road chat, talking to passengers can be as dangerous as using a cell phone

Upload: farid-jafarov

Post on 08-Apr-2018

221 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/7/2019 Road Chat, Talking to passengers can be as dangerous as using a cell phone

    1/2

    Feb. 13, 2006

    Road chat: Talking to passengers can be as dangerous as using a cell phone

    ANN ARBOR, Mich. Talking to passengers may be just as dangerous for drivers as talking on a cell

    phone, say University of Michigan researchers.

    A new study by the U-M Transportation Research Institute shows that drivers who have conversationswith passengers exhibit similar levels of driving performance as drivers who use cell phones. Forexample, the study found no statistical difference in terms of keeping in the correct lane or using propersteering behavior between people talking on a cell phone or those conversing with a passenger.

    "This may suggest that talking on a cell phone, from a driving performance perspective, is no worse thanholding a conversation with a passenger or taking part in a number of other potentially distractingbehaviors," said James Sayer, an assistant research scientist at UMTRI.

    Relative to driving performance when not engaged in anything but the task of driving, all forms of "non-driving" behavior resulted in at least some degraded performance, depending on the measure beingconsidered.

    "The use of cellular telephones while driving receives a lot of attention in the popular press, probablybecause their popularity and widespread use is relatively new," Sayer said. "But the results of our studyshow that many of the other behaviors that drivers engage in, such as eating, drinking, grooming andhaving conversations with passengers, are potentially just as detrimental to driving performance."

    Sayer and colleagues Joel Devonshire and Carol Flannagan studied the effects of secondary behaviorson normal, everyday driving performance. They analyzed hundreds of short video clips of 36 motoristswho drove cars equipped with cameras that recorded vehicle movement and driver behavior for nearlyfour weeks each.

    More than a third of the video clips showed drivers engaging in secondary tasks. Conversation withpassengers was the most common (15 percent of the clips), followed by grooming (6.5 percent), cellphone use (5 percent) and eating and drinking (2 percent).

    While all of the non-driving behaviors were associated with more erratic steering behavior, theresearchers found that other measures of driving performance, such as lane position, speed fluctuation,use of the accelerator pedal and glance behavior (checking mirrors, looking out side windows, etc.),showed mixed results.

    For example, cell phone use did not affect speed variation, although frequency and duration of glancesaway from the road in front of a driver was lowest when using a cell phone which could negativelyaffect scanning the roadway environment, they say. Eating and drinking while driving had little effect ondriving performance, except for modest increases in steering variance and glance frequency, as well asmore frequent braking. They found a similar pattern for grooming behaviors.

    The study also showed that women and younger drivers (30 and under) were more likely to engage insecondary tasks. Female drivers were much more likely to converse with passengers, but men had higherrates of cell phone use and grooming behaviors.

    Page 1 of 2University of Michigan

    29/03/2011http://ns.umich.edu/htdocs/releases/plainstory.php?id=116

  • 8/7/2019 Road Chat, Talking to passengers can be as dangerous as using a cell phone

    2/2

    In addition, the researchers examined contextual factors, such as road type, road curvature and roadcondition. They found that drivers performed differently when taking part in different tasks andappeared to selectively engage in secondary behaviors, depending on traffic and roadway conditions.

    Previous research using driving simulators and controlled on-road or test-track driving, rather thannatural conditions suggests that driving performance suffers when drivers engage in secondarybehaviors.

    "Until now, no published data has examined the effects of secondary tasks on driving performancemeasures under naturalistic conditions drivers using instrumented vehicles in their daily lives and fewhave examined the relative frequency with which these secondary behaviors even occur," Sayer said."Nonetheless, there is a growing public concern with driver distraction, including state and local lawsthat impose penalties for engaging in distracting behaviors while driving.

    "But our study showed relatively little effect of secondary behavior on basic driving performancemeasures. In fact, drivers may be performing secondary tasks when their driving skills are least neededand when the traffic environment tends toward being less challenging based upon a driver's ownassessment."

    Related Links:

    More information on UMTRI

    Contact: Bernie DeGroatPhone: (734) 647-1847

    Page 2 of 2University of Michigan

    29/03/2011http://ns.umich.edu/htdocs/releases/plainstory.php?id=116