predicting asian international students

Upload: hijazensis

Post on 06-Apr-2018

220 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    1/16

    International Journal of Intercultural Relations

    29 (2005) 561576

    Predicting Asian international students

    sociocultural adjustment: A test of two

    mediation models

    Andrew Li, Michael B. Gasser

    Department of Psychology, University of Northern Iowa, Cedar Falls, IA, USA

    Abstract

    Integrating a number of theoretical perspectives, this paper examined predictors of Asian

    international students sociocultural adjustment. A total of 117 students (aged 1846 years)from 17 Asian countries and regions completed questionnaires about their sociocultural

    adjustment, contact with the hosts, ethnic identity, and cross-cultural self-efficacy. As

    hypothesized, contact with the hosts partially mediated the effect of cross-cultural self-efficacy

    on sociocultural adjustment. The hypothesis that contact with the hosts would mediate the

    effect of ethnic identity on sociocultural adjustment was not supported.

    r 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Keywords: Sociocultural adjustment; Ethnic identity; Self-efficacy; Cross-cultural contact

    1. Introduction

    International mobility has become the hallmark of the 21st century as the world

    increasingly becomes a global village. Riding this trend of globalization, a growing

    number of international students have crossed their national boundaries to seek

    educational experiences in other cultures (Hechanova-Alampay, Beehr, Christian-

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    www.elsevier.com/locate/ijintrel

    0147-1767/$ - see front matter r 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2005.06.003

    Corresponding author. Department of Management and Policy, Eller College of Management,

    P.O. Box 210108, University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721-0108, USA.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Li).

    http://www.elsevier.com/locate/ijintrelhttp://www.elsevier.com/locate/ijintrel
  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    2/16

    sen, & Van-Horn, 2002; Lin & Yi, 1997; Sam, 2001). In the United States, the

    number of international students attending higher education has increased

    dramatically since the end of World War II. According to the 20002001 Open

    Doors Annual Report (2001), the total enrollment of international students hasalready exceeded 540,000, accounting for 3.9% of the total college student

    population.

    Adjusting to a new culture can be a challenging and stressful experience. Rigorous

    academic demands along with the challenges to adjust to a new culture may put

    international students at a greater risk than students in general. Recently, the

    adjustment difficulties experienced by international students have received increasing

    scholarly attention (Anderson & Myer, 1985; Hayes & Lin, 1994). In a

    comprehensive review of sojourners research, Church (1982) suggested that in

    addition to problems similar to what domestic students may have, international

    students also experience difficulties that are elicited by the new cultural experiences.

    Surveys of international student adjustment in the new culture have indicated that

    they expressed apprehension in their language proficiency and academic perfor-

    mance during their sojourns (Hayes & Lin, 1994; Kagan & Cohen, 1990; Ying &

    Liese, 1994). In addition, international students also reported feeling depressed,

    anxious, and lonely due to the loss of their social support network (Sandhu &

    Asrabadi, 1994; Yang & Clum, 1995).

    In light of the adjustment difficulties reported by international student sojourners,

    researchers have attempted to investigate the nature of their adjustment and

    variables that predict their effective adjustment. One recent development in theseefforts is the psychological and sociocultural adjustment model (Searle & Ward,

    1990). According to this model, sojourners adjustment can be conceptualized as two

    distinctive, but intertwining, dimensions: psychological adjustment and sociocultural

    adjustment. Psychological adjustment denotes the feelings of well-being and

    satisfaction and sociocultural adjustment denotes the ability to fit in and to

    negotiate interactive aspects of the new culture (Searle & Ward, 1990, p. 450).

    Although these two dimensions are closely related, they should be understood in

    different theoretical frameworks. Psychological adjustment is best understood within

    a stress and coping framework. Based on this framework, psychological adjustment

    is best predicted by personality factors, amount of social support, contact with fellownationals and hosts, life changes, and attitudes towards the hosts. Sociocultural

    adjustment, however, is understood based on the social learning model (Befus, 1988;

    Furnham & Bochner, 1982). According to the social learning model, sociocultural

    adjustment is influenced by cross-cultural contact, cultural distance, cross-cultural

    training, previous cross-cultural experiences, and length of residence in the new

    culture. The psychological and sociocultural adjustment model (Searle & Ward,

    1990) has received support in a number of empirical studies (Ward & Chang, 1997;

    Ward & Kennedy, 1992, 1993a,b, 1994, 1999, 2001; Ward, Okura, Kennedy, &

    Kojima, 1998).

    In the present study, we examined factors that influenced international studentssociocultural adjustment. Furnham and Bochner (1986) suggested that social

    adjustment entails the abilities of expressing attitude, feelings, and emotion,

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576562

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    3/16

    adopting the appropriate proxemic posture, understanding the gaze patterns of the

    people they are interacting with, carrying out ritualized routines such as greetings,

    leave taking, self-disclosure, making or refusing requests, and asserting themselves

    (pp. 1415). Sojourners who are socioculturally adjusted are well prepared to copewith the challenges in their cross-cultural experiences and to attain the goals of their

    sojourn. Searle and Ward (1990) suggest that the process of adjusting to a new

    culture socially and culturally entails learning and applying new cultural knowledge;

    therefore, a comprehensive model of sociocultural adjustment should be cast in a

    social learning-social cognition framework wherein variables that enhance cultural

    learning, promote behavioral and social competency, and facilitate a cross-cultural

    perspective should be included.

    Based on a social learning-social cognition framework, we propose that

    sojourners ethnic identity, cross-cultural self-efficacy, and contact with the hosts

    are important variables related to sociocultural adjustment. As proposed by Tajfel

    (1978), and Tajfel and Turner (1986) in their discussion of social identity theory,

    people who are more strongly identified with their in-group are less likely to initiate

    and maintain interaction with members of the out-group. In the context of

    international students in the US, this suggests that students who hold a greater sense

    of ethnic identity will be less likely to interact with the host country nationals. This

    reduced contact may hamper their sociocultural adjustment. Similarly, Banduras

    (1986, 1992, 1997) social learning theory places great importance on self-efficacy as

    an antecedent of many human behaviors. In the context of international students in

    the US, students with greater cross-cultural self-efficacy should be more comfortableand more likely to engage in contact with the host-culture nationals, thus enhancing

    their sociocultural adjustment (Fan & Mak, 1998). Central to the relationship

    between these antecedents and sociocultural adjustment is the amount of contact

    with the hosts. As noted by Church (1982), numerous studies have attested to the

    importance of contact in improving cross-cultural adjustment. Given the central

    focus of contact with the hosts in the conceptualization of sociocultural adjustment,

    international students who have more host contact should experience better

    adjustment.

    Contact with the hosts, ethnic identity, and cross-cultural self-efficacy will be

    discussed in more details in the next section. In addition, we develop two mediationmodels that integrate these variables. The proposed models posit that contact with

    the hosts mediates the effects of two independent variables (i.e., ethnic identity and

    cross-cultural self-efficacy) on international students sociocultural adjustment.

    2. Antecedents of sociocultural adjustment

    2.1. Cross-cultural contact

    One of the most robust findings in the cross-cultural adjustment literature is thatpositive contact between sojourners and host nationals is critical to their effective

    adjustment in the new culture (Church, 1982). The theoretical underpinnings of these

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576 563

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    4/16

    findings were based on the contact hypothesis typically examined in the inter-group

    relation literatures. According to the contact hypothesis (Allport, 1954; Amir, 1969),

    contact among different social groups may enhance mutual understanding and

    reduce inter-group stereotypes and discriminations. Amir (1969) posited that byhaving an opportunity to know the views and beliefs held by the out-group members,

    in-group members are able to appreciate and understand their way of life. Inter-

    group contact may also improve cross-group perspective taking and promote a

    positive re-evaluation of the out-group (Pettigrew, 1997).

    This theoretical perspective obviously applies to the cross-cultural context. Stening

    (1979) argued that intercultural relations are usually stained by stereotypes,

    prejudices, and ethnocentric perspective of the parties involved. In order to remove

    these barriers to healthy intercultural communication, it is important that

    individuals from one culture have first-hand experiences about others from another

    culture. Intercultural interaction affords the opportunities for sojourners to re-

    evaluate their erroneous views of others from a different cultural background and

    identify the similarities of belief and values that are believed to increase attraction

    and liking (Masson & Verkuyten, 1993). In addition, sojourners social interactions

    with the hosts may enable them to gain cultural knowledge, establish a local support

    network, and increase their language proficiency (Church, 1982; Toyokawa &

    Toyokawa, 2002).

    Although a causal linkage cannot be inferred, the positive effects derived from

    contact with host nationals upon sojourners sociocultural adjustment have been

    documented in several correlation studies. Selltiz and Cook (1962) found thatinternational students who have close host friends expressed more positive feelings

    towards Americans and were better adjusted than their peers who did not have cross-

    cultural contact. Similarly, Sewell and Davidson (1956) found that international

    students from Scandinavian countries who engaged in social interactions with the

    hosts were less likely to express frustrations, more likely to improve their English

    speaking capacities, and report overall satisfactions with their sojourns than those

    without such host contact. Using a longitudinal design, Ying and Liese (1994) found

    that international students from Taiwan who interacted with Americans reported

    better adjustment relative to those who simply formed their social networks among

    co-nationals. Given these findings, we expect that cross-cultural contact will be apredictor of sociocultural adjustment.

    2.2. Ethnic identity

    Church (1982) suggested that sojourners often have to cope with their membership

    conflicts between trying to maintain cultural ties with their ethnic groups and

    behaving in a way that is consistent with the host cultures expectations. Much of the

    research on membership conflicts has been conducted within the framework of social

    identity theory (Tajfel, 1978; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). According to social identity

    theory, individuals strive to maintain positive self-esteem that is derived from theirgroup membership. As a result, individuals are motivated to see their in-group

    members in the most favorable light to maintain positive self-esteem. The motivation

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576564

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    5/16

    to maintain positive self-esteem and social identity also leads them to favor in-group

    members in comparisons between groups.

    Based on social identity theory, sojourners ethnic identity (a self-concept derived

    from the ethnic membership that one belongs to) may predict their willingness toengage in contact with members from other ethnic groups, including the hosts

    (Piontkowski, Florack, Hoelker, & Obdrzalek, 2000). Those individuals who have a

    strong ethnic identity may be more likely to distance themselves from the out-group

    members in order to preserve their group distinctiveness conducive for group

    comparisons. Moreover, individuals who are strongly identified with their ethnic

    group may be more likely to experience threats from the out-group than those who

    are indifferent to their group memberships, thus influencing their willingness to

    engage in cross-cultural contact (Stephan, Stephan, & Gudykunst, 1999). In a study

    of Dutch adolescents attitudes towards ethnic minorities (Masson & Verkuyten,

    1993), it was found that ethnic identity was strongly related to in-group preference;

    whereas contact with minorities was significantly associated with less prejudice

    towards minorities and in-group preference. Moreover, ethnic identity was

    negatively related to the amount of contact between Dutch adolescents and

    minorities.

    Sojourners ethnic identity is also related to their sociocultural adjustment. The

    four acculturation strategies model proposed by Berry and colleagues (Berry &

    Annis, 1974; Berry, Kim, Power, Young, & Bujaki, 1989) provides a useful

    theoretical framework to understand the relation between sojourners ethnic identity

    and their sociocultural adjustment. Simply put, the model posited that sojournersacculturation attitudes revolve around two dimensions: (a) maintaining their ethnic

    identity and (b) maintaining ties with host nationals and act in the local way. A

    taxonomy of four acculturation strategies was developed based on these two

    dimensions: separation, integration, assimilation, and marginalization. Individuals

    who employ the separation strategy maintain their original ethnic identity and avoid

    cross-cultural contact. Individuals who employ the integration strategy try to achieve

    a balance between maintaining their ethnic identity and establishing contact with the

    hosts. Individuals who take an assimilation approach sever ties with the original

    culture and adopt the lifestyle of the new culture. Lastly, marginalization is

    characteristic of individuals who lose ties with both their original culture and the newculture.

    Scores of studies have been conducted to investigate the differentiating effects of

    the four strategies on cross-cultural adjustment (e.g., Ward & Kennedy, 1994; Ward

    & Rana-Deuba, 1999). Generally, empirical research has demonstrated that

    sojourners social adjustment is facilitated by a stronger association with the host

    culture and a dissociation with their own ethnic community (Fugita & OBrien, 1985;

    Yao, 1985). Sojourners who are identified with the host culture may establish their

    local network, receive social support from the locals, and learn the local cultures.

    Eshel and Rosenthal-Sokolov (2000) examined the acculturation attitudes of

    Russian students in Israel and their sociocultural adjustment. Their studydemonstrated that successful sociocultural adjustment entailed the relinquishment

    of the values and behaviors of their ethnic culture and the adoption of an identity

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576 565

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    6/16

    that conformed to the expectations of the new culture. Similarly, Ward and Rana-

    Deubas (1999) investigation of acculturation strategies of foreign students in Nepal

    showed that stronger identification with the hosts was associated with less social

    difficulties such as fulfilling social functions and understanding local languages. Allof these studies suggest that ethnic identity should be negatively related to cross-

    cultural contact and sociocultural adjustment.

    2.3. Cross-cultural self-efficacy

    Empirical research has also found that cross-cultural contact is related to

    sojourners cross-cultural self-efficacy (Black & Gregersen, 1991; Harrison, Chad-

    wick, & Scales, 1996). Self-efficacy is the belief that one can perform certain social

    functions to produce a desirable outcome (Bandura, 1986, 1992, 1997). Self-efficacy

    is especially important in the cross-cultural interactions. Church (1982) suggested

    that there are many barriers to effective interactions between sojourners and host

    nationals, such as verbal and non-verbal language, cultural ignorance, and fear of

    rejection, which renders cross-cultural interactions more distressing and anxiety-

    provoking than within-culture interactions. As a result, sojourners may attempt to

    cope with their social anxiety by avoiding contact with the hosts ( Fan & Mak, 1998).

    Fan and Mak (1998) contended that inhibitions of cross-cultural contact are a sign

    of low social self-efficacy. In the cross-cultural context, high self-efficacy denotes a

    positive evaluation that one can successfully perform certain social functions in

    another culture and achieve desirable outcomes. Bandura (1986, 1992, 1997), in hissocial cognitive theory, posited that self-efficacy influences human behavior through

    four processes: cognitive, affective, motivational, and selection. Taken into cross-

    cultural contexts, social cognitive theory predicts that individuals high in cross-

    cultural self-efficacy may be more likely to foresee successes in cross-cultural contact

    and anticipate positive experiences than their less efficacious peers (cognitive

    process). Similarly, individuals with high cross-cultural self-efficacy may be less

    anxiety stricken than those with low self-efficacy in cross-cultural interactions

    (affective process). Moreover, cross-culturally self-efficacious individuals may be

    more motivated to engage in cross-cultural contact and to reap the ensuing benefits

    (motivational process). In addition, individuals with high cross-cultural self-efficacymay choose to engage in contact with the hosts even though cross-cultural

    communication places a premium on cultural knowledge, language, and tolerance

    for ambiguities (selection process).

    Self-efficacy for cross-cultural interactions has also been found to be related to

    sojourners sociocultural adjustment. Tsang (2001) suggested that self-efficacious

    individuals may be more actively seeking new cultural experiences. As a result, they

    may receive constant feedback regarding their novel behavior, which enhances their

    cultural knowledge and reduces uncertainties in future cross-cultural interactions.

    Consistent with this contention, Harrison et al. (1996) found that more cross-

    culturally self-efficacious American expatriates reported better adjustment in Europecompared with their less self-efficacious peers. Similarly, using a longitudinal design,

    Hechanova-Alampay et al. (2002) found that international students self-efficacy was

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576566

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    7/16

    significantly related to their adjustment in the new culture, such that students high in

    cross-cultural self-efficacy reported better adjustment and less strains than those low

    in self-efficacy. The findings of these studies suggest that self-efficacy should be

    positively related to cross-cultural contact and sociocultural adjustment.

    3. The present study

    In the present study, we examined the relationships between international

    students contact with host nationals, their ethnic identity, cross-cultural self-

    efficacy, and their sociocultural adjustment. Consistent with the foregoing

    discussion, it is hypothesized that international students contact with host nationals

    is positively related to their sociocultural adjustment (H1). It is also hypothesized

    that their ethnic identity is negatively related to their contact with host nationals

    (H2a) and sociocultural adjustment (H2b). We also hypothesize that their cross-

    cultural self-efficacy is positively related to their contact with host nationals (H3a)

    and sociocultural adjustment (H3b).

    Based on these hypotheses, we also examined two mediation models. Model one

    posits that the effect of international students ethnic identity on their sociocultural

    adjustment is mediated by their contact with host nationals. Model two posits that

    the effect of international students cross-cultural self-efficacy on their sociocultural

    adjustment is mediated by their contact with host nationals. To examine these two

    mediation models, we determine whether the effects of the predictors (ethnic identityand cross-cultural self-efficacy) on sociocultural adjustment are weakened or become

    non-significant when host contact is held constant (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken,

    2003). A non-significant relation between the predictor and the criterion when the

    mediator is held constant signals a fully mediated model, whereas a weakened but

    still significant relation indicates a partially mediated model.

    The present study focused on international students from Asian countries.

    Previous research has suggested that although most international students encounter

    difficulties during their adjustment processes, these problems appear to be most

    acute among students from Asian countries (Abe, Talbot, & Geelhoed, 1998;

    Heikinheimo & Shute, 1986; Henderson, Milhouse, & Cao, 1993). Due to significantdisparities in language, culture, and communication styles between most Asian

    countries and America, Asian students appear to experience more difficulties than

    students from other parts of the world (Toyokawa & Toyokawa, 2002). Surveys of

    Asian international students indicated that they experience considerable difficulties

    in language, academic performance, and social interactions in their sojourns (Schram

    & Lauver, 1988; Yang, Teraoka, Eichenfield, & Audas, 1994). Given the size of the

    Asian international student population in the United States (Asian international

    students accounted for over 55% of the total international student population), it is

    somewhat surprising that little research effort has been devoted to their adjustment

    concerns (Yang & Clum, 1995). Therefore, an important purpose of the presentstudy is to examine their sociocultural adjustment process and variables that

    contribute to their effective adjustment.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576 567

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    8/16

    4. Method

    4.1. Participants

    A total of 117 Asian international students from two state universities in the mid

    western region voluntarily participated in the present study. The participants

    were recruited through a variety of networking sources. The participants were

    from 17 countries and regions in Asia. The majority of the students were female

    (N 72), single (N 81), and graduate students (N 67). The average age was

    26.29 years (SD 5:54) and their average length of stay in the United States was

    25.92 months (SD 27:97). Participants reported an average TOEFL score of 578

    (SD 48:48).

    4.2. Measurements

    All participants completed a demographic questionnaire and measures of their

    sociocultural adjustment, cross-cultural self-efficacy, their contact with the hosts,

    and ethnic identity.

    Sociocultural adjustment. A 29-item sociocultural adjustment scale (SCAS)

    developed by Ward and Kennedy (1999) was used in the present study. The scale

    measured the degree to which participants perceived difficulties in understanding the

    local values and cultures, interacting with the hosts, meeting the demands of daily

    life, and behaving in a culturally appropriate manner. Sample items includedUnderstanding American jokes and humor, and Adapting to the local norms of

    behavior. Respondents used a 4-point likert scale ranging from extreme difficulty

    (1) to no difficulty (4). High scores obtained in this measure indicated fewer

    difficulties in social activities and better sociocultural adjustment. The a in the

    present study was .87.

    Ethnic identity. Ethnic identity was assessed using the 19-item multi-group ethnic

    identity scale developed by Phinney (1992). This scale measured individuals

    perceived ethnic identification, sense of belonging to their ethnic group, attitudes

    towards their ethnic group, and levels of involvement in ethnic activities. Sample

    items included I am happy that I am a member of my ethnic group, and I have alot of pride in my ethnic group and its accomplishments. Respondents used a 4-

    point likert scale ranging from strongly disagree (1) to strongly agree (4). High scores

    obtained in this measure indicated strong ethnic identification. The a in the present

    study was .76.

    Contact with host nationals. Contact with host nationals was measured with the 10-

    item inter-group contacts scale developed by Islam and Hewstone (1993).

    Participants responded to items enquiring about their perceived quantity and

    quality of contact with Americans. Sample items included Since you arrived in the

    United States, how much contact have you had with the Americans, and Would

    you consider your contact with the Americans as generally being pleasant.Respondents used a 5-point likert scale. High scores indicated more frequent and

    positive contact with Americans. The a in the present study was .91.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576568

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    9/16

    Cross-cultural self-efficacy. Self-efficacy was assessed using a revised 17-item scale

    developed by Fan and Mak (1998). The scale is comprised of four subscales,

    including social difficulties, social confidence, sharing interests with others, and

    willingness to take initiatives to establish friendships. Sample items included Beproactive in social situations, and Initiate friendships with the locals. Items were

    rated on a 4-point likert scale ranging from not confident (1) to very confident (4).

    High scores signalled more confidence in social functions. The a in the present study

    was .95.

    5. Results

    5.1. Descriptive statistics

    The means, standard deviations, and correlations for the dependent variables are

    shown in Table 1. Reliabilities obtained in this study are also reported for all scales

    with multiple items.

    A multivariate analysis did not find significant differences in the mean level of

    their sociocultural adjustment, cross-cultural self-efficacy, and ethnic identity as a

    function of gender and academic level (graduate vs. undergraduate). Asian students

    who were single reported more identified with their ethnic group than their married

    peers.

    5.2. Test of hypotheses

    Hypothesis H1 posited that Asian international students cross-cultural contact

    with their hosts is positively related to their sociocultural adjustment. This

    hypothesis was supported. Asian students contact with their hosts was significantly

    correlated with their sociocultural adjustment (r :61, p :01).

    Hypothesis H2a stated that Asian international students ethnic identity is

    negatively related to contact with their hosts. This hypothesis was also supported in

    the present study. Asian students ethnic identity was significantly correlated with

    their contact with their hosts (r :

    19, p :

    05). Hypothesis H2b predicted that

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    Table 1

    Means, standard deviation, reliabilities, and correlations among measured variables

    M SD 1 2 3 4

    1. Self-efficacy 2.88 .67 (.95)

    2. Sociocultural adjustment 3.14 .50 .69** (.87)

    3. Ethnic identity 2.64 .37 .10 .04 (.76)

    4. Contact 3.67 .81 .73** .61** .19* (.91)

    N 117.

    Note. Po:05; Po:01.

    Coefficient alpha reliabilities are presented on the diagonal.

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576 569

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    10/16

    Asian international students ethnic identity is negatively related to their socio-

    cultural adjustment. This hypothesis was not supported in this study. Asian students

    ethnic identity was not significantly correlated with their sociocultural adjustment

    (r :

    04, ns). Since ethnic identity was not significantly related to the socioculturaladjustment, the first mediation model, which posited that the effects of ethnic

    identity on Asian international students sociocultural adjustment would be

    mediated by their contact with host nationals, was not supported.

    Hypothesis H3a stated that Asian international students self-efficacy is posi-

    tively related to contact with their hosts. This hypothesis was supported. Asian

    students contact with their hosts was significantly correlated with cross-cultural

    self-efficacy (r :73, p :01). Hypothesis H3b posited that Asian interna-

    tional students cross-cultural self-efficacy is positively related to their sociocultural

    adjustment. This hypothesis was also supported. Asian students cross-cultural

    self-efficacy was significantly correlated with sociocultural adjustment (r :

    69,

    p :01).

    5.3. Test of the mediation model

    We followed Baron and Kennys procedure (1986) in testing the second mediation

    model (for cross-cultural self-efficacy). Specifically, in the first step, the predictor

    should be significantly related to the mediator. In the second and third steps, both

    the predictor and the mediator should be significantly related to the outcome

    variable. In the final step, the effects of the predictor on the outcome variable shouldbe attenuated when the mediator is controlled.

    In the second model, we hypothesized that Asian international students contact

    with host nationals would mediate the effect of their cross-cultural self-efficacy on

    sociocultural adjustment. Contact with the hosts was a significant predictor of

    sociocultural adjustment. Cross-cultural self-efficacy was also a significant predictor

    of sociocultural adjustment. After partialling out the effect of contact with host

    nationals, cross-cultural self-efficacy was still a significant predictor of sociocultural

    adjustment, but the effect was attenuated, which indicated that the effect of cross-

    cultural self-efficacy on sociocultural adjustment was partially mediated by contact

    with the hosts (See Fig. 1).

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    Cross-CulturalSelf-Efficacy

    Host ContactSocioculturalAdjustment

    .61**.73**

    .69**/.52**

    Fig. 1. Contact with the hosts as a mediator in the relationship between cross-cultural self-efficacy and

    sociocultural adjustment. Note. po:05. po:01. The beta after the slash is the effect of cross-cultural

    self-efficacy controlling for the effect of contact with the hosts.

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576570

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    11/16

    6. Discussion

    The present study made two contributions to the cross-cultural adjustment

    literature. First, this study examined Asian international students socioculturaladjustment and the factors that influenced their adjustment. The findings from this

    study indicated that contact with the hosts and cross-cultural self-efficacy were both

    related to Asian students successful sociocultural adjustment. Second, by integrating

    a number of theoretical perspectives, the current study formulated and examined two

    mediation models in the Asian student sample. The study suggested that Asian

    students contact with the hosts partially mediated the relationship between their

    cross-cultural self-efficacy and their sociocultural adjustment.

    In the present study, we examined two mediation models. The first me-

    diation model predicted that contact with host nationals would mediate the

    relationship between ethnic identity and sociocultural adjustment. As predicted by

    Hypothesis H1, Asian students contact with host nationals facilitated their

    sociocultural adjustment process. Consistent with previous research (Church,

    1982), the increased amount of contact with the hosts may have enabled them to

    develop local networks, understand the local cultures, and acquire social skills

    necessary for the effective adjustment to the new culture. The present study

    also supported Hypothesis H2a that ethnic identity was negatively correlated

    with contact with host nationals. Piontkowski (Piontkowski et al., 2000) argued

    that individuals who are strongly identified with their own ethnic group may be

    more likely to use their own culture for self-definition and avoid contact withother ethnic groups. The present study replicated this finding and indicated that

    Asian students who were more identified with their own ethnic group may be

    reluctant to engage in contact with the hosts. This argument, however, was

    somewhat tempered by the weak (though still significant) correlation between the

    two variables.

    The lack of a significant relation between ethnic identity and sociocultural

    adjustment was not altogether surprising. Berry (1984) argued that identifying with

    the host culture does not entail the relinquishment of ones original cultural identity.

    Individuals may maintain their own cultural identity while establishing meaningful

    relationships with their hosts. In support of this argument, recent research has foundthat co-national identification is independent from host identification and both

    identities may co-exist harmoniously in one individual (Leong & Ward, 2000).

    Moreover, the two dimensions of identity were related to different adjustment

    outcomes, with co-national identity related to psychological adjustment and host

    identity related to sociocultural adjustment (Ward & Kennedy, 1994). In view of

    these findings, it is possible that the current sample of Asian students may have

    maintained a congruent co-national and host identification and their ethnic identity

    did not interfere with their sociocultural adjustment. Alternatively, Church (1982)

    suggested that sojourners may benefit from close ties with their co-nationals who

    may provide social and emotional support during the adjustment process. Therefore,it is possible that Asian students in the present study received social support from

    their ethnic network that protected them from social and cultural difficulties during

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576 571

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    12/16

    their adjustment process. Future research should further examine this possibility

    among Asian students.

    The second mediation model predicted that contact with host nationals would

    mediate the relationship between cross-cultural self-efficacy and socioculturaladjustment. As predicted by Hypothesis H3a, Asian students cross-cultural self-

    efficacy was related to contact with the hosts, which in turn resulted in better

    adjustment (Hypothesis H3b). Contact with the hosts was found to partially mediate

    the effect of cross-cultural self-efficacy on sociocultural adjustment. Bandura (1986,

    1992, 1997) contended that self-efficacy influences individuals decision to initiate an

    action, their determination to persist even in the face of difficulties, and the amount

    of effort they expend towards the completion of a task. Self-efficacy is especially

    important in cross-cultural interactions that are more stressful and entail more

    uncertainties than within-culture communication. In support of this argument, Mak

    and Tran (2001) found that immigrant students in Australia who had higher cross-

    cultural self-efficacy reported better adjustment. In the current study, Asian students

    who were more self-efficacious were more likely to report initiating and maintaining

    extensive contact with the hosts; as a result, they were more likely to experience

    better sociocultural adjustment compared with those who were less efficacious. These

    findings were analogous to previous research (Mak & Tran, 2001) and further

    provided support to the pivotal role of cross-cultural self-efficacy in Asian

    international students sociocultural adjustment process.

    The results of the present study have some practical implications for educational

    practices. Asian students cross-cultural self-efficacy was found to be significantlyrelated to their contact with the hosts and sociocultural adjustment. Bandura (1986,

    1997) suggested that self-efficacy can be enhanced in four ways: master experience,

    vicarious experience, social persuasion, and physiological arousal. Mak and Tran

    (2001) suggested that the four sources could be incorporated into cross-cultural

    trainings and interventions. Specifically, Asian international students cross-cultural

    self-efficacy can be enhanced through actively engaging in controlled cross-cultural

    social interactions, watching peer performance in social contexts, soliciting feedback

    and encouragement for their own performance, and focusing on their own

    performance instead of their emotional arousal in social interactions. These

    theoretically grounded interventions may be instrumental in building Asian studentscross-cultural self-efficacy in social interactions and enhancing their sociocultural

    adjustment.

    A significant relationship between Asian students identification with their ethnic

    groups and their sociocultural adjustment was not found in the present study.

    Indeed, their ethnic identification may insulate them from some difficulties in the

    adjustment process (Church, 1982). Likewise, previous studies have also indicated

    that newcomers to a culture adjust better with co-national support (Church, 1982).

    In this perspective, intervention programs that advocate Asian students dissociation

    with their ethnic communities may not achieve the desired effect.

    These implications should be taken cautiously in light of the limitations of thisstudy. In discussing the limitations, we also suggest useful directions for future

    investigations. First, although our study, like others, treated Asian students as one

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576572

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    13/16

    group, there is evidence showing that there is appreciably great within-group

    variability among Asian students. Although Asian countries are geographically

    adjacent, they are culturally heterogeneous. For example, in Hofstedes values survey

    (1980), Singapore and Japan were in the opposite poles of masculinity. Futureresearch should attempt to replicate the findings from the present study in specific

    countries in Asia.

    Second, common method variance may be a relevant issue in the present study and

    most survey studies. Our findings were completely based on students self-reports.

    Future research should include data from other sources in addition to the

    participants. For example, participants spouses or friends may rate their observed

    adjustment of the participants in addition to self-reports.

    Third, we were not able to infer causality among the variables in the present study

    because of the cross-sectional design. It is recommended that a longitudinal design

    be employed in place of a cross-sectional design in future research. Longitudinal

    designs allow researchers to further explore causality and to investigate the unfolding

    effects of the variables on Asian international students adjustment at different

    phases of their sojourns in the new culture.

    7. Conclusion

    In conclusion, the present study investigated Asian international students

    sociocultural adjustment and factors that influenced their adjustment process. Wesuggest that Asian international students adjustment is a dynamic process that is

    influenced by a host of factors. Results from this study underscore the critical roles

    of host contact and cross-cultural self-efficacy in their adjustment process. This study

    adds to the understanding of Asian students adjustment processes and offers many

    avenues for future investigations.

    Acknowledgements

    Both authors contributed equally to the completion of this manuscript. This paperis based on the first authors masters thesis under the supervision of the second

    author. We thank Andrew Gilpin and Melvin Gonnerman for serving in the thesis

    committee and providing invaluable comments. This study is partly supported by a

    research grant from the College of Social & Behavioral Science, University of

    Northern Iowa.

    References

    Abe, J., Talbot, D. M., & Geelhoed, R. J. (1998). Effects of a peer program on international student

    adjustment. Journal of College Student Development, 39, 539574.

    Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Amir, Y. (1969). Contact hypothesis in ethnic relations. Psychological Bulletin, 71, 319342.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576 573

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    14/16

    Anderson, T. R., & Myer, T. E. (1985). Presenting problem, counselor contacts, and no shows:

    International and American college students. Journal of College Student Personnel, 26, 500503.

    Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Upper Saddle

    River, NJ: Prentice Hall.Bandura, A. (1992). Self-efficacy mechanism in psychobiologic functioning. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-

    efficacy: Thought control of action (pp. 355394). Washington, DC: Hemisphere Publishing Corp.

    Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: W. H. Freeman and Co, Publishers.

    Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological

    research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 51, 11731182.

    Befus, C. P. (1988). A multi-level treatment approach for culture shock experienced by sojourners.

    International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 12, 381400.

    Berry, J. W. (1984). Cultural relations in plural societies. In N. Miller, & M. Brewer (Eds.), Groups contact

    (pp. 1127). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Berry, J. W., & Annis, R. C. (1974). Acculturative stress. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 5,

    382406.Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Power, S., Young, M., & Bujaki, M. (1989). Acculturation attitudes in plural

    societies. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 38, 185206.

    Black, J. S., & Gregersen, H. B. (1991). Antecedents to cross-cultural adjustment for expatriates in Pacific

    Rim assignment. Human Relations, 44, 497515.

    Church, A. T. (1982). Sojourner adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 91, 540572.

    Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis

    for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Eshel, Y., & Rosenthal-Sokolov, M. (2000). Acculturation attitudes and sociocultural adjustment of

    sojourner youth in Israel. Journal of Social Psychology, 140, 677691.

    Fan, C., & Mak, A. (1998). Measuring social self-efficacy in a culturally diverse student population. Social

    Behavior and Personality, 26, 131144.Fugita, S. S., & OBrien, D. J. (1985). Structural assimilation, ethnic group membership, and political

    participating among Japanese Americans: A research note. Social Forces, 63, 986995.

    Furnham, A., & Bochner, S. (1982). Social difficulty in a foreign culture: An empirical analysis of culture

    shock. In S. Bochner (Ed.), Culture in contact: Studies in cross-cultural interaction (pp. 161198).

    Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Furnham, A., & Bochner, S. (1986). Culture shock: Psychological reactions to unfamiliar environments.

    London: Methuen.

    Harrison, J. K., Chadwick, M., & Scales, M. (1996). The relationship between cross-cultural adjustment

    and the personality variables of self-efficacy and self-monitoring. International Journal of Intercultural

    Relations, 20, 167188.

    Hayes, R. L., & Lin, H. R. (1994). Coming to America: Developing social support systems for

    international students. Journal of Multicultural Counseling and Development, 22, 716.Hechanova-Alampay, R., Beehr, T. A., Christiansen, N. D., & Van-Horn, R. K. (2002). Adjustment and

    strain among domestic and international student sojourners: A longitudinal study. School Psychology

    International, 23, 458474.

    Heikinheimo, P. S., & Shute, J. C. (1986). The adaptation of foreign students: Student views and

    institutional implications. Journal of College Student Development, 27, 399406.

    Henderson, G., Milhouse, V., & Cao, L. (1993). Crossing the gap: An analysis of Chinese students culture

    shock in an American university. College Student Journal, 27, 380389.

    Hofstede, G. (1980). Cultures consequence: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills,

    CA: Sage.

    Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Dimensions of contact as predictors of intergroup anxiety perceived

    out-group variability and out-group attitude: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology

    Bulletin, 19, 700710.

    Kagan, H., & Cohen, J. (1990). Cultural adjustment of international students. Psychological Science, 1,

    133137.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576574

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    15/16

    Leong, C. H., & Ward, C. (2000). Identity conflict in sojourners. International Journal of Intercultural

    Relations, 24, 763776.

    Lin, J. C. G., & Yi, K. Y. (1997). Asian international students adjustment: Issues and program

    suggestions. College Student Journal, 31, 473479.Mak, A. S., & Tran, C. (2001). Big five personality and cultural relocation factors in Vietnamese

    Australian students intercultural social self-efficacy. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,

    25, 181201.

    Masson, C. N., & Verkuyten, M. (1993). Prejudice, ethnic identity, contact and ethnic group preferences

    among Dutch young adolescents. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 156168.

    Open doors. (2001). New York: Institute of International Education.

    Pettigrew, T. F. (1997). Generalized intergroup contact effects on prejudice. Personality and Social

    Psychology Bulletin, 23, 173185.

    Phinney, J. S. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity measure: A new scale for use with diverse groups.

    Journal of Adolescence Research, 7, 156176.

    Piontkowski, U., Florack, A., Hoelker, P., & Obdrzalek, P. (2000). Predicting acculturation attitudes of

    dominant and non-dominant groups. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 24, 126.Sam, D. L. (2001). Satisfaction with life among international students: An exploratory study. Social

    Indicators Research, 53, 315337.

    Sandhu, D. S., & Asrabadi, B. R. (1994). Development of an acculturative stress scale for international

    students: Preliminary findings. Psychological Reports, 75(1), 435448.

    Schram, J. L., & Lauver, P. J. (1988). Alienation in international students. Journal of College Student

    Development, 29, 146150.

    Searle, W., & Ward, C. (1990). The prediction of psychological and sociocultural adjustment during cross-

    cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 14, 449464.

    Selltiz, C., & Cook, S. W. (1962). Factors influencing attitudes of foreign students towards their host

    country. Journal of Social Issues, 18, 723.

    Sewell, W. H., & Davidson, O. M. (1956). The adjustment of Scandinavian students. Journal of SocialIssues, 12, 919.

    Stening, R. W. (1979). Problems in cross-cultural contact: A literature review. International Journal of

    Intercultural Relations, 3, 269313.

    Stephan, W. G., Stephan, C. W., & Gudykunst, W. B. (1999). Anxiety in intergroup relations: A

    comparison of anxiety/uncertainty management theory and integrated threat theory. International

    Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23, 613628.

    Tajfel, H. (1978). Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup

    relations. New York: Academic Press.

    Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel, & W.

    G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 724). Chicago: Nelson-Hall Publishers.

    Toyokawa, T., & Toyokawa, N. (2002). Extracurricular activities and adjustment of Asian international

    students: A study of Japanese students. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 26, 363379.Tsang, E. W. K. (2001). Adjustment of mainland Chinese academics and students to Singapore.

    International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 25, 347372.

    Ward, C., & Chang, W. C. (1997). Cultural fit: A new perspective on personality and sojourner

    adjustment. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 21, 525533.

    Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1992). Locus of control, mood disturbance, and social difficulty during cross-

    cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 16, 175194.

    Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1993a). Psychological and socio-cultural adjustment during cross-cultural

    transitions: A comparison of secondary students overseas and at home. International Journal of

    Psychology, 28, 129147.

    Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1993b). Wheres the culture in cross-cultural transition? Comparative studies

    of sojourner adjustment. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 24, 221249.

    Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1994). Acculturation strategies, psychological adjustment, and sociocultural

    competence during cross-cultural transitions. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 18,

    329343.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576 575

  • 8/2/2019 Predicting Asian International Students

    16/16

    Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1999). The measurement of sociocultural adaptation. International Journal of

    Intercultural Relations, 23, 659677.

    Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (2001). Coping with cross-cultural transition. Journal of Cross-Cultural

    Psychology, 32, 636642.Ward, C., Okura, Y., Kennedy, A., & Kojima, T. (1998). The U-curve on trial: A longitudinal study of

    psychological and sociocultural adjustment during cross-cultural transition. International Journal of

    Intercultural Relations, 22, 277291.

    Ward, C., & Rana-Deuba, A. (1999). Acculturation and adaptation revisited. Journal of Cross-Cultural

    Psychology, 30, 422442.

    Yang, B., & Clum, G. A. (1995). Measures of life stress and social support specific to an Asian student

    population. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 17, 5167.

    Yang, B., Teraoka, M., Eichenfield, G. A., & Audas, M. C. (1994). Meaningful relationships between

    Asian international and US college students: A descriptive study. College Student Journal, 28, 108115.

    Yao, E. L. (1985). Adjustment needs of Asian immigrant children. Elementary School Guidance and

    Counseling, 19, 222227.

    Ying, Y. W., & Liese, L. H. (1994). Initial adjustment of Taiwanese students to the United States: Theimpact of postarrival variables. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 25, 466477.

    ARTICLE IN PRESS

    A. Li, M.B. Gasser / International Journal of Intercultural Relations 29 (2005) 561576576