response inhibition in ad/hd, cd, comorbid ad/hd+cd, anxious

15

Click here to load reader

Upload: trinhdung

Post on 11-Jan-2017

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. Vol. 39, No. 3, pp. 411–425. 1998

Cambridge University Press

' 1998 Association for Child Psychology and Psychiatry

Printed in Great Britain. All rights reserved

0021-9630}98 $15.00­0.00

Response Inhibition in AD}HD, CD, Comorbid AD}HD­CD,Anxious, and Control Children: A Meta-analysis of Studies

with the Stop Task

Jaap Oosterlaan

University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Gordon D. Logan

University of Illinois, Champaign, U.S.A.

Joseph A. Sergeant

University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands

The aim of this study was to investigate whether impaired response inhibition is uniquelyrelated to AD}HD or whether deficits in response inhibition are also evident in otherpsychopathological disorders. Furthermore, the suggestion was examined that anxietydisorders are associated with abnormally high levels of response inhibition. This paperpresents the results of a meta-analysis of eight studies in which response inhibition wasassessed with the so-called stop task in five groups of children: children with attentiondeficit}hyperactivity disorder (AD}HD), children with conduct disorder (CD), children withAD}HD­CD, children with anxiety disorders, and control children. A total of 456 childrenparticipated in the 8 studies. All children were in the age range 6–12 years. Consistent androbust evidence was found for a response inhibition deficit in AD}HD. However, responseinhibition deficits did not distinguish children with AD}HD from children with CD, norfrom children with comorbid AD}HD­CD. Contrary to predictions, anxious children didnot demonstrate enhanced levels of response inhibition.

Keywords: Conduct disorder ; anxiety disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder,information processing, response inhibition, meta-analysis.

Abbreviations: AD}HD: attention deficit}hyperactivity disorder ; BAS: Behavioural Acti-vation System; BIS: Behavioural Inhibition System; CD: conduct disorder ; IF: inhi-bition function; LD: learning disorder ; MRT: mean reaction time; NAS: NonspecificArousal System; ODD: oppositional defiant disorder ; SSRT: stop signal reaction time;ZRFT-slope: slope of the IF plotted as a function of ZRFT (z score of the relativefinishing time).

Introduction

Executive functions are those mental control processesthat enable self-control and goal-directed behaviour(Barkley, 1996; Denckla, 1994; Lezak, 1983; Pennington& Ozonoff, 1996; Torgesen, 1994). These functions aremediated by the frontal lobes, in particular by theprefrontal cortex and its extended networks (Lezak, 1983;Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Torgesen, 1994). A fun-damental component of the executive functions is theability to inhibit inappropriate responding. Barkley

Requests for reprints to: Dr Jaap Oosterlaan, Departmentof Clinical Psychology, University of Amsterdam, Roeters-straat 15, 1018 WB Amsterdam, The Netherlands (E-mail :kpjoosterlaan!macmail.psy.uva.nl).

(1997) distinguishes three forms of response inhibition:(1) inhibiting prepotent responses, (2) stopping an on-going response, and (3) inhibiting interference. Theultimate goal of response inhibition is to enhance adap-tive functioning (Halperin, McKay, Matier, & Sharma,1994).

Abnormalities in response inhibition are a centralcomponent in the description and explanation of childpsychopathological disorders and, in particular, of at-tention deficit}hyperactivity disorder (AD}HD). Barkley(1994, 1997), for example, argued that AD}HD involvesa pervasive deficit in all forms of response inhibition.According to Barkley, this deficit leads to secondaryimpairments in four executive functions that depend onefficient response inhibition for their execution: workingmemory, internalisation of speech, self-regulation ofaffect-motivation-arousal, and reconstitution.As a result,

411

Page 2: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

412 J. OOSTERLAAN et al.

children with AD}HD present with a disorder charac-terised by impaired executive functions, which in turnlead to disturbances in self-control and goal-directedbehaviour. These disturbances ultimately result in thebehaviours observed in AD}HD.

Quay (1988a, b, 1997) used Gray’s (1987) neuro-psychological model of brain function to explain theorigin of poor response inhibition in AD}HD. In Gray’smodel, behaviour is explained in terms of the activity oftwo opposing brain systems: the Behavioural InhibitionSystem (BIS), which is sensitive to signals of punishment,and the Behavioural Activation System (BAS), which issensitive to signals of reward. The BIS serves to inhibitbehaviour, whereas the BAS controls the initiation ofbehaviour. Quay proposed that children with AD}HDhave a persistently underactive BIS, which results inresponse inhibition deficits.

A number of other theoretical accounts have emergedin which the inattentive, hyperactive, and impulsivebehaviour of children with AD}HD has been suggestedto arise from a deficit in response inhibition (Douglas,1988, 1989; Milich, Hartung, Martin, & Haigler, 1994;Newman & Wallace, 1993; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996;Wender, 1972). Importantly, Barkley (1994, 1997) hasargued that response inhibition deficits are unique toAD}HD. This suggestion is also made implicitly in othertheoretical accounts of AD}HD (Douglas, 1988, 1989;Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Wender, 1972).

The poor performance ofAD}HDchildren on a varietyof measures has been taken to support the responseinhibition deficit hypothesis for AD}HD. These measuresinclude the Matching Familiar Figures Test (Campbell,Douglas, & Morgenstern, 1971; DuPaul, Anastopoulos,Shelton, Guevremont, & Metevia, 1992; Weyandt &Grant, 1994), the Continuous Performance Task (see forreviews, Barkley, Grodzinsky, & DuPaul, 1992; Corkum& Siegel, 1993; see also Halperin et al., 1994), the Go}No-go Task (Iaboni, Douglas, & Baker, 1995; Milich et al.,1994; Shue & Douglas, 1992), delayed response tasks(Daugherty & Quay, 1991; McClure & Gordon, 1984;Schweitzer & Sulzer-Azaroff, 1995; Solanto, 1990), theWisconsin Card Sorting Test (see for review, Barkleyet al., 1992), and many others.

These measures, however, have been criticised for theirpoor construct validity and have been considered as tooglobal (Halperin et al., 1994). Performance on thesemeasures may be influenced by many factors other thanresponse inhibition, such as age and IQ (Milich et al.,1994; Schachar & Logan, 1990). The major criticismlevelled at these tasks is their failure to clarify themechanisms underlying impaired response inhibition(Milich et al., 1994; Schachar & Logan, 1990). Thesecriticisms do not apply to the stop task (Logan & Cowan,1984; Logan, Cowan, & Davis, 1984). This task ispurported to measure the ability to interrupt an ongoingresponse. Several studies have supported the reliabilityand validity of the stop task as a measure of re-sponse inhibition (Kindlon, Mezzacappa, & Earls, 1995;Tannock, Schachar, Carr, Chajczyk, & Logan, 1989).

The stop task is based on a well-established theory ofresponse inhibition, known as the race model (see forreview, Logan, 1994; Logan & Cowan, 1984). Accordingto this model, response inhibition depends on a race

between, on the one hand, the process underlyingresponse execution, and on the other hand, the inhibitoryprocess. This inhibitory process is triggered by infor-mation that tells the subject to discontinue or changea current course of action, such as an error duringperformance. The process that finishes first determinesperformance. If the inhibitory process runs to completionfirst, the response is inhibited. In the opposite case, theongoing action is completed. In contrast to other mea-sures of response inhibition, the stop task enables one toinvestigate whether poor response inhibition is due to adeficit in the inhibitory process.

Briefly, the stop task requires fast and accurateexecution of a reaction time task, denoted as the primarytask. Occasionally, a stop signal is presented, whichrequires the child to inhibit the response to the primarytask. Stop signals are presented at different intervalsbefore the subject’s expected response. The shorter theinterval, the more difficult it becomes to inhibit theresponse. Usually, the intervals are chosen such that theshortest interval will yield a probability of inhibition closeto 0, whereas the longest interval will produce a prob-ability of inhibition close to 1. A short description of thestop task, the race model, and the main dependentmeasures in the stop task is provided in the Appendix.

In the last few years, the stop task has been utilised ina series of studies to investigate deficits in response in-hibition in children with AD}HD and other psychopath-ology (Aman, Roberts, & Pennington, in press ;Daugherty, Quay, & Ramos, 1993; Jennings, Van derMolen, Pelham, Brock, & Hoza, 1997; Oosterlaan &Sergeant, 1996; Pliszka & Borcherding, 1995; Schachar& Logan, 1990; Schachar & Tannock, 1995; Schachar,Tannock, Marriott, & Logan, 1995). This body of re-search is reviewed in the current study. The principal aimof this study was to investigate whether data gatheredwith the stop task supports the response inhibition deficithypothesis for AD}HD. Furthermore, we investigatedwhether impaired response inhibition is uniquely relatedto AD}HD or whether deficits in response inhibition arealso evident in other psychopathological disorders.Finally, we examined the suggestion that anxiety dis-orders are associated with abnormally high levels ofresponse inhibition (Quay, 1988a, b).

Meta-analytic procedures were used to aggregatefindings across different studies. One of the advantages ofmeta-analytic procedures is the greater power to detectgroup differences (Rosenthal, 1991). This is particularlyimportant given the small groups employed in moststudies. Consequently, the failure to detect group dif-ferences in these studies might reflect inadequate powerrather than the absence of group differences.

The existing literature enabled us to evaluate thefollowing five group comparisons through meta-analysis.For each of these comparisons, hypotheses were derivedfrom current theories of child psychopathology.

AD}HD–controls (1 ). This comparison addresseswhether data derived from the stop task lend support fortheoretical accounts of AD}HD suggesting that thisdisorder is characterised by poor response inhibition(Barkley, 1994, 1997; Douglas, 1988, 1989; Newman &Wallace, 1993; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Quay,1988a, b, 1997; Wender, 1972). Thus, it was predicted

Page 3: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

413RESPONSE INHIBITION IN CHILD PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

that AD}HD children would demonstrate poor responseinhibition compared with controls.

CD–controls (2 ) and AD}HD–CD (3 ). As indicatedearlier, it has been suggested that deficits in responseinhibition are confined to children with AD}HD(Barkley, 1994, 1997; Douglas, 1988, 1989; Pennington &Ozonoff, 1996; Wender, 1972). However, consonant withAD}HD, poor response inhibition has also been de-scribed as a core phenomenon in conduct disorder (CD)(Farrington, 1993; Milich et al., 1994; Newman &Wallace, 1993; Quay, 1988a, b, 1993). Thus, responseinhibition deficits may not be specific to AD}HD, i.e.impaired response inhibition may not be the hallmark ofAD}HD.

According to Quay (1988a, b, 1993, 1997), bothAD}HD and CD are associated with response inhibitiondeficits. However, the dysfunction underlying poor re-sponse inhibition differs for the two disorders. Quay(1988a,b, 1993) has suggested that CD reflects an over-active BAS that dominates the BIS. According to thisview, the excessive BAS activity causes a strong tendencyto respond and interferes with the capability for responseinhibition. By contrast, poor response inhibition inAD}HDresults fromanunderactiveBIS (Quay, 1988a, b,1997).

Quay referred in particular to children with under-socialised aggressive conduct disorder (American Psy-chiatric Association, 1987), a group of children currentlyalluded to as CD–childhood-onset type (American Psy-chiatric Association, 1994). This type of CD generallyemerges prior to age 10 years. Given the age range ofchildren with CD in the present meta-analysis (6–12 yearsold), Quay’s hypothesis concerning CD is particularlyrelevant.

The existing data derived with the stop task enabled usto investigate both differences between CD and controlchildren and between AD}HD and CD children. The aimwas to determine whether response inhibition deficits areunique to children with AD}HD or whether these deficitsare also evident in children with CD. The stop task wouldbe particularly useful in clarifying the nature of AD}HD,if it is able to distinguish AD}HD children from childrenwith CD.

AD}HD­CD–AD}HD (4 ). This comparison wasdesigned to investigate whether children with comorbidAD}HD­CD differ from children with AD}HD only. Ithas been suggested that the combination of AD}HD andCD represents a distinct nosological entity (Biederman,Newcorn, & Sprich, 1991; Schachar & Tannock, 1995).Following Quay’s (1988a, b, 1993, 1997) model, onemight speculate that the AD}HD­CD group showsgreater deficiencies in the capability for response in-hibition due to a combination of underactivity in the BIS(resulting in poor response inhibition) and overactivity inthe BAS (which may cause an exaggerated tendency torespond). Thus, response inhibition deficits in childrenwith AD}HD­CD might be more pronounced than inchildren with AD}HD alone.

Anxiety disorder–controls (5 ). In contrast to AD}HDand CD, anxiety disorders have been associated withabnormally high levels of response inhibition. It has beensuggested by Quay (1988a, b) that an overactive BISunderlies anxiety disorders. Furthermore, several studies

have shown that a strong tendency for response inhibitionin children is a powerful predictor of later anxietydisorders (see for review, Biederman, Rosenbaum,Chaloff, & Kagan, 1995). Thus, children with anxiety dis-orders were predicted to demonstrate enhanced responseinhibition in comparison with controls.

Method

Description of the Studies

This review covers studies conducted between 1990 and 1997.Eight independent published and unpublished studies wereidentified that met the following inclusion criteria : (1) the studycontained one or more of the psychopathological groups ofinterest, and (2) the study included a comparison group ofcontrol children. All studies focused on children in the age range6 to 12 years. A brief description of study attributes is providedin Table 1. In some reports insufficient information was pro-vided to complete the meta-analysis. In these cases, authorswere contacted to obtain the missing information".

Seven studies contained a group of AD}HD children.Schachar et al. (1995) reported the results for three groups ofAD}HD children: a pervasive group, a school-only group, anda home-only group. Only the pervasive group was included inthe meta-analysis to avoid the problem of correlated results(Rosenthal, 1991) and to maximise the chance of finding groupdifferences (Luk, Leung, & Yuen, 1991; Schachar & Logan,1990; Schachar et al., 1995; Van der Meere, Wekking, &Sergeant, 1991).

Children with oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) or CDwere included in four of the studies. Five studies included acomorbid AD}HD­CD group. Finally, three studies includedchildren with anxiety disorders. Children with ODD or CDwere regarded as a single group and further dealt with under theheading CD. ODD is frequently seen as a milder form of CD,the two disorders are related to the same risk factors and formsof impairment, and ODD to some degree predicts the onset ofCD (Achenbach, 1993; American Psychiatric Association,1991; Lahey, Loeber, Frick, Quay, & Grimm, 1992; Loeber,Green, Kennan, & Lahey, 1995). A total of 456 childrenparticipated in the 8 studies.

Dependent Variables

The meta-analysis focused on the following four dependentvariables derived from the stop task (a detailed description ofthese measures is provided in the Appendix) :

(1) Mean reaction time (MRT), which measures the latencyof the processes involved in response execution. Since thismeta-analysis focused on abnormalities in responseinhibition, MRT was not the main focus of the presentstudy.

(2) The inhibition function (IF), which reflects the efficiencyof the inhibitory mechanism controlling for differencesin MRT. Most researchers take the slope of this IF(IF-slope) as an index of the subject’s capability forresponse inhibition. The flatter the IF-slope, the poorerthe capability for response inhibition (Logan, 1994).

According to the race model (see for review, Logan,1994; Logan & Cowan, 1984), the IF is determined by

"For two studies data on IF-slope and ZRFT-slope were notincluded in the meta-analysis. Jennings et al. (1997) did notcalculate within-group standard deviations for these twodependent measures. Part of the data from the Schachar andTannock (1995) study were not available.

Page 4: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

414 J. OOSTERLAAN et al.

Table 1Studies Included in Meta-analysis: Subject and Task Characteristics

Study Subjects Agea Subject selection Task characteristics

1. Schachar & 10 Controls 10.0 AD}HD diagnosis based on parent interview Stop taskLogan (1990)b 13 AD}HD 9.3 and}or teacher ratings. Other diagnoses Primary task: two-choice

9 CD}ODD 9.8 based on parent interview alone. Control reaction time task (letters X14 AD}HD­CD}ODD 9.3 children were free of any diagnosis. and O)13 Emotional disorderc 9.9 Diagnoses according to DSM III (-R) 25% stop trials, stop signals11 LDd 10.0 criteria. Criteria for LD diagnosis were at 500, 400, 300, 200, 100,

least average IQ and low reading attainment. 0 msec before MRTAll children had IQs & 80 and were free of 432 trials (21.6 min), 2 breaksmedication. ­96 practice trials

2. Daugherty, 15 Controls 11.0 Group assignment based on teacher ratings. See Study 1Quay, & 9 AD}HD 11.4 Psychopathological groups had elevatedRamos (1993) 8 CD 11.1 scores on relevant scale and low scores on

11 CD­AD}HD 11.7 other scales. Control children had low scores12 Anxiety-withdrawal 10.8 on all scales.

3. Aman, 22 Controls 12.1 Assignment to one of the groups based on Stop taskRoberts, & 22 AD}HDe 12.1 parent structured interview and parent Primary task: see Study 1Pennington questionnaires. AD}HD children showed 1}3 stop signal trials, stop(in press) favourable response to stimulants. Diagnoses signals 500, 350, 250,

according to DSM III-R criteria. Children 100 msec before MRTwith other psychiatric disorders and reading 192 trials (9.6 min)disability were excluded. All children had IQs ­96 practice trials& 80 and were free of medicationf.

4. Pliszka & 31 Controls 8.8 AD}HD diagnoses based on parent structured Stop taskBorcherding 26 AD}HD (or­ODD) 8.7 interview and teacher ratings. CD diagnoses Primary task: two-choice(1995) 8 AD}HD­CD 8.2 based on parent structured interview alone. reaction time task (red

17 AD}HD­overanxious 9.4 Diagnoses of overanxious disorder based on and green light)(or­ODD)d child structured interview. Absence of 25% stop trials, stop signals

18 psychiatric 9.5 psychiatric disorder in controls was assessed 500, 400, 300, 200, 100,controlsd with a variety of measures. 0 msec before MRT

432 trials (21.6 min)­48 practice trials

5. Schachar & 16 Controls 9.0 Diagnoses based on parent and}or teacher See Study 6Tannock 22 AD}HD 9.2 structured interview. Control children were(1995)g 5 CD 10.1 free of any diagnosis and learning problems.

18 AD}HD­CD 8.8 Diagnoses according to DSM III-R criteria.All children had IQs& 80 and % 130 andwere free of medication.

6. Schachar, 22 Controls 9.2 Diagnoses based on parent and}or Change taskTannock, 10 Home¯only AD}HDd 9.4 (depending on AD}HD subtype) teacher Primary task: see Study 1Marriott, & 9 School¯only AD}HDd 9.8 structured interview. Control children were 25% stop signals trials, stopLogan (1995)h 14 Pervasive AD}HD 8.7 free of any diagnosis and LD. AD}HD signals 500, 350, 200,

children had no additional diagnosis of ODD 50 msec before MRTor CD. Diagnoses according to DSM III-R 288 trials (14.4 min), 2 breakscriteria. All children had IQs & 80 and were ­a minimum of 72 practicefree of medication. trials

7. Oosterlaan & 17 Controls 8.7 Assignment to one of the psychopathological Stop taskSergeant 15 AD}HDi 9.3 groups based on parent, teacher and child Primary task: spatial(1996) 18 ODD}CD 9.3 ratings (agreement required between two compatible two-choice

20 Anxiety disorder 10.1 informants). Control children obtained low reaction time task (whitescores on all scales of all questionnaires. All squares)children had IQs& 80 and were free of 25% stop trials, stop signalsmedication. 500, 350, 200,

50 msec before MRT256 trials (12.8 min), 1 break­64 practice trials

Page 5: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

415RESPONSE INHIBITION IN CHILD PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Table 1 (cont.)

Study Subjects Agea Subject selection Task characteristics

8. Jennings, Van 26 Controls 9.8 AD}HD diagnosis based on parent structured Stop taskder Molen, 40 AD}HDd, j 9.7 interview and questionnaires completed by Primary task: simple reactionPelham, 25 AD}HD­ODD}CDj 9.1 parent and teacher. Diagnoses according to time task embedded in videoBrock, & DSM III-R criteria. Control children game format (stop lightHoza obtained low scores on parent and teacher changing from red to green)(1997) questionnaires. All children were free of 30% stop trials, stop signals

medication. 100 or 200 msec after gostimulus

Variable trial length,presentation of task stimulirelative to cardiac cycle andrespiratory phase

200 trials (40 min), 1 break­a minimum of 25 practicetrials

a Mean age in years.b Pathological groups could have additional diagnosis of emotional disorder or LD.c The term emotional disorder encompasses mainly anxiety disorders.d Groups not included in meta-analysis.e AD}HD group obtained high ratings of aggressive and delinquent behaviour on one of the parent questionnaires.f Subjects were tested twice, one week apart. AD}HD children were on methylphenidate in the first session and unmedicated in the

second session. The results of the second session were entered into the meta-analysis.g Lower reading attainment in AD}HD and AD}HD­CD groups than in CD and control group. Lower arithmetic attainment in

CD and AD}HD­CD groups than in AD}HD and control group.h Five AD}HD children met criteria for LD, two children met criteria for overanxious disorder.i Six children with AD}HD also met criteria for inclusion in ODD}CD group.j Twenty-five of the 40 AD}HD children carried a concurrent ODD diagnosis and three children met criteria for CD. Jennings

et al. reported both the results for the full sample and for the subsample of children with comorbid disorders. Three children weredropped from this subsample due to incomplete data. Only data of the subsample were included in the meta-analysis. For thissubsample, Jennings et al. did not specify the number of children meeting criteria for ODD and for CD. AD}HD group had lowerreading, spelling, and arithmetic attainment than control group. Nine AD}HD children met criteria for LD.

parameters of both the response execution process andthe inhibitory process. Using the race model, twomeasures can be derived to examine whether a deficiencyin the inhibitory process underlies a poor IF: stop signalreaction time, and the slope of the IF plotted as a functionof ZRFT.

(3) Stop signal reaction time (SSRT), which is an estimate ofthe latency of the inhibitory process. SSRT is one of theparameters that determines the probability of inhibitiongiven a stop signal. The slower the inhibitory process, theharder it becomes to inhibit the response to the primarytask (Logan, 1994).

(4) The slope of the IF plotted as a function of ZRFT(ZRFT-slope). In addition to a slow SSRT, poor responseinhibition could reflect two other deficits in the inhibitoryprocess. First, it might indicate that the inhibitory processwas triggered less often. Second, poor response inhibitioncould reflect greater variability in latency of the inhibitoryprocess (Logan, 1994). These two parameters of theinhibitory control process are reflected in the ZRFT-slope.

The following information was obtained for all studies andserved as the basis of the meta-analysis : (1) the number ofsubjects in each group, and for each of the dependent measures,(2) the group mean, and (3) the within-group standarddeviation. From these data, effect size estimations and theirdirection were calculated for all relevant group comparisonswithin each of the studies. The results of these analyses, in turn,were subjected to meta-analytic procedures (Mullen, 1989;Rosenthal, 1991). All analyses were conducted with the ad-vanced BASIC meta-analysis programme developed by Mullen

(1989). Calculations were weighted by sample size. The resultswere evaluated across studies in terms of their effect size and interms of their significance levels. Furthermore, diffuse tests wereconducted to assess the consistency across studies of bothestimated effect sizes and significance levels.

Possible Moderating Variables

Before presenting the results of the meta-analysis, severalpertinent study characteristics are described. Each of thesestudy characteristics is evaluated for its potential impact on thefindings obtained.

Subject selection. Studies differed sharply in the criteria andthe measures that were used to select the various groups (seeTable 1). This probably explains part of the heterogeneity in theresults across studies.

In five studies diagnoses were made using the third revisededition of the Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental dis-orders (DSM III-R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987).With the exception of the Daugherty et al. (1993) study,pathological groups were selected from clinic referred orotherwise treated children (e.g. placed in special educationalfacilities).

The presence or absence of psychopathological symptomswas assessed by means of reports from parents, teachers,children, or any combination of these informants. A variety ofmeasures was used across the studies, including standardisedrating scales, structured diagnostic interviews, or a combinationof both. In some studies, diagnoses were based on the report ofa single source. For example, in the Daugherty et al. (1993)study, teachers were the sole informants. In other studies, two or

Page 6: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

416 J. OOSTERLAAN et al.

three informants were used. In the majority of these studies,the reports by the different sources were not necessarily re-quired to converge. Since the degree of consistency betweendifferent informants’ reports is typically modest (Achenbach,McConaughy, & Howell, 1987), the use of multiple informantsresults in a greater chance of detecting children with actualdisorders.

Other studies used a more restrictive approach to combinethe information from different informants. In four studies,children received a diagnosis of AD}HD if both parent andteacher reports indicated its presence. Oosterlaan and Sergeant(1996) used parent and teacher ratings and a self-report anxietyquestionnaire to select AD}HD, CD, anxiety disorder andcontrol children. Children were assigned to one of the psycho-pathological groups only if the reports of two informantsconverged on this classification.

The distinction between pervasive and situational disordersseems important, at least for AD}HD. Several studies haveindicated that children with pervasive AD}HD show greatercognitive deficits than children with situational specific AD}HDsymptoms (e.g. Luk et al., 1991; Schachar & Logan, 1990;Schachar et al., 1995; Van der Meere et al., 1991).

Comorbidity. A large body of research has demonstratedthat AD}HD shows considerable overlap with a number ofother disorders, such as ODD and CD, mood disorders, anxietydisorders, and learning disabilities (Barkley, 1990; Biedermanet al., 1991; Semrud-Clikeman et al., 1992). Comorbidity mighthave influenced children’s performance on the stop task.

In all but two of the studies (Oosterlaan & Sergeant, 1996;Pliszka & Borcherding, 1995), AD}HD children were free ofcomorbid ODD or CD. In the Oosterlaan and Sergeant study, 6out of the 15 children showed associated ‘‘aggressive ordelinquent ’’ symptoms. The AD}HD sample studied by Pliszkaand Borcherding contained children with AD}HD alone andAD}HD with an additional ODD diagnosis. However, theinvestigators did not specify the corresponding proportions.

In four studies, children were examined for learning dis-abilities, although in most of these studies the assessment waslimited to reading disabilities (Aman et al., in press ; Jenningset al., 1997; Schachar & Logan, 1990; Schachar et al., 1995). Inonly one study were children with concurrent reading dis-abilities excluded (Aman et al., in press). The remaining fourstudies did not report on the possible presence of learningdisabilities.

Recent findings highlight the necessity to control for asso-ciated learning disabilities. A recent study by Tannock andMarriott (1992) suggested that learning disabilities, and notAD}HD, were associated with poor response inhibition and aslower inhibitory process. Furthermore, Daugherty et al. (1993)have reported a moderate positive correlation between readingachievement and IF-slope.

Conflicting predictions were made regarding the capabilityfor response inhibition in children with anxiety disorders andin children with externalising disorders (AD}HD, CD, andAD}HD­CD). Consequently, the distinction between thesetwo disorders seems important. In four of the eight studies,children with externalising disorders were free of anxietydisorders (Aman et al., in press ; Daugherty et al., 1993;Oosterlaan & Sergeant, 1996; Pliszka & Borcherding, 1995). Inthe Schachar and Logan (1990) study, an additional diagnosis ofanxiety disorder was allowed in all psychopathological groups.However, exact figures were not presented. The AD}HD sampleof Schachar et al. (1995) included two children with concurrentoveranxious disorder. In the remaining two studies, childrenwere not assessed for the possible presence of associated anxietydisorders (Jennings et al., 1997; Schachar & Tannock, 1995).

Gender and age. In five studies, boys served as subjects,whereas in the remaining three studies both sexes were included.Gender, however, does not seem to influence the child’s

performance on the stop task (Daugherty et al., 1993; Pliszka &Borcherding, 1995). In none of the studies were age differencesnoted between the groups. This suggests that age is not aconfounding variable in any of the studies. The mean age ofcontrol children ranged from 8.7 years in the Oosterlaan andSergeant (1996) study to 12.1 years in the Aman et al. (in press)study.

Intellectual functioning. IQ was assessed in six studies. Infive of these studies, children with below-average intellectualfunctioning (IQ less than 80) were excluded. The remaining twostudies did not provide information on the children’s IQ(Daugherty et al., 1993; Pliszka & Borcherding, 1995). Sinceseveral studies have demonstrated that IQ is not related tochildren’s performance on the stop task (Kindlon et al., 1995;Oosterlaan & Sergeant, 1996; Schachar & Logan, 1990;Schachar et al., 1995), it seems unlikely that group differences inintellectual functioning underlie any of the findings.

Medication. In six studies, children were free of medicationor discontinued medication before being tested with the stoptask. Daugherty et al. (1993) and Pliszka and Borcherding(1995) did not report on the possible use of medication.

Format of the stop task. In six studies the stop task wasused, whereas two studies (Schachar & Tannock, 1995;Schachar et al., 1995) utilised a modification of the stop task,known as the change task (De Jong, Coles, & Logan, 1995;Logan & Burkell, 1986). The stimuli in the change task areidentical to those in the stop task. However, the two tasks differwith respect to the demand exerted by the stop signal. In bothtasks, the stop signal instructs subjects to inhibit their responseto the primary task. In the change task, the stop signal, inaddition, requires subjects to immediately re-engage in anotherresponse, i.e. to execute the so-called change response. Severalstudies have indicated that the response execution and in-hibitory processes seem to function essentially the same in boththe stop task and the change task. However, the change taskappears to exert higher cognitive processing demands than thestop task, since it carries the additional demand of re-engagingin another response (De Jong et al., 1995; Logan & Burkell,1986).

In five studies an identical two-choice reaction time taskserved as the primary task. In this task, children responded tothe letters X and O by pressing one of the two correspondingbuttons on a response box. Children responded with a left- anda right-hand finger, usually their index fingers. In those studiesin which the change task was used, children responded with twoseparate fingers of the left hand. Modifications of this primarytask were used in two other studies. In the study by Pliszka andBorcherding (1995), children responded to a red and green light.In the Oosterlaan and Sergeant (1996) study, a spatial com-patible two-choice reaction time task served as the primary task.Jennings et al. (1977) used a simple reaction time taskembedded in a video game format. In this task, a green trafficlight signalled children to move a mouse-controlled ice-creamcart to a target area on the computer screen.

With the exception of the study by Jennings et al. (1997),the timing of stimuli was similar for all studies. Trials startedwith the presentation of a warning signal of 500 msec. Im-mediately thereafter the primary task stimulus was displayedfor 1000 msec. An intertrial interval of 1500 msec was used. Inall studies primary task stimuli were presented visually. Jenningset al. investigated the relationship between response inhibitionand heartbeat timing. In this study, stimuli were presentedrelative to the child’s cardiac cycle and respiratory phase. Notethat the presentation rate of stimuli in information processingtasks has been shown to play a crucial role in the performanceof AD}HD children (see Van der Meere, 1996, for review).

Jennings et al. (1997) presented stop signals relative to theonset of the primary task stimulus, whereas in the other studiesstop signals were presented relative to the child’s MRT (see

Page 7: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

417RESPONSE INHIBITION IN CHILD PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Appendix). In all studies, the longest stop signal interval was500 msec. The shortest stop signal interval varied from 0 msecto 100 msec. In six of the studies, stop signals were presented on25% of the trials. In the studies by Jennings et al. (1997) andAman et al. (1995), the percentage of stop trials was somewhathigher : 30% and 33.3%, respectively.

Stop signals were presented in the auditory modality. In theJennings et al. (1997) study, the stop signal resembled thesound of a car horn. In the other studies, a tone was presentedby the computer or through earphones (Oosterlaan & Sergeant,1996). Where reported, tones were 1 kHz in pitch and 100 msecin duration.

Studies with young adults have demonstrated that the stoptask is reasonably robust for differences in the format of thetask. First, a number of studies have shown that measuresderived from the stop task are not affected by differences in thenature of the primary task. Findings with the stop task do notseem to vary as a function of task difficulty, nor are the resultsdifferent for tasks that require continuous or discrete responses(see for reviews, Logan, 1994; Logan & Cowan, 1984). Second,the stop task has been found robust for changes in stop signalprobability. Typically, the primary task stimulus occurs onevery trial and the stop signal occurs on 25% of the trials.Logan (1981) and Logan and Burkell (1986) studied the effect ofvarying stop signal probability from 10% to 80%. Measuresderived from the stop task were not much affected when the stopsignal probability varied between 10% and 50%. Third, andfinally, Logan (1994; Logan & Cowan, 1984) reviewed studiesusing auditory and}or visual stop signals and concluded thatperformance on the stop task is not affected by stop signalmodality. In summary, the stop task has been found to bereasonably robust for differences in the nature of the primarytask, for differences in stop signal probability, and for dif-ferences in stop signal modality. Therefore, it seems unlikelythat differences in the format of the stop task underlie any of thefindings.

Between-studies differences were noted in the nature andamount of practice before starting the task. Furthermore,studies varied in the number of trials and the number of breakswithin the task. Estimates of time-on-task ranged from 9.6 minto 21.6 min (since in the majority of studies the length of thetask was not specified, an estimate of time-on-task was obtainedby multiplying the number of trials with the trial length). In theJennings et al. (1997) study, the length of trials varied. In thisstudy, four blocks were administered, each 10 min in duration.

Details on the instructions given to the children variedconsiderably between studies. Instructions are considered im-portant in the stop task, since the task involves a delicatebalance between fast and accurate responding, on the one hand,and inhibition of responses, on the other hand. To enhance thechild’s performance, Jennings et al. (1997) used feedback,monetary reward, and monetary loss. Where reported, theexperimenter remained with the child during the task. However,none of the studies provided information about the role of theexperimenter during the task. Most studies failed to reportwhether the experimenter was aware of the child’s groupassignment and the purpose of the study. Where reported,subjects were tested individually.

Results

As indicated previously, this meta-analysis concen-trates on five group comparisons, namely: (1) AD}HD–controls, (2) CD–controls, (3) AD}HD–CD, (4)AD}HD­CD–AD}HD, and (5) anxiety disorder–controls. Meta-analytic results for each of the dependentmeasures are presented in Tables 2 through 5. The tablesshow, for each of the five comparisons: (a) the total

number of subjects across studies for the contrastinggroups, (b) the number of relevant studies, (c) theweighted mean for both groups, (d) the combinedestimated effect size in terms of Cohen’s d (1988), and (e)the meta-analytic Z with the respective significance level.In agreement with Cohen’s guidelines, effect sizes of .20,.50, and .80 were used as thresholds to define small,medium, and large effects, respectively.

MRT. Meta-analytic results for the latency of res-ponding are presented in Table 2. MRTs of AD}HD andcontrol children were compared in seven studies. Themean effect size (d¯ .49) was close to Cohen’s standardof .50 for a medium effect, indicating 32% nonoverlapbetween the MRT distributions of the two groups.Differences between the other four pairs of groups weresmall and not significant. Note that the results of allgroup comparisons were consistent across studies, bothin terms of effect sizes and significance levels. Thisindicates that the results of the studies agree both on themagnitude of the group differences and on the significanceof these differences.

Note also that a comparison of weighted mean MRTsof AD}HD and CD children suggests slower MRTs forAD}HD children. The combined effect size and signifi-cance level, however, indicated that the two groups didnot differ in MRT. This finding could be explained asfollows.Across studies, substantial differenceswere notedin MRTs. For example, MRTs of control children variedfrom about 350 msec (e.g. Oosterlaan & Sergeant, 1996)to 900 msec (e.g. Schachar & Logan, 1990). Thesedifferences reflect variations in the cognitive load of thetasks employed. Since a greater proportion of CD thanAD}HD children was tested with a stop task withrelatively low cognitive demands (as indexed by the MRTof the control group), CD children showed a substantiallyshorter weighted mean MRT than AD}HD children.

IF-slope. Table 3 displays the results for IF-slope.Across six studies, AD}HD children had flatter IF-slopesthan controls, suggesting impairments in response in-hibition. The combined effect size of d¯ .94 exceedsCohen’s threshold for a large effect and corresponds to53% nonoverlap between the IF-slope distributions ofthe two groups. Diffuse tests indicated that effect sizeswere consistent across studies, whereas heterogeneitywas noted for the significance levels. This heterogeneityin significance levels was attributable mainly to theDaugherty et al. (1993) study, in which AD}HD andcontrol children showed highly comparable IF-slopes.In all other studies, AD}HD children showed signifi-cantly flatter IF-slopes than controls.

Similar findings were obtained for the comparisonbetween CD and control children. Averaged across threestudies, IF-slopes were flatter in CD children than incontrols. A medium combined effect size of d¯ .56 wasfound, which translates into 36% nonoverlap betweenthe IF-slope distributions of the two groups. Results wereuniform across studies, both in terms of effect sizes andsignificance levels.

In three studies AD}HD and CD children werecompared. Although AD}HD children had somewhatflatter IF-slopes than children with CD, this differencewas small and not significant. Thus, no support wasobtained for the hypothesis that AD}HD children would

Page 8: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

418 J. OOSTERLAAN et al.

Table 2Go Response Mean Reaction Time (MRT; in Msec)

Sample size Weighted mean

Group comparison Group 1 Group 2 Studies Group 1 Group 2 d Combined Z

AD}HD–Controls 121 133 7 772.6 702.5 0.49 3.67*CD–Controls 40 58 4 612.6 642.0 0.29 1.59AD}HD–CD 59 40 4 726.1 612.8 0.36 1.73AD}HD­CD–AD}HD 51 70 4 867.2 847.5 0.16 0.87Anxious–Controls 45 42 3 623.0 607.7 0.25 1.23

Diffuse comparisons were conducted to assess the homogeneity of effect sizes and significancelevels. All group comparisons yielded homogeneous results.

* p! .001.

Table 3Inhibition Function Slope (IF-slope)

Sample size Weighted mean

Group comparison Group 1 Group 2 Studies Group 1 Group 2 d Combined Z

AD}HD–Controls 99 117 6 10.2 14.8 0.94a 6.36**b

CD–Controls 35 42 3 12.6 16.0 0.56a 2.35*a

AD}HD–CD 37 35 3 11.5 12.8 0.25a 0.73a

AD}HD­CD–AD}HD 33 48 3 10.0 8.6 0.17b 0.73b

Anxious–Controls 45 42 3 14.4 15.9 0.34a 1.41a

Diffuse comparisons were conducted to assess the homogeneity of effect sizes and significancelevels : a Homogeneous effect ; b Heterogeneous effect.

* p! .05; ** p! .001.

Table 4Stop Signal Reaction Time (SSRT; in Msec)

Sample size Weighted mean

Group comparison Group 1 Group 2 Studies Group 1 Group 2 d Combined Z

AD}HD–Controls 121 133 7 349.4 246.4 0.64a 4.97**a

CD–Controls 40 58 4 265.7 248.0 0.51b 2.64*b

AD}HD–CD 40 59 4 364.7 265.7 0.07b 0.37b

AD}HD­CD–AD}HD 51 70 4 323.6 361.8 0.13a 0.78a

Anxious–Controls 45 42 3 231.3 207.6 0.20a 0.88a

Diffuse comparisons were conducted to assess the homogeneity of effect sizes and significancelevels : a Homogeneous effect ; b Heterogeneous effect.

* p! .01; ** p! .0001.

be more deficient in response inhibition than childrenwith CD. Again, findings were consistent across studies,both in terms of effect sizes and significance levels.

Meta-analysis revealed no differences between IF-slopes of children with comorbid AD}HD­CD andchildren with AD}HD alone. However, these results needto be interpreted with caution given the inconsistency inboth effect sizes and significance levels. In fact, two stud-ies demonstrated no significant differences between bothgroups (Daugherty et al., 1993; Pliszka & Borcherding,1995), whereas one study indicated that children withAD}HD alone had flatter IF-slopes than comorbidAD}HD­CD children (Schachar & Logan, 1990).

In three studies children with anxiety disorders werecompared with controls. Contrary to the predictions,children with anxiety disorders did not exhibit steeper IF-slopes than controls. Note also that these meta-analyticresults were consistent across studies.

SSRT. As indicated earlier, the speed of the in-hibitory process is one of the parameters that determinesthe IF. In other words, poor capability for responseinhibition may reflect a relatively slow SSRT (Logan,1994). The meta-analytic results for SSRT appear inTable 4. Across seven studies, AD}HD children were onaverage 103 msec slower than control children.A mediumcombined effect size of d¯ .64 was obtained, whichtranslates into 40% nonoverlap between the two groupdistributions. Both effect sizes and significance levels werehomogeneous across studies.

Similar results were noted for the comparison betweenCD and control children. Across four studies, SSRT wason average about 18 msec slower in CD children than incontrols. The average effect size of d¯ .51 equals Cohen’sthreshold for a medium effect. This effect size indicates34% nonoverlap between the SSRT distributions of thetwo groups. Diffuse tests indicated heterogeneity in terms

Page 9: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

419RESPONSE INHIBITION IN CHILD PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Table 5Inhibition Function Slope Plotted as a Function of ZRFT (ZRFT–slope)

Sample size Weighted mean

Group comparison Group 1 Group 2 Studies Group 1 Group 2 d Combined Z

AD}HD–Controls 99 117 6 21.0 23.8 0.19 1.31CD–Controls 35 42 3 19.3 20.7 0.04 0.20AD}HD–CD 35 37 3 19.3 17.7 0.07 0.04AD}HD­CD–AD}HD 33 48 3 22.1 20.6 0.12 0.52Anxious–Controls 42 42 3 21.1 20.7 0.03 0.26

ZRFT¯ a z score which represents the relative finishing time of the inhibitory process and theresponse execution process in SD units (a discussion of this measure is provided in the Appendix).Diffuse comparisons of effect sizes and significance levels indicated homogeneous results for allgroup comparisons. All Z scores nonsignificant.

of effect sizes and significance levels. In fact, three studiessupported the direction of the meta-analytic result, andone study revealed faster SSRTs in children with CD(Schachar & Tannock, 1995). However, only Oosterlaanand Sergeant (1996) found that CD children had sig-nificantly slower SSRTs than controls.

Although the speed of the inhibitory process was onaverage almost 100 msec slower in AD}HD children thanin CD children, the combined significance level and effectsize indicated no differences between the two groups. Thisfinding stems from the sharply conflicting findings ob-tained in each of the four studies. Two studies revealedsubstantially faster SSRTs for CD children. In contrast,the other two studies demonstrated that CD children hadsomewhat slower inhibitory processes. Given these in-consistencies, the meta-analytic findings need to beinterpreted with considerable caution.

Comorbid AD}HD­CD children did not differ fromchildren with AD}HD alone. Furthermore, meta-analy-sis revealed no differences in SSRT between anxious andcontrol children. Note also that these findings werehomogeneous both in terms of effect sizes and in terms ofsignificance levels.

ZRFT-slope. Table 5 presents the meta-analyticresults for ZRFT-slope. Combined effect sizes andsignificance levels indicated that there were no differencesbetween the contrasted groups. All groups showedcomparable ZRFT-slopes, indicating that group diff-erences in the capability for response inhibition wereneither related to the probability of triggering theinhibitory process, nor to differences in variability inlatency of the inhibitory process (Logan, 1994). Note alsothat effect sizes and significance levels were homogeneous.

Summary of Results

Despite important methodological differences betweenstudies, the present meta-analysis revealed fairly con-sistent results. In this section, we briefly summarise ourfindings.

First, the major finding was that, compared withcontrols, both AD}HD and CD children demonstratedflatter inhibition functions, indicating poor responseinhibition. In both groups, poor response inhibition wasrelated to a slow inhibitory process, although the findingswere inconsistent for CD children. In other words,

impairments in response inhibition were not unique tochildren with AD}HD. Moreover, AD}HD and CDchildren did not differ in the degree of their impairments.No differences were noted between these two groups interms of the inhibition function, nor in terms of theinhibitory process. It should be noted, however, that thefindings for the speed of the inhibitory process differedsharply between studies.

Second,AD}HDchildrenwith andwithoutCDneitherdiffered in their ability to inhibit responses, nor in termsof the inhibitory process. Thus, AD}HD­CD childrendid not show greater impairments than children with onlyAD}HD. However, findings were inconsistent betweenstudies for the slope of the inhibition function.

Third, no evidence was found for the prediction ofenhanced response inhibition in anxious children. In fact,anxious and control children could not be differentiatedon any of the dependent measures.

Finally, the latency of responding on the primary taskwas slower for AD}HD children than for controls. Therewere no differences in the speed of responding betweenthe other groups.

Obviously our findings are limited by the restrictednumber of studies currently available. This holds inparticular for the comparisons between CD and controlchildren, between AD}HD and CD children, and be-tween AD}HD­CD and AD}HD children. Some of themeta-analytic results for these group comparisons wereheterogeneous. Clearly, these inconsistencies call formore research. Furthermore, we need to mention that ourfindings for AD}HD were limited by the fact that in twostudies some children with this disorder also showedODD or CD symptomatology. With these limitations inmind, we now discuss the results of the meta-analysis.

Discussion

This paper presents the results of a meta-analysis ofstudies with the stop task. The aim was to investigatewhether data gathered with the stop task supports theresponse inhibition deficit hypothesis for AD}HD(Barkley, 1994, 1997; Douglas, 1988, 1989; Newman &Wallace, 1993; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Quay,1988a, b, 1997; Wender, 1972). Furthermore, we investi-gated whether impaired response inhibition is uniquelyrelated to AD}HD (Barkley, 1994, 1997; Douglas, 1988,1989; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Wender, 1972) or

Page 10: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

420 J. OOSTERLAAN et al.

whether deficits in response inhibition are also evident inother psychopathological disorders. Finally, we ex-amined the suggestion that anxiety disorders are asso-ciated with abnormally high levels of response inhibition(Quay, 1988a, b).

Consistent and robust evidence was found for aresponse inhibition deficit in AD}HD. Relative to con-trols, AD}HD children demonstrated flatter inhibitionfunctions, indicating poor response inhibition. Impairedresponse inhibition was related to a slow inhibitoryprocess. Our findings support recent theoretical notionsof AD}HD in which poor response inhibition is sug-gested to lie at the heart of this disorder (Barkley, 1994,1997; Douglas, 1988, 1989; Newman & Wallace, 1993;Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Quay, 1988a, b, 1997;Wender, 1972).

However, particularly important is that the meta-analysis did not support the notion that response in-hibition deficits are unique to AD}HD (Barkley, 1994,1997; Douglas, 1988, 1989; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996;Wender, 1972). Similar to AD}HD children, CD childrenhad flatter inhibition functions and slower inhibitoryprocesses compared with controls. Furthermore, res-ponse inhibition deficits did not distinguish children withAD}HD from children with CD, nor from children withcomorbid AD}HD­CD; the three groups showed simi-lar deficits in terms of their inhibition functions and thelatency of the inhibitory process. These collective findingssuggest that response inhibition deficits characterisechildrenwith behaviour labelled as undercontrolled or ex-ternalising (Achenbach, 1991; Achenbach & Edelbrock,1978). This conclusion confirms the predictions derivedfrom Quay’s psychobiological model of child psycho-pathology (Quay, 1988a, b, 1993, 1997).

The finding that both AD}HD and CD childrenevidence response inhibition deficits is not surprising.There is considerable overlap in the symptomatology ofthese two disorders (American Psychiatric Association,1994; Barkley, 1990; Biederman et al., 1991; Nottelman& Jensen, 1995; Russo & Beidel, 1994). In fact, it iscontroversial whether AD}HD and CD are distinctclusters of abnormal behaviour (Fergusson, Horwood, &Lloyd, 1991; Hinshaw, 1987; Hinshaw, Lahey, & Hart,1993). Furthermore, AD}HD has been found to bestrongly predictive for the later development of CD(McGee, Williams, & Feehan, 1992; Taylor, Chadwick,Heptinstall, & Dankaerts, 1996). Consequently, it seemsplausible that the two disorders share common deficits, aswas found in the present research.

Although AD}HD and CD share the impairment inresponse inhibition, this impairment does not necessarilyreflect the same dysfunction. Poor response inhibition inAD}HD children may be part of a general impairment inexecutive functions, which in turn may be attributable toa frontal lobe dysfunction (Lezak, 1983; Pennington &Ozonoff, 1996; Torgesen, 1994). Recently, such anaccount of AD}HD has been advanced by Barkley (1994,1997) and by Pennington and Ozonoff (1996). Indeed,AD}HD children have been found to demonstrateimpairments on a variety of tests purported to measurefrontal lobe functioning (see for review, Barkley et al.,1992; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; see also Grodzinsky& Diamond, 1992; Shue & Douglas, 1992).

A similar explanation for response inhibition deficits inCD children seems less likely. Although there is supportfor the presence of deficits in executive functioning in CD,Pennington and Ozonoff (1996) pointed out that thesefindings may be attributed to the presence of comorbidAD}HD. Recent studies that controlled for the presenceof AD}HD yield conflicting findings. Some studies haveshown that CD is indeed associated with impairedexecutive functions (Se! guin, Pihl, Harden, Tremblay, &Boulerice, 1995; White et al., 1994). Other studies,however, failed to do so (Linz, Hooper, Hynd, Isaac, &Gibson, 1990). The present meta-analysis shows thatchildren with CD, but without AD}HD, exhibit responseinhibition deficits. Since response inhibition is regardedas one of the key executive functions, our findings add tothe research indicating executive functions deficits in CD.

Besides an explanation in terms of a deficit in executivefunctioning, poor response inhibition in AD}HD chil-dren may also be explained in other ways, for example asreflecting a non-optimal activation state (see, for review,Sergeant & Van der Meere, 1990, 1991; Van der Meere,1996). In the cognitive energetical model of informationprocessing (Sanders, 1983, 1990), an optimal activationis required to maintain adequate motor processing(Sternberg, 1969a, b). That is, activation is concernedwith the readiness to respond (Pribram & McGuiness,1975; Sanders, 1983, 1990). A non-optimal activationstate causes impaired motor processing, which resultsin difficulties for both the execution and inhibition ofresponses. Thus, the non-optimal activation hypothesiscan account for our finding that AD}HD children had aslower response execution process, as well as for ourfinding of a response inhibition deficit in these children.

Extensive research supports the hypothesis that AD}HD involves a non-optimal activation state (see, forreview, Sergeant & Van der Meere, 1990, 1991; Van derMeere, 1996). Activation is influenced by the presentationrate of stimuli in a task (Sanders, 1983, 1990). A highpresentation rate of stimuli causes high levels of ac-tivation; a slow event rate causes low levels of activation.A recent study by Van der Meere, Stemerdink, andGunning (1995) demonstrated that AD}HD childrenwere less able to inhibit inappropriate responding thancontrols with fast and slow presentation rates of stimuli.However, AD}HD children did not differ from theircontrol peers in a medium stimulus presentation rate.

Particularly compelling for the non-optimal activationstate hypothesis is the research with methylphenidate.Stimulants have been found to influence the activationlevel (Sanders, 1983, 1990; Sergeant & Van der Meere,1991). Interestingly, research by Tannock and associateswith the stop task has shown that methylphenidateameliorates response inhibition deficits in AD}HD chil-dren (Tannock et al., 1989; Tannock, Schachar, & Logan,1995). Clearly, these findings warrant more research toinvestigate the relationship between response inhibitionand activation state.

Poor response inhibition in AD}HD children may alsobe understood as a motivational deficit. That is to say,these children do not expend the effort necessary toachieve and maintain optimal performance. In supportof this view, it has been found that the performance ofAD}HD children relies more strongly on the presence of

Page 11: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

421RESPONSE INHIBITION IN CHILD PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

contingencies than the performance of control children(Douglas, 1989; Haenlein & Caul, 1987; Newman &Wallace, 1993; Quay, 1988a, b, 1997; Wender, 1972). Inseveral explanatory models of AD}HD, deficits in re-sponse inhibition are implicitly linked to motivationalfactors. Douglas (1989), for example, proposed thatAD}HD encompasses a defective capability for inhi-bition and an unusually strong inclination to seekimmediate rewards. As discussed earlier, Quay (1988a, b,1993, 1997) explained the capability for response in-hibition in terms of the activity of the BIS and the BAS.These two brain systems, in turn, are activated by signalsof punishment and signals of reward, respectively.

Finally, AD}HD children’s lack of response inhibitionhas been explained as due to delay aversion. On the basisof several studies (Sonuga-Barke, Taylor, & Heptinstall,1992; Sonuga-Barke, Taylor, Sembi, & Smith, 1992;Sonuga-Barke, Williams, Hall, & Saxton, 1996), Sonuga-Barke (1995) set forth a model that explains the behaviourof AD}HD children in terms of a pervasive aversion ofdelay. According to this model, disinhibition in childrenwith AD}HD reflects an attempt to reduce delay anddoes not arise from the inability to inhibit as such. Thisexplanation, however, is unlikely to account for thepresent findings. In the stop paradigm used in the studiesdescribed here, successful inhibition or failing to inhibitdid not influence the time that children had to wait beforethe next trial commenced, since the intertrial interval wasfixed. Thus, poor response inhibition could not haveserved to reduce delay.

An important issue raised by our findings is whetherpoor response inhibition in AD}HD children represents astable deficit or a maturational lag in the development.Recently, Barkley (1994, 1997) has suggested that AD}HD is associated with such a delay in the attainment ofthe capability for response inhibition. Some support forthis explanation comes from the Jennings et al. (1997)study (see Table 1). In that study, 10–12-year-oldAD}HD boys showed better response inhibition thantheir 8–9-year-old counterparts. In contrast, no suchdifferences were found for a group of control boys.

Two recent studies (Oosterlaan & Sergeant, 1997;Schachar & Logan, 1990) suggest that the capability forresponse inhibition and the underlying inhibitory processdevelop early in childhood. However, both studies wererestricted to children aged 8 years and over. At presentdata for younger children are lacking. Thus, it is currentlynot possible to estimate the degree of delay in AD}HDchildren. A proper test of the maturational lag hypothesiswould be a longitudinal study of AD}HD children andcontrol children. It seems particularly important toinvestigate to what extent early emerging impairments inresponse inhibition predict the onset of externalisingbehaviour disorders later in childhood.

Meta-analytic findings did not support the predictionthat anxiety disorders are characterised by enhancedlevels of response inhibition, given their hypotheticallyoveractive BIS (Quay, 1988a, b). We found that anxiousand control children differed neither in terms of theirinhibition functions, nor in terms of the inhibitory processunderlying response inhibition. Our failure to demon-strate strong response inhibition in anxious childrenmight be explained in several ways. First, it is possible

that anxious children did not differ from controls becausethe performance of both groups represents the perform-ance that is maximally feasible. Second, it is possible thatanxious children only show enhanced response inhibitionin situations that trigger their hypothetically overactiveBIS. That is to say, enhanced response inhibition willonly be evident when anxious children are presented withsignals of impending punishment (Gray, 1987). A thirdexplanation was offered by Newman and Wallace (1993).They argued that increased activity in the BIS mightresult in heightened activity in the Nonspecific ArousalSystem (NAS). The NAS mediates the speed and force ofresponses (Gray, 1987). In this way, the overactive BISnot only leads to increased inhibitory control, but also tofaster and more vigorous responding. Increased NASactivity is thought to interfere with the capability toinhibit responses. Finally, the performance of anxiouschildrenmay have been hampered by worrisome thoughtsassociated with anxiety (Eysenck & Calvo, 1992). Ac-cording to this view, worrying (e.g. concern over taskperformance) pre-empts some of the processing andstorage resources of the working memory system. As aresult, less processing and storage resources are availableand cognitive performance is hampered.

Although the stop task has several advantages overother measures of response inhibition, the paradigm hasits limitations. First, since the stop task is purported tomeasure response inhibition, an important issue iswhether children with externalising disorders are alsoassociated with impairments in the ability to inhibitcognitive processes. Second, the stop task is purported tomeasure one form of response inhibition, i.e. the abilityto inhibit an ongoing response. It does not assess otherforms of response inhibition (Barkley, 1997) such asinhibiting interference or the ability to inhibit a responseover a protracted period of time (Halperin et al., 1994;Masters & Binger, 1978; Sonuga-Barke, 1995). It remainsto be seen whether the current findings generalise to theselatter definitions of response inhibition.

In summary, this meta-analysis provides consistentand robust support for contemporary accounts of AD}HD in terms of a response inhibition deficit. However,the results do not support the notion that this deficitis uniquely related to AD}HD (Barkley, 1994, 1997;Douglas, 1988, 1989; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996;Wender, 1972). Poor response inhibition seems to charac-terise children with externalising disorders. This findingfits well within Quay’s model (1988a, b, 1993, 1997), butposes a challenge for models of AD}HD in which deficitsin response inhibition are thought to be unique to thisdisorder (Barkley, 1994, 1997; Douglas, 1988, 1989;Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Wender, 1972).

Future studies should be directed to answering whetherdifferent processes underlie the similar findings forAD}HD and CD children or whether the similarity infindings points to a common background dysfunction.Furthermore, the task for future research is to uncoverthe conditions that enhance and challenge the inhibitorycontrol process. This type of research with the stop taskwill undoubtedly contribute to deepen our knowledge ofthe mechanisms underlying disinhibition in child psycho-pathology. Illustrative for this type of research are thestudies investigating the effects of methylphenidate on

Page 12: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

422 J. OOSTERLAAN et al.

response inhibition in children with AD}HD (Tannocket al., 1989, 1995). Finally, more attention needs to bedevoted to the impact of comorbid disorders, such aslearning disabilities.

Acknowledgements—We are most grateful to Dr Christine J.Aman, Dr Tim Daugherty, Dr J. Richard Jennings, Dr StevenR. Pliszka, Dr Russell J. Schachar, and Dr Rosemary Tannockfor providing us with all the necessary information forconducting the meta-analysis. We would like to thank DrKrystyna Rojahn for help with the meta-analysis. Finally, theauthors would like to express their appreciation to the reviewersfor their helpful comments on an earlier version of thismanuscript.

References

References marked with an asterisk indicate studies includedin the meta-analysis.Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL}

4–18, YSR, and TRF profiles. Burlington, VT: University ofVermont.

Achenbach, T. M. (1993). Taxonomy and comorbidity ofconduct problems: Evidence from empirically based ap-proaches. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 51–64.

Achenbach, T. M., & Edelbrock, C. S. (1978). The classificationof child psychopathology: A review and analysis of empiricalefforts. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 1275–1301.

Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., & Howell, C. T.(1987). Child}adolescent behavioral and emotional prob-lems: Implications of cross-informant correlations for situ-ational specificity. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 213–232.

*Aman, C. J., Roberts, R. J., & Pennington, B. F. (in press). Aneuropsychological examination of the underlying deficit inADHD: The frontal lobe vs. right parietal lobe theories.Developmental Psychology.

American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and stat-istical manual of mental disorders (3rd edn., rev.). Washing-ton, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1991). DSM-IV optionsbook: Work in progress. Washington, DC: Author.

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and stat-istical manual of mental disorders (4th edn.). Washington,DC: Author.

Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder:A handbook for diagnosis and treatment. New York: TheGuilford Press.

Barkley, R. A. (1994). Impaired delayed responding: A unifiedtheory of attention deficit disorder. In D. K. Routh (Ed.),Disruptive behaviour disorders in childhood (pp. 11–57). NewYork: Plenum Press.

Barkley, R. A. (1996). Linkages between attention and execu-tive functions. In G. R. Lyon & N. A. Krasnegor (Eds.),Attention, memory, and executive function (pp. 307–326).Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained at-tention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifyingtheory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94.

Barkley, R. A., Grodzinsky, G., & DuPaul, G. (1992). Frontallobe functions in attention deficit disorder with and withouthyperactivity : A review and research report. Journal ofAbnormal Child Psychology, 20, 163–188.

Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Sprich, S. (1991). Comorbidity ofattention deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, de-pressive, anxiety, and other disorders. American Journal ofPsychiatry, 148, 564–577.

Biederman, J., Rosenbaum, J. F., Chaloff, J., & Kagan, J.(1995). Behavioral inhibition as a risk factor for anxietydisorders. In J. S. March (Ed.), Anxiety disorders in childrenand adolescents (pp. 61–81). New York: The Guilford Press.

Campbell, S. B., Douglas, V. I., & Morgenstern, G. (1971).Cognitive styles in hyperactive children and the effect ofmethylphenidate. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,12, 55–67.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioralsciences (2nd edn.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Corkum, P., & Siegel, L. S. (1993). Is the continuous per-formance task a valuable research tool for use with childrenwith attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder? Journal of ChildPsychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1217–1239.

Daugherty, T. K., & Quay, H. C. (1991). Response persev-eration and delayed responding in childhood behaviordisorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32,453–461.

*Daugherty, T. K., Quay, H. C., & Ramos, L. (1993). Responseperseveration, inhibitory control, and central dopaminergicactivity in childhood behaviour disorders. Journal of GeneticPsychology, 154, 177–188.

De Jong, R., Coles, M. G. H., & Logan, G. D. (1995). Strategiesand mechanisms in nonselective and selective inhibitorymotor control. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21,498–511.

Denckla, M. B. (1994). Measurement of executive function. InG. R. Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment oflearning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp.117–142). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

Douglas, V. I. (1988). Cognitive deficits in children withattention deficit disorder with hyperactivity. In L. M.Bloomingdale & J. A. Sergeant (Eds.), Attention deficit dis-order: Criteria, cognition, intervention (pp. 65–82). Oxford:Pergamon Press.

Douglas, V. I. (1989). Can Skinnerian theory explain attentiondeficit disorder? A reply to Barkley. In L. M. Bloomingdale& J. Swanson (Eds.), Attention deficit disorder: Currentconcepts and emerging trends in attentional and behavioraldisorders of childhood (pp. 235–254). Elmsford, NY: Perga-mon Press.

DuPaul, G. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., Shelton, T. L., Guevre-mont, D. C., & Metevia, L. (1992). Multimethod assessmentof attention deficit hyperactivity disorder : The diagnosticutility of clinic-based tests. Journal of Clinical Child Psy-chology, 21, 394–402.

Eysenck, M. W., & Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and per-formance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition andEmotion, 6, 409–434.

Farrington, D. P. (1993). Motivations for conduct disorder anddelinquency. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 225–241.

Fergusson, D. M., Horwood, L. J., & Lloyd, M. (1991).Confirmatory factor models of attention deficit and conductdisorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32,257–274.

Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (2nd edn.).Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Grodzinsky, G. M., & Diamond, R. (1992). Frontal lobefunctioning in boys with attention-deficit hyperactivity dis-order. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 427–445.

Haenlein, M., & Caul, W. F. (1987). Attention deficit disorderwith hyperactivity : A specific hypothesis of reward dys-function. Journal of the American Academy of Child andAdolescent Psychiatry, 26, 356–362.

Halperin, J. M., McKay, K. E., Matier, K., & Sharma, V.(1994). Attention, response inhibition and activity level inchildren: Developmental neuropsychological perspectives. InM. G. Tramontana & S. R. Hooper (Eds.), Advances in child

Page 13: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

423RESPONSE INHIBITION IN CHILD PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

neuropsychology, Vol. 2 (pp. 1–54). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Hinshaw, S. P. (1987). On the distinction between attentionaldeficits}hyperactivity and conduct problems}aggression inchild psychopathology. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 443–463.

Hinshaw, S. P., Lahey, B. B., & Hart, E. L. (1993). Issues oftaxonomy and comorbidity in the development of conductdisorder. Development and Psychopathology, 5, 31–49.

Iaboni, F., Douglas, V. I., & Baker, A. G. (1995). Effects ofreward and response costs on inhibition in ADHD children.Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 104, 232–240.

*Jennings, J. R., Van der Molen, M. W., Pelham, W., Brock,K., & Hoza, B. (in press). Inhibition in boys with attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder as indexed by heart rate change.Developmental Psychology, 33, 308–318.

Kindlon, D., Mezzacappa, E., & Earls, F. (1995). Psychometricproperties of impulsivity measures : Temporal stability, val-idity and factor structure. Journal of Child Psychology andPsychiatry, 36, 645–661.

Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Frick, P. J., Quay, H. C., & Grimm,J. (1992). Oppositional defiant and conduct disorders : Issuesto be resolved for DSM-IV. Journal of the American Academyof Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 539–546.

Lezak, M. D. (1983). Neuropsychological assessment (2nd edn.).New York: Oxford University Press.

Linz, T. D., Hooper, S. R., Hynd, G. W., Isaac, W., & Gibson,L. J. (1990). Frontal lobe functioning in conduct disorderedjuveniles : Preliminary findings. Archives of Clinical Neuro-psychology, 5, 411–416.

Loeber, R., Green, S. M., Keenan, K., & Lahey, B. B. (1995).Which boys will fare worse? Early predictors of the onset ofconduct disorder in a six-year longitudinal study. Journal ofthe American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,34, 499–509.

Logan, G. D. (1981). Attention, automaticity, and the ability tostop a speeded choice response. In J. Long & A. D. Baddeley(Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 205–222). Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum.

Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought andaction: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D.Dagenbach & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes inattention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego,CA: Academic Press.

Logan, G. D., & Burkell, J. (1986). Dependence and inde-pendence in responding to double stimulation: A comparisonof stop, change, and dual-task paradigms. Journal of Ex-perimental Psychology, 12, 549–563.

Logan, G. D., & Cowan, W. B. (1984). On the ability to inhibitthought and action: A theory of an act of control. Psycho-logical Review, 91, 295–327.

Logan, G. D., Cowan, W. B., & Davis, K. A. (1984). On theability to inhibit thought and action: A model and a method.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 10, 276–291.

Luk, S. L., Leung, P. W. L., & Yuen, J. (1991). Clinic obser-vations in the assessment of pervasiveness of childhoodhyperactivity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 32,833–850.

Masters, J. C., & Binger, C. G. (1978). Interrupting the flow ofbehavior : The stability and development of children’s in-itiation and maintenance of compliant response inhibition.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 24, 229–242.

McClure, F. D., & Gordon, M. (1984). Performance of dis-turbed hyperactive and nonhyperactive children on anobjective measure of hyperactivity. Journal of AbnormalChild Psychology, 12, 561–572.

McGee, R., Williams, S., & Feehan, M. (1992). Attention deficitand age of onset of problem behaviors. Journal of AbnormalChild Psychology, 20, 487–502.

Milich, R., Hartung, C. M., Martin, C. A., & Haigler, E. D.(1994). Behavioural disinhibition and underlying processes inadolescents with disruptive disorders. In D. K. Routh (Ed.),Disruptive behaviour disorders in childhood (pp. 109–138).New York: Plenum Press.

Mullen, B. (1989). Advanced BASIC meta-analysis. Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum.

Newman, J. P., & Wallace, J. F. (1993). Diverse pathways todeficient self-regulation: Implications for disinhibitory psy-chopathology in children. Clinical Psychology Review, 13,699–720.

Nottelman, E. D., & Jensen, P. S. (1995). Comorbidity of dis-orders in children and adolescents : Developmental perspec-tives. In T. H. Ollendick & R. J. Prinz (Eds.), Advances inclinical child psychology, Vol. 17 (pp. 109–155). New York:Plenum Press.

*Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1996). Inhibition in ADHD,anxious and aggressive children: A biologically based modelof child psychology. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,24, 19–36.

Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (1997). Response inhibition andresponse re-engagement: A developmental investigation inchildren 8–12 years old. Manuscript submitted for pub-lication.

Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functionsand developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psy-chology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

*Pliszka, S. R., & Borcherding, S. H. (1995). The stop signaltask in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder andanxiety. Manuscript submitted for publication.

Pribam, K. H., & McGuiness, D. (1975). Arousal, activation,and effort in the control of attention. Psychological Review,82, 116–149.

Quay, H. C. (1988a). Attention deficit disorder and thebehavioral inhibition system: The relevance of theneuropsychological theory of Jeffrey A. Gray. In L. M.Bloomingdale & J. A. Sergeant (Eds.), Attention deficitdisorder: Criteria, cognition, intervention (pp. 117–125).Oxford: Pergamon Press.

Quay, H. C. (1988b). The behavioral reward and inhi-bition system in childhood behaviour disorder. In L. M.Bloomingdale (Ed.), Attention deficit disorder, Vol. 3(pp. 176–185). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

Quay, H. C. (1993). The psychology of undersocialized ag-gressive conduct disorder : A theoretical perspective. De-velopment and Psychopathology, 5, 165–180.

Quay, H. C. (1997). Inhibition and attention deficit hyper-activity disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 25,7–13.

Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for socialresearch (rev. edn.). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Russo, M. F., & Beidel, D. C. (1994). Comorbidity of childhoodanxiety and externalizing disorders : Prevalence, associatedcharacteristics, and validations issues. Clinical PsychologyReview, 14, 199–221.

Sanders, A. F. (1983). Towards a model of stress and per-formance. Acta Psychologica, 53, 61–97.

Sanders, A. F. (1990). Issues and trends in the debate ondiscrete vs. continuous processing of information. ActaPsychologica, 74, 123–167.

*Schachar, R. J., & Logan, G. D. (1990). Impulsivity andinhibitory control in normal development and childhoodpsychopathology. Developmental Psychology, 26, 710–720.

*Schachar, R., & Tannock, R. (1995). Test of four hypothesesfor the comorbidity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorderand conduct disorder. Journal of the American Academy ofChild and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 639–648.

Page 14: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

424 J. OOSTERLAAN et al.

*Schachar, R., Tannock, R., Marriott, M., & Logan, G. (1995).Deficient inhibitory control in attention deficit hyperactivitydisorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 23, 411–437.

Schweitzer, J. B., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1995). Self-control inboys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder : Effectsof added stimulation and time. Journal of Child Psychologyand Psychiatry, 36, 671–686.

Se! guin, J. R., Pihl, R. O., Harden, P. W., Tremblay, R. E., &Boulerice, B. (1995). Cognitive and neuropsychologicalcharacteristics of physically aggressive boys. Journal ofAbnormal Psychology, 104, 614–625.

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Biederman, J., Sprich-Buckminster, S.,Lehman, B. K., Faraone, S. V., & Norman, D. (1992).Comorbidity between ADDH and learning disability : Areview and report in a clinically referred sample. Journal ofthe American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,31, 439–448.

Sergeant, J. A., & Van der Meere, J. J. (1990). Convergingapproaches on localizing the hyperactivity deficit. In B. B.Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical childpsychology, Vol. 13 (pp. 207–245). New York: Plenum Press.

Sergeant, J. A., & Van der Meere, J. J. (1991). Ritalineffects and information processing in hyperactivity. In L. L.Greenhill & B. B. Osman (Eds.), Ritalin: Theory and patientmanagement (pp. 1–13). New York: Mary Ann Liebert.

Sergeant, J. A., & Van der Meere, J. J. (1994). Towards anempirical child psychopathology. In D. K. Routh (Ed.),Disruptive behaviour disorders in childhood (pp. 59–85). NewYork: Plenum Press.

Shue, K. L., & Douglas, V. I. (1992). Attention deficit hyper-activity disorder and the frontal lobe syndrome. Brain andCognition, 20, 104–124.

Solanto, M. V. (1990). The effects of reinforcement andresponse-cost on a delayed response task in children withattention deficit hyperactivity disorder : A research note.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 803–808.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (1995). Disambiguating inhibitory dys-function in childhood hyperactivity. In J. A. Sergeant (Ed.),European approaches to hyperkinetic disorder (pp. 209–223).Zurich: Fotorotar.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Taylor, E., & Heptinstall, E. (1992).Hyperactivity and delay aversion—II: The effect of self versusexternally imposed stimulus presentation periods on memory.Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 399–409.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Taylor, E., Sembi, S., & Smith, J.(1992). Hyperactivity and delay aversion—I: The effect ofdelay on choice. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,33, 387–398.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Williams, E., Hall, M., & Saxton, T.(1996). Hyperactivity and delay aversion—III : The effect oncognitive style of imposing delay after errors. Journal of ChildPsychology and Psychiatry, 37, 189–194.

Appendix

The Stop Task and the Race Model

The stop task requires fast and accurate execution of areaction time task, denoted as the primary task. Occasionally,an auditory stop signal is presented, which requires the subjectto inhibit the response to the primary task. Stop signals arepresented at different stop signal delays. That is, the delay isvaried between the onset of the primary task stimulus and theonset of the stop signal. The longer the delay, the more difficultit becomes to inhibit a response. By contrast, the shorter thedelay, the easier it becomes to inhibit a response. Usually,

Sternberg, S. (1969a). The discovery of processing stages:Extensions of Donders’ method. In W. G. Koster (Ed.),Attention and performance, Vol. 2 (pp. 276–315). Amsterdam:North-Holland.

Sternberg, S. (1969b). Memory-scanning: Mental processesrevealed by reaction time experiments. American Scientist,57, 421–457.

Tannock, R., & Marriott, M. (1992). Learning disabilities :Converging evidence of deficits in inhibitory processes.Proceedings of the 39th annual meeting of the AmericanAcademy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 8, 89.

Tannock, R., Schachar, R. J., Carr, R. P., Chajczyk, D., &Logan, G. D. (1989). Effects of methylphenidate on in-hibitory control in hyperactive children. Journal of AbnormalChild Psychology, 17, 473–491.

Tannock, R., Schachar, R., & Logan, G. (1995). Methyl-phenidate and cognitive flexibility : Dissociated dose effects inhyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,23, 235–266.

Taylor, E., Chadwick, O., Heptinstall, E., & Dankaerts, M.(1996). Hyperactivity and conduct problems as risk factorsfor adolescent development. Journal of the American Acad-emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 1213–1226.

Torgesen, J. K. (1994). Issues in the assessment of executivefunction: An information-processing perspective. In G. R.Lyon (Ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learningdisabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 143–162).Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

Van der Meere, J. J. (1996). The role of attention. In S. T.Sandberg (Ed.), Monographs on child and adolescent psy-chiatry: Hyperactivity disorders of childhood (pp. 109–146).New York: Cambridge University Press.

Van der Meere, J. J., Stemerdink, N., & Gunning, B. (1995).Effects of presentation rate of stimuli on response inhibitionin ADHD children with and without tics. Perceptual andMotor Skills, 81, 259–262.

Van der Meere, J. J., Wekking, E., & Sergeant, J. A. (1991).Sustained attention and pervasive hyperactivity. Journal ofChild Psychology and Psychiatry, 32, 275–284.

Wender, P. H. (1972). The minimal brain dysfunction syndromein children. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 155,55–71.

Weyandt, L. L., & Grant, W. W. (1994). Executive functions inschool-aged children: Potential efficacy of tasks in discrimi-nating clinical groups. Developmental Neuropsychology, 10,27–38.

White, J. L., Moffit, T. E., Caspi, A., Bartusch, D. J., Needles,D. J., & Stouthamer-Loeber, M. (1994). Measuring impul-sivity and examining its relationship to delinquency. Journalof Abnormal Psychology, 103, 192–205.

Manuscript accepted 11 July 1997

delays are chosen such that the longest delay will produce aprobability of inhibition close to 0, whereas the shortest delaywill yield a probability of inhibition close to 1.

The stop task is based on a well-established theory ofinhibition, known as the race model (see for review, Logan,1994; Logan & Cowan, 1984). According to this model,response inhibition depends on a race between, on the one hand,the response execution process (or go process) that responds tothe primary task stimulus (or go signal) and, on the other hand,the inhibitory process (or stop process) that responds to the stopsignal. The process that finishes first determines performance.

Page 15: Response Inhibition in AD/HD, CD, Comorbid AD/HD+CD, Anxious

425RESPONSE INHIBITION IN CHILD PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Thus, if the go process finishes before the stop process, theresponse is executed. If the stop process finishes before the goprocess, the response to the primary task stimulus is inhibited.

The faster the go process, the less likely it is that the stopprocess wins the race with a stop signal presented at a givendelay. Consequently, the faster the go process, the lower theprobability of inhibition. By contrast, the faster the stopprocess, the more likely it is that the stop process wins the race,and the higher the probability of inhibition. Many researcherscompensate for differences in go signal reaction time betweensubjects, presenting stop signals relative to mean go signalreaction time. That is, stop signals are presented at differentintervals before the subject’s expected response, i.e. at differentintervals defined as MRT minus delay, where MRT is defined asthe mean reaction time calculated across correctly executedresponses on go trials. We now discuss the three main dependentvariables in the stop task: (1) the inhibition function, (2) thestop signal reaction time, and (3) the ZRFT-slope.

Inhibition Function

The inhibition function (IF) is generated by plotting theprobability of inhibition against mean go signal reaction timeminus stop signal delay (MRT®delay). This function reflectsthe efficiency of the inhibitory mechanism controlling fordifferences in mean go signal reaction time. Most researcherstake the slope of this IF (IF-slope) as an index of the subject’scapability for response inhibition. This slope is calculated byfitting a regression line to the individual IF. The flatter theslope, the poorer the capability for response inhibition. Thesteeper the slope, the better the capability for responseinhibition.

If groups of subjects are found to differ in their IF, thequestion arises whether this result reflects differences in the stopprocess or differences in the go process. Two methods areavailable to investigate possible differences in the inhibitoryprocess.

According to the race model (see for review, Logan, 1994;Logan & Cowan, 1984), the IF is determined by: (1) the speedof the go process and variability in the speed of the go process,(2) the speed of the stop process and variability in the speed ofthe stop process, and (3) the probability of triggering the stopprocess. Thus, a shallow IF could reflect either parameters ofthe response execution process (i.e. 1) or it could reflect adeficiency in the stop process (i.e. 2 and 3). Using the racemodel, two measures can be derived to examine whether adeficiency in the stop process underlies a poor IF. The firstmeasure is stop signal reaction time (SSRT). SSRT is anestimate of the latency of the stop process. The second measureis the slope of the IF plotted as a function of ZRFT (ZRFT-slope). ZRFT corrects for all parameters of the responseexecution process and for SSRT. Differences in the ZRFT-slopereflect differences in the probability of triggering the stopprocess or differences in the variability of the speed of the stopprocess. We now describe each of these two measures in detail.

SSRT: The Latency of the Stop Process

The latency of the stop process is not observable, but can beestimated by using the race model. Three methods are availableto estimate SSRT (Logan, 1994). We describe the mostcommonly used method, in which the latency of the stop processis assumed to be constant.

The race model assumes that the stop and the go processesoperate independently of one another. Therefore, the dis-tribution of reaction times on go trials can be seen as thedistribution of latencies of the go process on stop trials. SinceSSRT is assumed to be constant, SSRT can be seen as a point onthe time axis of this distribution. At a given stop signal delay, all

responses to the right of this point are inhibited because the stopprocess finishes before the go process. Responses to the left ofthis point are not inhibited; the go process finishes before thestop process. The proportion of trials that were inhibited isequal to the probability of inhibition, whereas the proportion oftrials that were not inhibited is equal to 1 minus the probabilityof inhibition.

In theory, we integrate the distribution of go signal reactiontimes from zero to a point in time at which the integral equalsthe probability of responding given a stop signal (i.e. 1 minusthe probability of inhibition). We treat that point as an estimateof the time at which the stop process finished. This time isdefined relative to the onset of the go signal (because we use thedistribution of go signal reaction times to define it), thus wesubtract out stop signal delay to estimate SSRT.

In practice, SSRT is calculated as follows: first, reactiontimes on go trials are rank ordered on a time axis. Second, wepick the nth reaction time, where n is defined by the product ofthe number of reaction times in the distribution and theprobability of responding given a stop signal (or 1 minus theprobability of inhibition). For example, if there were 100reaction times in the distribution and the probability ofresponding given a stop signal was .3, the nth reaction timewould be the 30th in the rank-ordered distribution. The nthreaction time is an estimate of the time at which the stopprocess runs to completion, relative to the onset of the primarytask stimulus. Third, we subtract stop signal delay from the nthreaction time and estimate SSRT. For example, if the nthreaction time was 545 msec and the stop signal delay was200 msec, SSRT would be 345 msec. SSRT is calculated foreach stop signal delay and then averaged.

ZRFT-slope: The Probability of Triggering theStop Process and Variability in the Speed of StopProcess

In addition to a slow stop process (SSRT), two otherdeficiencies in the stop process may underlie poor responseinhibition: (1) a low probability of triggering the stop process,and (2) high variability in the latency of the stop process. Toinvestigate whether these two parameters of the stop processexplain differences in the IF, a so-called ZRFT transformationis applied to the IF (Logan, 1994). The ZRFT transformationcorrects for differences in MRT, for go signal reaction timevariability, and for SSRT. Specifically, the probability ofinhibition is plotted as a function of a z score that represents therelative finishing time of the go process and the stop process instandard deviation units, using the standard deviation ofreaction times on the primary task to define these units. ZRFTis obtained with the following formula: ZRFT¯ (MRT®stopsignal delay®SSRT)}standard deviation of reaction times onthe primary task. The slope of the IF plotted against ZRFT isknown as the ZRFT-slope.

If differences in the IF disappear after correction for ZRFT,this indicates that differences were completely accounted for bydifferences in MRT, SSRT, and variability in go signal reactiontime. That is, differences in the IF were neither related to theprobability of triggering the stop process, nor to differences invariability in the latency of the stop process. However, this doesnot compromise the results for the latency of the stop process(SSRT). Thus, differences in SSRT do not necessarily gotogether with differences in ZRFT-slope. In contrast, if dif-ferences remain after correction for ZRFT, this indicatesdifferences in the probability of triggering the stop process or inSSRT variability. Specifically, a relatively shallow ZRFT-slopeindicates that the stop process was triggered less often, or thatthere was greater variability in latency of the stop process. It isnot possible to discriminate between the latter two deficienciesin the stop process.