responses to organizational surprises in startups: the...

52
Responses to Organizational Surprises in Startups: The Impact of Improvisation and Memory on Response Outcomes Yan Gong The Paul Merage School of Business University of California, Irvine Irvine, CA 62697 Tel: (949) 824-8544 e-mail: [email protected] Ted Baker College of Management North Carolina State University Raleigh, NC 27695 Tel: (919) 513-7943 e-mail: [email protected] Dale Eesley Management Department University of Toledo Toledo, OH 43606 Tel: (419) 530-2643 e-mail: [email protected] Anne S. Miner School of Business University of Wisconsin – Madison 975 University Avenue Madison, WI 53706 Tel: (608) 263-4143 e-mail: [email protected] February 10, 2008 The authors thank Mary Crossan, Tim Pollock and Dusya Vera for their helpful contributions to this work. Earlier versions of this paper were presented at Harvard University, New York University, Babson Entrepreneurship Conference, Academy of Management Conference, INFORMS/Organization Science Conference, and West Coast Technology Entrepreneurship Symposium. We also thank the National Science Foundation Division of Decision, Risk and Management Science Division (Grant #: 0132827), and interview participants for their generous assistance with this project.

Upload: trannhi

Post on 19-Jun-2018

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Responses to Organizational Surprises in Startups:

The Impact of Improvisation and Memory on Response Outcomes

Yan Gong The Paul Merage School of Business

University of California, Irvine Irvine, CA 62697

Tel: (949) 824-8544 e-mail: [email protected]

Ted Baker

College of Management North Carolina State University

Raleigh, NC 27695 Tel: (919) 513-7943

e-mail: [email protected]

Dale Eesley Management Department

University of Toledo Toledo, OH 43606

Tel: (419) 530-2643 e-mail: [email protected]

Anne S. Miner

School of Business University of Wisconsin – Madison

975 University Avenue Madison, WI 53706 Tel: (608) 263-4143

e-mail: [email protected]

February 10, 2008

The authors thank Mary Crossan, Tim Pollock and Dusya Vera for their helpful contributions to this work. Earlier versions of this paper were presented at Harvard University, New York University, Babson Entrepreneurship Conference, Academy of Management Conference, INFORMS/Organization Science Conference, and West Coast Technology Entrepreneurship Symposium. We also thank the National Science Foundation Division of Decision, Risk and Management Science Division (Grant #: 0132827), and interview participants for their generous assistance with this project.

1

Responses to Organizational Surprises in Startups:

The Impact of Improvisation and Memory on Response Outcomes

ABSTRACT

This paper offers a conditional framework of improvisation, in a context of

organizational responses to surprise events. We propose that two key factors related to organizational knowledge use will shape whether a specific response to a surprise will have value to the organization. First, we argue that intermediate levels of improvisation during responses create conflicts on knowledge deployment that reduce the chances of an effective response. In contrast, both lower and higher levels of improvisation will have a relatively more positive impact on organizational outcomes. Second, the organization’s direct and indirect memory represents a reservoir of potential activities and interpretive schemes, which in turn enhances the chances that its surprise response will have a valued outcome. Finally, we propose that the value of improvisational response is enhanced by the presence of high levels of organizational memory. We test these ideas using a sample of 141 surprise events identified from 1,725 pages of interview transcripts, over 1,000 pages of informant self-rating reports, and rater assessments of these materials. Our study contributes to theories of improvisation, organizational learning, and emerging theories of organizational surprise.

2

Several research streams highlight the importance of organizational surprises. One

recurring perspective emphasizes the potentially negative effects of surprise events and argues

that organizations should attempt to avoid surprises through careful pre-planning and

sophisticated early detection systems (Watkins & Bazerman, 2003; Weick, 1998). A second

perspective argues that surprises are unavoidable but focuses on potential longer-term benefits

that occur when the experience of surprise generates organizational learning by spurring mindful

attention to errors in shared mental models and operating assumptions (Louis, 1980; Meyer,

Gaba, & Colwell, 2005; Okhuysen & Eisenhardt, 2002; Zellmer-Bruhn, 2003). In this paper, we

tackle a third set of issues concerning organizational surprises. Like the learning literature, we

too assume that surprises are unavoidable, but we focus on organizations’ immediate responses

to surprise events and how the organization’s improvisational pattern and its memory level shape

the outcome of such responses.

We define surprise as an event that is recognized as unexpected by a focal organization

(Louis, 1980). This definition is consistent with prior work that emphasizes the material aspects

of surprises, and is distinct from work that emphasizes cognitive or emotional states (Harrison &

March, 1984; Schutzwohl, 1998). We propose that two key factors related to organizational

knowledge use will shape whether a specific response to a surprise will have value to the

organization. First, we argue that intermediate levels of improvisation during responses create

conflicts on knowledge deployment that reduce the chances of an effective response. In contrast,

both lower and higher levels of improvisation will have a relatively more positive impact on

organizational outcomes. Second, the organization’s direct and indirect memory represents a

reservoir of potential activities and interpretive schemes, which in turn enhances the chances that

its surprise response will have a valued outcome. Finally, we propose that the value of

improvisational response is enhanced by the presence of high levels of organizational memory.

3

Our work contributes to theories of improvisation and organizational learning. The

increased scholarly attention to improvisation so far has revealed its potential complex

manifestations and impact on organizations, but also raised cautionary flags about how little

systematic evidence about its origins, dynamics or impacts in an organizational setting (Hatch,

1997; Miner, Bassoff, & Moorman, 2001; Levinthal & Rerup, 2006). Our study intends to extend

this important line of research by developing testing a conditional theory of improvisation. That

is, we tackle the question of when an improvisational generates more value in response to a

surprise event, in the presence of varying levels of organizational memory. Our study also

advances theory about organizational surprises.

We examine the proposed framework in a sample of one hundred forty one surprises in

thirty-one firms drawing on interview data, informant self-reporting data instruments, research

team action identification, and variable coding based on 1,725 pages of transcripts. Consistent

with our theories of the value of very low and very high levels of improvisation, we find a

curvilinear pattern in which intermediate levels of improvisation harms outcomes. The total

pattern of findings also supports a conditional model of improvisation in which higher level of

organizational memory helps drive value out of improvisation activities in response to surprise

events.

THEORY AND HYPOTHESES

Research Setting – Surprise Events Experienced by New Firms

In this paper, we focus on responses to surprises, defined as events that are recognized as

unexpected (Louis, 1980; Baker, Miner, & Eesley, 2003). Prior work has examined the impact of

surprise events on individuals, groups, industries, armies and even non-human subjects such as

information systems (Cunha, Kamoche, & Clegg, 2005; Cohen & Axelrod, 1984; Schutzwohl,

1998). In this study, we focus on the organizational level and examine how differences in

4

organizational memory and how it is deployed in response to surprise events influence an

organization’s satisfaction with the effectiveness of its own response. In this context, a surprise is

an event that is recognized as unexpected by the focal organization.

Prior studies of surprise have highlighted the importance of clearly delineating the chain

of events that will be examined (Harrison & March, 1984; Mendonca, 2005; Vanhamme, 2000).

For this study, we analyze the sequence of events into four elements: (1) a surprise event, (2) the

recognition/realization of the event by the organization, (3) the organization’s response to the

surprise event, and (4) the organization’s assessment of the immediate outcome of that response.

Our focus is on the crucial sub-processes that determine how an organizations existing body of

knowledge and how the organizations deploys that knowledge in response to a surprise event

shapes its satisfaction with the near-term outcome of its response.

Unlike other important work on the role of surprise in various social systems, we do not

examine precursors to organizational surprise, such as environmental jolts (Meyer 1980; Sine &

David, 2003), interruption (Zellmer-Bruhn, 2003), threat (Staw, Sandelands, & Sutton, 1981),

and crisis (Kim, 1998; Lin et al., 2006), nor do we study organizational effectiveness in avoiding

surprise. Our study takes the existence of a surprise event as given. Despite a common scholarly

focus on negative surprises (Kim, 1998; Lin et al., 2006), our definition based on the recognition

that an event is recognized as unexpected allows for both positive and negative surprises (Greve,

1998). Table 1 presents some examples of surprises from our study. The surprise event may

range from something as negative as a threat to the very existence of the organization, to an

unanticipated resource or opportunity (Meyer, 1980; Zellmer-Bruhn, 2003).

--- Insert Table 1 about here ---

We focus on organizations’ evaluation of the effectiveness of their responses to surprise

events in part because the immediate response to a surprise can play an important role in

5

organizational adaptation. For example, research on responses to disasters (Lin et al., 2006) and

to surprise attacks (Mendonca, 2005) indicate that if whether an organization can devise an

immediate fruitful response to some surprises can determine whether and how well the

organization recovers. Similarly, the literatures on improvisation and on entrepreneurship (e.g.,

Baker et al, 2003; Miner et al, 2001) provide numerous examples of the effective immediate

exploitation of positive surprise events.

Contemporary practitioners often encourage organizations to be nimble or flexible in the

face of unexpected events and change and valorize the practice of improvisation (e.g., Conner,

1998). Such generic advice, however, leaves open major questions about micro-processes and

factors that influence whether the particular response to a given surprise event will prove

valuable to the organization even in the short run. In our hypotheses we argue that an existing

knowledge, in the form of organizational memory, enhances an organization’s ability to respond

effectively to surprise events. We also argue that deploying organizational knowledge by

engaging in moderate levels of improvisation is likely to detract from effectiveness in responding

to surprises, but that either resisting improvisation or engaging in high levels of improvisation

may contribute to responses that are effective in the short term. Finally, we argue that

organizational knowledge and how it is deployed will interact such that higher levels of memory

and high levels of improvisation will interact to create effective responses.

We test our theories by studying surprises occurring in a sample of knowledge intensive

start-up firms. We chose this context for two reasons. First, many observers (e.g., Baker &

Nelson, 2005) suggest that start-ups may be more prone to experiencing surprises than other

firms, making new and young firms a fertile source of data for our study. Second, the start-up

setting allows us to test ideas in a setting where we can clearly specify scope conditions. Prior

work on improvisation and memory have shown mixed results and the start-up context allows us

6

to test our arguments under certain scope conditions that we will argue are likely to influence

observed results.

Improvisation and the Outcome of Responses to Surprise Events

Following Miner, Bassoff and Moorman (2001), we define improvisation as “the

deliberate and substantive fusion of the design and execution of a novel production” (Miner et

al., 2001: 314). Early studies of organizational improvisation focused on episodes of successful

improvisation and thereby focused attention on the positive consequences of improvised

behavior (Hutchins, 1991; Preston, 1991; Weick, 2003). Moorman and Miner (1998) laid out a

conditional theoretical model in which the nature of the knowledge deployed shapes the degree

to which improvisation leads to positive outcomes. Later empirical work has verified that

improvised behaviors can produce both positive and negative results (Baker, et al., 2003;

Eisenhardt & Tabrizi, 1995; Moorman & Miner, 1998; Vera & Crossan, 2005). Important

questions about whether and when improvisation will produce valued outcomes remain,

however, and additional research is required to establish boundary conditions around processes

of effective organizational improvisation (Miner et al., 2001).

In many cases, a firm’s best response to a surprise will be to continue following

established plans or organizational routines, which are built on organizational knowledge that

has survived prior internal selection processes and are in many cases still applicable despite the

surprise event (March, 1991; Miner, 1991). When planned and routine behavior persist in the

face of surprise events, existing organizational knowledge continues to be deployed in the

organization’s “usual way,” with employees playing familiar roles and coordinating their

behavior in well-understood and integrated patterns. Such routine behaviors may be enhanced

by small improvisational “embellishments” (Berliner, 1994; Miner, et al., 2001; Weick, 1998)

intended to deal with surprise events, without challenging the coherence and functioning of

7

established roles and behavioral patterns (Galbraith, 1973; Miner, 1991;Weick, 1993). For

example, Miner et al. (2001) described several cases in which established firms were able to

improvise solutions to surprise events in production and marketing. Employees engaged in small

and successful improvisational embellishments while continuing to follow elaborate product

development rules and routines. For example, one firm responded to the surprise of large

variations in the performance of a standard system component by improvising a solution that

offered the better-performing systems to favored customers as a signal of their preferred status,

and thus turned a potential problem into an opportunity. Following the logic that low levels of

improvisation permit some adaptation to surprise events while keeping intact the knowledge

deployment benefits of established roles and routines, we expect that low levels of improvisation

will generate positive outcomes in response to surprises.

High levels of improvisation may also help organizations respond productively to

surprises by heightening awareness that “business as usual” is being violated, and thereby

increasing shared attention to real-time information and signaling that roles and coordination

need to be adjusted on the fly. Consistent with this argument, Waller (1999) found in an

experimental study that the ability to respond to real-time information by reprioritizing and

redistributing tasks distinguished airline crews able to perform well in response to surprise

events from crews with poor performance outcomes. In a striking illustration, Hutchins (1990)

described how the crew of a large ship with a failed navigation system improvised a coordinated

set of roles that allowed them to bring the ship safely to harbor. This highly improvisational

response to a surprise event elicited participants’ close attention to real time information flows

and mindful collective engagement with the response (Weick, et al., 1999), even though most of

the crew members had little understanding of the overall improvised division of labor they were

helping to enact. Improvisation may disrupt normal patterns of knowledge deployment and

8

require that employees fulfill unfamiliar roles while dealing with challenges to the coordination

of complex behaviors. The benefits of previously vetted plans and routines are set aside in favor

of new and untried behaviors. Nonetheless, high levels of improvisation may offset some of

these disadvantages by encouraging organizational focus on real-time information flows

(Moorman & Miner, 1998) and by throwing the ongoing composition of behavior patterns open

to mindful task reassignment and reprioritization. We therefore expect that high levels of

improvisation will generate positive outcomes in response to surprises.

While we have described the benefits of both low and high levels of improvisation, we

argue that firms which engage in moderate levels of improvisation in response to surprise events

may suffer the disadvantages of putting aside proven patterns of knowledge deployment without

the advantages of increases in collective attention to real time information and role adjustment.

As a recent paper (Baker, 2007: 708-709) argued, the “ensemble models of improvisation that

many analyses use as metaphors for organizational improvisation” assume that everyone is

improvising, but an alternative model is “one in which some actors involved in a novel

production (Miner et al., 2001) are improvising, while others are not.” We expect that when

organizations respond to surprise events with moderate levels of improvisation, events may

unfold in ad hoc admixtures of improvised and non-improvised actions, resulting in confusion

about who is or should be doing what and when they should be doing it while crafting the

organization’s response. In this situation, improvised actions create emergent roles and

interaction patterns that may be poorly coordinated with the organization’s existing roles and

interaction routines (Miner, 1990; Weber, 1978). Even if the improvisation generates useful

novelty, the benefits of such innovation may be overshadowed by escalating coordination

challenges (Weick, 1993) and participants’ behaviors may become uncoordinated or even work

at cross purposes.

9

Our interviews illustrate role system and knowledge integration conflicts among

organizations responding to surprise events with mixtures of improvised and non-improvised

behaviors. For example, a software startup responded to sudden client demands with a

combination of highly improvisational actions to solve customer needs on the fly and

contradictory routine actions aimed at enforcing established procedures to constrain contractual

‘scope creep.’ As one founder commented on the failed project, “You’ve got three peoples’

efforts going into something. Two of them are interfering with the one who can get it (CloSoft,

10:36.”) A more dramatic illustration is Weick’s (1993) analysis of the Mann Gulch forest

firefighting disaster. As the firefighters responded to the surprise that a huge inferno was

suddenly right upon them, one of the senior members of the group improvised by burning an

escape fire that allowed him to survive the blaze. The rest of the crew failed to follow him in the

improvisation, the firefighters’ role system disintegrated, and most of the crew died. Combining

our arguments for low, high and moderate levels of organizational improvisation, we therefore

argue:

Hypothesis 1: Responses to surprise events characterized by either high or low levels of

improvisation will generate better outcomes than will responses characterized by

moderate levels of improvisation.

Organizational Memory and the Outcome of Responses to Surprise Events

We define organizational memory as the collective knowledge of an organization,

residing primarily in shared beliefs, behavioral routines and physical artifacts (Argote, 1999;

Moorman & Miner, 1997; 1998). Theories of organizational learning suggest that greater

organizational memory will increase the chances that a given surprise response will yield good

outcomes. There are two key mechanisms by which organizational memory can be value-

creating in response to surprises. First, greater organizational memory permits a focal firm to

access a more extensive and diverse repertoire of existing responses (Berliner, 1994; Hargadon

10

& Sutton, 1997). The repertoire includes standard operating procedures (Cohen et al., 1996;

Cyert & March, 1992[1963]), behavioral routines (Feldman & Pentand, 2003; Nelson & Winter,

1982), and previously developed contingency plans (Billings, Milburn, & Schaalman, 1980).

This opens the door to recombine or apply existing components of organizational memory to

effectively address surprise events (Hargadon & Sutton, 1997).

A high level of organizational memory also allows the focal firm to interpret events –

including a surprise event – by fitting them into pre-existing categories of events and actions.

This categorization helps a firm to retrieve and make connections with useful information from

its own prior actions and outcomes (Douglas, 1986; Hargadon & Fanelli, 2002; Lounsbury &

Rao, 2004). By understanding and categorizing the surprise event– often through use of analogy

(Hargadon & Sutton, 1997) – in terms of similarities with what it has experienced before, the

organization can make more effective selections from the repertoire of existing responses in its

memory (Cohen & Levinthal, 1990; Daft & Weick, 1984; Day & Nedungadi, 1994; Jackson &

Dutton, 1988; Sinkula, 1994).

Organizational memory thus guides action through making available repertoires of

potential responses and also through helping to match actions from these repertoires to the

surprise event (Moorman & Miner, 1997). In the context of entrepreneurial firms, prior research

has suggested two relevant sources of organizational memory – organizational age (Argote,

1999) and the prior experience of founders (Phillips, 2002).

Organizational age. Literature in organizational learning has adopted organizational age

as a proxy for organizational memory (Argote, 1999), showing that organizations benefit from

their operating history including both success and failure experience (e.g., Ingram & Baum,

1997; Kim & Miner, 2007). Firms accumulate organizational memory during its operating

history, which is expected to improve their ability to respond effectively to surprise events.

11

Specifically, older organizations have had the opportunity to develop diverse action repertoires,

making it more likely that a useful response to the surprise will reside in organizational memory

(Levitt & March, 1988; Walsh, 1995). Further, the operating history of older organizations

makes available a wider array of interpretive tools to make sense of the surprise (Moorman &

Miner, 1997). This implies that organizational age will play a positive role in guiding effective

responses to surprise events.

Another stream of research, however, suggests that organizational age should reduce the

effectiveness of organizational responses, if age is correlated with rigidity (Burgelman, 1983;

Dougherty, 1992; Leonard-Barton, 1992). From this perspective, even if memory offers a large

action-repertoire and rich interpretive framework, an organization may not take advantage of this

potential and instead maintain a highly consistent pattern of action to the point of maladaptive

rigidity. This line of reasoning implies that organizational age could have a negative effect on the

outcome of responding to surprise events.

To reconcile these conflicting perspectives on organizational age, we turn to the

particular research setting of this study. In a start-up context, most firms have not reached a stage

of maturity yet. As a result, organizational age related rigidities are less likely and more

controllable (Gersick, 1994; Maurer & Ebers, 2006). Under this circumstance, we expect that

among young firms, the benefits of organizational age will dominate any inertial tendencies

developed along firms’ operating history. Thus, we argue,

Hypothesis 2a: Older organizations’ responses to surprise events will generate better

outcomes than will the responses of younger organizations.

Organizational age and improvisation. Organizational age, as one important source of

organizational memory in start-ups, is expected to moderate the effect of improvisation on

outcome of responding to surprise events. One of the most fundamental developments in theories

of improvisation holds that the value of improvisation can be contingent on the experience level

12

of the focal actor (Moorman & Miner, 1998; Vera & Crossan, 2005). Improvisation, after all, is

rooted in and reflects an organization’s past learning from operating experience. This insight is

consistent with what scholars have long observed in many different fields. In jazz and theater, for

example, scholars have long noted that the mastery of a diverse repertoire provide the

foundations of skillful collective improvisation (Berliner, 1994). At organizational level,

Moorman and Miner (1998) further propose that improvisation should generate value in the

presence of a high level of organizational memory. They argue that a rich and diverse

organizational memory contributes to the coherency, speed and novelty of improvisational

actions.

Several important mechanisms may drive such positive interaction effect between

improvisation and organizational age. From a perspective of coordination, research on

improvisation suggests that the value of improvisational actions can be enhanced by formal or

informal systems of communication and coordination, both of which are developed along an

organization’s operating history. In older organizations, formal communication and coordination

are built into organizational structures and routines (Levitt & March, 1988; Weick, 1993). In

parallel, informal communication and coordination evolves along familiarity among

organizational members over time, which can absorb potential disruptions on the existing role

systems caused by improvisation. Learning about others’ abilities, resources and habits –

“knowing who is good at what in the group” (Ren, Carley, & Argote, 2006) – can improve the

joint ability of people in older organizations to improvise effectively. For example, in McGinn

& Kross (2002) study of social embeddedness and successful improvisation, bargaining partners

who were friends prior to negotiations were much more likely to improvise a satisfactory

solution than were strangers.

13

For organizations, a longer operating history may not only increase the level of fruitful

interaction via more efficient communication and coordination, but it may also allow for

members to learn to improvise together (Baker et al., 2003). Firms may develop improvisational

competencies hand in hand with other basic organizing capabilities (Weick, 1979). As

organizational members deal with challenges for which they have no obvious prior routines, they

learn how to improvise, who they can improvise with effectively, and how to avoid some of the

problems that poorly executed improvisation can create. This practiced and potentially nuanced

local understanding of improvisational processes helps build the organizational capacity to

improvise effectively. Consistent with this argument, Moorman & Miner (1998) uncovered a

variety of distinctive improvisational competencies that developed as the organizations they

studied gained experience in dealing with emergent problems and opportunities in specific areas

of activity. Given these mechanisms, we expect the effect of improvisation on the outcome of

responding to surprise events to be strengthened under conditions of high level of organizational

age:

H2b. Improvisational responses to surprise events will generate better outcomes in older

than in younger organizations.

Founder prior experience. Since start-ups, by definition, are still in the early stages of

accumulating their own collective organizational experience, they often call on the founding

teams’ prior work experience as a form of supplemental and less broadly accessible memory to

guide action in many areas (Baker et al., 2003; Beckman, 2006; Helfat & Lieberman, 2002;

Phillips, 2002). In the context of new ventures, relevant founder experience has long been

understood to have a positive association with new venture performance (Phillips, 2002). For

example, Phillips (2002) found that the mobilization of partners in Silicon Valley law firms led

to the transfer of routines from parent firms to progeny firms, which subsequently increased life

chances of progeny firms.

14

An experienced founding team often equips the organization with a range of “industry

recipes” (Carpenter & Westphal, 2001; Hambrick, Geletkanycz, & Fredrickson, 1993; Spender,

1989). This expands the firm’s repertoire of potential responses to surprise events (Mason &

Harrison, 2006; Wright, Robbie, & Ennew, 1997), while perhaps somewhat increasing the

challenges of accessing and integrating this knowledge while deploying it during improvised

responses to surprise events. In addition, founder experience can provide rich frameworks and

causal theories about appropriate responses to surprise events, which facilitates the

categorization and interpretation of surprise events. In the area of entrepreneurship, observers

have reported from empirical studies that founders’ prior experiences as employees and resulting

cognitive “blueprints” strongly shape how their new organizations develop and adapt to a variety

of challenges (Baron, Hannan, & Burton, 1999; Boeker, 1988). Taken together, these ideas and

data imply that prior founder experience, as a supplementary organizational memory in start-ups,

can enhance the outcome of organizations’ responding to surprise events.

Hypothesis 3a: Organizations that include high levels of prior founder experience will

generate better outcomes in response to surprise events than will the responses of

younger organizations.

Prior founder experience and improvisation. Following the similar logic of memory-

contingent improvisation value, we argue that there is potentially positive effect of prior founder

experience for the effect of improvisation on the outcome of responding to surprise events.

Specifically, an experienced founding team may act as catalysts to derive value out of

improvisational responses.

Under conditions of a high level of founder experience, firms can draw on founders’ prior

skills or competencies that may act as crucial creative engines in improvisational responses

(Miner et al., 2001). As note earlier, founders with extensive industry experience bring

“blueprints” and “industry recipes” that become rich materials for creating novel responses

15

through improvisation (Baker et al., 2003; Spender, 1989). This prior industry experience serves

as useful “referents,” which been shown to be play an important role in maintaining coherence

during the process of skillful collective improvisation (Miner et al., 2001). Prior founder

experience thus helps translate and link improvisational actions to the established stock of

knowledge resided in the founding team, thus enhancing its value in response to surprise events.

This reasoning implies that in the presence of sufficient founder experience, we will expect an

enhanced outcome out of improvisational responses.

In consistent with this line of argument, Hatch (1997) showed that the experience of a

jazz band’s leader aids in recognizing cues, knowing when to shift emphasis among different

players, and deciding in what direction to go next thereby creating a sense of collective

coherence and improvisational skill. Prior studies have also suggested that among musicians,

high levels of individual skills increase the speed and ease at which collective coordination and

skilled improvisation can be created, (Berliner, 1994). We therefore hypothesize,

H3b. Improvisational responses to surprise events will generate better (short term)

outcomes in organizations with higher levels of founder experience than in organizations

with lower levels of such experience.

METHODS

Sample

The unit of analysis in this study is the surprise event in a firm. Our sample consists of

141 surprise events described in interviews with 31 firms located in a Midwestern county. The

sampling process involved two separate phases: (1) constructing a sample frame of knowledge-

based firms and (2) deriving a sample of surprise events based on this sampling frame. The

initial sampling frame from three primary sources: the Dun & Bradstreet database (n=19,645); a

university start-up directory published by a major state university (n=178); and the Directory of

High Technology Companies published by a local utility company (n=372) and supplemented by

16

interviewing with local experts (n=63). We then created a sample meeting our target for a study

population of knowledge-based start-ups using sampling filters. The filters eliminated all firms

that was more than five years old, and, because this study focuses on organizational level

activities, all firms with fewer than 3 employees. We identified the Biotechnology & Drug sector

(SIC 283), Information Technology (IT) industry (SIC 737), and Research, Development, and

Testing Services (SIC 873) as important local industries for our study. We randomly selected 60

firms from the resulting sampling frame of 147 firms in these industries. Six of these firms

refused to participate and we were not able to make contact with three others.

The interviews conducted in the resulting 60 knowledge-based firms consisted of 23 pilot

interviews with experimental interview protocols and 37 interviews with the finalized protocol.

We captured 235 potential surprise events from these 37 interviews. Of these, 141 surprise

events from 31 organizations provided complete information for data analysis.

We used a multi-step process to create the sample of surprise events within these

randomly selected firms. Using in-depth semi-structured interviews described below, we

collected information regarding specific surprises and responses from company founders. The

interviews were conducted in a two-step process. In the first part, founders were asked to tell the

story of their business. The interview began with the following prompt/question: “There are a lot

of myths out there about how small businesses work. To get beyond these myths, we are trying

to learn more about founder’s day-to-day encounters with unexpected events. I’d like to hear the

story of your business, beginning with how you came to start it. We are interested in the history

of your company. As I wrote in my email, we especially interested in unexpected events. Let me

remind you of the two examples we gave.” Responses to this question were followed-up by

questions seeking to get descriptive detail on the specific the activities prior to and after each

surprise event.

17

In the second part of the interview, the interviewer reviewed the surprises that the

informant had identified during the interview. The informant then completed a brief written set

of instruments regarding each surprise. In a post-interview verification phase, the initial surprises

reported by informants and reviewed again by the informants at the end of the interview were

reviewed by two independent raters not at the interview, but trained in applying the definition of

a surprise used in this study. Table 2 outlines coding heuristics for this second process of surprise

assessment.

-- Insert Table 2 about here --

Data collection process

The main raw data used for both time varying and time invariant variables was initially

obtained through the interviews, with final measures developed using the steps detailed below.

Pilot interviews were conducted to improve the initial protocol and to study how the interview

materials affected respondents’ reporting behavior. Interview techniques followed standard

qualitative practices (Denzin & Lincoln, 1998) to minimize retrospective and other bias. At least

two and sometimes more members of the project team conducted each interview, with one

member guiding the interview and the other taking notes and asking occasional clarification

questions. A typical interview lasted 2.5-3 hours, with some lasting much longer. A professional

transcription service transcribed all interviews.

As retrospective bias by informants is always a threat to validity in interview studies

(Golden, 1992), we minimized this possibility by taking a number of safeguards. First, the

interview data were collected from firm founders and founding teams, who were well-positioned

to have firsthand knowledge about facts important to our study. Second, the effect of time was

reduced to some extent by limiting the firms to those five or fewer years old. Third, the interview

process followed “courtroom” procedure where respondents were directed to focus on concrete

18

facts and events rather than personal speculation (Birley, 1976; Eisenhardt 1989; Nisbet & Ross,

1980). Respondents were asked to date the events and construct the history of their business in

chronological order, reducing the tendency to frame and interpret those events to fit their current

view of the organization.

Social desirability could also threaten the systematic identification of surprise events.

Some respondents might have perceived having any surprises as inconsistent with their

administrative role of controlling the organization, assuming that they should be fully prepared to

any business events. To minimize the effect of social desirability, we provided written

confidentiality guarantees. Further, interviewers focused on the action sequences of events,

avoiding hinting at any evaluation of any activity the respondent described.

Dependent Variable

The dependent variable, Perceived Outcome, was measured with a four-item 7-point

Likert scale consisting of the following four questions: (1) The results of my company’s initial

response to this surprise were satisfactory; (2) I am pleased with the results of my company’s

initial response to this surprise; (3) I believe this surprise was handled well by my company; and

(4) The results of our response to this surprise were good for the company. For each question, the

response ranged from one being “Strongly Disagree” to four being “Neither Agree nor Disagree”

to seven being “Strongly Agree”.

The scale was tested on a sub-sample of 50 responses from 8 companies. Pilot reliability

assessment of the measure indicated an alpha of 0.870. Because respondents were required to

fill out a written instrument for each surprise at the conclusion of the interview, reliable scales

with fewer items were helpful to improve response rates. Removal of item 4 from the scale

improved the alpha to 0.872, and so a final scale composed of the first 3 items was used. Using

the final sample of 141 surprises, of the scale generate an alpha 0.907. Administering the written

19

instruments after all surprises had been discussed allowed respondents the opportunity to view

their responses in relation to each other, and improves the accuracy of the assessment of the

items by activating their memories before their written responses (Schwarz, Strack, & Mai, 1991;

Tourangeau, Rasinski, Bradburn, & D'Andrade, 1989).

Independent Variables

Improvisation. This variable captures the degree of improvisation that characterized

organizational behaviors just after the surprise event, and was coded by multiple raters who

separately evaluated the improvisational nature of the organizations’ post surprise activties by

reading transcripts of the interviews. The coding process involves two main steps: (1)

identification of specific post-surprise actions, and (2) coding the degree to which these actions

were improvisational.

In Step 1, two team members engaged in “line finding” related to behavioral responses to

each surprise event. This involved identifying specific words, lines, or paragraphs of the

transcript related to post-surprise behavior for a given surprise. The sets of lines identified in

Step 1 were put in random order in terms of which transcripts they came form, and in terms of

whether they represented pre or post surprise action. These randomized sets of lines from the

transcripts were then given to the coders for Step 2. This randomization reduced the potential for

bias should coders have views about specific firms or harbor tacit theories about how pre- and

post-surprise behavior should differ (Schwab, 2004).

In Step 2, five raters coded the degree to which behaviors were improvisational based on

their reading of the randomized transcript blocks produced in Step 1. Drawing on the

improvisational scale from Moorman & Miner (1998), a preliminary coding scale for

improvisation was developed for rating the post-surprise action mode. We followed an iterative

process of pilot coding, scale refinement, and coding heuristic development. Table 3 shows the

20

final coding sheet. Note that non-improvised activity includes several distinct behavioral

variations, such as following a routine, planning or following a plan. The inter-rater reliability of

the five coders on action mode coding was deemed acceptable with an ICC(2,5) of 0.805.

-- Insert Table 3 about here --

Firm age. The variable was calculated from reported firm start date, allowing calculation

of age at the time of the surprise event. Firm Age was scored as the number of months the

organization had existed at the time the surprise event occurred.

Founder experience. The variable was measured as the logarithmic transformation of

aggregated years of business experience of all founding team members, at the time the surprise

occurred. Years of business experience was collected by asking the respondents how much

business experience they and any founder not present at the interview had prior to starting their

business and then adding the age of the company in years at the time of the surprise.

Control Variables

In addition to the theoretical variables described above, we included a number of control

variables that prior theory or empirical evidence suggest might influence the outcome of

responding to a surprise.

To control for industry effect, Industry is a dummy variable set equal to 1 if the firm

operates in IT related industry, and 0 if it comes from biotechnology industry. To control for

potential differences in improvisational patterns among firms of different sizes, Firm Size was

measured as the logarithm transformation of the count of employees, on an annual basis.

Respondents were given a chart and asked to write the number of employees who were

employed at start-up and the beginning of each subsequent year. Sufficient Time at Time of

Surprise and Sufficient Money at Time of Surprise were collected by asking founders to complete

charts indicating quarterly adequacy of available material resources and available time. Having

21

slack resources of either time or funds may permit a greater number of feasible options in

responses, and hence more valued responses (Eisenhardt & Tabrizi, 1995). To control for

difference among founding teams, we included two variables related to founder background –

Average Founder Education Level and Aggregated Founder Prior Start-ups. To measure

Average Founder Education Level, the level of education of each founder was collected and

scored on an eight-point scale, (1 = some high school and 8 = at least one terminal degree)

beyond) and the average computed. Aggregated Founder Prior Start-ups, was calculated as the

total number of prior start-ups across all founders.

In addition to firm characteristics, we also controlled for some potentially important

characteristics of the surprise events themselves. Strategic Level of Surprise captures the degree

to which a surprise event is “strategic”. Consistent with prior use in the organizations literature

(Ansoff, 1975; Winter, 2004), we define a surprise as being more strategic the more it involves

the organization’s long term survival and prosperity, the more it in effect calls “into question its

foundation premises about what to do” (Lengnick-Hall & Beck, 2005: 739). Surprises are less

strategic when they involve threats or opportunities that appear likely to affect the organization

in narrower or short term ways. Much of the organizations literature has also focused on strategic

surprises as threatening events (Jauch & Kraft, 1986; Lampel & Shapira, 2001; Mintzberg,

1994), suggesting that the strategic level of surprise is negatively related to the perceived

outcome. Respondents rated the Strategic Level of Surprise using a four item measure: (1) This

surprise was important at the time to the company’s long-term survival and prosperity; (2) This

surprise was not a key factor in the long-term survival and prosperity of the company (reverse

coded); (3) This surprise affected the long-term survival and prosperity of the this company; and

(4) This surprise distracted us from far more important activities. Respondents rated on a seven-

point scale the degree to which they agreed to the above statements (1 = highly disagree; 7 =

22

highly agree). The inter-item alpha for this scale in a sub-sample was 0.679. We reviewed the

items for construct validity and excluded Item 4, which generated a reliability of 0.832 on the

subsample. Reliability for the three item scale on final sample of surprises was an inter-item

alpha of 0.886.

The other surprise related variable Positive Surprise captures whether the surprise event

is perceived as a positive event by the focal firm. Although both negative and positive surprises

can theoretically produce valued or problematic outcomes, we expect that positive surprises

should have a greater chance of generating good outcomes. To measure Positive Surprise, two

independent raters reviewed the detailed descriptions of the surprise event in the interview

transcript and field notes. Each coded whether a surprise was positive or negative. The coding

process resulted in a 96% agreement between the two raters. The two raters discussed and

reached agreements on the remaining 4% cases of surprise.

Analysis

We used Ordinary Least Square (OLS) regressions to test our hypotheses regarding

outcomes to responses to surprise. We developed a data set in which each surprise event

represents a case. One hundred forty-one surprise events were collected for which all necessary

data were available, from thirty-one firms, yielding an average of 4.5 surprise events per

organization. Using multiple observances within firms raises concerns about whether surprise

events are independent. Consequently, we used “cluster” modeling techniques to accommodate

any potential lack of independence between surprise events that occur within the same firm.

Essentially, the “cluster” function generates robust standard errors adjusted for repeat

observations by the same firm. For robustness check, we also ran linear mixed models, also

known as hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) (Hofmann, Griffin, & Gavin, 2000; Kozlowski &

23

Klein, 2000). Models were run using HLM and no significant differences were observed

compared to results from OLS. We report OLS results here.

RESULTS

Descriptives and model development. Table 4 presents descriptive statistics and

correlations for all variables in the models. The bivariate correlation coefficients among some

interaction variables were high, which raises the concern of multicollinearity. We performed a

test to estimate the variance inflation factors (VIFs) of all variables and found that they were all

well below 10, a conventional rule of thumb that is used to assess multicollinearity (Wooldridge,

2002). This exploratory procedure indicates that multicollinearity did not pose as a severe threat

to the models in stake.

Table 5 reports the results of our regression analyses. Model 1 is a baseline that includes

all control variables, while Model 10 represents the saturated model with all variables and

relevant interactions. Because of the hypothetical interactions, our first step of assessing

regression results lies in selecting the appropriate model to interpret the effects. To determine the

contribution of individual interaction terms to the overall model fit, we estimated models by

hierarchically adding interaction terms (Model 6 – 10). We evaluated the interaction terms under

model fitness guidelines, with an emphasis on whether the addition of interaction terms

significantly contribute to the improvement of model fit (Aiken and West 1991; Jaccard, Clark,

& Golder, 1990). Across all models, Model 6 represents the most parsimonious model without

losing the explanatory power. It shows significant improvements on model fit over the main

effect Model 5 (∆ adjusted R2 = 0.011, F = 4.32, p < .05). Further, Model 6 indicates a better

model fit over Model 8-10, as the two interaction terms—Improvisation x Founder Experience

and Improvisation Squared x Founder Experience—made little contribution to the overall model

fit. Thus, we base our interpretations on Model 6.

24

While we base our interpretation on Model 6, it is noteworthy that the interaction term

Improvisation Squared x Firm age is marginally significant (β = 0.007, p < .10) in Model 7, and

it yields a marginally significant model improvement (Model 7: ∆ adjusted R2 = 0.006, F = 4.20,

p < .10). We conducted robustness check on the basis of Model 7, which yields consistent

results.

-- Insert Table 4 and 5 about here --

Improvisation and perceived outcome (H1). We proposed a U-shaped relationship

between post-surprise improvisation and perceived outcome of responding to surprise events

(H1). We find support for H1. We evaluated the hypothesized U-shaped relationship by two

steps. First, the coefficient for Improvisation was negative and statistically significant (β = -

1.246, p < .01) and its squared term was positive and statistically significant (β = 0.127, p < .05)

in Model 6. These are based on a conservative two-tailed analysis. Put together, the two

coefficients present preliminary evidence of a U-shaped relationship between post-surprise

improvisation and the perceived outcome of responding to surprise events.

While the two coefficients provide initial support for H1, a more rigorous assessment of

the relationship should consider whether the U-shaped relationship holds up at different levels of

firm age, given the presence of the interaction term Improvisation x Firm Age in Model 6 (β =

0.010, p < .05). In this scenario, a simple “main effect” interpretation may lead to omission bias

(Echambadi, Campbell, & Agarwal, 2007). Thus as the second step of evaluation, we plotted the

effect of post-surprise improvisation on perceived outcome at different levels of firm age, i.e.,

one standard deviation above the mean, at the mean, and one standard deviation below the mean.

Figure 1 illustrates the plotting results. The horizontal axis shows the range of values of post-

surprise improvisation level; the vertical axis shows the perceived outcome. All three lines in

Figure 1 point to a similar U-shaped pattern. This indicates an unconditional U-shaped

25

relationship between post-surprise improvisation and perceived outcome at different levels of

firm age.

Results of the three coefficients, combined with the corresponding plotting, support the

predicted U-shaped relationship between post-surprise improvisation and perceived outcome. A

start-up’s perceived outcome improves when it shifts towards lower level (the left-hand tail) or

higher level (the right-hand tail) of improvisation. In contrast, the perceived outcome declines

when it engages at an intermediate level of improvisation, as reflected in the area around the

trough of the curve. Overall, firms with pure non-improvisational response or pure

improvisational response have a higher perceived outcome, while firms with a moderate level of

improvisational response have a lower perceived outcome.

-- Insert Figure 1 about here --

Firm age and perceived outcome (H2a). In Hypothesis 2a we predicted that firm age, as

one type of organizational memory, will be positively related to perceived outcome because of

the positive value embedded in organizational memory. H2a is not supported. Assessing the total

effect of firm age on perceived outcome involves looking at the combined effect of two

coefficients: The coefficient of Firm Age (β = -0.060, p < .01) and the coefficient of the

interaction term Improvisation x Firm Age (β = 0.010, p < .05). The significant, negative

coefficient for Firm Age rejects an unconditional positive value of memory as hypothesized in

H2a.

Improvisation and firm age (H2b). Hypothesis 2b predicted that firm age will increase

the potential value of improvisation on perceived outcome. H2b is supported. To test this

hypothesis, we first start with an assessment of the model fit improvements with the added

interaction term. As noted, adding the interaction term Improvisation x Firm Age significantly

improves model fit over Model 5 that only include main effects (Model 6: ∆ adjusted R2=0.011,

26

F = 4.32, p<.05). This presents initial evidence of the existence of interaction effect. At the same

time, the coefficient for the interaction term is significant and follows the hypothesized direction

(β = 0.594, p < .05), providing further support to a positive interaction between post-surprise

improvisation and firm age.

To further probe the nature of the interaction effect, we conducted marginal effect

analysis of the two variables involved. Appendix A contains the corresponding partial derivative

equations for all conditional analyses, following standard treatments of interaction models

(Aiken & West, 1991; Brambor, Clark, & Golder, 2005; Jaccard et al., 1990). We calculated

estimates of the change in perceived outcome given a marginal change (one standard deviation)

in post-surprise improvisation and firm age, as follows:

[∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=low, AGE=low] = -0.601** (0.172) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=low, AGE=mean] = -0.447* (0.166) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=low, AGE=high] = -0.293 (0.190) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=mean, AGE=low] = -0.292* (0.100) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=mean, AGE=mean] = -0.138 (0.092) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=mean, AGE=high] = 0.159 (0.133) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=high, AGE=low] = 0.016 (0.127) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=high, AGE=mean] = 0.170 (0.123) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=high, AGE=high] = 0.324* (0.158)

These conditional analyses show that post-surprise improvisation has a significant

negative effect on perceived outcome, when both post-surprise improvisation and firm age are at

low or medium levels (β = -0.601, p < .01; β = -0.447, p < .05; and β = -0.292, p < .01). It has a

significant positive effect on perceived outcome when both post-surprise improvisation and firm

age are at high levels (β = 0.324, p < .05).

As an interaction effect represents the joint impact of two variables, we also conducted

conditional analysis on the effect of firm age on perceived outcome. This revealed consistent

support for a positive moderating effect of post-surprise improvisation on the value of firm age.

27

These findings support the positive interaction effect between post-surprise improvisation and

firm age as hypothesized in H3a.

Figure 2 depicts the marginal impact of firm age on perceived outcome. The vertical axis

of the Figure 2 represents the marginal effect of firm age on perceived outcome; the horizontal

axis represents post-surprise improvisation within the full data range. Figure 2 indicates that at

low level of improvisation, firm age has a strong negative effect on perceived outcome (also see

Appendix A: β = -0.038, p < .01). This negative effect declines as the level of post-surprise

improvisation increases. At the high end of improvisation firm age no longer has a significant

reductive impact on perceived outcome (Also see Appendix A: β = -0.014, p > .05). Figure 2

underscores that the impact of post-surprise improvisation and firm age are not merely additive,

but that the impact of each is enhanced by the presence of the other.

-- Insert Figure 2 about here --

Founder experience and perceived outcome (H3a). Hypothesis 3a predicted that prior

founder experience, as supplementary organizational memory, is positively related to perceived

outcome. H3a is supported. There is no significant interaction effect found for improvisation and

fonder experience. As a consequence it is appropriate to assess this variable using the simple

coefficient of Founder Experience. Consistent with our prediction, prior founder experience at

time of surprise has a significant, positive effect on perceived outcome (β = 0.594, p < .05).

Founding teams with a higher level of experience have a higher perceived outcome, compared to

those with more limited prior experience.

Improvisation and founder experience (H3b). We proposed that post-surprise

improvisation will have a stronger effect when there is presence of high level of prior founder

experience (H3b). This is not supported. As noted, Model 8 and 9 includes two interaction terms,

28

Improvisation x Founder Experience and Improvisation Squared x Founder Experience. These

two terms did not yield a significant contribution to model fit.

Baseline model. Although our focus is on the theory driven variables, we examined the

baseline model as well. Average founder education level was also negatively related to perceived

outcome to surprise responses (β = -0.297, p < .01). The other controls are unrelated to perceived

outcome.

Robustness check on model selection. As a robustness check, we conducted separate

marginal effect analysis of post-surprise improvisation and age on perceived outcome, on the

basis of Model 7. We further visually depict the marginal impact of firm age on perceived

outcome in Figure 3. The vertical axis of the Figure 3 is the marginal effect of firm age on

perceived outcome; the horizontal axis represents post-surprise improvisation within the full data

range. Figure 3 indicates that at low level of improvisation, firm age has a strong reductive effect

on perceived outcome. This reductive effect declines as the level of post-surprise improvisation

increases to a medium level. At the medium and high levels of post-surprise improvisation the

reductive effect of firm age on response is no longer significant. The overall marginal effect

pattern of firm age is consistent with Figure 2 that is based on Model 6.

-- Insert Figure 3 about here --

DISCUSSION

Our study proposes that improvisation and organizational memory play a key role in what

happens after an organization responds to a specific surprise. We argued that the degree of

improvisation has a curvilinear impact on perceived outcome, and the presence of organizational

memory will influence the chances that an improvisational response will generate an outcome

valued by the organization. Taken as whole, our study supports our framework, but also presents

important puzzles and issues for further work. We first discuss the specific findings for our

29

hypotheses, and then consider rival interpretations of results and study limitations. We conclude

by considering implications of our work for the broader landscapes of research on organizational

surprise.

Improvisation and Perceived outcome

Our results for improvisation provide a more nuanced understanding of the value of

improvisation in a surprise response context. Consistent with prior research on improvisation,

sticking to modes of planning and executing plans, or drawing on prior routines in the response

produced better outcomes than mixing some improvisation with using planning and routines

(Moorman & Miner, 1997). If an organization operates in a setting with some stability, and has

had time to develop action routines and planning capabilities, then in general improvisation – or

any novel action – is on average a bad bet. A subset of novel actions will be beneficial but the

overall expected value should be negative (March, 1991).

However, our study provides important evidence that improvisation can offer value in

surprise conditions. Prior quantitative studies of improvisation have raised doubt about its value.

At the same time, prior qualitative studies gave vivid examples of improvisational activity that

avoided death in the presence of a fire-wall (Weick, 1993), re-interpreted rules to permit positive

activity during a strike (Preston, 1991), sustained a distributed navigation process (Hutchins,

1991), and responses to problems and unexpected opportunities in new product development

(Miner et al., 2001). Our results are consistent with the notion that improvisation may have more

value specifically in the context of surprises, than in a more general way.

In addition, we theorized that this is especially likely in the start-up setting because if the

greater chances of focused attention on the improvisational activity and greater potential for

coordination linked to incoming real-time information and knowledge (Moorman & Miner,

1998). For any firm, a surprise presumably represents a valid indicator that the organization

30

faces a changing context, or has an incomplete or incorrect set of automatic responses, making

some form of novel activity – including improvisation – potentially valuable (Mendonca, 2005;

Weick, 1993). The organization can stop and plan, but in many cases start-ups have fewer

planning resources, may have less resource buffers to deal with threats, making urgent action

more important. The fusion of designing and executing action, if informed with other knowledge,

can have value in this setting (Baker & Nelson, 2005; Gong et al., 2004).

Organizational Memory and Improvisation

We proposed that greater organizational memory will enhance the chances that a

perceived outcome will have value to the organization. We had theorized that both firm age and

prior founder experience can provide valuable structure and knowledge for a new venture’s

responding to surprises. Findings for the impact of founder related experience are consistent with

our theory. However, we did not find that firm age enhanced the value of perceived outcomes.

Instead, firm age is found negatively correlated with perceived outcome. We speculate that the

actual knowledge deployed in dealing with surprises may draw more on founder experience

rather than on the formal structure and routines that are accompanied by organizational inertia

and rigidity. A post hoc analysis of the specific descriptions of individual surprises seemed

consistent with this conclusion. Many surprises seemed to be idiosyncratic to a firm’s specific

context, rather than representative of a class of surprises that may frequently occur in start-ups.

We proposed a conditional framework of improvisation value. We had theorized that the

presence of organizational memory will enhance improvisational value in response to surprise.

Findings for the interaction between organizational memory and improvisation are consistent

with our theory. When both firm age and improvisation are at high levels, organizations are more

likely to drive valuable perceived outcomes out of improvisational actions. This implies that

improvisation can actually serve as an effective antidote to organizational rigidity and inertia.

31

Thus, our theory and setting offer more precise ideas about boundary conditions for when

improvisation can have a net positive impact.

Although our results are generally consistent with our predictions about how different

types of knowledge challenges influence the outcomes of surprise responses, some of our

theoretical and baseline results deserve further exploration. We found differential impacts from

the two types of organizational memory – firm age and prior founder experience. Further work

could seek to examine the impact of different types of organizational memory in response to

unexpected events. We also found that more highly educated founding teams tended to have less

valuable perceived outcomes, which seems counterintuitive. Our sample included some

university-linked start-ups; one potential interpretation of this finding is that faculty founding

teams were less likely to have business capabilities to respond to business surprises. However,

this somewhat counterintuitive result deserves further exploration.

Rival Interpretations and Study Limitations

Our study design contains several powerful features that help avoid potential dangers

such as common methods variance and results arising from informants’ trying to comply with

social norms. The identification of surprises was done by informants, but checked against

definitions by later objective researchers. The informants did not hear the word improvisation or

engage in discussions of their ideas about factors that will influence responses to surprises.

Informants rated the outcomes of their responses to surprises, while most variables in the causal

model were derived from objective fact sheets reporting items like founder experience or

organizational size. Finally, the coding of levels of improvisation in the actual response activities

occurred long after the interviews, and was done by raters reading randomized units of text

drawn from 1,725 pages of transcripts. The raters did not know whether they were rating a pre-

surprise or post-surprise action, reducing the chances that their tacit ideas about improvisation

32

and outcomes would impact results. These study design features strongly reduce the chances that

the pattern of results arises from common methods variance or social desirability effects.

One important question that does arise is whether theories about cognitive processes

related to surprise could account for the pattern of our findings. Several important literatures

imply that decision makers who observe the outcome of their decisions will tend to be

disappointed: better results will be systematically expected than are obtained (Harrison & March,

1984). Our setting does not fully correspond to the standard set-up for this causal model, which

focuses prior deliberate choice followed by specific related outcomes. Nonetheless, one could

argue that when the start-ups respond to the surprise, this can itself be conceptualized as a

‘decision’ and their perceived outcome the product of this decision. If the standard

disappointment process were in effect, the organizational ratings of perceived outcomes should

on average be negative. In our data, this did not occur. The mean rating of outcomes was slightly

above neutral. The general pattern of the perceived value of outcomes, then, cannot be explained

through the prior work on post-decision disappointment.

Implications: Organizational surprises. Surprises can lead to organizational actions

that directly influence a firm’s well being. Our study advances theory about organizational

surprises by studying this crucial area directly. Although our sample included both positive and

negative surprises, the majority were negative, so our evidence has more relevance to negative

surprises. Therefore the adaptation in question relates primarily to adaptation to threats or

negative changes.

Our work complements three lines of work on surprises that focus on different aspects of

the potential chain of surprises related activities. Much important work arises in the long history

in the military and political strategy literatures, where encountering a strategic surprise is

commonly viewed as a strongly negative event (Byman, 2005; Chan, 1979; Handel, 1984;

33

Mahnken, 1999). Our work moves from the issue of avoiding surprises to directly responding to

surprise, an important area worthy of attention. Our work also complements but is distinct from

the important emerging research on the cognitive contrast between the original expectations and

the revealed world as shown in the surprise event as a driver for changes in cognitive maps of the

world (Stiensmeier-Pelster et al., 1995). In that context, theorists envision surprise as one of the

most powerful engines for long-term learning: surprise events reveal weaknesses or

incompleteness in prior models of reality. Exploring their origins offers a chance to develop

more accurate models of the world (Cohen & Axelrod, 1984; Gavetti & Levinthal, 2001).

Our study contributes in a different way to long-term learning models that emphasize

more behavioral change rather than shifts in cognitive states. A surprise can stimulate

organization to try several novel behaviors, even though it does not directly revise the

organization’s cognitive map. The surprise event and its recognition can “unfreeze” an

organization and new behaviors or changes in action may occur (Edmondson, Bohmer, & Pisano,

2001; Lewin, 1947). Our study provides a framework to predict the outcome of a response to the

surprise – which then becomes a ‘trial’ and its outcome for such long-term trial and error

learning (Miner et al., 2001).

Finally, the literatures on strategic change suggest that unexpected threats or

opportunities may lead to broad changes in organizational strategy or behavior (Edmondson et

al., 2001; Huff, Huff, & Thomas, 1992; Meyer, 1982). Environmental jolts (Meyer, 1982; Sine &

David, 2003), interruption (Zellmer-Bruhn, 2003), threat (Staw et al., 1981), and crisis (Kim,

1998; Lin et al., 2006) all offer contexts where surprise events may occur. Our work

complements and extends this general body of work because we examine the outcomes of an

organization’s specific response to a given surprise. Our approach implies that fairly local

conditions in terms of the organization’s own memory, the strategic level of the surprises and the

34

degree of improvisation during a given response can play a role in the outcomes of responses.

These in turn may influence the long-term trajectories of adaptation. Exploring such possibilities

offers an important next step in research on knowledge deployment and organizational surprises.

35

REFERENCES

Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. 1991. Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.

Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Ansoff, H. I., 1979. Strategic Management. London: MacMillan. Argote, L. 1999. Organizational learning: Creating, retaining and transferring knowledge.

Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers. Baker, T. 2007. Resources in play: Bricolage in the toy store(y). Journal of Business Venturing,

22: 694-711.

Baker, T., Miner, A., & Eesley, D. 2003. Improvising firms: Bricolage, retrospective

interpretation and improvisational competencies in the founding process. Research Policy, 32: 255-276.

Baker, T. & Nelson, R. E. 2005. Creating something from nothing: Resource construction

through entrepreneurial bricolage. Administrative Science Quarterly, 50: 329-366. Baron, J. N., Hannan, M. T., & Burton, M. D. 1999. Building the iron cage: Determinants of

managerial intensity in the early years of organizations. American Sociological Review, 64(4): 527-547.

Beckman, C. M. 2006. The influence of founding team prior company affiliations on firm

behavior. Academy of Management Journal, 49: 741-758. Berliner, P. F., 1994. Thinking in Jazz: The Infinite are of Improvisation. Chicago: University

of Chicago Press. Billings, R. S., Milburn, T. W., & Schaalman, M. L., 1980. A model of crisis perception – a

theoretical and empirical analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 25(2): 300-316. Birley, S. 1976. Acquisition strategy or acquisition anarchy? Journal of General Management,

3: 67-73. Boeker, W. 1988. The permanence of organizational strategy. Academy of Management

Proceedings. Brambor, T., Clark, W. R., & Golder, M. 2005. Understanding interaction models: Improving

empirical analyses. Political Analysis, 13(1): 1–20. Burgelman, R. A., 1983. Corporate entrepreneurship and strategic management: Insights from a

process study. Management Science, 29(12): 1349-1364. Byman, D., 2005. Strategic surprise and the September 11 attacks. Annual Review of Political

Science, 8: 145-170.

36

Carpenter, M. A. & Westphal, J. D. 2001. The strategic context of external network ties: examining the impact of director appointments on board involvement in strategic decision making. Academy of Management Journal, 44: 639-660.

Chan, S., 1979. The intelligence of stupidity: understanding failure in strategic warning.

American Political Science Review, 73(1): 171-180. Cohen, M. D. & Axelrod, R. 1984. Coping with complexity: The adaptive value of changing

utility. The American Economic Review, 74(1): 30-42. Cohen, M. D., Burkhart, R., Dosi, G., Egidi, M., Marengo, L., Warglien, M., & Winter, S. 1996.

Routines and other recurring action patterns of organizations: Contemporary research issues. Industrial and Corporate Change, 5(3): 653-698.

Cohen, W. M. & Levinthal, D. A., 1990. Absorptive capacity: A new perspective on learning and

innovation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 35: 128-152. Conner, D. R. 1998. Leading at the edge of chaos. John Wiley & Sons. Cunha, M. P., Kamoche, K. & Clegg, S. R. 2005. Clues, cues and complexity: Unpacking the

concept of organizational surprise. Working paper.

Cyert, R. M. & March, J. G., 1992[1963]. A behavioral theory of the firm. Englewood-Cliffs,

NJ: Prentice-Hall. Daft, R. L. & Weick, K. E., 1984. Toward a model of organizations as interpretation systems.

Academy of Management Review, 9: 284-295. Day, G. S. & Nedungadi, P., 1994. Managerial representations of competitive advantage.

Journal of Marketing, 58: 31-44. Denzin, N. K. & Lincoln, Y. S. 1998. Collecting and Interpreting Qualitative Materials. Sage,

Thousand Oaks, CA. Dougherty, D., 1992. Interpretive barriers to successful product innovation in large firms.

Organization Science, 3: 179-202. Douglas, M., 1986. How Institutions Think (1st ed.). Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University Press. Edmondson, A. C., Bohmer, R. M., & Pisano, G. P. 2001. Disrupted routines: Team learning and

new technology implementation in hospitals. Administrative Sciences Quarterly, 46(4): 685–716.

Eisenhardt, K. M. 1989. Making fast strategic decisions in high-velocity environments.

Academy of Management Journal, 32(3): 543-576. Eisenhardt, K. M. & Tabrizi, B. N. 1995. Accelerating adaptive processes - Product innovation

in the global computer industry. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40(1): 84-110.

37

Feldman, M. S. & Pentand, B. T., 2003. Reconceptualizing organizational routines as a source of flexibility and change. Administrative Science Quarterly, 48: 98-118.

Galbraith, J. 1973. Designing Complex Organizations. Addison-Wesley. Gavetti, G., & Levinthal, D. Looking forward and looking backward: Cognitive and experiential

search. Administrative Science Quarterly, 45(1): 113-137. Gersick, C. J. G., 1994. Pacing strategic change - the case of a new venture. Academy of

Management Journal, 37(1): 9-45. Golden, B. R. 1992. The past is the past or is it - the use of retrospective accounts as

indicators of past strategy. Academy of Management Journal, 35(4): 848-860. Gong, Y., Baker, T., & Miner, A. S. 2004. Where do routines come from in new ventures? Paper

presented at the Paper presented at the Academy of Management Annual Meeting, New Orleans, August.

Greve, H. R. 1998. Performance, aspirations, and risky organizational change. Administrative

Science Quarterly, 43:58–86. Hambrick, D. C., Geletkanycz, M. A., & Fredrickson, J. W. 1993. Top executive commitment to

the status quo: Some tests of its determinants. Strategic Management Journal, 14(6): 401-418.

Handel, M. I., 1984. Intelligence and the problem of strategic surprise. Journal of Strategic

Studies, 7(3): 229-281. Hargadon, A. & Fanelli, A., 2002. Action and possibility: Reconciling dual perspectives of

knowledge in organizations. Organization Science, 13(3): 290-302. Hargadon, A. & Sutton, R. I., 1997. Technology brokering and innovation in a product

development firm. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42(4): 716-749. Harrison, J. R. & March, J. G. 1984. Decision making and post decision surprises.

Administrative Science Quarterly, 29: 26-42. Hatch, M. J. 1997. Jazzing up the theory of organizational improvisation. In J. P. Walsh & A. S.

Huff (Eds.), Advances in strategic management, 14: 181-191. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Helfat, C. E. & Lieberman, M. B., 2002. The birth of capabilities: market entry and the

importance of pre-history. Industrial and Corporate Change, 11(4): 725-760. Hofmann, D.A., Griffin, M.A., & Gavin, M.B. 2000. The application of hierarchical linear

modeling to organizational research. In K.J. Klein and S.W. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel Theory, Research, and Methods in Organizations, 467-511. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

38

Huff, J. O., Huff, A. S., & Thomas, H. 1992. Strategic renewal and the interaction of cumulative stress and inertia. Strategic Management Journal, Summer Special Issue 13: 55-75.

Hutchins, E. 1991. Organizing work by adaptation. Organization Science, 2: 14-39. Ingram, P. and Baum J. 1997. Opportunity and constraint: Organization’s learning from the

operating and competitive experience of industries. Strategic Management Journal, 18: 75-98.

Jaccard, J., Turrisi, R., & Wan, C. 1990. Interaction effects in multiple regression. Newbury

Park, CA: Sage Publications. Jackson, S. E. & Dutton, J. E., 1988. Discerning threats and opportunities. Administrative

Science Quarterly, 33: 370-387. Jauch, L. R. & Kraft, K. L., 1986. Strategic Management of Uncertainty. Academy of

Management Review, 11(4): 777-790. Kay, N. M. 1984. The Emergent Firm. Mcmillan. Kim, L. 1998. Crisis construction and organizational learning: Capability building in catching-up

at Hyundai motor. Organization Science, 9(4): 506-521. Kim, J., & Miner, A. S. 2007. Vicarious learning from the failure and near-failure of others:

Evidence from the U.S. commercial banking industry. Academy of Management Journal, 50(2): 687-714.

Kozlowski, S.W. & Klein, K.J. 2000. A multilevel approach to theory and research in

organizations: Contextual, temporal, and emergent processes. In K.J. Klein and S.W. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel Theory, Research, and Methods in Organizations, 3-90. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Lampel, J. & Shapira, Z., 2001. Judgmental errors, interactive norms, and the difficulty of

detecting strategic surprises. Organization Science, 12(5): 599-611. Lengnick-Hall, C. A. & Beck, T. E., 2005. Adaptive fit versus robust transformation: how

organizations respond to environmental change. Journal of Management, 31(5): 738- 757.

Leonard-Barton, D., 1992. Core capabilities and core rigidities: A paradox in managing new

product development. Strategic Management Journal, 13: 111-125. Levinthal, D. A. & Rerup, C. 2006. Crossing an apparent chasm: Bridging mindful and

less-mindful perspectives on organizational learning. Organization Science. 17(4):502- 513.

Levitt, B. & March, J. G., 1988. Organizational learning. Annual Review of Sociology, 14: 319-

340.

39

Lewin, K. 1947. Fronteirs in group dynamics: Concepts, method, and reality in social science. Human Relations, 1: 5-41.

Louis, M. R. 1980. Surprise and sense making: What newcomers experience in entering

unfamiliar organizational settings. Administrative Science Quarterly, 25(2): 226-251. Lounsbury, M., & Rao, H. 2004. Sources of durability and change in market classifications: A

study of the reconstitution of product categories in the American mutual fund industry, 1944-1985. Social Forces, 82: 969-999.

Mahnken, T. G., 1999. Uncovering foreign military innovation. Journal of Strategic Studies,

22(4): 26-54. March, J. 1991. Exploration and exploitation in organizational learning, Organization Science,

2(1): 101-123. Mason, C. M. & Harrison, R. T., 2006. After the exit: Acquisitions, entrepreneurial recycling and

regional economic development. Regional Studies, 40(1): 55-73. Maurer, I. & Ebers, M., 2006. Dynamics of social capital and their performance implications:

Lessons from biotechnology start-ups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 51(2): 262- 292.

Mendonca, D. 2005. Decision support for improvisation in response to extreme events: Learning

from the response to the 2001 World Trade Center attack. Decision Support Systems. Meyer, A. D. 1982. Adapting to environmental jolts. Administrative Science Quarterly, 27(4):

515-537. Meyer, A. D., Gaba, V., & Colwell, K. A. 2005. Organizing far from equilibrium: Nonlinear

change in organizational fields. Organization Science, 2005: 456-473. Miner, A. S. 1990. Structural Evolution through Idiosyncratic Jobs. Organization Science, 1(2):

195-205 Miner, A. S. 1991. Organizational evolution and the social ecology of jobs. American

Sociological Review, 54: 772-785. Miner, A. S., Bassoff, P., & Moorman, C. 2001. Organizational improvisation and learning: A

field study. Administrative Science Quarterly, 46: 304-337. Mintzberg, H., 1994. The rise and fall of strategic planning. New York: The Free Press.

Perlmutter, A., Gooch, J. (Eds.). 1982. Military Deception and Strategic Surprise. London: Routledge.

Moorman, C. & Miner, A. S. 1997. The impact of organizational memory on new product

performance and creativity. Journal of Marketing Research, 34: 91-107.

40

Moorman, C., & Miner, A. S. 1998. Organizational improvisation and organizational memory. Academy of Management Review, 23: 698-723.

Narver, J. C., & Slater, S. F., 1990. The effect of a market orientation on business profitability.

Journal of Marketing, 54: 20-35. Nelson, R. R., & Winter, S. G., 1982. An Evolutionary Theory of Economic Change.

Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Nisbet, R. E. & Ross, L. 1980. Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social

Judgment. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:: Prentice Hall. Okhuysen, G. A., & Eisenhardt, K. M. 2002. Integrating knowledge in groups: How formal

interventions enable flexibility. Organization Science, 13(4): 370-386. Phillips, D. J. 2002. A genealogical approach to organizational life chances: The parent-progeny

transfer among Silicon Valley law firms, 1946-1996. Administrative Science Quarterly, 47: 474-506.

Preston, A. 1991. Improvising order. In I. L. Mangham (ed.), Organization Analysis and

Development: 81-102. New York: Wiley. Ren, Y., Carley, K. M., & Argote, L. 2006. The contingent effects of transactive memory: When

is it more beneficial to know what others know? Management Science, 52: 671-682. Schützwohl, A. 1998. Surprise and schema strength. Journal of Experimental Psychology,

Learning, Memory and Cognition, 24(5): 1182-1199. Schwab, D. 2004. Research Methods for Organizational Studies. 2nd Edition. Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates. Schwarz, N., Strack, f., & Mai, H. P. 1991. Assimilation and contrast effects in part-whole

question sequences: A conversational logic analysis. Public Opinion Quarterly, 55: 3- 23.

Sine, W. D. & David, R. J., 2003. Environmental jolts, institutional change, and the creation of

entrepreneurial opportunity in the US electric power industry. Research Policy, 32(2): 185-207.

Sitkin, S. B. 1992. Learning through failure: The strategy of small losses. Research in

Organizational Behavior, 14: 231-266. Spender, J. C. 1989. Industry recipes: An inquiry into the nature and sources of managerial

judgment. Oxford, UK: Blackwell. Staw, B. M., Sandelands, L. E. & Dutton, J. E. 1981. Threat rigidity effects in organizational

behavior: A multilevel analysis. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(4): 501-524.

41

Tourangeau, R., Rasinski, K. A., Bradburn, N., & D'Andrade, R. 1989. Carryover effects in attitude surveys. Public Opinion Quarterly, 53: 495-524.

Van de Ven, A. H., & Polley, D. 1992. Learning while innovating. Organization Science, 3(1):

92-116. Vanhamme, J. 2000. The link between surprise and satisfaction: an exploratory research on

how best to measure surprise. Journal of Marketing Management, 16 (6): 565-582. Vera D. and Crossan M., 2005, Improvisation and innovative performance in teams.

Organization Science, 16(3): 203-224. Waller, M. J. 1999. The timing of adaptive group responses to nonroutine events. Academy of

Management Journal, 42: 127-137. Walsh, J. 1995. Managerial and organizational cognition: notes from a trip down memory lane.

Organization Science, 6(3): 280-321. Watkins, M. D., & Bazerman, M. H. 2003. Predictable surprises: The disasters you should have

seen coming. Harvard Business Review, 81(3): 72-80. Weber, M. 1978. Economy and Society. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Weick, K. 1979. The Social Psychology of Organizing. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Weick, K. E. 1993. The collapse of sensemaking in organizations: The Mann Gulch disaster.

Administrative Science Quarterly, 38: 628-652.

Weick, K. E. 1998. Improvisation as a mindset for organizational analysis. Organization science, 9(5): 543-555 Weick, K.E., & Sutcliffe, K. M. 2001. Managing the unexpected. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Weick, K. E., Sutcliffe, K. M, & Obstfeld, D. 1999. Organizing for high reliability: Process of

collective mindfulness. Research in Organizational Behavior, 21: 81-123. Winter, S. G., 2004. Specialised perception, selection, and strategic surprise: Learning from the

moths and bees. Long Range Planning, 37(2): 163-169. Wright, M., Robbie, K., & Ennew, C., 1997. Venture capitalists and serial entrepreneurs.

Journalof Business Venturing, 12(3): 227-249. Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E. 2003. Interruptive events and team knowledge acquisition. Management

Science, 49(4): 514-528.

44

Su

rpri

se I

DS

urp

rise

Ev

ent

Pag

e: L

ine

Rel

ated

Tex

ts i

n I

nte

rvie

w T

ransc

rip

tsP

osi

tiv

e o

r

Neg

ativ

e

Are

a

SC

H0

2F

orm

er e

mp

loy

ee

thre

aten

ed t

o p

ut

code

on I

nte

rnet

8:1

2S

o, h

e le

aves

, h

e ca

lls

up a

wee

k l

ater

and

say

s, y

ou

kn

ow

, y

ou w

ere

real

ly u

nder

pay

ing m

e.

Rig

ht.

T

hat

’s c

apit

alis

m,

you

kno

w,

pro

fiti

ng

fro

m t

he

lab

or

. . .

so I

’m g

oin

g t

o p

ut

you

r co

de

on

th

e In

tern

et. S

o,

then

the

FB

I go

t in

vo

lved

in

that

, an

d t

hat

was

a d

isas

ter.

NH

R

SC

H0

1S

oft

war

e g

litc

h4:3

4W

e w

ere

cras

hin

g c

om

mun

icat

ion

s an

d p

oli

ce n

etw

ork

s ar

ou

nd

th

e co

untr

y, h

ad t

o g

ive

peo

ple

bac

k t

hei

r m

oney

.It

was

just

dis

aste

r. S

o, th

at w

as w

ithin

a y

ear

of

inco

rpora

ting

. W

e sh

ould

pre

tty

mu

ch h

ave

rati

onal

ly p

ack

ed i

t in

.

NT

echn

ical

AB

S0

2L

ost

dea

l2

3:2

3S

o, w

e th

oug

ht

we’

d g

et t

hat

dea

l fo

r su

re.

And h

e sa

id,

wel

l, w

e’re

mer

gin

g. A

nd I

sai

d,

ok

ay, w

ell,

that

’s f

ine.

You

’re

goin

g t

o m

erge

wit

h a

new

com

pan

y a

nd

it’

s a

big

com

pan

y, I

said

, b

ut

yo

u’r

e su

re y

ou

sti

ll h

ave

an o

kay

on

this

pro

ject

? Y

es,

yes

, y

es, w

e’re

fin

e.

Sure

as

shit

, th

ey m

erg

ed, n

o d

eal.

T

he

real

hold

ers

of

the

new

com

pan

y h

ad a

bro

ther

-in-l

aw o

r so

meb

od

y w

ho c

ould

do

it.

NC

lien

t

Rel

atio

nsh

ip

INF

01

Fal

se r

eport

ing o

f

pre

gn

ancy

8:1

2W

e st

arte

d o

ut

wit

h a

n i

nit

ial

coll

abo

rato

r in

the

Un

iver

sity

wh

o w

as s

up

po

sed t

o b

e a

rep

rod

uct

ion

spec

iali

st .

.. I

t w

as

ver

y h

ard t

o k

eep t

hem

fo

cuse

d o

n w

hat

we

nee

ded

to

acc

om

pli

sh.

...

Then

we

had

a f

alse

rep

ort

ing

of

a p

regn

ancy

in

the

sum

mer

by

th

em .

.. W

hic

h a

ctual

ly t

urn

ed o

ut

thei

r ow

n s

lopp

y r

eco

rdk

eep

ing.

They

rep

ort

ed i

t as

a p

oss

ible

pre

gn

ancy

of

a n

ucl

ear

tran

sfer

em

bry

o a

nd i

t tu

rned

ou

t th

e bo

ar h

ad b

red

her

. Q

uit

e a

big

dif

fere

nce

.

NP

artn

ersh

ip

IST

01

Com

pet

ito

r p

rici

ng

cut

34:2

2A

fter

th

e fo

urt

h q

uar

ter

last

yea

r th

ey w

ere

real

ly i

n p

ain

. I

mea

n, al

l th

e co

mpo

nen

t co

mp

anie

s w

ere

feel

ing a

lo

t o

f

pai

n. B

ut

Com

p c

hose

to

dea

l w

ith t

hat

by

dra

stic

ally

lo

wer

ing t

hei

r co

nsu

ltin

g b

ill

rate

s. I

mea

n,

lik

e w

her

e th

ey w

ere

chop

pin

g t

hem

in

hal

f. I

mea

n, th

ey w

ere

go

ing

fro

m e

igh

ty s

om

e do

llar

s an

ho

ur

to $

40

an h

ou

r. A

nd

th

ey s

tart

ed

doin

g t

hin

gs

lik

e, i

f y

ou

bri

ng i

n t

his

con

sult

ant

we'

ll g

ive

yo

u t

his

co

nsu

ltan

t fo

r fr

ee. A

nd i

t's l

ike,

all

rig

ht,

what

do

you

do w

ith

th

at?

I m

ean,

so.

That

was

un

expec

ted

.

NC

om

pet

itio

n

MA

T0

1A

t L

ink g

oes

ou

t of

busi

nes

s

19:2

8A

nd

At

Lin

k w

ent

ou

t o

f bu

sin

ess

in A

pri

l o

f la

st y

ear.

S

o,

we

dea

lt w

ith

the

par

tner

s le

avin

g,

and

th

en t

wo

mo

nth

s

late

r, A

t L

ink g

oes

into

ban

kru

ptc

y a

nd e

ver

yb

od

y t

urn

s off

th

eir

pip

es. A

nd

th

en s

udd

enly

we

hav

e no

In

tern

et a

cces

s

in o

ur

off

ice.

NO

per

atio

nal

LU

H01

Acq

uir

ed m

ajo

r

clie

nt

4:0

4Y

eah

. I

mea

n, a

lot

of,

wel

l, o

ne

maj

or

clie

nt

was

, y

ou

kn

ow

, to

tall

y u

nex

pec

ted

. Y

ou k

no

w w

hat

I m

ean?

I m

ean

, w

e

did

n’t

rea

lly

hav

e a

mar

ket

ing p

lan

per

se,

so

we

kin

d o

f tr

y t

o d

o o

ur

thin

g.

But,

in

an

y c

ase,

yo

u k

no

w,

we’

re j

ust

sitt

ing t

her

e on

e day

and

th

is c

all

com

es a

nd s

om

ebo

dy

say

s, h

ey,

you

kn

ow

, y

ou g

uy

s m

ight

be

inte

rest

ed i

n t

his

. W

e

go o

ver

ther

e, a

nd

lik

e th

ree

day

s la

ter

we

hav

e th

is c

on

trac

t, y

ou

kno

w,

a b

ig o

ne,

rel

ativ

ely

sp

eak

ing

.You

kn

ow

what

I

mea

n?

And

so i

t’s

like,

wel

l, o

h g

oo

d,

yo

u k

now

wh

at I

mea

n, b

ut

it w

as v

ery r

and

om

.It

was

n’t

at

all

pla

nn

ed.

PS

ales

Examples of Surprises

TABLE 1

45

TABLE 2 - Coding Heuristics for Identifying Surprise

Descriptions

Definition of surprise A surprise is an event that is subjectively recognized as unexpected. Expansion: This may happen when some outcome is anticipated, and the actual outcome differs from the anticipation. The difference might be positive, negative, or neutral. A surprise may also occur when there was no conscious anticipation of a particular outcome other than an expectation that things would continue unchanged.

Process of surprise identification

The first identification of surprises was done by informants during substantial interviews. During each interview, the informant identifies surprises in “real time”, and interviewer writes a description of each surprise on a data sheet. To identify surprises for later coding, coders first looks at the surprises on the interview survey. Coders then read through the transcript to find the text that describes the data sheet surprises. Coders then decide if the text, in fact, provides evidence of a surprise as defined above. For now, we will disregard other things that may seem like surprises that may be identified in the coding process.

It’s a surprise if

Prior expectations are violated.

Includes instances where expectations are directly stated, and an opposing occurrence (or fact that indicates an opposing occurrence) is indicated – regardless of the interviewee(s) awareness of this as a surprise. Includes instances where expectations are diffuse (i.e. if one expects the status quo but the status quo is disrupted).

Key words to look for “ends up”, “it turns out”, “we found”, “find”, “couldn’t find”, “we got hit”, “we got faced”, “ends up”

It’s tied to a specific event, or is a specific event.

Includes instances where a more specific surprise is embedded within a larger surprise. We are particularly interested in identifying technical and scientific surprises embedded within other surprises.

“We found out he wasn’t as good as maybe he thought he was, or anybody else” (1st surprise, InfTech 8:14) “They reported it as a possible pregnancy of a nuclear transfer…and it turned out the boar had bred her” (another, more specific embedded in 1st surprise)

Surprises are not

Lessons learned Subjective perceptions of knowledge gained and “lessons learned” that respondents indicate has surprised them over the course of doing business.

“We realized the best way to find clients is through networking”. Or “We were surprised how easy it is to incorporate”.

Pattern recognition Respondent indicates a general pattern noticed over time.

“we’ve found is there are a lot of state contracts that you can get and a lot of them are what, you know, some people called wired”)

Uncertain results versus unexpected events.

Events that occur after uncertainty is expressed versus an indication of violated expectations (or no prior expectation).

“We had negotiations going on …two of them simultaneously. It’s kind of a horse race to see who we’d end up with, or who would end up with us…We ended up working with” (one of the firms negotiation with).

Instances where teams violates their own plan.

This confuses a surprise with a behavioral change, and it potentially confuses a surprise with the dependent variable.

There is not enough

information to code pre

or post behavior

This type of surprise can be identified as “not codable”.

46

TABLE 3

Coding Scale of Improvisation

Codin

g S

cale

of

Imp

rovis

atio

n

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Non Improvisation

L

ittl

e

Much

Full Improvisation

. Pure planning

. Pure execution of a plan or

routine

. N

egli

gib

le

. L

oose

pla

nnin

g

and/o

r lo

ose

ex

ecuti

on

. C

annot

rule

out

impro

vis

atio

n

. M

ore

Non

impro

vis

atio

n

than

im

pro

vis

atio

n

. G

lim

mer

of

impro

vis

atio

n

. B

alan

ce o

f im

pro

vis

atio

n

and n

on

impro

vis

atio

n

. M

ore

im

pro

vis

atio

n

than

non

impro

vis

atio

n

. M

uch

more

im

pro

vis

atio

n

than

non

impro

vis

atio

n

. S

ignif

ican

t am

ount

of

impro

vis

atio

n

. Full improvisation

. “Action followed a strict

plan/routine as it was taken.”

. “Strictly followed out plan in

carrying out this action.”

. “Made it up as they went

along.”

. “Figured out action as they

went along.”

. “Ad-libbed action.”

ICC(2,5) = .805

47

TABLE 4

Descriptive Statistics and Correlations

Variable

Mean

S D

12

34

56

78

9

1.

O

utc

om

e of

Surp

rise

Res

po

nse

0.9

11.5

71

2.

In

dust

ry0

.92

0.2

70

.05

1

3.

F

irm

Siz

e1

.93

1.0

00

.05

-0.1

01

4.

S

uff

icie

nt

Tim

e at

th

e T

ime

of

the

Surp

rise

3.5

21.8

00

.11

-0.1

6*

0.0

51

5.

S

uff

icie

nt

Mon

ey a

t th

e T

ime

of

the

Surp

rise

3.5

61.5

90

.09

-0.1

00

.27

**

0.4

6**

1

6.

A

ver

age

Fo

un

der

Edu

cati

on

Lev

el4

.61

2.0

4-0

.17

*0.0

00

.13

†0.0

40

.02

1

7.

Aggre

gat

ed F

ound

er P

rio

r S

tart

-ups

1.8

62.4

7-0

.04

0.2

3*

*-0

.08

0.0

40

.10

0.1

9**

1

8.

S

trat

egic

Lev

el o

f S

urp

rise

0.7

61.6

8-0

.07

-0.0

80

.09

-0.0

5-0

.23

**

0.1

00.0

01

9.

P

osi

tive

Surp

rise

0.1

10.3

10

.18

*0.1

00

.02

0.0

00

.04

0.0

40.0

20.1

8*

1

10. Improvisation

3.3

41.2

3-0

.02

-0.0

5-0

.05

-0.1

2-0

.19

*0.0

1-0

.12

†-0

.09

-0.0

9

11. Improvisation Squared

12

.68

9.0

30

.01

-0.0

4-0

.02

-0.1

3*

-0.1

8*

-0.0

1-0

.15

*-0

.08

-0.0

7

12. Firm Age

22

.82

20.1

4-0

.24

**

-0.1

00

.17

*0.0

30

.02

0.1

4*

0.1

4*

-0.0

7-0

.16

*

13. Founder Experience

3.3

20.8

70

.10

0.1

3†

0.0

5-0

.09

-0.0

3-0

.08

0.5

7**

0.2

0*

0.1

4*

14. Improvisation x Firm Age

82

.48

77.4

2-0

.20

**

-0.1

10

.13

†0.0

0-0

.07

0.1

5*

0.0

8-0

.03

-0.1

6*

15. Improvisation Squared x Firm Age

317

.62

38

0.2

4-0

.14

†-0

.11

0.1

10.0

1-0

.07

0.1

00.0

4-0

.02

-0.1

4*

16. Improvisation x Founder Experience

10

.89

4.6

80

.00

0.0

60

.00

-0.1

2-0

.14

†-0

.10

0.2

9**

0.0

8-0

.03

17. Improvisation Squared x Founder Experience

40

.46

29.3

80

.00

0.0

30

.02

-0.1

3-0

.16

*-0

.09

0.0

70.0

3-0

.06

Variable

Mean

S D

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

10. Improvisation

3.3

41.2

31

11. Improvisation Squared

22

.82

20.1

40

.98

**

1

12. Firm Age

3.3

20.8

70

.04

0.0

41

13. Founder Experience

1.8

62.4

7-0

.10

-0.1

20

.29

**

1

14. Improvisation x Firm Age

82

.48

77.4

20

.43

**

0.4

2*

*0

.87

**

0.2

5**

1

15. Improvisation Squared x Firm Age

317

.62

38

0.2

40

.60

**

0.6

1*

*0

.67

**

0.1

5†

0.9

4*

*1

16. Improvisation x Founder Experience

10

.89

4.6

80

.75

**

0.7

0*

*0

.27

**

0.5

5**

0.5

3*

*0.6

1**

1

17. Improvisation Squared x Founder Experience

40

.46

29.3

80

.89

**

0.8

9*

*0

.18

*0.2

6**

0.5

1*

*0.6

7**

0.9

3**

1

a n =

141

.

p

< .

10

*

p <

.0

5

**

p <

.0

1

48

TABLE 5

Ordinary Least Square Models with Outcomes of Responses to Surprises as the Dependent Variable

MODEL 1

MODEL 2

MODEL 3

MODEL 4

MODEL 5

MODEL 6

MODEL 7

MODEL 8

MODEL 9MODEL 10

Con

tsta

nt

2.5

00*

2.9

40

*4

.362

**

1.2

61

2.8

55†

4.0

41*

5.8

77*

*3

.753

*4.4

43*

*5

.155

*

(1.1

44)

(1.2

15

)(1

.21

0)

(1.4

67

)(1

.393)

(1.4

56)

(1.7

85

)(1

.60

8)

(1.4

24

)(1

.855)

Ind

ust

ry-0

.724

-0.7

66

-0.7

37

-0.8

59

-0.8

43

-0.8

76

-0.8

15

-0.8

39

-0.8

28

-0.8

22

(0.7

03)

(0.6

84

)(0

.68

8)

(0.7

72

)(0

.771)

(0.8

19)

(0.7

69

)(0

.76

4)

(0.7

67

)(0

.784)

Fir

m S

ize

0.3

07

0.3

05

0.2

70

0.3

18

†0.2

78

0.2

67

0.2

46

0.2

70

0.2

70

0.2

49

(0.2

19)

(0.2

24

)(0

.21

6)

(0.1

85

)(0

.183)

(0.1

70)

(0.1

62

)(0

.18

3)

(0.1

84

)(0

.166)

Suff

icie

nt

Tim

e at

th

e T

ime

of

the

Su

rpri

se0.0

70

0.0

68

0.0

79

0.1

19

0.1

28

0.1

09

0.0

98

0.1

26

0.1

27

0.0

97

(0.1

12)

(0.1

09

)(0

.10

6)

(0.1

03

)(0

.099)

(0.0

93)

(0.0

92

)(0

.10

0)

(0.1

01

)(0

.091)

Suff

icie

nt

Mo

ney

at

the

Tim

e o

f th

e S

urp

rise

0.0

27

0.0

15

0.0

08

-0.0

19

-0.0

31

-0.0

46

-0.0

47

-0.0

31

-0.0

33

-0.0

47

(0.1

25)

(0.1

19

)(0

.11

7)

(0.1

07

)(0

.104)

(0.1

06)

(0.0

98

)(0

.10

4)

(0.1

04

)(0

.098)

Av

erag

e F

ou

nd

er E

du

cati

on

Lev

el-0

.37

5*

-0.3

81

*-0

.36

4*

-0.2

97**

-0.2

83

**

-0.2

97*

*-0

.28

4**

-0.2

83

**

-0.2

78*

*

-0.2

87*

(0.1

34)

(0.1

33

)(0

.12

9)

(0.1

06

)(0

.097)

(0.0

95)

(0.0

93

)(0

.09

6)

(0.0

98

)(0

.097)

Agg

regat

ed F

oun

der

Pri

or

Sta

rt-u

ps

-0.0

10

-0.0

13

0.0

01

-0.1

13

-0.1

06

-0.1

12

-0.1

16

†-0

.10

0-0

.103

-0.1

19†

(0.0

52)

-0.0

51

(0.0

51)

(0.0

71

)(0

.068)

(0.0

67)

(0.0

66

)(0

.06

8)

(0.0

71

)(0

.069)

Str

ateg

ic L

evel

of

Surp

rise

-0.1

29

-0.1

29

-0.1

22

-0.1

82

†-0

.17

6-0

.180

-0.1

80

†-0

.17

1-0

.168

-0.1

85†

(0.1

13)

(0.1

13

)(0

.11

6)

(0.1

04

)(0

.108)

(0.1

05)

(0.0

97

)(0

.10

7)

(0.1

05

)(0

.098)

Posi

tiv

e S

urp

rise

0.8

08

0.7

86

0.8

05

0.5

11

0.5

17

0.5

13

0.5

36

0.5

28

0.5

32

0.5

28

(0.5

05)

(0.5

07

)(0

.50

3)

(0.4

15

)(0

.415)

(0.4

06)

(0.3

95

)(0

.41

3)

(0.4

13

)(0

.397)

Improvisation

-0.0

92

-1.0

22

**

-0.9

65*

-1.2

46*

*-2

.42

6**

-1.2

51*

-1.7

25

-2

.093†

(0.0

94

)(0

.32

5)

(0.3

53)

(0.3

67)

(0.7

89

)(0

.48

9)

(0.8

90

)(1

.021)

Improvisation Squared

0.1

26

**

0.1

23

*0.1

27*

0.2

79*

*0

.132

*0.1

98

0.2

48†

(0.0

41)

(0.0

46)

(0.0

47)

(0.0

99

)(0

.04

7)

(0.1

26

)(0

.137)

Firm Age

-0.0

25

*-0

.023*

-0.0

60*

*-0

.14

6**

-0.0

23*

-0.0

23*

-0

.157

**

(0.0

11

)(0

.011)

(0.0

18)

(0.0

43

)(0

.01

0)

(0.0

10

)(0

.055)

Founder Experience

0.5

72*

0.5

84

*0.5

94*

0.6

34

*0.3

38

0.1

06

0.9

33

(0.2

67

)(0

.241)

(0.2

43)

(0.2

36

)(0

.38

0)

(0.3

84

)(0

.594)

Improvisation x Firm Age

0.0

10*

0.0

61

*0

.066

*

(0.0

05)

(0.0

25

)(0

.032)

Improvisation Squared x Firm Age

-0.0

07

†-0

.007†

(0.0

03

)(0

.004)

Improvisation x Founder Experience

0.0

69

0.2

23

-0.1

32

-0.0

85

(0.2

63

)(0

.385)

Improvisation Squared x Founder Experience

-0.0

22

0.0

12

(0.0

39

)(0

.052)

Any m

igra

tion

Obse

rvat

ion

s1

41

14

1141

141

14

11

41

14

1141

141

14

1

F-t

est

(for

over

all

mod

el f

itn

ess)

2.1

7†

2.8

3*

8.5

1**

3.7

3**

7.6

9*

*8.5

4**

10

.23*

*8

.73*

*12

.60*

*13

.25**

Ad

just

ed R

-Squ

ared

0.1

23

0.1

21

0.1

39

0.1

88

0.2

01

0.2

12

0.2

18

0.1

97

0.1

91

0.2

07

∆ a

dj.

R2 (

vs.

bas

elin

e M

odel

1)

-0.0

02

0.0

16

0.0

55

0.0

78

0.0

83

0.0

89

0.0

68

0.0

62

0.0

84

∆ a

dj.

R2 (

for

added

var

iab

les)

-0.0

02

0.0

18

0.0

55

0.0

62

0.0

11

0.0

06

-0.0

04

-0.0

06

-0.0

05

F-t

est

(for

added

var

iable

s)0.9

59

.44**

3.5

6*

4.1

0*

4.3

2*

4.2

0†

0.6

60

.31

0.1

7

(vs.

mod

el 1

)(v

s. m

odel

2)

(vs.

mod

el 1

)(v

s. m

odel

3)

(vs.

mo

del

5)

(vs.

mod

el 6

)(v

s. m

odel

5)

(vs.

mo

del

8)

(vs.

model

6)

† p

< .

10;

* p

< .

05

; ** p

< .0

1 (

two-t

ail

test

s). S

tan

dar

d e

rrors

are

adju

sted

fo

r cl

ust

erin

g o

n f

irm

s.

49

FIGURE 1

Predicted Second-order Improvisation Effect on Perceived outcome

-3.0

-2.5

-2.0

-1.5

-1.0

-0.5

0.0

0.5

1.0

1.0

2.0

3.0

4.0

5.0

6.0

7.0

Improvisasion

Perceived Outcome

Firm

Age (

mean +

1 S

.D.)

Firm

Age (

mean)

Firm

Age (

mean -

1 S

.D.)

50

-.1-.08-.06-.04-.020.02.04.06

Marginal Effect of Firm Age

01

23

45

67

Impro

vis

ation

Marg

inal E

ffect of F

irm

Age

95%

Confidence Inte

rval

FIGURE 2

The Marginal Effect of Firm Age on Perceived Outcome

(Bas

ed o

n M

odel

6)

51

FIGURE 3

The Marginal Effect of Firm Age on Perceived Outcome

(Bas

ed o

n M

odel

7)

-.14-.12-.1-.08-.06-.04-.020.02

Marginal Effect of Firm Age

01

23

45

67

Impro

vis

ation

Ma

rgin

al E

ffe

ct

of

Firm

Ag

e

95

% C

on

fid

en

ce

In

terv

al

52

)4(43 i

i

i IMPAGE

yββ +=

)5()cov(2)var()var( 434

2

3

2 ∧∧∧∧

∂∧

++= ββββσ iiAGE

y IMPIMPi

i

)1(*43

2

210 iiiiiii BASELINEAGEIMPAGEIMPIMPy εβββββ ++++++=

)2(2 421 ii

i

i AGEIMPIMP

yβββ ++=

)3()cov(*4)cov(2)cov(4)var()var(4)var( 4241214

2

2

2

1

2 ∧∧∧∧∧∧∧∧∧

∂∧

+++++= βββββββββσ iiiiiiIMP

y AGEIMPAGEIMPAGEIMPi

i

APPENDIX A

Partial Derivative of Interaction Models

The investigation of conditional effects in models containing interaction terms is based on the corresponding partial derivative of the full model that includes the interaction term. The following section outlines the derivative equations used to calculate regression coefficient estimates for different values of moderator variables (Brambor et al., 2006; Cohen et al., 2003). The fully specified Model 6 is:

Marginal effect of improvisation on outcome. The change in the dependent variable Surprise Perceived outcome for a marginal change of Post-surprise Improvisation for firm i is:

Standard errors of the marginal change can be derived with the following formula: With formula (2) and (3), we substitute the relevant values of Post-surprise Improvisation

and Age at the mean, a standard deviation below, and a standard deviation above the mean to calculate regression coefficient estimates (Aiken & West, 1991; Brambor, Clark, & Golder, 2005; Jaccard et al., 1990).

[∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=low, AGE=low] = -0.601** (0.172) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=low, AGE=mean] = -0.447* (0.166) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=low, AGE=high] = -0.293 (0.190) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=mean, AGE=low] = -0.292* (0.100) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=mean, AGE=mean] = -0.138 (0.092) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=mean, AGE=high] = 0.159 (0.133) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=high, AGE=low] = 0.016 (0.127) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=high, AGE=mean] = 0.170 (0.123) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(IMP) | IMP=high, AGE=high] = 0.324* (0.158) Marginal effect of firm age on outcome. The change in the dependent variable Surprise Perceived outcome for a marginal change of Age for firm i is:

Standard errors of the marginal change can be derived with the following formula: With formula (4) and (5), we substitute the relevant values of Post-surprise Improvisation

at the mean, a standard deviation below, and a standard deviation above the mean to calculate regression coefficient estimates (Aiken & West, 1991; Brambor, Clark, & Golder, 2005; Jaccard et al., 1990). The corresponding conditional analyses are presented as:

53

[∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(AGE) | IMP=low] = -0.038** (0.011) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(AGE) | IMP=mean] = -0.026** (0.009) [∂(OUTCOME)/ ∂(AGE) | IMP=high] = -0.014 (0.011)