    the measures. Kogut and Zander (1992) assert, however, that individuals who identify with

    - 4 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    7/42

    specific functional experiences can thwart coordination and cooperation with employees serving

    in other business areas within a firm. In this study, therefore, we explore the following research

    question: will the functional background of managers within a balanced scorecard system affect

    their information-acquisition behavior and actual investment decisions?

    This research is important for several reasons. First, many firms employ the balanced

    scorecard (Calabro 2001; Rigby 2005) so there is significant practitioner interest in

    understanding how to obtain the maximum benefit from this management tool. Second, prior

    research on the effect of functional experience focuses on information acquisition. Dearborn and

    Simon (1958) ask participants to underline information that identifies the mostimportant

    strategic problem facing a company, while Walsh (1988) asks participants to underline relevant

    information that identifies allimportant strategic problems. Beyer et al. (1997) use both

    approaches to try to reconcile the results of the two earlier studies. None of these papers,

    however, investigates whether the use (or misuse) of that information leads to a biased choice.

    Walsh (1988, 887) writes, the managers studied were not asked to make actual decisions.

    Boritz (1992) suggests that it is important to connect process and outcome findings to determine

    if they lead to confirming or contradictory conclusions. By examining both information-

    acquisition behavior and the decision outcome, we can examine whether the effect is due to

    limited information search or to an information processing bias.

    Third, balanced scorecard studies find that numerous factors (common versus unique

    measures in Lipe and Salterio [2000]; organization of information in Lipe and Salterio [2002];

    strategic linkages in Banker et al. [2004]) affect the use of balanced scorecard information in

    performance evaluation judgments. There is little evidence to date, however, regarding how

    managers use balanced scorecard information in strategic decision-making. The information and

    - 5 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    8/42

    scorecard characteristics that create or mitigate bias in performance evaluation judgments will

    not necessarily have the same effects on strategic decision-making.

    Finally, prior research designs have limited the generalizability of the results. Studies have

    examined the effect of only a single characteristic on individuals behavior, while theory

    suggests (as we discuss below) that multiple characteristics may be involved. Other studies have

    provided information sets that emphasize particular types of information (Dearborn and Simon

    1958; Walsh 1988) while others have used participants with homogeneous functional

    backgrounds (Rosman et al. 1994). Our participants have diverse functional backgrounds and we

    consider the interactive effects of functional background and length of work experience. In the

    next section, we discuss information acquisition and processing and develop our hypotheses.

    Decision Makers Information Acquisition and Processing

    A well-designed balanced scorecard includes measures that capture the organizations

    strategic goals through functional areas such as finance, marketing, operations, information

    technology, and human resource management. The management literature suggests that

    functional experience can influence managers strategic decision-making. Hambrick and Mason

    (1984) propose a theory of information acquisition and cognitive processing which states that the

    demographic characteristics and experiences of individuals influence the way they acquire and

    process information.1These characteristics include the managers age, functional experience,

    length of service, education, socioeconomic roots, and financial position. Hambrick (2005)

    updates the theory and indicates several unexplored implications.

    Figure 1 depicts an adapted version of the Hambrick (2005) framework. First, managers must

    acquire information related to a particular decision from the information environment (from

    Stage 1 to Stage 2 in Figure 1). Bounded rationality (Simon 1978) limits the ability of the

    - 6 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    9/42

    manager to access and process all available information. Hambrick and Mason (1984) and

    Hambrick (2005) suggest that the personal characteristics of the manager affect the filtering

    process managers use to acquire specific information. Specifically, these characteristics can limit

    the managers field of vision, causing him or her to acquire information only from particular

    sources or with particular properties.

    [Insert Figure 1 here]

    Once the manager has completed the information acquisition step, he or she must process the

    information through cognitive decision algorithms (from Stage 2 to Stage 3 in Figure 1). Once

    again, Hambrick and Mason (1984) and Hambrick (2005) argue that his or her personal

    characteristics play a role in the managers processing of the information. These characteristics

    can affect the ways in which managers perceive and interpret the information as supportive of

    their preferred decisions. Miller and Toulouse (1986) and Gupta and Govindarajan (1984)

    provide evidence that managers personal characteristics alter the establishment of strategy and

    its implementation.

    One characteristic in particular has been of interest to researchers: the managers functional

    experience, such as finance and accounting, human resources and general management,

    marketing and sales, production and operations, and information systems. Kogut and Zander

    (1992) assert, moreover, that individuals who identify with specific functional experiences can

    impede coordination and cooperation with employees serving in other business areas within a

    firm. Studies on functional experience and strategic decision-making, however, have produced

    inconsistent and inconclusive results.

    Certain studies demonstrate a positive correlation between the functional experience of

    participants and their information acquisition (i.e., individuals acquire more information related

    - 7 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    10/42

    to their functional area). Dearborn and Simon (1958) show that executives frame problems in

    terms of their functional areas. Ettlie (1990) documents that CEOs with manufacturing

    backgrounds engage in more aggressive manufacturing technology policies. Hitt and Tyler

    (1991) find that the decision-making of the upper-level managers in their survey sample is

    associated with their work experience (among other demographic variables). Rosman et al.

    (1994) show that functional experience makes a difference in loan decisions but that such

    differences depend on the type of information (financial or strategic) being used.

    Other research finds no relation or even a negative correlation between functional experience

    and information acquisition (i.e., individuals acquire information in areas other than those related

    to their functional experience). Walsh (1988) finds only marginal support that such experience

    makes a difference in information acquisition and problem formation. He concludes that a

    managers belief structure (the lens through which the manager perceives information) is much

    broader than his or her discipline. Beyer et al. (1997, 728) document that functional experience

    triggers the individual to over-emphasize information in areas otherthan his or her background

    area. They state that [o]nly in finance and accounting were [functional] experience and

    problems identified related, and then only at a marginal level of significance. Furthermore, the

    relationship is not positive, as theory predicts, but negative.

    While our focus is on functional experience, the discrepancies in other studies might be due

    to the influence of other (omitted) managerial characteristics in combination with functional

    experience. Reed and Reed (1989), for example, find no relation between a CEOs background

    (finance, accounting, etc.) and the strategy he selects but do find an interaction between a CEOs

    experience and the chosen method of diversification (internally or through acquisition). Gunz

    - 8 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    11/42

    and Jalland (1996, 735) review numerous studies on managerial careers and business strategies

    and conclude the following:

    Thus, although it would seem intuitively obvious that there must be personal

    characteristics of managers that are affected by career backgrounds in such a wayas to have an impact on strategic choice and the effectiveness of strategyimplementation and that there is some evidence for this impact, this is clearly adifficult area to study, which perhaps explains why few clear findings haveemerged. The personal characteristics that mediate have yet to be identified.

    We therefore also examine whether length of work experience interacts with functional

    experience to affect information acquisition and decision-making. Fredrickson (1985) reveals

    that inexperienced managers naivet creates disparities between their information processes and

    those of their more experienced counterparts. Chiesi et al. (1979) find experienced participants

    acquire more relevant cues and Brucks (1985) finds experienced managers place greater weight

    on functional information. Lurigio and Carroll (1985) suggest that experienced participants

    employ knowledge in a more sophisticated fashion than those with less experience. Choo and

    Trotman (1991) find experienced auditors recall more and different types of information than

    inexperienced auditors.

    Hypotheses

    Our hypotheses address participants information-acquisition behavior (as is done in previous

    research) and their actual decisions, a dependent variable that other functional background

    studies do not examine. Based on Hambrick and Masons (1984) theory, we predict a significant

    association between functional background and both dependent variables in our balanced

    scorecard setting. Given the conflicting extant evidence, however, and in the absence of a

    compelling theory that favors one direction or the other in our setting, we do not make a

    directional prediction. Our hypothesis for functional experience, Hypothesis 1, is therefore:

    - 9 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    12/42

    H1: Balanced scorecard users who have functional experience in one area will (A)

    exhibit different information-acquisition behavior and (B) make different

    investment decisions than balanced scorecard users who have functional

    experience in other areas.

    Hambrick and Mason (1984, 200) also suggest that Other Career Experiences, such as

    length of work experience, affect strategic decision-making. They state that career experiences

    partially shape the lenses through which they [managers] view current strategic opportunities and

    problems. We propose that length of work experience will interact with functional background

    to influence information acquisition and strategic decision-making. Hall (1987) finds that, as

    individuals continue to work in a certain field, they become more socialized to that particular

    discipline, suggesting that more-experienced managers will have developed biases for certain

    information types. A viable alternative, however, is the more-experienced managers have

    overcome their preferences for information related to their functional background through

    practice and feedback. A balanced scorecard, which makes the functional nature of each

    information item salient, could either exacerbate or mitigate the managers tendency towards

    particular types of information, as well. We therefore do not believe that theory and existing

    evidence justify a directional prediction regarding length of work experience. Our hypothesis

    relating to the interaction of functional experience and the length of work experience, Hypothesis

    2, is therefore:

    H2: Balanced scorecard users who have more work experience and who have

    functional experience in one area will (A) exhibit different information-

    acquisition behavior and (B) make different investment decisions than balanced

    scorecard users who have less work experience and/or functional experience in

    other areas.

    - 10 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    13/42

    METHOD

    Participants

    We conduct an experiment in which participants choose which of eight new store locations to

    open. Our participants are 78 first- and second-year MBA students.2Table 1, Panel A, contains

    information about our participants and the selection process we use to reduce our sample to the

    55 participants with functional experience in only one balanced scorecard area. First, we

    eliminate 12 participants who do not have functional experience that aligns with a balanced

    scorecard perspective. We then eliminate 11 participants whose functional background aligns

    with more than one balanced scorecard perspective. We conduct our analyses on the remaining

    55 participants with pure functional experience. Our participants possess pure functional

    experience, unlike those of Walsh (1988) and Beyer et al. (1997). We believe that using only

    participants with pure backgrounds will enhance our ability to detect background-driven biases

    in information acquisition and investment choice behavior.

    Panel B of Table 1 contains demographic information about our participants. The average

    age of our participants is 28 and the mean work experience is 4.5 years. Twenty-one of the 55

    participants (38%) are female. Our participants have an average of 0.73 years of experience with

    the balanced scorecard but slightly more experience (1.27 years) working in a system that uses

    nonfinancial performance measures. Half of the participants have experience with the balanced

    scorecard through the classroom, their place of employment, or both.

    [Insert Table 1 here]

    Experimental Procedure

    Participants analyze an investment decision case in a computer laboratory equipped with

    MouseLab, a software program that keeps track of the order in which the user accesses specific

    - 11 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    14/42

    information items. MouseLab is a computer-based process tracing software program that

    presents information in an alternative by attribute matrix. Each piece of information is in a

    separate box and hidden by an opaque screen. Participants reveal the information in a box by

    moving the mouse-controlled cursor to the desired box (clicking is not necessary). MouseLab

    records which boxes the participants open, the order in which participants open boxes, and the

    length of time participants spend in each box. A box closes again once the participant moves the

    cursor to another part of the screen. Further explanation of the MouseLab software can be found

    in Payne et al. (1993).

    Researchers studying consumer behavior and marketing have been the primary users of

    MouseLab, using it to study brand choice in numerous product categories (Dhar and Nowlis

    1999, 2004; Dhar et al. 1999) and price/quality trade-offs in emotion-laden consumer goods

    decisions (Luce et al. 1999). Researchers in human resources have used MouseLab to study

    hiring decisions using an alternative by attribute matrix consisting of candidates and five

    candidate characteristics (Iglesias-Parro et al. 2002). Accounting researchers have employed

    similar computer-based process tracing programs (such as Search Monitor (Brucks 1988) in

    going concern decisions (Rosman et al. 1994; Seol 2006) and analytical review judgments

    (Bhattacharjee et al. 1999). Swain and Haka (2000) used ISLab (Cook 1993) to determine

    information load effects in capital budgeting tasks.

    Participants first complete a practice task to familiarize themselves with the MouseLab

    software and the task instructions.3Participants then receive the experimental case, which

    includes background information for a division of a womens clothing chain that specializes in

    teen apparel, the performance measures used in the divisions balanced scorecard (including

    general background information on the balanced scorecard), and target scores for each balanced

    - 12 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    15/42

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    16/42

    Each capital investment choice has a rating for each balanced scorecard perspective: low

    (value = 6), moderate (values = 7 or 8) or high (value = 9). The attribute values sum to 30 for

    each alternative, so there is no dominant choice (dominant alternatives reduce careful and

    thorough searches (Swain and Haka 2000)). The value disclosed in Figure 2, for example, is a

    moderate score of 8 for the financial perspective for the Store C investment option.6We

    randomize the order of the rows and columns in the participants information boards to prevent

    order effects.While there is not an explicit cost to the participants for acquiring an information

    item (i.e., clicking on a cell in the alternative-attribute matrix), there is an implicit cost in the

    time required to decide which item to acquire, click on the item, read it, and process the item.

    When they are ready, participants make their investment selection by clicking on the desired

    investment option (see the bottom of Figure 2).

    Dependent Variables

    We measure information acquisition in two ways. We calculate our first measure,IAFA

    (Information Acquisition by Functional Area), as the number of cues acquired for each balanced

    scorecard perspective (ignoring revisits) divided by the 32 cells available.IAFAcan range from

    zero (when a participant acquires no information in a particular balanced scorecard perspective)

    to 25% (eight cells in a balanced scorecard functional area divided by 32 total cells). We

    calculate our second measure,IAFA-R(Information Acquisition by Functional Area with

    Revisits), similarly, but account for revisits to specific cues (Russo [1978] proposes that

    reacquisitions indicate the decision makers assessment of the relative importance of cues).

    IAFA-Rrepresents the number of times a participant accesses a particular cue (even if acquired

    previously) divided by the total number of times the participant accesses all cues.IAFA-Rcan

    - 14 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    17/42

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    18/42

    backgrounds in human resources, general management, and information technology into the

    learning and growth perspective.

    [Insert Table 2 here]

    In our discussion below and in our analyses, we further aggregate participants with functional

    backgrounds in the internal business processes and learning and growth perspectives. There are

    only five participants with functional backgrounds in the internal business process category, and

    we cannot conduct empirical tests on such a small number of observations. This choice increases

    the power of our tests but reduces out ability to draw specific inferences about the influence of

    these two functional backgrounds on information acquisition and choice behavior. Arguably,

    there is significant overlap among the functional backgrounds associated with these balanced

    scorecard perspectives. For instance, information technology (categorized under learning and

    growth in Table 2) can also be viewed as part of an efficient operational system (an internal

    business processes area). Because of this overlap, we believe our results are still informative

    about the effect of functional background on our dependent variables.

    Our participants provide the number of years of work experience they possess on the

    demographic questionnaire. To test the effects of length of work experience, we follow Lurigio

    and Carroll (1985) and partition our sample by the median years of work experience (four). We

    consider our participants experience to be low when they have worked less than four years

    and high when they have worked four or more years.

    RESULTS

    We report the results of several debriefing questions in Table 3. Participants responded to

    each question by marking a Likert scale that ranged from 5 (Strongly Disagree) to 5 (Strongly

    Agree), with the mid-point labeled 0 (Indifference). The t-tests indicate whether the mean

    - 16 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    19/42

    response to each question is significantly different from zero (indifference). Participants found

    the case easy to understand (mean = 2.10,p-value < 0.0001), realistic (mean = 1.74,p-value FH1A:

    Functional Experience IAFA 0.8923 0.72 0.6749IAFA-R 0.8376 1.54 0.1716

    H2A:

    Functional Experience

    and Length of Work

    Experience IAFA 0.6777 0.96 0.5190IAFA-R 0.6874 1.26 0.2357

    - 34 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    37/42

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    38/42

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    39/42

    Figure 2

    Choice Display for the Capital Investment Decision

    Customer Internal Bus Learn/Grow Financial

    Store B

    Store G

    Store D

    Store C 8

    Store E

    Store H

    Store A

    Store F

    Which store would you choose?

    Choose one Store B Store G Store D Store C Store E Store H Store A Store F

    - 37 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    40/42

    Figure 3

    Store Choice by Functional Experience and Length of Work Experience

    Panel A: Financial Functional Experience

    Store Choice for Subjects With Financial Functional Experience

    0

    10

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    70

    80

    90

    100

    Low Financial Score Moderate or High Financial Score

    Store Choice

    PercentofSubjects

    WE 4

    WE < 4

    Panel B: Customer Functional Experience

    Store Choice for Subjects With Customer Functional Experience

    0

    0.1

    0.2

    0.3

    0.4

    0.5

    0.6

    0.7

    0.8

    0.9

    Low Customer Score Moderate or High Customer

    Score

    Store Choice

    PercentageofSubjects

    WE 4

    WE < 4

    - 38 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    41/42

    Figure 3 (Continued)

    Panel C: Internal Business Process/Learning and Growth Functional Experience

    Store Choice for Subjects With IBP/LG Functional Experience

    0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    1

    1.2

    Low Score for Either IBP or LG Neither IBP nor LG has LowScore

    Store Choice

    PercentageofSubjects

    WE 4

    WE < 4

    - 39 -

  • 8/10/2019 SSRN-id1014424

    42/42

    Endnotes

    1Hambrick and Mason (1984) called their theory Upper Echelons Theory because of their interest in top-

    management teams. As Hambrick (2005) points out, however, the theory is really about information

    acquisition and processing and thus applies to individuals at all organizational levels.2Dearborn and Simon (1958) use executives, Walsh (1988) uses mid-career managers, and Beyer et al.

    (1997) use MBA students. We believe MBA students are appropriate for the following reasons. First,

    Walsh (1988, 890) notes that his results may not apply to simple-minded processors or managers with

    more modest accomplishments. We try to determine if this is the case by using MBA students who have

    limited work experience. Second, Lipe and Salterio (2000) note that they used MBA students to minimize

    the effect of participants relying on their own companys (rather than the experimental case firms)

    strategy and related balanced scorecard measures when completing the case. Third, Ashton and Kramer

    (1980) and Mowen and Mowen (1986) do not find differences between information processing of students

    and non-students. All of our participants are full-time students. We find some demographic differences

    between our first- and second-year participants but no effects of program year on our dependent variables.

    3The practice task involves 16 information items and 4 decision alternatives. The experimental task

    involves 32 information items and eight decision alternatives.

    4We are grateful to Marlys Lipe and Steve Salterio for granting us permission to adapt their case materials

    from Lipe and Salterio (2000). Our case materials are available upon request.

    5We determined the appropriate amount of time during pilot testing.

    6To make the cognitive burden on our participants manageable, the investment attribute matrix provides an

    aggregate effect for each investment on each balanced scorecard perspective. This type of aggregation

    occurs in balanced scorecard firms: CorVu, for example, produces and sells balanced scorecard software

    that aggregates the separate performance measures for each perspective into overall performance scores.

    7

    Since MouseLab tracks not only what boxes participants open but how much time participants spend with

    each box open, we conducted similar tests using the percentage of total time spent acquiring information

    that each participant spent in each balanced scorecard category as the dependent variable. There were no

    significant effects of functional experience and its interaction with length of work experience on time spent